individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · on the participant...

49
Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity during visuomotor adaptation are related to execution noise Zeb D. Jonker (1,2,4), Rick van der Vliet (1,2), Guido Maquelin (1), Joris van der Cruijsen (2), Gerard M. Ribbers (2,4), Ruud W. Selles (2,3), Opher Donchin (5) and Maarten A. Frens (1,@) Affiliations Departments of (1) Neuroscience, (2) Rehabilitation Medicine, (3) Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, Erasmus MC, University Medical Center Rotterdam, 3015 CN, Rotterdam, The Netherlands; (4) Rijndam Rehabilitation, 3015LJ Rotterdam, The Netherlands; (5) Department of Biomedical Engineering and Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience, Ben Gurion University of the Negev, 8499000, Be’er Sheva, Israel. @ Corresponding Author Erasmus MC Room EE-1477, PO Box 2040, 3000 CA Rotterdam, the Netherlands [email protected] 1 . CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under a The copyright holder for this preprint this version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581 doi: bioRxiv preprint

Upload: others

Post on 19-Jul-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity during visuomotor adaptation are

related to execution noise

Zeb D. Jonker (1,2,4), Rick van der Vliet (1,2), Guido Maquelin (1), Joris van der Cruijsen (2), Gerard M. Ribbers

(2,4), Ruud W. Selles (2,3), Opher Donchin (5) and Maarten A. Frens (1,@)

Affiliations

Departments of (1) Neuroscience, (2) Rehabilitation Medicine, (3) Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery,

Erasmus MC, University Medical Center Rotterdam, 3015 CN, Rotterdam, The Netherlands; (4) Rijndam

Rehabilitation, 3015LJ Rotterdam, The Netherlands; (5) Department of Biomedical Engineering and Zlotowski

Center for Neuroscience, Ben Gurion University of the Negev, 8499000, Be’er Sheva, Israel.

@ Corresponding Author

Erasmus MC Room EE-1477, PO Box 2040, 3000 CA Rotterdam, the Netherlands [email protected]

1

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 2: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

ABSTRACT

Frontal midline EEG activity has been found to correlate with error magnitude during motor

adaptation. We replicated a previous visuomotor adaptation experiment with very small

perturbations, likely to invoke implicit adaptation, in a new group of 60 participants and combined it

with EEG recordings. We used this data to explore 1) whether frontal midline activity will be evoked

in the absence of awareness of the perturbation; 2) whether frontal midline activity is related to

implicit adaptation; 3) whether individual differences in frontal midline activity are related to

individual differences in motor learning. The results showed that frontal midline theta activity (FMΘ)

is also present during small perturbations, does not drive between-trial error correction, and that the

sensitivity of FMΘ to error magnitude was smaller for participants with greater execution noise. This

relation between FMΘ-error-sensitivity and execution noise could be fully explained by looking at the

relationship between FMΘ and error probability. This implies that frontal midline theta activity

represents a surprise-like saliency signal, potentially driving awareness and cognitive control in

situations with more salient errors.

2

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 3: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

INTRODUCTION

In motor learning, movement errors are used to update motor commands for subsequent actions.

We call this process motor adaptation. Motor adaptation can occur within a trial during continuous

movements, or between trials during discrete ballistic movements such as swinging a golf club or

throwing darts. In motor adaptation tasks, perturbations of visual or proprioceptive feedback drive

adjustments of the motor command to achieve the intended goal of the movement. These

adjustments may reflect unconscious implicit adaptation, conscious explicit strategy, or both (Taylor

et al., 2014). Typically, smaller gradual perturbations result in implicit adaptation whereas larger

sudden perturbations have been found to induce explicit strategy (Malfait and Ostry, 2004; Werner

et al., 2019).

Linear dynamical models of error-based learning that include an adaptation rate, a retention

rate, planning noise (state noise), and execution noise (output noise) have been shown to

qualitatively resemble between-trial implicit adaptation of reaching movements to a gradual

perturbation signal with incremental perturbation steps (Cheng and Sabes, 2007). In this model class,

planning noise represents stochastic noise in central movement planning, whereas execution noise

represents stochastic noise in peripheral movement execution that is not included in the motor

command (van Beers, 2009). Van der Vliet et al. (2018) showed that such a model can also explain

differences in motor learning between individuals. In that study we presented a novel Bayesian

fitting procedure to estimate the model parameters for each individual participant. In agreement

with optimal Kalman filter theory (Kalman, 1960), individuals with relatively more planning noise and

less execution noise showed a higher adaptation rate than individuals with relatively less planning

noise and more execution noise. However, an important question regarding these mathematical

models is the extent to which they reflect the reality of the underlying neurophysiological processes

(Tan et al., 2016).

Independently from the mathematical modelling approach, electroencephalography (EEG)

has been used increasingly to explore neurophysiological correlates of learning. Most studies have

3

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 4: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

focused on frontal midline activity, which has been found to correlate with error magnitude in motor

adaptation tasks (Krigolson et al., 2008; Anguera et al., 2009; Vocat et al., 2011; Torrecillos et al.,

2014; Maclean et al., 2015; Arrighi et al., 2016; Reuter et al., 2018). However, a recent study with

very small gradual perturbations did not find a relation between error magnitude and frontal midline

activity (Palidis et al., 2019). Furthermore, to our knowledge, no prior studies have tested whether

frontal midline activity was driving adaptation in the consecutive trial.

The current study therefore explores 1) whether frontal midline activity will be evoked in the

absence of awareness of the perturbation; 2) whether frontal midline activity is related to implicit

adaptation; 3) whether individual differences in frontal midline activity are related to individual

differences in motor learning. To achieve these objectives, we replicated our previous visuomotor

adaptation experiment (van der Vliet et al., 2018) in a new group of 60 participants and combined it

with EEG recordings.

4

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 5: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

METHODS

Participants

We recruited 60 right-handed (Oldfield, 1971) participants (19 men and 41 women; mean age = 25.6

years, range = 18-61) without any medical condition that might interfere with motor performance.

Participants received a small financial token for traveling and time compensation. The study was

performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the medical ethics

committee of the Erasmus MC University Medical Centre.

Experimental procedure

The experimental procedure was adapted from van der Vliet et al. (2018). Participants were seated in

front of a horizontal projection screen with a robotic handle underneath. This handle was situated at

elbow height and could be moved in the horizontal plane. The position of the handle was projected

on top of the screen as a cursor (green circle 5mm diameter). Furthermore, the screen displayed an

origin (white circle 10mm diameter) and a target (red circle 10mm diameter) at fixed positions. The

origin was located approximately 40cm straight in front of the participant and the target was

projected exactly 10cm behind the origin, approximately 50cm in front of the participant.

Participants were instructed to hold the handle in their dominant right hand and move the cursor

from the origin through the target in one smooth ballistic reaching movement. To prevent direct

feedback of hand position underneath the screen, participants were wearing an apron that was

secured to the top of the screen.

The experiment consisted of three types of trials: no-vision, unperturbed, and perturbed

trials (Figure 1A). At the start of each trial, participants held the cursor in the origin. The target

appeared after one second, indicating that the participant should start the movement. In all trial

types, the cursor disappeared when the handle left the origin. In no-vision trials, the cursor did not

reappear during the entire movement. In unperturbed and perturbed trials, the cursor reappeared

5

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 6: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

when the handle distance from the origin exceeded 5cm. However, in perturbed trials, the cursor

was projected at a predefined angle relative to the actual handle position.

The movement was damped with a force cushion (3.6Ns/m, ramped up over 7.5ms) when

the handle position exceeded 10cm distance from the origin, and thus exceeded the target distance.

We defined this time point as movement offset. In perturbed and unperturbed trials, the cursor froze

at this time point to provide feedback on the endpoint error. Furthermore, the target changed color.

If the movement duration was too short (<400ms), the target stayed red, if the movement duration

was correct (400-600ms), the target turned white, and if the duration was too long (>600ms), the

target turned blue. We instructed participants that these colors were merely a guideline to ensure

ballistic movements without online adjustments and emphasized that the goal of the task was to hit

the target. One second after onset of the force cushion, the robot pushed the handle back to the

starting position. When the handle was at the origin, the cursor reappeared at the handle position.

The experiment consisted of 900 trials, divided into two blocks of 450 trials: a baseline block

followed by a perturbation block (Figure 1B). The exact order of the trial types was randomized for

each participant. The baseline block contained 225 unperturbed trials and 225 no-vision trials in a

completely randomized order. In contrast, the perturbation block contained 400 perturbation trials

and 50 no-vision trials in a pseudorandomized order: in every epoch of 9 trials, there was one

randomly interspersed no-vision trial. Thus, the experiment contained 625 trials with visual feedback.

