the syntax of the reiterated numeral construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/rnc-e…  ·...

32
The Reiterated Numeral Construction * Jason Kandybowicz Rutgers University 1.0 Introduction Numerals in Nupe 1 are able to function as two kinds of modifiers. While in certain syntactic environments they can modify nominal phrases, as in English, in others they serve as adverbial modifiers. Consider the following sentences. (1) a. Bàági gútá ba nakàn. men three cut meat ‘Three men cut the meat.’ b. Musa ba nakàn gútá. Musa cut meat three. ‘Musa cut three pieces of meat.’ c. Musa ba nakàn gútá-gútá. Musa cut meat three-three ‘Musa cut the meat in threes.’ (e.g. ‘Musa cut the (plurality of) meat in batches of three.’) This paper is concerned primarily with the syntactic issues raised by numeral constructions such as those in (1c.) above, an under-researched phenomenon virtually undiscussed in the literature 2 . It will be argued that in this construction the numeral and its copy form a constituent which is right- adjoined to the VP. I will refer to this numeral complex as the Reiterated Numeral Construction-RNC hereafter. After sufficient discussion of Nupe’s inventory of adverbs and adverbials, the claim that RNCs admit of the category 1

Upload: others

Post on 07-Feb-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

The Reiterated Numeral Construction*

Jason KandybowiczRutgers University

1.0 Introduction

Numerals in Nupe1 are able to function as two kinds of modifiers. While in certain syntactic environments they

can modify nominal phrases, as in English, in others they serve as adverbial modifiers. Consider the following

sentences.

(1) a. Bàági gútá ba nakàn.

men three cut meat

‘Three men cut the meat.’

b. Musa ba nakàn gútá.

Musa cut meat three.

‘Musa cut three pieces of meat.’

c. Musa ba nakàn gútá-gútá.

Musa cut meat three-three

‘Musa cut the meat in threes.’

(e.g. ‘Musa cut the (plurality of) meat in batches of three.’)

This paper is concerned primarily with the syntactic issues raised by numeral constructions such as those in (1c.)

above, an under-researched phenomenon virtually undiscussed in the literature2. It will be argued that in this

construction the numeral and its copy form a constituent which is right-adjoined to the VP. I will refer to this

numeral complex as the Reiterated Numeral Construction-RNC hereafter. After sufficient discussion of Nupe’s

* This work represents a section of my ongoing B.A. honors thesis devoted to the syntax and semantics of numeral constructions in Nupe. I would like to extend my utmost appreciation and gratitude to my warm and patient fieldwork consultant Ahmadu Ndanusa Kawu. While the judgments expressed in this work are his, I do not hold him responsible for any errors or inconsistencies that may appear in this paper. For opening my eyes to relevant data in Malagasy and introducing me to the work of David Gill, I gratefully thank Ed Keenan. Special thanks to D. Lightswitche for valuable comments and insightful remarks offered along the way. I reserve my deepest thanks for my thesis committee, Maria Bittner and Mark Baker. I am honored to have worked under their watchful scrutinizing eyes and feel that I am a better theoretician for it. The fieldwork for this project was supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation (NSF grant BCS-9905600) for which I gratefully acknowledge.

1 Nupe is a Nupoid language of the Benue-Congo sub-family of the Niger-Congo language family of Africa. It is spoken by approximately one million people in the Middle Belt region of Nigeria on both sides of the River Niger. Other Nupoid languages include Ebira, Gade, and Gwari.

1

Page 2: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

inventory of adverbs and adverbials, the claim that RNCs admit of the category label adverb will be justified and

defended. The paper concludes with an investigation into the licensing constraints governing RNCs.

2.0 RNC Constituency

In light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to a difference in meaning between (1b.) and

(1c.), inquiry into the distinction between such numeral constructions as /gútá/ and /gútá-gútá/ seems well

warranted. Given the adjectival interpretation of the numeral in (1b.) and the seemingly adverbial counterpart

interpretation of the numerals in (1c.), two possibilities regarding the nature of these numeral arrays become evident.

The first possibility is that gútá1 and gútá2 are morphologically distinct. As such, the proposal here is that the first

occurrence of gútá right adjoins to the NP housing nakàn as an adjectival adjunct in both (1b.) and (1c.), while the

copied gútá adjoins as an adjunct to the VP comprising [ba nakàn] in (1c.). In this way, the adjectival reading of

(1b.) is preserved and the adverbial interpretation of (1c.) is accounted for. Schematically, this strategy for

representing the occurrence of the twin numerals in (1c.) as distinct from the occurrence of the solitary numeral in

(1b.) is given below in (2).

(2) VP

VP XP

NP V´ gútá2

Musa V NP

ba NP XP

nakàn gútá1

The remaining possibility, however, offers a less striking account of this distribution. On this version, the treatment

of the numeral in (1b.) retains the status proposed in possibility one. The handling of the numeral reduplicant in

(1c.) however, differs saliently on this account. Possibility two suggests that both lexical items (the numeral and its

copy) are base-generated as VP adjuncts. Morphologically, this amounts to the claim that RNCs are built up from a

process of reduplication, the target of which is the numeral stem3. This possibility is represented below in (3).

