california state university, northridge therefore

44
CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, NORTHRIDGE THEREFORE, SOCRATES IS A TALKING HEAD: BELIEF BIAS AND SYLLOGISTIC REASONING ERRORS IN CABLE NEWS A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of Master of Arts in Psychology, General Experimental By Alexander Benson Swan May 2011

Upload: others

Post on 06-Apr-2022

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, NORTHRIDGE

THEREFORE, SOCRATES IS A TALKING HEAD:

BELIEF BIAS AND SYLLOGISTIC REASONING ERRORS IN CABLE NEWS

A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements

For the degree of Master of Arts

in Psychology, General Experimental

By

Alexander Benson Swan

May 2011

The thesis of Alexander Benson Swan is approved:

Dustin P. Calvillo, Ph.D. Date'

Mark P. Otten, Ph.D. Date

Abraham M. Rutchick, Ph.D., Chair Date

California State University, Northridge

ii

Dedication

For Astrid,

whose support and love continue to amaze me.

111

Signature Page

Dedication

Abstract

Introduction

Table of Contents

Rational Choice and The Hostile Media Bias

The Power of Persuasion

The Belief Bias in Syllogistic Reasoning

The Present Study

Method

Participants

Results

Design and Materials

Procedure

Preliminary Analyses

Primary Analyses

Secondary Analyses

Discussion

References

Appendix A

Appendix B

IV

11

111

v

1

2

4

6

12

14

14

14

16

17

17

19

20

21

25

33

37

ABSTRACT

THEREFORE, SOCRATES IS A TALKING HEAD:

BELIEF BIAS AND SYLLOGISTIC REASONING ERRORS IN CABLE NEWS

By

Alexander Benson Swan

Master of Arts

in Psychology, General Experimental

Cable news media has grown exponentially in audience over the last 30 years, and in that

time, argumentation and debate have become fractured and fallacious. Political partisans

refuse to watch pundits or commentators with viewpoints that differ from their own. Due

to this divide, a hostile perception of media outlets increases. Though many factors

contribute to media hostility, belief bias in logical reasoning was considered for this

study. Political ideology and congruent arguments may be believable but not necessarily

valid, perpetuating fallacious argumentation and improper debate. Participants (n = 98)

completed an argument validity evaluation task, determining if 16 arguments paraphrased

from cable news pundits and commentators were structurally valid. Half of the arguments

were liberal and the other half conservative. Additionally, half were valid and the other

half invalid. Thus, four argument types were used in the study. Overall, the beliefbias did

not influence liberals and conservatives on the argument evaluation task. The data do

v

suggest a trend for bias, with participants without formal logic training the most

susceptible to beliefs. Implications of these findings, including limitations of the current

experiment and paths for future research of this topic, are discussed.

VI

Introduction

In the United States, 24-hour cable news channels began in the 1980s. In the last

10 years, these channels began to offer news, political discussion, and commentary nearly

every day ofthe year. The audience of these channels has nearly doubled from the mid-

1990s to the mid-2000s (Coe et al., 2007), and it appears this trend is not about to stop.

They now focus their evening programs on political commentary (MSNBC, Lawrence

O'Donnell and Rachel Maddow; Fox News Channel, Bill O'Reilly and Sean Hannity).

Pmiisanship is no stranger to these programs, with MSNBC accused with a liberal bias

and Fox News accused with a conservative bias, and CNN trying to strike a middle

ground between the two (Coe et al., 2007). Due to these divisions, liberals and

conservatives watch their respective channels, making little effort to hear

counterattitudinal arguments that might rattle the certainty of their position on the issues

(Tormala & Petty, 2002).

Researchers have shown that liberals and conservatives are different in their

worldviews and morals (e.g., Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009; Haidt & Graham, 2007;

Haidt & Joseph, 2004). Moreover, on a task examining open-ended responses on 12life­

narrative scenes, in which respondents were to describe the scene in as much detail as

possible (e.g., a positive childhood scene), liberals were more likely to recall scenes in

which they developed empathy and learned to open up to new perspectives, while

conservatives recalled the scenes recognizing the importance of self-discipline and

personal responsibility (McAdams et al., 2008). Liberals are identified by their

agreeableness or compassion, whereas conservatives are characterized by politeness and

orderliness (Hirsh, DeYoung, Xu, & Peterson, 2010). Surprisingly, conservatives show a

1

greater well-being (measured by scales oflife satisfaction, sexual activity, marital status,

church attendance, employment status, education, and income) than do liberals (Napier &

Jost, 2008). The authors explain this difference by arguing that liberals are unable to

frame the negative effects associated with a lower socioeconomic status in a positive or

neutral manner (Napier & Jost, 2008). Overall, these differences shape how issues and

events are viewed. Additionally, these studies of partisan bias and motivated reasoning

have shown that if a partisan is confronted with information that is emotionally aversive

(i.e., it brings about feelings of anger or disgust), he or she will actively seek an

alternative conclusion (Westen et al., 2007). Ifliberals and conservatives approach issues

from different moral foundations, and have different governing philosophies, it follows

that worldviews could be exaggerated when presented with information that is

counterattitudinal. As more people look to cable news for their daily information, it is

important to consider whether the current polarization in news could be problematic. That

is, the ever-increasing divide reflected in cable news could make possible the

perpetuation of logically flawed yet highly believable information to the public.

Rational Choice and The Hostile Media Bias

The rational choice model, from a political science and economic perspective,

assumes that people act rationally when presented with a choice, weighing the costs and

the benefits and making the most utilitarian decision with the most benefits and least cost

(Lovett, 2006). From an early age, people are taught to act rationally or at least trained to

give the appearance of rationality in daily decision-making (Scott, 2006). However, from

a psychological perspective, the theory fits normal human behavior poorly. A slightly

2

different perspective in reinforcing future behavior is suggested: that of melioration, or

the process ofimproving one's choices overtime (Herrnstein, 1990).

Reinforcing the notion of improving decisions over time (melioration) is the claim

that consuming information from biased sources that are only compatible with a person's

worldview is performed in order to minimize errors in judgment and decision-making

(Calvert, 1985). If a person is constantly inundated with information that he or she does

not agree with, large errors might appear and create problems with discourse and

decision-making. However, it has also been suggested that viewing information that a

person opposes to a degree (without being hostile toward) is essential for a properly

functioning democracy (Mutz & Martin, 2001 ), and that consuming a greater number of

biased sources allows for improved information-gathering (Xiang & Sarvary, 2007).

