don’t talk about it- active avoidance in organizations-woolley

Upload: larry-lyon

Post on 08-Apr-2018

225 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    1/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    1

    Dont talk about it: Active avoidance in organizations

    Julia Bear

    Anita Williams Woolley

    Carnegie Mellon University

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    2/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    2

    Abstract

    We introduce a new constructactive avoidancethat can be beneficial for organizations. Active

    avoidance is motivated by situational incongruence and takes two qualitatively different forms

    pretending and bypassing, depending on the salience of outcome versus process expectations in an

    aversive situation. Active avoidance can lead to positive consequences by facilitating process

    inventions that involve increased efficiency and helpful accommodations. We discuss the

    conditions under which organizations can harness the positive results of active avoidance.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    3/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    3

    Many of us face tasks on a daily basis that we do not want to do -- take out the garbage,

    talk with an unpleasant co-worker, or finish a difficult or tedious task. When faced with an aversive

    situation, we have several options: we can ignore our distaste for the task and do it anyway, change

    the situation to make it less unpleasant somehow, or avoid it. Research has cast situations in which

    people choose to persevere (Hamel & Valikangas, 2003; Luthans, 2002) and/or make

    organizational change (Bennis, 1963; Kanter, 1983) as particularly heroic. By contrast, avoidance

    of negative experiences has been largely viewed as a passive response with negative implications,

    especially in organizational research (Ashforth & Lee, 1990; Harris & Sutton, 1983; Keltner,

    Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003; Pruitt & Carnevale, 1993). We offer a different perspective, arguing

    that avoidance can be a useful strategy with positive effects under certain conditions. Toward this

    end, we introduce the idea of "active avoidance," in which individuals implicitly search for and

    covertly pursue alternative strategies in order to reduce their experience of aversion. Active

    avoidance can have informational value for organizations and can lead to the discovery of novel

    methods for handling situations that individuals would otherwise find aversive, possibly in a more

    effective manner than simply encouraging individuals to give voice to concerns, and particularly

    when the extent of a problem and its potential solutions are not well understood.

    By introducing the construct of active avoidance, we intend to advance organizational

    literature in the following ways. First, active avoidance is a common but unrecognized method that

    employees use to cope with aversive situations that arise in organizations as a result of

    incongruence between situational demands and employees preferences, perceived roles, or

    expected work processes. Second, avoidance has typically been understood to have negative

    consequences, but we argue that, when employee and organizational goals are aligned, active

    avoidance can lead to positive consequences for organizations involving process inventions that

    increase efficiency and create helpful accommodations for employees to deal with situational

    incongruence. Third, active avoidance can lead to useful organizational inventions by building a

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    4/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    4

    larger pool of alternative solutions from which to choose, made up of the local adaptations that

    employees have made to deal with their own personal situation.

    We first review literature relevant to active avoidance, and discuss how active avoidance differs

    from more traditional views of avoidance, as well as from more overt, change-oriented approaches

    to dealing with aversive situations. We then discuss the antecedents of active avoidance,

    particularly the role that process versus outcome constraints in organizations play in influencing the

    shape of active avoidance strategies. Next, we discuss two categories of active avoidance strategies,

    namely pretending and bypassing. Finally, we discuss the consequences of active avoidance,

    focusing particularly on the conditions under which active avoidance will lead to process inventions

    that enhance efficiency and accommodation of differences. We conclude by making suggestions for

    future research based on this construct.

    THEORETICAL BACKGROUND

    People face aversive situations in organizations all the time. Aversion, which refers to the

    experience of repugnance or intense dislike that creates the impulse to move away (Merriam-

    Webster, 1991) is a common experience, since the constraints and expectations inherent in the

    requirements to conform to an organizational role and to coordinate with others restrict the freedom

    of organizational actors in ways that can create a sense of loss of control and interpersonal friction

    (Katz & Kahn, 1978). For example, expectations related to work processes often lead employees to

    feel like they are being watched (Markus, 1978), whereas expectations related to outcomes can

    create performance pressures (Jensen & Meckling, 1976; Locke, Latham, & Smith, 1990), which

    some may find aversive. One less recognized source of aversion individuals experience in

    organizations is situational incongruence, or the lack of fit between an individuals preferences,

    perceived role, or expectations about work processes and outcomes. Situational incongruence can

    arise from the presence of organizational constraints that individuals cannot easily reconcile. For

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    5/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    5

    example, in the case of a preference incongruence, an employee may prefer to work a later schedule

    (e.g. to come in later in the morning and stay later) but be required to work with others who start

    the day and schedule meetings earlier. Role incongruence is experienced when responsibilities or

    situational demands are at odds with an individuals organizational or societal role, such as when

    the traditional gender roles of the actors are incongruent with the demands of the situation {Bear,

    2010 #1945; Eagly, 2002 #1946}. Incongruence between work processes and outcomes occurs

    when the prescribed set of practices is at odds with desired outcomes; for example, police who are

    attempting to apprehend a criminal quickly are frequently required to follow a set of rules and

    procedures that slow the process down. When these sources of situational incongruence are large,

    we argue that the traditional conceptions of avoidance as withdrawal take hold, as actors see no

    means of reconciling the incongruence. However, when the gap is perceived as moderate but

    manageable, we argue that individuals will employ a range of active avoidance strategies to

    accommodate the incongruence between the demands of the situation and their individual

    preferences and expectations.

    Despite what we maintain is the preponderance of active avoidance in response to these

    types of aversive situations in organizations, the dominant focus of organizational literature has

    been on proactive responses, such as overtly promoting organizational change. This is in the face

    of mounting evidence from research in social cognition and clinical psychology that calls into

    question the value of focusing on and discussing problems in every situation. Proponents of positive

    psychology and solution-focused brief therapy have convincingly argued that focusing on negative

    states of mind is not the same as creating positive states (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) and

    that discussing solutions does not depend directly on fully explicating problems (de Shazer, Berg,

    Lipchik, & Nunnally, 1986; Trepper, Dolan, McCollum, & Nelson, 2006). In addition, research on

    the efficacy of coping methods has shown that problem-focused coping can lead to frustration,

    especially when a stressor is intractable (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Even the act of asking

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    6/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    6

    individuals to think about problems may increase their salience and lead people to overlook

    solutions (Gingerich & Eisengart, 2000). Furthermore, there are costs to individuals who raise

    problems and "complain" in organizations. Despite extensive attempts by organizations to create a

    climate in which members are invited to raise issues, evidence suggests that they do so at

    significant cost to interpersonal relationships and careers (Near & Miceli, 1986).

    In addition to the drawbacks of inculcating a problem focus in individuals, organizations

    are also not particularly adept at dealing with the type of information that tends to surface when

    problems are discussed. Collectives are particularly weak in diagnosing and distinguishing

    big/system-wide/long-lasting problems from small/local/short-term problems (Forrester, 1994;

    Senge & Sterman, 1992) and projecting the potential value of novel solutions in the absence of

    demonstrable evidence (Jackson, Golub, & Building; Surowiecki & Silverman, 2007). Thus even

    when problems are surfaced, the likelihood that accurate diagnosis and useful change will follow as

    a result of having collectives talk about them is small (Surowiecki & Silverman, 2007). By contrast,

    collectives can deal very effectively with observable evidencesensing that a new approach or

    direction is better than the current one and following suit, until what starts as a subtle tendency

    becomes a swarm (J. Kennedy, Eberhart, & Shi, 2001).

