5th annual flux congress

50
www.fluxsociety.org 5th Annual Flux Congress Hilton Portland Executive Tower Portland, Oregon, USA September 16-18, 2017

Upload: others

Post on 03-Jan-2022

12 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: 5th Annual Flux Congress

www.fluxsociety.org

5th AnnualFlux Congress

Hilton Portland Executive TowerPortland, Oregon, USA

September 16-18, 2017

Page 2: 5th Annual Flux Congress

Program-at-a-Glance5th Annual Flux Congress • September 16–18, 2017

8:00 AM8:30 AM8:45 AM9:00 AM9:15 AM9:30 AM9:45 AM10:00 AM10:15 AM10:30 AM10:45 AM11:00 AM11:15 AM11:30 AM11:45 AM12:00 PM12:15 PM12:30 PM12:45 PM1:00 PM1:15 PM1:30 PM1:45 PM2:00 PM2:15 PM2:30 PM2:45 PM3:00 PM3:15 PM3:30 PM3:45 PM4:00 PM4:15 PM4:30 PM4:45 PM5:00 PM5:15 PM5:30 PM5:45 PM6:00 PM6:15 PM6:30 PM6:45 PM7:00 PM7:15 PM7:30 PM7:45 PM8:00 PM8:15 PM8:30 PM8:45 PM

Sundaymp00:6 -

ma 03:8 yalpsi D no sretsoP

Regi

stra

tion

Desk

Ope

n 7:

30am

- 6:

00pm

Monday18-Sep17-Sep

Saturday16-Sep

Flash Talks (3:40 - 4:30)

Hippocampal Development(2:00-3:40)

Translational Neuroscience Symposium - Part 2

(11:10-12:25)

Lunch (12:25-1:25)

Science of Learning (1:25-3:00)

FLUX Excursion (7:00 - 10:00)

NIH/NICHD Updates(1:30-2:00)

Opening Reception(6:00 - 7:00)

Lunch (11:50-12:40)

Development of Attention(10:00 - 11:50)

Regi

stra

tion

Desk

Ope

n 8:

30am

-6:0

0pm

Huttenlocher Lecture (3:15 - 4:00)

YIA Lecture (3:00 - 3:15)

Development of Psychopathology

(8:30 - 09:40)

Post

ers

on D

ispl

ay 8

:30a

m -

6:00

pm

Cellular and Molecular Mechanisms in Development

(12:40-2:45)

Translational Neuroscience Symposium - Part 1

(9:00-10:50)

Break (10:50-11:10)

Poster Session 2 (4:00-6:00)

Poster Session 1 (4:00-6:00)

Coffee (8:00 - 8:30) Coffee (8:00 - 8:30)Coffee (8:00 - 8:30)

Break (11:10 - 11:30)

Post

ers

on D

ispl

ay 8

:30a

m -

5:00

pm

Poster Awards & Closing (4:30-5:00)

Regi

stra

tion

Desk

Ope

n 8:

30am

-5:0

0pm

Methods for Developmental Imaging

(8:30 - 9:50)

Social and Motivational Processes

(9:50 - 11:10)

ABCD (3:00-4:00)

Welcome (8:30 - 9:00)

Break (09:40 - 10:00)

Break (2:45-3:00)

Flash Talks(11:30 - 12:30)

Poster Session 3/Lunch(12:30-2:00)

Page 3: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 1

Inside front Flux Congress 2017 Program-at-a-Glance

1 About the Flux Congress

2-5 Welcome Letters

5 Flux Leadership

6 General Congress Information

7 Congress Venue Floor Plan

8 Flux Social Functions with map

9-1 4 Flux Congress Program Schedule

15-28 Flux Congress Oral Presentations

29-33 Flux Congress Poster Author Index

34-42 Flux Congress PostersTitles, Authors and Affiliations

43-45 Flux Congress Poster Floor Plan

46-47 Flux Congress Sponsorsand Exhibitors

About the Flux Congress

The aim of the congress is to provide aforum for developmental cognitiveneuroscientists to share their findings onthe development of brain processes thatsupport cognition and motivation from anintegrative neuroscience perspective.Thus, it provides an opportunity forscientists in the field to expand theirknowledge base, and also be betterinformed of translational approaches.

The Flux Society was launched in June2014, and has seen growth in itsmembership each year. To learn moreabout the Flux Society, please visitwww.fluxsociety.org.

Program Contents

Page 4: 5th Annual Flux Congress

2 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

We are excited as a committee andboard to bring to Portland a set ofdiverse speakers that span many

important areas in our field. We focused thisyear’s program on “neurodevelopmentalinsights on fundamental aspects ofbehavior”. Examples include, theNeurodevelopment of Attention, Learningand Memory, Psychopathology, SocialBehaviors and Motivation, and a specialAdolescent Brain and CognitiveDevelopment (ABCD) session, amongstmany others, including the Allen Instituteand Gates Foundation. In addition to ourstandard program, this year, we are addinga Young Investigator Award plus FlashTalksessions, which will highlight hot topics inDevelopmental Neuroscience. Under this scope, andconsidering feedback from our members from priormeetings, we welcomed both top senior and juniorinvestigators to present at the oral sessions, as well asseveral graduate students and postdocs. The hope isthat a range of views and important discussions willemerge from this format. These presentations alongwith the 145 posters at this years meeting shouldmake for an eventful and fulfilling meeting. Last, weare honored to feature Dr. Linda Spear at theHuttenlocher Lecture who has shaped the field and thetrajectories of the science in so many ways.

Keep Portland Weird!

Portland, Oregon is known for its creativity, innovationand independence. The largest city in Oregon,Portland boasts a wonderful array of attractions,including a shocking number of food-trucks, a wideselection of restaurants, music and theater, artgalleries, over 10,000 acres of public parks, 58 activebreweries, and of course Voodoo Donuts.

Portland is home to a number ofexcellent colleges and universities,including Oregon Health & ScienceUniversity (OHSU), one of the two hostinstitutions. Originally the medicalschool under the University of Oregon(UO) banner, it moved to Portland in the1870s, and in 1974 officially split fromthe UO (the second host institution).With the main campuses only two hoursapart, there continues to be very strongcollaboration and complementarystrengths between the two institutions.

Flux Congress Venue: The HiltonPortland Executive Tower

Our host hotel, the newly redesignedHilton Portland Downtown, is conveniently located inthe heart of Portland. Walk a block from our hotel tothe MAX Light Rail system for easy access to nearbycities and attractions like the Portland SaturdayMarket, the Oregon Museum of Science and Industry,and the Portland Art Museum.

We look forward to this stimulating meeting and tointeraction with all of the wonderful Flux Congressattendees.

Sincerely,

Nim TottenhamFlux Congress Program Chair

Nick Allen Damien FairBonnie Nagel Jenn PfeiferFred SabbFlux Congress Local Organizing Committee Chairs

Welcometo Flux Congress attendees

Page 5: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 3

the International Congress for IntegrativeDevelopmental Cognitive Neuroscience, inPortland! Wow, it’s already been five years and goingstrong. We are delighted that we are meeting inPortland for this Year’s Flux 5, 2017 where we had 175abstract submissions and 227 memberships andcontinue to grow.

We are very thankful for being hosted in Portland byleaders in the field including Damien Fair andBonnie Nagel at Oregon Health & Science University(OHSU) and Fred Sabb, Jenn Pfeiffer, and NickAllen from the University of Oregon. We areparticularly thankful for the fantastic job they did ofsecuring an wonderful conference venue and keepingour tradition of providing a chance to interact andform lasting bonds in an outstanding entertainingenvironment at the Hilton Portland Executive Tower.

Thank you to Nim Tottenham for the outstanding jobyou did as the Program chair with your committeemembers: Damien Fair, Eveline Crone, KateMcLaughlin, Sarah Durston, Monica Rosenberg,Leah Somerville, Noa Ofen, and Bea Luna

ensuring a high-level and innovative scientificprogram.

We are particularly grateful to have support from theJacobs Foundation, Oregon Health & ScienceUniversity, University of Oregon Vice President forResearch and Innovation & the Robert and BeverlyLewis Center for Neuroimaging, University of OregonCenter for Teaching and Learning, Prevention ScienceInstitute, Department of Psychology and the Universityof Oregon Special Education and Clinical SciencesDepartment. These collaborations enhance ourscientific aims including the ability to provide studenttravel awards and our first Young Investigator Award.

We were thrilled to have awarded 10 North Americanand 5 International student travel awards, along withfour Early Career Awards for speakers in a symposiumon the Science of Learning.

Thank you to the 2017 Huttenlocher Awardee LindaSpear for her outstanding body of work informinginfluential animal models of Developmental CognitiveNeuroscience and for opening the meeting by

Welcome to our fifth meeting of Flux,

continues over

Downtown Portland

Page 6: 5th Annual Flux Congress

4 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

enlightening us with her view of the field.

Thank you to the 2017 Young Investigator AwardDamien Fair for your already impressive andsignificant body of work, your continued andextensive collaborations in the field, and yourtranslational work to the community.

A special thank you to Podium Conference SpecialistsMarischal DeArmond and Pam Prewett who haveworked tirelessly organizing every detail andsupporting the effective execution of our conference.

Finally, a warm thank you to the members of the Fluxsociety and conference participants for making thetime to attend the Flux conference and making it suchan exciting event!

We would like to extend a warm welcome to newmembers and invite new members to join. To thosewho are new or have forgotten, “Flux” is not anacronym but rather a term used to remind us that, asdevelopmental cognitive neuroscientists, we aredistinct in our investigations of the dynamic nature ofcognition through development as stated in the aim ofthe Flux society:

“To advance the understanding of human braindevelopment by serving as a forum for professionaland student scientists, physicians, and educators to:exchange information and educate the nextgeneration of developmental cognitive neuroscienceresearchers; make widely available scientific researchfindings on brain development; encouragetranslational research to clinical populations; promotepublic information by discussing implications on thefields of education, health, juvenile law, parenting, andmental health, and encourage further progress in thefield of developmental cognitive neuroscience.”

The Flux Society strives to support Flux meetingsgoing forward, but also to expand our ability toprovide venues for scientific discussion andtranslational application.

We have received tremendous positive feedback fromprevious Flux meetings as well as great suggestionson improvements that have been incorporated into thedesign of this meeting as we continue to make thisunique event serve the needs and ambitions of ourgrowing society. We are actively considering ways thatwe can expand as a Society, finding new andinteresting ways to enhance discussion anddissemination. We are always looking for those whowant to become involved in extending venues for us

as a field to advance our science through discussionsand collaborations. We have an open search for thosewho want to head the organization of Webinars tohold discussions on current topics in DCN as well as anewsletter. If you are interested please approach aboard member at the meeting. We are happy to hearany suggestions from members regarding either theconference or ways in which the Flux Society can bestserve our field.

We want to remind you or our ever growing job bankwhere there are postings for every level of careerdevelopment for those looking for a position andthose looking to hire.

Finally, we are delighted to invite you to plan onattending the international Flux 6, August 30-September 1, 2018, in Berlin, Germany. Flux 6 willbe hosted by Ulman Lindenberger, director of theprestigious Max Planck Institute of HumanDevelopment The Max Plank Institute of HumanDevelopment is dedicated to the study of humandevelopment and education and have generatedoutstanding contributions to the field. Thisinternational meeting promises to be anotherextraordinary experience in our continued quest tosupport growth in our field.

We are looking forward to expanding ourunderstanding of developmental cognitiveneuroscience and interacting with attendees and areconfident that you will leave with greaterunderstanding, new friends, and enhanced creativityin your approach.

Sincererly,

Beatriz Luna Brad SchlaggarPresident Vice-President

Silvia Bunge Bruce McCandlissExecutive Secretary Executive Treasurer

Eveline CroneEducation Chair

Page 7: 5th Annual Flux Congress

Flux Leadership

Flux CongressManagement

Podium ConferenceSpecialistsMarischal De ArmondPam Prewett

Society Executive CommitteeBeatriz Luna University of Pittsburgh, USAPresident

Brad Schlaggar Washington University in St. Louis, USAVice President

Silvia Bunge University of California, Berkeley, USAExecutive Secretary

Eveline Crone Leiden University, Netherlands

Bruce McCandliss Vanderbilt University, USAExecutive Treasurer

Congress Local Organizing CommitteeFred Sabb University of Oregon, USA

Damien Fair University of Oregon, USA

Nick Allen University of Oregon, USA

Bonnie Nagel Oregon Health and Sciences University, USA

Jenn Pfeifer University of Oregon, USA

Congress Scientific Program CommitteeNim Tottenham, Chair Columbia University, USA

Damien Fair Oregon Health and Sciences University, USA

Bea Luna University of Pittsburgh, USA

Eveline Crone Leiden University, Netherlands

Kate McLaughlin University of Washington, USA

Sarah Durston University Medical Centre, USA

Utrecht, Netherlands

Monica Rosenberg Yale University, USA

Leah Somerville Harvard University, USA

Noa Ofen Wayne State University, USA

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 5

Portland in the evening

Page 8: 5th Annual Flux Congress

6 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Meeting VenueHilton Portland Executive Tower921 SW 6th AvePortland, OR 97204 USATel: +1-503-226-1611Fax: +1-503-220-2565

All congress sessions and the Welcome Cocktail Receptionwill take place at this location, and the Flux Excursion willtake place at an offsite venue.

RegistrationCongress registration fees include access to all sessionsincluding the welcome reception, speaker presentations,grazing lunches, coffee breaks, and poster sessions.

Name BadgesYour name badge is your admission ticket to all conferencesessions, reception, lunch, and coffee breaks. Please wear itat all times. At the end of the conference we ask that yourecycle your name badge at one of the name badgerecycling stations, or leave it at the Registration Desk.

Registration and Information Desk HoursThe Registration and Information Desk, located in thePavilion/Atrium Foyer, will be open during the followingdates and times:Saturday, September 16 8:00 AM – 6:00 PMSunday, September 17 8:00 AM – 6:00 PMMonday, September 18 8:00 AM – 3:00 PM

If you need assistance during the meeting, please visit theRegistration Desk.

StaffCongress staff from Podium Conference Specialistscan be identified by orange ribbons on their name badges.For immediate assistance, please visit us at the registrationdesk in the Pavilion/Atrium Foyer.

Complimentary WIFI Information:Complimentary Wifi is available in the hotel lobby on theground floor and in your hotel guestroom. Please note thereis no wifi available in the meeting rooms.

Network: PSAV_Meeting_Room Code: flux2017

Nearby Amenities:The Market – Open from 6:00am daily, located in the LobbyLevel of the hotel.

Hopcity Tavern – Opens at 6:30am daily, located in theLobby Level of the hotel.

Hopcity Tavern Lounge – Opens at 11:00am daily, locatedin the Lobby Level the hotel.

Starbucks – Opens at 4:30am daily, located across thestreet from the hotel.

Flux Social Functions

Opening Reception

The Opening Reception will take place at the AtriumBallroom from 6:00 – 7:00 PM. Light refreshments will beserved, and there will be a cash bar.

Flux Congress Excursion

This year’s Flux excursion will take place at PortlandBrewing Company Taproom located at 2730 NW 31st Avein Portland. Advance ticket purchase is required for thisevent. The taproom is a 15-minute drive from the hotel. Ifyou prefer to take transport, shuttle service from the hotelbegins at 5:45 PM from the bus loading zone located on SWSalmon Street in between 5th and 6th Street. Shuttles willreturn to the hotel starting at 9:00 PM.

Poster InformationInformation on Poster Authors, Poster Numbers and PosterTitles begins on page 29. For a complete list of all posterabstracts visit the Flux website www.fluxsociety.org

Easy reference Poster Floor Plans for each session canalso be found on pages 43–45 of this program.

Set-Up / RemovalThere are three Poster Sessions during the Congress andposters have been allocated to one of the sessions based onposter themes. Poster presenters must set-up and removetheir posters during the following times.

Poster Session 1 – Saturday, September 16Poster Set-up: Saturday, September 16: 7:30 AM – 8:30 AM

Poster Hours: 11:30 – 12:30 AM - Lunch Break4:00 PM – 6:00 PM – Poster SessionRemoval of all posters by: 7:00 PM on September 16

Poster Session 2 – Sunday, September 17Poster Set-up: Sunday, September 17: 7:30 AM – 8:30 AM

Poster Hours: 11:50 AM – 12:40 PM – Lunch Break4:00 PM – 6:00 PM – Poster SessionRemoval of all posters by: 7:00 PM on September 17

Poster Session 3 – Monday, September 18Poster Set-up: Monday, September 18: 7:30 AM – 8:30 AM

Poster Hours: 12:30 PM – 2:00 PM – Poster SessionRemoval of all posters by: 3:00 PM on September 18

General Congress Information

Page 9: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 7

Congress Venue Floor Plan

Hilton PortlandExecutive Tower

BallroomLevel

PlazaLevel

Page 10: 5th Annual Flux Congress

8 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Flux Social Functions

Central Portland Venue Locations

Opening ReceptionSaturday, September 16The Opening Reception will take place at the AtriumBallroom at the venue hotel, Hilton Portland Executive Tower,from 6:00 – 7:00 PM. Light refreshments will be served, andthere will be a cash bar.

Flux Congress ExcursionThis year’s Flux excursion will take place at PortlandBrewing Company Taproom located at 2730 NW 31st Avein Portland. Advance ticket purchase is required for this event.The taproom is a 15-minute drive from the hotel. If you preferto take transport, shuttle service from the hotel begins at 5:45 PM from SW Salmon St. between SW 5th Ave and SW6th Ave. Shuttles will return to the hotel starting at 9:00 PM

Page 11: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 9

Day 1 Saturday, September 16

8:00 – 8:30 AM Coffee

8:30 – 9:00 AM Welcome CommentsBeatriz Luna University of Pittsburgh, USA

Brad Schlaggar Washington University in St. Louis, USA

Fred Sabb University of Oregon, USA

Damien Fair Oregon Health and Sciences University, USA

Nim Tottenham Columbia University, USA

Translational Neuroscience in the Northwest Symposium – Part 1Chair: Bita Moghaddam Oregon Health & Science University, USADiscussant: Bonnie Nagel Oregon Health & Science University, USA

9:00 – 9:25 AM Adolescent dopamine disobeying adult rules: recent electrophysiological and behavioral findings

Bita Moghaddam Oregon Health & Science University, USA

9:30 – 9:55 AM The rapidly accelerating pace of autism genetics as a model for genetic studies in mental health

Brian O’Roak Oregon Health & Science University, USA

10:00 – 10:25 AM Development and plasticity of local and long-range cortical circuits in the mouse

Cris Niell University of Oregon, USA

10:25 – 10:50 AM Q&A

10:50 – 11:10 AM Break

Translational Neuroscience in the Northwest Symposium – Part 2Discussant: Fred Sabb University of Oregon, USA

11:10 – 11:35 AM Healthy brain development programs in global healthDan Marks Gates Foundation, USA

11:35 – 12:00 AM Adult human cortical cell type diversity defined by single nucleus RNA-sequencingTrygve Bakken Allen Institute, USA

12:00 – 12:25 PM Q&A

12:25 – 1:25 PM Lunch

Science of Learning SymposiumCo-chair: Silvia Bunge University of California at Berkley, USACo-chair: Bruce McCandliss Stanford University, USA

1:25 – 1:45 PM S.1.1 Early cerebral constraints on academic learning in children, adolescents and adults

Gregoire Borst Paris Descartes University, USA

1:45 – 2:05 PM S.1.2 White matter plasticity and reading: Network level changes track the learning process

Jason Yeatman University of Washington, USA

Flux Congress Program Schedule

Page 12: 5th Annual Flux Congress

10 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

2:05 – 2:25 PM S.1.3 The relationships among SES, white matter, and reading development: a longitudinal investigation from kindergarten to 2nd grade.

