the effects of existential salience on state anxiety

55
Eastern Illinois University e Keep Masters eses Student eses & Publications 2014 e Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety Joseph Morger Eastern Illinois University is research is a product of the graduate program in Clinical Psychology at Eastern Illinois University. Find out more about the program. is is brought to you for free and open access by the Student eses & Publications at e Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters eses by an authorized administrator of e Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Morger, Joseph, "e Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety" (2014). Masters eses. 1294. hps://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/1294

Upload: others

Post on 15-Oct-2021

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

Eastern Illinois UniversityThe Keep

Masters Theses Student Theses & Publications

2014

The Effects of Existential Salience on State AnxietyJoseph MorgerEastern Illinois UniversityThis research is a product of the graduate program in Clinical Psychology at Eastern Illinois University. Findout more about the program.

This is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Theses & Publications at The Keep. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Thesesby an authorized administrator of The Keep. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Recommended CitationMorger, Joseph, "The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety" (2014). Masters Theses. 1294.https://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/1294

Page 2: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

The Graduate School~ EASTERN l!JJNOlS UNIVERSITY M

Thesis Maintenance and Reproduction Certificate

FOR: Graduate Candidates Completing Theses in Partial Fulfillment of the Degree Graduate Faculty Advisors Directing the Theses

RE: Preservation, Reproduction, and Distribution of Thesis Research

Preserving, reproducing, and distributing thesis research is an important part of Booth Library's responsibility to provide access to scholarship. In order to further this goal, Booth Library makes all graduate theses completed as part of a degree program at Eastern Illinois University available for personal study, research, and other not-for-profit educational purposes. Under 17 U.S.C. § 108, the library may reproduce and distribute a copy without infringing on copyright; however, professional courtesy dictates that permission be requested from the author before doing so.

Your signatures affirm the following: • The graduate candidate is the author of this thesis. • The graduate candidate retains the copyright and intellectual property rights associated with the

original research, creative activity, and intellectual or artistic content of the thesis. • The graduate candidate certifies her/his compliance with federal copyright law (Title 17 of the U.

S. Code) and her/his right to authorize reproduction and distribution of all copyrighted materials included in this thesis.

• The graduate candidate in consultation with the faculty advisor grants Booth Library the non­exclusive, perpetual right to make copies of the thesis freely and publicly available without restriction, by means of any current or successive technology, including by not limited to photocopying, microfilm, digitization, or internet.

• The graduate candidate acknowledges that by depositing her/his thesis with Booth Library, her/his work is available for viewing by the public and may be borrowed through the library's circulation and interlibrary loan departments, or accessed electronically.

• The graduate candidate waives the confidentiality provisions of the Family Educational Rights and Privacy Act (FERPA) (20 U.S. C. § 1232g; 34 CFR Part 99) with respect to the contents of the thesis and with respect to information concerning authorship of the thesis, including name and status as a student at Eastern Illinois University.

I have conferred with my graduate faculty advisor. My signature below indicates that I have read and agree with the above statements, and hereby give my permission to allow Booth Library to reproduce and distribute my thesis. My adviser's signature indicates concurrence to reproduce and distribute the thesis.

Faculty Adviser Signature

«.. \l SS' li.L l. GR_u..C> £1( Printed Name

10 - 'f- J ~ Date

Please su hmit in duplicate.

Page 3: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

THE EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE ON STATE ANXIETY

BY

Joseph Morger

THESIS

SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

Master of Arts in Clinical Psychology

IN THE GRADUATE SCHOOL, EASTERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY CHARLESTON, ILLINOIS

2014 YEAR

I HEREBY RECOMMEND THIS THESIS BE ACCEPTED AS FULFILLING THIS PART OF THE GRADUATE DEGREE CITED ABOVE

lO-C!\-\\.} THESIS COMMITTEE CHAIR DATE

~ ~ .. /'l ~---,;;;----;--- --

DATE THESIS COMMITTEE MEMBER DATE

ttJ 1 !( MEMBER DATE THESIS COMMITTEE MEMBER DA TE

Page 4: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

Running head: EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL ANXIETY

The Effect of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

Joseph E. Morger, B.A.

Eastern Illinois University

Page 5: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 2

Abstract

Existential anxiety is theorized to result from one's knowledge of human

existence. Four givens oflife have been identified that result in existential anxiety: death,

freedom, isolation, and meaninglessness (Yalom, 1980). Terror management theory

purposes the knowledge of one's own death to be a great source of distress. The theory

posits that to cope with these distressing thoughts, one is motivated to adhere to a cultural

worldview. Adhering to a cultural worldview allows one to view the world as a

structured place, in addition to raising one's self-esteem. Terror management research

has shown that making mortality salient results in harsher criticism against those who

oppose one's worldview, increased positive regard for those who uphold a similar

worldview, as well as influence over various behaviors. Uncertainty management theory

suggests that the distress resulting from acknowledging one's own mortality is due to the

uncertainty that surrounds death. Research has provided mixed results as to whether

mortality or uncertainty salience produces larger effects. The current study examined the

effect of both mortality and uncertainty salience on state anxiety levels compared to a

control condition. Results indicated that both uncertainty and mortality salience yielded

higher levels of state anxiety compared to the control condition. Additionally a

significant main effect of self-esteem was found, such that higher self-esteem resulted in

lower levels of state anxiety, regardless of salience condition. Results demonstrate the

relevance of existential concerns in the young target population's lives.

Page 6: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 3

Table of Contents

Title Page. . . . . . ........................................................ 1

Abstract ............................................................... 2

Table of Contents. . . . . . .................................................. 3

List of Tables. . . . . . . .................................................... 5

List of Figures. . . . . ...................................................... 6

Introduction. . . . . . . . ..................................................... 7

Death ............................................................ 10

Terror Management Theory ......................................... 13

Freedom and Uncertainty ............................................ 19

Uncertainty Management Theory ..................................... 23

Present Study ..................................................... 28

Methods ................................................................ 29

Participants ...................................................... 29

Materials ........................................................ 29

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. . . . . ............................. 29

Experimental Conditions. . . . . . . . . . . .......................... 30

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. . . . . . . .......................... .30

Results ................................................................ 31

Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ............................................ 3 2

References .............................................................. 36

Appendix A: Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale ................................... 4 7

Appendix B: Mortality Salience Prime ....................................... 48

Page 7: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 4

Appendix C: Uncertainty Salience Prime ..................................... 49

Appendix D: Control Manipulation .......................................... 50

Appendix E: Word Search Delay Task. ...................................... 51

Appendix E: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Form-Y ............................. 52

Page 8: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 5

List of Tables

Table 1: State anxiety scores for mortality salience, uncertainty salience, and control

conditions .............................................................. 44

Table 2: Multiple regression analysis for variables predicting state anxiety score. . . .. 45

Page 9: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 6

List of Figures

Figure 1: Predicted state anxiety scores for mortality, uncertainty, and control conditions

for low self-esteem (-1 standard deviation) and high self-esteem(+ 1 standard deviation) .

. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46

Page 10: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 7

The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

Anxiety, an emotion that is evident to some degree in all human beings, is

characterized by tension, worry, and marked by physical changes, such as increased heart

rate, rapid breathing, increased perspiration, and dilation of the pupils (American

Psychological Association, 2014). Evolutionary psychologists argue that anxiety arose

out of the need to escape threats in one's environment. A potential threat would activate

changes in an individual's autonomic nervous system, specifically one's fight-or-flight

response. In the 21st century, individuals are faced with less tangible threats. Barlow

(2002) uses this differentiation of tangible and less tangible threats to differentiate fear

from anxiety. Others view the distinction between anxiety and fear to be a matter of the

duration of the reaction. For example, a fear response would be short lived, while anxiety

persists after the stimulus is no longer present (Hartley & Phelps, 2012). Wicker, Payne,

Roberson, and Garcia-Falconi (1985) surveyed nonclinical populations and found that

when compared to fear, anxiety was more future-oriented, lasted longer, and was

associated with higher temporal uncertainty.

In addition to the physiological changes that anxiety produces, there is also a

major impact on one's cognitive processes. The presence of anxiety has been shown to

decrease reaction times when individuals are instructed to identify a threatening stimulus.

