introduction - school of engineering - civil engineering · web viewby mark p. rossow southern...

22

Click here to load reader

Upload: vuliem

Post on 01-Apr-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

Theoretical Basis for Learning Statics by Studying Worked Examples

by

Mark P. RossowSouthern Illinois University Edwardsville

Edwardsville, IL 62026May, 2005

IntroductionThis paper is concerned with developing engineering students’ “problem-solving skills”. Here the term refers to the ability to get an answer to a well-defined problem such as is found in a typical undergraduate engineering textbook. The wider meaning of “problem-solving skills”— the ability to analyze a complicated and confusing situation, such as one finds in real-life engineering, and simplify it into an appropriate textbook-type problem and then to solve the problem and interpret the answer—is beyond the scope of the paper. The ultimate goal of most engineering education is to develop problem-solving skills in the wider sense, but skills in the narrower sense must be developed first—hence the importance of the subject of the present paper.

Relationship to cognitive scienceAdvances in cognitive science in the last twenty years have provided new insights into how human beings learn problem-solving skills. These insights have led to impressive work in several areas of education, perhaps most notably in the creation of intelligent tutoring systems but also in a theory of learning and instructional design called “cognitive load theory” (CLT) that is the focus of this paper. CLT holds special promise for improving learning in a wide variety of technical subjects. CLT researchers have conducted experiments with students in high-school algebra and geometry, LISP programming, accounting, and elementary optics, among other subjects—and have shown that instructional materials designed according to CLT principles develop superior problem-solving skills in students compared to the skills developed by traditional instructional materials. Description of cognitive load theoryAn outline of CLT will now be given. The outline is drawn from publications by two researchers closely identified with the theory (Cooper 1998, Sweller 1999). In 2002, an entire issue of the journal, Learning and Instruction, was devoted to assessing the current state of CLT, and several articles in this issue have provided perspective on the outline given below (Van Merrienboer, et al. 2002, Kirschner 2002)

1. CLT is derived from information-processing models of cognitive architecture. A number of such models have been proposed, some quite elaborate (Anderson 2004, Baddeley 2003, and Soar 2004). Almost all contain at least two key elements that CLT treats as fundamental to the ability to learn: working memory and long-term memory. Working memory is what we are conscious of; it consists not only of a place for temporary storage of information but also an executive system that processes

1

Page 2: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

information. Long-term memory is stored information that we are not conscious of but that we can retrieve and move to working memory when the need arises.

2. A fundamental finding of cognitive science is that working memory is very limited. The number of “chunks” of information that working memory can contain is, as expressed in the title of the widely-cited article, “The magical number seven plus or minus two” (Miller 1956).

3. Long-term memory, on the other hand, appears to be essentially unlimited. Learning “may be defined as the encoding (storage) of knowledge and/or skills into long term memory in such a way that the knowledge and skills may be recalled and applied at a later time on demand.” (Cooper 1998).

4. One key concept in CLT is “schema” acquisition. A schema is an element of long-term memory that a) although it may refer to multiple pieces of data, may be brought into working memory and treated as a single “chunk” of information, and b) contains information about how the data will be used. A chess master seeing a particular configuration of chess pieces on a board will recall the appropriate schema from long-term memory that will match the configuration on the board and will inform the master which are the best moves (De Groot 1966, Chase and Simon 1973). Upon seeing a textbook problem in electrical circuits, an experienced physics teacher recalls the schema that both categorizes the problem and informs the teacher what principles of circuit analysis to use. The very word “schema” shares linguistics roots with “schematic”, and indeed a schema may be thought of as a schematic drawing of some process or relation stored in long term memory.

5. Another key aspect of learning is “automation of schemas,” by which is meant the ability to recall and apply the appropriate schema easily with little concentration. In a chess exhibition, a chess master is able to play multiple games simultaneously by pausing at each board, recalling the appropriate schema, and then applying the associated move easily and quickly because the schema has been automated. The novice player, on the other hand, must exert considerable concentration in considering many possible moves and choosing the best one; the novice is constructing, rather than recalling a schema, and the process is far from automatic.

6. A schema can be acquired only if information is first recognized and processed in working memory. But because working memory is limited, schema acquisition and automation will be inhibited or will not occur if the capacity of the learner’s working memory is exceeded, that is, if the “cognitive load” is too great.