    Therefore, we counter the notion that individuals can and should always raise problems for

    collective consideration by offering a different perspective, one that highlights the potential value in

    implicitly seeking out more subtle and local solutions in problematic situations, which can yield

    important benefits to organizations under certain conditions. Toward this end, we introduce the

    concept of active avoidance, which is the implicit search for and use of alternatives motivated to

    reduce the negative experience of aversion. In other words, active avoiders are addressing an

    aversive situation by doing something other than what is expected or prescribed, and they are doing

    it without discussing it with others. Active avoidance can be positive because it enables individuals

    to make short-term adaptations in the face of situational incongruence, but to do so in such a way

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    7/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    7

    that they can forego the negative interpersonal discomfort and relational consequences of raising

    problems. Additionally, given the challenges associated with collective problem diagnosis and

    projection of novel solutions, active avoidance can be beneficial to organizations by allowing

    individuals to make short-term adaptations to locally aversive situations, which provides some level

    of relief to them in the short term, and over time can provide system-wide diagnostic information

    (if many individuals are avoiding the same sorts of things) as well as demonstrable evidence of

    potential solutions (if individuals have innovated and tested new ways to handle a situation).

    Active Avoidance versus Traditional Conceptions of Avoidance

    Traditional conceptions of avoidance involve withdrawal or inaction. At its most basic,

    avoidance is an innate response to a threat or an aversive situation (Cannon, 1932; Thorndike,

    1898). On a psychological level, pursuing pleasure versus avoiding pain--the hedonic principle--is a

    basic tenet of psychology (Freud & Hubback, 1922). In organizational research, avoidance has also

    been viewed largely as withdrawal and examined with respect to turnover and exit in response to

    job dissatisfaction. Empirical evidence has shown that job dissatisfaction is a significant predictor

    of withdrawal from organizations (Mobley, 1977; Tett & Meyer, 1993). When complete withdrawal

    is not an option in organizations, neglect, meaning the passive disregard of performing expected

    behaviors, may occur. Examples of neglect include calling in sick, coming in late and allowing

    errors to occur due to negligence (Farrell, 1983; Rusbult, Farrell, Rogers, & Mainous Iii, 1988). In

    addition to withdrawal and neglect, researchers in OB have identified indefinite delay as another

    way that actors remove themselves from undesirable situations in organizations (Izraeli & Jick,

    1986). Similarly, Harris and Sutton (1983) identified "task procrastination" as a common form of

    non-action in organizations, primarily due to the aversive nature of certain tasks, namely

    unappealing, difficult, ambiguous and unimportant tasks.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    8/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    8

    The view of avoidance as inaction has been called in to question by more recent research in

    psychobiology and basic psychology. Past research on the physiology of avoidance has centered on

    seminal work by Gray (1970) on the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) and the Behavioral

    Activation System (BAS). While avoidance was initially associated with BIS and thus equated with

    behavioral inhibition and inaction, more recent work has focused on the more active approaches of

    behavior motivated to forego aversive experiences. The BIS is still understood to be the

    neuropsychological epicenter of anxiety. However, while previously serving to regulate responses

    to conditioned aversive stimuli, it is now understood to be activated by sources of conflict among

    any inputs that might activate both the BAS and the "Fight, Flight, or Freeze System" (FFFS). As

    such, the BIS is no longer considered solely a punishment system, but rather a conflict detection

    and resolution device. Thus, BIS-engagement elicits search behavior designed to resolve these

    conflicts (McNaughton & Corr, 2004). In the psychology literature, the approach versus avoid

    dichotomy has also been found to be more complex than was originally conceived. Dollard and

    Miller (1950) first theorized about the ways in which goals often contain both desirable and

    undesirable components, resulting in approach-avoid conflicts. Higgins and colleagues (E. Higgins,

    1987, 1989; E. Higgins, Roney, Crowe, & Hymes, 1994) subsequently showed that the classic,

    hedonic principle is an oversimplification of motivation and self-regulation, and that, within the

    broader goal of pursuing desired results, people implement promotion-focused or prevention-

    focused strategies depending upon the particular goal. Rather than conflate approach with pleasure

    and avoidance with pain, regulatory focus takes a broader view of the ways in which people self-

    regulate in order to obtain both achievement and security goals (see Higgins (1997) for a review).

    Thus, the existing literature provides some indication that avoidance is not only passive as

    previously conceived and that a one-sided view of avoidance as withdrawal or passive, negative

    behavior is too narrow.

    To supplement the literature on avoidance in organizations, which tends to focus on what

    the actor is *not* doing, i.e. avoiding engaging in an aversive task or withdrawing from an

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    9/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    9

    organization, active avoidance reflects the active pursuit of less aversive alternatives in an implicit

    manner, which has not been captured in previous, related research. Active avoidance occurs when

    individuals judge that the aversive situation they face is not insurmountable, and that changing the

    situation will yield benefits over not changing the situation, but that doing so explicitly will be

    personally costly or unsuccessful. Trying to make an explicit change to the organization can be

    costly in the interpersonal domain, where interpersonal judgments and sensitivities around

    impression management run high. Lack of success may be anticipated most in the task domain,

    where individuals judge that alternative methods might be viewed as invalid by others, or where

    they do not yet have the evidence to convince others that a new approach would work just as well

    or better than current approaches. In this way, active avoidance allows individuals to make changes

    without provoking interpersonal confrontation, or to test out new approaches and gather evidence of

    their success before exposing them to the collective opinion of others in the organization.

    Furthermore, active avoidance is most likely to be used when the situational incongruence being

    experienced is moderate, but not too great when preferences, roles, or processes are at odds with a

    desired or expected outcome but the actor sees a way to reconcile the conflicting demands of the

    situation.

    Proposition 1: Moderate levels of situational incongruence will result in a greater likelihood

    of active avoidance behavior.

    In the next section of the paper, we outline the situational constraints that influence the

    manifestation of active avoidance. In particular, we examine the role of constraints and expectations

    in organizations and the ways in which they interact with actors' experience of aversion to influence

    the shape of active avoidance behavior. We then delineate the ways in which process versus

    outcome expectations interact with aversion to shape the active avoidance strategies actors adopt --

    namely, pretending and bypassing.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    10/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    10

    MODERATORS OF ACTIVE AVOIDANCE: CONSTRAINTS AND EXPECTATIONS

    (OR "RULES ARE MEANT TO BE BROKEN")

    Some argue that the main function of organizational designers is to constrain behavior and

    focus attention so that workers might be more "boundedly rational" (March, Simon, & Guetzkow,

    1958; Ocasio, 1997). Constraints direct attention and shape behavior, and we argue that the form

    active avoidance will take will be influenced by the kinds of constraints that exist in the aversive

    context that the actors are attempting to implicitly change. Constraints can take the form of specific

    goals or end states of activity, or may specify the boundaries outside of which behavior is not

    permitted. Constraints in organizations can be communicated in a variety of ways. They can be

    formal, as official rules or policies, or informal, as social norms. They can be communicated

    somewhat negatively in terms of rules against prohibited behavior or more positively in the form of

    expectations. They can also be more general, i.e. in order to maintain a job an employee needs to

    show up for work, to a much more fine-grained level, i.e. in order to maintain one's position as a

    waitress it is necessary to always smile, be friendly, and introduce oneself by name. Even more

    informal and tacit are the normative social expectations around working hours, clothing, eating with

    co-workers at lunch, or attending company social functions. Implicitly, organizational actors can

    become focused on their own performance outcomes through competition and interpersonal

    comparisons with peers or relevant others (Menon, Thompson, & Choi, 2006). Likewise, implicit

    expectations about the processes characterizing work behavior (work hours, interpersonal conduct,

    habits and routines) can be communicated through norms and subtle environmental cues (Barker,

    1993; Bettenhausen & Murnighan, 1985; Feldman, 1984) without any explicit process mandates or

    enforcement. Thus, constraints shape the formal or informal expectations about the ways in which

    people are expected to behave, express themselves and perform their jobs in many different

    situations in organizations.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    11/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    11

    Constraints in organizational settings can also be characterized as residing within the

    outcome or process domains of organizational work. The process domain refers to the way in which

    work gets done whereas the outcome domain refers to the final product or intended end results of

    work (Woolley, 2009a, 2009b). The role of outcome and process constraints in focusing individuals

    on lower-level activities versus overarching goals is consistent with work on construal and action

    identification (Trope & Liberman, 2003; R. R. Vallacher & Wegner, 1985, 1987). Individuals can

    identify actions as low-level, specific activities (e.g. I am typing a report) or in higher-level terms

    that encompass multiple specific alternative activities for enactment (e.g. I am consolidating and

    communicating my knowledge). Low-level action identifications are associated with identifying

    what tasks need to be accomplished and what processes to follow, whereas high-level action

    identifications are associated with identifying larger goals and outcomes of the work itself.