This decreased response time would have advantages when one is faced with a threat to

one's self. Studies have also shown that anxiety interferes with an individual's ability to

learn, process information and regulate emotions (Hartley & Phelps, 2012). An example

of how anxiety interferes with an individual's ability to process information was

illustrated with a modification of the Stroop Task. In the original Stroop Task,

Page 11: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 8

participants were presented with a list of the names of colors, displayed in either a

consistent, or inconsistent ink color. For example, the word blue could be presented in

blue ink, or orange ink. Individuals were asked to state out loud the color of the ink and

ignore the actual word. Participants took a longer amount of time to respond to

inconsistently paired color word and ink color stimuli. Researchers attributed the longer

response times to a longer time spent processing the information (Williams, Mathews, &

MacLeod, 1996). In a modified version of the Stroop Task, individuals were presented

with a list of words that were either anxiety-related or neutral. Each word was presented

in different color ink. Individuals who had been diagnosed with an anxiety disorder were

found to take a longer amount of time stating the color of the words that were anxiety­

related. Similar results have also been found in regard to socially related words for

individuals with social phobia and trauma related words for individuals who have been

diagnosed with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (Williams et al., 1996). Researchers

speculate that this time response differential represents a longer period of time focusing

on the content of the word (Barlow, 2002).

Anxious individuals have also been shown to have a tendency to interpret neutral

stimuli as negative, or threatening. Examples include the rating of ambiguous facial

expressions as being more threatening, interpreting scenarios that could be interpreted in

various ways as being more negative, and being more likely to have negative judgments

of ambiguous future life events (Barlow, 2002; Hartley & Phelps, 2012).

Anxiety disorders occur when one's inability to control their anxious response

results in significant distress and impairment. Anxiety disorders are estimated to affect

around 18% of American adults above the age of 18. The median age of onset for

Page 12: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

anxiety disorders is in the mid-twenties, and has been tied to a variety of problems that

lead to a lower quality of life. Aspects of one's life that are negatively affected by the

presence of an anxiety disorder include interpersonal difficulties, lower educational

achievement, and various medical problems. One study, conducted in the 1990's,

estimated that the annual cost of anxiety disorders in the United States was $42 billion

(Kessler, Rusico, Shear, & Wittchen, 2009). In an online Harris poll, which was

conducted on behalf of the APA (2014), 34% of adolescents, age 13 to 17 who were

polled, believed that their stress levels would increase within the next year.

9

Existential psychology, rooted in philosophy, views anxiety as a logical response

to human existence, in addition to the knowledge that one has about the world. Frederick

Nietzsche and Soren Kierkegaard are often referred to as the predecessors of existential

thought. Martin Heidegger, who was influenced by Kierkegaard's work and Edmund

Husserl's phenomenology, introduced many topics of existential philosophy in his book

Being and Time. In Being and Time, Heidegger introduced the concept ofDasein, or

Being-in-the-world. (Crowell, 2010). Ludwig Binswanger and Medard Boss, two

psychiatrists, drew inspiration from both Heidegger and Husserl and developed

daseinanalysis (Smith, 2010). Binswanger and Boss are often viewed as bridging the gap

between existential philosophy and psychotherapy (Marshall, 1989).

Very often existential psychologists and philosophers viewed a similar, yet

different, part of existence to be the cause of anxiety. Paul Tillich, a theologian, saw

knowledge of one's own death, thoughts that life is meaningless, and the idea that one

has not lived up to one's moral code to be sources of anxiety (Weems, Costa, Dehon, &

Berman, 2004). Rollo May, a psychologist who was highly influenced by Kierkegaard,

Page 13: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 10

focused on one's freedom, or the ability to choose within life's natural and self-imposed

limits, as being the source of existential anxiety. In May's theory the actual ability to

choose, in and of itself, is not the cause of anxiety; instead it is the knowledge that one is

responsible for one's actions and decisions (Schneider & Krug, 2010). Irvin Y al om

(1980) defines existential anxiety as "a conflict that flows from the individual's

confrontation with the givens of existence" (p. 1 ); these givens being death, freedom,

meaninglessness, and isolation. Yalom (1980) notes that these givens are separate

concepts, but are closely related and interdependent. The impact that one of the givens

has on an individual's life would be extremely different if one of the four did not exist.

The current study will focus on the givens of death and freedom.

Death

Y al om (1980) cites death as a major influence on one's psychological health. The

finality of life can have many different effects on an individual's life. One could respond

by experiencing death anxiety, which manifests in avoidance of situations in which one

comes into contact with reminders of death (Kastenbaum, 2000). Death can also act as a

catalyst, moving an individual to lead a more goal-oriented and "meaningful" life.

Posttraumatic growth theory posits that facing one's own mortality can lead to positive

changes in individuals, such as a more profound appreciation of life and new life

priorities (Lykins, Segerstrom, Averill, Evans, & Kemeny, 2007). Death can have a wide

variety of meanings in society. Death, as a socially constructed concept, has the

possibility to elicit different reactions from different people. How a person dies has an

impact on how the person is viewed. For example, people speak of a "coward's death'',

Page 14: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

or people being heroic if they "stand up" to death. Death is viewed as unfortunate for

some, and a task to be completed by others (Kastenbaum, 2000).

11

Death anxiety results from the existential concerns that one has about their own

death or the death of another. Studies have shown that death anxiety is relatively stable

over time; however, older individuals tend to report lower death anxiety when compared

those who are younger (Sliter, Sinclair, Yuan, & Mohr, 2014). Studies have also found

that a fear of death is common in non-clinical populations. Higher levels of education

and socioeconomic status were related to lower levels of death anxiety. Surprisingly,

higher levels of religiosity and participation in religious practices were not related to

lower levels of death anxiety (Furer & Walker, 2008). Some researchers claim that

attitudes toward death are multidimensional. Florian and Kravetz (1983) developed a

scale to measure anxiety related to one's own death titled Fear of Personal Death Scale.

Using factor analysis, researchers found 6 factors, loss of social identity, consequences to

family and friends, transcendental consequences, self-annihilation, and punishment and

the hereafter, to be associated with individual's fear of personal death (Florian &

Kravetz, 1983). Significant life events, such as the early or recent loss of a family

member, have been shown to impact these 6 factors differently (Florian & Mikulincer,

1997).

There is a large difference in reactions to death in general, and one's own death.

Denial of one's own death is not uncommon (Yalom, 1980). Yalom (1980) refers to a

number of ways that people avoid thinking about their own death. Denial can occur either

through the belief that one is special, belief in life after death, or living symbolically after

death by leaving behind a legacy. The belief that one is special plays an important part in

Page 15: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 12

how one views death. Everyone knows that other people die, but often have a very

difficult time accepting the concept of their own mortality. People have been known to

adapt an "It won't happen to me" attitude toward the possibility of their own death. They

engage in reckless behavior, potentially fatal activities, knowing that there is potential

death, yet at the same time denying that this could really happen to them (Y alom, 1980).

Often, the incomprehensible aspect of one's own death is concept of one's

nonexistence. When we die the world will go on, yet we will not exist anymore (Y alom,

1980). Y alom (1980) cites cases in which many people cannot comprehend the fact that

the world will exist, but they will not. People feel angry that others will get to experience

the world, and they will be no more. The truth of the matter remains that compared to the

scope of time, one is only alive for a very small duration. To deal with the existential

anxiety that death brings about, people focus on "getting ahead, achieving, accumulating

material wealth, leaving works behind as imperishable monuments ... which effectively

conceals the mortal questions churning below." (Yalom, 1980 p. 121).

Ernest Becker (1973) viewed death, specifically the fear of one's own death, to be

the reason the motivation behind all human activity. Becker (1973) pointed out that

humans possess a biological drive to live, along with the knowledge that they would

ultimately die. This paradox causes the organism to experience "terror" (p. 15). In line

with Yalom's theory discussed above, Becker believed that people sought to alleviate this

terror brought on by death by adhering to a cultural worldview; one which allowed an

individual to make sense of the world as a structured place (Kastenbaum, 2000).