(3) VP

VP XP

Musa ba nakàn gútá1-gútá2

Stated as such, possibility one claims that despite being lexically identical and synonymous, gútá1 and gútá2 are

morphologically distinct and admit of separate identities under NP and VP. Possibility two is less adventurous

2

Page 3: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

because it proposes that neither gutá1 nor gutá2 has an identity without the other, that is, both numerals are bound to

each other as a morphosyntactic unit. As it turns out, the later proposal is to be favored. To convince ourselves of

this, we turn now to a series of constituency tests involving intervention and dislocation in Nupe.

2.1 Intervention as Test for Constituency

As a preliminary measure and segue into our discussion on intervention, I would like to justify the use of the term

Reiterated Numeral Construction. As we will see, this term is an accurate description of the phenomena under

scrutiny. Note that the following data confirm the claim that the construction must be built up out of a) the numeral

root and b) a single copy of that root.

(4) a. *Musa ba nakàn gúbà-gútá.

Musa cut meat two-three

b. *Musa ba nakàn gútá-gútá-gútá.

Musa cut meat three-three-three

Thus, the morphological doubling operation involved in the derivation of RNCs admits of a single application of

reiteration and permits no more. Interestingly enough however, while strings of exactly three identical numerals

occurring in a given sentence result in ungrammaticality, the insertion of a pause between the first occurrence of the

numeral and the remaining two occurrences can save the grammaticality of the sentence. As we will see, this fact

becomes important in establishing the constituency of the RNC. Moreover, in any given sentence, all strings of

iterated numerals greater than three in length give rise to judgments of ungrammaticality by native speakers. Thus,

given the proper morphosyntactic conditions, a numeral can co-occur with a copy of itself. Additional numerals,

however, be they identical or not are prohibited from intervening in the resulting complex housing the original

numeral and its copy, unless suprasegmental perceptual cues such as pausing are imposed. That is, once an RNC

has been established the intervention of subsequent numerals either internal to or external to the complex is

disallowed. The question that now remains is whether any lexical item besides the numeral can intervene to break

apart the Reiterated Numeral Construction. If this question can be answered in the negative, then the beginnings of

a basis for claiming that RNCs are constituents and morphological wholes (along the lines of possibility two) will

start to take shape. Toward the goal of answering this question, consider the following:

(5) a. * Musa ba nakàn gútá tó gútá.

Musa cut meat three and three

b. * Musa ba nakàn gútá tsúwó gútá.

Musa cut meat three yesterday three

3

Page 4: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

c. * Musa ba nakàn gútá sanyin gútá.

Musa cut meat three quietly three

d. * Musa ba nakàn gútá dan efo cigban o gútá

Musa cut meat three in hole tree LOC three

These four sentences represent plausible candidates for intervention, one might think. Indeed, the insertion of

adverbs and prepositional phrases similar to those italicized above might be so placed by a non-native speaker

attempting to approximate the following glosses for (5b.)-(5d.) respectively:

(5b') ‘Musa cut the meat in threes yesterday.’ or ‘Musa cut the meat yesterday in threes’.

(5c.') ‘Musa cut the meat in threes quietly.’ or ‘Musa cut the meat quietly in threes.’

(5d.') ‘Musa cut the meat in threes under the tree.’ or ‘Musa cut the meat under the tree in threes.’

All other attempts to split the original numeral from its copy cause the resulting sentence to crash. This finding

falsifies the prediction that possibility one gives rise to; namely, that the loose bond holding gútá1 and gútá2 together

will allow for intervening structure between the two items. Hence, the fact that there is no lexical or phrasal

intervention of any sort within the Reiterated Numeral Construction is the first damaging blow to the possibility that

both numerals are morphosyntactically independent.

2.2 Dislocation as Test for Constituency

In this section, convincing evidence that RNCs are constituents will be presented by means of three phenomena:

i. RNCs can be clefted

ii. RNCs can be stranded

iii. RNCs are conjoinable and adhere to the Coordinate Structure Constraint

2.2.1 RNC Clefts

Compelling evidence that arrays such as /gútá-gútá/ form a constituent comes from their ability to move as a

unit. In what follows, observe that whereas /gútá-gútá/ as a whole can be fronted, gútá1 independently of gútá2

cannot. This is not predicted to occur under possibility one.

(6) a. Musa ba nakàn gútá-gútá.

Musa cut meat three-three

‘Musa cut the meat in threes.’

4

Page 5: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

b. Gútá-gútá Musa ba nakàn o.

three-three Musa cut meat FOC

‘It was in threes that Musa cut the meat.’

c. *Gútá Musa ba nakàn gútá o.

three Musa cut meat three FOC

Perhaps more striking is the example alluded to in section 2.1.

(7) Musa ba nakàn gúbà (…) gútá-gútá.

Musa cut meat two (pause) three-three

‘Musa cut two pieces of meat in threes.’