    Furthermore, the level at which people identify their actions is highly influenced by cues provided

    by the task context (R. Vallacher, Wegner, & Somoza, 1989). In this way, action identification

    unfolds as an emergent process in both individuals and teams, as members reconceptualize what

    they are doing based on their experience and information in the environment (R. R. Vallacher &

    Wegner, 1985).

    Managerial actions regularly focus attention on the outcome versus process spheres of

    activity in organizations, and the distinction between how processes and outcomes operate in

    reasoning and collective functioning has been examined in prior research on the individual, group

    and organizational levels (e.g. (Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, & Trotschel, 2001; Benner

    & Tushman, 2003; Locke & Latham, 1990; Woolley, 2009a, 2009b). For example, the outcome-

    process distinction has been applied to research on accountability, an issue which is critical in many

    organizational settings. Accountability can have a variety of effects that are in part explained by the

    distinction between process accountability and outcome accountability (Lerner & Tetlock, 1999).

    Both can have positive and negative effects. Though outcome accountability can motivate effort

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    12/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    12

    toward a highly desirable goal, it can have dysfunctional consequences such as escalation of

    commitment to a prior course of action (Simonson & Staw, 1992) or the encouragement of

    unethical behavior (Siegel-Jacobs & Yates, 1996). Process accountability can mitigate the incidence

    of both escalation of commitment (Simonson & Staw, 1992) and process dysfunctions observed in

    the context of exclusive outcome accountability, as well as improve accuracy and confidence

    calibration in decision-making (Siegel-Jacobs & Yates, 1996). However, process accountability and

    the constraints it entails may also harm creativity through producing negative affect (Isen & Baron,

    1991) and narrow focus (Amabile, 1983; Sutton & Galunic, 1996).

    (Insert figure 1 here)

    Similarly, we contend that the form an active avoidance strategy takes is influenced by the

    constraints that are salient in the situation. Active avoiders subject to outcome constraints will

    understand their work process in terms of higher level action identities and should exhibit greater

    flexibility in conceiving of alternative processes for accomplishing it. In contrast, active avoiders

    subject to process constraints will think about their work process in terms of lower level action

    identities and, in the face of incongruence, should exhibit a greater focus on maintaining an

    appearance of complying with a relatively narrowly circumscribed set of activities. In terms of the

    former, outcome constraints focus actors and observers on the form of the final product or end

    results of work, with less focus on or consensus around the specific steps used to attain those

    outcomes. Consequently, actions aimed at reducing aversion in such situations will focus on

    "bypassing" undesirable activities in order to conform to outcome expectations. By contrast, the

    focus on specific behaviors engendered by process constraints leads the perceived deviation from

    expected activities to have greater negative consequences for the actor, and thus obscuring

    observation of the discrepancy allows the actor to implicitly change the situation without

    experiencing negative consequences. This obscuring behavior takes the form of "pretending," in

    which the actor distracts or misleads the observer regarding the departure from expected behavior.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    13/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    13

    Pretending is most likely to occur when the desired results of a process are poorly specified or not

    easily observable, and thus maintaining the appearance of compliance with expected processes

    leads to a positive assessment of the actor's behavior overall.

    By way of illustration, take for example the mandate to have clean hands in a food

    preparation or healthcare environment to avoid the spread of illness. This type of mandate is often

    challenging due to incongruence between employees preferences and the situational demands

    and/or incongruence between expected work processes and desired outcomes. The incongruence

    between preferences and work demands stems from workers disliking washing their hands because

    of the resulting skin irritation from the soap or concern about damaging jewelry. Incongruence

    between this process demand and desired outcomes stems from the time pressure employees may

    experience, especially in the case of healthcare, and may feel that the desired outcome seeing a

    certain number of patients per day is simply incompatible with the expected process washing

    hands for a protracted period before and after seeing every patient (Nembhard & Edmondson,

    2006). Based on our model, when process constraints are more salient, then attention is focused on

    more specific, proximal behaviors of hand washing, and non-compliance requires obscuring one's

    actions. As a result, pretending behavior is more likely to occur, in which actors maintain the

    appearance of compliance even when what they are purportedly doing is not the same as what they

    are actually doing. In the case of the hand washing mandate, this will result in workers walking in

    the vicinity of the hand washing area, making motions of washing hands, to convey the impression

    of compliance. If the outcome expectations are more salient, such as a focus on clean hands coming

    in contact with food or patients, then attention would be less on specific hand-washing steps and

    more on attainment of the expected outcomes. As a result, actors may bypass certain other steps

    (such as touching anything that would make their hands dirty) or invent alternative processes. In the

    case of the clean hands mandate, workers may wear gloves, accomplish activities without using

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    14/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    14

    their hands (e.g. foot- and voice-activated instruments), or invent things like hand gel, which more

    quickly accomplishes the same state of cleanliness.

    Active avoidance behavior in environments like this can be instructive; rather than simply

    issuing additional directives about the importance of hand washing, organizations could harness the

    diagnostic information inherent in the pretending and bypassing behaviors in order to create less

    aversive processes that will lead to better accomplishment of important objectives. We will return

    to this point in a later section of the paper concerning positive consequences of active avoidance.

    For now, the example illustrates the difference between pretending and bypassing, and the way in

    which the type of constraint that is salient--process versus outcome--influences the type of active

    avoidance that occurs--pretending and bypassing.

    Throughout this section we have discussed process and outcome constraints independently.

    However, organizational life is complex, and certain occupations and tasks may involve both forms

    of constraints simultaneously. In highly regulated occupations, for example, strong process and

    outcome constraints may co-occur. Furthermore, in some professions, such as academia, different

    aspects of the job may entail different types of constraints. For example, an academic may be under

    strict outcome constraints when it comes to research, but when it comes to teaching the same person

    may be held to both process (managing the syllabus, the classroom, interactions with students) and

    outcome constraints (what students learned, their evaluation of course quality). Nonetheless, even

    when multiple constraints are present, individuals think about constraints in a manner that leads

    outcomes or processes to be more salient (Woolley, 2009a). Thus, we maintain that, even in the

    presence of multiple constraints, the active avoidance strategy employedpretending versus

    bypassingwill be driven by the constraints that are most salient in a given context.

    In the next section, we will describe these active avoidance strategies in greater detail.

    Furthermore, within the categories of pretending and bypassing, we provide examples of both

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    15/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    15

    behavioral and communicative tactics, since tasks can contain either a behavioral and/or

    communicative components, which may be employed separately or in tandem. We describe each in

    detail below.

    Process Constraints and Pretending

    Process constraints and the focus they engender on specific activities result in the sense that

    individual employees or actors are being "watched." We know from the literature in social

    psychology that in some circumstances, the sense of being watched creates evaluation apprehension

    (Markus, 1978) whereas in other circumstances it can fend off the tendencies of social loafing and

    lead individuals or groups to work harder (Karau & Williams, 1993). Pretending involves the

    creation of a dichotomy between ones private reality and the one represented to observers. In other

    words, an actor creates the impression of doing one thing, such as complying with a regulation or

    engaging in a certain activity, but he or she is really doing something else. Pretending is largely

    motivated by the desire to find alternatives that will avoid provoking a confrontation or negative

    interpersonal judgment by overtly defying process expectations. Active avoidance in the presence

    of strong process expectations will lead to partial or "loose" compliance as a behavioral tactic and

    expressive ambiguity as a communicative tactic, because both of these tactics render ambiguous

    the degree of compliance with process expectations. Loose compliance involves maintaining the

    behavioral appearance of complying whereas expressive ambiguity involves appearing to agree or

    comply in terms of communication. Both are examples of tactics employed to facilitate the

    appearance of complying with process expectations.