Ola Ozernov-Palchik Tufts University / MIT, USA

2:25 – 2:45 PM S.1.4 Altered processing of reward and punishment following early life stressCarolyn Johnson Harvard University, USA

2:45 – 3:00 PM Q&A

Young Investigator Award3:00 – 3:15 PM Damien Fair Oregon Health & Science University, USA

Huttenlocher Lecture3:15 – 4:00 PM Adolescence: Experience-seeking, experience-sculpting and phenotypic stabilization

Linda Spear Binghamton University, USA

4:00 – 6:00 PM Poster Session 1

6:00 – 7:00 PM Opening Reception

Day 2 Sunday, September 17

8:00 – 8:30 AM Coffee

Oral Session 1: Development of PsychopathologyChair: Nick Allen University of Oregon, USA

8:30 – 8:50 AM O.1.1 Early postnatal development of prefrontal-amygdala synaptic transmission

Roger Clem Mount Sinai School of Medicine, USA

8:50 – 9:10 AM O.1.2 Temper tantrums as Indicators of emotion dysregulation in childrenAmy Roy Fordham University, USA

9:10 – 9:30 AM O.1.3 Affect-biased attention as a core mechanism of emotion reactivity and regulationKoraly Perez-Edgar Penn State University, USA

9:30 – 9:40 AM Q&A

9:40 – 10:00 AM Break

Oral Session 2: Development of AttentionChair: Sarah Durston University of Utrecht, Netherlands

10:00 – 10:20 AM O.2.1 Brain development in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Sarah Durston University of Utrecht

10:20 – 10:40 AM O.2.2 Beyond the attentional homunculus: The developmental dynamics of attention, learning and memory

Gaia Scerif University of Oxford, UK

Flux Congress Daily Schedule

Page 13: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 11

10:40 – 11:05 AM O.2.3 When children are more open-minded learners than adults are: computation, evolution and phenomenology

Alison Gopnik University of California at Berkley, USA

11:05 – 11:30 AM O.2.4 Developing Inhibitory Control: The Role of Temporal Dynamics in Children’s AttentionYuko Munakata University of Colorado Boulder, USA

11:30 – 11:50 AM Q&A

11:50 – 12:40 PM Lunch

Oral Session 3: Cellular & Molecular Mechanisms in DevelopmentChair: Nim Tottenham Columbia University, USA

12:40 – 1:10 PM O.3.1 Cortical reorganization during adolescence: what the rat can tell us about the cellular basisJanice Juraska University of Illinois, USA

1:10 – 1:40 PM O.3.2 Epigenetic variation in developmental trajectories: Role of prenatal and postnatal experiencesFrances Champagne Columbia University, USA

1:40 – 2:10 PM O.3.3 Leveraging Dynamic Changes in Neural Circuitry During Adolescence to Persistently Attenuate Fear Memories

Siobhan Pattwell Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, USA

2:10 – 2:35 PM O.3.4 Placental mechanisms underlying sex differences in neurodevelopmental vulnerabilityBridget Nugent University of Pennsylvania, USA

2:35 – 2:45 PM Q&A

2:45 – 3:00 PM Break

Oral Session 4: ABCD SymposiumChair: Monica Luciana University of Minnesota, USA

3:00 – 3:10 PM O.4.1 How to describe neurodevelopment at the population level: Recruitment and sampling characteristics of the ABCD study.

Hugh Garavan University of Vermont, USA

3:10 – 3:20 PM O.4.2 Assessing mental health and substance use in 9 and 10-year-olds: The ABCD Assessment Protocol and early outcomes

Susan Tapert University of California San Diego, USA

3:20 – 3:30 PM O.4.3 Neurocognition in early adolescence and risk for later substance use: findings from ABCD’s first year of study

Monica Luciana University of Minnesota, USA

3:30 – 3:40 PM O.4.4 Mapping neural development supporting cognitive and emotion process in the ABCD

Deanna Barch Washington University, USA

4:00 – 6:00 PM Poster Session 2

7:00 – 10:00 PM Flux Excursion at Portland Brewing Taproom – advance ticket purchase required

Page 14: 5th Annual Flux Congress

12 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Day 3 Monday, September 18

8:00 – 8:30 AM Coffee

Oral Session 5: Methods for Developmental ImagingChair: Monica Rosenberg Yale University, USA

8:30 – 8:50 AM O.5.1 Characterizing attention with connectome-based predictive models

Monica Rosenberg Yale University, USA

8:50 - 9:10 AM O.5.2 Developmental changes in the effect of emotional cues on value-based decision-making and information maintenance in borderline personality disorder

Michael Hallquist University of Pennsylvania, USA

9:10 - 9:30 AM O.5.3 Movies in the magnet: The use of naturalistic stimuli in developmental neuroimagingTamara Vanderwal Yale University, USA

9:30 – 9:50 AM Q&A

Oral Session 6: Social and Motivational ProcessesChair: Leah Somerville Harvard University, USA

9:50 – 10:10 AM O.6.1 Neurodevelopmental mechanisms underlying normative shifts in goal-directed behavior

Leah Somerville Harvard University, USA

10:10 – 10:30 AM O.6.2 The role of control and motivation in the development of prosocial behavior

Niko Steinbeis Leiden University, Netherlands

10:30 – 10:50 AM O.6.3 Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene modulates reward circuit connectivity in youth with and without autism

Mirella Dapretto University of California Los Angeles, USA

10:50 – 11:10 AM Q&A

11:10 – 11:30 AM Break

Flash Talks – Part 1Co-chair: Bea Luna

Co-chair: Nim Tottenham

11:30 – 11:35 AM F.1.1 Striatal reward anticipation decreases from adolescence to young adulthood - but only when watched by a peerRosa Li Duke University, USA

11:35 – 11:40 AM F.1.2 Patterns of functional connectivity predict maturity and diagnostic status of individuals with Tourette syndrome.Ashley Nielsen Washington University in St. Louis, USA

11:55 – 12:00 PM F.1.7 Anterior cingulate theta band oscillations support development of cognitive flexibility through adolescence into adulthoodScott Marek University of Pittsburgh, USA

Flux Congress Daily Schedule

Page 15: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 13

12:00 – 12:05 PM F.1.8 Pavlovian and instrumental contributions to motivated behaviors across developmentHillary Raab New York University, USA

12:05 – 12:10 PM F.1.9 “No, don’t do it!” Neural correlates of sibling closeness during risky decision-making

Christy Rogers University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, USA

12:10 – 12:15 PM F.1.10 Developmental stabilization of neural gain signals improves mean behavioral performance and behavioral variability

David Montez University of Pittsburgh, USA

12:15 – 12:20 PM F.1.11 The representative developing brain: Does sampling strategy matter for neuroscience?

Kaja LeWinn University of California, San Francisco, USA

12:30 – 2:00 PM Lunch/Poster Session 3

Oral Session 7: Hippocampal DevelopmentChair: Noa Ofen Wayne State University, USA

2:00 – 2:20 PM O.7.1 Progress and limitations in assessing hippocampal functional maturation

Noa Ofen Wayne State University, USA

2:20 – 2:40 PM O.7.2 Hippocampal contributions to the development of episodic memory

Simona Ghetti University of California Davis, USA

2:40 – 3:00 PM O.7.3 Self-derivation of new knowledge through memory integration: The importance of binding and detection of deviatio

Patricia Bauer Emory University, USA

3:00 – 3:20 PM O.7.4 Hippocampal neurogenesis, forgetting and infantile amnesia

Paul Frankland Hospital for Sick Children, USA

3:20 – 3:40 PM Q&A

Flash Talks – Part 2Co-chair: Bea Luna

Co-chair: Nim Tottenham

3:40 – 3:45 PM F.2.1 Reduced orbitofrontal functional network centrality characterizes high neuroticism across childhood and adolescence

Louise Baruël Johansen Centre for Functional and Diagnostic Imaging and Research, Copenhagen University Hospital Hvidovre, Denmark

3:45 – 3:50 PM F.2.2 Influence of sex and pubertal development on functional connectivity

Katherine Reding National Institutes of Health, USA

3:50 – 3:55 PM F.2.3 The developing frontoparietal network: Spatial imitation performance predicts activation in young children

Sylvia Rusnak Georgetown University, USA

3:55 – 4:00 PM F.2.4 Sex differences in the effect of nucleus accumbens volume on adolescent drinking: The mediating role of sensation seeking and positive alcohol expectancies

Stephen Boyd Oregon Health & Science University, USA

Page 16: 5th Annual Flux Congress

14 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

4:00 – 4:05 PM F.2.5 Neighborhood effects on the brain: Impoverishment in early childhood predicts amygdala reactivity to ambiguous faces in young adulthood

Arianna Gard University of Michigan, USA

4:05 – 4:10 PM F.2.6 Hatching a Pokémon egg by closing your eyes: A new paradigm for measuring resting-state in preschoolers

Moriah Thomason Wayne State University / Perinatology Research Branch, NICHD/NIH, USA

4:10 – 4:15 PM F.2.7 Visual learning is modulated by reward value in infancy

Kristen Tummeltshammer Brown University, USA

4:15 – 4:20 PM F.2.8 Ovarian hormones organize the maturation of inhibitory neurotransmission in the frontal cortex at puberty onset in female mice.

David Piekarski UC Berkeley, USA

4:20 – 4:25 PM F.2.9 Neural corrlates of latent internalizing and externalizing psychopathology during adolescence

Brenden Tervo-Clemmens University of Pittsburgh, USA

4:25 – 4:30 PM F.2.10 Automaticity in the reading circuitry: A hallmark of skilled reading

Sung Jun Joo University of Washington, USA

4:30 – 5:00 PM Poster Awards/Closing Ceremony

Flux Congress Daily Schedule

Japanese Maple in Fall

Page 17: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 15

Flux Congress Oral Presentations

Day1 Saturday, September 16

Translational Neuroscience Symposium – Part 1 Chair: Bita Moghaddam, Oregon Health & Science UniversityDiscussant: Bonnie Nagel, Oregon Health & ScienceUniversity

Bita Moghaddam, Oregon Health & Science UniversityAdolescent dopamine disobeying adult rules: recentelectrophysiological and behavioral findingsOur knowledge of the dopamine system and most influentialtheories related to the function of the dopamine neuron in thecontext of reward processing and cognition are primarily fromdata collected in adult models. The organization of dopaminecircuitry, however, is a protracted process, which peaks inadolescence and ends only in early adulthood. Accordingly,reward processing and higher order cognition includingbehavioral inhibition undergoes changes in adolescence.Because of their extended developmental course, the shapingof midbrain dopamine neurons is particularly susceptible tolife experiences, especially those occurring duringadolescence. Yet, we know very little about theneurobiological events underlying the adolescent maturationof the dopamine system. I will focus on recent behavioral andelectrophysiological studies in rodents that describe criticaldifferences in reward processing between adolescence andadulthood and their influence on cognition. These studiesshow that reduced, and not exaggerated, activity ofadolescent dopamine neurons during the encoding of rewardanticipation may underlie the uniqueness of rewardprocessing at this age.

Brian O’Roak, Oregon Health & Science UniversityThe rapidly accelerating pace of autism genetics as amodel for genetic studies in mental healthMoving from candidate gene discovery to definitive validationof risk genes has been tremendously difficult in complex braindisorders, such as autism. A large part of this difficulty is theresult of genetic heterogeneity, which refers to many differentgenes playing a role. However, over the past few years newapproaches and strategies have begun to unlock autismgenetics. I will review these recent developments, which haveled to tractable strategies for robustly implicating manyindividual genes that when mutated are likely to result in achild developing autism. I will discuss how these new high-confidence risk genes are informing our understanding ofautism at the molecular level and beyond. Finally, I willdiscussion several new frontiers in autism genetics and howthese efforts may be applied generally to improve mentalhealth.

Cris Niell, University of OregonDevelopment and plasticity of local and long-rangecortical circuits in the mouseBrain function depends on coordinated activity in neuralcircuits that are established during development and modifiedby learning. We have implemented imaging methods inmouse cortex that allow measurement of neuronal activityfrom the level of individual neurons within a cortical area, upto large-scale dynamics across cortical areas. Using thesemethods, we have studied how learning a sensorydiscrimination task changes patterns of neural activity acrossthese different length scales. We are now extending thisapproach in order to investigate the maturation of corticalconnectivity during adolescence, by linking structural andfunctional imaging methods.

Translational NeuroscienceSymposium - Part 2Discussant: Fred Sabb, University of Oregon

Dan Marks, Gates FoundationHealthy brain development programs in global healthThere is increasing global recognition of the importance ofsupporting early child neurodevelopment from birth as acritical component of fostering healthy and thriving childrenand also to promote gains in future human capital.Approximately 250M under-5 children are not reaching theirdevelopmental potential in resource-limited settings that areexposed to stunting or extreme poverty. Our vision is toensure all children reach their developmental potential inorder to foster gains in child thriving and global humancapital. We aim to do so by better characterizing the burdenof children who are not reaching their full potential,characterize targetable risk factors contributing to this deficitand develop preventative public health interventions in ourprioritized countries. Central to these objectives areassessment tools capable of identifying infants and childrennot on track to reach their potential that are sensitive enoughto evaluate the impact of interventions.

Trygve Bakken, Allen InstituteAdult human cortical cell type diversity defined bysingle nucleus RNA-sequencing

The human cortex is composed of approximately 16 billionneurons that are densely interconnected and have diversemorphology, molecular signatures, and firing properties.Neurons can be grouped into types based on sharedfeatures, and these cell types simplify the description ofcortical circuits. Recent technological advances, includinghigh-throughput transcriptomic profiling of single cells, haveled to a refined census of cell types in mouse cortex and amuch coarser census in human cortex. In this study, weidentify a comprehensive set of human cortical cell types byclustering single nucleus RNA-sequencing data from over10,000 nuclei isolated from middle temporal gyrus of adulthuman cortex. Neuronal types have dramatically differentexpression patterns, including ion channels, G-protein-

Page 18: 5th Annual Flux Congress

16 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Flux Congress Oral Presentations

coupled receptors, and synaptic genes. Putative homologouscell types between mouse and human were identified basedon shared marker gene expression, although there weresubstantial expression differences between species. Thesereference cortical cell types can be compared to diseasestates and will guide development of genetic tools to targetcell types in human tissue.

Science of Learning Symposium Co-chair: Silvia Bunge, University of California at BerkleyCo-chair: Bruce McCandliss, Stanford University This symposium showcases the work of four researcherswho study how experience-dependent brain plasticitysupports learning. First, Grégoire Borst will discuss how brainanatomy – specifically, individual variability in sulcalmorphology – influences an individual’s capacity to learn.Then, Jason Yeatman will tell us how experience caninfluence white matter microstructure on a fast timescale.Next, Olga Ozernov-Palchik will examine how changes inwhite matter over the first few years of schooling supportlearning, and how they are moderated by early lifeexperience. All three of these talks use structural brainimaging methods to explore how children learn to read. Lastbut not least, Carolyn Johnson will provide mechanisticinsights into the effects of early life stress on learning, using arodent model to examine, at a cellular level, how priorexperience influences the ability to learn from feedback.

Gregoire Borst, Paris Descartes UniversityS.1.1 Early cerebral constraints on academic learningin children, adolescents and adultsFundamental school learning such as reading and intensetraining produce fundamental functional and structuralchanges in the brain. We will present studies investigatingthe converse issue namely whether fundamental schoollearning and the receptivity to executive training areconstraint by the anatomy of the brain. In particular, we willfirst present converging evidence that the sulcal morphology(i.e., a qualitative feature of the brain determined in utero andnot affected by brain maturation, learning and training) of theleft lateral occipito-temporal sulcus (OTS) hosting the visualword form area (VWFA) predicts reading skills in children andadults. In addition, we will present data from three studiesshowing that inhibitory control efficiency is constrained bythe sulcal morphology of two key regions of the inhibitorycontrol network namely of the anterior cingulate cortex andof the inferior frontal sulcus in children and adolescents.Finally, we will present preliminary data suggesting that thesulcal morphology of these two regions (ACC and IFS)predicts in part the receptivity to inhibitory control training inchildren (9-year-old) and adolescents (16-year-old).

Jason Yeatman, University of WashingtonS.1.2 White matter plasticity and reading: Networklevel changes track the learning processWhite matter tissue properties correlate with children’sperformance across domains ranging from reading, to math,to executive function. These correlations are generally

interpreted as reflecting stable anatomical differences thataffect the way children learn particular skills. However, thisinterpretation rests on an untested assumption thatanatomical properties are stable, at least over a relativelyshort time-scale. Here, we use an intervention design toexamine experience-dependent growth in reading skills andwhite matter in a group of children with dyslexia. DiffusionMRI data were collected longitudinally at regular 2-weekintervals during an intensive, 8-week reading intervention.These measurements reveal large-scale changes throughoutan extensive network of white matter tracts, over a rapidtimescale. Changes within this network track individualimprovements in reading skill. Additionally, we identify anetwork whose properties predict reading skill but remainfixed throughout the intervention, suggesting that someanatomical properties stably predict the ease with which achild learns to read, while others dynamically reflect theeffect of immediate experience. In the latter case,correlations between white matter and behavior depend onrecent educational experience. Thus, altering a child’seducational environment through a targeted training programcan alter both white matter and behavior on the timescale ofweeks. Large-scale change within this network may be ahallmark of rapid, short-term plasticity associated withintensive training of reading skills.

Ola Ozernov-Palchik, Tufts University/MIT

S.1.3 The relationships among SES, white matter,and reading development: a longitudinalinvestigation from kindergarten to 2nd grade.

Reading is a learned skill crucial for educational attainment.Children from lower compared to higher socioeconomicstatus (SES) families tend to have poorer reading outcomesand this gap widens across years of schooling. Readingrelies on the integration of multiple neural systems and theformation of specific white matter pathways. An emergingliterature documents correlations between SES andstructural/functional brain measures. Here we examine therelations among SES, white matter, and readingdevelopment in 119 children longitudinally from earlykindergarten to 2nd grade. Three bilateral white matter tractsimportant for reading were selected: arcuate fasciculus (AF),superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), and inferiorlongitudinal fasciculus (ILF). There was a significant positiveassociation between SES and (pre-) reading skills inkindergarten and 2nd grade. SES was positively associatedwith fractional anisotropy (FA) in the left ILF in kindergartenand explained a significant proportion of its variance abovelanguage and home literacy variables. In the higher SESgroup, better 2nd grade reading performance wasassociated with decreased FA in the right SLF inkindergarten, likely reflecting an initial left-lateralization ofearly reading skills. In contrast, in the lower-SES groupreading was positively associated with right ILF FA. Theseresults suggest that SES may influence the development ofreading networks in pre-reading children but the underlyingneurobiological mechanisms and environmental variablesfacilitating this effect need to be further examined.

Page 19: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 17

Carolyn Johnson, Harvard University

S.1.4 Altered processing of reward and punishmentfollowing early life stress

The maturing brain is exquisitely sensitive to experience andenvironmental influences. We propose that early life stress(ELS) may alter the developmental trajectory of response toreward and punishment, resulting in abnormal learning. Weutilize a mouse model to probe neural circuit changesfollowing ELS. Dams are given limited access to nestmaterial from postnatal days 2-9 in the ELS group. Controland ELS mice then participate in behavioral testing in theearly adolescent period. A naturalistic foraging task assesseslearning of cue-reward associations and flexible updating ofassociations during reversal. Separate groups are implantedwith cranial windows and trained on a head-fixed task tofacilitate two-photon imaging of neural responses to rewardand punishment. Auditory cues predict the delivery ofsucrose solution or aversive air puff. We find sex-specificeffects of ELS, with females exhibiting slow learning andinflexible reversal learning in the foraging task.Reinforcement learning modeling of choice patterns revealeda deficit in learning from reward experience in ELS females.Histology uncovered precocious condensation of peri-neuronal nets around PV inhibitory neurons in the prefrontalcortex (PFC), a region previously implicated in taskperformance. We therefore focused our in vivo imagingexperiments on inhibitory neurons in the PFC. We find thatseparate inhibitory networks are tuned for reward andpunishment and that tuning sharpens with age. We willdiscuss the potentially altered developmental trajectory ofinhibitory networks in ELS mice.

Young Investigator Award Damien Fair, Oregon Health & Science University

Huttenlocher Lecture Linda Spear, Binghamton UniversityAdolescence: Experience-seeking, experience-sculpting and phenotypic stabilizationThough the brain dynamically responds to experiencesthroughout the lifespan, its intrinsic potential for plasticity isactively dampened gradually during ontogeny. As a result,neural systems developmentally sculpted by experience arestabilized and their efficiency is increased. Heightenedplasticity followed by subsequent stabilization continues inforebrain regions through adolescence, resulting in theemergence of relatively durable phenotypes that may bemolded to some extent by the physical, social, cognitive oremotional circumstances experienced by the adolescent.Thus, maturational changes occurring in the adolescent brainnot only support adolescent-typical “experience-seeking”behaviors, but may themselves be customized commensuratewith those experiences. As an example, basic science studieswill be briefly highlighted showing adolescent-specific alcoholsensitivities that: a) may promote relatively high, binge levelsof alcohol use among vulnerable adolescents; and b) oftenpersist into adulthood after such exposure, producing anenduring “adolescentized” phenotype. Much remains to be

learned about the nature of adolescent experiences that leadto lasting phenotypic change, their age-specificity, and themechanisms underlying different types of experience-associated plasticity and stabilization.

Day2 Sunday September 17

Oral Session 1 Development of PsychopathologyChair: Nick Allen, University of Oregon

Roger Clem, Mount Sinai School of Medicine O.1.1 Early postnatal development of prefrontal-amygdala synaptic transmission sensitive periods inthe development of this critical A brain network comprising the medial prefrontal cortex(mPFC) and amygdala plays important roles indevelopmentally regulated cognitive and emotionalprocesses. However, very little is known about the maturationof mPFC-amygdala circuitry. We conducted anatomical tracingof mPFC projections and optogenetic interrogation of theirsynaptic connections with neurons in the basolateralamygdala (BLA) at neonatal to adult developmental stages inmice. Results indicate that mPFC-BLA projections exhibitdelayed emergence relative to other mPFC pathways andestablish synaptic transmission with BLA excitatory andinhibitory neurons in late infancy, events that coincide with amassive increase in overall synaptic drive. During subsequentadolescence, mPFC-BLA circuits are further modified byexcitatory synaptic strengthening as well as a transient surgein feedforward inhibition. The latter was correlated withincreased spontaneous inhibitory currents in excitatoryneurons, suggesting that mPFC-BLA circuit maturationculminates in a period of exuberant GABAergic transmission.These findings establish a time course for the onset andrefinement of mPFC-BLA transmission and point to potentialsensitive periods in the development of this critical network.