Again, in production and perception among native speakers, a slight pause exists between /gúbà/ and /gútá-gútá/,

serving to demarcate the boundary between the adjectival /gúbà/ and the RNC. While this certainly adds to the

claim that RNCs are produced and perceived as units, the pattern of movement resulting from the transformation of

(7) (enumerated below in (8)) reaffirms the hypothesis that RNCs move as a complex. Consider the following:

(8) a. Gútá-gútá Musa ba nakàn gúbà o.

three-three Musa cut meat two FOC

‘It was in threes that Musa cut two pieces of meat.’

b. * Gúbà-gútá-gútá Musa ba nakàn o.

two-three-three Musa cut meat FOC

Under the assumption that transformational rules can only dislocate constituents, the grammaticality of (8a.), the

ungrammaticality of (8b.), and the existence of the intervening pause between /gúbà/ and /gútá-gútá/ all serve to

drive home the fact that /gúbà-gútá-gútá/ does not form a constituent, unlike [gútá-gútá], which does.

2.2.2 RNC Stranding

The derivation involved in the question below and the subsequent stranding of the RNC are sufficient conditions

for RNC constituency-hood. In what follows, the underlying representation I assume is given by (9a.) and two

outputs of wh-movement by 9b. & 9c.

(9) a. Musa ba ké gútá-gútá.

5

Page 6: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

Musa cut what three-three

b. Ké Musa ba gútá-gútá o.

what Musa cut three-three FOC

‘What (did) Musa cut in threes?’

c. * ké gútá-gútá Musa ba o.

what three-three Musa cut FOC

The sentences in (9) take on a two-fold significance. On the one hand, the fact that / gútá-gútá/ can be stranded, as

(9b.) plainly shows, further substantiates the claim that the RNC is a tight compact unit. On the other hand, (9c.)

crucially establishes that the RNC does not form a constituent with ké –‘what’. Thus, given this and previous facts,

we conclude that RNCs are constituents comprised of a numeral root and its copy, no more no less.

2.2.3 RNCs and Coordinate Structures

Further evidence that RNCs form constituents stems from the fact that, one, the original numeral together with its

copy are conjoinable with other pairs of numerals and copies and two, that in such a conjoined state, they adhere to

the Coordinate Structure Constraint (Ross 1967). Let us first examine the claim that RNCs are conjoinable. Note

the following examples:

(10) a. Musa ba nakàn gútá-gútá tò gúni-gúni.

Musa cut meat three-three and four-four

‘Musa cut the meat in batches of three and four.’

b. Musa ba nakàn gúni-gúni tò gútá-gútá.

Musa cut meat four-four and three-three

‘Musa cut the meat in batches of four and three.’

Sentences (10a.) and (10b.) make it perfectly clear that RNCs can both be conjoined and reversed. Furthermore,

from this fact we can conclude that both conjuncts are themselves constituents and are characteristically of the same

categorical type as each other. For evidence that both conjuncts together comprise a larger constituent consisting of

the conjunction of both RNC constituents, we turn to the following examples which exploit the results of section

2.2.1 concerning RNC clefts:

(11) a. * Gútá-gútá Musa ba nakàn o tò gúni-gúni.

three-three Musa cut meat FOC and four-four

6

Page 7: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

b. Gútá-gútá tò gúni-gúni Musa ba nakàn o.

three-three and four-four Musa cut meat FOC

‘It was in batches of three and four that Musa cut the meat.’

As is evident from (11a.), the clefting transformation cannot operate on one conjunct alone. This violation of the

Coordinate Structure Constraint confirms that the conjunction of the RNCs in (11b.) is itself a constituent of the

same type as the RNC sub-constituents which compose it. That is, the constituency of [[gútá-gútá] tò [gúni-gúni]] is

a function of the constituency of [gútá-gútá] and [gúni-gúni], both Reiterated Numeral Constructions. Given these

facts, we are once again justified in asserting that RNCs are morphologically and syntactically unitary.

3.0 Adverbs and Adverbial Modification in Nupe

Having established the constituency of the RNC, we now turn our attention to an investigation of the

construction’s modificational and distributional properties. As was alluded to in the introduction, RNCs are

fundamentally adverbial modifiers in Nupe. Demonstrating that this is the case is the goal of this section’s

argumentation. Toward this aim, a variety of adverbials will be considered, resulting in the proposal that adverbs in

Nupe admit of a partition into two distinct adverb classes; inflectional type adverbs (I-types) and phrasal adverbs

(P-types), each of which possess unique distributional properties. Reflecting on the distribution of the RNC, it will

be argued that Reiterated Numeral Constructions are instances of phrasal adverbs, the more productive of Nupe’s

two adverb classes. We begin our investigation with a survey of adverbial modifiers in Nupe, which in addition to

being split into two distributional types (I & P-types) take on one of two different morphological forms; single

word modifiers, or multi-word (phrasal) modifiers.

3.1 Single Word Modifiers

Quite generally, adverbs in Nupe conform to the following generalization; when limited to one word, they are

primarily manner or temporal adverbs. Multi-word modifiers in the language, however, in addition to their role as

manner and temporal adverbs, function to encode the remaining varieties of adverbial modification that single word

modifiers fail to represent, namely instrumental and locative modification (among others). Again comparatively

speaking, in terms of frequency of occurrence and sheer breadth of expressability, single word modifiers are slightly

(though not considerably) less productive than multi-word modifiers. Syntactically, single word modifiers differ

among themselves with respect to the site at which they attach to VP. That this variance is indeed regular and

predictable will be elaborated on in the sections dealing with I & P-type adverbs. Consider the following

representative examples of single word modifiers, noting the variation in syntactic position of the adverbs, as well as

the absence of any form of modification other than manner and temporal:

(12) a. Gana tsò tangi karayín.