However, because of this, cable news has become less homogenized and more slanted,

and framing of news stories to fit the slant has become the norm. It creates a power

struggle for being "fair," "balanced," or "most trusted." Proper argumentation and

discourse cannot survive in this type of environment. These activities are worsened when

perception becomes hostile and an accusation of bias becomes overt indignation with

overwhelmingly negative emotion (Hwang, Pan, & Sun, 2008). This can also be

described as a process of selective categorization, where liberals and conservatives apply

positive or negative emotions to events and facts in the same unbiased content differently

than their counterparts (Scmitt, Gunther, & Liebhart, 2004). In an environment full of

biased sources and hostility between opposing viewpoints arises the Hostile Media

Phenomenon (HMP).

3

The HMP was first identified during the mid-1980s in an experiment of people's

views on coverage of the Beirut Massacre (Vallone, Ross, & Lepper, 1985). The

experimenters found that people would claim that the news information was biased when

it contradicted their perspective on issues regarding the Middle East. The researchers

expanded it to account for any general tendency for partisans to claim bias of any source

that opposes their worldview. In other words, it is the pervasive belief that a media source

is biased if and only if the content contradicts a consumer's prior beliefs and attitudes.

However, at the time, the source of bias was identified as the consumer rather than the

media (Gunther, 1992). Additionally, the HMP has been identified in balanced news

stories, or stories free from slant or logical fallacy (Kim & Pasadeos, 2007). Now, the

political narratives chosen by these channels have influenced the commentators and

interviewers, creating a situation in which the interviewer has greater persuasive power

on the audience than a politician or expert (Babad, 2005). Many of these factors create a

highly partisan environment, where moderates and those not heavily involved in politics

cannot engage in public discourse and proper argumentation to discuss current and future

events (Eveland & Shah, 2003).

The Power of Persuasion

In the span of a day, people are faced with hundreds, if not thousands, of choices.

People have attitudes and beliefs on any number of topics and they are constantly

evolving and changing. Attitudes are defined as "general evaluations people hold in

regard to themselves, other people, objects, and issues. These general evaluations can be

based on a variety of behavioral, affective, and cognitive experiences, and are capable of

influencing behavioral, affective, and cognitive processes" (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986, p.

4

127). If attitudes are basic evaluations, it seems they can change on their own (evolve) or

change in the face of new information (persuasion).

Persuasion has been studied extensively (e.g., Cacioppo & Petty, 1979; Chaiken

& Eagly, 1976; Petty, Cacioppo, & Goldman, 1981). Early studies of attitude change

focused on the various media through which a persuasive message was presented. These

media consisted of written, audio, and audiovisual (video) messages, as the advent of the

Internet was years away. Audio and video were found to be more persuasive than written

messages, attributable to the non-message cues apparent in each medium, such as the

physical characteristics of the presenter, body language, or voice tone. Additionally,

messages that were easy to process (that is, fewer jargon words and shorter sentences)

were the most persuasive when given by audiovisual means (Chaiken & Eagly, 1976).

However, the persuasive power of the message should not be underestimated. If

the message's position on an issue that is proximal to a person's own position, then it is

said to be within the person's latitude of acceptance. As the distance between the

perceiver's beliefs and the position advocated in the message grows, then the message is

perceived as biased and eventually falls within the person's latitude of rejection. The

further the message's position from the perceiver's own, the less likely it is that attitude

change will occur (Hovland, Harvey, & Sherif, 1957).

The communicator of the persuasive message is an integral part for attitude

change. Likeable communicators are more persuasive through audio and video media,

suggesting that the effect is due to the nonverbal cues associated with audio and video

messages allowing faster and stronger attitude-shifting (Chaiken & Eagly, 1983). In

addition, the credibility of the message communicator affects people who are

5

unmotivated and unable to assess persuasive messages, with high source credibility more

persuasive to them than to people who are motivated and able to assess these messages

(Heesacker, Petty, & Cacioppo, 1983). These findings seem to suggest that the message

communicator (in this case, a news anchor or pundit) is as important in persuasion as the

message itself.

With a growing immersion in the bias-laden medium of cable news, people are

more likely to argue improperly and illogically reason through issues with others if

messages are highly persuasive and readily perpetuated. This is a worrisome trend. It is

easy to argue fallaciously while presenting truthful statements-the truth is not what is

important. A truthful argument without validity lacks strength, soundness, and cogency.

An important distinction between truth and validity would be to imagine the foundation

of a building as argument validity: the building and all that exists inside (truth) cannot

stand without a proper foundation. According to Hurley (2004), there are a number of

formal fallacies (which happen only to deductive arguments with an identifiable form)

and 22 informal fallacies that can occur in everyday argumentation (e.g., red herring, ad

hominem, straw man, etc.). Additionally, there are fallacies that are specific to syllogistic

reasoning (e.g., undistributed middle). Lurking behind many of these fallacies is the

belief bias, or the reasoning error in which a person judges an argument as logically valid

if the content is highly believable, even if the argument is logically invalid and/or false.

The Belief Bias in Syllogistic Reasoning

Syllogisms have been discussed since the time of Aristotle. They are the simplest

form of logical argumentation, and can take many different forms. For example, one

classic syllogism consists of the following: "All men are mortal. Socrates is a man.

6

Therefore, Socrates is mortal." Traditionally, a categorical syllogism contains two

premises (the first is termed the major premise; the second is termed the minor premise)

and a conclusion. These premises are linked by a middle term that appears in both

(Hurley, 2004). In deductive syllogisms, the two premises contain all the information

necessary and sufficient to draw a specific conclusion (Evans, Newstead, & Byrne,

1993). Due to their ease and structure, this thesis will focus on deductive categorical

(relating three categories together) syllogisms. In each of these, there is a major term, a

minor term, and a middle term. The conclusion contains the major and minor terms,

linked in the premises by the middle term (Evans, Barston, & Pollard, 1983). Evans et al.

describes four different forms (termed "figures") and combinations these terms can make

to form valid arguments. Note that the validity of an argument is based on its form, not its

content. For example, take the generic argument: "All A are B; All Bare C; Therefore,

all A are C". The items A and Care the major and minor terms, respectively, linked

together by the middle term, in this case, B. The argument is therefore valid; the actual

content of A, B, and Care not relevant to the argument's validity. If the Band Care

switched in the second premise, then the form is no longer valid, and the conclusion does

not necessarily follow from the premises, regardless of whether the premises are true.

Before a participant can judge validity, the argument's premises must be

evaluated for form and structure. There are two main theories of how syllogistic

arguments are represented in the brain. Mental Rules Theory ( cf. Braine, 1978) suggests

that when people evaluate arguments, they use the rules associated with that argument, in

this case, syllogisms using the words "all", "some are ... are not", and "no[ne]" to judge

validity (Goel, 2003; Gaiotti, Baron, & Sabini, 1986). The other is Mental Models

7

Theory, which suggests that people first formulate cognitive models of the premises and

then separately evaluate the conclusion multiple times to see if it fits these models

(Johnson-Laird, 1983; Johnson-Laird & Byrne, 1991; Johnson-Laird & Steedman, 1978).

