acanthocephalan worm rape

4
Homosexual Rape and Sexual Selection in Acanthocephalan Worms Author(s): Lawrence G. Abele and Sandra Gilchrist Source: Science, New Series, Vol. 197, No. 4298 (Jul. 1, 1977), pp. 81-83 Published by: American Association for the Advancement of Science Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1744233 Accessed: 20/09/2009 18:25 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=aaas. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with the scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. American Association for the Advancement of Science is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Science. http://www.jstor.org

Upload: kioue

Post on 13-Nov-2014

202 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Acanthocephalan Worm Rape

Homosexual Rape and Sexual Selection in Acanthocephalan WormsAuthor(s): Lawrence G. Abele and Sandra GilchristSource: Science, New Series, Vol. 197, No. 4298 (Jul. 1, 1977), pp. 81-83Published by: American Association for the Advancement of ScienceStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1744233Accessed: 20/09/2009 18:25

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available athttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unlessyou have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and youmay use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained athttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=aaas.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printedpage of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with thescholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform thatpromotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

American Association for the Advancement of Science is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve andextend access to Science.

http://www.jstor.org

Page 2: Acanthocephalan Worm Rape

in a quantitative descriptive theory of the vari- ables responsible for stereopsis. Reviews and discussions of this work can be found in H. Helmholtz [Physiological Optics (Optical So- ciety of America, Washington, D.C., 1925; Do- ver, New York, 1962)], K. N. Ogle [in The Eye, H. Davson, Ed. (Academic Press, New York, 1962), vol. 4, pp. 271-417], B. Julesz (2), and W. L. Gulick and R. B. Lawson [Human Stereop- sis: A Psychophysical Analysis (Oxford Univ. Press, London, 1976)].

2. B. Julesz, Foundations of Cyclopean Perception (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1971).

3. The emergence of binocularity during the course of mammalian evolution has attracted consid- erable attention, and several hypotheses have been advanced to account for it [see, for ex- ample, G. E. Smith, The Evolution of Man (Ox- ford Univ. Press, London, 1924); G. L. Walls (4); S. Polyak (5); W. E. LeGros Clark, Antecedents of Man (Edinburgh Univ. Press, Edinburgh, 1959); M. Cartmill, in The Function- al and Evolutionary Biology of Primates, R. H. Tuttle, Ed. (Aldine-Atherton, Chicago, 1972), pp. 97-122]. Only recently, however, has the as- sumption been confirmed that stereopsis is pres- ent in animals other than humans. R. Fox and R. Blake [Nature (London) 233, 55 (1971)], J. Pack- wood and B. Gordon [J. Neurophysiol. 38, 1485 (1975)], and R. Blake and H. V. B. Hirsch [Sci- ence 190, 1114 (1975)] have found behavioral evidence for stereopsis in the cat. E. W. Bough [Nature (London) 225, 42 (1970)], R. F. Sar- miento [Vision Res. 15,493 (1975)], and A. Cow- ey, A. M. Parkinson, and L. Warnick [Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 27, 93 (1975)] have found behav- ioral evidence for stereopsis in the rhesus mon- key. These data complement recent neurophysi- ological investigations that have revealed corti- cal neurons in cat and in monkey that respond optimally to retinally disparate stimuli [see, for example, H. B. Barlow, C. Blakemore, J. D. Pettigrew, J. Physiol. (London) 193, 327 (1967); P. 0. Bishop, in The Neurosciences: Second Study Program, F. 0. Schmitt, Ed. (Rockefeller Univ. Press, New York, 1970), pp. 471-485; D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, Nature (London) 225, 41 (1970)1.

4. G. L. Walls, The Vertebrate Eye (Cranbrook In- stitute of Science, Bloomfield Hills, Mich. 1942).

5. S. Polyak, The Vertebrate Visual System (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1957).

6. See, for example, the symposium edited by D. Ingle and G. E. Schneider, Brain Behav. Evol. 3 (Nos. 1-4), pp. 1-352 (1970); D. Ingle, Science 181, 1053 (1973); W. C. Hall, Brain Behav. Evol. 5, 95 (1972); T. A. Duff and S. 0. E. Ebbesson, Science 182, 492 (1973); H. B. Sarnat and M. G. Netsky, Evolution of the Nervous System (Ox- ford Univ. Press, London, 1974).

7. Neuroanatomical investigations by Karten and colleagues [for example, H. J. Karten, W. Hodos, W. J. H. Nauta, A. M. Revzin, J. Comp. Neurol. 150, 253 (1973)] have revealed many structures and pathways in the avian visual sys- tem that seem to be analogous to those in the mammalian visual system. In the bird telen- cephalon a region known as the visual Wulst re- ceives extensive input from both eyes and ap- pears to be an analog of the visual cortex in mammals. Pettigrew and Konishi (8) have re- corded from single neurons located within the Wulst of the owl and found neurons optimally responsive to disparate binocular stimuli.

8. J. D. Pettigrew and M. Konishi, Science 193, 675 (1976).

9. Behavioral tests of kestrel acuity revealed that it exceeds human acuity by a factor of 2.6 (10), a result consistent with estimates of falcon and hawk acuity derived from optical characteristics of the eye [R. Shlaer, Science 176, 922 (1972)] and from the anatomy of the retina [K. V. Fite and S. Rosenfield-Wessels, Brain Behav. Evol. 12, 97 (1975)]. The retinas of falcons and hawks have a well-developed temporal fovea in addi- tion to a central fovea; the lateral position of the temporal fovea provides binocular overlap [Walls (4), Polyak (5), Fite and Rosenfield-Wes- sels (above)].