Gana set trap carefully

7

Page 8: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

‘Gana set the trap carefully.’

b. Gana dada du cènkafa.

Gana quickly cooked rice

‘Gana quickly cooked the rice.’

c. Gana à du cènkafa káńdóńdò.

Gana FUT cook rice always

‘Gana will always cook rice.’

d. Esìgi è tséká gbó.

Dog PRS sometimes bark

‘The dog barks sometimes.’

3.2 Multi-Word Modifiers

Not to be confused with phrasal-type adverbs, which are defined in terms of their distributional properties (to be

discussed in the upcoming section), multi-word modifiers are named solely in virtue of the fact that as units larger

than a word they modify VPs. Perhaps if RNCs did not exist in Nupe a more suitable name for these adverbials

would be PP-modifiers, since the preponderance of multi-word modifiers are in fact almost exclusively prepositional

phrases. However, sensitivity to the likelihood that additional phrasal adverbial modifiers might be discovered (as

with the discovery of RNCs) has led me to opt in favor of the more conservative terminology. Given this PP-like

tendency, such modifiers are able to take on a number of modificational roles including those of manner,

instrumental, and locative adverbials. Furthermore, the pervasiveness of the PP-modifier as the dominant multi-

word modifier is cashed out in the fact that as a class, they agree with respect to the site of VP adjunction. That is,

all multi-word adverbials are distributed identically. Bearing these properties in mind, note the following sentences

which highlight multi-word modification.

(13) a. Gana nì eyàn bè kágbó yin.

Gana played drum with strength

‘Gana played the drum forcefully.’

b. Gana ba nakàn bè èbi yin.

Gana cut meat with knife

‘Gana cut the meat with the knife.’

c. Gana tsò tangi dan efo cigban o.

Gana set trap in hole tree LOC

‘Gana set the trap under the tree.’

8

Page 9: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

3.3 Nupe Adverb Classes

O.T. Stewart (1996, 1998) claims that two kinds of adverbs exist in Edo (a geographically related language

spoken in mid-western Nigeria); I-type adverbs and N-type adverbs. I-type (inflectional type) adverbs exhibit signs

of being tense-related (inflection for tense is borne by the tone-bearing units of such adverbs, as in verbs), while N-

type (noun type) adverbs parallel the behavior of Edo nominals in that they do not tonally inflect for tense. As an

additional point of comparison, he notes that whereas I-type adverb adjunction is pre-verbal, N-type adjunction is

post-verb phrasal. I propose that a similar distinction holds for adverbs in Nupe. Building on the lower level

division into adverb classes developed in 3.1 and 3.2, we are now in a position to add to our Nupe adverb taxonomy.

Through an analysis of the licensing and distribution of the various flavors of adverbials introduced in the previous

two sections, a more restrictive partition among adverb types can be discerned. This will ultimately give rise to a

means for defending the claim that RNCs are fundamentally adverbial modifiers.

3.3.1 I-Type Adverbs

More so than the I-type adverbs of Edo, Nupe I-types are deeply INFL-related, although they are not inflected for

tense. Distributionally, however, in certain respects the two adverb classes converge in both languages4. The

fundamental properties of I-type adverbs in Nupe are given in (14).

(14) A. I-type adverbs follow INFL and occur pre-verbally.

B. I-type adverbs cannot be fronted.

C. I-type adverbs are licensed local to INFL.

(14) A. and B. further the claim that the distribution of I-type adverbs in Nupe is similar to the distribution in Edo.

To substantiate this proposal, consider the following set of sentences.

(15) Edo

a. (*Giégié) Òzó giégié rrí èvbàré (*giégié).

(*quickly) Ozo quickly eat food (*quickly)

‘(*Quickly) Ozo quickly ate the food (*quickly).’ (Stewart 1996)

b. * Òzó kó!kó giégié ògó

Ozo gather quickly bottle (Stewart 1998)

Nupe

c. (*Dàdà) Musa dàdà gí cènkafa (*dàdà).

(*quickly) Musa quickly ate rice (*quickly)

‘(*Quickly) Musa quickly ate the rice (*quickly).’

9

Page 10: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

d. * Musa gí dàdà cènkafa.

Musa ate quickly rice

e. (* Tséká) Musa è tséká gí cènkafa (*tséká).

(*sometimes) Musa PRS sometimes eat rice (*sometimes)

‘(*Sometimes) Musa sometimes eats rice (*sometimes).’

f. * Musa gí tséká cènkafa.

Musa ate sometimes rice

Given the data in (15), it is clear that the distribution of I-type adverbs in Nupe parallels Edo’s I-type distribution.

Specifically, I-types (which in Edo and Nupe are strictly single-word modifiers) cannot follow the verb or its object

in either language. Rather, they seem to be post-INFL heads which take VP complements, similar to auxiliary verbs

in English. Furthermore, it is evident that I-type adverbs in neither language can be preposed (cf. 15a,c).