The main difference between these two theories of reasoning is in the type of reasoning

that is being evaluated. Mental Rules Theory suggests that people tend to follow more

formal paths of reasoning, such as conditional reasoning ("if, then" statements; Braine,

1978), whereas Mental Models Theory is more attuned to looser reasoning, such as

categorical reasoning (e.g., deductively linking three items together; Johnson-Laird &

Steedman, 1978). According to Mental Rules Theory, errors result from not following the

rules oflogic (i.e., the structure or the words). According to Mental Models Theory,

errors result from arguments not being tested exhaustively, through each iteration of

possible combinations of premises and conclusions. Most of the time, deduction goes

from general to specific (Hurley, 2004), so Mental Models Theory posits that proper

argumentation is about "spatial manipulation and search"-that is, organizing the

iterations of an argument visually and spatially (Goel, 2003, p. 3). The two theories posit

different mechanisms in the brain to allow for argument evaluation and production:

Mental Rules should engage the brain's language system in the brain (usually during

abstract reasoning), while Mental Models should engage the visuo-spatial system (usually

during concrete reasoning). According to brain imaging done by Goel and colleagues

(Goel, 2003; Goel, 2007; Goel & Dolan, 2004; Vartanian, Goel, Tierney, Huey, &

Grafman, 2009), both systems work together in what they described as a dual mechanism

theory, whereby the content of the argument dictates the path of reasoning in the brain.

8

The belief bias, in various forms, has been the most closely examined (e.g., Evans

et al., 1983; Feather, 1964; Morgan & Morton, 1944) and has survived the most scrutiny

(Evans, Handley, & Harper, 2001; Newstead, Pollard, Evans, & Allen, 1992) of the errors

that can occur in syllogistic reasoning tasks. Two other explanations for errors in logical

reasoning have also drawn considerable interest: atmosphere effects and illogical

conversion. The atmosphere effect is described as the general mood (positive or negative,

universal or particular) of the premises dictating the valid conclusion (Begg & Denny,

1969; Sells, 1936; Sells & Koob, 1936; Woodworth & Sells, 1935). Illogical conversion

was an alternate explanation for errors made during reasoning tasks not attributable to the

atmosphere effect. This process was described as participants taking the premise "All A

are B" and interpreting/encoding it as "All Bare A" (Chapman & Chapman, 1959,

Revlis, 1975a, 1975b; Revlin, Ammerman, Petersen, & Leirer, 1978; Revlin & Leirer,

1978). However, both of these errors can be easily trained against and removed

(Dickstein, 1975; Simpson & Johnson, 1966). Beliefbias was the only factor for errors

that remained in content-laden arguments; thus, it is the perspective that likely governs

the arguments from cable news that will be considered in the current study.

Studies on the content of syllogisms affecting error making began in the mid-20th

century. Researchers argued that people were adept at logical reasoning (Henle, 1962) or

at least were competent if given simple training (Morgan & Morgan, 1953), and errors

developed due to prior beliefs and attitudes (Janis & Frick, 1943). Whether it was

studying agreeable or disagreeable premises (Janis & Frick, 1943) or premises meant to

elicit personal convictions (Morgan & Morton, 1944), the fact remained that "when an

issue is injected which relates to the personal opinions, wishes, fears, or convictions of an

9

individual, the distortion shifts ... to the meaning of the terms of the syllogism" (Morgan

& Morton, 1944, p. 59). Various aspects of the same problem have appeared in many

places. Agreeable positions to arguments are sought out, while disagreeable positions are

ignored (Dawson, Gilovich, & Regan, 2002).

Additionally, syllogisms were used to study personal prejudices and attitude

effects on reasoning, wherein highly emotional content based upon existing racial and

prejudicial beliefs will elicit falsely judged arguments (Thistlethwaite, 1950). Along the

same vein, ethnocentrism is positively correlated with rigidity in beliefs and less latitude

of movement in those beliefs. This, in tum, was positively correlated with poor abstract

reasoning ability (O'Connor, 1952). It makes sense: If someone believes very strongly

about something, he or she will make every effort to judge arguments containing those

beliefs as valid. Moreover, arguments to the contrary will be met with judgments of

invalidity, even if such arguments are equally valid. An additional early case of attitude

and belief biases apparent in reasoning tasks was an experiment in which high and low

dogmatic individuals were tested to evaluate religious syllogisms (Feather, 1964). Highly

dogmatic (or those participants who were rigid and unwavering in their thinking and

attitudes) and pro-religious participants tended to judge pro-religious arguments valid and

antireligious arguments as invalid. Not surprisingly, these participants also had low

critical reasoning ability. However, one difficulty remained in interpreting the results:

because the antireligious participants held only a slightly unfavorable view of religion as

opposed to the pro-religious participants, they did not implement the same evaluation

processes for the arguments. Essentially, the pro-religious were more in favor of religion

than the anti-religious were against religion. The findings suggests the more dogmatic

10

and rigid a person is in their attitudes, the less he or she will judge an argument as valid

the further it is from his or her unalterable worldview.

In recent research, the belief bias has been refined and elaborated. It has been

shown that arguments that are both invalid and believable are more likely to be judged

valid than arguments that are both valid and unbelievable (71% to 56%; Evans et al.,

1983; Evans et al., 2001). In addition, beliefbias has been differentiated into positive and

negative belief biases (Evans et al., 2001). Positive belief bias occurs when a normally

avoided fallacious argument is enoneously judged valid; negative belief bias occurs

when the fallacies are plausible (or even ignored), thus reducing validity rates for

unbelievable arguments. Further refmement has included production tasks of valid

conclusions, whereby the task requires the participant to input a logical conclusion to a

set of given premises (rather than evaluation of included conclusions). In these tasks, the

belief bias was found in the conclusions produced by the participants, with the incidence

of bias significantly greater in produced conclusions than evaluated conclusions

(Markovits & Nantel, 1989; Oakhill & Johnson-Laird, 1985).