Adherence to this worldview serves as a buffer against the thoughts of one's own death,

and the subsequent anxiety that these thoughts would cause. (Becker, 1973). Adhering to

Page 16: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

a specific cultural worldview bolsters one's self-esteem. Due to a cultural worldview

only offering an individual a sense of protection from the terrors of death to those who

meet or live up to the individual's cultural standards, one is motivated to defend their

own worldview, while discounting those who oppose it (Rosenblatt, Greenberg,

Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Lyon, 1989).

Terror Management Theory

13

Terror management theory is based on Ernest Becker's theory that humans are

uniquely aware that they are going to die. This knowledge separates humans from the

rest of the animal kingdom, and in tum, causes humans a great deal of anxiety. There is a

biological drive to live, but humans know that ultimately they will die (Pyszczynski,

Greenberg, Solomon, & Maxfield, 2006). Terror management theory states that this

knowledge has a major impact on the way that human beings go about living their lives.

In order to deal with these thoughts of death, humans have developed a cultural anxiety

buffer, most notably comprised of things like cultural worldview and self-esteem. In line

with Becker's previously mentioned theory, Terror management theory posits that the

cultural anxiety buffer works to keep these thoughts of one's own death out of conscious

thought (Solomon, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski, 2004). An individual's cultural

worldview provides a framework for the world to be a structured place, free from anxiety

related to one's impending fatality. (Rosenblatt et al., 1989).

One's cultural worldview is socially constructed, and by nature, susceptible to

threats from new information. People often rely on a social consensus to justify their

beliefs in a cultural worldview. People can be exposed to a large number of individuals

who endorse a vast array of lifestyles on any given day. Each exposure to an individual

Page 17: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 14

with a lifestyle that differs from one's own is a potential threat to one's own worldview.

Based upon this idea and consistent with Becker's theory, individuals would then be

motivated to defend their own worldview and criticize cultural worldviews that opposed

their own harshly. Holding this position that one's worldview is correct and other's are

wrong allows an individual to view himself or herself as a valuable member of a

structured and meaningful world, instead of a vulnerable mortal who could perish at any

moment (Rosenblatt et al., 1989). Terror management research provided the first

empirical evidence that fear of one's own mortality could affect a wide variety of

behaviors (Pyszczynski et al., 2006).

Early experiments on Terror Management Theory were accomplished by inducing

mortality salience (Solomon et al., 2004). Mortality salience is defined as the recognition

and realization of one's own mortality (Greenberg et al., 1990). Individuals were told

that they were participating in research that compared personality styles and interpersonal

judgments and randomly assigned to either mortality salience or control conditions

(Solomon et al., 2004). When assigned to a mortality salience condition, participants

were asked to answer the following questions: "describe the emotions that the thought of

their own death arouses in them" and "imagine what will happen to them physically as

they die" (Hayes, Schimel, Arndt, & Faucher, 2010, p. 701). Research has shown that

participants in mortality salience conditions judged people who violated the participant's

cultural norms more harshly and those who endorsed the participant's cultural norms

more positively (Rosenblatt et al., 1989; Greenberg et al., 1990). For example,

Rosenblatt and colleagues (1989) found that municipal court judges who had been

exposed to a mortality salience prime set higher bail for alleged prostitutes compared to

Page 18: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 15

those exposed to a control condition. Another experiment in the same study provided

evidence that mortality salience would lead to more favorable judgments of those who

upheld one's cultural worldview. This was evidenced by participants giving a higher

reward to an individual who assisted in apprehending of a criminal. The harsh criticism

was specific to those who violated the cultural norms, and not generalized ratings of other

individuals, including the experimenter. Researchers attributed this criticism to be the

result of negative reactions to violations of their own cultural worldview. Likewise, the

positive reaction was a result of a person upholding their cultural worldview (Rosenblatt

et al., 1989).

More recent research has examined the effect of mortality salience on worldview,

in bicultural individuals. Evidence has been shown that individuals who were exposed to

a mortality salience prime renounced their worldview when they were made to believe

that such a worldview would reflect poorly on the self. Bicultural immigrants who read

an article identifying the incompatibility of their two cultures, were less likely to endorse

their ethnic identify, compared to those who read an article identifying the compatibility

of their two cultures (Gonclaves Portelinha, Verlhiac, Meyer, & Hutchison, 2012).

Research has shown that mortality salience can be primed in a wide variety of

ways. Examples of mortality salience priming include having the participant watch

automobile accident footage, conducting the experiment near a funeral home, and

subliminally priming for thoughts of death (Solomon et al., 2004). To answer the

question of whether or not the effects were the cause of an adverse stimulus, or specific

to thoughts of one's own death, researchers compared mortality salience to a wide variety

of adverse situations including public speaking, worries after college, failure, becoming

Page 19: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 16

paralyzed as a result of a car crash, or physical pain. These adverse stimulus conditions

failed to produce the same effects of mortality salience (Greenberg, Solomon, &

Pyszczynski, 1997).

Terror management theory views self-esteem as another existential buffer. Self­

esteem is the belief that one is not just an animal that is going to die, but will have a sense

of purpose or value to the world. As was stated above, this is accomplished through

adherence to a cultural worldview (Routledge et al., 2010). A belief that one has a

purpose or a sense of meaning acts as an anxiety buffer against death-thoughts. Research

has shown individuals with higher self-esteem, or who were able to bolster their self­

esteem through false positive feedback, were less likely to make harsh judgments of those

who violated cultural norms after being primed with mortality salience (Solomon et al.,

2004). Individuals were also shown to display increased behavior that they thought to be

source of self-esteem after being exposed to mortality salience. For example, participants

who viewed driving ability to be relevant to one's self-esteem engaged in more risky

driving behaviors when exposed to a mortality salience condition, compared to those who

did not endorse driving ability as a source of self-esteem (Taubman - Ben-Ari, Floria, &

Mikulincer, 1999). Additionally, individuals were also found to be more likely to believe

information about them that increased their self-esteem, for example, information from

horoscopes, after being exposed to thoughts about their own death (Dechesne et al.,

2003).

While Terror Management Theory has shown that mortality salience induction

can lead to harsher judgments on conflicting cultural worldviews, research has also

explored the cognitive basis for how mortality salience affects one's worldview defenses.

Page 20: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 17

Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Solomon, Simon, and Breus (1994) hypothesized that when

faced with thoughts of one's own death, the mind attempts to suppress these thoughts.

This hypothesis was based upon Wegner's theory of thought suppression. Wegner's

theory of thought suppression states that there is a rebound effect when individuals

attempt to suppress thoughts. Wegner demonstrated this by instructing participants to

verbalize their stream of consciousness while being instructed to either suppress or

express thoughts of a white bear. Participants who were initially told to suppress, then

express thoughts of a white bear more frequently mentioned the white bear as time

passed, as measured by mentions in one-minute intervals. For those who were initially

told to express, and subsequently suppress thoughts of a white bear, mentions of the

white bear decreased over time (Wegner, 1987). The state in which a thought is not

actively conscious, but still continues to influence conscious thoughts and behaviors is

called deep activation (Wegner & Smart, 1997). Terror management researchers have

provided support for the claim that mortality salience affects one's unconscious mental

processes, by providing evidence that one's affect, as measured by the Positive Negative

Affect Scale, is not negatively impacted upon mortality salience induction (Simon et al.,

2007). However, some researchers have disputed this claim, citing improper

measurement of participant's affect (Lambert et al., 2014).

Terror management theory views thoughts of one's own death as needing to be

avoided, or suppressed, in order to buffer the person from the terror that accompanies the

knowledge of one's own death. In theory, after being primed for mortality salience, one

would initially suppress these thoughts, but thoughts of one's own death would become

increasingly accessible as time went on (Greenberg et al., 1994). When an individual

Page 21: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 18

attempts to suppress a thought, the thought is ironically activated, as demonstrated by the

white bear task described above (Wegner, 1987). Attempts to suppress unwanted

thoughts will result in deep activation of these unwanted thoughts (Wegner & Smart,

1997).

Terror management research has supported this hypothesis, in that mortality

salience effects have been shown to be larger after a delay task. A typical delay task

includes requiring the participant to complete a word search puzzle that contains only

neutral words, or reading a passage that is not related to the subject matter. Additionally,

death thoughts have been shown to be more accessible as time passed after the mortality

salience induction (Greenberg et al., 1994).