Amy Roy, Fordham UniversityO.1.2 Temper tantrums as indicators of emotiondysregulation in childrenIn recent years, there has been an increase in empiricalattention paid to the phenomenology of irritability in childrenand adolescents. Most studies have focused on populationsof children suffering from chronic irritability, such as thosewith disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (DMDD).However, in our work, we have found that the presence ofsevere temper outbursts (STO) in middle childhood, even inthe absence of chronic mood concerns, is associated withsignificant functional impairment and is not adequatelyaddressed with current interventions. Further, we haveidentified alterations in intrinsic functional brain networks thatappear to be specifically associated with STO, even whencontrolling for other psychopathology such as ADHD. Thus,

Page 20: 5th Annual Flux Congress

18 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Flux Congress Oral Presentations

we propose that when STO persist beyond the preschoolyears, they represent a clinically-relevant, transdiagnosticindicator of underlying deficits in emotion reactivity andregulation. Our work on the phenomenology andneurobiology of STO will be discussed along withimplications for future research and treatment development.

Koraly Perez-Edgar, Penn State UniversityO.1.3 Affect-biased attention as a core mechanism ofemotion reactivity and regulationAffect-biased attention, particularly if stable and entrenched,may act as a developmental tether that helps sustain earlysocioemotional and behavioral profiles over time, placingsome individuals on maladaptive developmental trajectories.Much of the evidence focuses on anxiety, although it is likelya domain-general mechanism. The current presentation willoutline the neural circuitry that may underlie the linksbetween temperament, behavioral markers of affect-biasedattention, and emerging risk for anxiety.

Oral Session 2 Development of AttentionChair: Sarah Durston, University of Utrecht

Sarah Durston, University of UtrechtO.2.1 Brain development in Attention DeficitHyperactivity DisorderStudies of brain development in Attention DeficitHyperactivity Disorder have shown relatively stable decreasesin cortical thickness that relate to outcome: individuals withremitting symptoms also show some normalisation of corticalthickness, suggesting that individual differences in may beindicative of resilience (Shaw et al., 2006). However, differentmeasures of the cortex may reflect differing aspects of earlycortical development (Rakic, 2000). In this presentation, I willdiscuss a study of the development of cortical thickness,surface area and gyrification in ADHD.

Gaia Scerif, University of OxfordO.2.2 Beyond the attentional homunculus: The developmental dynamics of attention, learningand memoryAttentional control plays a crucial role in biasing incominginformation in favour of what is relevant to further processing,action selection and long-term goals. Developmentalcognitive neuroscience illustrates how attentional processesare best understood not simply as a control homunculus, butrather as bidirectionally influencing and influenced by priorexperience. Our recent data highlight change and stability inthe interplay between attentional control, memory andlearning. Children and young adults differ in the extent towhich they deploy visuo-spatial attentional control to optimizemaintenance in short-term memory. At the same time,attentional effects on memory are not unidirectional:previously learnt information and resistance to distractionduring learning guide later attentional deployment, inadulthood and in childhood. In conclusion, assessing

attentional development and its dynamics point to thebidirectional influences between attention, learning andmemory.

Alison Gopnik, University of California at BerkleyO.2.3 When children are more open-minded learnersthan adults are: computation, evolution andphenomenologyOur recent studies show a surprising developmental patternacross several different kinds of problems and age ranges.Younger learners are better than older ones at learningunusual abstract causal principles from evidence. I explorethe possibility that this is because younger minds and brains,with less frontal control, are intrinsically more flexible andexploratory, although less efficient as a result. From acomputational perspective, this developmental shift mayprovide the benefits of "simulated annealing" in machinelearning. An initial broad "high-temperature" search through ahypothesis space, followed by a narrower and more focusedsearch, allows optimal learning in complex and variableenvironments. From an evolutionary perspective, ourdistinctively long human childhood and slow frontalmaturation may reflect this computational strategy. Such earlyhigh-temperature searches may have a distinctive, unfocusedand uncontrolled but vivid phenomenology, analogous tocertain kinds of adult experiences in which similarly flexibleand open-ended learning takes place.

Yuko Munakata, University of Colorado BoulderO.2.4 Developing inhibitory control: The role oftemporal dynamics in children’s attentionChildren show remarkable limitations and developments intheir ability to inhibit inappropriate thoughts, actions, andemotions. I will show how these changes in inhibitory controlare likely driven by developments in how children engageattention. Children’s inhibitory control can also be improvedusing novel approaches motivated by this attentionalframework.

Oral Session 3 Cellular & Molecular Mechanisms in DevelopmentChair: Nim Tottenham, Columbia University

Janice Juraska, University of IllinoisO.3.1 Cortical reorganization during adolescence:what the rat can tell us about the cellular basisThe cerebral cortex decreases in volume during adolescencein humans while the underlying white matter increases. Thesechanges also occur in the adolescent/peripubertal rat. In therat prefrontal cortex, synapses, dendrites and neurons arepruned peripubertally. These decreases are larger in femalesand more definitively tied to puberty. The increase in thewhite matter is due to myelination, not differences in thenumber or size of axons. The basolateral amygdala will alsobe presented where the relationship between the size andcellular composition of a structure is not as clear as in thecortex.

Page 21: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 19

Frances Champagne, Columbia UniversityO.3.2 Epigenetic variation in developmentaltrajectories: Role of prenatal and postnatalexperiencesDevelopment is shaped by environmental influencesoccurring at various life stages and there is increasingevidence for the role of epigenetic mechanisms in thisprocess. The experience of parents can likewise shape thedevelopment of offspring leading to environmental impactsthat persist across generations. In this talk, I will highlightresearch investigating the epigenetic impact of prenatalmaternal exposure to stress/toxins, variation in the quality ofpostnatal mother-infant interactions shaped by maternalexposure to adversity and the impact on development ofpaternal exposure to stress. These studies individuallyexplore the epigenetic influence of parental environmentalexposures and collectively illustrate the dynamic andinteractive routes through which the environment can lead tobehavioral and neurobiological effects across generations.

Siobhan Pattwell, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research CenterO.3.3 Leveraging dynamic changes in neural circuitryduring adolescence to persistently attenuate fearmemoriesFear can be highly adaptive in promoting survival, yet it canalso be detrimental when it persists long after a threat haspassed. Malleability of the fear response may be mostadvantageous during adolescence when there is an increasedprevalence to explore novel, potentially threateningenvironments. Using microprisms to image prefrontal-corticalspine maturation longitudinally and retrograde tracing ofneurons across development, we delineate dynamic circuitreorganization associated with shifts in adolescent fearbehaviors. Exploiting this sensitive-period of neuraldevelopment, we modified existing behavioral interventionsin an age-specific manner to attenuate adolescent fearmemories persistently into adulthood by highlightingcontextual contributions.

Bridget Nugent, University of PennsylvaniaO.3.4 Placental mechanisms underlying sexdifferences in neurodevelopmental vulnerabilityGestational stress is a risk factor for male-biasedneurodevelopmental disorders, including schizophrenia andautism. Our mouse model of early prenatal stress (EPS)imparts HPA stress axis and metabolic deficits to maleoffspring, endophenotypes similar to male-biased disorders.The placenta provides necessary factors for early braindevelopment, thus sex differences in placental function mayinfluence sex biases in neurodevelopmental vulnerability. Weidentified placental OGT (a nutrient sensing enzyme) as amediator of the effects of EPS on brain development. OGTmodifies the H3K27me2/3 methyltransferase, EZH2,enhancing its activity. Using trophoblast-specific OGTreduction, we found that OGT determines higher levels ofplacental H3K27me3 in females and genome-wide sexdifferences in placental H3K27me3 patterns. We hypothesizedthat this female-biased epigenetic repression is protectiveagainst prenatal insults. To test this hypothesis, we reducedH3K27me3 using trophoblast-specific manipulations of EZH2

in conjunction with EPS. Decreasing placentalEZH2/H3K27me3 created female vulnerability to EPS,sensitizing HPA axis reactivity and causing long-termincreases in body weight. To evaluate the role of X and Y -linked H3K27 demethylases in establishing sex differences inH3K27me3, we generated trophoblast-specific mouse lineswith reducible UTX and inducible UTY. We predict thatreducing placental UTX (hence enhancing H3K27me3) willprotect males from the developmental deficits produced byEPS. In addition, we predict that reducing UTX while inducingUTY expression in female trophoblasts will masculinizegenome-wide placental H3K27me3 patterns andneurodevelopmental responses to environmentalperturbations. These studies bring us closer elucidating theetiology of sex-biased neurodevelopmental disorders byinvestigating the complex interactions of genetic/epigeneticprograms with prenatal environment.

Oral Session 4 ABCD Symposium Chair: Monica Luciana, University of Minnesota

The Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development(ABCD) Study: early mental health, substance use,and neurocognitive outcomesThe NIH-funded Adolescent Brain and CognitiveDevelopment (ABCD) Consortium aims to be the largestlongitudinal multi-site study of brain and behavioraldevelopment to date. The goal is to enroll over 11,000 9-to-10 year-old singleton and twin participants who represent theUnited States population and follow them throughadolescence and into young adulthood. Participants and theirfamilies complete comprehensive behavioral assessments ofmental health, substance use, cognitive function, socialfunction, and personality as well as a two-hour brain imagingprotocol that includes structural, diffusion tensor, resting-state, and task-based scans. This symposium will featureinitial findings from the first year of assessment, including adescription of the study sample, mental health and substanceuse outcomes, early findings from the neurocognitiveassessment and imaging findings. A particular focus will beon individual variations in these outcomes due to high riskstatus and substance use. The presenters will engage indiscussion with the audience regarding the utility of thisepidemiological approach, challenges in multi-siteintegration, and implications for the prospective study ofsubstance use liability.

Hugh Garavan, University of VermontO.4.1 How to describe neurodevelopment at thepopulation level: Recruitment and samplingcharacteristics of the ABCD studyThe ABCD study, being a landmark study of adolescentneurodevelopment, has prioritized an epidemiologicallyrigorous approach to recruitment. This is motivated by anappreciation that larger, more diverse samples are required tocapture the sociodemographic variability that is needed togeneralize to the larger US population. The ABCD study is

Page 22: 5th Annual Flux Congress

20 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

recruiting 11,000+ children aged 9/10 from 20 sites acrossthe USA. I will describe the school-based, stratified randomsampling approach ABCD employs to recruit childrenmatched to national demographics for sex, race and ethnicity,socioeconomic status and urbanicity.

Susan Tapert, University of California San DiegoO.4.2 Assessing mental health and substance use in 9and 10-year-olds: The ABCD Assessment Protocoland early outcomesAdolescence is a time of dramatic physical, emotional, andintellectual growth, and also a critical neurodevelopmentalperiod associated with dramatic increases in rates ofsubstance use and psychiatric disorders. Alcohol andmarijuana use are common in adolescence; rates of bingedrinking remain high, and the potency of many marijuanaproducts now used is higher than in past decades. Identifyingpathways to substance use, mental illness, and their effectson development is critically important. The Adolescent BrainCognitive Development (ABCD) study is enrolling over 11,000children and following them from age 9 - 10 years of agethrough adolescence and into early adulthood. Participantsundergo a comprehensive baseline assessment, includingdevelopmentally appropriate assessment of substance useand mental health that show stable sensitivity and constructvalidity across childhood and adolescence, minimizeparticipant burden, and capture emergence of substance useand subtle changes in mental health. These data willelucidate: 1) effects of substance use on the adolescent brain;2) effects of substance use on behavioral and healthoutcomes; 3) bidirectional relationships betweenpsychopathology and substance use; 4) effects of genetic,behavioral, neurobiological, and environmental differences onrisk profiles and substance use outcomes; and 5) “gatewayinteractions” between substances.

Monica Luciana, University of MinnesotaO.4.3 Neurocognition in early adolescence and riskfor later substance use: findings from ABCD’s firstyear of studyAdolescence is characterized by strivings towardindependence, numerous social, physical changes, andincreased risk-taking. Dual systems models attributeadolescent risk-taking to tensions between developingcapacities for cognitive control and heightened rewardsensitivity. A comprehensive understanding of adolescents’neurocognitive development is necessary so thatconsequences of behaviors such as substance use can beclarified in relation to these dynamics. The prospectiveassessment of cognitive development is fundamental to theaims of the newly launched Adolescent Brain and CognitiveDevelopment (ABCD) Consortium. This presentation willprovide an overview of ABCD’s neurocognitive battery, whichinclude the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery, a one-item CashChoice task, a novel variant of the Rey Auditory VerbalLearning Test (RAVLT), the Matrix Reasoning task, and ameasure of visuospatial ability, the Little Man Task. Initialfindings from ABCD’s first year of data collection, includingassessments of nearly 5000 children, will be presented. Todate, we observe that children from high risk backgrounds

demonstrate lower levels of working memory as well asdiminished performance on learning and memory trials of theRAVLT. These outcomes will be considered in relation toindividual differences in specific mental health and substanceuse risk factors. Implications for neural development will bediscussed.

Deanna Barch, Washington UniversityO.4.4 Mapping neural development supportingcognitive and emotion process in the ABCDThis presentation will outline the constructs and approachesbeing collected for the functional neuroimaging component ofthe ABCD project, which includes both resting state and taskrelated functional activity, along with the details of theparadigms being used to assess the cognitive and affectiveconstructs of working memory, inhibitory control, rewardanticipation and receipt, emotional face processing, andepisodic memory. We will present initial results on bothvalidation of these approaches in this sample and relationshipto key variables of interest to neurodevelopment in the 9 and10-year-old children being assessed as part of this large-scaleproject.

Day 3 Monday September 18

Oral Session 5 Methods for Developmental ImagingChair: Monica Rosenberg, Yale University

Michael Hallquist, University of PennsylvaniaO.5.1 Developmental changes in the effect ofemotional cues on value-based decision-making and information maintenance in borderlinepersonality disorderBorderline personality disorder (BPD) often emerges inadolescence and is characterized by emotion dysregulationand interpersonal hypersensitivity. Although decision-makingin emotional contexts is impaired in BPD, little is known aboutits neurodevelopmental basis. In this study, 92 participants(47 with BPD symptoms, 45 matched controls) between theages of 13 and 30 (M = 20.61) completed 8 runs of areinforcement-based timing task during an fMRI scan. Runsconsisted of fifty trials in which a dot revolved 360° in 4seconds around a central stimulus (fearful, happy, orscrambled face). Participants pressed a button to obtain aprobabilistic reward from a time-varying contingency.Behavioral data were fit using a novel computational model ofexpected value, complexity of the value distribution (entropy),prediction error (PE), and decay of unchosen actions. HRF-convolved decision signals were entered in model-basedfMRI analyses using FLAME1+2 software (FSL 5.0.9).Behaviorally, whereas controls selected high-value actionsafter a negative PE in the fearful face condition, those withBPD symptoms did not (p < .001), suggesting disrupted

Flux Congress Oral Presentations

Page 23: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 21

learning from PEs by negative emotion in BPD. In model-based fMRI analyses, the BPD group had weakerrepresentation of value entropy in the frontal eye fields andintraparietal sulcus. Moreover, modulation of social cognitiveregions (e.g., dmPFC, MTG, TPJ, and temporal pole) to fearfulPEs diminished in with age in BPD, but was relatively stable incontrols. Implications of these findings for the developmentof BPD will be discussed.

Tamara Vanderwal, Yale UniversityO.5.2 Movies in the Magnet: The use of naturalisticstimuli in developmental neuroimagingThe use of naturalistic viewing conditions, including movies,in fMRI continues to expand rapidly. This talk will provide anoverview of the advantages of using movies as fMRI stimuliand cover current efforts underway in developmentalneuroimaging. One focus will be on the paradigm Inscapes, a7-minute publicly available movie we created to serve as analternative to resting state for young children (Vanderwal etal., 2015, NeuroImage). The talk will also highlight publiclyavailable data sets that use movies as acquisition statesacross the lifespan (e.g., O’Connor D., et al. 2017,GigaScience, Alexander L.M., et al., bioRxiv 2017) and the useof movies to study individual differences in functionalconnectivity (Vanderwal et al., 2017 NeuroImage).

Oral Session 6 Social and Motivational Processes

Chair: Leah Somerville, Harvard University

Niko Steinbeis, Leiden UniversityO.6.1 The role of control and motivation in thedevelopment of prosocial behaviorA central question in the study of prosocial behavior iswhether this occurs automatically and as a function ofpeople’s inherent kindness or instead results from effortfulcontrol and regulation. I argue that prosocial behavior carriesa cost and this cost needs to be regulated for prosocialbehavior to occur. In a series of developmental studies I showthat impacting behavioral control influences how childrenshare valuable resources. I also draw on recent lines ofresearch on motivational aspects of behavioral control andhow this can affect prosocial behavior. These insights can beleveraged to devise behavioral control interventions thatincrease sharing during childhood.

Mirella Dapretto, University of California Los AngelesO.6.2 Variation in the oxytocin receptor genemodulates reward circuit connectivity in youth withand without autismOxytocin is a key mediator of social behavior across speciesand common variants in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR)have been linked to alterations in brain structure and functionin neurotypical adults, as well as increased risk for autismspectrum disorders (ASD). In this talk, I will present data froma recent study where we examined how cumulative genetic

variation across several OXTR single-nucleotidepolymorphisms affect functional connectivity of the rewardnetwork in youth with and without ASD. By showingdifferential genetic effects on neuro-endophenotypes in thesepopulations, our findings highlight how integrating geneticrisk across multiple loci with neuroimaging data can furtherelucidate neural mechanisms of vulnerability vs. resilience incarriers of disease-associated risk alleles.

Flash Talks: Part 1

Co-chair: Nim Tottenham, Columbia UniversityCo-chair: Bea Luna, University of Pittsburgh

Rosa Li, Duke University

F.1.1 Striatal reward anticipation decreases fromadolescence to young adulthood - but only whenwatched by a peer

Many everyday decisions occur in the presence of peers andfeature outcomes that are shared with those peers. Bothpeer presence and reward for others have been found toindependently activate reward- and social-processing neuralregions, but it is still unknown how peer presence andreward for peers interact to influence neural activity. Wecollected fMRI data from 40 late adolescents and youngadults (18-28 y.o.) while they completed a monetaryincentive delay task to earn reward for self and for a friend.Half of the participants completed the task alone, and halfcompleted the task while watched by the friend. Across allparticipants, reward for self and friend were similarlyprocessed in striatal and insular reward-related regions,while peer presence increased activity in medial PFC.Additionally, peer presence significantly interacted with ageto predict activity in bilateral striatum: There was asignificant age-related decrease in striatal rewardanticipation for self and for friend in participants who werewatched by their friend, but no significant correlationbetween age and striatal reward anticipation in those whocompleted the task alone. We show that reward for self andfor friend are similarly neurally processed, and that the effectof peer presence on reward-related neural responses doesnot end at legal adulthood, but instead linearly diminishesfrom late adolescence to young adulthood. Thus,interventions to reduce maladaptive reward-seekingbehaviors in peer groups should not end at age 18 andshould instead extend into the early 20s.

Ashley Nielsen, Washington University in St. Louis

F.1.2 Patterns of functional connectivity predictmaturity and diagnostic status of individuals withTourette syndrome.

While a common developmental course for tic symptomshas been described for Tourette Syndrome (TS), manypatients do not follow this typical trajectory. Previously, wedemonstrated that multivariate support vector machine(SVM) learning can classify children with TS based oncorrelations in spontaneous fMRI activity between regions

Page 24: 5th Annual Flux Congress

22 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

across the brain (resting-state functional connectivity: RSFC).Here, we extended this work to test if patterns of RSFC canindicate maturity and diagnostic status of individuals withand without TS across development. Resting state fMRI datawas collected from a group of tic-free children (C-TF; N =39), tic-free adults (A-TF; N = 39), children with TS (C-TS; N= 39), and adults with TS (A-TS; N = 39). RSFC data among264 regions underwent strict preprocessing to minimizemotion-related artifact. While SVM is most commonly usedto predict binary class labels, it can be extended to predictmultiple classes (mc-SVM). We used mc-SVM to create amultivariate model separating C-TF, C-TS, A-TF, and A-TSindividuals with RSFC and tested this model with leave-one-out cross validation. The mc-SVM model was able to classifyindividuals according to maturity and diagnostic status with64% accuracy (chance=25%). Individuals were more likelyto be misclassified according to diagnostic group than agegroup. The way in which an individual is misclassified withmc-SVM provides a richer characterization of the individualthan binary SVM, which may be useful for predicting theclinical outcomes and developmental course of symptomsfor TS individuals.

LM Wierenga, University Leiden

F.1.3. Unraveling age, sex, puberty and testosteroneeffects on subcortical brain development acrossadolescence

The onset of adolescence in humans is marked by hormonalchanges that give rise to secondary sexual characteristics,noted as puberty. It has, however, proven challenging tounravel to what extent pubertal changes may haveorganising effects on the brain beyond chronological age, asreported in animal studies The present longitudinal studyaimed to characterise the unique effects of age and pubertyon subcortical brain volumes and included three waves ofdata collection at two-year intervals and 680 MRI scans of271 participants aged between 8 and 29 years old. Gammmodel procedures were used to assess the effects of age,self-report pubertal status and testosterone level on basalganglia, thalamus, hippocampus, amygdala and cerebellumgray matter volumes. We observed age-related increases inputamen and pallidum volumes, and decreases inaccumbens and thalamus volumes, all also showing maineffects of sex. Only the cerebellum showed an interactioneffect of age by sex. Furthermore, we showed that changesin puberty status and testosterone described developmentalchange in several structures better than chronological age.These effects differed per structure and between sexes.Changes in testosterone level were related to developmentof striatum, hippocampus and amygdala volumes in malesand caudate and hippocampal volumes in females. Theapproach of the present study allowed us to characterise thecomplex interactions between chronological age andpubertal maturational changes, and the findings indicatepuberty unique changes in brain structure that are sexspecific.