In addition, dual processing research has been extended to belief bias. In dual

processing theory, System 1 processing is quick and prone to errors, whereas System 2

processing is slower and more effortful. In belief bias research, studies have found that

System 1 is typically controlled by the beliefs of a person, whereas System 2 is typically

controlled by logical rules (Evans, 2003, 2008). For example, Evans and Curtis-Holmes

(2005) performed an experiment with a syllogistic reasoning task that required either a

rapid response or a normal, free-time response. They found that the rapid responses

(System 1) had more errors indicative ofbeliefbias than the untimed condition (System 2

11

could be engaged during this condition). Evidence suggests that reasoners with a high

level of skill can engage System 2 more successfully than reasoners with a low skill level,

but not more often (Evans, 2008). Furthermore, people with high working memory show

less bias overall, but can still be susceptible to the bias if under heavy cognitive load (De

Neys, 2006). This suggests an overall trend toward System 1 processing while reasoning,

even if System 2 will lead one to the correct evaluation.

For a more realistic perspective, research on the belief bias in syllogistic

argumentation has moved toward using arguments that are less abstract and more

concrete in content (Morley et al., 2004). The implications in other situations may allow

for proper education of proper argumentation, or fundamentally change the way people

argue and debate in everyday situations.

The Present Study

The purpose of this thesis is to examine whether the belief bias effect is present in

paraphrased arguments made by actual cable news commentators, such as those pundits

and anchors from MSNBC and Fox News, which may account as a driving force in HMP.

If identification of argumentation errors can be found, then an effort can be made to

address these errors. This could conceivably result in a change in cable news from

entertainment-oriented to harder, fact-driven news, which in turn could decrease

partisanship and increase civil public discourse.

In the present experiment, participants encountered arguments in syllogistic form

paraphrased from commentators and pundits (e.g., Glenn Beck, Keith Olbermann, Bill

O'Reilly, and Rachel Maddow). They evaluated if arguments' conclusions followed

necessarily and logically from the premises. Half of the conclusions were valid and the

12

other half invalid. Additionally, half were liberal arguments and the rest were

conservative arguments. Arguments were either compatible or incompatible with the

participants' own political ideologies. It is hypothesized that liberal participants will rate

liberal arguments as more valid than conservative arguments, while conservative

participants will do the opposite; that is, compatible arguments will be more likely to be

viewed as valid, indicating a stronger overall bias.

13

Method

Participants

Ninety-eight (61.2% female) undergraduate psychology students participated in

this study. Each received course credit for their participation. Ages ranged from 18 to 51

years, with a mean of20.76 years. More than half (62.2%) of the participants stated that

they had not received any formal logic training (e.g., college courses). This trend is likely

due to many of the participants being one or two semesters out of high school, where

minimal emphasis is placed on logical reasoning.

Design and Materials

Arguments. The arguments used were paraphrased from cable news sources.

These sources include Fox News Channel, CNN and MSNBC. The commentators

included Glenn Beck, Keith Olbermann, Bill O'Reilly, Sean Hannity, Roland Martin and

Rachel Maddow, among others. Sixteen arguments were used, halfliberal and the other

half conservative. The context of each argument was established by the overall political

ideology or stated affiliation ofthe pundit or commentator. Half of each set were valid

and the other invalid. The arguments varied in mood, incorporating universal affirmatives

and negatives as well as particular affirmatives and negatives. The purpose of the mixture

of syllogism type was to prevent atmosphere effects. Thus, there were four types of

argument: liberal-valid (LV), liberal-invalid (LI), conservative-valid (CV), and

conservative-invalid (CI). Appendix B contains a complete list of all arguments used in

this study.

Argument Pretest. A pretest of the believability of the conclusions of the

arguments was performed on a large social psychology class (n = 115). To achieve the

14

best selection, 24 total arguments were created, with eight intended to be discarded after

the pretest. On a scale of 0- 10 (1 0 representing maximum believability), participants

were shown only the conclusions of the arguments (given to them as "statements") and

asked to record how plausible the statement was to them and to others. The 16 best

conclusions were chosen by correlating political identity (also asked of the pretest

participants) with believability scores. The strongest correlations of each conclusion type

were chosen (four of each). A mean believability score was computed for each participant

for liberal and conservative conclusions. A two-way Analysis ofVariance (ANOVA) of

the composite scores and political identity revealed a significant interaction, F(l, 83) =

11.17, p = .00 1. Those indicating a liberal identity tended to believe liberal conclusions

more and conservative conclusions less than the participants indicating a conservative

identity. The opposite was true for conservatives and conservative vs. liberal conclusions.

Thus, the 16 chosen conclusions fit the believability manipulation and were used in the

main task.

Design. Political ideology of the participant, rated on a scale from 1 to 7, 1

representing extreme liberalism and 7 representing to extreme conservatism, was

prescreened prior to involvement in the study. To ensure maximum partisanship

evaluation on each argument, subject pool students rating themselves as moderates (based

on the sample distribution, a response of 3 ), were excluded from participation. A

response of 4 was included for comparison to the liberal and conservative poles.

Recruitment was achieved by conducting three separate sub-studies, with a specific

response requirement for each. The design of this experiment was a three-group

comparison on the four argument types; one independent variable was the political

15

identity of the participant (a pre-existing quasi-experimental variable) and the other

independent variable was whether the argument was valid or invalid. The dependent

measure was calculated as the number of conclusions accepted as valid.

All materials were presented using Media Lab 2008 on Windows PCs. Personal

data were not collected by the program.

Procedure

Two participants entered the laboratory space and were instructed to sit at a

computer station. Each participant worked independently while evaluating each

argument. All of the instructions necessary to complete the task were displayed prior to

starting the evaluation task. The instructions were as follows:

Welcome to our study. This is an experiment to test people's reasoning ability. You will be given sixteen problems in total. For each problem you will be shown two statements and you are asked if a certain conclusion (given below the statements) may be logically deduced from them. You should answer this question on the assumption that the two statements are, in fact, true. If, and only if, you judge the conclusion necessarily follows from the statements, you should select "valid" using the mouse, otherwise select "invalid." Please take your time and be sure you have the right answer before giving your response.

The arguments displayed on the computer screen with a choice of either "valid"

or "invalid." The order of arguments was randomized. On each screen, a single argument

appeared. After the participant completed all 16 arguments, the final screen asked basic

demographic information such as age and gender, including if he or she had any previous

formal logic training. A posttest question using the same political ideology scale was also

used. If asked for help, the experimenter was instructed to state they could not give any

assistance. All participants were debriefed upon completion and thanked for their

participation.

16

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Prior to testing group differences, preliminary analyses were performed. Political

identity was prescreened prior to participation in the task (the pretest score). A check of

political identity was assessed following the argument validity task. The two scores were

highly correlated (r = .70) and the means were not significantly different from each other

(t(97) = -.36,p = .72); however, some participants did change their answers between the

time of the pretest and the time of the task. An average of the scores was utilized for all

subsequent analyses. The average political identity score was then grouped into nominal

political identity, with scores of 1.00 to 2.99 classified as "liberal," scores of 3.00 to 4.49

classified as "moderate," and scores of 4.50 to 7.00 classified as "conservative." The

coding was performed this way to match the distribution of this sample and the overall

characteristics of the university.