As was stated earlier, there are a variety of ways in which mortality can be made

salient. Researchers of Terror Management Theory hypothesized that because death

serves as a motivational factor, death thought activation could occur as a result of very

subtle, even unconscious, reminders. To test this hypothesis, participants were

subliminally primed with the words "dead" or "death" or with the control word "pain".

In Terror Management Theory research, the word "pain" is often used as a control to

demonstrate that the effect is a result of mortality salience, and not just a result of an

aversive stimulus. Participants that were exposed to the mortality salience condition,

compared to participants who were exposed to an aversive stimulus condition, were

found to have increased death-thought accessibility (Arndt, Greenberg, Pyszczynski, &

Solomon, 1997). Death thought accessibility was also increased by subliminally having

the person view the letters "WTC" or the numbers "9/11" shortly after the World Trade

Page 22: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 19

Center Bombings, and by interviewing participants in front of a funeral parlor (Landau et

al., 2004).

Terror management research has shown that mortality salience can affect a wide

variety of behaviors, including those of individuals with of anxiety disorders. Anxiety

disorders are thought by many to be unsuccessful attempts to cope with anxiety. Through

the lens of Terror Management Theory, anxiety disorders may be an unsuccessful attempt

to cope with existential fear (Strachan et al., 2007). To test this hypothesis, Strachen and

colleagues (2007) induced mortality salience in participants who had been diagnosed

with an animal phobia and obsessive-compulsive tendencies. Participants who had been

assessed to have a spider phobia displayed increased avoidance and perception of a spider

threat after having mortality salience induced, compared to a control group and non­

phobic participants. Strachen and colleagues (2007) also found that exposing participants

who displayed high and low obsessive-compulsive behaviors to a mortality salience

induction resulted in both groups spending more time washing their hands. Routledge

and colleagues (2010) demonstrated that mortality salience did increase the state anxiety

scores on the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory of participants who were shown to have low

self-esteem as a measured by scores on the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Individuals

who were rated as having higher self-esteem showed no effects as a result of the

induction of mortality salience.

Freedom and Uncertainty

As was mentioned previously, freedom is an existential given oflife. On the

surface, many would think of freedom as a source of psychological well-being. The more

options that one has to choose amongst, the more likely it is that a person will be able to

Page 23: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 20

make an optimal choice (May, 1981 ). Rational Choice Theory identifies the choices that

individuals make as an attempt to optimize the benefits one will receive from that

particular choice (Hemstein, 1990). Through the lens of Rational Choice Theory, more

choices should equal more options and potential for optimal satisfaction. Statistically, the

increase in the availability of choices increases the likelihood that one will be able to

select an optimal choice. Additionally, one could choose not to take advantage of being

able to choose, a freedom of no choice (Schwartz, 2010).

The freedom to choose among many options is not a simple issue - with freedom

also comes responsibility. Since a person has the freedom to choose how to act, think, or

make choices, they must also take full responsibility for these actions. Even though

many things are out of a person's control, for example one's genetics or life

circumstances, they are still responsible for their actions within their limitations (Rollo

May, 1981; Schneider & Krug, 2010). This freedom and subsequent responsibility have

tremendous implications (Maglio, Butterfield, & Borgen, 2005). People can never

exactly be certain of the effect a choice is going to make. For example, an individual

often decides on his or her career early on in life. A career choice has a major impact on

one's life, but one can never be certain if this is the optimal choice. One choice could

lead to a multitude of other options, but at the same time, thousands of other possibilities

are no longer available. Due to this relinquishing of possibilities, people often express a

fear of making the wrong decision. There is lack of further choice once a decision has

been made. People seek to avoid the regret associated with less than optimal decisions

(Schneider & Krug, 201 O; Magilio et al., 2005).

Page 24: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 21

Lucas (2005) describes regret as a desire to go back and change past decisions.

She goes on to state that when people realize the finality of one's choices, they tend to

focus on previous choices and the inability to change them, even at the expense of future

decisions that could alleviate the consequences of past choices. An example of this is the

student who focuses on past procrastination and the resulting poor grades instead of

focusing on getting what work they can accomplished to bring up their poor grades

(Lucas, 2005). Gilovich and Medvec (1995) examined regret in individuals across the

lifespan and found that a majority of people expressed regret about inaction compared to

action, and that regret over inaction tends to be long lasting. More specifically, people

expressed higher levels of regret about inaction related to major life decisions such as

education or career. When individuals did express regret over actions or things they did,

like making a purchase, the regret was short-term.

In present day, the number of choices that one is able to make is enormous. For

example, today's high school senior is faced with an ever-increasing amount of options

on what to do with their life. They could choose to go to college, a technical school,

enter the workforce, or join the military. For the student who wishes to attend college,

they must then select which institutions to apply to. Once they have been accepted to a

college or university they must then choose a major. A New York Times article reports

there were over 1,500 academic programs reported to the Department of Education by

colleges and universities in 2010 (Konnikova, 2013). The implications of choosing a

major are far-reaching, and could influence things such as life satisfaction, where one

lives, and how much money one makes (Porter & Umbach, 2006).

Page 25: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 22

Even among rather constrained decisions, such as what cereal to buy, there is a

broad range of options to choose from (Schwartz, 2004). In a series of experiments,

Iyengar and Lepper (2000) found evidence that providing someone with more choices

does not always lead to more satisfaction. In one experiment, individuals who were

presented with limited options, six, were more likely to purchase a jar of jam compared to

those who were given more options, thirty. In another experiment, students were more

likely to complete an extra credit assignment, and turned in better written assignments

when given six potential topics, compared to students who were given thirty potential

topics. In a final experiment, participants reported higher levels of regret and less

satisfaction with their choice when given the possibility of choosing from thirty chocolate

bars, compared to those who were allowed to choose from six chocolate bars. The

authors go on to suggest that people were not simply unhappy with their choices, but

were unsure and "burdened by the responsibility of distinguishing good from bad

decisions" (Iyengar & Lepper, 2000, p. 1004).

Research has shown that after a choice has been made, and the decision is final,

individuals are more likely to view the outcome as positive. This is true even if they did

not initially indicate the specific choice as a preference (Gilbert & Ebert, 2002).

Researchers demonstrated this effect by having individuals rank universities before and

after they had been accepted. Once a university had rejected participants they ranked that

university lower, and universities that they had been accepted to as higher (Lyubomirsky

& Ross, 1999). This effect of increasing one's satisfaction with the outcome of an event

appears to be especially strong in events that have permanent outcomes. Individuals were

found to be more satisfied with a poster they purchased ten days after making a choice

Page 26: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 23

when the decision was final, compared to those who were able to change their choice in

poster. Authors suggest the effect was due to leaving the possibility of a better option

available (Gilbert & Ebert, 2002). The ability to change one's mind is often thought of as

a positive aspect of a decision (Schwartz, 2010). The above-mentioned research has

shown that this may not be the case, and individuals may not be satisfied when they are

left with more possible options. (Gilbert & Ebert, 2002).

Much of the research on choice and freedom has focused on minor economic

issues, such as the purchasing of jars of jam or the purchase of a poster. However, many

of life's decisions are much more profound, and have far greater consequences. In

modern society, the number of choices, even among life's most important decisions, is

increasing every day. In the past, one was often confined to the region they grew up in,

or to a limited list of potential occupations. Present day allows for much more freedom

in regard to potential careers and places of living (Schwartz, 2010).

Uncertainty Management Theory

Uncertainty Management Theory, which arose from existing research being

conducted on judgments of fairness, is based upon Fairness Heuristic Theory. The

Fairness Heuristic Theory states that in the absence of actual solid information, people

make fairness-related judgments based upon a heuristic. This heuristic then guides an

individual's interpretations of future events (Van den Bos, 200la). The theory was

developed so as to better understand why fairness is important to people. Research has

shown that when people were not provided with information about whether or not an

authority figure could be trusted, they displayed a greater need for procedural fairness

information (Van den Bos, Wilke, Lind, & Vermunt, 1998). Procedural fairness pertains

Page 27: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 24

to the process of how one is treated. For example, in a court trial, the process and manner

in which the trial was conducted would be considered elements of procedural fairness

(Van den Bos, V ermunt, & Wilke, 1997). When individuals were given information

about whether or not an authority figure could be trusted, they did not display as large a

need for procedural fairness information. In other words, if the participant was given

information on whether or not the authority figure could be trusted, they did not pay as

much attention to the process by which the authority figure determined the judgment

(Van den Bos et al., 1998). Van den Bos and colleagues (1997) illustrated this process by

manipulating the information that participants were given pertaining to a decision made

by an authority figure. In the experiment, participants were given scenarios in which they

were told they would be applying for a fictional job that consisted of a certain number of

duties. Hiring decisions about this job were presented so that individuals were told either

that performance on all duties, or a limited number of duties would be used to make the

hiring decision. Participants who were told that a limited number of duties were used in

the hiring decision reported more negative affective reactions toward the way they were

treated compared to those who had all duties evaluated (Van den Bos et al., 1997).