7. Technical subjects such as engineering often are intrinsically difficult and impose an intrinsic cognitive load on the learner. In contrast, extraneous cognitive load, that is, cognitive load resulting from activities that are extraneous to schema acquisition is avoidable and should be minimized through proper design of instructional materials.

2

Page 3: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

8. Novices attempting to learn by practicing problem-solving experience a heavy extraneous cognitive load because they are engaged in a goal-attainment activity rather than a learning activity. The goal-attainment activity consists of searching for ways to reduce differences between the goal state (knowing the answer), sub goals (intermediate steps that will lead to the goal state), and the data given in the original problem statement. This procedure has been labeled “means-end analysis” and is widely employed by persons who are not familiar with the specific type of problem being solved (Larkin, et al. 1980, Simon and Simon 1978). That is, persons who do not possess well-developed schemas for a particular type of problem resort to means-end analysis as a general-purpose problem-solving strategy. Unfortunately, while means-end analysis may lead to solving the problem, it interferes with learning because working-memory capacity is devoted to searching for ways to obtain a goal or perhaps sub-goals leading to the final goal, rather than being devoted to formulating principles, identifying common characteristics, and generalizing approaches—in short, schema acquisition and automation. If learning happens to occur in the process, it is only as an incidental side-effect. Indeed, experiments have been reported wherein subjects repeatedly used means-ends analysis to solve problems of an identical kind yet completely failed to learn from the activity. That is, they were unable to discover the underlying principle that was common to all the problems and that allowed the problems to be solved much more simply and rapidly (Sweller and Levine, 1982).

9. Under the right conditions, learning by studying worked examples is superior to learning by practicing problem-solving because the former imposes less cognitive load on the learner than the latter. Thus more working memory resources are available for schema acquisition and automation. Obviously students have learned in the past and continue to learn by solving problems, but much of that learning may well take place during review and contemplation of the solution (Chi, et al. 1989), once it has been obtained. The efficiency of the process could be considerably improved by simply giving students the solution in the first place: at a minimum, the students are not distracted by having to locate trivial mistakes such as miscopying problems from the textbook or making an error with their calculators, but more importantly they do not waste working memory resources on searching.

10. Worked examples involving a combination of text and graphics may produce a cognitive load similar in magnitude to that produced by solving problems—that is, worked examples may not lead to improved learning—if the text and graphics are physically separated on the page (the “split-attention” effect) or if the text and graphics repeat the same material (the “redundancy” effect). Thus for effective schema acquisition and automation, 1) text and graphics in worked examples should be “physically integrated”, that is, text should be positioned as close as possible to related graphics, and 2) information should be presented only once, either in text or in graphics, but not in both. For many years, publishers of textbooks have implicitly recognized the validity of the split-attention effect by laying out the material in the book so that the reader need not turn the page to find a figure mentioned in the text, that is, the figure and the text referring to the figure are either on the same page or facing pages. CLT says that the publishers should go farther still in placing text next to or actually on the figure.

3

Page 4: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

11. Working memory capacity can be enlarged by presenting information in both visual and auditory form. Most cognitive scientists now believe that working memory is not a monolithic component of human cognitive architecture. The exact formulations vary from theorist to theorist, but a commonly found feature is that one processor in working memory is available for processing auditory information and a different processor is available for processing visual information. The two processors are not completely independent, but a number of researchers (Allport et al. 1972, Frick 1984, Penney 1989) have shown that human beings learn more if information is given to them over visual and auditory channels simultaneously compared to what is learned if the same information is presented over either channel exclusively. In particular, this “modality effect” increases learning significantly when graphical material alone is presented visually, and text is presented exclusively in auditory form (for example, the instructor reads the text while students study the figure) (Tindall-Ford, et al. 1997). To prevent the redundancy effect, the text should not repeat information provided graphically (Kalyuga 2000).

Summary of CLT’s findings related to the development of instructional materials for technical subjects involving problem solving

1. Instructional materials should be designed to eliminate extraneous cognitive load, that is, demands on working memory.

2. Worked examples formatted to avoid split attention and redundancy are such materials.

3. Learning will be facilitated if textual information is presented in auditory form at the same time that the student views graphical information.