10. R. Fox, S. W. Lehmkuhle, D. H. Westendorf, Science 192, 263 (1976).

11. The two-choice discrimination task we used is

in a quantitative descriptive theory of the vari- ables responsible for stereopsis. Reviews and discussions of this work can be found in H. Helmholtz [Physiological Optics (Optical So- ciety of America, Washington, D.C., 1925; Do- ver, New York, 1962)], K. N. Ogle [in The Eye, H. Davson, Ed. (Academic Press, New York, 1962), vol. 4, pp. 271-417], B. Julesz (2), and W. L. Gulick and R. B. Lawson [Human Stereop- sis: A Psychophysical Analysis (Oxford Univ. Press, London, 1976)].

2. B. Julesz, Foundations of Cyclopean Perception (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1971).

3. The emergence of binocularity during the course of mammalian evolution has attracted consid- erable attention, and several hypotheses have been advanced to account for it [see, for ex- ample, G. E. Smith, The Evolution of Man (Ox- ford Univ. Press, London, 1924); G. L. Walls (4); S. Polyak (5); W. E. LeGros Clark, Antecedents of Man (Edinburgh Univ. Press, Edinburgh, 1959); M. Cartmill, in The Function- al and Evolutionary Biology of Primates, R. H. Tuttle, Ed. (Aldine-Atherton, Chicago, 1972), pp. 97-122]. Only recently, however, has the as- sumption been confirmed that stereopsis is pres- ent in animals other than humans. R. Fox and R. Blake [Nature (London) 233, 55 (1971)], J. Pack- wood and B. Gordon [J. Neurophysiol. 38, 1485 (1975)], and R. Blake and H. V. B. Hirsch [Sci- ence 190, 1114 (1975)] have found behavioral evidence for stereopsis in the cat. E. W. Bough [Nature (London) 225, 42 (1970)], R. F. Sar- miento [Vision Res. 15,493 (1975)], and A. Cow- ey, A. M. Parkinson, and L. Warnick [Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 27, 93 (1975)] have found behav- ioral evidence for stereopsis in the rhesus mon- key. These data complement recent neurophysi- ological investigations that have revealed corti- cal neurons in cat and in monkey that respond optimally to retinally disparate stimuli [see, for example, H. B. Barlow, C. Blakemore, J. D. Pettigrew, J. Physiol. (London) 193, 327 (1967); P. 0. Bishop, in The Neurosciences: Second Study Program, F. 0. Schmitt, Ed. (Rockefeller Univ. Press, New York, 1970), pp. 471-485; D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, Nature (London) 225, 41 (1970)1.

4. G. L. Walls, The Vertebrate Eye (Cranbrook In- stitute of Science, Bloomfield Hills, Mich. 1942).

5. S. Polyak, The Vertebrate Visual System (Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1957).

6. See, for example, the symposium edited by D. Ingle and G. E. Schneider, Brain Behav. Evol. 3 (Nos. 1-4), pp. 1-352 (1970); D. Ingle, Science 181, 1053 (1973); W. C. Hall, Brain Behav. Evol. 5, 95 (1972); T. A. Duff and S. 0. E. Ebbesson, Science 182, 492 (1973); H. B. Sarnat and M. G. Netsky, Evolution of the Nervous System (Ox- ford Univ. Press, London, 1974).

7. Neuroanatomical investigations by Karten and colleagues [for example, H. J. Karten, W. Hodos, W. J. H. Nauta, A. M. Revzin, J. Comp. Neurol. 150, 253 (1973)] have revealed many structures and pathways in the avian visual sys- tem that seem to be analogous to those in the mammalian visual system. In the bird telen- cephalon a region known as the visual Wulst re- ceives extensive input from both eyes and ap- pears to be an analog of the visual cortex in mammals. Pettigrew and Konishi (8) have re- corded from single neurons located within the Wulst of the owl and found neurons optimally responsive to disparate binocular stimuli.

8. J. D. Pettigrew and M. Konishi, Science 193, 675 (1976).

9. Behavioral tests of kestrel acuity revealed that it exceeds human acuity by a factor of 2.6 (10), a result consistent with estimates of falcon and hawk acuity derived from optical characteristics of the eye [R. Shlaer, Science 176, 922 (1972)] and from the anatomy of the retina [K. V. Fite and S. Rosenfield-Wessels, Brain Behav. Evol. 12, 97 (1975)]. The retinas of falcons and hawks have a well-developed temporal fovea in addi- tion to a central fovea; the lateral position of the temporal fovea provides binocular overlap [Walls (4), Polyak (5), Fite and Rosenfield-Wes- sels (above)].

10. R. Fox, S. W. Lehmkuhle, D. H. Westendorf, Science 192, 263 (1976).

11. The two-choice discrimination task we used is very similar to the one described in our prior in- vestigation of kestrel acuity (10). The bird learns to fly from the starting perch to a perch located below each stimulus display. The distance be- tween the starting perch and the stimulus perch is 168 cm. The weight of the bird landing on the correct perch triggers the reward delivery sys- tem, which exposes a small cup containing a morsel of beef heart that can be seized by the bird's beak. If the bird lands on the incorrect perch the cup is not exposed and the stimulus

1 JULY 1977

very similar to the one described in our prior in- vestigation of kestrel acuity (10). The bird learns to fly from the starting perch to a perch located below each stimulus display. The distance be- tween the starting perch and the stimulus perch is 168 cm. The weight of the bird landing on the correct perch triggers the reward delivery sys- tem, which exposes a small cup containing a morsel of beef heart that can be seized by the bird's beak. If the bird lands on the incorrect perch the cup is not exposed and the stimulus

1 JULY 1977

display and other illumination in the alley is turned off briefly. Training to perform in the test apparatus is accomplished by gradually shaping behavior, following the tenets of operant condi- tioning, with food as the reward. The bird, weighed daily before and after testing sessions, was maintained in excellent health at an average weight of 100 g. All the bird's daily food ration was earned in two daily training-testing sessions of 20 to 25 trials, one session in the morning and one in the afternoon. Since beef heart does not satisfy all the bird's digestive and nutritive re- quirements, it was fed a mouse once a week.

