cultural dimensions in twitter: time, individualism and power the two theories: pace of life,...

10
Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power Ruth Garcia-Gavilanes Yahoo! Research Universitat Pompeu Fabra Barcelona, Spain [email protected] Daniele Quercia Yahoo! Research Barcelona, Spain [email protected] Alejandro Jaimes Yahoo! Research Barcelona, Spain [email protected] Abstract Previous studies have established the link between one’s actions (e.g., engaging with others vs. minding one’s own business) and one’s national culture (e.g., collec- tivist vs. individualistic), and such actions have been shown to be important as they are collectively affiliated with a country’s economic outcomes (e.g., Gross Do- mestic Product). Hitherto there has not been any sys- tematic study of whether one’s action on Twitter (e.g., deciding when to post messages) is linked to one’s cul- ture (e.g., country’s Pace of Life). To fix that, we build different network snapshots starting from 55,000 seed users on Twitter, and we do so for 10 weeks across 30 countries (after filtering those with low penetration rates) for a total of 2.34 M profiles. Based on Hofstede’s theory of cultural dimensions and Levine’s Pace of Life theory, we consider three behavioral patterns on Twitter (i.e., temporal predictability of tweets, engaging with others, and supporting others who are less popular) and associate them with three different dimensions derived from the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla- tions: activity predictability negatively correlates with Pace of Life (r = -0.62), tweets with mentions nega- tively correlates with Individualism (r = -0.55), and power (e.g, Twitter popularity) imbalance in relation- ships (between, for example, two users mentioning each other) is correlated with Power Distance (r =0.62). These three cultural dimensions matter because they are associated with a country’s socio-economic aspects - with GDP per capita, income inequality, and education expenditure. Introduction Researchers have found that the ways people perceive and accept power differences, interact with each other, and per- ceive time, drastically differ across countries. For exam- ple, in certain countries (e.g., Japan), direct disagreement is synonym of confrontation, while speaking one’s mind is a virtue in others (e.g., USA). Also, cultures with a fast Pace of Life (e.g., Germany, Switzerland) tend to give more im- portance to punctuality and have less flexible schedules; by contrast, cultures with a slower Pace of Life (e.g., Brazil) Copyright c 2013, Association for the Advancement of Artificial Intelligence (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. are more flexible and give importance more to human inter- actions than to keeping the schedule (Levine 2006). Cultural variations across countries have been empirically studied using small-scale experiments and surveys in the real world. Geert Hofstede administered opinion surveys to IBM employees in over 70 countries (Hofstede and Minkov 2010). This data, with over 100,000 questionnaires, were one of the largest cross-national databases that existed in 1971. By analyzing it, Hofstede discovered that there were significant differences between cultures: he found that five main factors explained most of the variance in the data and called those factors cultural dimensions, and two of those have been widely studied. The first is Power Distance and reflects the extent to which people (especially those less powerful) expect and accept that the power is distributed un- equally (e.g., employees would rarely contradict their man- agers). The second dimension is called Individualism vs. Collectivism and reflects the extent to which social relation- ships are loose (e.g., people look after themselves and are likely to have friends outside their immediate families) as opposed to relationships integrated in strong and cohesive groups (e.g., friends are likely to be within families). An additional aspect that varies across countries and has been widely studied is Pace of Life. Robert Levine run dif- ferent experiments to capture Pace of Life in a variety of countries. In 31 countries, he and his students measured the time it takes for people to walk 100 meters in coffee shops, for post clerks to send a parcel, and they also kept track of the accuracy of clocks in public spaces (e.g., in post offices). That resulted into ranking those countries by what they then called Pace of Life (Levine 2006). Individualism, Power Distance, and Pace of Life have been found to determine how people behave differently in the same situations in the real world. The main goal of this work is to assess the extent to which such differences can also be captured from online interactions. We will see that these differences matter because they are associated with the economic aspects of GDP per capita, income inequality and education expenditure. To go beyond small-scale experiments and surveys, we consider Twitter, a microblog massively used worldwide, and set out to answer the following research question: Does national culture determine the temporal randomness with which Twitter users post, or the extent to which they men-

Upload: others

Post on 23-May-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power

Ruth Garcia-GavilanesYahoo! Research

Universitat Pompeu FabraBarcelona, Spain

[email protected]

Daniele QuerciaYahoo! ResearchBarcelona, Spain

[email protected]

Alejandro JaimesYahoo! ResearchBarcelona, Spain

[email protected]

Abstract

Previous studies have established the link between one’sactions (e.g., engaging with others vs. minding one’sown business) and one’s national culture (e.g., collec-tivist vs. individualistic), and such actions have beenshown to be important as they are collectively affiliatedwith a country’s economic outcomes (e.g., Gross Do-mestic Product). Hitherto there has not been any sys-tematic study of whether one’s action on Twitter (e.g.,deciding when to post messages) is linked to one’s cul-ture (e.g., country’s Pace of Life). To fix that, we builddifferent network snapshots starting from 55,000 seedusers on Twitter, and we do so for 10 weeks across30 countries (after filtering those with low penetrationrates) for a total of 2.34 M profiles. Based on Hofstede’stheory of cultural dimensions and Levine’s Pace of Lifetheory, we consider three behavioral patterns on Twitter(i.e., temporal predictability of tweets, engaging withothers, and supporting others who are less popular) andassociate them with three different dimensions derivedfrom the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism andPower Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity predictability negatively correlates withPace of Life (r = −0.62), tweets with mentions nega-tively correlates with Individualism (r = −0.55), andpower (e.g, Twitter popularity) imbalance in relation-ships (between, for example, two users mentioning eachother) is correlated with Power Distance (r = 0.62).These three cultural dimensions matter because they areassociated with a country’s socio-economic aspects -with GDP per capita, income inequality, and educationexpenditure.