    For example, pretending in the form of loose compliance refers to the execution of a

    process in such a way that meets the "letter of the law" but avoids undesirable outcomes or

    experiences without evoking a confrontation concerning non-compliance. One example is found in

    the grading process used in many educational settings. Instructors institute a grading system as a

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    16/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    16

    means of giving feedback to students on their performance and to differentiate students from one

    another. However, the increasing incidence of grade inflation (Eiszler, 2002) suggests that, though

    there is the appearance of a grading system, more and more instructors implement it in such a way

    that results in little to no differentiation in the grades assigned, since dealing with unhappy students

    who have received a bad grade is a highly aversive experience. Likewise, loose compliance in terms

    of industrial safety has been shown to be related to the safety climate (or lack thereof) in an

    organization, which is characterized by managers attitudes concerning safety (Griffin & Neal,

    2000; Zohar, 1980). At the same time, loose compliance can also be credited with the invention of

    flex-time work policies that have enabled women to remain active in more areas of the work force.

    Current day flex-time policies were preceded by individuals who gradually and covertly shifted

    their work hours, sometimes with the help of co-workers who would turn on lights or computer

    workstations or punch timecards to create the impression of their presence, until management had a

    chance to become comfortable with the idea that work could be accomplished with some degree of

    non-overlap in work hours (Avery & Zabel, 2001; Milliken, Martins, & Morgan, 1998; Swart,

    1978).

    Pretending by engaging in expressive ambiguity is a tactic within the communication

    domain that actors employ, sometimes in conjunction with loose compliance, to facilitate the post-

    hoc portrayal or interpretation of communication acts as having been in accordance with mandates

    or expectations, while avoiding confrontation or discomfort in real-time. Specific forms of this

    tactic come in the form of "couching" negative comments in the midst of positive or ambiguous

    speech, or "skirting" an issue by talking about related topics while not addressing the contentious

    topic directly. The presence of observers demands that an answer be given, but answers are

    constructed so as to be interpretable in a multitude of directions, allowing for the re-construal of

    meaning after the fact (Weaver, 1986).

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    17/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    17

    Expressive ambiguity is related to the larger domain of strategic ambiguity, which refers

    to the strategic application of ambiguity in terms of both creating and communicating about an

    organizations mission (Eisenberg, 1984). Though the ethics and potential outcomes of strategic

    ambiguity have been debated (Markham, 1996; Ulmer & Sellnow, 1997), as well as the

    circumstances under which it should be used (Eisenberg & Witten, 1987), one of the touted benefits

    is that in certain situations it behooves managers to express an organizations goals in an ambiguous

    manner such that various options remain open and a diversity of viewpoints are fostered. Likewise,

    expressive ambiguity enables actors to actively avoid the aversiveness of confronting and

    potentially angering important stakeholders while still engaging in communication as required by

    the process expectations of the situation. Expressive ambiguity may also facilitate new inventions

    when it allows actors to "buy time" in situations that require a long-term, interpersonally sensitive

    process. Thus, pretending, and specifically tactics such as expressive ambiguity and loose

    compliance, may facilitate accommodation among various parties and interests. We consider the

    positive consequences of pretending in more detail later in the paper.

    Outcome Constraints and Bypassing

    Outcome constraints are generally instituted to hold employees responsible for their results,

    often in the form of "pay for performance" systems and goal setting, which can lead to many

    positive consequences (Jensen & Meckling, 1976; Locke et al., 1990). Bypassing by engaging in

    shortcuts involves the use of an unconventional process for accomplishing a desired end state while

    reducing the aversion experienced in the process. Though negative examples of short cuts abound,

    such as dishonesty in accounting practices in settings that emphasize outcomes while lacking

    oversight of processes (Mazar & Ariely, 2006), shortcuts can also result in the development of tools

    and processes that handle required steps more efficiently. Indeed, from Platos famous exhortation

    about necessity being the mother of invention (Jowett & Campbell, 1941) to early work by

    economists about the ways in which inventions are often the byproducts of attempts at process

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    18/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    18

    improvement (Nelson, 1959), short cuts are known to lead to time-saving and efficient inventions.

    Many computer-based tools were motivated by the desire to avoid arduous computational or data

    processing tasks and attain desired performance outcomes. Similarly, a whole host of physical tools

    (think "electric hammer" or "riding lawn mower") have been innovated with the desire to facilitate

    the accomplishment of physically demanding tasks. What these and many other shortcuts have in

    common is that they are the result of search processes initiated by actors in the face of an aversive

    situation, that subsequently enable the attainment of desired outcomes while reducing the aversion

    associated with the process.

    Indirect communication in lieu of more direct communication methods is an example of

    bypassing in the realm of interpersonal communication in which organizational actors transmit

    information in a more unidirectional and indirect manner in order to enhance speed (by reducing the

    need to synchronize schedules with recipients) and reduce the opportunities for feedback and

    interaction. Indirect communication occurs through the use of messages that are primarily not

    delivered face-to-face and that actors assume will be received and attended to by the intended

    recipients. The rise in the use of technologies allowing indirect communication (i.e. email,

    voicemail, texting, etc.) is an example of bypassing methods that stem from these avoidance

    impulses (Kurtzberg, Naquin, & Belkin, 2005; OLeary & Cummings, 2007) . Such technologies

    have become particularly popular in contexts lacking norms or process expectations regarding face-

    to-face communication (Jarvenpaa & Lang, 2005; Kraut, Rice, Cool, & Fish, 1998), which tend to

    be more results-oriented, outcome-focused environments such as manufacturing production and

    finance (Mazmanian, Yates, & Orlikowski, 2006). Though certainly not all email or voicemail

    communication is the result of avoidance behaviorand people previously avoided quite

    successfully using simpler technologies like letterssome have noted the trend that these new

    technologies have encouraged the younger generations to forego interpersonal encounters in favor

    of these more indirect approaches (Richtel, 2008). More generally speaking, indirect

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    19/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    19

    communication parallels bypassing in the realm of communication, since it is a way of working

    around an aversive interpersonal encounter while accomplishing desired outcomes.

    Proposition 2: Aversion interacts with constraint type to influence active avoidance strategy:

    The salience of process constraints moderates the degree to which pretending occurs, whereas the

    salience of outcome constraints moderates the degree to which bypassing occurs.

    OUTCOMES OF ACTIVE AVOIDANCE: EFFICIENCY AND ACCOMMODATION

    We maintain that pretending and bypassing can lead to positive outcomes in organizations

    by facilitating process inventions that enhance helpful accommodations and efficiency,

    respectively. Of course, active avoidance can have negative consequences, and thus there are

    important boundary conditions that circumscribe the situations under which positive outcomes may

    occur.

    The most obvious negative consequence of pretending is dishonesty in organizations.

    Indeed, both expressive ambiguity and loose compliance involve an element of dishonesty in that

    the truth of the situation is somehow simulated to be unknown, skirted around, or only loosely

    undertaken. The most obvious negative consequence of bypassing is poor quality, both tangibly in

    terms of products and more intangibly in terms of management. For example, incentivizing

    employees on production output when processes for production have been correctly specified (but

    are not monitored) can easily lead to the finding of "short cuts" and consequently undesired

    variance in the product (Parasuraman, Zeithaml, & Berry, 1985) or safety incidents (Zohar, 1980).