Van den Bos (2001 b) proposed that uncertainty played a crucial role in people's

perceptions of fairness. Previous research on the Fairness Heuristic Theory provided

evidence that people relied less on procedural fairness when they had more information,

or were more certain, about the authority figure. In an experiment designed to assess the

effect of uncertainty salience on reactions toward procedural justice, participants were

required to complete a number of tasks. After completing the tasks, the participant was

informed that another individual, referred to as Other, performed exactly as well as the

Page 28: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 25

participant. Then uncertainty salience or control conditions were induced. Participants

were then informed they would or would not be allowed to voice how many tickets the

Other should receive compared to themselves. Individuals who were exposed to an

uncertainty salience prime and not allowed to voice their opinion were more likely to

endorse negative affective responses, in regard to procedural fairness, compared to those

who were allowed to voice their opinion, as well as those participants in the control

condition (Van den Bos, 2001a).

Studies have shown that mortality and uncertainty salience produce similar

effects. Studies which examined procedural fairness by manipulating one's ability or

inability to voice how many tickets another receives (in the same manner as was

described in Van den Bos and colleagues (2001a) experiment above) have shown that

people who were exposed to a mortality salient condition displayed a greater need for

fairness information, compared to those who were exposed to a control condition (Van

den Bos & Miedema, 2000).

Uncertainty Management theory posits that one's level of uncertainty has a major

impact on one's reactions toward cultural norms. Much like Terror Management Theory,

the theory proposes that belief in one's cultural worldview, as well as the self-esteem

derived from adherence to said cultural worldview, serves to reduce the anxiety that one

feels when they are uncertain (Y avuz & Van den Bos, 2009). It is important to note that

in these studies comparing uncertainty and mortality salience, the variables manipulated

remained as close to those in previous Terror Management Theory studies as possible.

For example, the uncertainty salience manipulation was induced by prompting the

participant to "please briefly describe the emotions that the thought of your being

Page 29: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 26

uncertain arouses in you" and "please write down, as specifically as you can, what you

think physically will happen to you as you feel uncertain", thus only replacing the word

death with the word uncertain (Van den Bos, 2001b).

Some supporters of Uncertainty Management Theory have even gone as far as to

say that the effect of mortality salience is due in part to the uncertainty that surrounds

death. Evidence for this claim was found when examining what participants wrote when

asked the salience inducing questions. Those asked to write about death sometimes wrote

about the uncertainty that surrounds death, while those who were instructed to write

about uncertainty never mentioned death. Furthermore, an analysis showed those who

wrote down thoughts of uncertainty while in the mortality salience condition displayed

stronger effects when making judgments of an essay that went against their cultural

worldview (Y avuz & Van den Bos, 2009).

Other experiments comparing uncertainty and mortality salience conditions have

provided conflicting results. As was mentioned above, research has provided some

evidence for uncertainty salience primes producing greater effects compared to mortality

salience primes (Van den Bos, 2001b; Yavuz & Van den Bos, 2009). Other studies have

provided evidence that mortality salience primes have resulted in larger effects on various

dependent variables. In an experiment conducted by Landau and colleagues (2004),

participants were measured for personal need for structure and subsequently exposed to a

mortality or uncertainty salience condition. Participants were then asked to rate their

liking for an individual who was depicted as having either consistent or inconsistent

personality characteristics. In the study, participants with a high personal need for

structure and were subsequently exposed to a mortality salience condition reported liking

Page 30: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 27

the individual with inconsistent personality characteristics less, compared to participants

who were exposed to an uncertainty salience condition. Another study conducted by

Routledge, Arndt, and Goldenberg (2004) compared the effects of mortality and

uncertainty salience conditions on individuals' willingness to buy suntan lotion. The

researchers found that individuals who were exposed to a mortality salience prime,

compared to individuals who were exposed to an uncertainty salience prime, were more

likely to endorse buying sun-tanning products after viewing an ad featuring an attractive

suntanned woman. These findings are in line with previous research that demonstrated

individuals when exposed to a mortality salience prime will endorse activities that could

be potentially harmful, if the individual views the activities as relevant to their self­

esteem. The authors attributed being tan with elevating self-esteem. It should also be

noted that in the study, there were no differences in the number of sun-tanning items

endorsed by individuals who were exposed to a control ad featuring a beach ball

(Routledge et al., 2004).

One important aspect to consider is that the studies that have found uncertainty

salience to produce stronger effects compared to mortality salience were conducted in the

Netherlands. Studies that have found the opposite to be true have been conducted using

individuals in the United States; therefore, a cultural confound could be present (Van den

Bos & Lind, 2010; Pyszcynski et al., 2006). Researchers have also noted that the wide

variety of variables being measured could account for the difference in mortality and

uncertainty salience effects. For example, studies examining the effects of uncertainty

and mortality salience on procedural fairness have found that uncertainty salience

produces larger effects. In contrast, studies examining the effects of uncertainty and

Page 31: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

mortality salience on cultural worldview defense have found mortality salience to

produce larger effects (Pyszcynski et al., 2006).

28

Van den Bos and Lind (2010) proposed that perhaps mortality and uncertainty are

two of many possible existential motives behind behavior, and different experimental

methods and measurements of different dependent variables will be needed to uncover

the potential effects of both mortality and uncertainty. Van den Bos (2009) proposes a

"hypothesis that personal uncertainty may be a very important motive underlying process

of self-regulation, meaning-making, and especially worldview defense" (p. 256).

Present Study

The present research examines the effects of mortality salience and uncertainty

salience on state anxiety. Prior research conducted on mortality and uncertainty salience

has provided mixed results, with either mortality or uncertainty salience eliciting stronger

effects (Pyszcynski et al., 2006; Yavuz & Van den Bos 2009; Routledge et al., 2004).

Experiments have looked at the effect of either uncertainty or mortality salience against a

control condition on social judgments, fairness judgments, or negative affect. Few

studies have been conducted that focus on the effects of mortality or uncertainty salience

on emotional states (Strachen et al., 2007). The present study attempts to explore the

relationship between mortality salience and uncertainty salience on one's state anxiety

based upon participant's levels of self-esteem.

Based upon previous research, it is hypothesized that both mortality and

uncertainty salience conditions will result in higher levels of state anxiety in individuals

with low self-esteem. Additionally, previous research suggests individuals assigned to

the mortality salience condition sometimes indicated they were thinking about the

Page 32: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 29

uncertainty that surrounds death. Based upon these findings, and the high prevalence of

uncertainty in the target population's life, the uncertainty salience condition will yield

higher levels of state anxiety compared to the mortality salience condition.

Method

Participants

One hundred and seventeen undergraduate Introductory Psychology students ( 42

males, 75 females) participated in exchange for course credit in a study described as

examining the relationship between personality characteristics. Four participant's

responses were not included in the analysis due to not answering all of the questions or

only circling an answer of one. After giving informed consent, participants were given a

packet of materials and told to complete a number of tasks. Participants were given

instructions to answer the questions to the best of their ability, complete the packet in

order, and not to go back and change answers after they had completed a page. Materials

were presented in the following order.

Materials

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Participants filled out the Rosenberg Self-Esteem

Scale. The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale is a self-report measure of one-dimensional

self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965). Participants are required to endorse statements based

upon how the statement reflects one's self. The scale is constructed such that higher

scores indicate higher levels of self-esteem. The scale consists of ten items, which are

answered in a Likert-scale form from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree). Items

are both positively and negatively oriented. Negatively oriented items were reversed

Page 33: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

scored. The ten items were averaged to give a single measure. Examples of positively

questions include "On the whole, I am satisfied with myself." Examples of negatively

oriented questions include "At times I think I am no good at all" (Rosenberg, 1965).