In the perturbation block, the perturbation angle changed from 0° to 9° to -9° and back to 0° with

increments of 1.5° every 8 to 12 trials. The experiment was paused after every 150 trials for

approximately 2 minutes.

Movement recording and preprocessing

The experiment was controlled by a custom C++ program. The position and velocity signals of the

robot handle were sampled at 500Hz. The velocity signal was filtered with an exponential moving

average filter (smoothing factor = 0.18s). Movement onset was defined as the time point when

6

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 7: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

movement velocity exceeded 0.03 m/s and movement offset was defined as the moment that the

hand location exceeded 10cm distance from the starting position. The hand angle was defined as the

angle between the vector connecting the origin and the hand at the end of the movement, relative

to the vector connecting the origin and the target. The clockwise direction was defined as positive.

Trials were removed if movement duration exceeded 1s or if the absolute hand angle exceeded 30

degrees. On average, 1% (range [0 18]) of the total 900 movement trials were excluded per

participant.

Movement analysis

From the movement recordings, we estimated the execution noise, planning noise, and adaptation

rate of each participant. The analysis was performed with Bayesian Markov Chain Monte Carlo

simulations in JAGS (Plummer, 2017) and is extensively described in van der Vliet et al. (2018). In

short, we fitted a state-space model of trial-to-trial behavior (Cheng and Sabes, 2006).

𝑥[𝑠, 𝑛 + 1] = 𝐴[𝑠] ∗ 𝑥[𝑠, 𝑛] − 𝐵[𝑠] ∗ 𝑒[𝑠, 𝑛] + 𝜂[𝑠, 𝑛] (1)

𝑦[𝑠, 𝑛] = 𝑥[𝑠, 𝑛] + 𝜀[𝑠, 𝑛] (2)

𝑒[𝑠, 𝑛] = 𝑦[𝑠, 𝑛] + 𝑝[𝑠, 𝑛] (3)

𝜂[𝑠, 𝑛]~𝑁(0, 𝜎9:[𝑠]) (4)

𝜀[𝑠, 𝑛]~𝑁(0, 𝜎<:[𝑠]) (5)

For each trial 𝑛 of participant 𝑠, 𝑥[𝑠, 𝑛] is the movement plan, 𝑦[𝑠, 𝑛] the angle of the hand relative

to the target at the endpoint, 𝑝[𝑠, 𝑛] the perturbation angle and 𝑒[𝑠, 𝑛] the error angle of the cursor

on the screen relative to the target. Participant-specific motor adaptation parameters estimated with

this model are the retention rate 𝐴[𝑠], which is the fractional retention of the movement plan 𝑥[𝑠, 𝑛]

in the previous trial, and the adaptation rate 𝐵[𝑠], which is the fractional change caused by the error

𝑒[𝑠, 𝑛]in the previous trial. The noise terms include planning noise 𝜂[𝑠, 𝑛], and execution noise

7

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 8: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

𝜀[𝑠, 𝑛]. Planning noise and execution noise are modeled as zero-mean Gaussians. The standard

deviations of these Gaussians 𝜎9[𝑠] and 𝜎<[𝑠] represent the magnitude of planning and execution

noise for each participant. The participant-specific motor learning parameters were estimated

hierarchically with uninformative priors. Additionally, we calculated the lag-1 autocorrelation (R(-1)),

and the standard deviation of the signed errors (𝜎=) for each participant.

EEG recording and preprocessing

Participants were wearing a 128 channel EEG cap connected to a 136 channel REFA system (TMSi,

Oldenzaal, The Netherlands). The EEG data were recorded with a sampling rate of 2,048Hz from 62

EEG channels: FP1, FPz, FP2, AF7, AF3, AF4, AF8, F7, F5, F3, F1, Fz, F2, F4, F6, F8, FT7 FC5, FC3, FC1,

FCz, FC2, FC4, FC6, FT8, T7, C5, C3, C1, Cz, C2, C4, C6, T8, TP7, CP5, CP3, CP1, CPz, CP2, CP4, CP6, TP8,

P7, P5, P3, P1, Pz, P2, P4, P6, P8, PO7, PO5, PO3, POz, PO4, PO6, PO8, O1, Oz, O2, and 2 EOG

channels. The EOG electrodes were located between the eyebrows and to the right of the right eye.

Impedance was kept below 4 kΩ. The recording of each channel was referenced to the average signal

across all channels.

After recording, the EEG signals were preprocessed with the EEGLAB toolbox (version

14.1.2b; Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience, La Jolla, USA) and custom scripts in MATLAB

version 2018b (Mathworks, Natick, USA). The raw average referenced EEG signals were digitally

filtered between 2-40Hz with a 6th order Butterworth filter, cut into trial epochs of 2000ms centered

around the onset of visual feedback, and down-sampled to 128Hz. Non-stereotypical artifacts were

automatically removed by excluding trials in which the average log power was an outlier compared

to the other trials within the same channel (absolute z-score >3.5). Data quality of the remaining

trials was verified by visual inspection. Similarly, stereotypical artifacts were automatically removed

by performing a fast-independent component analysis (Delorme et al., 2007) and excluding

components in which the average log power was an outlier (absolute z-score >3.5). Visual inspection

confirmed that the removed components corresponded to eye movements. The other components

8

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 9: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

were projected back to the channel space. On average, 40 (range [14 79]) of the 900 EEG traces were

excluded and 1.1 (range [1 2]) of the 64 components were removed. The remaining traces were used

for analysis in the time domain.

For the analysis in the frequency domain, the remaining EEG traces were first filtered with a

surface Laplacian to improve the spatial resolution (Perrin et al., 1989). Subsequently, the traces

were decomposed into their time-frequency representations using Morlet wavelet convolution

applied to the concatenated trial data (Tallon-Baudry et al., 1996; Cohen, 2014). The wavelet

frequencies ranged from 2 to 30 Hz in 29 steps of 1Hz (with the number of cycles ranging from 4 to

10 in 29 logarithmically-spaced steps, respectively, for the 29 frequencies). After convolution, the

deconcatenated traces were trimmed between –800ms and 800ms, and centered on the onset of

visual feedback to remove edge artifacts. Consecutively, the log power at each time-frequency point

within each trial was normalized as a percentage change relative to the average log power of that

frequency in the predefined baseline window (from 800ms until 600ms before visual feedback)

across all trials for the subject.

EEG analysis

The EEG analysis had three parts: a trial level analysis in the time domain, a trial level analysis in the

frequency domain and a participant level analysis. We used linear mixed-effects models (MATLAB’s

fitlme function) to estimate trial level effects across participants and a multivariate linear regression

(MATLAB’s fitlm function) to estimate effects on the participant level. All variables were z-score

normalized for the purpose of the regression. Tables show the normalized effects in order to

compare their relative strength. When effect sizes are shown in their original units, they have been

transformed back using the standard deviations of the dependent and independent variables.

A signed error was defined as the angle between the cursor on the screen relative to the

target; error magnitude was defined as the absolute of the signed error; and error correction was

defined as the hand angle in the following trial minus the hand angle in the corresponding trial,

9

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 10: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

multiplied with the sign (1 or -1) of the error angle in the corresponding trial. Thus, negative values

represent corrections in the direction of the target, whereas positive values represent corrections in

the opposite direction. In the EEG analysis, we only included trials with visual feedback.

For the trial level analysis in the time domain, a separate regression was performed for

different time windows. We used a fine-grained collection of windows for generating figures. For a

given time window, we regressed EEG amplitude on signed error, error magnitude, and error

correction, as well as possible confounding variables such as the number of previous trials and the

feedback color (which indicated whether movement duration was within the desired range, see

above).

𝐸𝐸𝐺[𝑠, 𝑛]~𝛼[𝑠] + 𝛽B[𝑠] ∗ |𝑒|[𝑠, 𝑛] + 𝛽:[𝑠] ∗ e[𝑠, 𝑛] + 𝛽E[𝑠] ∗ 𝑐[𝑠, 𝑛] +

𝛽G[𝑠] ∗ 𝑡[𝑠, 𝑛] + 𝛽I[𝑠] ∗ 𝑓KLMNO[𝑠, 𝑛] + 𝛽P[𝑠] ∗ 𝑓QMRS[𝑠, 𝑛]

(6)

For each trial 𝑛 of participant 𝑠, 𝐸𝐸𝐺[𝑠, 𝑛] is the average EEG amplitude in a time window, |𝑒|[𝑠, 𝑛] is

the movement error magnitude, e[𝑠, 𝑛] is the signed error, 𝑐[𝑠, 𝑛]is error correction, 𝑡[𝑠, 𝑛] is the

original trial number and 𝑓KLMNO[𝑠, 𝑛] and 𝑓QMRS[𝑠, 𝑛] are binary variables indicating if the movement

duration was “too short” (𝑓KLMNO[𝑠, 𝑛]= 1) or “too long” (𝑓QMRS[𝑠, 𝑛] = 1). All participant-specific

estimates were modeled as random effects around the group average fixed effects. Throughout the

paper, we often refer to the coefficient of the error magnitude (𝛽B[𝑠]) as the EEG-error-sensitivity.