The symmetry between Edo and Nupe I-type adverbs breaks down, however, when we consider adverb

placement non-local to INFL. Given that I-types occupy a pre-verb phrasal position, if we want to examine them

distanced from INFL we must have recourse to a sentence with a structure admitting of two VPs; one situated in the

typical post-INFL position and the other embedded somewhere below the first. Fortunately, there are such

constructions in Nupe, namely, Serial Verb Constructions (SVCs). Thus, we can ascertain the nature of the I-type

locality restriction by examining the distribution of I-type adverbs in the Serial Verb Constructions of both

languages. Informally, Serial Verb Constructions can be characterized as a complex consisting of two or more finite

verb phrases, occurring without marking of coordination or subordination. Although it has been proposed that SVCs

themselves admit of a typology (Stewart 1998), I will restrict my discussion of SVCs to the construction that Stewart

dubs the Consequential Serial Verb Construction (CSVC). When two verbs are serialized in this construction 5, V1

and V2 are transitive verbs sharing the same subject and object. As a consequence of this, V2 appears on the surface

to be devoid of its object argument, but actually assigns its object theta role to the NP intervening between V1 and

V2. In this way, CSVCs achieve object sharing (Baker 1989). The fundamental feature of the CSVC is that the

verbs of the construction are temporally ordered in the following way: the event denoted by VP1 precedes the

eventuality denoted by VP2 and so on for the other occurrences of verbs. As such, these verbs and verb phrases

denote discrete events, which together compose the macro event denoted by the matrix VP. The data in (16)

illustrate the CSVC in Edo and Nupe.

(16) Edo

a. Òzó lé èvbàré ré

Ozo cook food eat

‘Ozo cooked food and (then) ate it.’ (Stewart 1998)

10

Page 11: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

b. Òzó giégié lé èvbàré ré.

Ozo quickly cook food eat

‘Ozo quickly cooked the food and (then) ate it.’ (Stewart 1998)

Nupe

c. Musa du nakàn gí.

Musa cooked meat ate.

‘Musa cooked the meat and (then) ate it.’

d. Musa dàdà du nakàn gí.

Musa quickly cooked meat ate.

‘Musa quickly cooked the meat and (then) ate it.

Noting that in both languages I-types can occur to the left of VP1 in CSVCs, we might predict that leftward

modification of I-type adverbs with respect to VP2 is permissible in both Edo and Nupe, given the independent

status of VP2 as denoting a discrete event. This, however, is not the case. Whereas leftward modification of VP2 is

allowed in Edo, it is prohibited in Nupe.

(17) Edo

a. Òzó giégié lé èvbàré ré.

Ozo quickly cook food eat

‘Ozo quickly cooked the food and (then) ate it.’ (Stewart 1998)

b. Òzó lé èvbàré gié!gié ré.

Ozo cook food quickly eat

‘Ozo cooked the food and (then) quickly ate it.’ (Stewart 1998)

c. Òzó giégié dùnmwún èmà khién.

Ozo quickly pound yam sell

‘Ozo quickly pounded the yam and (then) sold it.’ (Stewart 1998)

d. Òzó dùnmwún èmà gié!gié khién.

Ozo pound yam quickly sell

‘Ozo pounded the yam and (then) quickly sold it.’ (Stewart 1998)

Nupe

e. Musa dàdà du cènkafa gí.

Musa quickly cooked rice ate

11

Page 12: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

‘Musa quickly cooked the rice and (then) ate it.’

f. *Musa du cènkafa dàdà gí.

Musa cooked rice quickly ate

*‘Musa cooked the rice and (then) quickly ate it.’

g. Musa è tséká du cènkafa gí.

Musa PRS sometimes cook rice eat

‘Musa sometimes cooks rice and (then) eats it.’

h. * Musa è du cènkafa tséká gí.

Musa PRS cook rice sometimes eat

*‘Musa cooks rice and (then) sometimes eats it.’

Given these facts concerning the possible sites of I-type placement, I propose that there is a locality restriction

governing the licensing of I-type adverbs in Nupe, namely, that there can be no intervening structure between INFL

and the adverb. That no such restriction is imposed on I-type adverbs in Edo is evident in their ability to occur

before VP2 in certain Serial Verb Constructions. Furthermore, this licensing condition explains 1) why I-types in

Nupe must be situated to the left of VP and 2) why they cannot be fronted. If they occurred in a post verb phrasal

position, the locality restriction would be violated, as the modified VP projection would intervene between INFL

and the adverb. Similarly, in the clefted position, the adverb would c-command INFL, contrary to the licensing

constraint previously mentioned.

3.3.2 Phrasal Adverbs

Phrasal adverbs (P-type adverbs) in Nupe are the analogues of N-type adverbs in Edo. As such, they admit of the

following distributional properties.

(18) A. P-type adverbs right adjoin to the VP.

B. P-type adverbs can be fronted.

C. There is no locality restriction governing the placement of P-type adverbs.

Consider the following examples which illustrate the uniformity of this distribution in both Edo and Nupe.

(19) Edo

a. Òzó (*ègiégié) rrí (*ègiégié) èvbàré ègiégié.