Barbara Braams, Harvard University

F.1.4. Developmental trajectories of social influenceon ambiguous decision-making

Adolescence is a life period associated with increased risk-taking, especially in the context of peers. Two factors thatare important for the propensity for risk-taking are attitudestowards ambiguity and risk. Risk refers to variability inoutcome, whereas ambiguity refers to unknown chances foroutcomes. Studies have investigated social influence onrisky decisions, but it remains unclear how social influenceshapes adolescent risk-taking in ambiguous situations.Participants (N=99, age range 12-22) completed aneconomic choice task. Choice options were systematicallyvaried on levels of risk and ambiguity. On each trial a safechoice (low outcome variability) and a risky choice (highoutcome variability) were presented. Participants madechoices in three conditions: a solo condition, a socialcondition in which they saw choices of peers and acomputer condition in which they saw choices of acomputer. Data were analyzed with non-linear mixed effectsmodels with factors risk, ambiguity and condition. For thesolo condition, results showed no developmental changesfor risk and ambiguity tolerance. For the social condition,results showed that participants’ choices conform to thepreferences indicated by the peers, but not the computer.Furthermore, when ambiguity was high and peers preferredthe risky choice, especially young adults (19-21 years) weremore likely to make a risky choice. These results show thattolerance towards ambiguity, but not risk, might depend onchoice preference of peers. Furthermore, we show that thiseffect is specific to early adulthood.

Anthony Dick, Florida International University

F.1.5. Development of the lateral lemniscus and itsrelation to receptive vocabulary

The lateral lemniscus (LL) is a bilateral fiber pathwaycomprised of axonal projections from the superior olivarycomplex to the inferior colliculus (Naidich et al., 2009). It isthus the major conduit for the transmission of auditoryperceptual information in the brainstem. Although thepathway is an important component of this early auditorysystem, its development has not been investigated usingmodern diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI) techniques. Ourstudy aims to be the first, to our knowledge, to track the LLin vivo and to explore potential behavioral associations in asample of typically developing invdividuals. In this study, weexamined the LL in 129 participants (70 females, age = 0-18years, M= 8.67 years) using DWI. Bilateral ROIs wheremanually drawn in the midbrain using the superior cerebralpeduncle as a landmark. Tracking was successful in 94participants. Fractional anisotropy (FA) increased linearly inthe LL from infancy to late adolescence, which is consistentwith extended development of the auditory system morebroadly (Litovsky, 2015; t(90) = 7.21, p < .001; controllingfor age, whole brain FA, and gender). We also assessed theLL’s relation to vocabulary development and found that axialdiffusivity (AD) of the LL is associated with improved PPVTscores (t(77) = 2.21, p < .05, controlling for age, gender, and

Flux Congress Oral Presentations

Page 25: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 23

wholebrain AD). This study provides preliminary evidence ofthe development and behavioral associations of the LL.Successful tracking of this pathway is potentially importantfor clinical treatment of auditory disorders in children.

Tara Madhyastha, University of Washington

F.1.6. Modeling fMRI Data in R using Neuropointillist

fMRI analysis has become an important tool for scientistsacross disciplines with different modeling needs. Inparticular, developmental cognitive neuroscienceresearchers are concerned with longitudinal growth andcorrelated change of brain and behavior. At the same time,constraints imposed by major fMRI analysis packages limitthe range of models that can be applied, and the tightconnection between image preprocessing and statisticalanalysis make it difficult for scientists from differentdisciplines to bring new modeling expertise to fMRI analysis.We describe a new R package, Neuropointillist(http://ibic.github.io/neuropointillist/), designed by our groupto address this issue by facilitating implementation of morecomplex longitudinal models. The presentation will include atutorial that translates fMRI analysis implemented in theFMRIB Software Library (FSL) into an R longitudinal mixedeffects framework. Because conducting fMRI analyses in Rcan be memory and CPU-intensive, Neuropointillist was alsodesigned to parallelize execution and take advantage ofcloud-based computing. We show how this software andapproach can be used to compare longitudinal growthmodels on simulated fMRI data. More generally, usingparameters estimated from first level analyses, it can beused to facilitate any voxel-wise analysis that can be definedin R, including structural equation frameworks for growth.The presentation will cover the wide array of new modelingcapabilities that will now be available for examiningdevelopmental change in fMRI data.

Scott Marek, University of Pittsburgh

F.1.7 Anterior cingulate theta band oscillationssupport development of cognitive flexibility throughadolescence into adulthood

Adolescence is a unique developmental period characterizedby improvements in cognitive control abilities, includingcognitive flexibility. Theta band (4-8 Hz) activity within theanterior cingulate cortex (ACC) increases several hundredmilliseconds after onset of a cue signaling the need toswitch rule sets in adults. However, the developmental ofACC theta band oscillations and their contribution to thedevelopment of cognitive flexibility have not been examined.MEG data was collected from 47 subjects aged 14-31 years.Subjects completed a modified version of the multi-sourceinterference task where subjects had to switch betweencongruent and incongruent trial types. After preprocessingand a novel approach for deconvolution of MEG data, wecontrasted switch vs. repeat trials and projected theseresults into source space for each subject. We next executeda time/frequency decomposition and tested for age effects.Lastly, we related brain activity during task switching to

behavioral performance, once again testing fordevelopmental effects. Across ages, there was significantlygreater theta band ACC activity in switch vs. repeat trialswithin 200ms of a cue signaling the need to switch tasks. Inaddition, increased theta band power was related toincreased switch cost. Developmentally, ACC theta bandpower decreased with age with increased theta band powerresulting in a greater switch cost for adolescents comparedto adults. These findings present electrophysiologicalevidence that mechanisms supporting cognitive controlinstantiation are immature during adolescence.

Hillary Raab, New York University

F.1.8 Pavlovian and instrumental contributions tomotivated behaviors across development

Across development individuals must acquire a repertoire ofbehaviors to function adaptively in diverse environments.Pavlovian “stimulus-outcome” learning elicits reflexivereactions, whereas instrumental “action-outcome” learningaffords flexible adaptive behaviors that yield beneficialoutcomes. Through Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (PIT),hard-wired Pavlovian responses either facilitate orundermine instrumental learning. When Pavlovian andinstrumental outcomes are aligned, Pavlovian responsesinvigorate reward-driven instrumental actions and inhibitaction in the face of punishment. When in opposition,Pavlovian responses interfere with adaptive, instrumentalbehaviors. PIT has yet to be well characterizeddevelopmentally. Here, we examined PIT in children,adolescents, and adults (aged 8-25), who performed aGo/No-go task in which valence and action wereorthogonalized. Across trial types, Pavlovian andinstrumental responses were either aligned (Go to win, No-go to avoid losing) or in opposition (Go to avoid losing, No-go to win). We found a valence-by-action-by-trial effect thatwas differentially impacted by age. Overall, learning wasenhanced when Pavlovian and instrumental responses werealigned versus in opposition. Moreover, a Go bias furtherfacilitated ‘Go to win’ learning. The degree of PIT wasgreatest in children, with instrumental action growingincreasingly resistant to Pavlovian interference with age. Thisshift in the balance between learning systems may fostermore flexible adaptive behaviors during the transition fromadolescence into adulthood.

Christy Rogers, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

F.1.9 “No, don’t do it!” Neural correlates of siblingcloseness during risky decision-making

Accumulating evidence suggests that sibling relationshipsare a prominent influence on the development of risk-takingbehavior across adolescence. Siblings predict adolescents’engagement in risk taking above and beyond the effects ofparent and peers. Yet, no prior study has tested how siblingsinfluence the neurobiology of risk taking. We investigatedthe neural correlates of sibling relationships on adolescentrisk-taking behavior. The sample included 73 adolescents(Mage = 13.37 years; 38 females) who played a risk-taking

Page 26: 5th Annual Flux Congress

24 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

task during an fMRI scan. Participants reported on closeness(e.g., trust, communication, support) with their siblings,parents, and peers, and indicated the frequency with whichthey engaged in risky behaviors such as substance use.Higher sibling closeness was associated with suppressedactivation in the dorsolateral PFC and insula during riskydecision-making. Moreover, higher sibling closenessindirectly predicted less adolescent real-life risk-taking viasuppression of the insula during risky decision-making. Birthorder effects were also found, indicating that siblingcloseness differentially predicts activation in the caudatedepending on whether adolescents are the oldest child inthe family or not. Importantly, these findings persisted aboveand beyond parental and peer closeness, highlighting thesignificant influence of sibling relationships on adolescentrisk taking through the brain.

David Montez, University of Pittsburgh

F.1.10 Developmental stabilization of neural gainsignals improves mean behavioral performance andbehavioral variability

Cognitive development during adolescence is characterizedby improvements in mean performance and by decreases inbehavioral variability, which is an important barometer ofcognitive functioning. Mechanistically accounting for thestabilization of behavior is critical to our understanding ofadolescent neural development. Here, we report results froma longitudinal working memory study performed over 10years in a cohort of 126 subjects between the ages of 8 and33 years. We develop a computational model of memory-guided saccade (MGS) performance and provide evidencethat improvement in mean behavioral performance andbehavioral variability can be accounted for by the stabilizingneural variability. We find that behavioral performance in thememory-guided saccade task improves and stabilizes duringadolescence. By incorporating multiple sources ofindependent neural gain variability in a high-dimensionaldrift diffusion race model we accounted for theimprovements in mean performance and variability that areobserved during adolescent development. Analysis of thetrial-to-trial relationship between memory-guided saccadereaction times and accuracies reveals a U-shaped speed-accuracy relationship, which was accounted for by a balanceof independent variability affecting working memory andresponse threshold gain signals. Our results indicate thatindependent trial-to-trial variability in gain signals that affectworking memory maintenance and response thresholds canaccount for the speed-accuracy relationships observed inour data.

Kaja LeWinn, University of California, San Francisco

F.1.11 The representative developing brain: Doessampling strategy matter for neuroscience?

Despite calls to incorporate population science intoneuroimaging research, most studies recruit small, non-representative samples. We examined whether samplecomposition influences conclusions about age-relatedvariation in global measurements of grey matter volume,

thickness and surface area in a large, community-basedsample of children aged 3-18 (N=1,162) from the PediatricImaging, Neurocognition and Genetics Study (PING).Structural MRI data were analyzed using Freesurfer togenerate cortical area and thickness measures for each lobeof the brain (frontal, parietal, occipital, temporal), as well asfor total cortical thickness, area, and volume. To approximateassociations of age with brain structure in a representativesample of U.S. children, we applied a commonly-usedepidemiologic method called raking to weight the sampleaccording to the distributions of socioeconomic status,race/ethnicity, and sex in the U.S. Census. We comparedassociations between age and brain structure in thisweighted sample to estimates derived from the unweightedoriginal sample. Compared to unweighted models, weobserved a more complex functional form (cubic versusquadratic) for cortical surface area and subcortical volume,earlier maturation of sub-cortical structures, and regionalpatterns of cortical maturation that better reflected knowndevelopmental trajectories in weighted models. Ourempirical examination of non-representative sampling inneuroimaging studies suggests that sample composition islikely to have a meaningful impact on cognitiveneuroscience findings.

Oral Session 7 Hippocampal DevelopmentChair: Noa Ofen

Noa Ofen, Wayne State University

O.7.1 Progress and limitations in assessinghippocampal functional maturation

There is growing interest in measuring the structuralproperties of the human hippocampus and assessing itsfunctional maturation, yet the little data available, combinedwith differences in methodological applications limit whatwe currently know. In this talk I will summarize recentadvances in our understanding of hippocampaldevelopment, while highlighting several of the limitations inour current knowledge, as well as some of the productiveefforts to achieve progress in our understanding of how thedevelopment of the human hippocampus and the functionalspecialization of hippocampal sub-components supportsmemory functioning across the lifespan. Bettercharacterization of the typical trajectories of hippocampaldevelopment is highly desirable not only for understandinghow this structure supports memory but for generatingclinically important insights in populations wherehippocampal development is altered.

Simona Ghetti, University of California Davis

O.7.2 Hippocampal contributions to the developmentof episodic memory

Behavioral research has consistently shown that episodicmemory, or the ability to remember events in their spatio-temporal context, improves during childhood and

Flux Congress Oral Presentations

Page 27: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 25

adolescence. The hippocampus plays a critical role informing and reinstating representations that integrateinformation about events and their spatio-temporal context.However, little is known about how developmental changesin the hippocampus support these behavioral improvements.Furthermore, despite a purported role of the hippocampus inthe emergence of episodic memory in the first few years oflife, little direct evidence links hippocampal function to earlymanifestations of episodic memory in humans. I will presentnew findings that begin to address both of these gaps inknowledge. First, results from a large longitudinal studyshow that volumetric structural and functional changes inthe hippocampus predict developmental changes in episodicmemory from 7- to 14 years of age; I will discuss the role ofpuberty. Second, results from a functional neuroimagingstudy show that hippocampal activation associated with apast experience in 2-year-olds is associated with overtmemory for that experience. Overall, these findings suggestthat it is possible to trace early memory capacity tohippocampal function, but this function continues to changefrom early childhood into adolescence.

Patricia Bauer, Emory University

O.7.3 Self-derivation of new knowledge throughmemory integration: The importance of binding anddetection of deviatio

Building a semantic knowledge base requires integration ofmemory traces established at different times and in differentcontexts. Rapid accumulation of knowledge further dependson productive processes that allow self-derivation of newknowledge based on integrated memory traces. Throughwork with children and college students, we have identifiedcomponent processes involved in self-derivation throughintegration. fMRI indicates that the processes arehippocampally dependent. Behavioral assays, eye tracking,and ERPs suggest that limitations on memory integration orbinding present challenges to successful self-derivation inchildhood, whereas failures to detect deviation betweennewly and previously learned information limits performanceamong college students.

Paul Frankland, Hospital for Sick Children

O.7.4 Hippocampal neurogenesis, forgetting andinfantile amnesia

Neurogenesis persists throughout life in the hippocampus,and there is a lot of interest in how the continuous additionof new neurons impacts hippocampal memory functionacross development. Our studies in rodents have shown thathigh rates of neurogenesis during the post-natal periodcontribute to accelerated forgetting (i.e., infantile amnesia)(e.g., Akers et al [2014] Science). Our more recent studiesaddress whether amnesia is associated with storage vs.retrieval failure. Using optogenetic approaches we find thatotherwise ‘lost’ infant memories may be recovered via directstimulation of ensembles of neurons that were active duringinitial encoding.

Flash Talks: Part 2

Co-chair: Nim Tottenham, Columbia UniversityCo-chair: Bea Luna, University of Pittsburgh

Louise Baruël Johansen, Centre for Functional andDiagnostic Imaging and Research, Copenhagen UniversityHospital Hvidovre

F.2.1 Reduced orbitofrontal functional networkcentrality characterizes high neuroticism acrosschildhood and adolescence

Neuroticism is a risk factor for anxiety and mood disorders.Recently, we showed that children and adolescents scoringhigher on neuroticism had a less efficient informationexchange in resting-state functional brain networks and aless influential orbitofrontal (OFC) network. In the presentlongitudinal study, we investigate if associations betweenfunctional network topology and neuroticism change overthe course of adolescence. Resting-state functional magneticresonance imaging was acquired for up to six times from 74typically-developing children and adolescents aged 10-18years. Using graph theory we quantified global efficiencyand modularity, and local network features includingbetweenness centrality and participation coefficient of fivenetworks of interest. Linear mixed-effects models confirmedthat the OFC played a less central role (i.e. lowerbetweenness centrality) in adolescents with highneuroticism. The lack of a significant neuroticism by ageinteraction suggests that this association may be stableacross childhood and adolescence. Although we observed afair reliability (intraclass correlation (ICC)=0.4-0.5) for theOFC betweenness centrality measure, the reliability withwhich graph theoretical measures could be measuredlongitudinally was generally low (ICC<0.4). Fluctuations incognitive and mental states during and between thescanning sessions may well underlie the observed within-subject variability. Our results stress the importance ofreplicating findings of cross-sectional studies, as these areinherently “blind” to intra-individual variability.

Katherine Reding, National Institutes of Health

F.2.2 Influence of sex and pubertal development onfunctional connectivity

Sex differences in the prevalence of psychopathologyemerge during adolescence and may reflect the pubertalsurge in gonadal steroid production. Despite this, little isknown about the direct effects of puberty on brain function.To examine neurodevelopment across puberty, we studiedfunctional connectivity (FC) in children categorized byclinician-rated pubertal stage (PS). Participants (N=72) weregrouped as prepubertal (PS1, N=41, 8.7±0.3yrs, 18 girls) orpubertal (PS2-5, N=31, 13±0.7yrs, 14 girls). Resting statefMRI scans were collected at 3T, processed with AFNI andANTs, and analyzed using a connectome-wide associationstudy (CWAS) to identify 1) prepubertal sex differences and2) sex-by-pubertal group main and interaction effects inwhole-brain FC. Identified clusters were used as seedregions in voxel-wise analyses to clarify underlying regional

Page 28: 5th Annual Flux Congress

26 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

patterns of FC. Prepubertal sex differences were identified inthe medial prefrontal cortex, a key region of the defaultmode network (DMN, p<.001). Seed-based analysis (p<.05FDR-corrected) showed that girls had more robust FC thanboys within the DMN and the exective control network(ECN). In the sex-by-pubertal group model (p’s<.05 FWE-corrected) main effects of sex were reproduced inDMN/ECN, while main and interaction effects of pubertalgroup converged on basal ganglia FC. Seed-based analysis(p<.001) showed that caudate FC with the salience networkincreased across puberty in girls, but decreased in boys.These data suggest that development of networkconnectivity may be influenced by both sex and pubertalstatus.

Sylvia Rusnak, Georgetown University

F.2.3 The developing frontoparietal network: Spatialimitation performance predicts activation in youngchildren

The neural signature of visuo-spatial working memory(VSWM), the short-term ability to retain and manipulateinformation, is well characterized in older children and adultsby frontoparietal activation that strengthens withdevelopment and memory load (Moriguchi & Hiraki, 2013).Due to the technical difficulties associated with studyingyoung children using fMRI, knowledge of the neural basis ofVSWM in preschoolers is limited. Functional near infraredspectroscopy (fNIRS) may be a suitable alternative. Due to alack of standardization of VSWM measurement in fNIRSstudies, prior findings have been mixed (Moriguchi & Hiraki,2013). In the present study, we collected fNIRS data from 5-to 8-year-olds during two multi-step spatial imitation tasks:one well-established (Subiaul et al., 2015) and one novel. Anexperimenter demonstrated a sequence of actions and thenthe child was tested. We hypothesized frontoparietalactivation during the test phase due to the demands ofholding multiple steps in mind. Memory load wasmanipulated by sequence length (2 to 5 steps), with allchildren performing at ceiling across loads. All childrenexhibited frontoparietal activation during the test phase ofeach task. However, for high relative to low load, youngerchildren showed only parietal activation, while older childrenexhibited the canonical frontoparietal activation. Datacollection with 3- to 8-year-olds is ongoing. Understandingthe neural mechanisms underlying the development ofimitation and load-dependent VSWM has importantimplications for the science of learning.

Stephen Boyd, Oregon Health & Science University

F.2.4 Sex differences in the effect of nucleusaccumbens volume on adolescent drinking: Themediating role of sensation seeking and positivealcohol expectancies

The nucleus accumbens (NAcc) is implicated in rewardsensitivity and development of alcohol use disorder (AUD).Larger NAcc volume has been found among adolescents athigh-risk for AUD; though, it is unclear whether NAccvolume predicts drinking. This study examined direct and

indirect effects of NAcc volume on adolescent drinking aftertwo years. Mediation through sensation seeking (SS) andpositive alcohol expectancies (PAE), both known risk factorsof AUD, was explored, as well as sex differences in a sampleof 808 adolescents (mean age = 16.2±6.4; 51% female).Baseline bilateral NAcc volume was determined bysegmenting subcortical brain structures on T1-weighted,magnetic resonance images. SS and PAE were assessed oneyear post-baseline, and alcohol use measured after twoyears. Controlling for intracranial volume and age, NAccvolume predicted later drinking in males (β=.09, p=.04) andfemales (β=.11, p=.02). After accounting for SS and PAE,the effect of NAcc volume remained significant for females(β=.09, p = .04), but not males (β=.05, p=.29). In males, theindirect effect through SS and PAE accounted for more thanhalf of the effect of NAcc volume (β=.05, p=.01). Thesefindings suggest that delayed structural maturation of theNAcc may be a risk factor for alcohol use. In males, a largerNAcc was associated with greater SS and PAE, which in turnpredicted more alcohol use. Mediation was not evident infemales. Although NAcc volume influenced later drinking forboth sexes, these results suggest that the mechanism bywhich this region infers risk differs by sex.