For the trial level analysis in the frequency domain, a separate regression was used for each

time-frequency window. We used both fine-grained time-frequency binning to generate figures and

a larger bin (4-8Hz and 100-600ms) to analyze frontal midline theta activity in channel FCz (Cavanagh

et al., 2012; Arrighi et al., 2016; Savoie et al., 2018). This bin was chosen after visual inspection of the

fine-grained picture. For these analyses, we use the linear mixed-effects model in Eq. 6, but with

𝐸𝐸𝐺[𝑠, 𝑛] representing the average EEG power in the time-frequency bin being analyzed. We

compared different versions of the model with a backward elimination method. Stepwise removal of

10

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 11: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

variables with the smallest effect size was continued as long as the Bayesian Information Criterion

(BIC) decreased. The model for which further removal of the smallest effect size would increase the

BIC was taken to be the one that best explained the data. FMΘ-error-sensitivity was defined as the

relation between frontal midline theta power (FMΘ) and error magnitude in this final model (𝛽B[𝑠]).

For the participant level analysis, we tested for linear relationships between FMΘ-error-

sensitivity (𝛽1[𝑠]), planning noise 𝜎9[𝑠], execution noise 𝜎<[𝑠],and adaptation rate 𝐵[𝑠]. We tested

two regression models specifically: FMΘ-error-sensitivity regressed on the noises and adaptation

rate, and adaptation rate regressed on the noises and the FMΘ-error-sensitivity. The choice of these

two models reflects an underlying assumption that the noises characterize an individual while

adaptation and neural signals vary under different conditions.

𝐵[𝑠]~𝛼 + 𝛾B ∗ 𝜎9[𝑠] + 𝛾: ∗ 𝜎<[𝑠] + 𝛾E ∗ 𝛽B[𝑠] (7)

𝛽B[𝑠]~𝛼 + 𝛾B ∗ 𝜎9[𝑠] + 𝛾: ∗ 𝜎<[𝑠] + 𝛾E ∗ 𝐵[𝑠] (8)

In these models, we used the participant-specific point estimates from the movement analysis and

the trial level EEG analysis. In order to compare the strength of the correlations, all variables were z-

score normalized. Stepwise removal of variables with the smallest effect size was continued as long

as the Bayesian Information Criterion decreased, and the last model for which this criterion held was

selected. As an internal control, we also investigated whether EEG sensitivity to the signed error

(𝛽:[𝑠]) in occipital channels PO5 and PO6 (4-8Hz and 0-500ms) was related to the participant-specific

motor learning parameters.

After performing the analysis for the first time, we redid the trial level analysis in the

frequency domain with error probability (𝛽U[𝑠]) instead of error magnitude (𝛽B[𝑠]) (Eq. 6). Error

probability was defined as the probability for an error magnitude larger than the error magnitude in

the corresponding trial, assuming a normal distribution for the errors.

11

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 12: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

𝑒𝑟𝑟𝑜𝑟𝑝𝑟𝑜𝑏𝑎𝑏𝑖𝑙𝑖𝑡𝑦 = 𝑝(|𝑒| > |𝑒|[𝑠, 𝑛]) = 1 − ]^𝐶𝐷𝐹]𝑒[𝑠, 𝑛], 0, 𝜎=:[𝑠]b − 0.5^b ∗ 2 (9)

In formula 9, 𝐶𝐷𝐹(𝑥, 𝜇, 𝜎) is the cumulative density function of the normal distribution with

mean 𝜇 and standard deviation 𝜎 at the point x.

Data and code availability

The JAGS code for the Bayesian state-space model can be accessed without restrictions at https://

github.com/rickvandervliet/Bayesian-state-space. Other scripts and data are available on reasonable

request.

12

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 13: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

RESULTS

Movement analysis

On average, target onset, movement onset, and movement offset occurred at -441ms (range across

participants [-512 -401]), -193ms [-247 -151] and 78ms [50 118] respectively, relative to the onset of

visual feedback. Over the course of the experiment, participants adapted their reaching movements

to the perturbation signal (Figure 2). As a result, the average error magnitude was similar in the

baseline block and the perturbation block (Figure 2). In 40% [23 58] of the trials, the cursor was

completely inside the target (error magnitude < 1.43°) and in an additional 47% [36 51] of the trials,

the cursor was partially inside the target (error magnitude < 4.29°) (Figure 3). Figure 3 also highlights

the error distribution of 5 participants with different error distributions whose data will be used as

example data in subsequent figures. The Bayesian fitting procedure showed that the average

planning noise, execution noise, and adaptation rate were 0.47° [0.23 0.78], 2.46° [1.40 4.49], and

0.14 [0.06 0.27], respectively. The movement data of the 5 example participants, ordered by

increasing execution noise, is illustrated in Figure 4.

We inspected the validity of the Bayesian fitting procedure by correlating the estimated

participant-specific factors with the lag-1 autocorrelation, and the standard deviation of the signed

errors (Supplementary Figures 1-2). As expected, planning noise was related to the lag-1

autocorrelation in the baseline block (normalized slope = 0.58, CI95% = [0.36 .79]) and execution

noise was strongly related to the standard deviation of signed errors throughout the experiment

(normalized slope = 0.97, CI95% = [0.91 1.03]).

EEG analysis

In the time domain, the overall EEG amplitude showed a negative peak over the motor cortices

between approximately -100 and 100ms and a large positive peak between approximately 200 and

400ms after visual feedback distributed spatially from the frontal midline region back to the parietal

cortex (Figure 5A). Only the positive peak showed a correlation with error magnitude. Within this

13

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 14: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

larger response, the EEG amplitude over the frontal midline region showed negative sensitivity to

error magnitude between 200 and 300ms and positive sensitivity between 300 and 400ms (Figure

5B). The correlation with signed error was much smaller, but some relationships could be seen in the

occipital cortices between approximately 100 and 300ms (Figure 5C). Any relationship between EEG

amplitude and error correction was vanishingly small (Figure 5D).

In the frequency domain, overall theta power showed widespread synchronization between -

400ms (approximately target onset) and 600ms (Figure 6A). This can be divided into two phases with

a first phase before visual feedback over the frontal midline region and the somatosensory cortices

and a second phase after visual feedback over the frontal midline region and the occipital cortices,

but then migrating to the prefrontal cortex after approximately 400ms. Within this second

synchronization, theta power over the frontal midline region was related to error magnitude (Figure

6B) and also weakly related to signed error in the counterclockwise direction (Figure 6C).

Furthermore, theta power in the left and right occipital cortices was related to signed error in the

contralateral direction (Figure 6C). Finally, we did not find a relation between theta power and error

correction (Figure 6D). Figure 7 illustrates that EEG-error-sensitivity in the frontal midline region

(channel FCz) was most prominent in the theta (4-8Hz) frequency band, with foothills in the delta (2-

3Hz) and alpha (9-14Hz) frequency bands. These foothills of EEG-error-sensitivity are also visible in

the scalp plots of delta and alpha power (Supplementary Figures 3-4). In this experiment we did not

see a relation between beta power and error magnitude, signed error, nor error correction

(Supplementary Figure 5).

Figure 8 illustrates that the positive and negative error related peaks over the frontal midline

region (Figure 8A-B) are reflections of theta waves in the frequency domain (Figure 8C-D). The signal

in the frequency domain contains less noise and is unipolar, which makes the analysis less

susceptible for the borders of the timewindow (Supplementary Figure 6). In channel FCz, the relation

between theta power and error magnitude was visible from approximately 100 to 600ms. In this

post-feedback window, larger error magnitudes resulted in higher FMΘ (Figure 8F). This relation was

14

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 15: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

slightly stronger for errors in the counterclockwise direction (Figure 8E). Backward elimination of

regression terms confirmed that post-feedback FMΘ activity was related to error magnitude (FMΘ-

error-sensitivity), the number of preceding trials, and the signed error, and not to error correction

nor changes in the target color as feedback on the movement duration (Table 1). The average FMΘ-

error-sensitivity was 0.57% change in theta power per 1 degree of error magnitude (range across

participants [0.07 1.84]). While post-feedback FMΘ activity gradually decreased over the course of

the experiment, FMΘ-error-sensitivity was consistent across the baseline and perturbation blocks

(Figure 8G). Furthermore, Supplementary Figure 7 illustrates that FMΘ-error sensitivity was also

present within the subset of trials that were completely inside the target (that is when error

magnitude < 1.43).