Ozo (*hurriedly) eat (*hurriedly) food hurriedly

‘Ozo (*hurriedly) ate the food hurriedly.’ (Stewart 1996)

12

Page 13: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

b. Ègiégié Òzó rrí èvbàré.

hurriedly Ozo eat food

‘Hurriedly, Ozo ate the food.’ (Stewart 1996)

c. Òzó dùnmwún èmà khién ègiégié.

Ozo pound yam sell quickly

‘Ozo pounded the yams and (then) sold it quickly.’ (Stewart 1996)

Nupe

d. Musa (*karayín) tsò (*karayín) tangi karayín.

Musa (*carefully) set (*carefully) trap carefully

‘Musa set the trap carefully.’

e. Karayín Musa tsò tangi o.

carefully Musa set trap FOC

‘Carefully, Musa set the trap.’

f. Musa du cènkafa gí karayín.

Musa cooked rice ate carefully

‘Musa cooked the rice and (then) ate it carefully.’

g. Musa (*bè kágbó yin) nì (*bè kágbó yin) eyàn bè kágbó yin.

Musa (*with strength) played (*with strength) drum with strength

‘Musa played the drum forcefully.’

h. Bè kágbó yin Musa nì eyàn o.

with strength Musa played drum FOC

‘Forcefully, Musa played the drum.’

i. Musa si eyàn nì bè kágbó yin.

Musa bought drum played with strength

‘Musa bought the drum and (then) played it forcefully.’

Note that in each batch of sentences for each language, the following are respectively illustrated: the right-edge of

VP as the site of P-type adverb adjunction; the ability of P-type adverbs to front; and the lack of a locality restriction

governing P-type placement (as manifest in the modification of VP2 in CSVCs). As is evident from the data, both

single word modifiers and multi-word modifiers (PPs) in Nupe share the distributional properties of Edo N-type

adverbs. That is, in addition to one-word adverbials, phrases in Nupe can act as N-type modifiers. This is clearly

13

Page 14: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

opposed to Nupe I-type adverbs, which are exclusively syntactic heads and differ saliently from Edo I-types in the

one crucial distributional property (locality to INFL). Thus, from the facts surrounding the syntactic environments

and licensing conditions of I-type and P-type adverbs, we conclude that in Nupe both classes of adverbs are in

complementary distribution.

3.4 The Reiterated Numeral Construction as a Phrasal Adverb

Although they behave as single morphological units, I claim that RNCs are instances of phrasal-type adverbs. To

drive this point home, consider the behavior of RNCs in the following syntactic environments.

(20) a. Gana (*gútá-gútá) ba (*gútá-gútá) nakàn gútá-gútá.

Gana (*three-three) cut (*three-three) meat three-three

‘Gana cut the meat in threes.’

b. Gútá-gútá Gana ba nakàn o.

three-three Gana cut meat FOC

‘It was in threes that Gana cut the meat.’

c. Gana ba nakàn gí gútá-gútá.

Gana cut meat ate three-three.

‘Gana cut the meat (any which way) and (then) ate it in threes.’

Sentences (20 a. & b.) confirm that RNCs right adjoin to VP and can be preposed. Furthermore, the occurrence

of the RNC in the Consequential Serial Verb Construction in (20)c affirms that its placement is not bound by any

locality restriction. Clearly, the distributional properties of the RNC mirror those of P-type adverbs. We are

justified in asserting the claim that RNCs are adverbial modifiers.

4.0 RNC Licensing

Thus far, our study has centered on the issues of RNC constituency, distribution, and modification. In the interest

of providing a well-rounded syntactic analysis, we now turn our attention to the issue of uncovering the constraints

licensing RNCs in Nupe. These constraints, although interesting in their own right, will provide further evidence

that RNCs are indeed adverbial modifiers, in addition to rounding out our investigation into the syntax of the

construction. Given the results of section three, which revealed that the domain of RNC modification is the verb

phrase, we begin our search for licensing elements within the VP.

4.1 RNCs and Object-Taking Verbs

Consider the following sentences of Nupe with RNCs and transitive verbs.

14

Page 15: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

(21) a. Musa pa ecizì gútá-gútá.

Musa pounded yams three-three

‘Musa pounded the yams in threes.’

b. *Musa pa eci ńdoci gútá-gútá.

Musa pounded yam certain three-three

*‘Musa pounded a yam in threes.’

c. Musa ba nakàn gúbà (…) gútá-gútá.

Musa cut meat two (pause) three-three

‘Musa cut two (pieces of) meat in threes.’

d. *Musa ba nakàn nini (…) gútá-gútá.

Musa cut meat one (pause) three-three

*‘Musa cut one (piece of) meat in threes.’

As the data in (21) suggest, the presence of an RNC seems to be correlated with the presence of a non-singular

transitive verb object. Thus, we might propose that in sentences with transitive verbs, RNCs are licensed by some

non-singular lexical item internal to the VP. As a test for this hypothesis, consider the following sentences with

transitive verbs, singular objects, and plural subjects.

(22) a. *Egizì ya èzè gútá-gútá.

children gave response three-three

*‘The children answered the question in threes.’

b. *Yinzàgizì lo dzukó gútá-gútá.

women went market three-three

*‘The women went to the market in threes.’