Arianna Gard, University of Michigan

F.2.5 Neighborhood effects on the brain:Impoverishment in early childhood predictsamygdala reactivity to ambiguous faces in youngadulthood

Although children from disadvantaged neighborhoods are atrisk for poor cognitive and social outcomes, the biologicalmechanisms linking neighborhood impoverishment to youthoutcomes is unclear. We previously found (Gard et al., underrevie) that a Census-derived measure of neighborhoodimpoverishment at age 2 predicted greater amygdalareactivity (AR) to ambiguous neutral faces at age 20. Thecurrent study extends and deepens this work by examining(1) neighborhood danger as a potential mediator of thispathway; and (2) unique effects of each Census-basedindicator of neighborhood impoverishment in earlychildhood on amygdala reactivity to ambiguity at age 20.The Pitt Mother & Child Project (Shaw et al., 2003) is alongitudinal study of low income boys followed from 18months to 23 years. Neighborhood impoverishment duringearly childhood (2, 5 years) was measured using sevenblock-level Census tract variables used across many studiesof neighborhood effects: median family income, % familiesbelow poverty line, % households on public assistance, %unemployed, % single-mother households, % AfricanAmerican, and % with > Bachelor’s degree. Neighborhooddanger was parent-reported. AR to neutral faces (versusshapes) was measured using fMRI at age 20. Resultsindicated that neighborhood danger did not mediateneighborhood impoverishment associations with greater ARto neutral faces in adulthood. Within the neighborhoodimpoverishment variable, median family income was moststrongly related to AR to neutral faces, over and above otherneighborhood attributes and family income.

Flux Congress Oral Presentations

Page 29: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 27

Moriah Thomason, Wayne State University / PerinatologyResearch Branch, NICHD/NIH

F.2.6 Hatching a Pokémon egg by closing your eyes:A new paradigm for measuring resting-state inpreschoolers

Resting-state paradigms are becoming increasingly popularin neuroscience. In adults, resting-state is often measured ineyes-closed condition. This procedure is not easilyapplicable in preschool populations, since they often refuseto close their eyes for extended periods. A standardized taskto increase the feasibility of measuring eyes-closed resting-state in this population is needed. Here, we present a noveleyes-closed paradigm for measuring resting-state inpreschoolers. Continuous EEG data were collected in 50preschoolers (ages 4.5-5.1) while they participated in a novelPokémon resting-state experiment. Children were signaled toplace their chin on a custom-built ?incubator? and close theireyes in order to hatch a Pokémon egg. An egg appeared onthe screen to cue the start of a trial. After 25sec, they wouldbe signaled by a cracking sound to open their eyes. The eggwould then virtually hatch and they would be given amatching sticker for the hatched character. This procedurewas repeated up to 8 times. The average number of trailscompleted across all subjects was 7.7 trials. EEG dataprocessed for 12 participants revealed rapid emergent alphafrequency at the onset of rest epochs in all cases. This taskmeets criteria of being both well tolerated by preschoolers,and producing desired neurophysiological results,suggesting it is a sound approach for investigatingspontaneous neural activity across the brain. A complexitywarranting discussion is the low-level processingcomponent of this procedure.

Kristen Tummeltshammer, Brown University

F.2.7 Visual learning is modulated by reward value in infancy

Using eye-tracking, we investigated whether 7-month-oldinfants would demonstrate a key feature of predictionlearning: the transfer of reward value from rewarding stimulionto reward predictive cues. Fifty infants (M=7 months, 8.3days) were presented with high and low value faces andcartoons (i.e., infant’s own mother and an unfamiliar female;colorful dynamic and gray-scale static) as well as four cuestimuli (i.e., unique shapes). Infants viewed the cues andrewards in isolation, and then in 24 randomized cueing trials,in which a cue was closely followed by a paired reward inthe same quadrant of the screen. Results show that infants’distribution of looking times to the reward stimuli transferredto the cues during the cueing task: infants looked longer atcues paired with high value than with low value cartoons,while looking times for cues paired with faces did not differ.Infants’ pattern of pupil dilations also transferred to the cues,as pupil dilations were larger at post-test for cues pairedwith high value than with low value rewards, and particularlyfor the cue predicting the infant’s own mother compared toan unfamiliar female face. In addition, infants’ increasedpupil size for the cue paired with their own mother’s facewas associated with the amount of time mothers reported

spending with their infants. Lastly, infants showed decreasedsaccadic latencies, reflecting greater spatio-temporallearning, to cues that preceded high value rewards. Theseresults demonstrate that visual prediction learning is indeedmodulated by stimulus reward value in infancy.

David Piekarski, UC Berkeley

F.2.8 Ovarian hormones organize the maturation ofinhibitory neurotransmission in the frontal cortex atpuberty onset in female mice.

The frontal cortex matures late in development, showingdramatic changes after puberty onset, yet few experimentshave directly tested the role of pubertal hormones in corticalmaturation. One mechanism thought to play a primary rolein regulating the maturation of the neocortex is an increasein inhibitory neurotransmission, which alters the balance ofexcitation and inhibition. We hypothesized that pubertalhormones could regulate maturation of frontal cortex by thismechanism. Here, we report that manipulations of gonadalhormones do significantly alter the maturation of inhibitoryneurotransmission in the cingulate region of the mousemedial frontal cortex, an associative region that maturesduring the pubertal transition and is implicated in decisionmaking, learning, and psychopathology. We find thatinhibitory neurotransmission increases onto cingulatepyramidal neurons during puberty and that this increase canbe blocked by prepubertal but not post-pubertalgonadectomy. Further, prepubertal hormone treatment caninduce this effect in frontal cortex, but not somatosensorycortex, suggesting that earlier puberty can advance corticalmaturation in a regionally specific manner. Prepubertalhormone treatment also affects a frontal cortex-dependentreversal learning task. These data provide rare evidence ofenduring, organizational effects of ovarian hormones atpuberty and provide a potential mechanism by whichgonadal hormones could regulate the maturation ofassociative neocortex.

Brenden Tervo-Clemmens, University of Pittsburgh

F.2.9 Neural corrlates of latent internalizing andexternalizing psychopathology during adolescence

Across the lifespan, latent variable modeling revealsdimensional, internalizing and externalizing factors thataccount for patterns of comorbidity amongst commonmental health disorders. However, little is known about theassociation between these transdiagnostic factors and brainfunction during adolescence.To identify functional braincorrelates of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology,we utilized resting-state functional magnetic resonanceimaging (rsfMRI) data and psychopathology symptomendorsement from 598 subjects from the PhiladelphiaNeurodevelopmental Cohort. Latent internalizing andexternalizing factors were estimated using confirmatoryfactor analysis (CFA) informed by Kruegger et al., (1998).rsfMRI data (6 min) were preprocessed using standardmeasures and adjacency matrices were computed using theparcellation from Power et al. (2011). The CFA model ofpsychopathology symptom endorsement demonstrated

Page 30: 5th Annual Flux Congress

28 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

good fit (robust CFI: = .964, RMSEA = .06). PreliminaryrsfMRI results revealed subjects with high latent internalizingscores had increased connectivity within canonical defaultmode network (DMN) nodes (t’s > 2.75, p < .05, corrected).In contrast, subjects with higher latent externalizing scoreshad increased connectivity between frontoparietal (DLPFC)and DMN nodes (MTL)(t = 3.99 , p< .05, corrected). Ourresults suggest distinct connectivity profiles of internalizingand externalizing psychopathology. However, we highlight apotential common role of the DMN in transdiagnosticpsychopathology risk during adolescence.

Sung Jun Joo, University of Washington

F.2.10 Automaticity in the reading circuitry: Ahallmark of skilled reading

Skilled reading requires years of practice with learning toassociate visual symbols (letters) with speech sounds(phonemes), and over the course of the learning process,this association becomes almost effortless. Indeed, skilledreaders show activation to visually presented words in bothventral temporal cortex (VTC), which is involved in

orthographic processing, and the superior temporal sulcus(STS), which is involved in phonological processing. Herewe hypothesize that automatic activation of this circuit inresponse to a visually presented word is a hallmark ofsuccessfully learning to read. To test this hypothesis, weused magnetoencephalography (MEG) to measure corticalresponses to printed words while children engaged in anattention-demanding task (color discrimination on a fixationdot) for which the words were irrelevant. We found that thestimulus-evoked visual response to words in VTC peaked at180 ms and all children, regardless of reading skill, showedsimilar VTC responses. Importantly, even though childrenwere not actively reading the words, we found significantactivation in the STS which peaked at 240 ms. Thisautomatic response to the visual stimulus in a canonicallanguage region was indicative of good reading skills: thestimulus-driven STS response was only present in goodreaders but not in children with dyslexia. Our results suggestthat automatic recruitment of phonological processingcircuits is a hallmark of skilled reading; with practice, readingbecomes effortless as the brain learns to automaticallytranslate letters into sound and meaning.

Flux Congress Oral Presentations

Portland cycles!

Page 31: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 29

Author Poster No. Author Poster No. Author Poster No.

Flux Congress Poster Author Index

Poster Session 1Saturday September 16 4:00-6:00 PM

Poster Session 2Sunday September 17 4:00-6:00 PM

Poster Session 3Monday September 18 12:30-2:00PM

Poster board numbers are indicated as follows: Poster Session – Theme – Board Number (Example: 2-A-10)

Location of the individual poster boards are indicated on posterboard floor plans following the poster author index list. Poster setup and removal is the responsibility of the presenter. Please haveyour poster set up no later than 8:30 AM on your scheduledpresentation day and removed by 7:00 PM each day. For those

presenting on Monday, please remove your poster by 3:00 PM. Anyposters not removed by the designated time will be held atRegistration until 4:00 PM on Monday.

ThemesA Cognition

B Decision Making

C Functional/Structural Neuroanatomy

D Motivation/Affect

E Neurotransmitter Function

F Learning

G Clinical Populations

For a complete list of poster abstracts please visit www.fluxsociety.org

Abrams, D 2-G-89

Achterberg, M 3-C-123

Adamson, C 3-D-130

Adedipe, O 2-C-64

Ahn, A 3-A-103

Alarcon, G 2-G-88

Albaugh, M 3-C-117

Alberti, S 2-G-97

Alcauter, S 2-C-67

Alkire, D 1-A-1

Allen, N 1-C-15, 1-C-19, 1-G-45, 3-C-121

Almy, B 1-B-10

Altamirano, O 3-D-127

Amso, D 1-F-39, 3-O-166

Anandakumar, J 2-B-59

Anderson, P 3-D-130

Anderson, V 2-C-74

Armstrong-Kelly, C 3-D-130

Askren, K 3-G-141

Austin, N 2-G-98

Aziz-Zadeh, L 1-G-44

Bachevalier, J 3-D-129

Bagley, J 3-C-120

Baker, A 2-G-89

Ball, G 2-C-74

Barbosa, C 3-C-111

Barch, D 1-C-23, 1-A-7, 1-C-18, 3-A-102, 3-G-140

Barone, J 1-C-13, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Barr, R 3-O-163

Barrios, F 2-C-67

Baruël, L 3-O-165

Bassett, D 3-C-112

Bateup, H 1-F-38

Baum, G 3-C-112

Beare, R 3-C-121, 3-D-130

Becker, B 2-F-87

Berman, K 1-A-8, 1-C-13, 1-G-46, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Betzel, R 3-C-112

Blair, C 3-A-99

Blankenship, S 2-A-55

Blasi, A 2-C-65

Boivin, J 2-O-159

Bookheimer, S 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 2-G-92, 3-A-102

Boomen, C 2-C-65

Bora, S 2-G-98

Boroshok, A 1-C-13, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Borst, G 1-D-27, 2-F-86, 3-D-125

Bos, M 3-O-167

Bostan, S 3-D-127

Botdorf, M 2-A-55, 2-A-57, 3-C-113

Bouhours, L 1-D-27, 3-D-125

Boyd, S 1-O-148

Boyle, E 2-O-161

Braams, B 1-O-149

Braconnier, M 3-A-103

Brady, S 1-A-8

Buard, I 1-G-41

Buckner, R 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 3-A-102

Burgess, G 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 3-A-102

Burns-Yocum, T 1-G-40

Butera, C 1-G-44

Byrne, M 1-C-15

Cachia, A 2-F-86

Cai, W 3-D-127

Calabro, F 1-A-3, 3-D-124

Caldera, C 3-C-114

Camarda, A 3-D-125

Camp, N 2-D-77

Canada, K 1-C-14

Cardon, G 1-G-41

Carlozzi, B 3-C-117

Carrion, V 3-D-127

Casey, B 2-D-77

Cassotti, M 1-D-27, 3-D-125

Castro, N 2-C-70

Cermak, S 1-G-44

Cervera, A 2-D-77

Chahal, R 2-G-90

Charron, S 2-F-86

Chein, J 2-A-57, 3-B-109

Chen, H 3-G-137

Chen, X 3-B-107

Cheng, T 1-C-15

Cheng, T 2-G-97

Cheries, E 3-B-107

Church, J 2-A-56, 3-A-100, 3-F-135, 3-G-146, 2-O-158

Ciric, R 3-C-112

Clarke, B 3-F-133

Clarkson, T 3-G-137

Cohen, A 2-D-77

Cohodes, E 2-F-85, 2-G-93, 3-G-138

Collins, P 1-B-10

Consortium, I 3-B-108

Constable, RT 1-C-17, 3-A-101

Cook, P 3-C-112

Cosme, D 3-D-126

Courtney, K 2-C-70

Cox, L 2-C-64

Crone, E 1-C-21, 1-C-24, 3-C-123, 3-A-106, 3-O-167

Cullen, K 1-D-28

Culver, J 1-G-40, 2-G-91

Cummings, K 2-G-92

Currin, D 1-G-46

Custers, R 1-A-4

Page 32: 5th Annual Flux Congress

30 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Flux Poster Author Index | By Author Name

Czarapata, J 2-O-161

Dahl, R 2-C-69

Dapretto, M 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 2-G-92, 3-A-102, 3-C-118, 3-D-126

Daubert, E 2-F-84

Daumeyer, N 2-D-77

Davidow, J 2-A-54, 3-C-112, 3-F-134, 1-O-149

de Vos, F 1-C-24

DeJoseph, M 2-A-51, 3-D-128

Del, P 1-C-20

Delevich, K 1-B-11

Dellarco, D 2-D-77

Demers, L 1-D-28

Demeter, D 2-B-59

Dennison, M 1-D-30, 2-C-69

DePasque, S 1-F-36

Di, L 2-C-65

Dick, A 1-O-150

DiStefano, C 1-G-42

Do, Q 1-G-48

Doherty, B 2-A-52

Dombrovski, A 1-G-43

Dosenbach, N 2-C-68, 3-C-119, 2-O-158

Dougherty, L 2-A-55

Doyle, L 3-D-130

Duberg, K 3-D-127

Dudgeon, P 3-C-111

Dumontheil, I 1-A-4

Earl, E 2-B-59, 2-C-68, 2-C-73, 3-C-119, 3-C-120

Eberhardt, J 2-D-77

Edwards, A 2-G-95

Efron, D 2-C-74

Eggebrecht, A 1-G-40, 2-G-91, 2-G-98

Eilbott, J 3-A-103

Eisenberg, D 1-G-46

Ekstrand, A 2-C-66

Elliot, M 3-C-112

Engelhardt, L 2-A-56, 3-A-100

Epping, E 1-G-49

Espie-Pfeiffer, P 1-G-49

Fair, D 2-B-59, 2-C-68, 2-C-72, 2-D-77, 2-C-73, 3-C-119, 3-C-120

Fareri, D 3-C-114

Feczko, E 2-C-73, 3-C-120

Feldman, E 3-O-166

Fettich, K 2-A-57

Fichtenholtz, H 3-A-103

Fisher, P 2-C-63, 2-G-97

Flannery, J 1-C-15, 2-C-63, 2-G-97, 3-C-114

Fletcher, J 3-F-135

Flournoy, J 1-C-15, 2-C-63, 2-G-97, 3-D-126

Foran, W 1-A-3, 1-O-156, 3-O-164

Forbes, E 2-D-83, 2-G-88, 2-G-90, 1-O-151

Fowler, C 1-B-12

Fox, N 3-G-143

Frans Christiaan Baaré, W 2-C-66, 3-C-116, 3-O-165

Gaab, N 2-F-87

Gabard-Durnam, L 3-C-114

Galarce, E 1-F-38

Galvan, A 1-F-36, 3-C-122, 3-D-132

Gard, A 1-O-151

Garic, D 1-O-150

Gao, S 3-A-101

Garavan, H 3-B-108

Gee, D 2-F-85, 2-G-93, 3-C-114, 3-G-138

Gelardi, K 2-D-83

Genc, S 2-C-74

Geng, F 1-C-14

Georgiou-Karistianis, N 3-D-130

Ghysels, J 1-D-26

Gimbel, S 1-C-20

Glenn, C 2-D-76

Goddings, A 2-C-69

Goff, B 3-C-114

Goldenberg, D 3-C-122

Goldman, D 1-D-29

Goncharova, A 2-C-72

Grabowski, T 1-O-155

Gracia, T 2-C-67

Green, S 2-G-92

Greene, A 3-A-101

Greene, D 2-C-68, 2-O-158

Gregory, M 1-G-46, 2-O-161

Grogans, S 1-G-46

Groot, W 1-D-26

Grossman, H 3-G-143

Guassi, M 1-D-34, 2-D-75

Gunnar, M 3-C-115

Gunther, K 1-F-39

Gur, R 3-C-112

Gur, R 3-C-112

Guyer, A 2-D-83, 2-G-90

Haberman, J 2-F-85, 2-G-93

Hajcak, G 3-G-137

Hall, N 2-G-96, 3-G-139

Hallquist, M 2-G-96, 3-G-139

Hamilton, A 1-O-153

Hampton, J 2-C-68

Harden, K 2-A-56, 3-A-100

Harmon, C 3-C-114

Harms, M 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 3-A-102

Harrison, L 1-G-44

Hartley, C 3-G-138, 2-O-158

Hawkey, E 3-G-140

Hegarty, C 1-A-7

Heleniak, C 2-G-94

Helion, C 1-D-29

Helzer, A 1-C-15

Hernandez, L 3-C-118

Hernke, C 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 3-A-102

Herting, M 2-C-69

Herzberg, M 3-C-115

Hipwell, A 2-G-90

Hipwell, A 2-D-83

Holbrook, H 3-C-117

Holm-Skjold, J 2-C-66, 3-C-116

Horien, C 1-C-17

Houda, O 2-F-86

Houde, O 1-D-27, 3-D-125

Hougaard Madsen, K 3-O-165

Houhou, M 1-O-147

Howell, B 2-C-73

Hoyt, D 2-C-68

Hudziak, J 3-C-117

Huettel, S 1-O-154

Huggins, J 1-A-8

Hughes, B 2-D-77

Humphreys, K 3-C-114

Hunt, R 1-D-28, 3-C-115

Hérent, P 2-F-86

Hyde, L 1-O-151

Ibrahim, D 2-A-51, 3-D-128

Infante, A 2-C-70

Insel, C 2-D-76, 3-F-134

Irwin, L 2-B-59

Jabbi, M 1-G-46

Jacobus, J 2-C-70

Jaeggi, S 2-F-84

Jarcho, J 1-G-48, 3-G-137, 3-G-143

Jedd, M 3-C-115

Jenness, J 1-D-30

Jernigan, T 2-C-66, 3-C-116

Jeste, S 1-G-42

Johnson, C 2-C-69

Johnson, K 3-C-121

Jones, S 2-A-53

Joo, SJ 1-O-152

Josev, E 3-D-130

Jovanovic, T 1-C-22, 2-D-81

Junge, C 2-C-65

Juranek, J 2-A-56, 3-F-135

Author Poster No. Author Poster No. Author Poster No.