The 16 individual arguments were quite varied in their acceptance rates. As

shown in Table 1, a larger rate for the valid arguments indicated greater accuracy,

whereas a larger rate for the invalid arguments indicated a greater tendency for error. One

argument, a conservative-invalid, had a mere 7% acceptance rate, while two liberal-valid

arguments had an 84% acceptance rate, indicating a greater accuracy overall. The

conservative-invalid argument had a moderate correlation during pretest as a conservative

conclusion (r = .24). However, the liberal-valid conclusions suffered from very weak

correlations as liberal conclusions (rs = -.09). This might explain why some arguments

were more easily accepted as valid than others. Most arguments, however, fell in the 50%

to 70% acceptance range.

17

For the main analysis, the 16 arguments were condensed into sub-scores based on

argument type. Thus, there were four sub-scores: CV, LV, CI, and LI. A participant could

have a score ranging from zero to four on each sub-score, indicating how many times a

conclusion was accepted as valid. Three indices were then calculated based on these sub­

scores (per Evans & Curtis-Holmes, 2005). The first index measured a participant's

logical response to the arguments. It combines the acceptance scores of valid conclusions

minus the acceptance scores of the invalid conclusions (CV +LV- LI- CI). The higher

this score, the more logical the reasoning attempts by the participant to accept only valid

conclusions. The second index was the belief index. Due to the overwhelming political

ideology of the sample, a liberal perspective was chosen for this index. It assumes that all

liberal arguments are believable while all conservative arguments are unbelievable (LV +

LI- CV- CI). The higher the number, the stronger the liberal bias is, and the more belief

bias might be accounting for this liberal bias (a conservative belief index was calculated

and it was the direct inverse of the liberal belief index). The third index was also

calculated from a liberal perspective, and it determines the interaction of belief and

political ideology, specifically the desire to choose invalid over valid conclusions (CV +

LI- LV- CI). The larger the number is on this index means that, from a liberal

perspective, the participant showed stronger belief bias on invalid than valid conclusions.

One-sample t-tests were conducted on each of these indices to determine if they

were significantly different from zero (a value that has no bias). Both the logic index and

the liberal belief index were significantly greater than zero (ps < .001), suggesting that

most participants were successful on the reasoning task and there was a tendency for

liberal bias, respectively. The interaction index was also significantly different from zero

18

(p = .026), suggesting participants overall tend to show belief bias on valid conclusions

more than invalid conclusions from a liberal perspective.

Separating the data by whether or not the participant had any formal logic

training, some significant differences between liberals and conservatives did arise. For

those participants stating they had never received formal logic training (n = 61),

conservatives (n = 21) tended to accept conservative-valid arguments (t(43) = -2.72,p <

.01, Cohen's d= .82), as well as conservative-invalid arguments (t(43) = -2.14,p = .04,

Cohen's d = .64), more than liberals (n = 24). Interestingly, they also tended to accept

more liberal-invalid arguments (t(43) = -2.75,p < .01, Cohen's d = .81). For those

participants stating they had some form of formal logic training (n = 37; conservative, n =

15; liberal, n = 13), each ofthese effects disappear. None of the indices showed any

significant belief bias effects.

Primary Analyses

A series of Independent-Samples t-tests were conducted to determine if liberals (n

= 37) and conservatives (n = 36) differed on the three belief bias indices. Liberals and

conservatives did not differ significantly on any of the indices (illustrated by Figure 1),

but the liberal belief index was in the predicted direction, with liberals showing a stronger

liberal bias (M= 1.78, SD = 1.67) than conservatives (M= 1.25, SD = 1.30; t(71) = 1.52,

p = .13, Cohen's d = .35). Liberals tended to have a stronger logic index score (M = 1.73,

SD = 1.37) than conservatives (M = 1.42, SD = 1.50). Liberals and conservatives showed

no overall difference on the interaction index, suggesting a lack of belief bias tendency

on invalid conclusions over valid conclusions; in fact, the opposite was trending: liberals,

M= -0.43 (SD = 1.72) vs. conservatives, M= -0.41 (SD = 1.83).

19

To determine overall group differences on the sub-scores, another set of

Independent-Samples t-tests was conducted. Liberals and conservatives did not differ

significantly on any of the sub-scores, but acceptance rates were in the predicted

directions for conservative sub-scores (CV and CI). As shown in Figure 2, conservatives

tended to have higher acceptance rates (M= 2.25, SD = .91) than liberals (M= 2.00, SD =

.91) on conservative-valid arguments (t(71) = -1.17,p = .24, Cohen's d = .27). This is

also shown on conservative-invalid arguments, which is marginally significant (t(71) =-

1.68,p = .09, Cohen's d = .39). However, these trends are not shown on the liberal sub­

scores, with liberals (M= 3.11, SD = .94) slightly higher on liberal-valid arguments than

conservatives (M= 3.08, SD = .87), while the reverse trend on liberal-invalid arguments

shows conservatives (M = 2.17, SD = .91) higher than liberals (M = 2.03, SD = .93).

Secondary Analyses

Pmiicipants identifying themselves as "moderates" behaved similarly to those

who identified themselves as "liberals", not significantly changing the comparisons of

liberals and conservatives. Additionally, post-hoc analyses were conducted to determine

differences in participant scores using demographic variables.

Dividing the data by gender revealed that liberal and conservative men and liberal

and conservative women do not differ in their reasoning abilities, nor do they differ when

all males and females are compared.

Reaction time was recorded for each argument and then averaged across all 16

arguments. Conservatives, on average, took approximately 5-6 seconds longer to make a

decision (t(71) = -2.57,p = .01, Cohen's d= .60). However, reaction time was not

correlated with overall performance on the task.

20

Discussion

In the investigation to determine if political liberals and conservatives succumb to

the belief bias effect in syllogisms containing content from cable news pundits and

commentators, the hypothesis was partially supported. More specifically, liberals and

conservatives did not differ significantly in their acceptance rates of the four. Overall,

conservatives accepted valid arguments of a conservative slant 56% of the time and

invalid arguments 44% of the time, while liberals accepted these arguments 50% and

34%, respectively. Liberal arguments seemed to affect conservative participants more

than liberals in this instance. Liberals did not perform as predicted. However, from a

liberal perspective on the index of belief bias, liberals had a stronger bias than that of

conservatives; the same can be reported for conservatives on the inverse index of belief

bias.