Observations on CLT and worked examplesWorked examples have of course been used in science and engineering education for many years, in lectures, in textbooks and in the familiar Schaum’s Outline Series. Furthermore, students studying for a test will typically study worked examples—their own solutions to homework problems. So worked examples by themselves are nothing new. But two aspects of the CLT approach to worked examples are quite novel.

The non-traditional appearance of CLT worked examplesThe first aspect that is new is the formatting of the examples to avoid the split attention effect and careful editing to avoid the redundancy effect. Fig. 1 shows a worked example in traditional form. The example is a typical problem in Coulomb (dry, un-lubricated) friction such as would be found in a statics textbook or in a set of published problems (Shelley 1990). (Because of space limitations in this paper, only the first part of the example is shown). Note that the text describing both diagrams is separated from the diagrams themselves. Similarly the text describing the equations is separated from the equation. The reader must retain the meaning of the text in working memory while simultaneously searching for the relevant part of the figure and the related equation. The reader’s attention is split and extraneous cognitive load results.

4

Page 5: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

N AB + 207.29 - P cos 25° + f

A = 0

N A - 444.54 - P sin 25° + f

AB = 0

Problem: Two uniform cylinders A and B each have a mass of 50 kg and radius of 300 mm. A horizontal force P is applied to the center of cylinder A. The coefficients of friction between the cylinders and the plane and each other are shown in the figure. Determine the largest value of P that can be applied without moving the cylinders up the inclined plane.

Solution: In the free-body diagram of cylinder A shown, the weight of the cylinder, (50 kg)(9.81 m/s2 ) = 490.5 N, has been decomposed into components parallel and perpendicular to the plane, as has the force P. The force P tends to move the cylinder up the plane. The friction force, f

A, from the plane opposes this motion. In addition, the component of P parallel to the plane tends to rotate the cylinder about the point of contact with the plane. The friction force, f

AB, opposes this rotation. Taking into account the forces shown on the free-body diagram, we can write the equilibrium equations shown. Note that point O is a good point to choose for summing moments because only two forces appear in the equation.

B = 0.4

A = 0.6

AB = 0.5

A

BP

25°

F x = 0:

F y = 0:

y

xf

AN

A

N AB

f AB

(490.5) cos 25° = 444.54 N(490.5) sin 25° =

207.29 N

A

300 mm

300 mm

P sin 25°

P cos 25°O

M O = 0: (300)f

A - (300)f AB = 0

25°

Fig. 1. Part of a typical friction problem presented in traditional format

5

Page 6: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

Contrast this situation with that shown in Fig. 2, in which the same example has been designed according to CLT principles. Now the text and associated diagram or related equations are physically adjacent on the page. The reader’s search effort has been considerably reduced. Precious cognitive resources are not expended on searching but are instead available for schema acquisition and automation.

One other aspect of Fig. 2 calls for comment: it is cluttered. Clutter is typical of CLT worked examples. They offend one’s aesthetic sense of how an example should appear in a textbook or on a computer screen; they violate conventional ideas about how examples should be formatted. However, the conventional format for presenting worked examples is derived from a) the convenience of the printer (who finds it easier to separate text and figures), b) the thought processes of the writer (who reduces his cognitive load by finishing one task—say, drawing the figure—before beginning another—writing the text), and c) tradition (Sweller 1999). From the (crucial) viewpoint of improving student learning, there seems to be little theoretical support for organizing worked examples in the neat and tidy fashion seen in current textbooks.

What about redundancy in Figs 1 and 2? A moment’s consideration of the figures show that identifying redundancy is not quite as immediately clear cut as one might at first expect. The difficulty is that what is redundant depends on the degree of expertise of the learner. Looking at Fig. 2, novice problem solvers most likely do not recognize that the weight has been decomposed into components. These students need a textual explanation of why the trigonometric factors are present. Experienced problem solvers, on the other hand, do not need to be told—with text—that the weight of the cylinder in Fig. 2 has been decomposed into two components. These students know that immediately when seeing the trigonometric factors in the drawing. So the text is redundant for experienced students and should be omitted from the figure. For these students, much of the information in the worked examples is redundant and thus imposes a higher cognitive load than is imposed by simply working the problems. In fact, students who are approaching the status of experts may actually make more progress by solving problems than by studying worked examples with detailed textual explanations (Kalyuga, et al 2001).