Clearly then, our initial hypothesis is on the right track. For if we were to propose that RNCs are licensed by some

non-singular antecedent without specifying which syntactic position was crucial, we would predict the sentences in

(22) to be grammatical. Thus, in sentences with transitive verbs, it appears that plural noun phrases condition the

grammatical appearance of RNCs. These plural NPs, however, are themselves restricted to object position if they

are to license RNCs within sentences with transitive verbs. At this point, we might inquire whether these same facts

hold if RNCs were to occur in sentences with ditransitive verbs. Must both objects be plural if RNCs are to occur

grammatically? Which object actually does the licensing? The answers to these questions will shed light on the

nature of the RNC as well as the facts surrounding its licensing. Toward an answer to these questions, consider the

following sentences.

15

Page 16: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

(23) a. (V1 object = singular, V2 object = plural)

*Musa ya Gana ebìzì gútá-gútá.

Musa gave Gana kola nuts three-three

b. (V1 object = plural, V2 object = singular)

*Musa ya egizì eze gútá-gútá.

Musa gave children response three-three

c. (V1 object = singular, V2 object = singular)

*Musa ya Gana eci gútá-gútá.

Musa gave Gana yam three-three

d. (V1 object = plural, V2 object = plural)

*Musa ya egizì ebìzì gútá-gútá.

Musa gave children kola nuts three-three

Thus, despite the facts that obtain in simple sentences with transitive verbs, RNCs are not licensed in sentences with

verbs taking more than one object. That is, the licensing requirement previously proposed is in need of

strengthening. RNCs not only require a non-singular NP to occur in object position in sentences with transitive

verbs, they require that this non-singular object position be unique and that it be filled by a non-singular noun

phrase. That these facts are not the result of a particular quirk of Nupe involving the verb ‘give’ is given by the

ungrammaticality of sentences similar to those in (23) involving other ditransitive verbs, such as ‘sold’ and ‘show’.

Armed with these results, we now consider the issue of RNC licensing in sentences with intransitive verbs.

4.2 RNCs and Non Object-Taking Verbs

Given the results of the preceding section, we might predict that RNCs are unable to occur in sentences with

intransitive verbs. This, however, is not the case. Rather than discrediting the licensing hypothesis put forth so far,

the ability of RNCs to occur in such sentences provides the basis for a principled theory of RNC licensing (to be

formulated later). Consider the following.

(24) a. *Ekungùncì á tsu gútá-gútá.

warrior PRF die three-three

*‘The warrior has died in threes.’

b. Ekungùncìzì á tsu gútá-gútá.

warriors PRF die three-three

‘The warriors have died in threes.’

16

Page 17: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

c. *Egi be gútá-gútá.

child came three-three

*‘The child arrived in threes.’

d. Egizì be gútá-gútá.

children came three-three

‘The children arrived in threes.’

e. *Egi nìkin gútá-gútá.

child fell three-three

*‘The child fell in threes.’

f. Egizì nìkin gútá-gútá.

children fell three-three

‘The children fell in threes.’

Given this data and the results of the preceding section, we might propose that the RNC licenser depends on the type

of verb. That is, we might claim that when transitive, RNCs are licensed if and only if the verb’s object is unique

and non-singular and that when intransitive, a non-singular NP in subject position does the licensing. In the interest

of evaluating the plausibility of this claim, consider the following sentences.

(25) a. *Egizì lotùn gútá-gútá.

children work three-three

*‘The children worked in threes.’

b. *Egizì lele gútá-gútá.

children sleep three-three

*‘The children slept in threes.’

c. *Egizì yànyàn gútá-gútá.

children danced three-three

*‘The children danced in threes.’

d. *Egizì dazàn gútá-gútá.

children walked three-three

*‘The children walked in threes.’

Contrary to the claims inferred from the data in (24), the presence of a plural subject cannot save the grammaticality

of such RNC-containing sentences with intransitive verbs. Thus, in the following manner we might argue that the

17

Page 18: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

hypothesis offered at the outset of this section cannot be entertained. Taking the data in (24) and (25) into account,

along with the findings of section 4.1, we might formulate a unified theory of VP-internal RNC licensing that does

not require the positing of a separate licensing constraint for each type of verb. Such a unified account, we might

claim, becomes apparent once we consider the nature of the verbs in (24) and (25).

The sentences in (24) differ significantly from those in (25) despite the fact that both sets contain sentences with

intransitive verbs. Whereas the verbs in (24) are unaccusative, those in (25) are unergative. The cash-value of this

difference is that unaccusative/unergative verbs differ in the syntactic position in which their arguments are base-

generated (Burzio 1986). Thus, in (24b) for example, it is hypothesized that ekungùncìzì-‘warriors’ is underlyingly

generated in object position and raises to subject position for case-related reasons. On the other hand, in sentences

with unergative verbs, agents are underlyingly generated in subject position. Thus, in (25a) for example,

egizì-‘children’ occupies the subject position underlyingly, leaving the object position vacant. Taking into account

the facts governing RNC licensing in sentences with transitive verbs as well as the data in (24) and (25), a clear

pattern regarding VP-internal licensing emerges. Namely, if RNCs are to be licensed in simple non-serialized

sentences, the verb must have a unique object position that underlyingly gets filled by a plural noun phrase . This