    Thus, the negative results of these active avoidance strategies are real and certainly need to be

    acknowledged.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    20/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    20

    Therefore, the positive outcomes of active avoidance can only accrue when at least two

    conditions are present: first, the short- and long-term goals of the organization and the actor are

    aligned and, second, the active avoidance strategies pass basic tests of ethics. Regarding goal

    alignment, the importance of goal alignment has been examined in a wide range of contexts, with

    respect to compensation (Hollensbe & Guthrie, 2000; Jensen & Meckling, 1976), organizational

    citizenship behavior (Smith, Organ, & Near, 1983), and job satisfaction and tenure (Guth &

    MacMillan, 1986) to name a few examples. With respect to active avoidance, when the goals of an

    individual and the organization are aligned, then the cost/benefit analyses driving active avoidance

    at the individual level are likely to be aligned with the organizations interests (Kaplan & Norton,

    1996). In this way, the active avoidance behavior facilitates aversion-reduction for the individual

    actor but does not detract from organizational goals. Regarding ethicality, our discussion of positive

    outcomes of active avoidance is limited to the sphere of ethical behavior, in which selected actions

    conform to the commonly accepted ethical tests of generalization and utilitarianism (Hooker, 2008).

    Though standards of ethicality might vary somewhat from culture to culture (E. Kennedy &

    Lawton, 1996) we maintain that strategies that fail these important ethical tests cannot yield

    outcomes that could be broadly construed as beneficial.

    Despite the potential for negative implications from bypassing and pretending, we also

    maintain that, within the boundary conditions mentioned above, active avoidance can facilitate

    efficiency, due to bypassing, and accommodation, due to pretending, which may benefit both

    individual employees and organizations. More specifically, bypassing can lead to process

    inventions that more efficiently accomplish desired outcomes, and pretending can lead to inventive

    ways of accommodating sources of situational incongruence. In this section, we develop

    propositions concerning the ways in which bypassing and pretending can lead to efficiency and

    accommodation inventions, respectively, on an individual level. In the section that follows, we

    develop a proposition about the ways in which organizations can harness the positive results of

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    21/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    21

    active avoidance and, by doing so, foster a diverse environment in terms of both task solutions and

    management processes.

    Bypassing and Efficiency

    Organizational innovation depends upon invention, which is an original solution resulting

    from the synthesis of information about a need or want and information about the means by which

    the need or want may be met (Marquis, 1969; Schmookler, 1966; Utterback, 1971; Utterback &

    Abernathy, 1975). Innovation is thus an invention that has been implemented in a product or a

    process (Utterback, 1971). The search for alternatives that underlies active avoidance can raise the

    probability that new solutions will be invented, and generating a larger pool of solutions can

    increase the chances that organizational innovation will occur.

    The notion that employees can and should invent novel and efficient means of performing

    tasks is not new; indeed, setting challenging goals while leaving open the means for achieving

    them has been argued to be an effective way to draw out employees' knowledge and skills

    (Hackman, 2002). Similarly, task revision, or deviating from the prescribed task, will lead to

    innovation and improved performance when the work role is inadequately specified (Staw &

    Boettger, 1990). We argue that bypassing can lead to inventions that improve the efficiency or

    effectiveness of a work process while holding the outcome of the activity constant. For example, in

    1970, unbeknownst to their instructors initially, two business school students developed an

    alternative method producing correct answers for their accounting homework more quickly and

    accurately using a computer-based tool. These students invented Visicalc, the precursor to our

    modern spreadsheets. Rather than completing the process by hand, they invented a more efficient

    way to get the right answers in much less time. This invention exemplifies the potential for positive

    benefits from bypassing, which leads to greater efficiency by modifying an aversive task to make it

    less aversive and more efficient. Bypassing can lead to invention when individuals experience

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    22/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    22

    aversion while working within an outcome constraint, using processes that are only loosely

    specified or not observable.

    To be sure, it is possible that in some cases bypassing could result in less efficient

    processes when the typical approach is the most efficient but aversive. However, work in

    individual cognitive psychology and group performance both point to the dominant tendency of

    individuals and groups to drift toward alternatives that are less effortful (Ebenbach & Keltner,

    1998; Johnson et al., 2006; Orbell & Dawes, 1991). Furthermore, the major commodity for trade

    within organizations is time, where employers and employees barter for individual effort and

    attention (Ocasio, 1997; Schriber & Gutek, 1987). Any technique that allows the actor to spend less

    time on a particular task gives him/her additional autonomy for an activity of his or her own

    choosing and/or to incorporate additional and more desirable organizational activities into his or her

    role. Thus we argue that in most situations, the incentives are present for individuals to bypass in

    ways that enhance the efficiency of their work.

    Proposition 3: Bypassing will lead to process inventions that facilitate more efficient

    accomplishment of specified outcomes.

    Pretending and Accommodation

    Accommodation refers to the adaptation or reconciliation of two or more points of

    difference (Merriam-Webster, 1991). As situations demand different approaches or as workers with

    diverse needs or perspectives approach the same situation, it will frequently be the case that the

    same process will not fit them equally. Though organizations often create process constraints in an

    attempt to ensure fairness across employees or to ensure a certain level of coordination and

    consistency of performance, these process constraints may exist at odds with individual or

    situation-specific needs. Similarly, despite all of the praise heaped on to "straight talk," some

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    23/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    23

    interpersonal differences cannot be resolved. Thus, actors in organizations frequently find

    themselves in a bind -- if they proceed as they explicitly "ought" to, it will leave situational

    incongruence unresolved, but if they make explicit that they will not proceed, it will bring trouble

    in a different form. So what is one to do? Pretending enables actors to accommodate local needs

    without creating system-wide upheaval.

    For example, employees may pretend about their whereabouts when certain personal issues

    take precedence, such as medical appointments or attending an event at a childs school,

    particularly in situations in which there are no official policies about flex-time or personal time.

    These same individuals may also make up for lost work time through working at home after hours,

    again technically in violation of policy in many organizations. This pretending behavior, especially

    when organizational and individual goals are aligned, can enable employees to accommodate the

    various demands in their lives in a manner that avoids confrontation for themselves or their

    manager over policy violations. Likewise, people in organizations do not always get along with

    their co-workers on a personal level, but the requirements of their work are inherently interactive.

    Pretending helps employees to manage these relationships such that they remain workable without

    aggravating underlying tensions, which may involve issues that are not under anyone's control or

    may not be appropriate to address in the work context. For example, process expectations often

    exist in the workplace concerning employee socializing, and employees may often feel implicitly (if

    not explicitly) required to attend such events even when they do not enjoy doing so. Pretending may

    help employees deal with these situations, either through the use of expressive ambiguity so as to

    avoid any confrontations or uncomfortable conversations, or through the use of loose compliance,

    i.e. by arriving late and leaving early and thus only loosely complying with the expectation to be

    present in the social situation. We will return to this issue in the next section concerning the ways

    in which active avoidance can be diagnostic for organizations and lead to positive systemic

    changes.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    24/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    24

    Proposition 4: Pretending will facilitate the accommodation of competing demands and/or

    tensions in the work environment.

    Harnessing the Positive Consequences of Active Avoidance: Systems Observation

    At the organizational level, harnessing the positive consequences of active avoidance

    ultimately leads to a greater diversity of invented solutions and management processes. Such

    diversity is welcome given that most workplaces have become more demographically diverse, and

    the needs and demands of organizational work are likewise diverse and sometimes incompatible. In

    the task domain, active avoidance can foster innovation through the proliferation of multiple local

    inventions that increase efficiency. These inventions are stimulated by the search for alternatives to

    adapt to constraints and are optimized to be useful to the user. Moreover, these inventions may

    prove to be useful to others in the organization as well and lead to organization-wide innovations,

    and be better than those that would have arisen if more explicit change-oriented processes had been

    engaged to collectively search for alternatives in the absence of such individual inventions. In the

    interpersonal domain, active avoidance can result in local variations in work processes that

    accommodate the idiosyncratic needs of a situation. For instance, pretending may be beneficial in

    organizations in the interpersonal realm, particularly when it comes to overlooking minor

    differences that may otherwise provoke unproductive or relationship-harming conflict. In the arena

    of interpersonal communication, research on cultural differences in conflict management styles has

    shown that managers from the U.S. tend to employ a competing, direct style whereas managers

    from Asia tend to employ an avoiding conflict style (Morris et al., 1998). These cultural differences

    have been shown to hinder positive working relationships and outcomes (Graham & Sano, 1991;

    Hofstede, 1980). In such circumstances, the encouragement of pretending with an eye toward

    cultural sensitivity, i.e. employing tactics such as expressive ambiguity in order to allow the other

    party to save face rather than engaging in direct confrontation, may lead to positive outcomes in an

    organization, especially when long-term relationships are crucial. Similar strategies of pretending in

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    25/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    25

    the face of otherwise conflict-provoking circumstances have been shown to have beneficial effects

    in preserving relationships even in the absence of cultural differences (Pike & Sillars, 1985).