The effects can be attributed to those who stated they had not received any formal

logic training, or those who would be considered novice reasoners. These reasoners lack

the skills to determine accurately an argument's validity. As evidenced by the exclusion

ofthose with training, the overall marginal effects on the conservative arguments were

stronger. One interpretation for this trend is that beliefs and attitudes overwhelmingly

control evaluation of arguments for those with no training.

Although not as marked, taken together, these results suggest a trend toward the

results seen in Evans et al. (1983) and Evans and Curtis-Holmes (2005), and an important

first step in determining the overall impact of cable news in the United States and on

political partisans in particular. Liberal participants tended to accept liberal arguments as

valid while conservative participants tended to accept conservative arguments as valid.

21

With myriad arguments on cable news every hour of every day, it is difficult to pinpoint

when and how often beliefbias or persuasion is overriding the rational aspect of political

debate. Liberals and conservatives are shown to be fundamentally different (Graham et

al., 2009; Hirsh et al., 2010; McAdams et al., 2008; Napier & Jost, 2008), and it is

possible each approach argumentation differently and are more willing to accept

congruent arguments over incongruent arguments. While affect was not tested directly,

this willingness can alter perceptions of media sources and instigate HMP (Hwang et al.,

2008).

However, there were limitations to the study that had significant effects on the

data produced. Perhaps the main issue was the argument collection and production. These

materials, paraphrased and converted into syllogisms from full prose arguments,

prevented uniformity in political slant of the arguments. Some arguments were easier to

accept as valid, while others proved more difficult. Due to this high variance, the

difference between liberals and conservatives had little variance on argument subtypes.

Additionally, many of the arguments were created almost a year prior to their

presentation, so it is possible that the timeliness of the "current events" were no longer in

the minds of the participants. For example, some arguments were taken from the 2008

election campaign season, the debate over torture, and the 2009 health care debate. Many

of these issues have been discussed thoroughly or are no longer relevant. Although the

arguments passed the believability manipulation in the pretest, their overall correlations

with the intended political slant were small. This might have led to dynamic political

attitudes (of the participants) on specific worldview issues; a liberal argument might have

been plausible enough to appeal to a conservative participant and vice versa.

22

Furthermore, the sample used was from a large and diverse California university,

skewing the political identity distribution toward liberalism. The "conservatives" in the

sample, while the minority, are consistently exposed to liberal ideology, especially on

this particular college campus, known for its activism and promotion of diversity. In

addition, the participants were college students, with often changing political attitudes or

attitudes that are an eclectic mix based on individual issues, stemming from parental

attitudes or the fledgling independent attitudes of a new adult. It is not far-fetched to

suggest this sample is not as well defined as the divide in the US House of

Representatives or the US Senate. The skewness toward liberalism is strong and

particularly difficult to separate partisans on a continuum.

Future research can address both of these limitations, as well as others.

Arguments should begin as written prose taken verbatim from the cable news pundits or

commentators, substituted carefully into both valid and invalid syllogisms (or only

invalid), and then the conclusions thoroughly tested for their believability and whether

the correlations are strong enough with political ideology and the intended slant of the

argument. To achieve a more representative sample of political partisans, placing the task

in an online setting is recommended. Both greater sample size and partisanship is

intended by this method. A smaller issue to modify would be to change the question

asked of participants during the task, from "valid" and "invalid" to "yes" and "no", which

might have a subtle effect in the thought process of the participants.

Other paths of research in this topic could include associating the argument with

the speaker, or manipulating argument with speaker, to test ifbeliefbias is overridden by

the suggestions of the pundit and the associations that might be linked. For example,

23

Glenn Beck could be associated with unearthing left-wing propaganda arguments, which

could either strengthen or weaken the belief bias effect. Moreover, an effort to verify

political identity on the traditional 7 -point scale could be instituted with a collection of

scales on political ideology (e.g., Weber & Federico, 2007).

This investigation has shown the belief bias effect on reasoning from arguments

presented on cable news channels, particularly with individuals without logic training. If

politically active Americans continue to watch and place importance on these networks

and those who speak on the networks, it stands to reason that a political partisan must be

cognizant of the powerful forces of belief congruency and persuasion on these channels.

Blindly accepting an argument as valid due to its believability adds to fallacious

argumentation, perpetuating HMP and increasing the political divide. While it appears

the majority of audience members might not have the aptitude to reason through each

argument heard or read, seeking incongruent arguments or counterexamples has its

benefits.

24

References

Babad, E. (2005). The psychological price of media bias. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Applied, 11(4), 245-255.

Begg, I. & Denny, J.P. (1969). Empirical reconciliation of atmosphere and conversion

interpretations of syllogistic reasoning errors. Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 81(2), 351-354.

Braine, M.D. S. (1978). On the relation between the natural logic of reasoning and

standard logic. Psychological Review, 85(1 ), 1-21.

Cacioppo, J. T. & Petty, R. E. (1979). Effects of message repetition and position on

cognitive response, recall, and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 37(1), 97-109.

Calvert, R. L. (1985). The value of biased information: A rational choice model of

political advice. The Journal of Politics, 47(2), 530-555.

Chaiken, S. & Eagly, A. H. (1976). Communication modality as a determinant of

message persuasiveness and message comprehensibility. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 34(4), 605-614.

Chaiken, S. & Eagly, A. H. (1983). Communication modality as a determinant of

persuasion: The role of communicator salience. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 45(2), 241-256.

Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J.P. (1959). Atmosphere effect re-examined. Journal of

Experimental Psychology, 58(3), 220-226.

25

Coe, K., Tewksbury, D., Bond, B. J., Drogos, K. L., Porter, R. W., Yahn, A., & Zhang,

Y. (2008). Hostile news: Partisan use and perceptions of cable news

programming. Journal ofCommunication, 58, 201-219.

Dawson, E., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (2002). Motivated reasoning and performance

on the Wason selection task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(10),

1379-1387.

De Neys, W. (2006). Dual processing in reasoning: Two systems but one reasoner.

Psychological Science, 17(5), 428-433.

Dickstein, L. S. (1975). Effects of instructions and premise order on errors in syllogistic

reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory,

104(4), 376-384.

Evans, J. St. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts ofreasoning. TRENDS in

Cognitives Sciences, 7(1 0), 454-459.

Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning judgment and social

cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255-278.

Evans, J. St. B. T., Barston, J. L., & Pollard, P. (1983). On the conflict between logic and

belief in syllogistic reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 11 (3), 295-306.

Evans, J. St. B. T. & Curtis-Holmes, J. (2005). Rapid responding increases belief bias:

Evidence for dual-process theory of reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 11(4),

382-389.

