when cold becomes hot

1
J Physiol 587.23 (2009) p 5511 5511 PERSPECTIVES When cold becomes hot Penelope A. McNulty 1 and David Burke 2 1 Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute and University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia 2 Sydney Medical School, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia Email: [email protected] Microneurography was originally deve- loped by Vallbo and Hagbarth in the 1960s to bridge the gap between human psychophysical studies and neuro- physiological investigations in animal preparations. Soon after the first recordings of single nerve fibre potentials were made in large-diameter myelinated cutaneous and muscle afferents, microneurography was used to examine human C-fibres (e.g. Torebj¨ ork, 1974). Despite its technical difficulties and limited yield, micro- neurography enables an extraordinarily detailed examination of the properties of single fibres in human peripheral nerves in vivo. This intact, physiological protocol can also be used to explore sub- jective sensations arising from intraneural stimulation of individual nerve fibres, an application ideally suited to the study of pain. These and like studies have helped cement the view that many features of a percept are directly related to the afferent species and its discharge, as much as, if not more than, central mechanisms. The resurgent field of pain research has been constrained by the difficulty of defining the neural origins of pain to quantify what is ultimately a subjective experience. Functional imaging techniques have provided an objective means to quantify some pain responses and newer techniques have discriminated patterns of responses in the brain arising from selective activation of cutaneous Aδ and C-fibres (Weiss et al. 2008). There are limitations to such techniques: they cannot determine which class or subclass of C-fibre mediates specific pain sensations, such as those evoked by nociceptive thermal stimuli. Most of our present physiological concepts about sensory transduction have been derived from studies in rats, cats and monkeys. However we know there are fundamental differences between animal and human skin. These include the presence of Ruffini endings or slowly adapting type II afferents (SA II) in human glabrous skin, found in neither cat nor monkey; the termination of Aδ fibres in unmyelinated C-fibre type endings; and the strong synaptic coupling between cutaneous afferents, particularly SA IIs, and the motoneurone pool (McNulty & Macefield, 2001). Behavioural, histochemical and molecular studies in animal models provide a framework on which to explore the structural differences and paradoxical sensations of human skin. In this issue of The Journal of Physiology , Campero et al . (2009) explore one such paradox, that of an unpleasant burning sensation in response to innocuous cooling stimuli. Using microneurography they provide compelling evidence for a specific sub- class of C-fibre afferent that responds to both warming and cooling, a class of afferents that they have termed type 2 afferent C fibres (‘C2’). Their intriguing hypothesis is that these fibres do not trans- duce thermal sensation per se but mediate an unconscious modulation of thermo- regulation to preserve homeostatic stability. It implies a trade-off between the metabolic costs of sustaining this sensory channel and the greater efficiency of long-term maintenance within a physiologically safe thermal range. Their findings specifically differentiate between the noxious sensations mediated by type 1 C-fibres and the unpleasant sensations that can sometimes be evoked by apparently innocuous cooling. The results from this study suggest a simple mechanism to test for interrupted biological functioning of Aδ fibres clinically. The pre- sence of a paradoxical burning sensation in response to a cold stimulus, in the absence of ischaemia, implies the dysfunction of A-fibre signalling (Fruhstorfer, 1984). It is conceivable that thermal dysregulation may be associated with a specific C2 neuro- pathy. The potential for pharmacological targeting of C2 fibres is raised by the differences in membrane properties of these afferents in comparison to the two classes of type 1 C-fibres (Bostock et al. 2003). This differentiation can only be made using microneurography. This paper is one of a remarkable series of experiments led by Ochoa and Bostock. The difficulty of recording from single nerve fibres in human peripheral nerves is compounded when specifically targeting type-identified C-fibres, and cannot be overstated. To distinguish the activity of single C-fibres is technically quite challenging using microneurography. The diameter of C-fibres is at least one order of magnitude smaller than Aα and Aβ fibres and their aggregation into Remak bundles further lessens the likelihood of selective recordings. In practice this reduces the ability to distinguish between individual fibres using spike morphology and amplitude, and Campero et al . (2009) have developed sophisticated procedures to assist in this. When this is combined with firing rates <5 Hz and sluggish response times to external thermal and mechanical stimulation, only limited identification is possible based on firing patterns, as is used in micro- neurographic studies of large-diameter myelinated afferents. Selective recordings of C-fibre potentials therefore rely on the cardinal identifiers of latency and the pattern of activity-dependent slowing in axonal conduction velocity. To distinguish sub-populations of C-fibres unambiguously, an extended sequence of electrical stimulation and baseline recordings is required before the receptive fields can be mapped in response to natural stimuli. Only then can experimental interventions be implemented. The work of Campero and colleagues is the latest in a series of papers that together elucidate the physiological properties of C-fibre afferents in detail similar to that well established for larger diameter afferents. References Bostock H, Campero M, Serra J & Ochoa J (2003). J Physiol 553, 649–663. Campero M, Baumann TK, Bostock H & Ochoa JL (2009). J Physiol 587, 5633–5652. Fruhstorfer H (1984). Pain 20, 355–361. McNulty PA & Macefield VG (2001). J Physiol 537, 1021–1032. Torebj¨ ork HE (1974). Acta Physiol Scand 92, 374–390. Weiss T, Straube T, Boettcher J, Hecht H, Spohn D & Miltner WHR (2008). Neuroimage 41, 1372–1381. C 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation C 2009 The Physiological Society DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.183483

Upload: penelope-a-mcnulty

Post on 25-Jul-2016

216 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: When cold becomes hot

J Physiol 587.23 (2009) p 5511 5511

PERSPECT IVES

When cold becomes hot

Penelope A. McNulty1 and David Burke2

1Prince of Wales Medical Research Instituteand University of New South Wales, Sydney,Australia2Sydney Medical School, Universityof Sydney, Sydney, Australia