12. Random element stereograms [developed and systematically investigated by B. Julesz; see, for example, Bell Syst. Tech. J. 39, 1125 (1960) and (2)] produce compelling stereoscopic depth per- cepts in the absence of monocularly discernible contours.

13. The technique of dichoptic stimulation through color separation, the anaglyph method, is well known [see, for example, Y. LeGrand, Form and Space Vision (Indiana Univ. Press, Bloom- ington, 1967); R. S. Woodworth, Experimental Psychology (Holt, New York, 1938)]. We have found that Wratten filters 29 (red) and 58 (green) provide acceptable color separation for the red and green colors produced by most modern col- or television receivers. The system we used for generating stereograms consisted of an electron- ic device that controlled the sweep and on-time of the red and green electron guns of a color tele- vision receiver. A large number of red dots and green dots, produced by a random generator, appeared to be continuously present on the screen (physically, a new dot pattern was gener- ated every 16 msec, the time required for one scan of the cathode-ray tube). Disparity was in- troduced by delaying the on-time of one of the guns at predetermined points during the scan. Variations in disparity magnitude and configura- tion of the stereoscopic form could be in- troduced. The device controlled two identical television receivers (Hitachi model 100-C), one for each alley of the testing apparatus; from the starting perch the display portion of each receiv- er was 4.4? by 6.2?. The stereoscopic form could be generated on either one of the receivers at the option of the experimenter.

14. The abscissa of Fig. 2 indicates the eight dis-

display and other illumination in the alley is turned off briefly. Training to perform in the test apparatus is accomplished by gradually shaping behavior, following the tenets of operant condi- tioning, with food as the reward. The bird, weighed daily before and after testing sessions, was maintained in excellent health at an average weight of 100 g. All the bird's daily food ration was earned in two daily training-testing sessions of 20 to 25 trials, one session in the morning and one in the afternoon. Since beef heart does not satisfy all the bird's digestive and nutritive re- quirements, it was fed a mouse once a week.

12. Random element stereograms [developed and systematically investigated by B. Julesz; see, for example, Bell Syst. Tech. J. 39, 1125 (1960) and (2)] produce compelling stereoscopic depth per- cepts in the absence of monocularly discernible contours.

13. The technique of dichoptic stimulation through color separation, the anaglyph method, is well known [see, for example, Y. LeGrand, Form and Space Vision (Indiana Univ. Press, Bloom- ington, 1967); R. S. Woodworth, Experimental Psychology (Holt, New York, 1938)]. We have found that Wratten filters 29 (red) and 58 (green) provide acceptable color separation for the red and green colors produced by most modern col- or television receivers. The system we used for generating stereograms consisted of an electron- ic device that controlled the sweep and on-time of the red and green electron guns of a color tele- vision receiver. A large number of red dots and green dots, produced by a random generator, appeared to be continuously present on the screen (physically, a new dot pattern was gener- ated every 16 msec, the time required for one scan of the cathode-ray tube). Disparity was in- troduced by delaying the on-time of one of the guns at predetermined points during the scan. Variations in disparity magnitude and configura- tion of the stereoscopic form could be in- troduced. The device controlled two identical television receivers (Hitachi model 100-C), one for each alley of the testing apparatus; from the starting perch the display portion of each receiv- er was 4.4? by 6.2?. The stereoscopic form could be generated on either one of the receivers at the option of the experimenter.

14. The abscissa of Fig. 2 indicates the eight dis-

parities that could be generated by the display system. The disparities are derived, with appro- priate correction for bird interpupillary distance, from the formula used for calculating disparity induced by discrete, monocularly vigible con- tours [C. H. Graham, in Handbook of Experi- mental Psychology, S. S. Stevens, Ed. (Wiley, New York, 1951), pp. 868-920]. While it was not possible to compare human performance with falcon performance under comparable condi- tions, we did ask humans to report on the magni- tude of perceived depth induced by the eight dis- parities. With their heads located adjacent to the starting perch they estimated, in arbitrary units, how far in front of the background the vertical rectangle appeared. For the first six disparities perceived depth increased. For the two largest disparities it become difficult to maintain the depth percept. Some observers reported that the rectangle no longer appeared in depth but lay in the same plane as the background. This diffi- culty with the larger disparities parallels the decrement in performance exhibited by the fal- con for the same disparities. These observations suggest to us that there are no dramatic dif- ferences in the range of disparities to which fal- con and human are sensitive.

15. Stereopsis by discrete contours has been called local stereopsis, while stereopsis by random ele- ment stereograms has been called global stere- opsis. See Julesz (2) and P. 0. Bishop and G. H. Henry [Annu. Rev. Psychol. 22, 119 (1971)] for a discussion of the distinction between local and global stereopsis.

16. Since many nonmammalians are oviparous, so- phisticated techniques derived from embryolog- ical research might be used to investigate the de- velopment of the binocular visual system. In this regard, Pettigrew and Konishi (8) report that early experience influences the development of binocular vision in owls.

17. We thank the Tennessee Wildlife Resources Agency and the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service for the excellent cooperation extended to us. The work was authorized by state scientific col- lectors permit No. 516 and federal scientific col- lectors permit PRT-7-01-C-Z-NV. Support was provided by NIH grant EY0093 1.