    The degree to which an organization has developed the ability to observe and diagnose

    active avoidance behavior at a system level will moderate the extent to which they can capitalize on

    the information value that active avoidance behavior provides. Others have pointed out that

    avoidance can have positive signaling effects in organizations (Piderit, 2000) as individuals with

    more local knowledge and a different vantage point can draw attention to those organizational

    processes that are maladaptive for their particular context. Organizations can harness the value of

    these local level inventions through the development of system-level observational tools. Since

    individuals have elected to engage in active avoidance rather than direct confrontation of a situation

    that requires change, direct reporting or questioning of any individual about his or her behavior is

    unlikely to yield useful solutions. Furthermore, confronting a single individual about his or her

    active avoidance behavior is unlikely to provide good, diagnostic information about how

    widespread vs. idiosyncratic an issue really is. However, system-level observation may suggest

    areas in which active avoidance is occurring and the manner in which existing processes could be

    modified. Though system-level observation could be easily interpreted as a "Big Brother"

    mechanism for employee monitoring, the goal of this type of mechanism is exactly the opposite--

    rather than monitoring individuals, managers can make more general observations about systemic

    issues. Doing so equates to a form of swarm intelligence evaluating the quality of a new

    nesting location not because one individual thinks it is great, but because a series of individuals are

    beginning to move in a particular direction (Bonabeau, Dorigo, & Theraulaz, 1999).

    Returning to the example of hand washing described earlier in the paper, monitoring of

    individual employees may be difficult and may ultimately not solve a problem (e.g. lack of proper

    hand washing) that is chronic throughout an organization. Instead, managers could monitor soap

    levels in different locations, or paper towel usage (a system indicator) as well as outcome indicators

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    26/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    26

    in different locations (i.e. infection rates associated with different rooms in the hospital).

    Furthermore, rather than interpreting active avoidance in this domain as deviant behavior by

    individuals, managers could use the information about active avoidance to decide whether it is a

    local problem or a system-wide problem that needs to be addressed. Atul Gawande, in his book

    Better: A Surgeon's Notes on Performance, recounts a fascinating success story of a Veterans

    Administration hospital in Pennsylvania in which the rate of bacterial infection was reduced almost

    to zero as a result of significantly improved hand washing rates. The process of improving this

    behavior can be interpreted as resulting from observation of active avoidance and the use of that

    information to make systemic changes to processes in the hospital in order to make hand washing

    less aversive in that case, by cutting down the amount of time needed to accomplish the necessary

    level of hand hygiene.

    Pretending behavior can also provide organizations with diagnostic information and enable

    organizations to make process changes that will ultimately foster a more diverse workforce. An

    organization in which people are able to accommodate their various needs and demands on could

    operate more effectively as a diverse organization. Returning to the example of loose compliance

    with work hours in order to accommodate work and family needs discussed above, the observation

    of this behavior could lead organizations to make systemic changes, i.e. introducing flex-time

    policies, which ultimately facilitate the hiring and retention of a more diverse workforce. These

    policies have been shown to have predominantly positive effects on work outcomes (Baltes, Briggs,

    Huff, Wright, & Neuman, 1999). Furthermore, employees are more likely to be successful in

    getting flex-time schedules following active avoidance behavior versus making a direct request

    (Hochschild, 2001). Current employees who gradually (but implicitly) shift their work hours while

    demonstrating that they can still be effective are generally more successful in negotiating these

    arrangements than new employees who attempt to directly ask for them.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    27/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    27

    Thus, cultivating system-level observation of the behaviors and inventions of employees

    who are resolving situational incongruence will provide an organization with better information

    about where change is needed as well as potential alternative solutions. For example, if an

    individual complains loudly to management that the work hours are unreasonable, it is difficult for

    management to determine how widespread the problem is or what is a workable solution. By

    contrast, if management observes how full the parking lot is at different hours, how busy the

    meeting rooms are, and the amount of computer network and telephone usage over the course of the

    day, then they can determine when the need for worker overlap is greatest and where flexibility

    might be appreciated. They may also observe some alternative techniques that workers innovate to

    handle demands from outside of the office which are beneficial for everyone (Morgan 1977). If

    instead they chastise individual workers who are not at their desk or attempt in other ways to

    observe or enforce requirements locally, they might force workers to engage in some of the other

    behaviors described above (withdrawal, deviance, etc.), which are not as beneficial for the

    organization, thereby ostracizing some workers and not discovering more effective ways of

    operating that would facilitate greater diversity. Thus, we argue that observation of patterns of

    behavior across people and over time provides better insight into difficulties and potential solutions

    than does the interrogation or correction of a single avoider and/or the open invitation to individuals

    to voice complaints without evidence of solutions. Consequently, we propose that system-level

    observation will moderate the degree to which organizations obtain diagnostic information about

    which problems require addressing and which solutions might be more appropriate.

    Proposition 5: The adoption of system-level observation and diagnostic capability will

    moderate the degree to which organizations recognize and capitalize on the positive outcomes of

    efficiency and accommodation resulting from individual-level active avoidance.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    28/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    28

    Conclusion

    We have introduced a new constructactive avoidancethat involves the implicit search

    for and use of alternatives by individuals motivated to reduce the negative experience of aversion

    due to situational incongruence in organizations. Unlike traditional conceptualizations of

    avoidance, active avoidance does not entail passivity or inhibition, but rather active strategies to

    pursue alternatives in light of aversive circumstances. Furthermore, we have argued that process-

    versus outcome-focused constraints will interact with aversion and determine whether active

    avoidance will entail pretending versus bypassing. We also have proposed two ways in which

    active avoidance can have positive ramifications for organizations in the forms of process

    inventions that facilitate efficiency and accommodation on the individual level, which ultimately

    translate into a greater diversity of solutions and the possibility of better diagnostic processes on the

    organizational level.

    Some may read our discussion here as overly pessimistic about organizational voice and

    learning, and as such, we would like to emphasize the point that we see it not as either/or, but rather

    when to use one approach versus another. In situations in which problems and potential solutions

    are unfamiliar and possibly idiosyncratic, active avoidance will allow individuals and their

    organizations to pursue temporary local fixes. As situations evolve and it becomes clear that a

    problem is relatively widespread and persistent, then raising those issues as well as pointing to the

    kinds of solutions individuals have invented can facilitate a more productive discussion. Thus we

    see active avoidance as a potential precursor to and facilitator of more productive, solution-focused

    and change-oriented discussions in organizations. Active avoidance can also help employees cope

    with aversive situations and prevent more irreparable actions, such as withdrawal and neglect.

    A broader understanding of active avoidance will lead to research about active forms of

    avoidance in organizations that have not been captured previously in organizational research. One

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    29/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    29

    of the reasons that this area has been understudied is that action is much easier to identify and

    measure than inaction, and previous conceptualizations of avoidance involved inaction and

    withdrawal. Not only was this a narrow conceptualization of avoidance in organizations, but it is

    also difficult to study this conceptualization of avoidance empirically using traditional methods

    such as self-report measures, since social desirability biases impede reporting of outright avoidance

    and presumably leads to floor effects. However, measurement of the active avoidance strategies

    described in this paper will enable researchers to obtain a deeper understanding of avoidance

    behaviors in organizations. Better recognition of the existence of active avoidance and measuring

    its prevalence will help advance the precision of organizational research.