On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated

to planning noise and negatively related to execution noise and were not further explained by FMΘ-

error-sensitivity (Table 2, Figure 9). Conversely, individual differences in FMΘ-error-sensitivity were

best explained by execution noise (Table 3, Figure 10). The control analysis showed that individual

differences in the occipital sensitivity to signed error (Supplementary Table 1, Supplementary Figure

8) were not related to execution noise (Supplementary Table 2-3, Supplementary Figure 9).

The effect of execution noise level on FMΘ sensitivity led us to consider the possibility that

FMΘ reflected the extent to which a given error was expected, considering the individual standard

deviation of the signed errors. To test this, we redid the analysis in the frequency domain with error

probability instead of error magnitude (Figure 11) and found that this improved the model (Table 4).

Using FMΘ sensitivity to error probability instead of error magnitude reduced the individual

differences in EEG-sensitivity and effectively removed the relation between EEG-error-sensitivity and

execution noise (Table 5, Figure 12). This is further illustrated by the looking at the FMΘ power of the

5 participants who have been used as examples in previous figures (Figure 13).

15

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 16: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

DISCUSSION

This study shows that frontal midline theta activity (FMΘ) is also present during very small

perturbations, does not drive between-trial error correction and that the sensitivity of FMΘ to error

magnitude was smaller for participants with greater execution noise. With 60 participants and, on

average, 585 trials (range [430 611]) per participant, this is the largest study on visuomotor

adaptation and EEG activity to date, allowing us to investigate individual differences in motor

learning and FMΘ. In group average EEG activity, FMΘ appeared to reflect error magnitude (Figure

8). However, this relationship between FMΘ and error magnitude (FMΘ-error-sensitivity) was much

weaker in individuals with a larger execution noise and thus a larger standard deviation of signed

errors. We reasoned that if FMΘ is affecting subsequent errors, we should see a relation between

trial-to-trial differences in FMΘ and error correction. However, our results show no such relationship

(Figure 7). Furthermore, we see that individual differences in FMΘ-error-sensitivity are strongly

related to execution noise and not to adaptation rate (Figure 10). This left us with an alternative

interpretation: FMΘ may reflect a surprise-like saliency signal. We tested this by calculating the error

probability in each trial (Figure 11). Defining FMΘ-probability-sensitivity instead of FMΘ-error-

sensitivity reduced the individual differences and substantially weakened the correlation with

execution noise (Figure 13). These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that FMΘ reflects

surprise.

This study replicates our earlier findings that individual differences in adaptation rate are

positively related to planning noise and negatively related to execution noise (van der Vliet et al.,

2018) which strengthens the evidence that the adaptation rate is tuned to the noise terms according

to Kalman filter theory (Kalman, 1960). It also expands the previous literature on post-feedback

frontal midline activity in three essential ways.

First of all, this study shows that frontal midline theta activity (FMΘ) is present during very

small gradual perturbations. Arrighi et al. (2016) only found a relationship between frontal midline

activity and error magnitude for error magnitudes above a certain threshold. Furthermore, Palidis et

16

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 17: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

al. (2019) only used very small gradual perturbations, but did not see a relationship between frontal

midline activity and error magnitude. Finally, Savoie et al. (2018) found that FMΘ, corrected for error

magnitude, was higher when participants were made aware of a perturbation paradigm and

instructed how to counteract it. The results of these previous studies suggest that the relation

between frontal midline activity and error magnitude may be dependent on awareness of the

perturbation paradigm. However, the current study shows that FMΘ-error-sensitivity is present

during very small perturbations and even in the absence of perturbations (during movement

calibration in the baseline block), making it safe to assume that participants were unaware of

external perturbations and that adaptation was implicit.

Secondly, this study shows that trial-to-trial differences frontal midline theta activity (FMΘ)

are not related to between trial error corrections. To our knowledge, this is the first study to

investigate the relation between EEG activity and adaptation on the trial level and our results are a

strong argument that frontal midline activity does not boost implicit motor adaptation, which is

thought to be dependent on the olivocerebellar system (Shadmehr and Krakauer, 2008; De Zeeuw et

al., 2011).

Third and finally, this study shows that FMΘ is better explained by error probability than by

error magnitude. This supports the hypothesis of Torrecillos et al. (2014) that frontal midline theta

activity during motor adaptation may represent a surprise-like saliency signal and is in apparent

contrast with the interpretation of Palidis et al. (2019) who concluded that frontal midline activity is

related to reward error. Interestingly, the difference between our interpretation and theirs echoes a

long-standing dispute on frontal midline theta in the cognitive literature (Cavanagh and Frank, 2014;

Sambrook and Goslin, 2015). The argument of Palidis et al. was made by process of elimination. In a

first experiment with endpoint error feedback, they ruled out a relation between frontal midline

activity and the magnitude of the endpoint errors, which were all inside the target (success). In a

second experiment with probabilistic reward feedback, they showed a relation between frontal

17

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 18: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

midline activity and reward. Combining these two, they concluded that frontal midline activity in the

second experiment was solely related to reward error and not to saliency.

We argue that they may not have found a relation between frontal midline activity and the

endpoint errors in their first experiment due to two major differences in their methodology

compared to ours. This would weaken the process of elimination argument such that the frontal

midline activity in their second experiment may also reflect a surprise-like saliency signal. The first

factor is that their analysis was in the time domain and not the frequency domain. The average EEG

amplitude of theta oscillations (4-8Hz) in a time window of 150ms may include both positive and

negative peaks that partially cancel each other out (Supplementary Figure 6A-B). The second

methodological factor is the difference in the statistical power of the two data sets. With fewer

participants and fewer trials, the presence and significance of these smaller peaks within a larger

positive response may not have been apparent (Supplementary Figure 6C-D). Indeed, our study

shows FMΘ sensitivity to error magnitude even in those trials where the cursor was completely

inside the target (Supplementary Figure 7).

Nevertheless, an important limitation of our own study is that we cannot exclude the

possibility that the participants constructed their own subgoals. FMΘ was not related to feedback on

the movement duration, which confirms that participants understood the instructions that the goal

of the task was to hit the target. However, participants may have constructed their own subgoals in

which endpoints in the middle of the target were more rewarding than endpoints slightly off-center

but still completely inside the target. A dedicated experiment which successfully dissociates error

and reward signals, possibly using varying target sizes and reward contingencies, is necessary to

resolve this important point.

Other limitations are introduced by the focus on post-feedback EEG activity and implicit

adaptation. While we find that frontal midline theta does not drive implicit adaptation, the focus on

implicit adaptation means that we cannot exclude the possibility that it drives explicit strategy.

Another important point is that we do not see error sensitivity in the beta band. This could be due to

18

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 19: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

a trial structure that was not properly designed to uncover beta band error sensitivity. Previously,

beta sensitivity to error magnitude has been found in the pre-movement period and the post-

movement beta rebound (Tan et al., 2014; Torrecillos et al., 2015). However, we did not have a

preparatory pre-movement period in our task. Furthermore, the use of a force cushion and the

automatic pushback at the end of each trial may have obscured the post-movement period.

Regarding the relation between frontal midline theta and error probability, we hypothesize

that FMΘ may drive cognitive control (Cavanagh and Frank, 2014). While this study convincingly

shows that FMΘ is not dependent on awareness, it is still true that FMΘ may influence awareness

and cognitive control. In a visuomotor adaptation task with larger errors or otherwise more salient

errors, FMΘ power may facilitate awareness of changes in the environment. This, in turn, might

evoke cognitive strategies such as cautious movement, memorization or re-aiming. In individuals

with a larger execution noise, the same error magnitude may not indicate a change in the external

environment and would thus warrant less cognitive control. An alternative to this hypothesis is that

FMΘ power reflects estimation uncertainty (Tan et al., 2016). When confronted with unlikely events,

individuals may drop the priors of their internal model and exhibit increased search behavior

(planning noise) in order to form new priors. Thus, an experiment where estimation uncertainty and

the ability to develop explicit strategies are manipulated independently would be an important step

in understanding how the FMΘ response ultimately affects behavior.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

None.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We would like to thank Nicole Malfait and Dimitrios J. Palidis for constructive feedback on the

manuscript.

19

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 20: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

REFERENCES

Anguera JA, Seidler RD, Gehring WJ (2009) Changes in performance monitoring during sensorimotor

adaptation. J Neurophysiol 102:1868–1879.