IntroductionResearchers have found that the ways people perceive andaccept power differences, interact with each other, and per-ceive time, drastically differ across countries. For exam-ple, in certain countries (e.g., Japan), direct disagreementis synonym of confrontation, while speaking one’s mind is avirtue in others (e.g., USA). Also, cultures with a fast Paceof Life (e.g., Germany, Switzerland) tend to give more im-portance to punctuality and have less flexible schedules; bycontrast, cultures with a slower Pace of Life (e.g., Brazil)

Copyright c© 2013, Association for the Advancement of ArtificialIntelligence (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved.

are more flexible and give importance more to human inter-actions than to keeping the schedule (Levine 2006).

Cultural variations across countries have been empiricallystudied using small-scale experiments and surveys in thereal world. Geert Hofstede administered opinion surveys toIBM employees in over 70 countries (Hofstede and Minkov2010). This data, with over 100,000 questionnaires, wereone of the largest cross-national databases that existed in1971. By analyzing it, Hofstede discovered that there weresignificant differences between cultures: he found that fivemain factors explained most of the variance in the data andcalled those factors cultural dimensions, and two of thosehave been widely studied. The first is Power Distance andreflects the extent to which people (especially those lesspowerful) expect and accept that the power is distributed un-equally (e.g., employees would rarely contradict their man-agers). The second dimension is called Individualism vs.Collectivism and reflects the extent to which social relation-ships are loose (e.g., people look after themselves and arelikely to have friends outside their immediate families) asopposed to relationships integrated in strong and cohesivegroups (e.g., friends are likely to be within families).

An additional aspect that varies across countries and hasbeen widely studied is Pace of Life. Robert Levine run dif-ferent experiments to capture Pace of Life in a variety ofcountries. In 31 countries, he and his students measured thetime it takes for people to walk 100 meters in coffee shops,for post clerks to send a parcel, and they also kept track ofthe accuracy of clocks in public spaces (e.g., in post offices).That resulted into ranking those countries by what they thencalled Pace of Life (Levine 2006).

Individualism, Power Distance, and Pace of Life havebeen found to determine how people behave differently inthe same situations in the real world. The main goal of thiswork is to assess the extent to which such differences canalso be captured from online interactions. We will see thatthese differences matter because they are associated with theeconomic aspects of GDP per capita, income inequality andeducation expenditure.

To go beyond small-scale experiments and surveys, weconsider Twitter, a microblog massively used worldwide,and set out to answer the following research question: Doesnational culture determine the temporal randomness withwhich Twitter users post, or the extent to which they men-

Page 2: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

(a) Pace of life ranking (b) Collectivism vs. Individualism (c) High vs. Low Power Distance

Figure 1: Maps showing (a) Levine‘s Pace of Life ranking, (b) Hofstede’s Individualism and (c) Power Distance. Darker colorsreflect lower Pace of Life, higher Individualism score and higher Power Distance. Gray areas mark countries that have not beenincluded in Levine’s study or Hofstede’s.

tion, follow, recommend and befriend others? We crawlmore than 2.34 million user profiles (starting from 55K seedusers), their tweets during 10 weeks from March to May2011, their geographic locations, and corresponding timestamps (Section “Data Description”). Upon this data cov-ering the 30 most represented countries in our sample, wetest three main hypotheses associated with the three culturalaspects and, in so doing, we make four main contributions:

• We test whether the higher a country’s Pace of Life, themore predictable its citizens’ temporal patterns (Section“Pace of Life”). The link between Pace of Life and tem-poral predictability comes from the finding that countrieswith higher Pace of Life tend to schedule their time inmore predictable ways (Levine 2006), (Woods 2003). Totest this on Twitter, in our period of ten weeks, we divideeach working day into 5 segments and compute the extentto which each user tweets or mentions others in the samedaily segments. We aggregate all users in each of the 30countries, produce a country-level temporal predictabil-ity, and correlate it with the country’s Pace of Life. Thecorrelations are r = −0.62 for tweets’ temporal unpre-dictability, r = −0.68 for user mentions’, and r = −0.58for tweeting activity within working hours. These con-sistent results confirm that countries with higher Pace ofLife tend to be more predictable not only offline but alsoonline.

• We also test whether people in collectivist countries in-teract more with each other than those in individualisticcountries (Section “Individualism vs. Collectivism”). Wedo so by computing the percentage of users who mentioneach other. We find that the correlation between Individ-ualism index (one of Hofstede’s cultural dimensions) andthe extent to which users mention each other is as high asr = −0.55.

• We test whether users in countries comfortable with un-equal distribution of power (high power-distance coun-tries) will follow, recommend, and accept recommen-dations preferentially from users who are more popu-lar (Section “Power Distance”). To this end, we con-sider three types of relationships: a) who follows whom;b) who recommends whom; and c) who starts to fol-low whom upon a recommendation. For each relation-

ship, we compute the difference of followers betweenthe pair of users in the relationship, and call that powerimbalance. We then correlate country-level imbalancewith corresponding Power Distance (another one of Hof-stede’s cultural dimensions). We find that the correla-tions are r = 0.62 for “who follows whom” relationships;r = 0.33 for “who recommends whom” relationships, andr = 0.42 for “who starts to follow whom” relationships.

• We finally show that those three cultural dimensions areassociated with the three economic indicators of GDP percapita, income inequality and education expenditure. Wefind correlations as strong as r = 0.60.

These strong correlations suggest that cultural differencesare not only visible in the real world but also emerge in theway people use social media. To show why these culturaldimensions matter, we will study their relationships withsocio-economic indicators, including GDP per capita. Weconclude by discussing the theoretical and practical impli-cations of this work (Section “Discussion”).