Clearly, this aspect of redundancy being dependent on the expertise of the learner complicates the work of the designer of CLT-based instructional materials. One approach to this problem, when one is in doubt about whether or not material is redundant, is to make the conservative choice of including the possibly redundant material (Sweller 1999). Skipping redundant material (as an experienced learner would do) provides less cognitive load than the search strategy that a novice would have to follow, if the explanatory material has been omitted.

6

Page 7: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

Free-body diagram of cylinder A

Equations of equilibrium for cylinder A

N AB + 207.29 - P cos 25° + f

A = 0

N A - 444.54 - P sin 25° + f

AB = 0

Problem: The horizontal force P is applied to the center of cylinder A. Determine the largest value of P that can be applied without moving the uniform cylinders up the inclined plane.

1

B = 0.4

A = 0.6

AB = 0.5

mass A = 50 kg = mass

B (weight = (50)(9.81) = 490.5 N)

radius A = 300 mm = radius

B

A

BP

25°

F x = 0:

F y = 0:

y

xf

AN

A

N AB

f AB

y-component of weight = (490.5) cos 25° = 444.54 N

x-component of weight =(490.5) sin 25° = 207.29 N

A

300 mm

300 mm

P sin 25°

P cos 25°

The force P is pushing the cylinder up the plane. The friction force from the plane opposes this upward motion.

O

The Pcos 25° component of force P tends to roll the cylinder about point A. The friction force from cylinder B opposes this counterclockwise rolling motion

M O = 0: (300)f

A - (300)f AB = 0

6

4

5

25°

2 3

Fig. 2. Integration of text and graphics in CLT format

7

Page 8: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

Another approach to resolving the issue of whether or not to include possibly redundant material is to let the learner decide. This can be done by computerizing the worked examples. See, for example, Fig. 3, which shows the same example (as the previous figures) as it would appear on a computer screen in a CLT-based program employing both visual and auditory modes of transmitting information. When the user clicks on one of the circled numbers in the figure, the computer plays a message over the computer’s loudspeakers (or headphones in a computer lab). The message would be a recording of the text (or something very similar) associated with a circled number given in Fig. 2. This set-up has several attractive features: 1) as noted above, using the modality effect may increase learning; 2) the program user may solve the designer’s problem about deciding what material is or is not redundant by simply ignoring (if he or she is relatively experienced) the circled numbers near material that is understood or by clicking on everything (if he or she is a novice). In other words, the program allows learners considerable control over the cognitive load to be borne: novices click on explanations and thus avoid the cognitive load associated with having to search for a sub goal to problem solution; experienced students avoid the cognitive load of having to process information that they already know.

8

Page 9: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

F x = 0:

F y = 0:

y

xf

AN

A

N AB

f AB

(490.5) cos 25° = 444.54 N(490.5) sin 25° =

207.29 N

A

300 mm

300 mm

P sin 25°

P cos 25°O

M O = 0: (300)f

A - (300)f AB = 0

6

4

5

25°

2

3

N AB + 207.29 - P cos 25° + f

A = 0

N A - 444.54 - P sin 25° + f

AB = 0

Problem: The horizontal force P is applied to the center of cylinder A. Determine the largest value of P that can be applied without moving the uniform cylinders up the inclined plane.

1

B = 0.4

A = 0.6

AB = 0.5

mass A = 50 kg = mass

B (weight = (50)(9.81) = 490.5 N)

radius A = 300 mm = radius

B

A

BP

25°

Fig. 3. Problem as presented by computer program combining visual and auditory modes

9

Page 10: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

Worked examples as the primary vehicle of instructionThe second aspect of the CLT approach to worked examples that is new is the reliance on worked examples, rather than solving problems, as the primary vehicle of instruction. The most striking illustration of the successful use of worked examples (Zhu and Simon 1987) is one in which students learned algebra by studying worked examples only —no lectures, no textbook, no problems to solve—and completed an entire three-year course in two years and scored slightly better than students following the traditional approach. Results like these are discomfiting to many instructors. Indeed, the writer of this paper has spent twenty-five years as a mechanics instructor assigning homework problems to his students in the belief that that was the best way for them to learn. Furthermore, the writer has invested several thousand hours in developing educational computer programs that provide an unlimited number of problems (problems with randomly generated input data) for students to practice with and solve. Learning about CLT and experimental studies showing the superiority of studying worked examples over solving problems was surprising, to say the least.