14 January 1977; revised 22 March 1977

parities that could be generated by the display system. The disparities are derived, with appro- priate correction for bird interpupillary distance, from the formula used for calculating disparity induced by discrete, monocularly vigible con- tours [C. H. Graham, in Handbook of Experi- mental Psychology, S. S. Stevens, Ed. (Wiley, New York, 1951), pp. 868-920]. While it was not possible to compare human performance with falcon performance under comparable condi- tions, we did ask humans to report on the magni- tude of perceived depth induced by the eight dis- parities. With their heads located adjacent to the starting perch they estimated, in arbitrary units, how far in front of the background the vertical rectangle appeared. For the first six disparities perceived depth increased. For the two largest disparities it become difficult to maintain the depth percept. Some observers reported that the rectangle no longer appeared in depth but lay in the same plane as the background. This diffi- culty with the larger disparities parallels the decrement in performance exhibited by the fal- con for the same disparities. These observations suggest to us that there are no dramatic dif- ferences in the range of disparities to which fal- con and human are sensitive.

15. Stereopsis by discrete contours has been called local stereopsis, while stereopsis by random ele- ment stereograms has been called global stere- opsis. See Julesz (2) and P. 0. Bishop and G. H. Henry [Annu. Rev. Psychol. 22, 119 (1971)] for a discussion of the distinction between local and global stereopsis.

16. Since many nonmammalians are oviparous, so- phisticated techniques derived from embryolog- ical research might be used to investigate the de- velopment of the binocular visual system. In this regard, Pettigrew and Konishi (8) report that early experience influences the development of binocular vision in owls.

17. We thank the Tennessee Wildlife Resources Agency and the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service for the excellent cooperation extended to us. The work was authorized by state scientific col- lectors permit No. 516 and federal scientific col- lectors permit PRT-7-01-C-Z-NV. Support was provided by NIH grant EY0093 1.

14 January 1977; revised 22 March 1977

Homosexual Rape and Sexual Selection in

Acanthocephalan Worms

Abstract. Acanthocephalan males have cement glands associated with the vas deferens, secretions from which seal the female vagina with a cap after copulation and so prevent subsequent insemination. Homosexual rape results in the male victim having the genital region sealed off with cement and effectively removed from the reproductive population. Sperm and cement are transferred to females during copu- lation, but apparently only cement is transferred to males during homosexual rape. Acanthocephalans conform to a parental investment model, and we interpret the evolution of the cement gland and sexual behavior as the result of sexual selection.

Homosexual Rape and Sexual Selection in

Acanthocephalan Worms

Abstract. Acanthocephalan males have cement glands associated with the vas deferens, secretions from which seal the female vagina with a cap after copulation and so prevent subsequent insemination. Homosexual rape results in the male victim having the genital region sealed off with cement and effectively removed from the reproductive population. Sperm and cement are transferred to females during copu- lation, but apparently only cement is transferred to males during homosexual rape. Acanthocephalans conform to a parental investment model, and we interpret the evolution of the cement gland and sexual behavior as the result of sexual selection.

Rape occurs in a variety of circum- stances, but in nonhumans it usually is related to male reproductive success. In some species of Drosophila, males are able to rape females that have already copulated (1). The sperm of the rapist, since it is the last insemination, is then packed into the seminal receptacle on top and is used first by the female fertiliz- ing a majority of the eggs (2). In the rhab- docoel turbellarian Stenostomum oeso- phagium, behavior has been reported (3) that we interpret as rape. Individuals are nonselfing hermaphrodites, and cross- fertilization occurs; but individuals will approach quiescent individuals and rap- idly evert the penis, hypodermically im- pregnating the quiescent individual with-

Rape occurs in a variety of circum- stances, but in nonhumans it usually is related to male reproductive success. In some species of Drosophila, males are able to rape females that have already copulated (1). The sperm of the rapist, since it is the last insemination, is then packed into the seminal receptacle on top and is used first by the female fertiliz- ing a majority of the eggs (2). In the rhab- docoel turbellarian Stenostomum oeso- phagium, behavior has been reported (3) that we interpret as rape. Individuals are nonselfing hermaphrodites, and cross- fertilization occurs; but individuals will approach quiescent individuals and rap- idly evert the penis, hypodermically im- pregnating the quiescent individual with-

out allowing cross-fertilization. There are other reports (4) of this sort of behav- ior among turbellarians, suggesting that individuals will promiscuously in- seminate but selectively allow them- selves to be inseminated. After homo- sexual rape in the anthocorid bug Xylo- caris maculipennis, the sperm of the rapist enters the vas deferens of the vic- tim and is used by the victim during copulation. An individual may be raped even while he is copulating with a female (5). We report here homosexual rape in the acanthocephalan worm Moniliformis dubius and interpret this behavior and other aspects of the biology of acantho- cephalans in the context of parental in- vestment and sexual selection.

81

out allowing cross-fertilization. There are other reports (4) of this sort of behav- ior among turbellarians, suggesting that individuals will promiscuously in- seminate but selectively allow them- selves to be inseminated. After homo- sexual rape in the anthocorid bug Xylo- caris maculipennis, the sperm of the rapist enters the vas deferens of the vic- tim and is used by the victim during copulation. An individual may be raped even while he is copulating with a female (5). We report here homosexual rape in the acanthocephalan worm Moniliformis dubius and interpret this behavior and other aspects of the biology of acantho- cephalans in the context of parental in- vestment and sexual selection.