Page 33: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 31

Kabotyanski, K 1-D-31

Kabotyanski, K 2-D-80

Kamarsu, S 2-F-84

Kandala, S 1-C-18

Kaplan, J 1-C-20

Kastman, E 1-C-23, 1-D-31, 2-D-76, 2-D-78, 2-D-80

Kastner, S 1-A-2

Kaufman, J 3-C-117

Kazama, A 3-D-129

Keenan, K 2-D-83, 2-G-90

Kemner, C 2-C-65

Keyes, K 1-O-153

Kilroy, E 1-G-44

Kim, N 1-A-2

Kim, Y 1-C-22, 2-D-81

Kippenhan, J 1-A-8, 1-C-13, 1-G-46, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Kircanski, K 1-G-48

Klein, R 2-C-68, 3-C-119

Klimes-Dougan, B 1-D-28

Koh, N 1-O-155

Kohn, P 1-A-8, 1-C-13, 1-G-46, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Koliba, N 3-C-117

Koller, J 2-C-68, 3-C-119

Kordyban, L 3-A-102

Kosillo, P 1-F-38

Krabbendam, L 3-B-110

Kuskowski, M 1-B-10

Kwak, Y 3-B-107

Lambert, H 2-D-79, 3-G-141

Langley, A 2-G-92

Lara, J 3-D-127

Larsen, B 1-A-3, 3-D-124, 1-O-156

Larsen, M 3-G-146

Lattal, M 2-C-72

Laurent, H 1-G-45

Lee, A 1-O-155

Lee, K 3-D-130

Lee, N 3-B-110

Leibenluft, E 1-G-48

Leve, L 1-C-19

Levitas, D 1-A-1

LeWinn, K 1-O-153

Lewis, T 1-O-147

Li, A 2-D-77

Li, R 1-O-154

Lin, G 2-F-84

Lin, L 1-B-12

Lin, W 1-F-38

Lin, Y 1-A-5

Lindquist, K 2-B-61

Lion, S 2-F-86

Liu, C 2-C-69

Livingston, J 3-D-126

Lopez, K 1-C-18

Loso, H 3-B-108

Lowery, A 2-D-77

Luby, J 3-G-140

Luciana, M 1-B-10

Luna, B 1-A-3, 3-D-124, 1-O-156, 1-O-57, 3-O-164

Lyden, H 1-C-20

Maassen van den Brink, H 1-D-26

Machlin, L 1-D-32, 1-D-33

Magis-Weinberg, L 1-A-4

Magnotta, V 1-G-49

Malone, S 1-B-10

Mandell, J 3-G-138

Maplas, C 2-C-74

Margolin, G 1-C-20

Martin, E 1-G-49

Martinez, P 1-A-8, 1-C-13, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Masucci, M 2-A-51

Mathews, K 1-G-49

Mattson, W 3-G-144

Mazziotta, J 3-D-126

McCormick, E 2-C-71, 2-O-162

Mcdannald, M 1-F-38

McDonald, N 3-A-103

McIntyre, L 2-C-68

McLaughlin, K 1-D-30, 2-D-75, 2-D-79, 2-G-94, 3-G-142, 3-G-141, 3-A-105, 1-O-153, 1-O-155

Meltzoff, A 3-A-105

Menon, V 2-G-89, 3-D-127

Mervis, C 1-G-46

Meyer, K 2-A-54

Miller, A 1-D-32, 1-D-33

Mills, B 2-C-72

Mills, K 1-C-15, 2-B-59, 2-C-63, 2-C-69

Miranda-Dominguez, O 2-B-59, 2-C-72, 2-C-73, 3-C-119, 3-C-120

Mitchell, M 3-G-146

Mitchell, S 2-C-72

Mobasser, A 1-C-15, 2-C-63, 2-G-97

Mogensen, J 3-C-116

Montagna, A 2-G-95

Moore, T 3-C-112

Moraczewski, D 3-A-104

Morales, S 2-A-58, 2-D-82

Moreno, B 2-C-67

Morin, E 2-C-73

Mostofsky, S 3-G-145

Mumford, J 3-F-136

Mundy, L 3-C-111

Munier, E 1-D-32, 1-D-33

Murphy, L 3-D-129

Murray, S 2-C-62

Murty, V 1-A-3

Murty, V 3-D-124

Nagel, B 2-A-5, 1-O-148

Nardos, B 2-D-77

Nash, T 1-C-13, 1-G-46, 2-C-62

Nelson, B 1-C-15, 1-G-45

Nelson, E 1-G-48, 3-G-143, 3-G-144

Newcombe, N 1-A-5

Ngo, C 1-A-5

Nguyen, A 2-C-68

Nicholson, J 2-C-74

Nieman, L 1-A-8, 1-C13, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Nigg, J 2-B-59

Nketia, J 3-A-104

Nobre, A 2-A-52

Nobre, K 1-A-6

Nook, E 2-D-79

Nopoulos, P 1-G-49

Nosarti, C 2-G-95, 3-D-130

Nugiel, T 3-F-135

Nunez, A 3-D-127

Nussenbaum, K 1-A-6

O’Brien, M 1-G-46

O’Loughlin, K 3-C-117

O’Muircheartaigh, J 2-G-95

Obregon, J 2-A-51

Ochsner, K 1-D-29

Odriozola, P 2-A-50, 2-F-85, 2-G-93

Olson, I 1-A-5

Oppenheim, C 2-F-86

Orr, C 3-C-117

Ortiz, J 2-C-67

Ozernov-Palchik, O 2-F-87

Padgaonkar, N 3-C-118

Padmanabhan, A 2-G-89, 3-D-127

Paisley, C 3-A-103

Pascoe, L 3-D-130

Patai, E 2-A-52

Patrianakos, J 2-A-57

Patton, G 3-C-111

Peake, S 2-C-63, 2-G-97

Pelphrey, K 3-A-103

Peper, J 3-O-167

Perdue, K 3-A-103

Perdue, M 2-F-87

Author Poster No. Author Poster No. Author Poster No.

Page 34: 5th Annual Flux Congress

32 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Perez-Edgar, K 2-A-58

Perino, M 1-D-34

Perrone, A 3-C-119

Peters, S 1-C-21, 3-A-106

Petersen, S 2-C-68, 2-O-158

Peverill, M 3-G-142, 3-G-141

Pfeifer, J 1-C-15, 2-C-63, 2-G-97, 3-D-126

Piekarski, D 2-O-159

Pincus, M 2-C-73

Pine, D 1-G-48, 3-G-143

Pinsk, M 1-A-2

Potter, A 3-B-108

Powers, K 1-D-31, 2-D-78, 2-D-80

Pruessner, L 2-F-85

Pérez-Edgar, K 2-D-82

Qin, S 3-D-127

Quarmley, M 3-G-137, 3-G-143

Raab, H 2-C-62, 2-O-160

Ramani, G 2-F-84

Ramirez, J 3-C-120

Raney, T 2-F-87

Rappaport, B 1-G-48

Raschle, N 2-F-87

Raver, C 2-A-51, 3-D-128

Ravindranath, O 1-G-46

Redcay, E 1-A-1, 3-A-104

Reding, K 1-A-8, 1-C-13, 2-C-62, 2-C-73, 2-O-161

Rehert, R 3-D-127

Rheinschmidt-Same, M 2-D-77

Richeson, J 2-D-77

Richmond, S 3-C-121

Riggins, T 1-C-14, 2-A-55, 2-C-64, 3-C-113

Roalf, D 3-C-112

Roberts, G 3-D-130

Roberts, N 2-B-60

Robinson, E 1-A-8, 1-C-13, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Rodman, A 1-D-31, 2-D-78, 2-D-80

Roe, M 2-A-56, 3-F-135

Rogers, C 2-O-162

Rojas, D 1-G-41

Roman Siebner, H 3-O-165

Romero, M 3-F-134

Rooijen, R 2-C-65

Rosen, M 1-D-30, 3-A-105

Rosenbaum, G 2-A-57, 3-B-109

Rubien-Thomas, E 2-D-77

Ruparel, K 3-C-112

Rusnak, S 3-O-163

Sabb, F 3-F-133

Salvia, E 2-F-86

Sambrook, K 1-D-30, 2-G-94, 3-A-105, 3-G-141

Sanchez, M 2-C-73

Sasse, S 2-D-79

Satterthwaite, T 3-C-112

Saxbe, D 1-C-20

Scerif, G 1-A-6, 2-A-52

Scheinost, D 1-C-17, 3-A-101

Schenkel, L 1-A-8

Schifsky, E 2-D-77

Schlaggar, B 2-C-68, 2-O-158

Schmidt, P 1-A-8, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Schreiber, A 2-G-96

Schreuders, E 3-O-167

Sciberras, E 2-C-74

Seal, M 3-C-121, 3-D-130

Shablack, H 2-B-61

Shanley, L 3-F-133

Shaw, D 1-O-151

Shen, X 1-C-17, 1-C-17

Sheridan, M 1-D-30, 1-D-32, 1-D-33, 2-A-54, 3-A-105, 3-G-141, 3-G-142

Sherman, L 2-A-57, 3-C-118

Shic, F 3-A-103

Shinohara, R 3-C-112

Short, S 3-F-136

Shunmugavel, A 2-C-72

Siebner, H 2-C-66, 3-C-116

Sijtsma, H 3-B-110

Silk, T 2-C-74

Silvers, J 2-D-75

Simmons, J 3-C-111

Singer, J 3-G-144

Skak Madsen, K 2-C-66, 3-C-116, 3-O-165

Smith, A 1-G-48, 2-A-57, 3-G-143

Smith, J 1-C-19, 3-F-133

Smith, R 1-C-22

Smith, S 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 3-A-102

Smyser, C 2-G-98

Snyder, J 2-A-54

Somerville, L 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 1-D-31, 2-A-54, 2-D-76, 2-D-78, 2-D-79, 2-D-80, 23-A-102, 3-F-134

Sowell, E 2-C-69

Spaans, J 1-C-21

Spencer-Smith, M 3-D-130

Squeglia, L 2-C-70

Stark, A 1-D-31, 2-D-80

Steinbeis, N 3-C-123

Steinberg, L 3-B-109

Steiner, R 3-F-136

Stenberg, L 2-A-57

Steven, S 1-A-8

Stevens, J 1-C-22, 2-D-81

Stoycos, S 1-C-20

Strange, N 2-A-57

Sturgeon, D 3-C-120

Styner, M 2-C-73

Subiaul, F 3-O-163

Sullivan, E 3-C-120

Sutherland, M 3-F-133

Taber-Thomas, B 2-A-58

Tamnes, C 2-C-69, 3-O-167

Tapert, S 2-C-70

Tashjian, S 3-C-122, 3-D-132

Taylor, W 3-F-135

Taylor-Thompson, K 2-D-77

Telzer, E 1-B-12, 1-D-34, 2-C-71, 2-B-61, 3-C-114, 2-O-162

Tereshchenko, A 1-G-49

Tervo-Clemmens, B 1-O-156, 3-O-164

Thai, N 2-A-58

Thomas, K 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 1-D-28, 3-A-102, 3-C-115

Thomason, M 1-O-147

Thompson, D 3-D-130

Tissier, C 2-F-86

Tottenham, N 1-D-35, 3-C-114

Tucker, Z 1-A-7

Tucker-Drob, E 2-A-56, 3-A-100

Tummeltshammer, K 3-O-166

Turtura, J 3-F-133

Uddin, L 2-A-50

Uy, J 3-C-122

Vallorani, A 2-D-82

Van, A 3-B-110

van der Cruijsen, R 3-A-106

van den Heuvel, M 1-O-147

vd Kamp, F 3-O-167

van Hoorn, J 2-O-162

van Rooij, S 1-C-22, 2-D-81

Van, A 2-C-68, 3-C-119

Vance, L 1-C-22

VanTieghem, M 1-D-35

Vedula, S 1-B-11

Venkatraman, V 3-B-109

Venticinque, J 3-G-144

Vidal, B 1-C-23

Vijayakumar, N 2-C-69, 2-G-97

Vilgis, V 2-D-83

Wall, C 3-A-103

Walsh, F 3-B-107

Walton, W 2-B-59

Author Poster No. Author Poster No. Author Poster No.

Flux Poster Author Index | By Author Name

Page 35: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 33

Wang, T 3-G-146

Warnell, K 1-A-1

Warren, S 3-D-127

Waterman, J 2-G-92

Wei, S 1-A-8, 1-C-13, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Werchan, D 1-F-39

Wertman, V 3-D-129

Wesevich, V 2-C-68

Westhoff, B 1-C-24

Westlund-Schreiner, M 1-D-28

Wheelock, M 2-G-98

Whittle, S 2-C-69, 3-C-111, 3-C-121

Wierenga, L 3-O-167

Wilbrecht, L 1-B-11, 1-D-35, 1-F-38, 2-O-159

Wilkey, E 1-A-9

Winther Andersen, K 3-O-165

Wodzinski, A 2-F-84

Woodward, L 2-G-98

Wymbs, N 3-G-145

Xia, C 3-C-112

Ya, W 3-B-107

Yacoub, E 1-A-7, 1-C-23, 3-A-102

Yanovski, J 1-A-8, 1-C-13, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Yeatman, J 1-O-152

Yoon, H 3-F-133

Yu, X 2-F-87

Zawadzki, M 1-C-13, 2-C-62, 2-O-161

Zetina, H 1-O-150

Zhang, J 3-F-134

Zhang, Y 1-B-11

Zheng, A 3-G-146

Zhorne, L 1-G-49

Zhu, J 3-C-120

Zuk, J 2-F-87

Author Poster No.

2018Berlin, GermanyAug 30-Sept 1, 2018

Flux Congress

Page 36: 5th Annual Flux Congress

34 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Poster Session 1Saturday September 16

1-A-1 Social interaction recruits mentalizing andreward systems in middle childhoodDiana Alkire¹, Daniel Levitas¹, Katherine Warnell², ElizabethRedcay¹¹University of Maryland, College Park, ²Texas State University

1-A-2 Probing the biased competition theory ofselective attention in the developing brain: An fMRIstudy in school-aged childrenNa Yeon Kim¹, Mark Pinsk¹, Sabine Kastner¹¹Princeton University

1-A-3 Greater learning-dependent change inhippocampal systems relates to reward learningBart Larsen¹, Finnegan Calabro¹, Vishnu Murty¹, WilliamForan¹, Beatriz Luna¹¹University of Pittsburgh

1-A-4 Prospective memory in adolescence andadulthoodLucia Magis-Weinberg¹, Ruud Custers², Iroise Dumontheil³¹University College London, ²Utrecht University, ³Birkbeck,University of London

1-A-5 Relational memory and pattern separationacross the lifespanChi Ngo¹, Ying Lin¹, Nora Newcombe¹, Ingrid Olson¹¹Temple University

1-A-6 Salient visual events disrupt memory-guidedattention in adults but not childrenKate Nussenbaum¹, Kia Nobre¹, Gaia Scerif¹¹University of Oxford

1-A-7 Introducing the human connectome project -development: behavioral, cognitive, and biologicalmeasuresZachary Tucker¹, Deanna Barch², Susan Bookheimer¹, RandyBuckner³, Gregory Burgess², Michael Harms², CatherineHegarty¹, Cynthia Hernke², Stephen Smith⁴, Leah Somerville³,Kathleen Thomas⁵, David Van Essen², Essa Yacoub⁵, MirellaDapretto¹¹University of California, Los Angeles, ²Washington Universityin St. Louis, ³Harvard University, ⁴Oxford University,⁵University of Minnesota1-A-8 The NIMH longitudinal study of theneurobiologic and endocrine events of puberty:Hormonal and metabolic changes that accompanypuberty in typically developing childrenShau-Ming Wei¹, Pedro Martinez¹, Soldin Steven¹, LindaSchenkel¹, Katherine Reding¹, J. Shane Kippenhan¹, PhilipKohn¹, Jefferson Huggins¹, Elizabeth Robinson¹, SheilaBrady¹, Lynnette Nieman¹, Jack Yanovski¹, Karen Berman¹,Peter Schmidt¹¹NIH

1-B-10 Neural correlates of improved decision-making as assessed by the Iowa Gambling TaskBrandon Almy¹, Paul Collins, Mike Kuskowski¹, Steve Malone¹,Monica Luciana¹¹University of Minnesota

1-B-11 How do D2R-expressing MSNs in thedorsomedial striatum contribute to goal directedchoice?Kristen Delevich¹, Yuting Zhang¹, Satya Vedula¹, LindaWilbrecht¹¹UC Berkeley

1-B-12 Like me back: Youths’ feelings aboutunknown peers influences neural response duringpredicted peer evaluationCarina Fowler¹, Lynda Lin¹, Eva Telzer¹¹University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

1-C-13 Working memory-related fMRI activation as afunction of pubertal status and sex in typically-developing children and adolescentsAustin Boroshok¹, Shau-Ming Wei¹, Katherine Reding¹, TiffanyNash², Miriam Zawadzki¹, Jordan Barone¹, Pedro Martinez¹, E.Lisa Robinson², J. Shane Kippenhan¹, Philip Kohn¹, LynetteNieman³, Jack Yanovski⁴, Karen Berman¹¹National Institute of Mental Health, ²National Institutes ofHealth, ³National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive andKidney Diseases, ⁴National Institute of Child Health andHuman Development

1-C-14 Relations between pattern separation abilityand hippocampal subfield volume in childhoodKelsey Canada¹, Fengji Geng¹, Tracy Riggins¹¹University of Maryland

1-C-17 Individual connectomes are unique and stablein the developing brain from adolescence to youngadulthoodCorey Horien¹, Xilin Shen¹, Dustin Scheinost¹, R. ToddConstable¹¹Yale School of Medicine

1-C-18 Characterizing the functional connectivitydevelopment of the prefrontal cortexKatherine Lopez¹, Deanna Barch¹, Sridhar Kandala¹¹Washington University in St Louis

1-C-20 Community violence exposure: Longitudinalassociations with hippocampal structure andfunctionDarby Saxbe¹, Hannah Lyden¹, Larissa Del Piero², SarahStoycos¹, Sarah Gimbel¹, Gayla Margolin¹, Jonas Kaplan¹¹University of Southern California, ²University of Washington

1-C-21 Neural correlates of rewards for self andcharity: prosocial development during adolescenceJochem Spaans¹, Sabine Peters¹, Eveline Crone¹¹Leiden University

Flux Congress Posters | Titles, Authors and Affiliations

Page 37: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 35

Flux Congress Posters | Titles, Authors and Affiliations

1-C-22 Effects of trauma exposure on fear inhibitioncircuitry in the developing brainSanne van Rooij¹, Ryan Smith¹, Jennifer Stevens¹, Ye Ji Kim¹,L. Alexander Vance¹, Tanja Jovanovic¹¹Emory University

1-C-23 Introducing the human connectome project -devleopment: Task-fMRI paradigmsConstanza Vidal Bustamante¹, Deanna Barch², SusanBookheimer³, Randy Buckner¹, Gregory Burgess⁴, MirellaDapretto³, Michael Harms⁴, Cynthia Hernke⁴, Erik Kastman¹,Stephen Smith5, Kathleen Thomas6, David Van Essen⁴, EssaYacoub6, Leah Somerville¹¹Harvard University, ²Washington University in St. Louis,³UCLA, ⁴Washington University in St. Louis University,5University of Oxford, 6University of Minnesota

1-C-24 Developmental trajectories of resting-statefunctional connectivity in adolescence: a longitudinalstudyBianca Westhoff¹, Anna van Duijvenvoorde¹, Frank de Vos¹,Eveline Crone¹¹Institute of Psychology, Leiden University

1-D-27 How does peer evaluation influence hot andcool inhibitory control in adolescence and adults?Lison Bouhours¹, Olivier HOUDE¹, Gregoire BORST¹, MathieuCASSOTTI¹¹Paris Descartes University

1-D-28 Alexithymia is associated with neuralreactivity to masked emotional faces in adolescentswho self-harmLauren Demers¹, Melinda Westlund-Schreiner¹, Ruskin Hunt¹,Bonnie Klimes-Dougan¹, Kathleen Thomas¹, Kathryn Cullen¹¹University of Minnesota

1-D-29 Regulating responses to social and appetitiverewards across developmentDanielle Goldman¹, Chelsea Helion¹, Kevin Ochsner¹¹Columbia University

1-D-30 Differential associations of distinct forms ofchildhood adversity with neurobehavioral measuresof reward processing: Neurodevelopmental pathwaysto depressionJessica Jenness¹, Meg Dennison¹, Maya Rosen¹, KellySambrook¹, Margaret Sheridan¹, Katie McLaughlin¹¹University of Washington

1-D-31 Quantifying objective monetary reward valuein adolescents and adults using a physical effortparadigmKatherine Kabotyanski¹, Alexandra Rodman¹, KatherinePowers¹, Erik Kastman¹, Abigail Stark¹, Leah Somerville¹¹Harvard University

1-D-32 Behavioral and neural correlates of socialevaluation in adolescent girlsLaura Machlin¹, Adam Miller¹, Emily Munier¹, MargaretSheridan¹¹University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

1-D-33 The effects of a target interpersonal rejectionon emotion regulation in typically developing girls: Apilot studyAdam Miller¹, Emily Munier¹, Laura Machlin¹, MargaretSheridan¹¹University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

1-D-34 These violent delights have violent ends:Neural correlates of aggression selectivity indelinquent youth based on differential motivationsMichael Perino¹, Joao Guassi Moreira², Eva Telzer³¹University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, ²University ofCalifornia, Los Angeles, ³University of North Carolina, Chapel-Hill

1-D-35 Developmental change in four-choice reversallearning coincides with puberty onsetMichelle VanTieghem¹, Linda Wilbrecht², Nim Tottenham¹¹Columbia University, ²University of California Berkeley

1-F-36 Using multiple learning paradigms tocharacterize brain networks supporting adolescentlearningSamantha DePasque¹, Adriana Galvan¹¹UCLA

1-F-38 Developmental experience of food insecurityimpairs cognitive flexibility and alters dopaminerelease in the striatum in adulthoodWan Chen Lin¹, Polina Kosillo¹, Ezequiel Galarce¹, MichaelMcdannald², Helen Bateup¹, Linda Wilbrecht¹¹University of California, Berkeley, ²Boston College

1-F-39 9-month-olds use higher-order contexts toorganize working representations in a A-not-B taskDenise Werchan¹, Kelley Gunther¹, Dima Amso¹¹Brown University

1-G-40 Quantitative assessment of image quality ofsparse functional near infrared spectroscopy vs high-density diffuse optical tomographyTracy Burns-Yocum¹, Adam Eggebrecht¹, Joseph Culver¹¹Washington University in St. Louis School of Medicine