Arrighi P, Bonfiglio L, Minichilli F, Cantore N, Carboncini MC, Piccotti E, Rossi B, Andre P (2016) EEG

theta dynamics within frontal and parietal cortices for error processing during reaching

movements in a prism adaptation study altering visuo-motor predictive planning. PLoS One

11:1–27.

Cavanagh JF, Frank MJ (2014) Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn Sci

18:414–421

Cavanagh JF, Zambrano-Vazquez L, Allen JJB (2012) Theta lingua franca: A common mid-frontal

substrate for action monitoring processes. Psychophysiology 49:220–238.

Cheng S, Sabes PN (2006) Modeling Sensorimotor Learning with Linear Dynamical Systems. Neural

Comput 18:760–793.

Cheng S, Sabes PN (2007) Calibration of visually guided reaching is driven by error-corrective learning

and internal dynamics. J Neurophysiol 97:3057–3069.

Cohen MX (2014) Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice. In: MIT Press.

De Zeeuw CI, Hoebeek FE, Bosman LWJ, Schonewille M, Witter L, Koekkoek SK (2011) Spatiotemporal

firing patterns in the cerebellum. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:327–344.

Delorme A, Sejnowski T, Makeig S (2007) Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higher-

order statistics and independent component analysis. Neuroimage 34:1443–1449.

Kalman RE (1960) A new approach to linear filtering and prediction problems. J Fluids Eng Trans

ASME 82:35–45.

Krigolson OE, Holroyd CB, Van Gyn G, Heath M (2008) Electroencephalographic correlates of target

and outcome errors. Exp Brain Res 190:401–411.

Maclean SJ, Hassall CD, Ishigami Y, Krigolson OE, Eskes GA (2015) Using brain potentials to

understand prism adaptation: The error-related negativity and the p300. Front Hum Neurosci

20

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 21: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

9:1–14.

Malfait N, Ostry DJ (2004) Is interlimb transfer of force-field adaptation a cognitive response to the

sudden introduction of load? J Neurosci 24:8084–8089.

Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory.

Neuropsychologia 9:97–113.

Palidis DJ, Cashaback JGA, Gribble PL (2019) Neural signatures of reward and sensory error feedback

processing in motor learning. J Neurophysiol 121:1561–1574.

Perrin F, Pernier J, Bertrand O, Echallier JF (1989) Spherical splines for scalp potential and current

density mapping. Clin Neurophysiol:184–187.

Plummer M (2017) JAGS Version 4.3.0 user manual. :0–41.

Reuter EM, Pearcey GEP, Carroll TJ (2018) Greater neural responses to trajectory errors are

associated with superior force field adaptation in older adults. Exp Gerontol 110:105–117

Sambrook TD, Goslin J (2015) A Neural reward prediction error revealed by a meta-analysis of ERPs

using great grand averages. Psychol Bull 141:213–235.

Savoie F-A, Thénault F, Whittingstall K, Bernier P-M (2018) Visuomotor Prediction Errors Modulate

EEG Activity Over Parietal Cortex. Sci Rep 8:1–16.

Shadmehr R, Krakauer JW (2008) A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Exp Brain Res

185:359–381

Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O, Delpuech C, Pernier J (1996) Stimulus specificity of phase-locked and

non-phase-locked 40 Hz visual responses in human. J Neurosci 16:4240–4249.

Tan H, Jenkinson N, Brown P (2014) Dynamic neural correlates of motor error monitoring and

adaptation during trial-to-trial learning. J Neurosci 34:5678–5688.

Tan H, Wade C, Brown P (2016) Post-movement beta activity in sensorimotor cortex indexes

confidence in the estimations from internal models. J Neurosci 36:1516–1528.

Taylor JA, Krakauer JW, Ivry RB (2014) Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a

Sensorimotor Adaptation Task. J Neurosci 34:3023–3032

21

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 22: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

Torrecillos F, Alayrangues J, Kilavik BE, Malfait N (2015) Distinct modulations in sensorimotor

postmovement and foreperiod β-band activities related to error salience processing and

sensorimotor adaptation. J Neurosci 35:12753–12765.

Torrecillos F, Albouy P, Brochier T, Malfait N (2014) Does the processing of sensory and reward-

prediction errors involve common neural resources? evidence from a frontocentral negative

potential modulated by movement execution errors. J Neurosci 34:4845–4856.

van Beers RJ (2009) Motor learning is optimally tuned to the properties of motor noise. Neuron

63:406–417.

van der Vliet R, Frens M a, Vreede L De, Jonker Z, Ribbers GM, Selles RW, Geest J Van Der, Donchin O

(2018) Individual differences in motor noise and adaptation rate are optimally related. Eneuro

5:1–14

Vocat R, Pourtois G, Vuilleumier P (2011) Parametric modulation of error-related ERP components by

the magnitude of visuo-motor mismatch. Neuropsychologia 49:360–367.

Werner S, Strüder HK, Donchin O (2019) Intermanual transfer of visuomotor adaptation is related to

awareness. PLoS One 14:e0220748.

22

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 23: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

TABLES

FMΘ Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5

𝜶 Intercept [95% CI]

.01 [-.05 .08]

.01 [-.05 .08]

.01 [-.05 .08]

.01 [-.05 .08]

.01 [-.05 .08]

𝜷𝟏 Error magnitude [95% CI]

.17 [.13 .21]

.17 [.13 .21]

.17 [.13 .21]

.18 [.15 .22]

.18 [.14 .21]

𝜷𝟐 Signed error [95% CI]

-.04 [-.06 -.02]

-.04 [-.06 -.02]

-.04 [-.06 -.02]

-.04 [-.06 -.02]

𝜷𝟑 Error correction [95% CI]

-.02 [-.03 -.01]

-.02 [-.03 -.01]

-.02 [-.03 -.01]

𝜷𝟒 Trial number [95% CI]

-.10 [-.13 -.08]

-.10 [-.13 -.08]

-.10 [-.13 -.08]

-.10 [-.13 -.08]

-.11 [-.13 -.08]

𝜷𝟓 Fast movement [95% CI]

.01 [.00 .02]

.01 [.00 .02]

𝜷𝟔 Slow movement [95% CI]

.00 [-.01 .01]

Fit statistics R2 BIC

.115

95941

.115

95922

.114

95905

.114

95896

.111

95971 Table 1: Linear mixed-effects model on the trial level (35117 trials, 60 participants) with frontal midline theta power (FMΘ) as the dependent variable (Eq. 6). In order to compare the strength of the correlations, all variables were z-score normalized. Stepwise removal of variables with the smallest effect size was continued as long as the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) decreased, and the last model for which this criterion held was selected. Trial-to-trial differences in FMΘ were best explained by error magnitude, the number of preceding trials and the signed error.

Adaptation rate (B) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

𝜶 Intercept [95% CI]

.00 [-.21 .21]

.00 [-.21 .21]

.00 [-.23 .23]

𝜸𝟏 Planning noise (𝝈𝜼) [95% CI]

0.47 [.24 .69]

0.45 [.22 .68]

𝜸𝟐 Execution noise (𝝈𝜺) [95% CI]

-.55 [-.78 -.31]

-.60 [-.82 .-37]

-.44 [-.67 .-20]

𝜸𝟑 FMΘ-error-sensitivity (𝜷𝟏) [95% CI]

.14 [-.09 .37]

Fit statistics R2 BIC

0.384 157

0.367 154

0.190 165

Table 2: Multivariate linear model on the participant level (60 participants), with adaptation rate (B) as the dependent variable (Eq. 7). FMΘ-error-sensitivity is the relation between frontal midline theta power (FMΘ) and error magnitude on the trial level estimated by the selected model in Table 1. In order to compare the strength of the correlations, all variables were z-score normalized. Stepwise removal of variables with the smallest effect size was continued as long as the Bayesian Information Criterion decreased, and the last model for which this criterion held was selected. Individual differences in adaptation rate were best explained by planning noise and execution noise.

FMΘ-error-sensitivity (𝜷𝟏) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 𝜶 Intercept [95% CI]

.00 [-.24 .24]

.00 [-.24 .24]

.00 [-.24 .24]

.00 [-.26 .26]

𝜸𝟏 Planning noise (𝝈𝜼) [95% CI]

-.20 [-.50 .09]

-.12 [-.38 .14]

𝜸𝟐 Execution noise noise (𝝈𝜺) [95% CI]

-.24 [-.56 .07]

-.35 [-.61 -.10]

-.40 [-.64 -.16]

𝜸𝟑 Adaptation rate (B) [95% CI]

.19 [-.12 .49]

Fit statistics R2 BIC

.193 173

.171 170

.158 167

173 Table 3: Multivariate linear model on the participant level (60 participants), with FMΘ-error-sensitivity as the dependent variable (Methods, Eq. 8). FMΘ-error-sensitivity is the relation between frontal midline theta power (FMΘ) and error magnitude on the trial level estimated by the selected model in Table 1. In order to compare the strength of the correlations, all variables were z-score normalized. Stepwise removal of variables with the smallest effect size was continued as long as the Bayesian Information Criterion decreased, and the last model for which this criterion held was selected. Individual differences in FMΘ-error-sensitivity were best explained by execution noise.