Related WorkOur goal is to study variations of Twitter use across coun-tries. In a similar way, researchers have already analyzedhow a variety of aspects of the online world change acrosscountries. They have studied country-variability of the fol-lowing aspects:

Twitter network metrics and use of emotion words. Pobleteet al. studied more than 4.5 million Twitter users with corre-sponding reciprocal links and tweets (Poblete et al. 2011).Network features such as the modularity, density, diame-ter, assortiativeness and graph degree differed significantlyacross countries. For example, Indonesia’s network modu-larity is far higher than Australia, and the country with thehighest use of positive emotion words is Brazil.

Flickr pictures about the same thing but in different regions.Yanai et al. used state-of-the-art object recognition tech-niques to find representative geotagged photos related toa given concept and then studied how photos related to aconcept change across countries (Yanai, Yaegashi, and Qiu

Page 3: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

(a) Country-level presence on Twitter.

United States

BrazilUnited KingdomIndonesia

Canada

Mexico JapanNetherlandsVenezuela SpainAustralia

GermanyFranceArgentinaChile South Korea

ColombiaIrelandSouth Africa IndiaPhilippines TurkeyItalyRussiaSweden

New ZealandNorwayMalaysiaBelgiumSingapore

3

4

5

6

6.5 7.0 7.5 8.0 8.5

Log(Internet Penetration)

Log(

Use

r Cou

nt)

(b) Users in sample vs. Internet users.

Figure 2: (a) Plot of country-level presence on Twitter vs. the number of countries in our sample. The highest number ofcountries for which Twitter presence is significant is around 30. That is, by considering the top 30 countries (by top, we meancountries ordered by number of users our sample has), we strike the right balance between representative presence on Twitterand number of countries under study. (b) Number of users in our sample versus number of Internet users in a country. Bothquantities are log-transformed.

2009). They found, for example, that pictures of weddingcakes in US are much taller than those in Europe.

Travel destinations derived from Flickr posting. Based onwhere pictures are taken, Kling et al. derived travel patternsof a large number of Flickr users across countries (Klingand Gottron 2011). They then used clustering methods todetermine the extent to which any given pair of countries isrelated. They found that residents in Brazil and Chile havecommon travel destinations, for instance.

Color preferences in Instagram pictures. Hochman et al.extracted colors from pictures and found notable differ-ences across pictures of different countries (Hochman andSchwarts 2012). For instance, hues of pictures in New Yorkare mostly blue-gray, while those in Tokyo are characterizedby dominant red-yellow tones.

Download times of research publications. Wang et al. col-lected real-time data on which publication was downloadedat which time from the Springer Verlag website for 5 week-days and 4 weekends (Wang et al. 2012). Upon the result-ing dataset of 1,800,000 records, they found that downloadsduring weekends were the most common in Asian countries,and the least common in Germany.

Use of the Doodle scheduling web tool. Reinecke et al. stud-ied how the use of the web scheduling tool varies across 211countries (Reinecke et al. 2013). They did so by relatingactivity features (e.g., consensus, availability) to Hofstede’sColllectivism vs. Individualism dimension, and with Ingle-hart Survival and self expression values. They found thatusers of the tool in Germany tended to schedule far ahead oftime (around 28 days in advance, while those in Colombia

schedule up to 12 days in advance).

Expression of emotions in Twitter status updates. Golderand Macy studied the 500 million English tweets that 2.4million users produced during almost 2 years. Based ontheir hour-by-hour analysis, they found that offline patternsof mood variations also hold on Twitter: mood variationswere associated with seasonal changes in day length, peoplechanged their mood as the working day progressed, andthey were happier during weekends (Golder and Macy2011).

Query logs. Baeza-Yates et al. studied the geographiclocations of users who clicked on 759,153 Internet hosts.They found that users tended to click on hosts in other coun-tries in which people speak the same language, and clickscoming from countries with similar human developmentindex tend to end up into the same countries (Baeza-Yates,Middleton, and Castillo 2009). More recently, Borra andWeber analyzed the queries that resulted into clicks on thetop-155 U.S. political blogs to infer users’ political leanings(Borra and Weber 2012).

There has not been any work on how cross-country vari-ations of language independent features (e.g., predictabil-ity, mentions and subscription activity) in a general-purposeplatform (e.g., in Twitter) are associated with indicatorswell-established in anthropological studies (e.g, cultural di-mensions, Pace of Life). That is why we run such a studynext.

Page 4: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

Indonesia

JapanMexicoBrazil

Singapore

IrelandUnited KingdomNetherlands ItalyFranceUnited States

Canada

South Korea Sweden

Germany

0.20

0.25

0.30

0.35

0.40

0 10 20 30

Pace of Life (rank)

Entropy

(a) Entropy vs. Pace of Life.

IndonesiaVenezuela

MexicoJapanBrazilColombia

ChileSouth Korea Argentina

PhilippinesMalaysia Spain NetherlandsTurkeySouth Africa

Singapore Ireland Canada

FranceBelgiumSwedenNorway

New Zealand

Italy

Russia India

Germany

80

85

90

95

100

20 40 60 80

Individualism Index

Frac

tion

of E

ngag

emen

t

(b) Engagement vs. Individualism.

Indonesia

Venezuela

Norway

MalaysiaSingapore

Chile MexicoPhilippinesColombia

United States South Korea IndiaBrazilCanadaArgentinaAustralia RussiaItalyNew Zealand SpainGermany Japan FranceSouth AfricaUnited KingdomIreland TurkeyNetherlands BelgiumSweden

-1000

0

1000

2000

3000

4000

5000

30 60 90

Power Distance Index

In-d

egre

e Im

bala

nce

(c) Imbalance vs. Power Distance.

Figure 3: (a) Entropy of posting and mentioning activities versus Pace of Life. Countries with high pace of life tend to betemporally predictable. (b) Fraction of users engaged with others versus Individualism. In countries with low Individualism,users tend to engage with each other more. (c) Indegree imbalance between user-followee versus Power Distance users havestronger in-degree imbalance.