30

Experimental Condition. The experimental salience condition was then

implemented. Participants were randomly assigned to either a mortality salience,

uncertainty salience, or control condition. In the mortality salience condition,

participants were asked to "please briefly describe the emotions that the thought of your

own death arouses in you" and "please write down, as specifically as you can, what you

think physically will happen to you as you die." In the uncertainty condition the same

questions were asked, with "your own death" replaced with "feeling uncertain" in the

first question. In the second question "feel uncertain" replaced "die". In the control

condition, participants were asked the same question with "watching television"

replacing "your own death" in the first question, and "watch television" replacing "die"

in the second question. Watching television served as a neutral task, and has been

utilized as a control condition in previous Terror and Uncertainty Management research.

Participants were then asked to complete a word search puzzle that contained neutral

words. As with previous research in Terror Management Theory, this served as a delay

task. The delay task lasted approximately 2.5 to 3 minutes.

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Participants were then administered the STAI­

Yl Form (state scale) of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, which served as the

dependent variable. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory is a self-report of both current

anxiety in the moment and as a personality trait. For the purposes of this study, only

participant's state anxiety was examined (Spielberger, 1983). The STAI- YI Form

Page 34: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 31

consists of twenty statements that could reflect how a person feels in the current moment.

The questions are written at a sixth grade reading level. Participants are required to rate

their anxiety on a four point Likert scale from I (Not at All) to 4 (Very Much So). The

scores of the STAI- YI Form have been shown to increase when individuals are faced

with a stressful event, such as a job interview or upcoming test, and decrease when

relaxation techniques are implemented (Spielberger, I983).

Internal consistency for the STAI- YI were high (a= .90) for groups of working

adults, high-school students, and military recruits. Test-retest reliability was less

consistent, .I6 to .65, but this was to be expected, as the measure can be influenced by

one's current mood. To assess the content validity of the STAI, researchers compared the

scores of participants who were told to respond according to how they thought they

would feel before taking an important exam and participants in a non-stressful condition.

Participants who where told to answer as if they were about to take an important exam

scored significantly higher (Spielberger, I 983).

Results

In order to test the first hypothesis, that both mortality and uncertainty salience

increases state anxiety at low, but not high, levels of self esteem, a hierarchical multiple

regression analysis was conducted. In the first step, the main effects of salience

(mortality, uncertainty, control) conditions were entered (dummy coded). Results show

that the main effects of salience condition accounted for 6% of the variance in state

anxiety scores, F(2,I I4) = 3.42,p = .04.

State anxiety scores for participants in the uncertainty salience group (M = I .87,

SD= .48) were significantly higher than those in a control group (M = I .62, SD= .43), b

Page 35: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

= .26, SE = .10, t = 2.46, p < .025, when taking into account a Bonferroni correction.

Furthermore, results indicate that participants in the mortality salience condition (M =

1.82, SD = .46) were trending toward having higher levels of state anxiety compared to

participants in the control condition, but were not significant with a Bonferroni

correction, b =.20, SE= .10, t= l.95,p > .025

32

In the second step, self-esteem scores were added to determine if self-esteem to

examine the main effect of self-esteem. The results indicate a significant main effect of

self-esteem, R2 change= .34, F(l,113) = 62.31, p < .001, such that higher levels of self­

esteem resulted in lower levels of state anxiety. Furthermore, self-esteem accounted for

34% of the variance in state anxiety scores,p < .001.

In the third step, the two-way interaction terms were entered. Results indicated

that there were no significant interactions, R2 change= .01, F(2,111) = l.30,p >.05. A

summary of the results of the multiple regression analysis is found in Figure 2.

Discussion

The results of the current study add to the literature on terror management and

uncertainty management theory. In line with previous terror management and uncertainty

management research, both mortality salience and uncertainty salience were shown to

have an effect on participants, compared to the control condition (Van den Bos, 2001 b;

Y avuz & Van den Bos, 2009). While results indicated that the state anxiety scores of

participants in the mortality salience group did not significantly differ from those of the

control condition when taking into account a Bonferroni correction (p > .025), state

anxiety levels were trending toward a significant result (see Figure 3).

Page 36: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 33

Previous research has provided mixed results, with mortality or uncertainty

salience producing larger effects in different studies (Y avuz & Van den Bos 2009;

Pyszcynski et al., 2006; Routledge et al., 2004; Van den Bos, 2001b). One possible

explanation of the effect of uncertainty salience found in this study is that mortality and

uncertainty salience affect state anxiety differently than the dependent variables

examined in past studies (e.g. worldview judgments, fairness judgments). Van den Bos

and Lind (2010) proposed a similar hypothesis, suggesting that mortality or uncertainty

salience may result in larger or smaller effects depending upon what dependent variable

is measured. Future research that examines the effect of both mortality and uncertainty

salience on various dependent variables would provide more information in regard to

what processes are affected by different existential motivators.

In line with previous research, results indicate a significant main effect of self­

esteem, such that higher levels of self-esteem resulted in lower levels of state anxiety

(Routledge et al, 2010). These findings support terror and uncertainty management's

claim that self-esteem serves as a buffer to distressing thoughts of mortality and

uncertainty. Furthermore, self-esteem was by far the best predictor of state anxiety

scores, more so than the salience manipulations.

The lack of a significant two-way interaction between salience condition and level

of self-esteem is inconsistent with previous terror management research. As Figure 3

shows, there is an obvious trend, with participants with lower self-esteem experiencing

more state anxiety, compared to those with high self-esteem, across all salience

conditions. Previous terror management research has found that individuals in control

conditions, regardless of self-esteem level, did not evidence an increase in state anxiety

Page 37: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 34

scores (Routledge et al., 2010). The increase in state anxiety scores found for individuals

in the control condition with lower self-esteem is a possible explanation for the lack of

significant two-way interactions.

One should note that the group of participants, as a whole, was relatively young

(M = 19.41 years old, SD = 1.23). Thoughts of death might not be as accessible or

applicable to the participant's lives, where as thoughts of uncertainty are very likely

present and applicable to nearly all individuals. Formal content analysis was not

performed on participant responses, however, many participants in the uncertainty

salience condition mentioned aspects of their life that they were currently uncertain about

(e.g. grades, life choices, relationships). In contrast, participants in the mortality salience

condition tended to only mention discomfort with thinking about their own mortality or

their belief in an afterlife (e.g. heaven, eternal life, ect.). Terror management theory

states that one is motivated to deny one's own mortality. Denying uncertainty is more

difficult, and therefore might result in a larger effect on state anxiety. Formal content

analysis in future research would allow for an examination of specific responses that

might be causing state anxiety levels to rise.

The limitations of this study should be noted. As was mentioned above, this study

lacked any formal content analysis of the responses. Uncertainty management research

has shown that the content of participant responses to the salience inducing questions can

affect the dependent variable, such that responses to the mortality salience questions that

mentioned the uncertainty surrounding death resulted in stronger effects. Another

limitation of the current study was the lack of affect measurement. A number of studies

have repeatedly demonstrated that mortality salience manipulation does not result in

Page 38: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 35

negative affect, as measured by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS),

however these claims have recently been disputed in the literature (Lambert et al., 2014).

Given recent claims that previous attempts to measure affect by using the PANAS have

been susceptible to measurement error, future research would benefit from examining

participants positive and negative affect after the mortality and uncertainty salience have

been induced. Furthermore, the time of the delay task was approximated. Research has

shown that measuring the dependent variable too soon after inducing mortality salience

can result in not having as strong an effect on dependent variables. While the delay task

that was selected for this study has been used in previous terror and uncertainty

management research, no exact measure of the length of the delay task was taken.

Therefore, it is possible that this affected the outcome of the study. Future research

would benefit from an exact measure of time to completion of the delay task.

In summary, the present study demonstrated both mortality salience and

uncertainty salience increased state anxiety. Results indicate existential concerns can be

heightened by fairly simple references to mortality and uncertainty. The relatively

innocuous directive to simply think about the emotions that "death" or "feeling

uncertain" arouses in individuals was able to produce a substantial effect on one's state

anxiety. The observed effect demonstrates the relevance of existential concerns in the

lives ofrelatively young individuals.