81

Page 3: Acanthocephalan Worm Rape

Acanthocephalans are dioecious para- sitic worms that undergo larval devel- opment in arthropods and occur as adults in the alimentary tract of various vertebrates (6). In experimental infec- tions of M. dubius in laboratory rats the sex ratio of immature adults is approxi- mately 1 : 1 (7). The juveniles segregate spatially in the intestine with females in the carbohydrate-rich anterior region (15 to 35 cm posterior to the stomach) and males posterior to the females (8). At maturity males apparently migrate ante- riorly to the females. Thus, from a single infection all females reach maturity at about the same time in the same place, about a 20-cm portion of the intestine. Copulation begins when the worms are 16 days old, and under experimental con- ditions in which a single male has access to many females he can fertilize as many as 17 females (X = 7.88 + 4.79, range 2 to 17) (9).

Males have cement glands posterior to the level of the testes, which even- tually empty into the vas deferens, which then enters the cirrus; the cirrus terminates in a muscular eversible cup, the bursa (6). During copulation the everted bursa of the male wraps around the posterior end of the female, the cir- rus enters the female gonopore or vagi- na, and spermatozoa are transferred (6). The female vagina and genital region is packed and then "capped" by secretions from the cement gland, which block the vaginal region. The external cap is lost after a few days (10). The length of time that the cement remains in the genital tract is unknown, but it must be dis- sipated when egg release, via a remark- able and unique "egg sorter" (11), be- gins on or about day 38 (9, 12). A single insemination is apparently sufficient for the fertilization of a single batch of eggs. This is due, in part, to the fact that the sperm are long-lived and that females will continue to release fertilized eggs for up to 105 days in the absence of males (9) and to the possibility that females may store sperm (13). However, a prolonged egg-producing period seems to depend on continued male-female contact (9). Males have a higher mortality rate than females and may live for as long as 154 days, whereas females on the average live longer (7).

The above data on the biology of M. dubius can be interpreted in the context of sexual selection and parental invest- ment (14). Acanthocephalans conform to the model where one sex (female) invests considerably more than the other; males should therefore compete among them- selves to mate with females. We examine the biology of males to determine wheth-

82

er competition possibly exists and, if so, which form it takes.

Acanthocephalan sperm are long- lived, and the potential exists for mul- tiple inseminations resulting in progeny of mixed parentage, conditions under which sperm competition can occur. This results in evolutionary forces favor- ing males that either (i) displace stored sperm from a previous insemination or (ii) reduce the probability of subsequent inseminations (2). Since there is no phys- ical structure, such as a spermatheca, for sperm storage in acanthocephalans, the evolution in males of mechanisms that reduce the probability of subsequent in- seminations may be predicted. While we do not reject previous hypotheses that the cement gland and capping behavior have a function in preventing the escape of sperm (4, 6, 15) or that cement holds the couple together during copulation since the act requires precise positioning (16), we emphasize the possibility that cement glands and capping behavior evolved in response to sexual selection and function in preventing subsequent inseminations.

The distribution in time and space of reproductive females also affects sexual selection among males (14). In M. dubius and other acanthocephalans females are contagiously distributed in the intestine, and all mature from a single infection at about the same time. Thus the availabili- ty and location of reproductive females is highly predictable; consequently com- petitive interactions may be severe. Se- lection may favor male adaptations that lead to high reproductive success at the cost of increased mortality. There is dif- ferential male mortality in M. dubius (7) and in other acanthocephalans (6). We suggest that it occurs by "washout" dur- ing the migration to females (against per- istalsis and the spiraling action of the in- testine) and by additional male-male in- teractions that may cause dislodgement from the intestinal wall. In support of our hypothesis it is important to note that differential male mortality does not oc- cur until well after the time of sexual maturity (7).

Homosexual rape, by removing com- petitors from the reproductive popu- lation, may be another manifestation of the intense interactions for the resource of reproductive females. We observed an instantaneous frequency of homosexual rape of 2.5 percent in M. dubius (17). From 0 to 1 percent of males of Acan- thocephalas parksidei were capped, whereas capped males were noted only occasionally in Echinorhynchus truttae and Polymorphus minutus (18-20). The estimates are probably conservative

since it is difficult to detect males that had been raped and subsequently lost the exterior portion of the cap. In addition, the 0 to 2.5 percent figures represent in- stantaneous rates. If the external portion of the cap remains only 2 or 3 days, then, over the course of a single infection, as many as 50 percent of the males may be capped (21). We observed at least three additional males that appeared to have lost the external portion of the cap be- cause the bursa was partially inverted, and portions of what appeared to be ce- ment were present. Loss of the external portion of the cap would not necessarily allow the male to copulate since cement in the bursa area may prevent the full ex- tension of the bursa cup required for copulation. We examined sections of capped and uncapped males of M. dubius to determine whether sperm were present in the bursal portion of the gen- ital area of the capped male. We also ex- amined whole mounts of the bursal re- gion of two additional capped males. Sperm in the bursal region beneath the cap would likely belong to the rapist since sperm of the victim would be in the sperm duct of the cirrus. We were able to identify sperm in the duct of the un- capped males but were unable to locate any evidence that sperm was transferred with the cement during the rape. This supports our hypothesis that males dif- ferentiate between males and females, although we caution that sperm in the bursal region is probably in a foreign environment and could rapidly dis- integrate. We do not reject the hypothe- ses that homosexual rape may be due to poor sex recognition (19) or indiscrimi- nate copulation (18, 19). (However, two instances of homosexual rape occurred in the absence of females, suggesting that the males were not stimulated by any possible pheromone.) We postulate, however, that sperm competition may have led to the evolution of the cement gland and capping behavior and that this may represent a preadaptation that un- der sexual selection may have assumed the additional function of removing male competitors from the reproductive popu- lation. Caps found away from the genital region (20) on both males and females would then not be the result of poor sex recognition or indiscriminate copulation, but the result of activities of individual males and females attempting to avoid being capped. Males should avoid being capped since it prevents them from re- producing and females because it pre- vents them from releasing eggs (22).