    Finally, a better understanding of active avoidance and its potential benefits may create a

    more effective bridge between theory and practice (Pfeffer, 1993). Studying active avoidance is an

    example of the ways in which organizational research can make a significant contribution by

    building on existing psychological principles such as avoidance, but also considering the ways in

    which organizational contexts create unique spaces for these psychological phenomena to be

    manifested in different ways. The demands of the work domain, both in terms of characteristics of

    organizations as well as fulfillment of the basic need for employment, lead to behaviors that are

    particular to that context. Avoiding painespecially when "pain" manifests itself in the form of

    demanding organizational constraints may entail pretending and/or bypassing, particularly when

    traditionally passive avoidance techniques, such as withdrawal and delay, are not possible or

    desirable. In this way, future research on active avoidance has the potential to better inform a

    realistic portrait of people's experiences in organizations. Finally, a better understanding of active

    avoidance can aid managers to harness the potential benefits of resulting process inventions,

    leading to a more diverse organization that fosters more effective processes and solutions.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    30/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    30

    References

    Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(2), 357-376.Ashforth, B. E., & Lee, R. T. (1990). Defensive behavior in organizations: A preliminary model.

    Human Relations, 43(7), 621-648.Avery, C., & Zabel, D. (2001). The flexible workplace: A sourcebook of information and research:

    Greenwood Publishing Group.

    Bargh, J., Gollwitzer, P., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Trotschel, R. (2001). The automated will:Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 81(6), 1014-1027.

    Barker, J. (1993). Tightening the iron cage: Concertive control in self-managing teams.

    Administrative science quarterly, 38(3), 408-437.

    Benner, M. J., & Tushman, M. L. (2003). Exploitation, exploration, and process management: The

    productivity dilemma revisited. The Academy of Management Review, 238-256.

    Bennis, W. (1963). A new role for the behavioral sciences: Effecting organizational change.

    Administrative science quarterly, 8(2), 125-165.

    Bettenhausen, K., & Murnighan, J. (1985). The emergence of norms in competitive decision-making groups. Administrative science quarterly, 30(3), 350-372.

    Bonabeau, E., Dorigo, M., & Theraulaz, G. (1999). Swarm intelligence: From natural to artificial

    systems: Oxford University Press, USA.

    Cannon, W. B. (1932). The wisdom of the body. New York: Norton.

    de Shazer, S., Berg, I., Lipchik, E., & Nunnally, E. (1986). Brief therapy: Focused solution

    development. Family Process, 25(2), 207-221.

    Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy: An analysis in terms of

    learning, thinking, and culture: McGraw-Hill.

    Ebenbach, D., & Keltner, D. (1998). Power, emotion, and judgmental accuracy in social conflict:

    Motivating the cognitive miser. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 20(1), 7-21.

    Eisenberg, E. M. (1984). Ambiguity as strategy in organizational communication. Communication

    Monographs, 51(3), 227-242.Eisenberg, E. M., & Witten, M. G. (1987). Reconsidering openness in organizational

    communication. ACADEMY OF MANAGEMENT REVIEW, 418-426.

    Eiszler, C. F. (2002). College students' evaluations of teaching and grade inflation. Research in

    Higher Education, 43(4), 483-501.

    Farrell, D. (1983). Exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect as responses to job dissatisfaction: A

    multidimensional scaling study. ACADEMY OF MANAGEMENT JOURNAL, 596-607.

    Feldman, D. (1984). The development and enforcement of group norms. ACADEMY OF

    MANAGEMENT REVIEW, 47-53.

    Forrester, J. (1994). System dynamics, systems thinking, and soft or. System.

    Freud, S., & Hubback, C. J. M. (1922). Beyond the pleasure principle. New York: The International

    Psycho-Analytical Press.

    Gingerich, W., & Eisengart, S. (2000). Solution-focused brief therapy: A review of the outcomeresearch. Family Process, 39(4), 477-498.

    Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behaviour

    Research and Therapy, 8(3), 249.

    Griffin, M. A., & Neal, A. (2000). Perceptions of safety at work: A framework for linking safety

    climate to safety performance, knowledge, and motivation. Journal of Occupational Health

    Psychology, 5(3), 347-358.Guth, W. D., & MacMillan, I. C. (1986). Strategy implementation versus middle management self-

    interest. Strategic Management Journal, 7(4), 313-327.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    31/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    31

    Hackman, J. R. (2002). Leading teams: Setting the stage for great performances. Boston: Harvard

    Business School Press.

    Hamel, G., & Valikangas, L. (2003). The quest for resilience. Harvard Business Review, 81(9), 52-

    65.

    Harris, N. N., & Sutton, R. I. (1983). Task procrastination in organizations: A framework for

    research. Human Relations, 36(11), 987.

    Higgins, E. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review,94(3), 319-340.

    Higgins, E. (1989). Self-discrepancy theory: What patterns of self-beliefs cause people to suffer.

    Advances in experimental social psychology, 22, 93-136.

    Higgins, E., Roney, C., Crowe, E., & Hymes, C. (1994). Ideal versus ought predilections for

    approach and avoidance: Distinct self-regulatory systems. Journal of Personality and

    Social Psychology, 66, 276-276.

    Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST, 52, 1280-1300.

    Hochschild, A. (2001). The time bind: When work becomes home and home becomes work: Holt

    Paperbacks.

    Hollensbe, E. C., & Guthrie, J. P. (2000). Group pay-for-performance plans: The role of

    spontaneous goal setting. The Academy of Management Review, 25(4), 864-872.

    Hooker, J. (2008). Ethics as rational choice. fromhttp://web.tepper.cmu.edu/ethics/rationalchoice.pdf

    Isen, A. M., & Baron, R. A. (1991). Positive affect as a factor in organizational behavior.

    RESEARCH IN ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR, 13(1), 1-53.

    Izraeli, D. M., & Jick, T. D. (1986). The art of saying no: Linking power to culture. Organization

    Studies, 7(2), 171-192.

    Jackson, M., Golub, B., & Building, L. Naive learning in social networks: Convergence, influence

    and wisdom of crowds.

    Jarvenpaa, S. L., & Lang, K. R. (2005). Managing the paradoxes of mobile technology. Information

    Systems Management, 22(4), 7-23.

    Jensen, M. C., & Meckling, W. (1976). Theory of the firm: Managerial behavior, agency costs, and

    capital structure. Journal of Financial Economics, 3(4), 305-360.

    Johnson, M., Hollenbeck, J., Humphrey, S., Ilgen, D., Jundt, D., & Meyer, C. (2006). Cutthroatcooperation: Asymmetrical adaptation to changes in team reward structures. ACADEMY

    OF MANAGEMENT JOURNAL, 49(1), 103-119.

    Jowett, B., & Campbell, L. (1941). Plato's republic. New York: The Modern Library

    Kanter, R. (1983). The change masters: Innovation for productivity in the american corporation:

    Simon & Schuster.

    Kaplan, R. S., & Norton, D. P. (1996). The balanced scorecard: Translating strategy into action:

    Harvard Business School Press.

    Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical

    integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 681-681.

    Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. (1978). The social psychology of organizations. New York: John Wiley &

    Sons.

    Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition.Psychological Review, 110(2), 265-284.

    Kennedy, E., & Lawton, L. (1996). The effects of social and moral integration on ethical standards:

    A comparison of american and ukrainian business students. Journal of Business Ethics,

    15(8), 901-911.

    Kennedy, J., Eberhart, R., & Shi, Y. (2001). Swarm intelligence: Springer.

    Kraut, R. E., Rice, R. E., Cool, C., & Fish, R. S. (1998). Varieties of social influence: The role of

    utility and norms in the success of a new communication medium. ORGANIZATION

    SCIENCE, 437-453.

    http://web.tepper.cmu.edu/ethics/rationalchoice.pdfhttp://web.tepper.cmu.edu/ethics/rationalchoice.pdfhttp://web.tepper.cmu.edu/ethics/rationalchoice.pdf
  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    32/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    32

    Kurtzberg, T. R., Naquin, C. E., & Belkin, L. Y. (2005). Electronic performance appraisals: The

    effects of e-mail communication on peer ratings in actual and simulated environments.