Data DescriptionFrom the Twitter stream API, we randomly selected 55Kusers who tweeted at least once in March 2011 and thathad an outdegree and indegree in the range [100, 1K]. Thischoice is imposed by API restrictions but has the side bene-fit of filtering away less legitimate (e.g., spam) users: themajority of spam users tend to have outdegree and inde-gree outside the range [100, 1K] (Lee, Eoff, and Caverlee2011). From these users, we selected those who specifiedtheir location in their profile; often these locations are ei-ther strings specified by the users themselves or GPS co-ordinates coming from their mobile devices. We then mapthese locations into (long,lat) points (using Yahoo! Place-Maker for user-specified strings), resulting into 12.6K seedusers that are geo-located. For these seed users, we collectedtheir outbound links (followees) and found that 1.96M ofthem had location information. Since one of our hypothe-ses require the study of recommendations (which are madeusing the follow friday hashtag #ff or #followfriday in Twit-ter), we also collected the recommendations (i.e., users tofollow) that were made by the followees (outbound links)during the subsequent 10 weeks. This resulted in 362Krecommended users with valid geolocations. Overall, wewill study 2.34M users (12.6K+1.96M+362K), their times-tamped tweets (considering all different timezones), andtheir locations.

The way we sample our users is convenient and easy tointerpret but might be biased by our particular choice ofseeds. To partly address this concern, we only consideredthe top-30 countries in our sample. We choose 30 becauseit is the highest number of countries in which presence onTwitter highly correlates with presence on the Internet (Fig-ure (2a)), and in which the number of per country users isalways more than 5K, ensuring statistically significance ofour results. Figure (2b) plots the number of users in oursample as a function of the number of Internet users. Mostof the countries follow a straight line. USA deviates consid-

erably from it simply because of its high Twitter penetrationrate.

Next, we consider those users and their countries andstudy their specific cultural aspects in the sections “Pace ofLife,” “Individualism vs. Collectivism,” and “Power Dis-tance”.

Pace of LifeHaving this data at hand, we can now start with the first di-mension of our analysis: Pace of Life. This differs acrosscountries: for example, Levine found that USA’s Pace ofLife is higher than Brazil’s (Levine 2006). One could ordercountries by the value residents give to time and would seethat Sioux Indians do not have a notion of time (they evendo not have a word for it); Brazilians have a ‘relaxed’ notionof it (e.g., Levine fount that students defined ‘being late’ asbeing 33 minutes late on average); and people in USA givehigh importance to time up to the point of associating it withmoney (e.g., people experience considerable levels of stressif deadlines are not met).

Another time system was proposed by Hall (Hall andHall 1990). He divided time perception into two cate-gories: monochronic and polychronic time. Monochronictime refers to paying attention to only one thing at a time. Inmonochronic cultures, people tend to schedule their activ-ities in a linear way, tend to be less flexible, and perceivetime as a measurable, quantifiable entity, something withreal weight and value. For these reasons, monochronic coun-tries are also considered more predictable, not least becausea fixed time is often allocated to any task or meeting. Suchcountries include United States, Germany, Switzerland, andJapan. By contrast, polychronic time refers to being in-volved with many things at once. In polychronic countries,people are more flexible with time, adapt their schedules toothers’ needs, and see time as a general guideline, somethingwithout substance or structure. Consequently, polychromiccountries are less (temporarily) predictable. Such countries

Page 5: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

include France, Italy, Greece, Mexico and some Eastern andAfrican countries.

The problem is that Hall did not provide any countryscores we could use for this study but “Levine’s Pace ofLife research has been indirectly linked to the observationsof Hall (1983) to suggest that polychronicity and Pace ofLife are negatively related” (Conte and Rizzuto 1999), andthat insight was recently used in the study of the schedulingtool of Doodle (Reinecke et al. 2013).

To paraphrase these ideas in the context of Twit-ter, we hypothesize the following relationship:

[H1.1]The activities (e.g., mentions, status updates) ofusers in countries with higher Pace of Life are moretemporarily predictable.

To test this hypothesis, since there are several factorsthat influence people’s routine during weekends, we leavethem out and analyze activities during working days, dur-ing which the differences between monochronic and poly-chronic cultures are more salient (Centre for Good Gover-nance 2011). After adjusting for the different time zones,we divide each day in five time intervals: sleeping time(00:00 - 05:59), rising time (6:00 - 8:59), working hours(9:00-17:59), dinner (18:00-20:59) and late night (21:00-23:59). This division allows us to separate working hoursfrom the rest of the day and effectively mark changes ofactivities (Golder and Macy 2011). Then, to capture eachuser’s predictability, we compute the user’s entropy in thosefive intervals, and we choose entropy because it is oftenused to characterize unpredictability in time series (Songet al. 2010). Specifically, we consider a measure of en-tropy proposed by (Krumme 2010; Song et al. 2010) calledtemporal-uncorrelated entropy and adapt it to our context.The temporal-uncorrelated entropy calculates the tweetingrandomness across time intervals for a given user and is de-fined by:

−Ni∑j=1

pi(j)log2pi(j) (1)

where pi(j) is the historical probability that user i posted intime interval j and Ni is the number of distinct time inter-vals in which user i posted his/her tweets. The whole sumreflects the (un)predictability of user i posting across all j′sintervals.

This metric is computed for the two main activities ofposting updates tweets and mentioning others. After obtain-ing these entropies for all users for these two activities, wecompute the Pearson product-moment correlation betweenthe geometric average of the country-level entropy and itscorresponding Pace of Life rank. Pearson’s correlation r∈ [-1, 1] is a measure of the linear relationship betweentwo random variables, whereby 0 indicates no correlationand +1(-1) perfect positive (negative) correlation. Table 1and Figure (3a) summarize the results. The higher the Paceof Life (monocronic countries), the lower the tweets’ tem-poral unpredictability (r(15) = −0.62) and user mentions’(r(15) = −0.68).