Moniliformis dubius conforms to a pa- rental investment model, and we inter- pret the evolution of the cement gland

SCIENCE, VOL. 197

Page 4: Acanthocephalan Worm Rape

and sexual behavior to be the result of sexual selection, generated by (i) small male parental investment, (ii) long-lived sperm, (iii) males being capable of multiple inseminations, and (iv) females being contagiously located at a predict- able site and time in the host's intestines. Since the reproductive morphology of males and at least some of the population characteristics of M. dubius are charac- teristics of the entire phylum Acanthoce- phala, we suggest that our interpretation is a general situation in the phylum.

LAWRENCE G. ABELE SANDRA GILCHRIST

Department of Biological Science, Florida State University, Tallahassee 32306

References and Notes 1. A. Manning, Anim. Behav. 15, 239 (1967). 2. G. A. Parker, Biol. Rev. Cambridge Philos. Soc.

45, 525 (1970). 3. W. A. Kepner, J. S. Carter, M. Hess, Biol. Bull.

(Woods Hole, Mass.) 64, 405 (1933). 4. L. H. Hyman, The Invertebrates, vol. 2, Platy-

helminthes and Rhynchocoela; The Acoelomate Bilateria (McGraw-Hill, New York, 1951), p. 125.

5. J. Carayon, C.R. Acad. Sci. Paris 278, 2803 (1974).

6. D. W. T. Crompton, Cambridge Monogr. Exp. Biol. 17, 1 (1970); T. C. Cheng, General Para- sitology (Academic Press, New York, 1973), p. 545.

7. D. W. T. Crompton and D. E. Walters, Para- sitology 64, 517 (1972).

8. We found no overlap in the distribution of males and females prior to the 16th day of infection when males overlapped with females. P. L. Bur- lingame and A. C. Chandler [Am. J. Hyg. 33, 1 (1941)] report maximum densities of adult fe- males and males to be 18 and 38 cm posterior to the stomach, respectively; J. C. Holmes, J. Parasitol. 47, 209 (1961).

9. D. W. T. Crompton, Parasitology 68, 229 (1974).

10. This is based on our observation that mature fe- males with fertilized eggs can be found without caps 2 to 5 days after copulation. Cement may remain in the vaginal region.

11. P. J. Whitfield, Parasitology 61, 111 (1970).

and sexual behavior to be the result of sexual selection, generated by (i) small male parental investment, (ii) long-lived sperm, (iii) males being capable of multiple inseminations, and (iv) females being contagiously located at a predict- able site and time in the host's intestines. Since the reproductive morphology of males and at least some of the population characteristics of M. dubius are charac- teristics of the entire phylum Acanthoce- phala, we suggest that our interpretation is a general situation in the phylum.

LAWRENCE G. ABELE SANDRA GILCHRIST

Department of Biological Science, Florida State University, Tallahassee 32306

References and Notes 1. A. Manning, Anim. Behav. 15, 239 (1967). 2. G. A. Parker, Biol. Rev. Cambridge Philos. Soc.

45, 525 (1970). 3. W. A. Kepner, J. S. Carter, M. Hess, Biol. Bull.

(Woods Hole, Mass.) 64, 405 (1933). 4. L. H. Hyman, The Invertebrates, vol. 2, Platy-

helminthes and Rhynchocoela; The Acoelomate Bilateria (McGraw-Hill, New York, 1951), p. 125.

5. J. Carayon, C.R. Acad. Sci. Paris 278, 2803 (1974).

6. D. W. T. Crompton, Cambridge Monogr. Exp. Biol. 17, 1 (1970); T. C. Cheng, General Para- sitology (Academic Press, New York, 1973), p. 545.

7. D. W. T. Crompton and D. E. Walters, Para- sitology 64, 517 (1972).

8. We found no overlap in the distribution of males and females prior to the 16th day of infection when males overlapped with females. P. L. Bur- lingame and A. C. Chandler [Am. J. Hyg. 33, 1 (1941)] report maximum densities of adult fe- males and males to be 18 and 38 cm posterior to the stomach, respectively; J. C. Holmes, J. Parasitol. 47, 209 (1961).

9. D. W. T. Crompton, Parasitology 68, 229 (1974).

10. This is based on our observation that mature fe- males with fertilized eggs can be found without caps 2 to 5 days after copulation. Cement may remain in the vaginal region.

11. P. J. Whitfield, Parasitology 61, 111 (1970).

Considerable attention has been paid to disclosing the site and mechanism of action of narcotic analgesic drugs. Stere- ospecific opiate binding sites have been discovered and mapped in the mamma- lian brain (1-3). A substance (enkepha- lin) has been identified in brain tissue that appears to be an endogenous ligand for the opiate receptor, and its peptidic 1 JULY 1977

Considerable attention has been paid to disclosing the site and mechanism of action of narcotic analgesic drugs. Stere- ospecific opiate binding sites have been discovered and mapped in the mamma- lian brain (1-3). A substance (enkepha- lin) has been identified in brain tissue that appears to be an endogenous ligand for the opiate receptor, and its peptidic 1 JULY 1977

12. D. W. T. Crompton, S. Arnold, D. Barnard, Int. J. Parasitol. 2, 319 (1972).

13. K. H. Atkinson, thesis, Florida State University (1973).

14. R. L. Trivers, in Sexual Selection and the De- scent of Man, B. Campbell, Ed. (Aldine, Chi- cago, 1972), p. 136.

15. This is the most widely cited explanation. G. H. Ball [Univ. Calif. Berkeley, Publ. Zool. 33, 301 (1930)] suggested that the caps prevent sperm from entering immature females but this was based on the misconception that the females themselves secrete the caps.