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 98(2), 216-226.

    Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer Pub Co.

    Lerner, J. S., & Tetlock, P. E. (1999). Accounting for the effects of accountability.

    PSYCHOLOGICAL BULLETIN, 125(2), 255-275.

    Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance. EnglewoodCliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Locke, E. A., Latham, G. P., & Smith, K. J. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance:

    Prentice-Hall.

    Luthans, F. (2002). The need for and meaning of positive organizational behavior. Journal of

    Organizational Behavior, 23(6), 695-706.

    March, J. G., Simon, H. A., & Guetzkow, H. S. (1958). Organizations: John Wiley & Sons Inc.

    Markham, A. (1996). Designing discourse: A critical analysis of strategic ambiguity and workplace

    control. Management Communication Quarterly, 9(4), 389.

    Markus, H. (1978). The effect of mere presence on social facilitation: An unobtrusive test. Journal

    of Experimental Social Psychology, 14(4), 389-397.

    Marquis, D. (1969). A project team plus pert equals success. Or does it. Innovation, 1(3), 28-37.

    Mazar, N., & Ariely, D. (2006). Dishonesty in everyday life and its policy implications. Journal ofPublic Policy & Marketing, 25(1), 117-126.

    Mazmanian, M., Yates, J., & Orlikowski, W. (2006). Ubiquitous email: Individual experiences and

    organizational consequences of blackberry use. Paper presented at the Academy of

    Management, Atlanta, GA.McNaughton, N., & Corr, P. J. (2004). A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense:

    Fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28(3), 285-

    305.

    Menon, T., Thompson, L., & Choi, H. S. (2006). Tainted knowledge vs. Tempting knowledge:

    People avoid knowledge from internal rivals and seek knowledge from external rivals.

    Management Science, 52(8), 1129.

    Merriam-Webster, I. (1991). Webster's ninth new collegiate dictionary: Merriam-Webster.

    Milliken, F., Martins, L., & Morgan, H. (1998). Explaining organizational responsiveness to work-family issues: The role of human resource executives as issue interpreters. The Academy of

    Management Journal, 41(5), 580-592.

    Mobley, W. H. (1977). Intermediate linkages in the relationship between job satisfaction and

    employee turnover. Journal of applied psychology, 62(2), 237-240.

    Near, J., & Miceli, M. (1986). Retaliation against whistle blowers: Predictors and effects. Journal

    of applied psychology, 71(1), 137-145.

    Nelson, R. R. (1959). The economics of invention: A survey of the literature. Journal of Business,

    101-127.

    Nembhard, I., & Edmondson, A. (2006). Making it safe: The effects of leader inclusiveness and

    professional status on psychological safety and improvement efforts in health care teams.

    Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(7), 941-966.

    OLeary, M., & Cummings, J. (2007). The spatial, temporal, and configurational characteristics ofgeographic dispersion in work teams. Mis Quarterly, 31(3), 433.

    Ocasio, W. (1997). Towards an attention-based view of the firm. Strategic Management Journal,

    18, 187-206.

    Orbell, J., & Dawes, R. (1991). A" Cognitive miser" Theory of cooperators' advantage. The

    American Political Science Review, 85(2), 515-528.

    Parasuraman, A., Zeithaml, V. A., & Berry, L. L. (1985). A conceptual model of service quality and

    its implications for future research. Journal of Marketing, 49(4), 41-50.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    33/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    33

    Pruitt, D. G., & Carnevale, P. J. (1993). Negotiation in social conflict. Pacific Grove, CA:

    Brooks/Cole Pub. Co.

    Richtel, M. (2008, August 2, 2008). Don't want to talk about it? Order a missed call. The New York

    Times,

    Rusbult, C. E., Farrell, D., Rogers, G., & Mainous Iii, A. G. (1988). Impact of exchange variables

    on exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect: An integrative model of responses to declining job

    satisfaction. ACADEMY OF MANAGEMENT JOURNAL, 599-627.Schmookler, J. (1966). Innovation and economic growth. Cambridge, Mass.

    Schriber, J., & Gutek, B. (1987). Some time dimensions of work: Measurement of an underlying

    aspect of organization culture. Journal of applied psychology, 72(4), 642-650.

    Seligman, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. AMERICAN

    PSYCHOLOGIST, 55(1), 5-14.

    Senge, P., & Sterman, J. (1992). Systems thinking and organizational learning: Acting locally and

    thinking globally in the organization of the future. Transforming organizations, 353371.

    Siegel-Jacobs, K., & Yates, J. F. (1996). Effects of procedural and outcome accountability on

    judgment quality. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(1), 1-17.

    Simonson, I., & Staw, B. M. (1992). Deescalation strategies: A comparison of techniques for

    reducing commitment to losing courses of action. Journal of applied psychology, 77(4),

    419-426.Smith, C., Organ, D. W., & Near, J. P. (1983). Organizational citizenship behavior: Its nature and

    antecedents. Journal of applied psychology, 68(4), 653-663.

    Staw, B. M., & Boettger, R. D. (1990). Task revision: A neglected form of work performance.

    ACADEMY OF MANAGEMENT JOURNAL, 534-559.Surowiecki, J., & Silverman, M. (2007). The wisdom of crowds. American Journal of Physics, 75,

    190.

    Sutton, R. I., & Galunic, C. (1996). Consequences of public scrutiny for leaders and their

    organizations. RESEARCH IN ORGANIZATIONAL BEHAVIOR, 18, 201-259.

    Swart, J. (1978). A flexible approach to working hours: Amacom.

    Tett, R. P., & Meyer, J. P. (1993). Job satisfaction, organizational commitment, turnover intention,

    and turnover: Path analyses based on meta-analytic findings. Personnel Pyschology, 46(2),

    259-293.Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in

    animals. . Psychological Review, 2(4), 1-109.

    Trepper, T., Dolan, Y., McCollum, E., & Nelson, T. (2006). Steve de shazer and the future of

    solution-focused therapy. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 32(2), 133.

    Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110(3), 403-421.

    Ulmer, R. R., & Sellnow, T. L. (1997). Strategic ambiguity and the ethic of significant choice in the

    tobacco industrys crisis communication. Communication Studies, 48(3), 215233.Utterback, J. (1971). The process of technological innovation within the firm. The Academy of

    Management Journal, 14(1), 75-88.

    Utterback, J., & Abernathy, W. (1975). A dynamic model of product and process innovation.

    Omega, 3(6), 639-656.

    Vallacher, R., Wegner, D., & Somoza, M. (1989). That's easy for you to say: Action identificationand speech fluency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2), 199-208.

    Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1985). A theory of action identification: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they're doing? Action

    identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94(1), 3-15.

    Weaver, R. K. (1986). The politics of blame avoidance. Journal of Public Policy, 6(4), 371-398.

    Woolley, A. W. (2009a). Means versus ends: Implications of outcome and process focus for team

    adaptation and performance. ORGANIZATION SCIENCE, 20, 500-515.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    34/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    34

    Woolley, A. W. (2009b). Putting first things first: Outcome and process focus in knowledge work

    teams. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30, 427-452.

    Zohar, D. (1980). Safety climate in industrial organizations: Theoretical and applied implications.

    Journal of applied psychology, 65(1), 96-102.

  • 8/7/2019 Dont talk about it- Active avoidance in organizations-Woolley

    35/35

    Active Avoidance in Organizations

    FIGURE 1

    Model of proposed antecedents, moderators and outcomes of active avoidance

    Incongruence

    -induced

    Aversion

    Accommodatio

    ActiveProcess

    constraints

    Pretending

    Bypassing Efficiency

    Outcome

    constraints

    Process