Entropy(Tweets)

Entropy(Men-tions)

Pace of Life (overall) -0.62** -0.68**Pace of Life (walking speed) -0.56** -0.61**Pace of Life (post office) -0.44 -0.50*Pace of Life (clock accuracy) -0.45* -0.51*

Table 1: Pearson correlation coefficients between the en-tropy of the activity in twitter and three measures of the paceof life, p-values are expressed with *‘s: p < 0.05 (∗ ∗ ∗),p < 0.05 (∗∗), and p < 0.1 (∗)

Figure (3a) shows the negative relationship between un-predictability and Pace of Life. Thirteen countries fol-low this relationship but two do not: Japan and Indone-sia. Japan’s Pace of Life is “one of the most demand-ing on earth” (Levine 2006) (after Switzerland, Ireland andGermany), and one would thus expect to find predictable(monochromic) temporal patterns for it. Instead, we findhigh unpredictability, and that matches what Hall foundmore than 20 years ago (Hall and Hall 1987): Japan is anoutlier, in that, it mixes high Pace of Life with strong poly-chronic characteristics, not least because of, Hall suggested,the importance attributed to social relationships. Also In-donesia (our second outlier) shows considerably higher un-predictability than the remaining countries, and that matcheswhat Levine found when he went to one of Jakarta’s post of-fice to buy stamps: “It took us considerably longer than inmany other countries to find this out.” The postal clerk wasmore interested in conversing about Levine’s life rather thanfulfilling his request.

Next, we focus on working hours only. Since peoplein countries with high Pace of Life schedule their time ina linear way, we expect hat they would tweet less duringworking hours, in proportion, to avoid any interruption:[H1.2] The percentage of a country’s users who havetweeted during working hours negatively correlates with thecountry’s Pace of Life.

The daily fraction of users in a country who tweet duringworking hours does indeed negatively correlate with Pace ofLife (r(15) = −0.58).

Individualism vs. CollectivismIn addition to pace of life, also human relationships differacross cultures. In high collectivist cultures, users tend tofocus more on the community to which they belong: forexample, peers tend to unconditionally support superiors’opinions. Such countries (e.g., Indonesia) are characterizedby “in-groups”, and their members are expected to look af-ter each other. By contrast, people from high individual-ist countries like the U.S. are in a more loosely knit so-cial network, and are generally expected to look after them-selves or only after immediate family members (Hofstedeand Minkov 2010).

Another characteristic that differentiate collectivist coun-tries from individualist ones is that the former tend to adopt

Page 6: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

Figure 4: Fraction of users engaging with others at different times of the day. Users in Indonesia and Brazil (collectivistcountries) engage with others more than those in UK, USA, and Canada (individualistic countries), and they consistently do sothroughout the day.

high-context communication as opposed to low-context. Inhigh-context cultures, people tend to emphasize interper-sonal relationships. According to Hall, these cultures prefergroup harmony and consensus to individual achievement:“flowery language, humility, and elaborate apologies aretypical” (Hall and Hall 1990). Also, the way people acquireinformation also varies between cultures. According to Hof-stede et al., the primary source of information is one’s socialnetwork in collectivist countries, while it is (news) media inindividualistic countries (Hofstede and Minkov 2010). Fi-nally, the right to privacy is relevant in many individualistsocieties, while letting one’s in-group invade one’s privatelife is acceptable in collective societies.

Based on these studies, one should thus expect that peo-ple in collectivist countries will engage into public conver-sations more than what people in individualist countries do.We thus hypothesize that:

[H2] The fraction of users who mention (engage in aconversation with) others negatively correlates with Indi-vidualism.

Using Pearson coefficients, we correlate each country’sfraction of users engaging in conversations with the coun-try’s Individualism index reported by (Hofstede and Minkov2010). We find that users in individualistic countries men-tion others far less than those in collectivist countries (thecorrelation coefficient is as high as rs(30) =-0.55 (p <0.005). This consistently holds at different times of theday, and Figure 4 exemplifies that by contrasting two highcollectivist countries (Indonesia and Brazil) with three highindividualistic countries(USA, UK, Canada). Figure (3b)then shows the correlation between lack of engagement andIndividualism, which is high for all countries except forGermany. This result matches that of a previous study onmicroblogs: German tweets received the least number ofmentions out of the 10 most common languages in Twit-ter (Hong, Convertino, and Chi 2011). Also, in Germany,few comments are left in blogs, and users react to commentslower than what users in less individualist countries such asRussia and China do (Mandl 2009).

Power DistanceHofstede defines Power Distance as the “extent to whichthe less powerful members of institutions and organizationswithin a country expect and accept that power is distributedunequally.” (Hofstede and Minkov 2010). In countries com-fortable with Power Distance, subordinates expect to be toldwhat to do: employees tend to prefer to have a boss whodecides autocratically (Biatas 2009). As such, hierarchyin organizations and inequalities are expected and desired,and that applies not only to work environments but also toschools and families.

The number of a user’s followers (indegree) does not nec-essarily reflect influence but does reflect popularity (Bakshyet al. 2011; Cha et al. 2010; Kwak et al. 2010)). Therefore,the power relationship between a pair of users is leveled, iftheir numbers of followers are comparable; while it is im-balanced, if the numbers of followers greatly differ. Basedon this observation, we posit that:

[H3] In countries comfortable with Power Distance, a pairof users who engage in any type of relationship is likely toshow indegree imbalance.