Evans, J. St. B. T., Handley, S. J., & Harper, C. N.J. (2001). Necessity, possibility and

belief: A study of syllogistic reasoning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 54A(3), 935-958.

26

Evans, J. St. B. T., Newstead, S. E., & Byrne, R. M. J. (1993). Human reasoning:

Psychology of deduction. East Sussex, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Ltd.

Eveland, W. P., Jr., & Shah, D. V. (2003). The impact of individual and interpersonal

factors on perceived news media bias. Political Psychology, 24(1), 101-117.

Feather, N. T. (1964). Acceptance and rejection of arguments in relation to attitude

strength, critical ability, and intolerance of inconsistency. Journal of Abnormal

and Social Psychology, 69(2), 127-136.

Gaiotti, K. M., Baron, J., & Sabini, J.P. (1986). Individual differences in syllogistic

reasoning: Deduction rules or mental models? Journal of Experimental

Psychology: General, 115(1), 16-25.

Goel, V. (2003). Evidence for dual neural pathways for syllogistic reasoning.

Psychologia, 32, 301-309.

Goel, V. (2007). Anatomy of deductive reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11 (1 0),

435-441.

Goel, V. & Dolan, R. (2004). Differential involvement of left prefrontal cortex in

inductive and deductive reasoning. Cognition, 93, B109-B121.

Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different

sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5),

1029-1046.

Gunther, A. C. (1992). Biased press or biased public? Attitudes toward media coverage

of social groups. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56, 14 7-167.

Haidt, J. & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral

intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1 ), 98-116.

27

Haidt, J. & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate

culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133(4), 55-66.

Heesacker, M., Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1983). Field dependence and attitude

change: Source credibility can alter persuasion by affecting message-relevant

thinking. Journal of Personality, 51(4), 653-666.

Henle, M. (1962). On the relation between logic and thinking. Psychological Review,

69(4), 366-378.

Hernnstein, R. J. (1990). Rational choice theory: Necessary but not sufficient. American

Psychologist, 45(3), 356-367.

Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., Xu, X., & Peterson, J. B. (2010). Compassionate liberals

and polite conservatives: Associations of agreeableness with political ideology

and moral values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(5), 655-664.

Hovland, C. I., Harvey, 0. J., & Sherif, M. (1957). Assimilation and contrast effects in

reactions to communication and attitude change. Journal of Abnormal and Social

Psychology, 55(2), 244-252.

Hurley, P. J. (2004). A concise introduction to logic: Selected chapters. Belmont, CA:

Wadsworth Thomson Learning.

Hwang, H., Pan, Z., & Sun, Y. (2008). Influence of hostile media perception in

willingness to engage in discursive activities: An examination of mediating role

of media indignation. Media Psychology, 11, 76-97.

Janis, I. R. & Frick, F. (1943). The relationship between attitudes toward conclusions and

errors in judging logical validity of syllogisms. Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 33(1), 73-77.

28

Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language,

inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Byrne, R. M. J. (1991). Deduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates, Ltd.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Steedman, M. (1978). The psychology of syllogisms. Cognitive

Psychology, 10, 64-99.

Kim, K. S. & Pasadeos, Y. (2007). Study of partisan news readers reveals hostile media

perceptions of balanced stories. Newspaper Research Journal, 28(2), 99-106.

Lovett, F. (2006). Rational choice theory and explanation. Rationality and Society, 18(2),

237-272.

Markovits, H. & Nantel, G. (1989). The belief-bias effect in the production and

evaluation of logical conclusions. Memory & Cognition, 17(1 ), 11-17.

McAdams, D.P., Albaugh, M., Farber, E., Daniels, J., Logan, R. L., & Olson, B. (2008).

Family metaphors and moral intuitions: How conservatives and liberals narrate

their lives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95( 4), 978-990.

Morely, N.J., Evans, J. St. B. T., & Handley, S. J. (2004). Belief bias and figural bias in

syllogistic reasoning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57 A( 4),

666-692.

Morgan, J. J. B. & Morton, J. T. (1944). The distortion of syllogistic reasoning produced

by personal convictions. The Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 39-59.

Morgan, W. J. & Morgan, A. B. (1953). Logical reasoning: With and without training.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 37(5), 399-401.

29

Mutz, D. C. & Martin, P. S. (2001). Facilitating communication across lines of political

difference: The role of mass media. American Political Science Review, 95(1), 97-

114.

Napier, J. L. & Jost, J. T. (2008). Why are conservatives happier than liberals?

Psychological Science, 19(6), 565-572.

Newstead, S. E., Pollard, P., Evans, J. St. B. T., & Allen, J. L. (1992). The source of

belief bias effects in syllogistic reasoning. Cognition, 45, 257-284.

O'Connor, P. (1952). Ethnocentrism, "intolerance of ambiguity", and abstract reasoning

ability. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47(2 Suppl.), 526-530.

Oakhill, J. V. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1985). The effects of belief on the spontaneous

production of syllogistic conclusions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental

Psychology, 37A, 553-569.

Petty, R. E. & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The Elaboration Likelihood Model of persuasion.

In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp.

123-205). New York: Academic Press.

Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal involvement as

determinant of argument-based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 41(5), 847-855.

Revlin, R. & Leirer, V. (1978). The effects of personal biases on syllogistic reasoning:

Rational decisions from personalized representations. In R. Revlin & R. E. Mayer

(Eds.), Human reasoning. Washington, D.C: Winston-Wiley.

Revlin, R., Ammerman, K., Petersen, K., & Leirer, V. (1978). Category relations and

syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(4), 613-625.

30

Revlis, R. (1975a). Syllogistic reasoning: Logical decisions from a complex data base. In

R. Falmagne (Ed.), Reasoning: Representation and process. Hillsdale, N.J.:

Erlbaum.

Revlis, R. (1975b). Two models of syllogistic reasoning: Feature selection and

conversion. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 180-195.

Scmitt, K. M., Gunther, A. C., & Liebhart, J. L. (2004). Why partisans see media as

biased. Communication Research, 31,623-641.

Scott, J. (2000). Rational choice theory. In G. Browning, A. Halcli, & F. Webster (Eds.),

Understanding contemporary society: Theories of the present (pp.126-138).

London: Sage Publications, Ltd.

Sells, S. B. (1936). The atmosphere effect: An experimental study of reasoning. Archives

of Psychology, 29, 3-72.

Sells, S. B. & Koob, H. F. (1937). A classroom demonstration of"atmosphere effect" in

reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 28(7), 514-518.

Simpson, M. E. & Johnson, D. M. (1966). Atmosphere and conversion errors in

syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 197-200.