Results of testing a prototypeTo test the CLT approach to teaching statics, the writer prepared a small number of worked examples and associated “paired problems,” problems that were similar to the worked examples and that were used for testing the students’ knowledge. Students in a statics class were given the examples to study and were then required to work in-class quizzes consisting of the paired problems. Several weeks later, after students knew their scores on the quizzes, a questionnaire was given to the class asking for opinions about the worked-example approach. The results from the questionnaire are given in Table 1 below.

Taken as a whole, the data in the table show that a large majority of the students preferred the worked-example approach over the traditional approach of working homework problems. Upon tabulating and reviewing the data in the table for the first time, the writer of this paper viewed the result with relief: the positive response of the students, even though the worked-examples approach is more stressful (because students must take a quiz), was an essential hurdle to pass if the writer was to continue working on the worked examples approach.

In Question 3, a large majority of students indicated that they learned more or at least the same amount from worked examples as from solving problems. Of course, students may not be good judges of how well they learn from different teaching techniques, and the writer had no control group with which to compare, so no claims can be made about the educational effectiveness of the prototype worked examples. It is encouraging, however, that students did not perceive that they learned less.

10

Page 11: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

Question 1 In the worked examples, text explaining a part of a diagram was placed right next to that part of the diagram, rather than in a separate paragraph at the beginning of the example. Was this helpful?Not helpful Helpful

Scale: 1-5 1 2 3 4 5No. of responses 0 3 4 24 30Question 2 Did you spend more time or less time studying worked examples

compared to what you would have spent solving the same problems?Less time More time

Scale: 1-5 1 2 3 4 5No. of responses 4 22 20 10 6Question 3 Do you think that you learned more studying worked examples than

you would have learned solving the same problems?Learned less

Learned more

Scale: 1-5 1 2 3 4 5No. of responses 6 7 15 18 13Question 4 On balance, which do you prefer (Note that “Studying worked

examples without an in-class quiz” is not an option)?Solving problems

Studying examples

Scale: 1-5 1 2 3 4 5No. of responses 7 8 7 21 16

Table 1. Results of questionnaire about student opinion of worked-examples

A caveat on worked examplesIt is important not to overstate the case for worked examples. A major disadvantage of their use, especially when they have been designed to minimize extraneous cognitive load (for example, by spatially integrating text and graphics), is that they may be “too easy,” that is, students may read rapidly through the examples without attempting to grasp their deeper significance. The result may be the “illusion of understanding” (Bannert 2002). To the extent that “learners’ mental effort largely influences the learning outcomes” (Stark et al. 2002), a means must be provided to ensure that students extract the ideas behind the example and make them their own—that is, acquire well-developed schemas. Requiring students to solve problems has the undeniable advantage of ensuring that at least some mental effort must be expended; otherwise no answer will be produced. In addition, solving a problem and producing an answer provides feedback to students about the effectiveness of their learning activities, one of Gagne’s nine “events of instruction” (Gagne, et al. 1992)

11

Page 12: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

Several ideas for overcoming this disadvantage of worked examples have been suggested (Cooper 1998, Sweller 1999). Among these are pairing a worked example with a similar problem to be solved. The student must study the worked example carefully to be able to solve the paired problem. Considerable care must be taken in designing the problem to be solved. On the one hand, if the problem resembles the worked example too closely, students may employ mindless “pattern-matching” that enables them to calculate an answer without understanding any of the underlying principles (no schema acquisition). On the other hand, if the problem does not resemble the worked example sufficiently closely, then the situation reverts to attempting to learn by solving problems—with the attendant waste of limited cognitive resources in a means-ends search.

Preparation of CLT worked examples. Constructing good examples involves three things: The author of the example must determine 1) which aspects of an example need explanation, 2) how best to explain them, and 3) which aspects should be ignored (because they cause extraneous cognitive load). These determinations are best met through a trial-and-error approach based on giving students a worked example, measuring how effective it is, as demonstrated by student performance on related quizzes, and then possibly revising the example in light of subsequent student performance and comments.

Design guidelines for dual modality instruction based on CLT principles are available in the literature (Kalyuga 2000): "… audiovisual instructional presentations might not be efficient if they do not eliminate any avoidable load on working memory. Generally, when dealing with diagrams and text: (a) Units of textual explanations should be presented in auditory rather than written form; (b) The same units of textual explanations should not be presented concurrently in both auditory and written form (if both auditory and written text are required, written materials should be delayed and presented after auditory explanations were fully articulated); (c) When presented in auditory form, textual explanations should be easily turned off or otherwise ignored by more experienced learners."