We correlate country-level indegree imbalance with cor-responding Power Distance (another one of Hofstede’s cul-tural dimensions). We find imbalance online and PowerDistance offline go together for all three types of relation-ships (Table 2): the correlations are r = 0.65 for “who fol-lows whom” relationships; r = 0.33 for “who recommendswhom” relationships, and r = 0.42 for “who starts to followwhom” relationships. Figure (3c) shows the correlation forthe “who follows whom” relationship. Norway, Venezuelaand Indonesia are outliers. It is difficult to see why this is thecase for Norway and Venezuela as there is no previous studyfor them in this matter. For Indonesia, instead, we found thatour results match those on blogs: 27% of all blog trends inthis country are about pop and celebrities, which may resultin Indonesian users following more celebrities than users inother countries (Silang 2011).

Critics might rightly argue that the number of followersdoes not necessarily reflect one’s popularity, not least be-cause there are “spammers” who accumulate followers by

Page 7: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

subscribing to random users profiles. This was the case es-pecially in the early years of Twitter (Mowbray 2010). Tocounter that criticism, in addition to indegree as proxy forpopularity, we consider the ratio in-degree/out-degree, cor-relate the corresponding country-level popularity imbalancewith Power Distance, and obtain the same correlation aswhen considering indegree (r = 0.67).

Why It MattersWe have found strong correlations between country-levelbehavioral patterns on Twitter and the three cultural aspectsof pace of life, individualism and power distance. Thosecorrelations translate into being able to track these three as-pects at fine-grained temporal levels - one does not needto wait for the next 10-year effort that replicates Levine’sstudy or Hofstede’s; on the contrary, by simply trackingbehavioral patterns on Twitter, one could predict the threecultural aspects to a considerable extent for countries thatare well represented on Twitter. However, before doingso, one may well wonder why these three aspects matterat all. To see why it would be important to use Twitterto track them, one should consider that the three culturaldimensions have been found to be associated with threemain economic indicators: GDP per capita, income inequal-ity and education expenditure (Hofstede and Minkov 2010;Wilkinson and Pickett 2010). We now test whether theseeconomic indicators do also correlated with our three Twit-ter features: temporal predictability, activity levels duringworking hours, engagement with others, and popularity im-balance. To ease explanation, we collate the results in Ta-ble 3 and comment them next.

GDP per capita. High collectivism was found to be re-lated to countries with low national wealth (Hofstede andMinkov 2010). To test whether this holds also for our Twit-ter features, we get hold of the Gross Domestic Productvalues for our 30 countries (these values reflect purchasingpower normalized by population) and correlate them withour four Twitter features. We find that GDP is associatedwith three features in a statistically significant way (firstrow in Table 3): low-GDP countries tend to be temporallyunpredictable (r = 0.55), be active during working hours(r = −0.57), and feel comfortable with popularity imbal-ance (r = −0.48). Figure (5a) shows this relationship andassociated outliers - Japan and Singapore. The result forJapan is explained by what we found in the Section “Pace ofLife.” The result for Singapore is explained by consideringthat the country has faced rapid economic growth rate andis thus “highly developed and enjoys remarkably open andcorruption-free environment, stable prices, and a per capitaGDP higher than that of most developed countries” 1. At thesame time, however, it preserves high collective character-istics typical of most Asian countries, and that explains theassociation of higher entropy with GDP.

Education Expenditure. We correlate education expendi-

1Information taken from the Central Intelligence Agency ofUSA

ture (as percentage of GDP) with our four Twitter featuresand find that countries with low education expenditure arecharacterized by the same features as countries with lowGPD, even if expenditure is normalized by it. They are (sec-ond row in Table 3): temporally unpredictable (r = 0.58),be active during working hours (r = −0.51), and feelcomfortable with popularity imbalance (r = −0.60).Figure (5b) depicts high correlation for most countries.Again, Indonesia is an outlier, as one would expect from theprevious results.

Income Inequality. Power distance was found to berelated to the use of violence in domestic politics andto income inequality(Hofstede and Minkov 2010). Onewidely-used way to measure income inequality is the Ginicoefficient. This measures the degree of inequality in thedistribution of family income in a country (Wilkinson andPickett 2010).The lower its value, the more equal a societyis. We find that unequal countries tend to be (third row inTable 3): temporally unpredictable (r = −0.53), be activeduring working hours (r = 0.49), and feel comfortablewith popularity imbalance (r = 0.58). It should comeas no surprise this last result: that the strongest predictorof income inequality is popularity imbalance (popularityinequality) in Twitter. Figure (5c) shows that India, Philip-pines, Singapore and Brazil are outliers. That is becausethese countries are characterized by disproportionately highlevels of inequality (Wilkinson and Pickett 2010).

DiscussionSocial media sites often assume that people from differentcountries use their services in very similar ways. By con-trast, we find that the use of Twitter considerably changesacross them. Fortunately, these changes are not random butare predictable so much so that simple country-level behav-ioral features derived from Twitter strongly correlate withcultural dimensions. Users in monochronic countries tendto be temporarily predictable, those in collectivist countriesconsiderably talk with each other, and those in countriesuncomfortable with power distance will not preferentiallyengage only with popular users. These findings might notonly have theoretical implications for future cross-culturalstudies but might also have practical implications, includ-ing the prediction of country-level economic indicators atfine-grained temporal level, and the design of culture awarerecommender system.