Page 39: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

References

American Psychological Association (2014, March 1). Anxiety. Retrieved from

http://apa.org/topics/ anxiety/index.aspx

36

Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1997). Subliminal exposure to

death-related stimuli increases defense of the cultural worldview. Psychological

Science, 8(5), 379-385. doi:10.l 111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00429.x

Barlow, B. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: the nature of treatment of anxiety & panic

(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York, NY US: Free Press.

Crowell, S. (2010), "Existentialism", The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Edward

N. Zalta (ed.), URL=

http://plato.stanford.edu/ archives/win2010/ entries/ existentialism/

Dechesne, M., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., Ransom, S., Sheldon, K. M., van Knippenberg,

A., & Janssen, J. (2003). Literal and symbolic immortality: The effect of evidence

of literal immortality on self-esteem striving in response to mortality salience.

Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 84( 4), 722-73 7.

doi: 10.1037 /0022-3514.84.4. 722

Florian, V., & Kravetz, S. (1983) Fear of personal death: Attribution, structure, and

relation to religious belief. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(3),

600-607. doi: 0022-3514/83/4403-0600

Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1997). Fear of personal death in adulthood: The impact of

early and recent losses. Death Studies, 21, 1-24.

Page 40: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

Furer, P., & Walker, J. R. (2008). Death anxiety: A cognitive-behavioral approach.

Journal Of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 22(2), 167-182. doi:l0.1891/0889-

8391.22.2.167

37

Gilbert, D. T., & Ebert, J. J. (2002). Decisions and revisions: The affective forecasting of

changeable outcomes. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 82( 4), 503-

514. doi: 10.103 7 /0022-3514.82.4.503

Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. (1995). The experience ofregret: What, when, and why.

Psychological Review, 102(2), 379-395. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.379

Goncalves Portelinha, I., Verlhiac, J., Meyer, T., & Hutchison, P. (2012). Terror

management and biculturalism: When the salience of cultural duality affects

worldview defense in the face of death. European Psychologist, 17(3), 237-245.

doi:l0.1027/1016-9040/aOOOl l l

Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Simon, L., & Breus, M. (1994). Role of

consciousness and accessibility of death-related thoughts in mortality salience

effects. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 67(4), 627-637.

doi: 10.1037 /0022-3514.67.4.627

Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S., &

Lyon, D. (1990). Evidence for terror management theory II: The effects of

mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural

worldview. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 58(2), 308-318.

doi: 10.1037 /0022-3514.58.2.308

Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror management theory of self­

esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual

Page 41: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 38

refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology,

Vol. 29 (pp. 61-139). San Diego, CA US: Academic Press.

Hartley, C. A. & Phelps, E. A. (2012). Anxiety and decision-making. Biological

Psychiatry, 72, 113-118.

Hayes, J., Schimel, J., Arndt, J., & Faucher, E. H. (2010). A theoretical and empirical

review of the death-thought accessibility concept in terror management research.

Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 699-739. doi:10.1037/a0020524

Hemstein, R. J. (1990). Rational choice theory: Necessary but not sufficient. American

Psychologist, 45(3), 356-367.

Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating: Can one desire too

much of a good thing?. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 79( 6),

995-1006. doi:l0.1037/0022-3514.79.6.995

Kastenbaum, R. (2000). The psychology of death (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Springer

Publishing Company.

Konnikova, M. (2013, Novermber 16) You're so self-controlling. New York Times.

Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2013/11/17/opinion/sunday/youre-so­

self-controlling.html?pagewanted=all&_r=O

Kessler, R. C., Rusico, A. M., Shear, K., & Wittchen, H. (2009) Epidemiology of anxiety

disorders. In M.B. Stein and T. Steckler (eds.), Behavioral neurobiology of

anxiety and its treatment (pp. 22-35). DOI: 10.1007/7854_2009 _9

Lambert, A. J., Eadeh, F. R., Peak, S. A., Scherer, L. D., Schott, J.P., & Slochower, J.M.

(2014). Toward a greater understanding of the emotional dynamics of the

mortality salience manipulation: Revisiting the "affect-free" claim of terror

Page 42: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

management research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106( 5),

655-678. doi: 10.1037/a0036353

39

Landau, M. J., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., Miller,

C. H., Ogilvie, D. M., & Cook, A. (2004). Deliver us from Evil: The Effects of

Mortality Salience and Reminders of 9/11 on Support for President George W.

Bush. Personality And Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(9), 1136-1150.

doi:l0.1177/0146167204267988

Lucas, M. (2005). Existential regret: A crossroads of existential anxiety and existential

guilt. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 44, 58-70. doi:

10.1177/022167803259752

Lykins, E. B., Segerstrom, S. C., Averill, A. J., Evans, D.R., & Kemeny, M. E. (2007).

Goal shifts following reminders of mortality: Reconciling posttraumatic growth

and terror management theory. Personality And Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(8),

1088-1099. doi:l0.1177/0146167207303015

Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1999). Changes in attractiveness of elected, rejected, and

precluded alternatives: A comparison of happy and unhappy individuals. Journal

Of Personality And Social Psychology, 76(6), 988-1007. doi:l0.1037/0022-

3514.76.6.988

May, R. (1981). Freedom and destiny. New York, NY US: WW Norton & Co.

Maglio, A. T., Butterfield, L. D., & Borgen, W. A. (2005). Existential considerations for

contemporary career counseling. Journal of Employment Counseling, 42, 75-92.

Marshall, J.M. (1989). Heidegger as depicted by Binswanger and Boss. Theoretical &

Philosophical Psychology, 9(2), 37-43.

Page 43: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

Porter, S. R., & Umbach, P. D. (2006). College major choice: An analysis ofperson­

environment fit. Research in Higher Education, 47(4), 429-449.

40

Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Maxfield, M. (2006). On the unique

psychological import of the human awareness of mortality: Theme and variations.

Psychological Inquiry, 17(4), 328-356. doi:l0.1080/10478400701369542

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton

University Press.

Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon, D. (1989).

Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on

reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal Of Personality

And Social Psychology, 57(4), 681-690. doi:l0.1037/0022-3514.57.4.681

Routledge, C., Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2004). A Time to Tan: Proximal and Distal

Effects of Mortality Salience on Sun Exposure Intentions. Personality And Social

Psychology Bulletin, 30(10), 1347-1358. doi:l0.1177/0146167204264056

Routledge, C., Ostafin, B., Juhl, J., Sedikides, C., Cathey, C., & Liao, J. (2010).

Adjusting to death: The effects of mortality salience and self-esteem on

psychological well-being, growth motivation, and maladaptive behavior. Journal

Of Personality And Social Psychology, 99(6), 897-916. doi:10.1037/a0021431

Schwartz, B. (2004). The paradox of choice: Why more is less. New York, NY US:

HarperCollins Publishers.

Schwartz, B. (2010). Be careful what you wish for: The dark side of freedom. In R. M.

Akin, K. C. Oleson, & P. J. Carroll (Eds.), Handbook of the uncertain self(pp. 62-

77). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Page 44: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 41

Schneider, K. J., & Krug, 0. T. (2010). Existential-humanistic therapy. Washington, DC

US: American Psychological Association.

Simon, L., Greenberg, J., Harmon-Jones, E., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., &

Abend, T. (1997). Terror management and cognitive-experiential self-theory:

Evidence that terror management occurs in the experiential system, Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 72(5), 1132-1146. doi: 0022-3514/97

Sliter, M. T., Sinclair, R.R., Yuan, Z., & Mohr, C. D. (2014, February 3). Don't Fear the

Reaper: Trait Death Anxiety, Mortality Salience, and Occupational Health.

Journal of Applied Psychology. Advance online publication.

http://dx.doi.org/l 0.103 7 /a0035729

Smith, D. L., (2010). From passive to psyche to dynamic dasein. The Humanistic

Psychologist, 38, 203-220. doi: 10.1080/08873267.2010.485908

Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2004). The Cultural Animal: Twenty

Years of Terror Management Theory and Research. In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole,

T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of Experimental Existential Psychology (pp.