ConclusionThe CLT approach, including the modality effect and based on the study of worked examples, holds the promise of being a more effective way of teaching engineering problem-solving than traditional approaches. Given the large portion of the typical engineering curriculum that is devoted to teaching problem-solving skills, a significant improvement in efficiency in teaching would mean that more time would be available in an already over-burdened curriculum for important topics that currently are often treated superficially if at all. Furthermore, a CLT approach based on the principle of providing information in both visual and auditory form using the modality effect holds significant potential for improving future educational multi-media intended to develop problem solving skills.

12

Page 13: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

References

Allport, A. D., Antonis, B., and Reynolds, P. (1972). “On the division of attention: A disproof of the single channel hypothesis.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 225-235.

Anderson, J.R, et al (2004). “ACT-R Homepage.” http://act-r.psy.cmu.edu/about/ (accessed 8 May 2004)

Baddeley, A.D. (2003). “Working memory: looking back and looking forward.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4, 829 -839.

Bannert, M. (2002). “Commentary. Managing cognitive load—recent trends in cognitive load theory.” Learning and Instruction 12, 139-146.

Chase, W. G., and Simon, H.A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55-81.

Chi, M., Bassok, M., Lewis, M., Reimann, P. and Glaser, R. (1989). Self –explanations: How students study and use examples in problem solving. Cognitive Science, 13, 145-182.

Cooper, G., (1998). Research into Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design at UNSW, http://education.arts.unsw.edu.au/CLT_NET_Aug_97.HTML (accessed 8 May 2004)

De Groot, A. D. (1966). “Perception and memory versus thought: Some old ideas and recent findings. Problem Solving, B. Kleinmuntz eds., Wiley, New York, NY, 19-50.

Frick, R. (1984). “Using an auditory and visual short-term store to increase digit span. Memory and Cognition, 12, 507-51.

Gagne, R., Briggs, L. and Wager, W. (1992). Principles of Instructional Design (4th Ed.). HBJ College Publishers, Fort Worth, TX.

Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J. Sweller, J. 2001. “When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples.” Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 579-588.

Kalyuga S. (2000). “When using sound with a text or picture is not beneficial for learning.” Australian Journal of Educational Technology, 16(2), 161-172.

Kirschner, P. A. (2002). “Cognitive load theory: implications of cognitive load theory on the design of learning.” Learning and Instruction, 12, 1–10.

Larkin, H., McDermott, J., Simon, D., and Simon, H. (1980). “Models of competence in solving physics problems.” Cognitive Science, 11, 65-99.

13

Page 14: Introduction - School of Engineering - Civil Engineering · Web viewby Mark P. Rossow Southern Illinois University Edwardsville Edwardsville, IL 62026 May, 2005 Introduction This

Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.

Penney, C. (1989). “Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory.” Memory and Cognition, 17, 398-442.

Shelley, J. F., (1990). 800 Solved problems in vector mechanics for engineers statics. Schaum’s solved problems series, McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.

Simon, D. P., and Simon, H. A. (1978). “Individual differences in solving physics problems”, Children's thinking: What develops? R.S. Stigler, ed., Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Soar (2004). “Soar Homepage.” http://sitemaker.umich.edu/soar/

Stark, R., Mandl, H., Gruber, H., and Renkl, A. (2002). “Conditions and effects of example elaboration.” Learning and Instruction 12, 39-60.

Sweller, J. (1999). Instructional design in technical areas. ACER, Victoria, Australia.

Sweller, J., and Levine, M. (1982). “Effects of sub-goal density on means-ends analysis and learning.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 8, 463-474.

Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (1997). “When two sensory modes are better than one.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 257-287.

Van Merrienboer, J.J.G., Schuurman, J.G., de Croock, M.B.M., and Paas, F.G.W.C. (2002). “Redirecting learners’ attention during training: effects on cognitive load, transfer test performance and training efficiency.” Learning and Instruction 12, 11–37.

Zhu, X., and Simon, H.A. (1987). “Learning mathematics by examples and doing.” Cognition and Instruction, 4, 137-166.

14