16. H. J. Van Cleave, J. Morphol. 84, 427 (1949). 17. For M. dubius, rape occurred under male

to female ratios of 3:0, 2:0, and 12:18. We found no obvious relation between sex ratio or density and rape. This is due in part to the small sample size for capped males. We ran experi- mental ratios (male to female) of 1:1, 1.5:1, 2:1, 3:1, 0.8:1, 0.5:1, and 0.3:1. All were run with infections of 10, 20, and 30 cystacanths to test for density. We again emphasize that all estimates are conservative since many authors probably do not report capped males, and lost external caps are difficult to detect. For ex- ample, M. dubius is a widely used experimental animal, yet we could find no reference, other than our own, to capped males.

18. For references to capped males for A. parksidei, see 0. M. Amin, J. Parasitol. 61, 318 (1975).

19. For references to capped males for E. truttae, see J. B. E. Awachie, J. Helminthol. 40, 11 (1966).

20. For references to capped males for P. minutus, see W. L. Nicholas and H. B. N. Hynes, Ann. Trop. Med. Parasitol. 52, 36 (1958).

21. This is based on males reaching sexual maturity at about 16 days and living for 125 to 150 days. If we assume an average figure of 1.4 percent for homosexual capping and estimate that the exter- nal portion of the cap remains 3 days, then (125 - 16) divided by 3, times 1.4 percent is equal to 50.8 percent.

22. It benefits a female to avoid copulation at the time she begins releasing eggs. If the sperm be- comes depleted following the release of eggs, it would be advantageous to copulate again at such a time so as to maximize egg production. The report of 0. M. Amin (18) of capped females with mature embryos suggests that more than one copulation can occur. In fact it should oc- cur, for, if the cap was 100 percent effective, se- lection would eventually not favor the male ca- pacity for multiple inseminations.

23. We thank E. Connor, J. Farr, K. Heck, W. Lindberg, E. McCoy, R. Short, D. Simberloff, D. Strong, R. Trivers, W. Tschinkel, and N. Williams for comments; J. Byram for the initial stock of M. dubius; N. Contos and W. Heard for technical assistance; and M. Greenberg for en- thusiasm.

23 February 1977, revised 18 April 1977

12. D. W. T. Crompton, S. Arnold, D. Barnard, Int. J. Parasitol. 2, 319 (1972).

13. K. H. Atkinson, thesis, Florida State University (1973).

14. R. L. Trivers, in Sexual Selection and the De- scent of Man, B. Campbell, Ed. (Aldine, Chi- cago, 1972), p. 136.

15. This is the most widely cited explanation. G. H. Ball [Univ. Calif. Berkeley, Publ. Zool. 33, 301 (1930)] suggested that the caps prevent sperm from entering immature females but this was based on the misconception that the females themselves secrete the caps.

16. H. J. Van Cleave, J. Morphol. 84, 427 (1949). 17. For M. dubius, rape occurred under male

to female ratios of 3:0, 2:0, and 12:18. We found no obvious relation between sex ratio or density and rape. This is due in part to the small sample size for capped males. We ran experi- mental ratios (male to female) of 1:1, 1.5:1, 2:1, 3:1, 0.8:1, 0.5:1, and 0.3:1. All were run with infections of 10, 20, and 30 cystacanths to test for density. We again emphasize that all estimates are conservative since many authors probably do not report capped males, and lost external caps are difficult to detect. For ex- ample, M. dubius is a widely used experimental animal, yet we could find no reference, other than our own, to capped males.

18. For references to capped males for A. parksidei, see 0. M. Amin, J. Parasitol. 61, 318 (1975).

19. For references to capped males for E. truttae, see J. B. E. Awachie, J. Helminthol. 40, 11 (1966).

20. For references to capped males for P. minutus, see W. L. Nicholas and H. B. N. Hynes, Ann. Trop. Med. Parasitol. 52, 36 (1958).

21. This is based on males reaching sexual maturity at about 16 days and living for 125 to 150 days. If we assume an average figure of 1.4 percent for homosexual capping and estimate that the exter- nal portion of the cap remains 3 days, then (125 - 16) divided by 3, times 1.4 percent is equal to 50.8 percent.

22. It benefits a female to avoid copulation at the time she begins releasing eggs. If the sperm be- comes depleted following the release of eggs, it would be advantageous to copulate again at such a time so as to maximize egg production. The report of 0. M. Amin (18) of capped females with mature embryos suggests that more than one copulation can occur. In fact it should oc- cur, for, if the cap was 100 percent effective, se- lection would eventually not favor the male ca- pacity for multiple inseminations.

23. We thank E. Connor, J. Farr, K. Heck, W. Lindberg, E. McCoy, R. Short, D. Simberloff, D. Strong, R. Trivers, W. Tschinkel, and N. Williams for comments; J. Byram for the initial stock of M. dubius; N. Contos and W. Heard for technical assistance; and M. Greenberg for en- thusiasm.

23 February 1977, revised 18 April 1977

structure has been described (4). The midbrain periaqueductal gray matter (PAG) seems to be a major site of opiate analgesic action. Significant opiate bind- ing as well as enkephalin-containing fi- bers and fiber terminals are found here (2, 3, 5). Microinjections of morphine in- to PAG and electrical stimulation of this structure cause particularly potent anal-

structure has been described (4). The midbrain periaqueductal gray matter (PAG) seems to be a major site of opiate analgesic action. Significant opiate bind- ing as well as enkephalin-containing fi- bers and fiber terminals are found here (2, 3, 5). Microinjections of morphine in- to PAG and electrical stimulation of this structure cause particularly potent anal-

gesia (6, 7), and microinjections of en- kephalin and other, more recently dis- covered, opioid peptides into the ven- tricular system or into the PAG likewise yield analgesic effects (8). We have re- ported that analgesic doses of either sys- temically administered morphine or PAG electrical stimulation augment spon- taneous multiple unit firing in the PAG (9). These and other findings suggest, as we have previously concluded (9, 10), that activation of the PAG might normal- ly be associated with pain inhibition.