1-G-41 Neural correlates of observation andimitation of human and robot hand motion in autismspectrum disorderGarrett Cardon¹, Donald Rojas¹, Isabelle Buard²¹Colorado State University, ²University of Colorado AnschutzMedical Campus

Page 38: 5th Annual Flux Congress

36 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

1-G-42 Using EEG to assay language processing inminimally verbal children with ASDCharlotte DiStefano¹, Shafali Jeste¹¹University of California, Los Angeles

1-G-44 Reduced neural activity in the actionobservation network in children and adolescentswith autism spectrum disorder during observation ofsocial and motor actionsEmily Kilroy¹, Laura Harrison¹, Christiana Butera¹, SharonCermak¹, Lisa Aziz-Zadeh¹¹USC

1-G-45 Infant HPA axis as a potentail mechanismlinking maternal mental health and infant telomerelengthBenjamin Nelson¹, Nicholas Allen¹, Heidemarie Laurent¹¹University of Oregon

1-G-46 Neural substrates of gustatory emotionprocessing in children with Williams Syndrome and7q11.23 Duplication SyndromeMadeline O’Brien¹, Tiffany Nash¹, Mbemba Jabbi¹, MichaelGregory¹, Orma Ravindranath¹, Danielle Currin¹, ShannonGrogans¹, J. Shane Kippenhan¹, Philip Kohn¹, DanielEisenberg¹, Carolyn Mervis², Karen Berman¹¹National Institute of Mental Health, ²University of Louisville

1-G-48 Social anxiety severity and age influenceneural responses to social feedbackAshley Smith¹, Eric Nelson², Katharina Kircanski¹, BrentRappaport³, Quyen Do¹, Ellen Leibenluft¹, Daniel Pine¹,Johanna Jarcho⁴¹National Institute of Mental Health, ²Nationwide Children’sHospital, ³Washingtion University, ⁴Stony Brook University1-G-49 The role of the cerebellum in juvenileHuntington’s DiseaseAlexander Tereshchenko¹, Eric Epping¹, Katherine Mathews¹,Leah Zhorne¹, Erin Martin¹, Patricia Espie-Pfeiffer¹, VincentMagnotta¹, Peg Nopoulos¹¹University of Iowa

Poster Session 2Sunday September 17

2-A-51 Attention bias to threat moderates theassociation of poverty and anxiety with internalizingamong low-income adolescentsMeriah DeJoseph¹, Deanna Ibrahim¹, Javanna Obregon¹,Michael Masucci¹, C. Cybele Raver¹¹New York University

2-A-52 The functional consequences of socialdistraction with complex scenes: alpha oscillationsand developmentBrianna Doherty¹, Frederik van Ede², Eva Patai³, Anna Nobre²,Gaia Scerif²¹University of California San Francisco, ²University of Oxford,³University College, London

2-A-53 Neurocognitive development in binge-drinking adolescents with and without concomitantmarijuana useScott Jones¹, Bonnie Nagel¹¹Oregon Health & Science University

2-A-54 Inhibiting reward-related responses requiresgreater frontal control than inhibiting prepotentresponses aloneKristin Meyer¹, Juliet Davidow², Jenna Snyder¹, LeahSomerville², Margaret Sheridan¹¹UNC, ²Harvard

2-A-55 Positive, but not negative, parenting in earlychildhood predicts both hippocampal volume andepisodic memory ability in middle childhoodTracy Riggins¹, Morgan Botdorf¹, Sarah Blankenship¹, LeaDougherty¹¹University of Maryland

2-A-56 Overlapping regions of error-related activityacross three tasks in childrenMary Abbe Roe¹, Laura Engelhardt¹, Jenifer Juranek², K. PaigeHarden¹, Elliot Tucker-Drob¹, Jessica Church¹¹University of Texas at Austin, ²University of Texas HealthScience Center at Houston

2-A-57 Interactive effects of alcohol consumptionand peer presence on connectivity between thenucleus accumbens and the response inhibitionnetworkLauren Sherman¹, Gail Rosenbaum², Ashley Smith³, MorganBotdorf⁴, Karla Fettich, Jamie Patrianakos5, Nicole Strange6,Laurence Stenberg¹, Jason Chein¹¹Temple University, ²NYU, ³NIMH, ⁴University of Maryland,5Loyola University Chicago, 6Centre for Addiction and MentalHealth

Page 39: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 37

Flux Congress Posters | Titles, Authors and Affiliations

2-A-58 Electrophysiological marker underlyingbehavioral inhibition, attentional disengagement, andanxiety in childrenNhi Thai¹, Bradley Taber-Thomas, Santiago Morales², KoralyPerez-Edgar¹¹Pennsylvania State University, ²University of Maryland

2-B-59 Age and neural maturation predict changes intemporal discounting in the transition to adolescenceJeya Anandakumar¹, Kathryn Mills², Eric Earl¹, Lourdes Irwin¹,Oscar Miranda-Dominguez¹, Damion Demeter³, AlexandraWalton Weston⁴, Joel Nigg¹, Damien Fair¹¹Oregon Health & Science University, ²University of Oregon,³University of Texas, ⁴Janelia Research Campus2-B-60 Examining differences in healthy weight vs.overweight adolescents in reward sensitivity andcognitive controlNicole Roberts¹¹The Pennsylvania State University

2-B-61 Incorporating the social context inneurocognitive models of adolescent risk-taking: Aneuroimaging meta-analysisJorien van Hoorn¹, Holly Shablack¹, Kristen Lindquist¹, EvaTelzer¹¹UNC Chapel Hill

2-C-62 Influences of sex and pubertal status onstructural brain development in typically-developingchildrenJordan Barone¹, Katherine Reding¹, Jonathan Kippenhan¹,Shau-Ming Wei¹, Tiffany Nash¹, Miriam Zawadzki¹, AustinBoroshok¹, Shanna Murray¹, Hillary Raab¹, Pedro Martinez¹,Elizabeth Robinson², Philip Kohn¹, Lynnette Nieman³, JackYanovski⁴, Peter Schmidt¹,¹National Institute of Mental Health, ²National Institutes ofHealth, ³National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive andKidney Diseases, ⁴National Institute of Child Health andHuman Development

2-C-63 Dimensions of adversity in resting statefunctional connectivity of the amygdala andhippocampusTheresa Cheng¹, Kathryn Mills¹, John Flournoy¹, JessicaFlannery¹, Shannon Peake¹, Arian Mobasser¹, Philip Fisher¹,Jennifer Pfeifer¹¹University of Oregon

2-C-64 Relations between autobiographical memoryand hippocampal subregion volume in earlychildhoodLisa Cox¹, Oyindamola Adedipe¹, Tracy Riggins¹¹University of Maryland

2-C-65 Emotion discrimination of facial expressionsin 5-month-old infants: an fNIRS studyRenata Di Lorenzo¹, Anna Blasi², Rianne Rooijen¹, CarolineJunge¹, Carlijn Boomen¹, Chantal Kemner¹¹Utrecht University, ²Birkbeck, University of London

2-C-66 Maturation of major white matter tractsduring childhood and adolescence: A longitudinalstudy with up to 11 time pointsAmalie Ekstrand¹, William Frans Christiaan Baaré¹, JonathanHolm-Skjold¹, Hartwig Siebner¹, Terry Jernigan, Kathrine SkakMadsen¹¹Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance

2-C-67 Homotopic resting state functionalconnectivity correlates with visuospatial abilities inschool-age childrenZeus Gracia Tabuenca¹, Beatriz Moreno¹, Juan Ortiz¹,Fernando Barrios¹, Sarael Alcauter¹¹Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico

2-C-68 Real-time visual head motion feedback andmovie watching reduce head motion during MRIscanning in children under 11 years oldDeanna Greene¹, Jonathan Koller¹, Victoria Wesevich¹,Jacqueline Hampton¹, Annie Nguyen¹, Lindsey McIntyre¹,Catherine Hoyt Drazen¹, Andrew Van¹, Eric Earl², RachelKlein², Steven Petersen¹, Bradley Schlaggar¹, Damien Fair²,Nico Dosenbach¹¹Washington University School of Medicine, ²Oregon Health &Sciences University

2-C-69 Development of subcortical volumes acrossadolescence in males and females: A multisamplestudy of longitudinal changesMegan Herting¹, Cory Johnson¹, Kathryn Mills², NanditaVijayakumar², Chang Liu¹, Meg Dennison, Anne-LiseGoddings, Ronald Dahl³, Elizabeth Sowell4, Sarah Whittle5,Christian Tamnes6

¹University of Southern California, ²University of Oregon,³University of California Berkley, ⁴Children’s Hospital LosAngeles, 5The University of Melbourne, 6University of Oslo

2-C-70 Adolescent surface area pre- and postmarijuana and alcohol initiationAlejajandra Infante¹, Kelly Courtney¹, Norma Castro¹, LindsaySqueglia², Susan Tapert¹, Joanna Jacobus¹¹University of Caifornia San Diego, ²Medical University ofSouth Carolina

2-C-71 Functional network organization of the socialbrain in childhood and adolescenceEthan McCormick¹, Jorien van Hoorn¹, Eva Telzer¹¹University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill

2-C-72 Chronically elevated prenatal cytokineexposure changes rodent offspring behavior andfunctional connectivity network structureBrian Mills¹, Anandakumar Shunmugavel¹, AlinaGoncharova¹, Oscar Miranda-Dominguez¹, Matt Lattal¹,Suzanne Mitchell¹, Damien Fair¹¹Oregon Health & Science University

Page 40: 5th Annual Flux Congress

38 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

2-C-73 Developmental outcomes of early adversecare: elevated cortisol and altered Amygdalafunctional connectivityElyse Morin¹, Brittany Howell¹, Kathy Reding², Eric Feczko¹,Eric Earl³, Oscar Miranda-Dominguez³, Melanie Pincus¹,Martin Styner , Damien Fair³, Mar Sanchez¹¹Emory University, ²NIH/NIMH, ³OHSU, UNC

2-C-74 Multimodal structural neuroimaging markersof ADHD symptomsTim Silk¹, Gareth Ball¹, Charles Maplas¹, Sila Genc¹, DarylEfron², Vicki Anderson², Jan Nicholson³, Emma Sciberras⁴¹Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, ²Royal Children’sHospital, ³LaTrobe University, ⁴Deakin University2-D-75 Specializaiton of lateral prefrontal corticalactivity underlies successful emotion regulation inyouthJoao Guassi Moreira¹, Katie McLaughlin², Jennifer Silvers¹¹University of California, Los Angeles, ²University ofWashington, Seattle

2-D-76 Corticostriatal circuit development constrainsgoal directed behavior through adolescenceCatherine Insel¹, Erik Kastman¹, Catherine Glenn², LeahSomerville¹¹Harvard University, ²University of Rochester

2-D-77 The Fusiform Face Area shows distinctpatterns of fMRI activity to black vs. white faces indifferent emotional contextsBinyam Nardos¹, Estée Rubien-Thomas², Alexandra Cohen³,Emma Schifsky¹, A. Li⁴, A. Cervera², A. Lowery², DanielleDellarco5, M. Rheinschmidt-Same6, N. Daumeyer², N. Camp7,Brent Hughes8, Kim Taylor-Thompson³, Jennifer Eberhardt7,Jennifer Richeson², B. Ca¹Oregon Health & Science University, ²Yale University, ³NewYork University, ⁴Weill Cornell Medical College, 5University ofMiami, 6Northwestern University, 7Stanford University,8University of California Riverside

2-D-78 The development of self-protective biases:Adolescents internalize and adults externalizeevaluative social feedbackAlexandra Rodman¹, Katherine Powers¹, Erik Kastman¹, LeahSomerville¹¹Harvard University

2-D-79 Characteristics of contra-hedonic decision-making vary across development: Evidence from avalenced choice taskStephanie Sasse¹, Erik Nook¹, Hilary Lambert², KateMcLaughlin², Leah Somerville¹¹Harvard University, ²University of Washington

2-D-80 How valuable is social feedback toadolescents and adults? Objective quantification ofsocial motivation using a physical effort paradigmAbigail Stark¹, Alexandra Rodman¹, Katie Powers¹, EricKastman¹, Katya Kabotyanski¹, Leah Somerville¹¹Harvard University

2-D-81 Ongoing violence exposure in late childhoodpredicts increased amygdala reactivity to threatJennifer Stevens¹, Ye Ji Kim¹, Sanne van Rooij¹, TanjaJovanovic¹¹Emory University School of Medicine

2-D-82 Rejection distress coupled with reducedattention to and neural processing of social rewardrelates to internalizing symptomsAlicia Vallorani¹, Santiago Morales², Koraly Pérez-Edgar¹¹The Pennsylvania State University, ²University of Maryland

2-D-83 A 4-year longitudinal analysis of whole-brainactivation to emotional faces in late adolescent girlsand associations with emotion regulation skillsVeronika Vilgis¹, Kristina Gelardi¹, Erika Forbes², AlisonHipwell², Kate Keenan³, Amanda Guyer¹¹UC Davis, ²University of Pittsburgh, ³University of Chicago

2-F-84 Can playing memory games improveexecutive function skills?Grace Lin¹, Snigdha Kamarsu¹, Emily Daubert², AlainaWodzinski², Geetha Ramani², Susanne Jaeggi¹¹University of California, Irvine, ²University of Maryland,College Park

2-F-85 Safety signal learning as a novel method offear reduction in adolescents and young adultsPaola Odriozola¹, Luise Pruessner¹, Jason Haberman¹, EmilyCohodes¹, Dylan Gee¹¹Yale University

2-F-86 The regional homogeneity (reho) of resting-state FMRI signal, a biomarker of the receptivity toinhibitory contral training?Emilie Salvia¹, Paul Hérent¹, Cloélia Tissier¹, Sylvain Charron²,Catherine Oppenheim², Stéphanie Lion², Olivier Houda¹,Grégoire Borst¹, Arnaud Cachia¹¹Paris Descartes, ²INSERM

2-F-87 Emergence of the neural network underlyingphonological processing from the pre-reading to theemergent reading stage: a longitudinal studyXi Yu¹, Talia Raney², Meaghan Perdue², Jennifer Zuk³, OlaOzernov-Palchik⁴, Bryce Becker², Nora Raschle5, Nadine Gaab¹¹Boston Children’s Hospital/Harvard Medical School, ²BostonChildren’s Hospital, ³Boston Children’s Hospital/HarvardUniversity, ⁴Boston Children’s Hospital/Tufts University,5University of Basel, Psychiatric University Hospital

2-G-88 Longitudinal analysis of depression riskfactors in a large sample of adolescent girls screenedfor high depressive symptomsGabriela Alarcon¹, Erika Forbes¹¹University of Pittsburgh

2-G-89 Hyperconnectivity of voice processing brainnetworks in females with autismAmanda Baker¹, Daniel Abrams¹, Aarthi Padmanabhan¹, VinodMenon¹¹Stanford University School of Medicine

Page 41: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 39

Flux Congress Posters | Titles, Authors and Affiliations

2-G-90 Depressive symptomatology and brainnetwork architecture in adolescent girlsRajpreet Chahal¹, Kate Keenan², Erika Forbes, Alison Hipwell,Amanda Guyer¹¹Center for Mind and Brain, UC Davis, ²University of Chicago

2-G-91 Imaging brain function in children withautism spectrum disorder with diffuse opticaltomographyAdam Eggebrecht¹, Joseph Culver¹¹Washington University School of Medicine

2-G-92 Sensory over-responsivity in youth adoptedfrom foster careShulamite Green¹, Kaitlin Cummings¹, Mirella Dapretto¹,Susan Bookheimer¹, Jill Waterman¹, Audra Langley¹¹University of California, Los Angeles

2-G-93 Distress tolerance and anxiety acrossdevelopment: Interactions with age and sexJason Haberman¹, Paola Odriozola¹, Emily Cohodes¹, DylanGee¹¹Yale University

2-G-94 Atypical neural function during affectivetheory of mind: a developmental mechanism linkingviolence exposure and externalizing psychopathologyCharlotte Heleniak¹, Kelly Sambrook¹, Katie McLaughlin¹¹University of Washington

2-G-95 Socio-economic status and the neonatalbrainAnita Montagna¹, Jonathan O’Muircheartaigh¹, ChiaraNosarti¹, A. David Edwards¹¹King’s College London

2-G-96 Developmental changes in evidenceaccumulation and decision thresholds in borderlinepersonalityAlison Schreiber¹, Nathan Hall¹, Michael Hallquist¹¹Pennsylvania State Univeresity

2-G-97 Neural responses to peer interactions inadolescents with early life adversity: an investigationof social exclusion and over-inclusionNandita Vijayakumar¹, Theresa Cheng¹, John Flournoy¹,Shannon Peake, Jessica Flannery¹, Arian Mobasser¹, SarahAlberti¹, Philip Fisher¹, Jennifer Pfeifer¹¹University of Oregon

2-G-98 Motor performance relates to resting state-functional connectivity MRI in term- and preterm-born childrenMuriah Wheelock¹, Nicola Austin, Samudragupta Bora, AdamEggebrecht¹, Lianne Woodward, Christopher Smyser¹¹Washington University

Poster Session 3 Monday September 183-A-100 Common BOLD activity over three executivefunction tasks in middle childhoodLaura Engelhardt¹, K. Paige Harden¹, Elliot Tucker-Drob¹,Jessica Church¹¹The University of Texas

3-A-101 Connectome-based predictive modeling: Theimpact of brain state and sex in a developmentalcohortAbigail Greene¹, Siyuan Gao¹, Dustin Scheinost¹, R. ToddConstable¹¹Yale University

3-A-102 Introducing the human connectome project- development: general overviewLaurel Kordyban¹, Deanna Barch², Susan Bookheimer³, RandyBuckner¹, Gregory Burgess², Mirella Dapretto³, MichaelHarms², Cynthia Hernke², Stephen Smith4, Kathleen Thomas5,David Van Essen², Essa Yacoub5, Leah Somerville¹¹Harvard University, ²Washington University, ³UCLA,4University of Oxford, 5University of Minnesota

3-A-103 Infant brain responses to social sounds: alongitudinal fNIRS studyNicole McDonald¹, Katherine Perdue², Jeffrey Eilbott³, HarlanFichtenholtz4, Amy Ahn5, Megan Braconnier³, Carla Wall6,Courtney Paisley7, Frederick Shic5, Kevin Pelphrey8

¹UCLA, ²Boston Children’s Hospital, ³Yale School of Medicine,4Keene State College, 5Seattle Children’s Hospital, Universityof Washington, 6University of South Carolina, 7University ofAlabama, 8George Washington University, Children’s NationalHealth Sy

3-A-104 Cortical temporal hierarchy and social-cognitive comprehension in middle childhoodDustin Moraczewski¹, Jazlyn Nketia¹, Elizabeth Redcay¹¹University of Maryland

3-A-105 Socioeconomic status and brain structureand function across development: Implications foracademic achievementMaya Rosen¹, Margaret Sheridan², Kelly Sambrook¹, AndrewMeltzoff¹, Katie McLaughlin¹¹University of Washington, ²University of North Carolina,Chapel Hill

3-A-106 Neural and behavioral development of directversus reflected self-evaluations in adolescenceRenske van der Cruijsen¹, Sabine Peters¹, Eveline Crone¹¹Leiden University

3-A-99 Stress system genes and cognitivedevelopoment in childhoodClancy Blair¹¹New York University

Page 42: 5th Annual Flux Congress

40 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

3-B-107 Feelings about the future: The effect ofperceived stability on decision making in collegestudents across US and ChinaYoungbin Kwak¹, Francesca Walsh¹, Xingjie Chen¹, ErikCheries¹, Wang Ya¹UMass Amherst

3-B-108 The relationship between impulsivity andpeer problems across adolescenceHannah Loso¹, Hugh Garavan¹, Alexandra Potter¹, IMAGENConsortium¹University of Vermont Medical Center

3-B-109 Adolescents and adults learn differentlyfrom description and experienceGail Rosenbaum¹, Vinod Venkatraman², Laurence Steinberg²,Jason Chein²¹New York University, ²Temple University

3-B-110 Friendly and unfriendly social interactionsaffect subsequent trust behavior in adolescentsHester Sijtsma¹, Nikki Lee¹, Nienke Van Atteveldt¹, LydiaKrabbendam¹¹Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam

3-C-112 Mapping network-level coupling ofstructural and functional connectivity duringadolescenceGraham Baum¹, Rastko Ciric¹, Cedric Xia¹, David Roalf¹,Richard Betzel¹, Tyler Moore¹, Russell Shinohara¹, PhilipCook¹, Mark Elliot¹, Kosha Ruparel¹, Christos Davatzikos¹,Raquel Gur¹, Ruben Gur¹, Danielle Bassett¹, TheodoreSatterthwaite¹¹University of Pennsylvania

3-C-113 Relations between working memory andcortical thickness in anterior cingulate cortex anddorsolateral prefrontal cortex in early childhoodMorgan Botdorf¹, Tracy Riggins¹¹University of Maryland, College Park

3-C-114 Earlier and atypical structural connectivityfollowing early caregiver deprivationChelsea Harmon¹, Laurel Gabard-Durnam², Dylan Gee³,Bonnie Goff, Dominic Fareri⁴, Christina Caldera5, JessicaFlannery6, Eva Telzer7, Kathryn Humphreys8, Nim Tottenham¹¹Columbia University, ²Harvard University, ³Yale University,⁴Adelphi University, 5UCLA, 6U Oregon, 7Duke University,8Stanford University