23

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 24: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

FMΘ Model 6 𝜶 Intercept [95% CI]

.00 [-.06 .06]

𝜷𝟕 Error Probability [95% CI]

-.17 [-.20 -.14]

𝜷𝟐 Signed Error [95% CI]

-.04 [-.06 -.02]

𝜷𝟒 Trial Number [95% CI]

-.11 [-.13 -.08]

Fit statistics R2

BIC

.111

95730 Table 4: Linear mixed-effects model on the trial level (35117 trials, 60 participants) with frontal midline theta power (FMΘ) as the dependent variable. This table shows the selected model of Table 1, but with error magnitude (𝛽B) replaced by error probability (𝛽U). Replacing error magnitude by error probability lowered the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC).

FMΘ-probability-sensitivity (𝜷𝟕) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 𝜶 Intercept [95% CI]

.00 [-.26 .26]

.00 [-.26 .26]

.00 [-.26 .26]

.00 [-.26 .26]

𝜸𝟏 Planning noise (𝝈𝜼) [95% CI]

.17 [-.15 .48]

.15 [-.12 .42]

𝜸𝟐 Execution noise (𝝈𝜺) [95% CI]

-.03 [-.38 .31]

𝜸𝟑 Adaptation rate (B) [95% CI]

-.22 [-.54 .11]

-.20 [-.46 .07]

-.16 [-.42 .10]

Fit statistics R2 BIC

.048 183

.048 179

.026 176

173 Table 5: Multivariate linear model on the participant level (60 participants), with FMΘ-probability-sensitivity as the dependent variable. FMΘ-probability-sensitivity is the relation between frontal midline theta power (FMΘ) and error probability on the trial level estimated by the model in Table 4. In order to compare the strength of the correlations, all variables were z-score normalized. Stepwise removal of variables with the smallest effect size was continued as long as the Bayesian Information Criterion decreased, and the last model for which this criterion held was selected. Individual differences in FMΘ-probability-sensitivity were not explained by planning noise, execution noise and adaptation rate.

24

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 25: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

FIGURE LEGENDS

Figure 1: Design of the experiment. A: Trial design. B: Experimental design. Figure 2: Average movement data across participants throughout the experiment. The error bars on the brown line indicate the standard deviation of the hand angle across subjects. Figure 3: The probability density function of the signed errors for each participant. In trials with error magnitudes below 1.43 degrees (solid black line), the cursor was completely inside the target and in trials with error magnitudes between 1.43 and 4.29 degrees (dotted black line), the cursor was partially in the target. Figure 4: Individual movement data of five individuals with increasing execution noise (𝜎<). Colored circles represent the hand angle in individual trials. The colored lines represent the estimated movement plan from the Bayesian fitting procedure. Figure 5: Trial level analysis in the time domain. For this figure, the full mixed-effects model is used (Eq. 6). A: EEG amplitude. B: Relation with error magnitude. C: Relation with signed error. D: Relation with error correction. Figure 6: Trial level analysis in the theta frequency band (4-8Hz). For this figure, the full mixed-effects model is used (Eq. 6) A: Theta power. B: Relation with error magnitude. C: Relation with signed error. D: Relation with error correction. Figure 7: Trial level analysis in the frequency domain in channel FCz. For this figure, the full mixed-effects model is used (Eq. 6). A: EEG power. B: Relation with error magnitude. C: Relation with signed error. D: Relation with error correction. Dotted lines represent the average target onset (-441ms), movement onset (-193ms) and movement end (78ms), relative to the onset of visual feedback. Solid lines represent the post-feedback window (100-600ms) and the theta frequency band (4-8Hz). Figure 8: Error sensitivity in channel FCz. For visualization purposes, trials have been binned and averaged based on signed error (A,C,E) or error magnitude (B,D,F,G). Colored dotted lines represent bins with a negative sign. Black dotted lines represent the average target onset (-441ms), movement onset (-193ms) and movement end (78ms), relative to the onset of visual feedback. Black solid lines represent the post-feedback window (100-600ms). A-B: Average EEG amplitude across subjects. C-D: Average theta power (4-8Hz) across subjects. In panel D, error bars represent the standard error of the mean. E-F: Average theta power in the in the post-feedback window. The diameter of the circles is related to the average number of trials in the bin. G: Average theta power in the post-feedback window throughout the experiment. Trials 1-450 are part of the baseline block and thus without any perturbations. Figure 9: Linear model on the participant level (60 participants), with adaptation rate (B) as the dependent variable (Eq. 7). The best model included planning noise (𝜎9) and execution noise (𝜎<), but not FMΘ-error-sensitivity (𝛽B) (Table 2, Model 2). The vertical lines represent the dependent variable. The dots at the end of the lines represent the residuals of the dependent variable after subtracting the estimated effect of the (other) independent variables included in the best model. Black lines represent the absolute slope (‘aslope’) and the confidence interval. Note that the confidence intervals of z-score normalized slope (‘nslope’) of planning noise (𝜎9) and execution noise (𝜎<) are slightly smaller than in Table 2, because, in this figure, the effect of the other parameter is ‘fixed’ in order to create the residuals. Figure 10: Linear model on the participant level (60 participants), with FMΘ-error-sensitivity (𝛽B) as the dependent variable (Eq. 8). The best model included execution noise (𝜎<), but not planning (𝜎9) noise nor adaptation rate (B) (Table 3, Model 3). The vertical lines represent the dependent variable. The dots at the end of the vertical lines represent the residuals of the dependent variable after subtracting the estimated effect of the independent variables included in the best model. Black lines represent the absolute slope (‘aslope’) and the confidence interval. The z-score normalized slope is also shown (‘nslope’). Figure 11: Error probability sensitivity in channel FCz. A: Relation between the signed error and error probability for each participant (Formula 9). In trials with error magnitudes below 1.43 degrees (solid black line) the cursor was completely inside the target, and in trials with error magnitudes between 1.43 and 4.29 degrees (dotted black line), the cursor was partially in the target. B: Average EEG amplitude across subjects. For visualization purposes, trials have been binned and averaged based on error probability. Dotted lines represent the average target onset (-441ms), movement onset (-193ms) and movement end (78ms), relative to the onset of visual feedback. Solid lines represent the post-feedback window (100-600ms). C: Average theta power (4-8Hz) across subjects. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Figure 12: Linear model on the participant level (60 participants), with FMΘ-probability-sensitivity (𝛽U) as the dependent variable. FMΘ-surprise-sensitivity was not related to planning (𝜎9), execution noise (𝜎<), nor adaptation rate (B) (Table 5, Model 4). Black lines represent the absolute slope (‘aslope’) and the confidence interval. The z-score normalized slope is also shown (‘nslope’). Note that the y-axis is inverted for easier comparison with figure 10. Figure 13: Post-feedback error sensitivity and surprise sensitivity in channel FCz of five individuals with increasing execution noise. Colored circles represent the post-feedback theta power in individual trials. Black lines represent the absolute slope (‘aslope’) and the confidence interval. The z-score normalized slope is also shown (‘nslope’). A-E: FMΘ-error-sensitivity. F-J: FMΘ-probability-sensitivity. Note that the x-axis is inverted for easier comparison to panel A-E.

25

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 26: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

SUPPLEMENTARY TABLES

Post-feedback theta power Model PO5 Model PO6 𝜶 Intercept [95% CI]

.00 [-.11 .11]

.00 [-.11 .11]

𝜷𝟏 Error Magnitude [95% CI]

-.02 [-.03 .00]

.01 [.01 .03]

𝜷𝟐 Signed Error [95% CI]

.10 [.07 .13]

-.13 [-.16 -.10]

𝜷𝟒 Trial Number [95% CI]

-.11 [-.15 -.06]

-.07 [-.10 -.03]

Fit statistics R2

BIC

0.111 90240

0.111 90376

Supplementary Table 1: Linear mixed-effects model on the trial level (35117 trials, 60 participants) with theta power (4-8Hz) in the left (PO5) and right (PO6) occipital cortex as the dependent variable. This table shows the selected model of Table 1, but with frontal midline theta power replaced by theta power in the occipital cortices. BIC is the Bayesian Information Criterion.