13-34). New York, NY US: Guilford Press.

Spielberger, C. D. (1983). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults Manual, Instrument,

and Scoring Guide. Menlo Park, CA: Mindgarden Inc.

Strachan, E., Schimel, J., Arndt, J., Williams, T., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., &

Greenberg, J. (2007). Terror mismanagement: Evidence that mortality salience

exacerbates phobic and compulsive behaviors. Personality And Social Psychology

Bulletin, 33(8), 1137-1151. doi:l0.1177/0146167207303018

Taubman Ben-Ari, 0., Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1999). The impact of mortality

Page 45: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 42

salience on reckless driving: A test of terror management mechanisms. Journal Of

Personality And Social Psychology, 76(1), 35-45. doi:l0.1037/0022-3514.76.1.35

Van den Bos, K. (2001a). Uncertainty management: The influence of uncertainty salience

on reactions to perceived procedural fairness. Journal Of Personality And Social

Psychology, 80(6), 931-941. doi:l0.1037/0022-3514.80.6.931

Van den Bos, K. (2001b). Reactions to perceived fairness: The impact of mortality

salience and self-esteem on ratings of negative affect. Social Justice Research,

14(1), 1-23. doi:10.1023/A:1012501506803

Van den Bos, K. (2009). On the psychology of the uncertain self and the integration of

the worldview defense zoo. Psychological Inquiry, 20(4), 252-261.

doi: 10.1080/104 78400903448532

Van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (2010) The social psychology of fairness and the

regulation of personal uncertainty. In R. M. Akin, K. C. Oleson, & P. J. Carroll

(Eds.) Handbook of the uncertain self(pp. 122-141) New York, NY: Psychology

Press.

Van den Bos, K., & Miedema, J. (2000). Toward understanding why fairness matters:

The influence of mortality salience on reactions to procedural fairness. Journal Of

Personality And Social Psychology, 79(3 ), 355-366. doi: 10.103 7 /0022-

3514. 79.3.355

Van den Bos, K., Vermunt, R., & Wilke, H. M. (1997). Procedural and distributive

justice: What is fair depends more on what comes first than on what comes next.

Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 72(1), 95-104. doi:l0.1037/0022-

3514.72.1.95

Page 46: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 43

Van den Bos, K., Wilke, H. M., Lind, E., & Vermunt, R. (1998). Evaluating outcomes by

means of the fair process effect: Evidence for different processes in fairness and

satisfaction judgments. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 7 4( 6),

1493-1503. doi: 10.103710022-3514.74.6.1493

Weems, C. F., Costa, N. M., Dehon, C., & Berman, S. L. (2004). Paul Tillich's theory of

existential anxiety: A preliminary conceptual and empirical examination. Anxiety,

Stress & Coping: An International Journal, 17(4), 383-399.

doi:l0.1080/10615800412331318616

Wicker, F. W., Payne, G. C., Roberson, K. E., & Garcia-Falconi, R. (1985). Participant

differentiation of nonclinical fear and anxiety. Motivation And Emotion, 9(1 ), 53-

70. doi: 10.1007 /BF00991550

Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects

of thought suppression. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 53(1), 5-

13. doi:l0.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5

Wegner D. M, & Smart L. (1997). Deep cognitive activation: A new approach to the

unconscious. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(6), 984-995.

Williams, J.M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional stroop task and

psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24.

Yalom, I. (1980). Existential Psychotherapy. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Yavuz, H., & Van den Bos, K. (2009). Effects of uncertainty and mortality salience on

worldview defense reactions in Turkey. Social Justice Research, 22(4), 384-398.

doi:l0.1007/sl 1211-009-0104-5

Page 47: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 44

Table 1

State anxiety scores for mortality salience, uncertainty salience, and control conditions (N = 117)

Condition Mean Standard Deviation

Mortality Salience 1.82 .46

Uncertainty Salience 1.88 .48

Control 1.62 .43

Page 48: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

Table 2

Multiple regression analysis for variables predicting state arzxiety score (N = 117)

Variable Step 1

Mortality vs. Control Uncertainty vs. Control

Steo2 Morality vs. Control Uncertainty vs. Control Self Esteem

Step 3 Mortality vs. Control Uncertainty vs. Control Self-Esteem Morality vs. Control x Self-Esteem Uncertainty vs. Control x Self-Esteem

,z -Step 2 R change - .34 Step 3 R2 change= .01 *p < .05 **p < .01

B

.20

.26

.12

.18

-.57

.92

.94

-.42

-.25

-.23

SEB /J

.10 .20

.10 .26*

.08 .13

.08 .18

.07 -.58**

.60 .94

.54 .94

.12 -.43**

.18 -.80

.17 -.75

45

Page 49: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 46

Figure 1

Predicted state anxiety scores for mortality, uncertainty, and control conditions for low self-esteem (-1 standard deviation) and high self-esteem (+ 1 standard deviation)

2.25

2.05

-+-Mortality Salience

- Uncertainty Salience

__.__Control

1.45

Page 50: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

Appendix A

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale

Please circle the number that best represents your answer.

1. On the whole, I am satisfied with myself Strongly Disagree Disagree

1 2

2. At times I think I am no good at all Strongly Disagree Disagree

1 2

Agree 3

Agree 3

3. I feel that I have a number of good qualities Strongly Disagree Disagree Agree

1 2 3

4. I am able to do things as well as most other people Strongly Disagree Disagree Agree

1 2 3

5. I feel I do not have much to be proud of Strongly Disagree Disagree

1 2

6.) I certainly feel useless at times Strongly Disagree Disagree

1 2

Agree 3

Agree 3

Strongly Agree 4

Strongly Agree 4

Strongly Agree 4

Strongly Agree 4

Strongly Agree 4

Strongly Agree 4

7.) I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others Strongly Disagree Disagree Agree Strongly Agree

1 2 3 4

8.) I wish I could have more respect for myself Strongly Disagree Disagree Agree

1 2 3

9.) All In all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure Strongly Disagree Disagree Agree

1 2 3

10.) I take a positive attitude toward myself Strongly Disagree Disagree Agree

1 2 3

Strongly Agree 4

Strongly Agree 4

Strongly Agree 4

47

Page 51: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

AppendixB

Mortality Salience Prime

Please briefly describe the emotions that the thought of your own death arouses in you

Please write down, as specifically as you can, what you think physically will happen to

you when you die.

48

Page 52: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 49

Appendix C

Uncertainty Salience Manipulation

Please briefly describe the emotions that the thought of feeling uncertain arouses in you

Please write down, as specifically as you can, what you think physically will happen to

you when you feel uncertain.

Page 53: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE

AppendixD

Control Manipulation

Please briefly describe the emotions that the thought of watching television arouses in you

Please write down, as specifically as you can, what you think physically will happen to

you when you watch television.

so

Page 54: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 51

Appendix E

Word Search Delay Task

Word Search Puzzle

Circle as many words as you can in the puzzle below.

Book Computer Desk Phone Movie Train Paper School Grass Beer Music Actor

s R E T u p M 0 c 0 w p H 0 N E R E E B A M u s I c p z s N B T N R 0 T c A s K B M R K s E D E A 0 R F 0 A G 0 L B R 0 E L G v I z B 0 G B p A N u I N E L w Q A G T A B E T G D 0 p s c H 0 0 L N I T

Page 55: The Effects of Existential Salience on State Anxiety

EFFECTS OF EXISTENTIAL SALIENCE 52

Appendix F

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Form Y

A number of statements which people have used to describe themselves are given below. Read each statement and then circle the appropriate number to the right of the statement to indicate how you feel right now, that is, at this moment. There are no right or wrong answers. Do not spend too much time on any one statement but give the answer which seems to describe your present feelings best

I feel calm Not at all Somewhat Moderately so Very Much So

1 2 3 4

I feel secure Not at all Somewhat Moderately so Very Much So

1 2 3 4

I am tense Not at all Somewhat Moderately so Very Much So

1 2 3 4

I feel strained Not at all Somewhat Moderately so Very Much So

1 2 3 4

I feel at ease Not at all Somewhat Moderately so Very Much So

1 2 3 4

Note: Due to copyright issues, only five (out of20) items on the State-Trait Anxiety

Inventory can be reproduced.