Theoretical Implications. Twitter is a distal commu-nication modality (distal in the sense that users areseparated in space and time), and it has been arguedthat it is not a social-networking tool but a broadcastingplatform of, for example, news and opinions (Kwak etal. 2010). Yet, our cultural analysis suggests that Twitterenjoys social-networking features, and that engagement ispredominant among users in collectivist countries. Thisstudy not only has suggested the extent to which Twitteruse is associated with specific culture dimensions, but alsopoints to the possibility that social media sites could be

Page 8: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

Pace of Life Correlation[H1.1]The activities (e.g, mentions, status updates) of users in countries with higherPace of Life are more temporarily predictable

r(15) = −0.62**r(15) = −0.68**

[H1.2]The percentage of a country’s users who have tweeted during working hoursnegatively correlates with the country’s Pace of Life

r(15) = −0.58**

Individualism Correlation[H2] The fraction of users who mention (engage in a conversation with) others nega-tively correlates with Individualism index

rs(30) = −0.55***

Power Distance Correlation[H3] In countries comfortable with Power Distance, a pair of users who engage inany type of relationship is likely to show indegree imbalance

r(30) = 0.62***r(30) = 0.33*r(30) = 0.42**

Table 2: Pearson correlation coefficients: (H1.1) between Pace of Life and the temporal predictability of users’ activity (men-tions and tweets); (H1.2) between Pace of Life and the percentage’s of a country’s users tweeting during working hours; (H2)between Individualism and the fraction of users engaged with others; and (H3) between Power Distance and in-degree imbal-ance shown in three types of relationships (“who follows whom”, “who recommends whom” and “who starts to follow whom”).p-values are expressed with *’s: p < 0.005 (∗ ∗ ∗), p < 0.05 (∗∗), and p < 0.1 (∗).

Indicator [HP1.1] Predictability [HP1.2] Users (%)in working hours

[HP2] Mentions [HP3] Imbalance

GDP per capita r(30) = 0.55*** r(30) = −0.57** r(30) = −0.41* r(30) = −0.48**Education r(30) = 0.58*** r(30) = −0.51*** r(30) = −0.24 r(30) = −0.60***Inequality r(30) = −0.53*** r(30) = 0.49** r(30) = 0.39* r(30) = 0.58***

Table 3: Pearson correlation coefficients of three socio-economic indicators (first column) with: predictability (second column),activity in working hours (third column), mentions (fourth column) and in-degree imbalance (fifth column). p-values areexpressed with *’s: p < 0.005 (∗ ∗ ∗), p < 0.05 (∗∗), and p < 0.1 (∗).

used to run large-scale cross-cultural studies and couldultimately become tools that promote computational socialscience. This is is a new discipline that aims at usinglarge archives of naturalistically-created behavioral data(of, for example, emails, tweets, Facebook contacts) toanswer social science questions (Lazer, D. et al. 2009;NSF 2011).

Practical Implications. Our findings could also be used todesign:

Culture-aware engagement tools. In collectivist countries,users do engage with each other by exchanging messagesand recommending others. One could design country-tailored tools that: promote interactions with strangers inindividualistic countries, and with strong ties in collectivistcountries; rank status updates based on interestingness insmall-power-distance countries, and on popularity in large-power-distance; targets ads in specific time of the day formonochronic countries, and in user-tailored for polychroniccountries.

Culture-aware people recommender. One increasingly im-portant feature in Twitter is its people recommender system,which suggests people one might know. This tool makes

suggestions based on structural features (e.g., common fol-lowers) and on content features (e.g., matching one’s topicsof interest). However, the tool might well benefit from cul-tural dimensions as well: recommending strangers is fine inindividualist countries but not in collectivist ones; or usersin large-power-distance are likely to preferentially followhighly-popular users.

Limitations and Future Work. Despite the strong corre-lations, this study suffers from five limitations. First, oursample was collected in a specific time frame. Critics mightrightly say that our findings may be co-founded by the daysdata was crawled. However, the sample spans 10 weeks and,as such, it might be large enough to capture the normal rou-tine of users. Second, we might run the risk to promotestereotyping of individuals based on their countries of ori-gin. This study is about ‘mean behavior’, and one shouldconsider that there is high variability across individuals inthe same country. Third, we naively equated use of men-tions with “engagement with others”, but that might not benecessarily the case. That is why, in the future, it might bebeneficial to propose a taxonomy that will distinguish one’spurposes when mentioning others (i.e., conversational, in-formative, attribution). Fourth, this has been an exploratorystudy in which causal inference has not been established

Page 9: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

United States

Brazil

United Kingdom

Canada

Mexico Japan

Netherlands

Venezuela

Spain

Australia

Germany

France

ArgentinaChile

South Korea

Colombia

IrelandSouth Africa

Turkey

ItalyRussia

Sweden

New Zealand

Norway

Malaysia

Belgium

Singapore

0.2

0.3

0.4

20000 40000 60000

GDP per capita

Entropy

(a) Entropy vs. GDP.

United States

Brazil

United Kingdom

Indonesia

Canada

MexicoJapan

Netherlands

Venezuela

Spain

Australia

Germany

France

ArgentinaChile

South Korea

Colombia

IrelandSouth Africa

India

PhilippinesTurkey

ItalyRussia

Sweden

New Zealand

Norway

Malaysia

Belgium

Singapore

0.2

0.3

0.4

2 3 4 5 6 7

Education Expenditure

Entropy

(b) Entropy vs. Education Expenditure.

United States

Brazil

United Kingdom

Indonesia

Canada

Mexico

Japan

Netherlands

Venezuela

SpainAustralia

Germany

France

Argentina

Chile

South Korea

Colombia

Ireland

South Africa

India

Philippines

Turkey

Italy

Russia

SwedenNew Zealand

Norway

Malaysia

Belgium

Singapore

0.0

2.5

5.0

7.5

10.0

20 30 40 50 60

Inequality

Imbalance

(c) Indegree vs. Inequality.

Figure 5: The relationships between Twitter features and socio-economic indicators

(and it was not the aim of the study). However, there aretwo remarks to be made: a) many of the observed relationson Twitter confirm those that are already known in the realworld; and b) some of the correlations are weak, but oth-ers are very strong, suggesting a dose-response form fromcountry characteristic to behavior on Twitter. Fifth, we havefocused on language-independent features. In the future, wewill explore how the use of language changes depending oncultural dimensions (Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan 2003)- for example, do individualistic countries use more singularfirst-person pronouns (e.g., I, my, mine, yo, eu, moi)?