PO5 Theta sensitivity to signed error (𝜷𝟐) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 𝜶 Intercept [95% CI]

.00 [-.26 .26]

.00 [-.26 .26]

.00 [-.26 .26]

.00 [-.26 .26]

𝜸𝟏 Planning noise (𝝈𝜼) [95% CI]

-.16 [-.48 .16]

-.11 [-.39 .18]

-.09 [-.35 .18]

𝜸𝟐 Execution noise (𝝈𝜺) [95% CI]

.13 [-.22 .48]

.06 [-.22 .34]

𝜸𝟑 Adaptation rate (B) [95% CI]

.11 [-.22 .45]

Fit statistics R2 BIC

.019 185

-.010 181

-.007 177

173 Supplementary Table 2: Multivariate linear model on the participant level (60 participants), with left occipital (PO5) theta sensitivity to the signed error as the independent variable (Supplementary Table 1, Model PO5). In order to compare the strength of the correlations, all variables were z-score normalized. Stepwise removal of variables with the smallest effect size was continued as long as the Bayesian Information Criterion decreased, and the last model for which this criterion held was selected. Individual differences in theta sensitivity to the signed error were not explained by planning noise, execution noise and adaptation rate.

PO6 Theta sensitivity to signed error (𝜷𝟐) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 𝜶 Intercept [95% CI]

.00 [-.26 .26]

.00 [-.26 .26]

.00 [-.26 .26]

.00 [-.26 .26]

𝜸𝟏 Planning noise (𝝈𝜼) [95% CI]

.10 [-.22 .41]

.14 [-.14 .42]

.17 [-.09 .43]

𝜸𝟐 Execution noise (𝝈𝜺) [95% CI]

.15 [-.19 .49]

.09 [-.19 .37]

𝜸𝟑 Adaptation rate (B) [95% CI]

.10 [-.23 .43]

Fit statistics R2 BIC

.043 183

.036 179

.030 176

173 Supplementary Table 2: Multivariate linear model on the participant level (60 participants), with right occipital (PO6) theta sensitivity to the signed error as the independent variable (Supplementary Table 1, Model PO6). In order to compare the strength of the correlations, all variables were z-score normalized. Stepwise removal of variables with the smallest effect size was continued as long as the Bayesian Information Criterion decreased, and the last model for which this criterion held was selected. Individual differences in theta sensitivity to the signed error were not explained by planning noise, execution noise and adaptation rate.

26

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 27: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

SUPPLEMENTARY FIGURE LEGENDS

Supplementary Figure 1: Relations between planning (𝜎9), execution noise (𝜎<), adaptation rate (B) and the lag-1 autocorrelation in the baseline block R(1). Dots represent the absolute values and ‘nslope’ represents the linear relation of the z-score normalized values (slope [95%CI]). Supplementary Figure 2: Relations between planning (𝜎9), execution noise (𝜎<), adaptation rate (B) and the standard deviation of signed errors 𝜎=[𝑠] in the baseline block (A-C), the adaptation block (D-F) and both blocks combined (G-I). Dots represent the absolute values and ‘nslope’ represents the linear relation of the z-score normalized values (slope [95%CI]). Supplementary Figure 3: Trial level analysis in the delta frequency band (2-3Hz). For this figure, the full mixed-effects model is used (Eq. 6) A: Delta power. B: Relation with error magnitude. C: Relation with signed error. D: Relation with error correction. Supplementary Figure 4: Trial level analysis in the alpha frequency band (9-14Hz). For this figure, the full mixed-effects model is used (Eq. 6) A: Alpha power. B: Relation with error magnitude. C: Relation with signed error. D: Relation with error correction. Supplementary Figure 5: Trial level analysis in the beta frequency band (15-30Hz). For this figure, the full mixed-effects model is used (Eq. 6) A: EEG amplitude. B: Relation with error magnitude. C: Relation with signed error. D: Relation with error correction. Supplementary Figure 6: Error sensitivity in channel FCz in the time domain. For visualization purposes, trials have been binned and averaged based on error magnitude. Black dotted lines represent the movement end (78ms), relative to the onset of visual feedback. Black solid lines represent a post-feedback window of 200-350ms (Palidis et al., 2019). A: Average EEG amplitude across subjects. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. The average EEG amplitude shows a wave-like signal within a larger positive response. Note that the post-feedback window contains both the negative and a part of the positive peak of the wave-like signal. B: Average EEG amplitude in the in the post-feedback window. The diameter of the circles is related to the average number of trials in the bin. C: Similar to panel A, but with a smaller sample. For visualization purposes, only the first 20 participants were included and of these participants only the first 150 trials of the adaptation block were included. With a smaller sample size the wave-like signal within the large positive response becomes less visible. D: Similar to panel B, but with the smaller sample. Note that the average EEG amplitude in the post-feedback window (200-350ms) appears similar for small and large error magnitudes. Supplementary Figure 7: Error sensitivity in channel FCz in the subset of trials that were completely inside the target (error magnitude < 1.43). For visualization purposes, trials have been binned and averaged based on error magnitude. A: Average theta power (4-8Hz) across subjects. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Black dotted lines represent the average target onset (-441ms), movement onset (-193ms) and movement end (78ms), relative to the onset of visual feedback. Black solid lines represent the post-feedback window (100-600ms). B: Average theta power in the in the post-feedback window. The diameter of the circles is related to the average number of trials in the bin. Supplementary Figure 8: Sensitivity to the signed error in channel PO5 (left column) and PO6 (right column). For visualization purposes, trials have been binned and averaged based on signed error. Colored dotted lines represent bins with a negative sign. Black dotted lines represent the average target onset (-441ms), movement onset (-193ms) and movement end (78ms), relative to the onset of visual feedback. Black solid lines represent the post-feedback window (0-500ms). A,B: Average EEG amplitude across subjects. C,D: Average theta power (4-8Hz) across subjects. E,F: Average theta power in the post-feedback window. The diameter of the circles is related to the average number of trials in the bin. Supplementary Figure 9: Linear model on the participant level (60 participants), with occipital theta (4-8Hz) sensitivity to the signed error as the dependent variable (𝛽:) (Supplementary Table 1). A-C: Left occipital (PO5) theta sensitivity to the signed error was not related to planning (𝜎9[𝑠]), execution noise (𝜎<[𝑠]), nor adaptation rate (B) (Supplementary Table 2). D-F: Right occipital (PO6) theta sensitivity to the signed error was also not related to planning (𝜎9[𝑠]), execution noise (𝜎<[𝑠]), nor adaptation rate (B) (Supplementary Table 3). Black lines represent the absolute slope (‘aslope’) and the confidence interval. The z-score normalized slope is also shown (‘nslope’).

27

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 28: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

100 200 300 400 500 600 700 800 900Trial number

-10

-5

0

5

10

Angl

e (de

g)

PerturbationNo Vision

A

Unperturbed Perturbed Unperturbed PerturbedNo Vision No Vision

Waiting Target Movement Endpoint

Cursor

Target

Origin

B

Figure 1 28

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 29: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0 150 300 450 600 750 900Trial number

-15

-10

-5

0

5

10

15

Angl

e (de

g)

Average hand angleAverage error magnitudeAverage perturbation signal (neg)

Figure 2 29

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 30: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

-15 -10 -5 0 5 10 15Signed error (deg)

0

0.03

0.06

0.09

0.12

0.15

0.18

0.21

0.24

Prob

abilit

y den

sity

Figure 3 30

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 31: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0 150 300 450 600 750 900-15-10

-505

1015

Angl

e (de

g)

= 1.40A

0 150 300 450 600 750 900-15-10

-505

1015

Angl

e (de

g)

= 1.86B

0 150 300 450 600 750 900-15-10

-505

1015

Angl

e (de

g)

= 2.30C

0 150 300 450 600 750 900-15-10

-505

1015

Angl

e (de

g)

= 3.00D

0 150 300 450 600 750 900Trial (#)

-15-10

-505

1015

Angl

e (de

g)

= 3.74E

Figure 4 31

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 32: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

Figure 5 32

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 33: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

Figure 6 33

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 34: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

Figure 7 34

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 35: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-5-4-3-2-1012345

Gran

d Av

erag

e (V)

A

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-5-4-3-2-1012345

Gran

d Av

erag

e (V)

B

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-10123456789

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

C

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-10123456789

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

D

-8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 8Signed error (deg)

0

1

2

3

4

5

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

E

-1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7Error magnitude (deg)

0

1

2

3

4

5

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

F

[0 1] deg[1 2] deg[2 3] deg[3 4] deg[4 5] deg

[1 150]0123456

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

G

[151 300] [301 450]Trial number

[451 600] [601 750] [751 900]

Figure 8 35

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 36: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 (deg)