Email: [email protected]

Microneurography was originally deve-loped by Vallbo and Hagbarth inthe 1960s to bridge the gap betweenhuman psychophysical studies and neuro-physiological investigations in animalpreparations. Soon after the first recordingsof single nerve fibre potentials were madein large-diameter myelinated cutaneousand muscle afferents, microneurographywas used to examine human C-fibres(e.g. Torebjork, 1974). Despite its technicaldifficulties and limited yield, micro-neurography enables an extraordinarilydetailed examination of the propertiesof single fibres in human peripheralnerves in vivo. This intact, physiologicalprotocol can also be used to explore sub-jective sensations arising from intraneuralstimulation of individual nerve fibres, anapplication ideally suited to the study ofpain. These and like studies have helpedcement the view that many features of apercept are directly related to the afferentspecies and its discharge, as much as, if notmore than, central mechanisms.

The resurgent field of pain researchhas been constrained by the difficulty ofdefining the neural origins of pain toquantify what is ultimately a subjectiveexperience. Functional imaging techniqueshave provided an objective means toquantify some pain responses and newertechniques have discriminated patterns ofresponses in the brain arising from selectiveactivation of cutaneous Aδ and C-fibres(Weiss et al. 2008). There are limitationsto such techniques: they cannot determinewhich class or subclass of C-fibre mediatesspecific pain sensations, such as thoseevoked by nociceptive thermal stimuli. Mostof our present physiological concepts aboutsensory transduction have been derivedfrom studies in rats, cats and monkeys.However we know there are fundamental

differences between animal and humanskin. These include the presence of Ruffiniendings or slowly adapting type II afferents(SA II) in human glabrous skin, found inneither cat nor monkey; the terminationof Aδ fibres in unmyelinated C-fibre typeendings; and the strong synaptic couplingbetween cutaneous afferents, particularlySA IIs, and the motoneurone pool (McNulty& Macefield, 2001).

Behavioural, histochemical and molecularstudies in animal models provide aframework on which to explore thestructural differences and paradoxicalsensations of human skin. In this issueof The Journal of Physiology, Camperoet al. (2009) explore one such paradox,that of an unpleasant burning sensationin response to innocuous cooling stimuli.Using microneurography they providecompelling evidence for a specific sub-class of C-fibre afferent that responds toboth warming and cooling, a class ofafferents that they have termed type 2afferent C fibres (‘C2’). Their intriguinghypothesis is that these fibres do not trans-duce thermal sensation per se but mediatean unconscious modulation of thermo-regulation to preserve homeostatic stability.It implies a trade-off between the metaboliccosts of sustaining this sensory channeland the greater efficiency of long-termmaintenance within a physiologically safethermal range. Their findings specificallydifferentiate between the noxious sensationsmediated by type 1 C-fibres and theunpleasant sensations that can sometimesbe evoked by apparently innocuous cooling.

The results from this study suggest a simplemechanism to test for interrupted biologicalfunctioning of Aδ fibres clinically. The pre-sence of a paradoxical burning sensation inresponse to a cold stimulus, in the absenceof ischaemia, implies the dysfunction ofA-fibre signalling (Fruhstorfer, 1984). It isconceivable that thermal dysregulation maybe associated with a specific C2 neuro-pathy. The potential for pharmacologicaltargeting of C2 fibres is raised by thedifferences in membrane properties of theseafferents in comparison to the two classesof type 1 C-fibres (Bostock et al. 2003).This differentiation can only be made usingmicroneurography.

This paper is one of a remarkable seriesof experiments led by Ochoa and Bostock.

The difficulty of recording from singlenerve fibres in human peripheral nerves iscompounded when specifically targetingtype-identified C-fibres, and cannot beoverstated. To distinguish the activityof single C-fibres is technically quitechallenging using microneurography. Thediameter of C-fibres is at least one orderof magnitude smaller than Aα and Aβ

fibres and their aggregation into Remakbundles further lessens the likelihoodof selective recordings. In practice thisreduces the ability to distinguish betweenindividual fibres using spike morphologyand amplitude, and Campero et al.(2009) have developed sophisticatedprocedures to assist in this. When thisis combined with firing rates <5 Hzand sluggish response times to externalthermal and mechanical stimulation, onlylimited identification is possible basedon firing patterns, as is used in micro-neurographic studies of large-diametermyelinated afferents. Selective recordingsof C-fibre potentials therefore relyon the cardinal identifiers of latencyand the pattern of activity-dependentslowing in axonal conduction velocity. Todistinguish sub-populations of C-fibresunambiguously, an extended sequenceof electrical stimulation and baselinerecordings is required before the receptivefields can be mapped in response to naturalstimuli. Only then can experimentalinterventions be implemented. The workof Campero and colleagues is the latest in aseries of papers that together elucidate thephysiological properties of C-fibre afferentsin detail similar to that well established forlarger diameter afferents.

References

Bostock H, Campero M, Serra J & Ochoa J(2003). J Physiol 553, 649–663.

Campero M, Baumann TK, Bostock H & OchoaJL (2009). J Physiol 587, 5633–5652.

Fruhstorfer H (1984). Pain 20, 355–361.McNulty PA & Macefield VG (2001). J Physiol

537, 1021–1032.Torebjork HE (1974). Acta Physiol Scand 92,

374–390.Weiss T, Straube T, Boettcher J, Hecht H, Spohn

D & Miltner WHR (2008). Neuroimage 41,1372–1381.

C© 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation C© 2009 The Physiological Society DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.183483