AcknowledgmentsWe thank Eduardo Graells for his beautiful maps, andAmin Mantrach, Carmen Vaca and Neil Ohare for theirfeedbacks. This research was partially supported byEuropean Community’s Seventh Framework ProgrammeFP7/2007- 2013 under the ARCOMEM project; by theSpanish Centre for the Development of Industrial Tech-nology under the CENIT program, project CEN-20101037(www.cenitsocialmedia.es) “Social Media”; and by the EUSocialSensor FP7 project (contract no. 287975).

ReferencesBaeza-Yates, R. A.; Middleton, C.; and Castillo, C. 2009.The geographical life of search. In Web Intelligence.Bakshy, E.; Hofman, J. M.; Mason, W. A.; and Watts, D. J.2011. Everyone’s an influencer: quantifying influence ontwitter. In Proceedings of the fourth ACM InternationalConference on Web Search and Data Mining (WSDM).Biatas, S. 2009. Power distance as a determinant of re-lations between managers and employees in the enterpriseswith foreign capital. Journal of Intercultural Management.Borra, E., and Weber, I. 2012. Political insights: Exploringpartisanship in web search queries. First Monday.Centre for Good Governance. 2011. Soft skills for publicmanagers: Handbook of time management skills.Cha, M.; Haddadi, H.; Benevenuto, F.; and Gummadi, K. P.2010. Measuring User Influence in Twitter: The Million

Follower Fallacy. In Association for the Advancement ofArtificial Intelligence.Conte, J. M., and Rizzuto, T. E. 1999. A construct-orientedanalysis of individual-level polychronicity. Journal of Man-agerial Psychology.Golder, S. A., and Macy, M. W. 2011. Diurnal and Sea-sonal Mood Vary with Work, Sleep, and Daylength AcrossDiverse Cultures. Science.Hall, E., and Hall, M. 1987. Hidden differences: doingbusiness with the Japanese. Anchor Press/Doubleday.Hall, E., and Hall, M. 1990. Understanding Cultural Differ-ences. Intercultural Press.Henrich, J.; Heine, S.; and Norenzayan, A. 2003. Psy-chological aspects of natural language. use: our words, ourselves. Annual Review Psychology.Hochman, N., and Schwarts, R. 2012. Visualizing insta-gram: Tracing cultural visual rhythms. In Proc. 6th Inter-national AAAI Conference on Weblogs and Social Media(ICWSM).Hofstede, G., and Minkov, M. 2010. Cultures and Organi-zations: Software of the Mind, Third Edition. McGraw-Hill.Hong, L.; Convertino, G.; and Chi, E. H. 2011. Languagematters in twitter: A large scale study. In The 5th Inter-national AAAI Conference on Weblogs and Social Media(ICWSM).Kling, C. C., and Gottron, T. 2011. Detecting culture incoordinates: cultural areas in social media. In InternationalWorkshop on DETecting and Exploiting Cultural diversiTyon the Social Web (DETECT). ACM.Krumme, K. 2010. How Predictable: Patterns of HumanEconomic Behavior in the Wild. Massachusetts Institute ofTechnology, School of Architecture and Planning, Programin Media Arts and Sciences.Kwak, H.; Lee, C.; Park, H.; and Moon, S. 2010. Whatis twitter, a social network or a news media? In Proc. 19thACM International Conference on World Wide Web (WWW).

Page 10: Cultural Dimensions in Twitter: Time, Individualism and Power the two theories: Pace of Life, Individualism and Power Distance. We find the following strong correla-tions: activity

Lazer, D. et al. 2009. Computational Social Science. Sci-ence.Lee, K.; Eoff, B.; and Caverlee, J. 2011. Seven months withthe devils: A long-term study of content polluters on twitter.In Proc. The 5th International AAAI Conference on Weblogsand Social Media (ICWSM).Levine, R. 2006. A Geography of Time: The Temporal Mis-adventures of a Social Psychologist or How Every CultureKeeps Time Just a Little Bit Differently. University Press.Mandl, T. 2009. Comparing Chinese and German blogs. InProceedings of the 20th ACM Conference on Hypertext andHypermedia.Mowbray, M. 2010. The twittering machine. In Proc. 6thInternational Conference on Web Information Systems andTechnologies (WEBIST).2011. Rebuilding the Mosaic. National Science Foundation.Poblete, B.; Garcia, R.; Mendoza, M.; and Jaimes, A.2011. Do All Birds Tweet the Same? Characterizing Twit-ter Around the World. In Proc. 20th ACM Conference onInformation and Knowledge Management (CIKM).Reinecke, K.; Nguyen, M. K.; Bernstein, A.; Naf, M.; and

Gajos, K. Z. 2013. Doodle around the world: Onlinescheduling behavior reflects cultural differences in time per-ception and group decision-making. In Proc. 16th ACMConference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work andSocial Computing (CSCW).Silang, S. 2011. Snapshot of Indonesia Social Media Users(2011).Song, C.; Qu, Z.; Blumm, N.; and Barabasi, A.-L. 2010.Limits of predictability in human mobility. Science.Wang, X.; Xu, S.; Peng, L.; Wang, Z.; Wang, C.; Zhang,C.; and Wang, X. 2012. Exploring scientists’ workingtimetable: Do scientists often work overtime? Journal ofInformetrics.Wilkinson, R., and Pickett, K. 2010. The Spirit Level: WhyEquality is Better for Everyone. Penguin Books.Woods, W. 2003. Tick Tock!...Who Broke the Clock? Inno-vations International.Yanai, K.; Yaegashi, K.; and Qiu, B. 2009. Detecting cul-tural differences using consumer-generated geotagged pho-tos. In 2nd International Workshop on Location and the Web(LOCWEB) in CHI.