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

0.2

0.25

B un

expl

ained

by

aslope = 0.15 [0.08 0.21]nslope = 0.45 [0.24 0.66]

A

1 2 3 4 5 (deg)

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

0.2

0.25

B un

expl

ained

by

aslope = -0.04 [-0.06 -0.03]nslope = -0.60 [-0.81 -0.39]

B

0 0.25 0.5 0.75 1 1.25 1.5 1.75 21 (%/deg)

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

0.2

0.25

B un

expl

ained

by

and

aslope = 0.01 [-0.01 0.03]nslope = 0.12 [-0.09 0.33]

C

Figure 9 36

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 37: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 (deg)

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

1 une

xplai

ned

by

(%/d

eg) aslope = -0.33 [-1.08 0.42]

nslope = -0.11 [-0.34 0.13]

A

1 2 3 4 5 (deg)

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

1 (%

/deg

)

aslope = -0.28 [-0.45 -0.11]nslope = -0.40 [-0.64 -0.16]

B

0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25B

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

1 une

xplai

ned

by

(%/d

eg) aslope = 0.69 [-1.64 3.02]

nslope = 0.07 [-0.17 0.31]

C

Figure 10 37

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 38: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

-10 -5 0 5 10Signed error (deg)

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Erro

r pro

babi

lity (

p)

A

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-5-4-3-2-1012345

Gran

d Av

erag

e (V)

B

p=[0 .2]p=[.2 .4]p=[.4 .6]p=[.6 .8]p=[.8 1]

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-10123456789

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

C

Figure 11 38

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 39: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 (deg)

-10-9-8-7-6-5-4-3-2-101

7 (%

/p)

aslope = 1.66 [-2.50 5.82]nslope = 0.10 [-0.16 0.37]

A

1 2 3 4 5 (deg)

-10-9-8-7-6-5-4-3-2-101

7 (%

/p)

aslope = 0.43 [-0.51 1.38]nslope = 0.12 [-0.14 0.38]

B

0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25B

-10-9-8-7-6-5-4-3-2-101

7 (%

/p)

aslope = -7.96 [-20.74 4.81]nslope = -0.16 [-0.42 0.10]

C

Figure 12 39

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 40: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10Absolute error (deg)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

aslope = 1.80 [1.42 2.17]nslope = 0.57 [0.45 0.68]

A

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10Absolute error (deg)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

aslope = 1.37 [1.04 1.71]nslope = 0.43 [0.33 0.54]

B

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10Absolute error (deg)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

aslope = 0.96 [0.67 1.25]nslope = 0.30 [0.21 0.40]

C

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10Absolute error (deg)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

aslope = 0.76 [0.52 1.00]nslope = 0.24 [0.16 0.32]

D

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10Absolute error (deg)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

aslope = 0.66 [0.45 0.86]nslope = 0.21 [0.14 0.27]

E

00.20.40.60.81Error probability (p)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

aslope = -1.13 [-1.37 -0.89]nslope = -0.36 [-0.43 -0.28]

F

00.20.40.60.81Error probability (p)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

aslope = -1.00 [-1.24 -0.76]nslope = -0.32 [-0.39 -0.24]

G

00.20.40.60.81Error probability (p)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

aslope = -0.81 [-1.05 -0.57]nslope = -0.26 [-0.33 -0.18]

H

00.20.40.60.81Error probability (p)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

aslope = -0.82 [-1.05 -0.58]nslope = -0.26 [-0.33 -0.18]

I

00.20.40.60.81Error probability (p)

-20-15-10

-505

101520

aslope = -0.88 [-1.12 -0.63]nslope = -0.28 [-0.35 -0.20]

J

Figure 13 40

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 41: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0.1 0.3 0.5 0.7 0.9 (deg)

-0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

R(1)

nslope = 0.58 [0.36 0.79]A

1 2 3 4 5 (deg)

-0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3nslope = -0.18 [-0.44 0.07]

B

0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.3B

-0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3nslope = 0.28 [0.03 0.54]

C

Supplementary Figure 1 41

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 42: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0.1 0.3 0.5 0.7 0.91

2

3

4

5

y bas

eline

blo

cks (

deg)

nslope = 0.53 [0.31 0.75]A

1 2 3 4 51

2

3

4

5nslope = 0.95 [0.86 1.03]

B

0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.31

2

3

4

5nslope = -0.29 [-0.54 -0.04]

C

0.1 0.3 0.5 0.7 0.91

2

3

4

5

y adap

tatio

n bl

ocks

(deg

) nslope = 0.35 [0.11 0.60]D

1 2 3 4 51

2

3

4

5nslope = 0.93 [0.83 1.03]

E

0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.31

2

3

4

5nslope = -0.55 [-0.77 -0.33]

F

0.1 0.3 0.5 0.7 0.9 (deg)

1

2

3

4

5

y all b

lock

s (de

g)

nslope = 0.44 [0.20 0.68]G

1 2 3 4 5 (deg)

1

2

3

4

5nslope = 0.97 [0.91 1.03]

H

0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.3B

1

2

3

4

5nslope = -0.47 [-0.70 -0.24]

I

Supplementary Figure 2 42

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 43: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

Supplementary Figure 3 43

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 44: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

Supplementary Figure 4 44

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 45: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

Supplementary Figure 5 45

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 46: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0 100 200 300 400 500 600Time (ms)

-2-101234567

Gran

d Av

erag

e (V)

A

-1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7Error magnitude (deg)

0

1

2

3

4

5

Gran

d Av

erag

e (V)

B[0 1] deg[1 2] deg[2 3] deg[3 4] deg[4 5] deg

0 100 200 300 400 500 600Time (ms)

-2-101234567

Gran

d Av

erag

e (V)

C

-1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7Error magnitude (deg)

0

1

2

3

4

5

Gran

d Av

erag

e (V)

D

Supplementary Figure 6 46

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 47: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-10123456

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

A

0 0.475 0.95 1.425Error magnitude (deg)

0.5

1

1.5

2

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

B

[0.000 0.475] deg[0.475 0.950] deg[0.950 1.425] deg

Supplementary Figure 7 47

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint

Page 48: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-5-4-3-2-1012345

Gran

d Av

erag

e (V)

Left Occipital (PO5)A

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-10123456789

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

C

-8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 8Signed error (deg)

0

1

2

3

4

5

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

E

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-5-4-3-2-1012345

Gran

d Av

erag

e (V)

Right Occipital (PO6)B

-600 -300 0 300 600Time (ms)

-10123456789

Thet

a pow

er (%

)D

-8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 8Signed error (deg)

0

1

2

3

4

5

Thet

a pow

er (%

)

F[0 1] deg[1 2] deg[2 3] deg[3 4] deg[4 5] deg

Supplementary Figure 8 48

.CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license(which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. It is made available under aThe copyright holder for this preprintthis version posted July 12, 2020. . https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.07.12.188581doi: bioRxiv preprint

Page 49: Individual differences in frontal midline theta activity ... · 7/12/2020  · On the participant level, individual differences in adaptation rate were positively correlated to planning

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 (deg)

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5PO

5 2 (

%/d

eg)

aslope = -0.14 [-0.56 0.28]nslope = -0.09 [-0.35 0.18]

A

1 2 3 4 5 (deg)

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

PO5

2 (%

/deg

)

aslope = 0.01 [-0.09 0.10]nslope = 0.02 [-0.24 0.28]

B

0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25B

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

PO5

2 (%

/deg

)

aslope = 0.11 [-1.20 1.42]nslope = 0.02 [-0.24 0.28]

C

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 (deg)

-1.5

-1

-0.5

0

0.5

PO6

2 (%

/deg

)

aslope = 0.24 [-0.12 0.61]nslope = 0.17 [-0.09 0.43]

D

1 2 3 4 5 (deg)

-1.5

-1

-0.5

0

0.5PO

6 2 (

%/d

eg)

aslope = 0.04 [-0.04 0.13]nslope = 0.14 [-0.12 0.40]

E

0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25B

-1.5

-1

-0.5

0

0.5

PO6

2 (%

/deg

)

aslope = 0.25 [-0.90 1.41]nslope = 0.06 [-0.21 0.32]

F

Supplementary Figure 9 49

.C

C-B

Y-N

C-N

D 4

.0 In

tern

atio

nal l

icen

se(w

hich

was

not

cer

tifie

d by

pee

r re

view

) is

the

auth

or/fu

nder

. It i

s m

ade

avai

labl

e un

der

aT

he c

opyr

ight

hol

der

for

this

pre

prin

tth

is v

ersi

on p

oste

d Ju

ly 1

2, 2

020.

.

http

s://d

oi.o

rg/1

0.11

01/2

020.

07.1

2.18

8581

doi:

bioR

xiv

prep

rint