Thistlethwaite, D. (1950). Attitude and structure as factors in the distortion of reasoning.

Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 45(3), 442-458.

Tormala, Z. L. & Petty, R. E. (2002). What doesn't kill me makes me stronger: The

effects of resisting persuasion on attitude certainty. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 83(6), 1298-1313.

31

Vallone, R. P., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1985). The hostile media phenomenon: Biased

perception and perceptions of media bias in coverage of the Beirut Massacre.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(3), 577-585.

Vartanian, 0., Goel, V., Tierney, M., Huey, E. D., Grafman, J. (2009). Frontotemporal

dementia selectively impairs transitive reasoning about familiar spatial

environments. Neuropsychology, 23(5), 619-626.

Weber, C. & Federico, C. M. (2007). Interpersonal attachment and patterns of ideological

belief. Political Psychology, 28(4), 389-416.

Westen, D., Blagov, P. S., Harenski, K., Kilts, C., & Hamann, S. (2006). Neural bases of

motivated reasoning: An fMRI study of emotional constraints on partisan political

judgment in the 2004 U.S. presidential election. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, 18(11), 1947-1958.

Woodworth, R. S. & Sells, S. B. (1935). An atmosphere effect in formal syllogistic

reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(4), 451-460.

Xiang, Y. & Sarvary, M. (2007). News consumption and media bias. Marketing Science,

26(5), 611-628.

32

Appendix A: Tables and Figures

Table 1

Acceptance Rates of Conclusions with Intended Political Slant and Validity

Arguments Acceptance as

Valid Conclusion

Argument Conclusion Political Slant Validity Rate SD

Therefore, some torture is vital Conservative Valid 0.6 0.49

for America's safety.

Therefore, no TV reporting is on Conservative Valid 0.53 0.5

expansion of government.

Therefore, Barack Obama is anti-Conservative Valid 0.13 0.34

American. Therefore, some Obama followers do not understand Obama' s Conservative Valid 0.82 0.39 thought process. Therefore, some Congresspeople

Liberal Valid 0.84 0.37 are not for Obama's policies.

Therefore, some endorsements of protests against the government Liberal Valid 0.69 0.46 are by Fox News hosts.

Therefore, all guns are harmful. Liberal Valid 0.73 0.44

Therefore, some things that are beneficial for corporations would

Liberal Valid 0.84 0.37 not be supported by liberal politicians.

Therefore, all illegal immigrants Conservative Invalid 0.56 0.5

should be deported.

Therefore, global warming has Conservative Invalid 0.27 0.44 decreased.

Therefore, Barack Obama is not Conservative Invalid 0.07 0.26

for free speech. Therefore, all abortion is killing

Conservative Invalid 0.62 0.49 life. Therefore, JP Morgan Chase is not readily disclosing details Liberal Invalid 0.24 0.43 about bailouts. Therefore, health care reform is

Liberal Invalid 0.59 0.49 protecting the people. Therefore, some Obama presidency deniers are not

Liberal Invalid 0.65 0.48 demanding Obama' s birth certificate.

33

Therefore, some bankers were damaged by corporate greed.

Liberal Invalid 0.67 0.47

Note. Rates represent the average rate participants accepted each conclusion as valid.

Rates either show a trend of correct or incorrect responses by using the Validity column

and comparing it to the rate.

34

• Liberal ~ Conservative

2.5

2

1.5

1

0.5

0 Liberal Belief Index Logic Index

-0.5

-1

Figure 1. Mean differences between liberals and conservatives on the three belief bias

indices. The belief index is taken from a liberal perspective. No significant differences

were found between liberals and conservatives on the indices. Standard errors are

represented in the figure by the error bars attached to each column.

35

• Liberal 11 Conservative

3.5

3

0.5

0 Conservative-Valid Liberal-Valid Conservative-Invalid Liberal-Invalid

Argument Type

Figure 2. Mean acceptance scores (out of a possible score of 4) for each argument type.

Scores were in the predicted direction for the conservative arguments, but the liberal

arguments were unclear in trending scores. Standard errors are represented in the figure

by the error bars attached to each column.

36

Appendix B: Experimental Arguments

Legend: C = Conservative argument, L = Liberal argument; V = Valid, I = Invalid; (Mood)

All coerced interrogation techniques are vital for America's safety. Some coerced interrogation techniques are torture. Therefore, some torture is vital for America's safety. CVl (All)

All expansion of government is a left-wing conspiracy. No TV reporting is on left-wing conspiracies. Therefore, no TV reporting is on expansion of government. CV2 (AEE)

Barack Obama attended Jeremiah Wright's church for 20 years. Jeremiah Wright is anti-American. Therefore, Barack Obama is anti-American. CV3 (AAA)

All illegal immigrants are breaking the law. Deportation is the only punishment for breaking this law. Therefore, all illegal immigrants should be deported. en (AAA)

Global warming causes extreme heat deaths. Deaths from extreme heat have decreased in recent years. Therefore, global warming has decreased. CI2 (AAA)

Some Democrats are not for free speech. Barack Obama is a Democrat. Therefore, Barack Obama is not for free speech. CI3 (IAA)

All abortion is murder. Killing life is murder. Therefore, all abortion is killing life. CI4 (AAA)

Some banks are not readily disclosing details about bailouts. JPMorgan Chase is a bank. Therefore, JP Morgan Chase is not readily disclosing details about bailouts. Lll (IAA)

37

Health care reform is good for this country. Protecting the people is good for this country. Therefore, health care reform is protecting the people. LI2 (AAA)

Some Tea Party members are not demanding Obama's birth certificate. All Obama presidency deniers are Tea Party members. Therefore, some Obama presidency deniers are not demanding Obama's birth certificate. LI3 (OAO)

No Republicans are for Obama's policies. Some Congress people are Republicans. Therefore, some Congresspeople are not for Obama's policies. LVl (EIO)

Some Fox News hosts are promoting the Tea Party movement. All promotion of the Tea Party movement is an endorsement of protests against the government. Therefore, some endorsements of protests against the government are by Fox News hosts. LV2 (IAI)

All members of the middle class were damaged by corporate greed. Some bankers are not members of the middle class. Therefore, some bankers were damaged by corporate greed. LI4 (AOI)

Some working Americans do not understand Obama's thought process. Some Obama followers are working Americans. Therefore, some Obama followers do not understand Obama's thought process. CV4 (010)

All guns are associated with violence and force. All violence and force is harmful. Therefore, all guns are harmful. LV3 (AAA)

No liberal politician supports exporting jobs. Exporting some jobs is beneficial for corporations. Therefore, some things that are beneficial for corporations would not be supported by liberal politicians. LV4 (EIO)

38