3-C-115 Resting state brain network differences inyouth adopted from international orphanagesMax Herzberg¹, Kelly Jedd McKenzie¹, Ruskin Hunt¹, MeganGunnar¹, Kathleen Thomas¹¹University of Minnesota

3-C-116 Development of emotional processing islinked to maturational changes in left-right cingulumasymmetry during adolescenceJonathan Holm-Skjold¹, William Frans Christiaan Baaré²,Jesper Mogensen³, Hartwig Siebner², Terry Jernigan, KathrineSkak Madsen²¹Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance / The Unitfor Cognitive Neuroscience, ²Danish Research Centre forMagnetic Resonance, ³The Unit for Cognitive Neuroscience

3-C-117 The relationship between child adversity,anxiety symptoms and white matter integrityNaomi Koliba¹, Catherine Orr¹, Kerry O’Loughlin, HannahHolbrook¹, Brian Carlozzi¹, Matthew Albaugh¹, Joan Kaufman²,James Hudziak¹¹University of Vermont, ²Kennedy Krieger Institute

3-C-118 Salience network connectivity and risk-taking behavior in high school and college studentsNamita Padgaonkar¹, Lauren Sherman², Leanna Hernandez¹,Mirella Dapretto¹¹UCLA, ²Temple University & Haverford College

3-C-119 Filtering artificial motion caused bymagnetic field distortions from cardiopulmonaryfunctionAnders Perrone¹, Oscar Miranda-Dominguez¹, Eric Earl¹,Jonathon Koller², Andrew Van², Rachel Klein¹, NicoDosenbach², Damien Fair¹¹Oregon Health and Science University, ²WashingtonUniversity in St. Louis

3-C-120 The influence of maternal diet on macaqueoffspring structural brain volume and behaviorJulian Ramirez¹, Darrick Sturgeon¹, Eric Feczko¹, JenniferZhu¹, Jennifer Bagley¹, Oscar Miranda-Dominguez¹, Eric Earl¹,Elinor Sullivan¹, Damien Fair¹¹Oregon Health & Science University

3-C-121 Association between family environmentsand modularity on structural brain networks in latechildhoodSally Richmond¹, Marc Seal², Nicholas Allen³, KatherineJohnson¹, Richard Beare², Sarah Whittle¹¹University of Melbourne, ²Murdoch Childrens ResearchInstitute, ³University of Oregon

3-C-122 Physical environment relates tosocioeconomic disparities in cortical structure andreading achievement in adolescentsJessica Uy¹, Diane Goldenberg¹, Sarah Tashjian¹, AdrianaGalvan¹¹UCLA

3-C-123 Heritability of neural reactions to socialexclusion in middle childhoodMara van der Meulen¹, Niko Steinbeis¹, Michelle Achterberg¹,Marinus van IJzendoorn¹, Eveline Crone¹¹Leiden University

Page 43: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 41

Flux Congress Posters | Titles, Authors and Affiliations

3-D-124 Development of reward learning behaviorand striatal activation through adolescenceFinnegan Calabro¹, Vishnu Murty¹, Bart Larsen¹, Beatriz Luna¹¹University of Pittsburgh

3-D-12 5 How does social pressure influencecreativity in children and adults?Anaëlle Camarda¹, Lison Bouhours¹, Olivier Houdé¹, GregoireBorst¹, Mathieu Cassotti¹¹Lapsydé

3-D-126 Neurodevelopmental trajectories of self andsocial evaluation across adolescenceDanielle Cosme¹, John Flournoy¹, Jordan Livingston¹, JohnMazziotta², Mirella Dapretto², Jennifer Pfeifer¹¹University of Oregon, ²University of California, Los Angeles

3-D-127 Children’s emotion regulation abilitiespredict functional connectivity and cognitive controlKatherine Duberg¹, Stacie Warren¹, Shaozheng Qin¹, WeidongCai¹, Aarthi Padmanabhan¹, Rachel Rehert¹, Sarah-NicoleBostan¹, Alejandro Nunez¹, Olivia Altamirano¹, Jannet Lara¹,Victor Carrion¹, Vinod Menon¹¹Stanford School of Medicine

3-D-128 Emotional Go/No-Go in the field: Applying alab-based standard measure of emotional regulationto a high-poverty adolescent populationDeanna Ibrahim¹, Meriah DeJoseph¹, Cybele Raver¹¹New York University

3-D-129 Increased sensitivity to negative stimuli inadolescent rhesus macaquesLauren Murphy¹, Virginia Wertman¹, Andrew Kazama¹,Jocelyne Bachevalier¹¹Emory University

3-D-130 Whole-brain structural connectivity relatesto intrinsic motivation in children born extremelypretermLeona Pascoe¹, Deanne Thompson², Megan Spencer-Smith³,Richard Beare², Chris Adamson², Katherine Lee², ClaireArmstrong-Kelly², Nellie Georgiou-Karistianis³, Chiara Nosarti4,Elisha Josev², Gehan Roberts5, Lex Doyle6, Marc Seal², PeterAnderson¹¹Monash University and Murdoch Childrens Research Institute,²Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, ³Monash University,4Kings College London, 5Murdoch Childrens Research Instituteand Royal Children’s Hospital, 6Royal Womens Hospital andMurdoch Children

3-D-132 Nucleus accumbens activation relates tomarginalized individuals response to 2016 USPresidential electionSarah Tashjian¹, Adriana Galván¹¹University of California, Los Angeles

3-F-133 Mapping non-response to math intervention:A resting state MRI study of first gradersBen Clarke¹, Lina Shanley¹, Jolinda Smith¹, HyeonJin Yoon¹,Marah Sutherland¹, Jessica Turtura¹, Fred Sabb¹¹University of Oregon

3-F-134 Twice as nice: Learning benefits from valenceand action in adolescenceJuliet Davidow¹, Catherine Insel¹, Marilyn Romero, JoanZhang¹, Leah Somerville¹¹Harvard University

3-F-135 Neural markers for reading gains in childrenwith reading difficultiesTehila Nugiel¹, Mary Abbe Roe¹, W. Patrick Taylor², JackFletcher², Jenifer Juranek³, Jessica Church¹¹The University of Texas at Austin, ²University of Houston,³The University of Texas at Houston

3-F-136 Mindfulness intervention is associated withworking memory training gains in school-agedchildrenRachel Jessica Steiner¹, Sarah Short¹, Jeanette Mumford¹¹Center for Healthy Minds

3-G-137 Ecologically valid neural predictors of socialanxiety disorder in youthsTessa Clarkson¹, Hung-Wei Chen¹, Megan Quarmley¹, GregHajcak¹, Johanna Jarcho¹¹Stony Brook University

3-G-138 Mechanisms of stressor controllabilityacross human development: A novel developmentally-informed paradigmEmily Cohodes¹, Jeffrey Mandell¹, Catherine Hartley², DylanGee¹¹Yale University, ²New York University

3-G-139 Developmental changes in resting-statefunctional connectivity in borderline personalitydisorder: A network analysis approachNathan Hall¹, Michael Hallquist¹¹Pennsylvania State University

3-G-140 Longitudinal associations of childhoodexecutive function deficits, resting state functionalconnectivity, and ADHD/MDD symptoms acrossschool ageElizabeth Hawkey¹, Joan Luby¹, Deanna Barch¹¹Washington University in St. Louis

3-G-141 Child trauma disrupts hippocampus-dependent associative learning in the presence ofthreatHilary Lambert¹, Matthew Peverill¹, Kelly Sambrook¹, MargaretSheridan², Katie Askren¹, Katie McLaughlin¹¹University of Washington, ²The University of North Carolina

Page 44: 5th Annual Flux Congress

42 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

3-G-142 Childhood adversity and prefrontal-amygdala functional connectivity during emotionregulation: Specificity to abuse but not otheradversitiesMatthew Peverill¹, Margaret Sheridan², Katie McLaughlin¹¹University of Washington, ²University of North Carolina atChapel Hill

3-G-143 Neural correlates of peer victimization inyouth at highest risk for social anxiety conferresilience for development of social anxietysymptoms 2-years laterMegan Quarmley¹, Eric Nelson, Nathan Fox, Ashley Smith,Hannah Grossman, Daniel Pine, Johanna Jarcho¹¹Stony Brook University

3-G-144 The effect of temporal lobe epilepsy onemotion recognition in childrenJoseph Venticinque¹, Stanley Singer, Jr.¹, Whitney Mattson¹,Eric Nelson¹¹Nationwide Children’s Hospital

3-G-145 Abnormal frontoparietal praxis networkconnectivity in children with autismNicholas Wymbs¹, Stewart Mostofsky¹¹Kennedy Krieger Institute

3-G-146 Externalizing behavior problems impact taskperformance across executive function domains in adevelopmental sampleAnnie Zheng¹, Mackenzie Mitchell¹, Tiffany Wang¹, MatthewLarsen¹, Jessica Church¹¹The University of Texas at Austin

A D I V I S I O N O F

Visit us online at www.podiumconferences.com

ConferenceManagement, Planningand DeliveryFrom idea conception throughto conference delivery andthe post review stage, weare here to help you deliveran outstanding conference.

Website Design,Hosting & MaintenanceOur conference websiteservice engages with yourconference to focus oncreating a comprehensiveand specialized websitedesigned to describe,promote and generateconference leads and reachyour audience.

Online Registration,Abstract, Membershipand Exhibit BoothManagementWith our range of conferencetools, you will be able to findefficient solutions to thatsave precious administrativeand volunteer hours, freeingup your staff to focus onother critical areas.

AssociationManagementWe provide your associationwith the ability to focus oncore responsibilities and rolesrelated to organizationalgrowth while a system is inplace to manage the dailyaffairs and activities.

To learn more about us or to secure our services for your conference or organization, please call 1 800 472-7644 or e-mail us: [email protected].

We specialize in planning, organizing anddelivering exceptional international scientific,academic and research conferences.

Podium offerseffective andefficient conferencemanagementsolutions through arange of conferencetools, conferenceplanning, andconferencemarketing services.

Page 45: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 43

Buffe

tLu

nch

Coffe

eCoff

ee

1

23

1-A-1

1-A-2

1-A-4

1-A-3

1-A-5

1-A-6

1-A-8

1-A-7

1-O-15

4

1-O-15

5

1-A-10

1-A-11

1-B-13

1-B-12

1-B-14

1-C-17

1-C-20

1-C-18

1-O-15

3

1-O-15

2

1-O-15

7

1-O-15

6

1-O-15

0

1-O-15

1 1-O

-149

1-O-14

8 1-G

-49

1-G-50

1-G-43

1-G-42

1-G-40

1-F-41

1-G-48

1-G-46

1-G-44

1-G-45

1-F-39

1-F-38

1-D-37

1-F-35

1-D

-34 1

-D-33

1-D-31

1-D

-32

1-D-30

1-D

-29

1-D-27

1-D

-28

1-C-24

1-C

-23

1-C-21

1-C

-22

Flux Congress Posters Floor Plan

|Saturday, September 16

Atrium Room, Portland Hilton

Page 46: 5th Annual Flux Congress

44 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Buffe

tLu

nch

Coffe

eCoff

ee

1

23

2-A-51

1-A-52

2-A-54

2-A-53

2-A-55

2-A-56

2-A-58

2-A-57

2-G-97

2-G-96

2-B-59

2-B-60

2-B-62

2-B-61

2-B-64

2-B-65

2-C-67

2-C-66

2-G-94

2-G-95

2-G-98

2-O-15

8

2-O-16

0

2-O-15

9

2-O-16

1

2-G-93

2-G-92

2-G-90

2-G-91

2-G-89

2-G-88

2-F-86

2-F-87

2-F-85

2-F-84

2-D-82

2-D-83

2-D-81

2-D-80

2-D

-79 2

-D-78

2-D-76

2-D

-77

2-C-71

2-C

-70

2-C-68

2-C

-69

2-D-75

2-D

-74

2-C-72

2-C

-73

Flux Congress Posters Floor Plan

|Sunday, September 17

Atrium Room, Portland Hilton

Page 47: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 45

Buffe

tLu

nch

Coffe

eCoff

ee

13-A

-99

3-A-10

0 3-A

-102

3-A-10

1 3-A

-103

3-A-10

4 3-A

-106

3-A-10

5

3-D-14

4

3-D-14

3

3-O-16

5

3-O-16

4

3-D-14

5

3-O-14

6

3-B-10

7

3-B-10

8 3-B

-110

3-B-10

9 3-B

-112

3-B-11

3 3-C

-115

3-C-11

4

3-D-14

1

3-D-14

2

3-O-16

6

3-O-16

7

3-D-14

0

3-D-13

9 3-D

-137

3-D-13

8

3-D-13

6

3-D-13

5 3-D

-133

3-D-13

4

3-D-13

2

3-D-13

0 3-D

-128

3-D-12

9

3-C-11

9 3-

C-118

3-C-11

6 3-

C-117

3-D-12

0 3-

D-12

1

3-C-12

0 3-

C-121

3-C-12

3 3-

C-122

3-D-12

7 3-

D-12

6

23

Flux Congress Posters Floor Plan

|Monday, September 18

Atrium Room, Portland Hilton

Page 48: 5th Annual Flux Congress

46 | FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

Flux Congress Sponsors

Jacobs Foundationwww.jacobsfoundation.org/

The Jacobs Foundation supportsresearch and intervention projectsleading to significant outcomes forchildren and youth all over the world.Within our research priority Science ofLearning, we explore the biologicalbases of skill acquisition anddevelopment of children and youth andtheir consequences for learningenvironments and institutions.

Oregon Health & ScienceUniversitywww.ohsu.edu/xd/research/

OHSU Behavioral NeuroscienceDepartment & Office of the Senior VicePresident for Research - TheDepartment of Behavioral Neuroscienceis a basic science departmentperforming cutting edge research andteaching focused on behavioralpharmacology, genetics, and cognitiveneuroscience. As the state’s onlyacademic health center, OHSU’sbreakthrough research leads to newcures, new standards of care, and abetter understanding of the basicscience that drives biomedicaldiscovery. OHSU researchers areexploring new basic, clinical, andapplied research frontiers.

University of Oregon VicePresident for Research andInnovation & the Robert andBeverly Lewis Center forNeuroimagingwww.research.uoregon.edu/news/around-campus/lewis-integrative-science-building-fact-sheet

The Office of the Vice President forResearch and Innovation promotesexcellence in research at the Universityof Oregon. Research, both basic andapplied, is fundamental to the missionof the University and is essential toOregon’s economic and civic vitality.

The office supports interdisciplinaryresearch centers including the Centeron Teaching and Learning and thePrevention Science Institute as well asessential core facilities including theRobert and Beverly Lewis Center forNeuroimaging and emboldensinnovation and economic development

through strategic partnering andtechnology transfer initiatives.

The UO Robert and Beverly LewisCenter for Neuroimaging is a corefacility supporting a wide range ofinterdisciplinary, multifaceted researchin neuroscience and biological imaging.We strive to provide world-classequipment and service in support offaculty and student research.

Elsevierwww.journals.elsevier.com/developmental-cognitive-neuroscience/

Just as the tools used in scientificresearch are changing, so too are thetools used in scientific communication.Elsevier has taken a leadership role inadvancing the technologies necessaryto create a seamless electronicinformation delivery environment.

University of Oregon Centerfor Teaching and Learninghttps://ctl.uoregon.edu/

The Center on Teaching and Learning(CTL) is one of the largest researchcenters at the University of Oregon andis part of the College of Education(COE). The COE is one of the highest-ranking graduate schools of educationin the country. In the last decade, CTLhas contributed significantly to thisranking due to its largely successfulresearch enterprise.

CTL’s mission is to conduct, translate,and disseminate research that focuseson the solutions and resolutions toserious but practical problems inschool systems. CTL’s current researchfocuses on the rigorous evaluation ofinstructional strategies and materials inthe teaching and assessment of readingand mathematics skills in K-8. CTL’scurrent portfolio of research projects isavailable on the CTL website(http://ctl.uoregon.edu/research/projects).

CTL also provides screening andreporting services to schools throughthe DIBELS Data System (DDS)(https://dibels.uoregon.edu/). The DDSserves millions of students in publicand private schools across the US andinternationally. CTL also activelydevelops instructional programs andeducational technology games to

enhance student learning across arange of academic subjects. Wedisseminate rigorously researchedproducts through the CTL Marketplace(https://dibels.uoregon.edu/market/).

University of OregonPrevention Science Institutewww.psi.uoregon.edu/

The Prevention Science Institute (PSI)at the University of Oregon is a multi-disciplinary institute focused onunderstanding human development,preventing behavioral health problems,and implementing effectiveinterventions in community settings.The core mission of the PSI is toimprove the lives and wellbeing of at-risk children, individuals, and familiesthroughout the lifespan. The PSI is aresearch institute designed forcollaboration between faculty acrossdisciplines, including psychology, socialand affective neuroscience,development, education, and otherswho are interested in prevention. Ourwork is conducted in partnership withcommunity collaborators in Oregon,across the US, and internationally,including child welfare serviceproviders, school district leaders,mental health providers, criminal justicesystem leaders, and policy makers. ThePSI encompasses three major focusareas consistent with the field ofprevention science, includingtranslational neuroscience, preventionand intervention, and implementationscience. Understanding effectiveintervention strategies and theoutcomes associated with theseinterventions across multiple domainsof functioning, including biological,social, and contextual, is the focus ofresearch at the PSI. The PSI seeks tocollaborate with local, state, national,and international organizations andresearchers to understand and promotehealthy adaptation in children andfamilies. The PSI is particularlyinterested in working with communitieson dissemination of effectiveinterventions to real world settings. ThePSI is committed to research thatexpands our understanding ofinterventions and development withdiverse populations, and actively seeksto promote research that reduceshealth disparities in service utilization.

Page 49: 5th Annual Flux Congress

FLUX: THE SOCIETY FOR DEVELOPMENTAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE | 47

Flux Congress Exhibitors

Electrical Geodesics. Inc. (EGI)www.egi.com

EGI’s dense array EEG with 32, 64, 128, or 256 channelsprovides EEG data with whole-head coverage and thehighest spatial resolution available. EGI also offersGeoSource 3 advanced electrical source imaging softwareand our new GTEN 100 electrical neuromodulation system,which both take advantage of high-definition EEG data.EGI?s complete Geodesic EEG Systems include theGeodesic Sensor Net for fast electrode application andoptimal comfort, amplifiers for up to 256 channels, andsoftware for acquisition, review, and analysis. Systemssupport multimodal imaging with MRI, MEG, TMS, andNIRS. Stop by the EGI booth for a demo!

Framewise Integrated Real-Time MRIMonitoringwww.firmm.us

Got motion? Get FIRMM! Easy to set up and use, theFramewise Integrated Real-Time MRI Monitoring (FIRMM)software suite provides MRI scanner operators with dataquality metrics in real time. Using FIRMM to identify theideal scan time for each person can reduce total brain MRIscan times and associated costs by 50% or more. FIRMMcalculates head motion on MRI data as they’re beingacquired and requires no additional hardware. It’scompletely free, we just want FLUXers to use it. Jointlydeveloped at OHSU & WashU. Go to www.firmm.us formore information.

University of OregonSpecial Education andClinical Scienceswww.education.uoregon.edu/department/special-education-and-clinical-sciencesThrough teaching, research, andservice, SPECS seeks to improve thequality of education, employment, andcommunity living for children andadults with special needs and theirfamilies. The department has threegraduate majors: communicationdisorders and sciences, schoolpsychology, and special education.The department also offers anundergraduate degree incommunication disorders andsciences and a minor in specialeducation. Programs in theDepartment of Special Education andClinical Sciences improve the qualityand outcomes of education,employment, and community livingfor children and adults with special

needs and their families throughteaching, research, and service.SPECS faculty actively recruits andsupports opportunities for graduatestudents of all ethnicities, abilities, andorientations.

University of OregonDepartment of Psychologywww.psychology.uoregon.edu/

Nestled in the lush Willamette Valley,with an easy drive to both the PacificOcean and the Cascade Mountains,the University of Oregon is renownedfor its research prowess andcommitment to teaching. The UO isone of just two schools in the PacificNorthwest selected for membership inthe prestigious Association ofAmerican Universities, with an annualportfolio of over $115m in competitiveresearch awards and a long history ofinterdisciplinary research acrossseven UO colleges.

The department is co-located in twobuildings. Much of our researchoccurs in the Lewis IntegrativeScience Building: state-of-the-artfacilities with faculty labs, a research-dedicated 3T fMRI scanner, and TMSfacilities. After a two year $44mrenovation, Straub Hall offersadditional research labs and isdepartment home to faculty andstudents.

We are committed to diversity andinclusivity. The PsychologyDepartment hosts an active andinnovative Committee for an InclusiveCommunity (CIC) composed ofgraduate students, faculty and staffinvested in the shared interest of adiverse and inclusive environmentwithin the department.

Check out our news and events onFacebook at “uopsychology” & Twitterat “UOPsych”.

Flux Congress Sponsors

Page 50: 5th Annual Flux Congress

Thank you to our Sponsors

Thank you to our Exhibitors