In our study we sought to examine fur- ther the effects of systemically injected morphine on PAG multiple unit firing and to determine whether intracerebro- ventricular (ICV) administration of anal- gesic doses of morphine and enkephalin would also influence neural activity in this brain region. We found that analge- sic doses of morphine, whether adminis- tered via the systemic or ICV route, re- liably augmented PAG multiple unit ac- tivity. The ICV injections of enkephalin provided comparable analgesia in only some animals, but only those showing analgesia exhibited a significant increase in PAG multiple unit firing. An unexpect- ed but striking observation was that en- kephalin caused potent and long-lasting electrographic seizures in most animals.

Male Sprague-Dawley rats (300 to 400 g) were prepared with permanently im- planted cannula guides, multiple unit re- cording electrodes, and skull electroen- cephalogram (EEG) leads according to standard procedures. Guides were made of 23-gauge stainless steel tubing and aimed for the lateral ventricle on one side. Multiple unit electrodes consisted of bundles of three to five 70-/xm Ni- chrome wires cemented together and in- sulated except at their cross-sectioned tips. Each rat received one such bundle of electrodes aimed at caudal PAG; sev- eral rats were implanted with a second bundle in locus coeruleus. Stainless steel screws were threaded into the skull over frontal and occipital cortex for EEG re- cording. The ICV injections were admin- istered via 27-gauge cannulas inserted in- to the guides. Testing began more than 1 week after surgery.

A modification of the tail-flick method (7, 11) was used for analgesia testing. Animals were restrained in plexiglass tubes from which their tails extended. An opening in the top of the tube gave access to the animal's electrode con-

gesia (6, 7), and microinjections of en- kephalin and other, more recently dis- covered, opioid peptides into the ven- tricular system or into the PAG likewise yield analgesic effects (8). We have re- ported that analgesic doses of either sys- temically administered morphine or PAG electrical stimulation augment spon- taneous multiple unit firing in the PAG (9). These and other findings suggest, as we have previously concluded (9, 10), that activation of the PAG might normal- ly be associated with pain inhibition.

In our study we sought to examine fur- ther the effects of systemically injected morphine on PAG multiple unit firing and to determine whether intracerebro- ventricular (ICV) administration of anal- gesic doses of morphine and enkephalin would also influence neural activity in this brain region. We found that analge- sic doses of morphine, whether adminis- tered via the systemic or ICV route, re- liably augmented PAG multiple unit ac- tivity. The ICV injections of enkephalin provided comparable analgesia in only some animals, but only those showing analgesia exhibited a significant increase in PAG multiple unit firing. An unexpect- ed but striking observation was that en- kephalin caused potent and long-lasting electrographic seizures in most animals.

Male Sprague-Dawley rats (300 to 400 g) were prepared with permanently im- planted cannula guides, multiple unit re- cording electrodes, and skull electroen- cephalogram (EEG) leads according to standard procedures. Guides were made of 23-gauge stainless steel tubing and aimed for the lateral ventricle on one side. Multiple unit electrodes consisted of bundles of three to five 70-/xm Ni- chrome wires cemented together and in- sulated except at their cross-sectioned tips. Each rat received one such bundle of electrodes aimed at caudal PAG; sev- eral rats were implanted with a second bundle in locus coeruleus. Stainless steel screws were threaded into the skull over frontal and occipital cortex for EEG re- cording. The ICV injections were admin- istered via 27-gauge cannulas inserted in- to the guides. Testing began more than 1 week after surgery.

A modification of the tail-flick method (7, 11) was used for analgesia testing. Animals were restrained in plexiglass tubes from which their tails extended. An opening in the top of the tube gave access to the animal's electrode con- nector for simultaneous electrophysio- logical recording. Baseline latencies to tail-flick withdrawal from the radiant heat source were averages of the first four (before treatment) trials in any test session and ranged between 3.0 and 3.5

83

nector for simultaneous electrophysio- logical recording. Baseline latencies to tail-flick withdrawal from the radiant heat source were averages of the first four (before treatment) trials in any test session and ranged between 3.0 and 3.5

83

Morphine and Enkephalin: Analgesic and Epileptic Properties

Abstract. Systemic and intracerebroventricular administration of analgesic doses of morphine resulted in large increments of spontaneous multiple unit activity in the periaqueductal gray matter of the awake rat. Intracerebroventricular injection of methionine enkephalin gave analgesia in only 8 of 19 rats, but in all 8, and in no others, increased periaqueductal multiple unit firing was also seen. These findings support the view that the periaqueductal gray matter is actively involved in endoge- nous mechanisms of analgesia. A striking observation was that enkephalin caused electrographic and behavioral epileptic phenomena in most animals. This observa- tion together with other recent findings suggests that endogenous enkephalin may play some role in epileptogenesis.

Morphine and Enkephalin: Analgesic and Epileptic Properties

Abstract. Systemic and intracerebroventricular administration of analgesic doses of morphine resulted in large increments of spontaneous multiple unit activity in the periaqueductal gray matter of the awake rat. Intracerebroventricular injection of methionine enkephalin gave analgesia in only 8 of 19 rats, but in all 8, and in no others, increased periaqueductal multiple unit firing was also seen. These findings support the view that the periaqueductal gray matter is actively involved in endoge- nous mechanisms of analgesia. A striking observation was that enkephalin caused electrographic and behavioral epileptic phenomena in most animals. This observa- tion together with other recent findings suggests that endogenous enkephalin may play some role in epileptogenesis.