proposal nicely accounts for the data presented so far. As for sentences with transitive verbs, RNCs are licensed just

in case the verb takes no more than one non-singular argument. This stems from the fact that transitive verb objects

are base-generated in object position. RNCs can also exist in sentences with unaccusative verbs, in so far as the

verb’s argument is non-singular. This is a consequence of the position in which unaccusative verb arguments are

base-generated. Since RNC licensing is a function of a sentence’s underlying structure, as we have proposed, the

underlying object-position of an unaccusative verb’s argument gives rise to acceptable RNC sentences just in case

the verb’s argument is plural. Finally, given the licensing constraint proposed, we predict that RNCs cannot exist in

sentences with unergative verbs in virtue of the fact that both underlyingly and at the surface, the object position of

an unergative verb is vacant. Although this accounts for the data in (25), this prediction is not borne out in the

language. Consider sentence (26) below.

(26) Elúgizì fù gútá-gútá.

birds flew three-three

‘The birds flew in threes.’

Clearly the analysis previously sketched is in trouble, for the non-singular object position constraint previously

hypothesized is not across-the-board. Given the acceptability of (26) then, it seems that our initial proposal was on

the right track. That is, certain constraints governing RNC licensing vary according to the type of verb. In order to

drive this home, we must understand why the sentences in (25) are ungrammatical, while (26) is perfectly

acceptable. Such an understanding, however, depends upon grasping the nature of the verbs themselves in the

sentences in question.

Note that the verbs in (25) are the first polysyllabic verbs to be encountered in this study thus far. While this

might seem trivial, it plays a significant role in the analysis to be sketched. Crucially, the verbs in (25) are not the

18

Page 19: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

proper sort to be called “transitive” or “intransitive”. Rather, they are all instances of “complex verbs”, verbs which

together with a nominal element form a verbal complex that syntactically functions as if it were a single verb. Nupe

admits of two types of verbal complexes; cognate object verbs (as in (25)) and split verbs, in which the verb’s object

splits the components of the complex, as italicized in (27) below.

(27) a. Musa dà tákùn gwa.

Musa pushed rock hand

‘Musa pushed the rock.’

b. Musa le tákùnzì yé.

Musa saw rocks eye

‘Musa saw the rocks.’

As cognate object verbs, the predicates in (25) can be broken down into their verbal and nominal components. In all

cases, cognate object verbs are formed by attaching the verb to its nominal form, after deleting the word-initial

vowel of the nominal. This is illustrated in (28) below.

(28) a. lo + etùn → lotùn

‘worked’ ‘work’ → ‘worked’

b. le + ele → lele

‘slept’ ‘sleep’ → ‘slept’

c. yàn + eyàn → yànyàn

‘danced’ ‘dance’ → ‘danced’

d. da + ezàn → dazàn

‘walked’ ‘walk’ → ‘walked’

Given the nature of their formation, the verbs in (25), although not “transitive” verbs in the strict sense of the term,

can be analyzed as object-taking verbs. The conclusions drawn in 4.1 and the data analyzed at the outset of this

section can now be generalized to account for the unacceptability of the sentences in (25) and the acceptability of

those in (24) and (26). If a verb takes an immediate object , that object must be unique and non-singular if RNCs are

to be licensed; if a verb does not take an object, then its subject must be non-singular in order to license RNCs.

Because the nominal component (immediate object) of a cognate object verb can never be pluralized, we predict

RNCs in Nupe to be disallowed in sentences with complex verbs. This prediction is borne out in the language.

Furthermore, because the nominal component of a split verb is not an immediate object, RNCs are licensed in

sentences with split verbs if the immediate object that splits the verbal from the nominal element is non-singular.

Thus, by restricting our attention to the nature of the immediate object of a verb, rather than the verb’s status as

19

Page 20: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

“transitive” or “intransitive”, we can account for the possibility of RNCs in sentences with split verbs as well as the

impossibility of RNCs in sentences with cognate object verbs. In this way, our theory of RNC licensing explains the

grammaticality judgments of (21)-(27), while making correct predictions about significant classes of sentences in the

language. Such findings accord nicely with the adverbial analysis of RNCs previously given. Since RNCs right-

adjoin to VP as adverbial adjuncts, it is not surprising that the principal licensing constraint centers on aspects

related to the nature of the verb.

4.3 Further Considerations in RNC licensing

Thus far, the examples of sentences with RNCs we’ve seen have been strikingly uniform in several respects. In

all cases, the main verb of the sentence has been event denoting and the verb’s subject or object has always assumed

the status of a count noun. Furthermore, all sentences considered up to this point have had animate subjects and

inanimate objects. The goal of this section is to determine whether or not the sentences we have seen so far have

these properties necessarily. That is, do the properties previously mentioned in any way license RNCs? To answer

these questions, let us consider the following sentences.

(29) a. *Musa è yebo egizì gútá-gútá.

Musa PRS likes children three-three

b. *Musa du cènkafa gútá-gútá.

Musa cooked rice three-three

c. Musa bà eyandzòzì gútá-gútá.

Musa counted toys three-three

d. *Ebi ba nakàn gútá-gútá.

knife cut meat three-three

e. Musa yán nangizì ló èmì gútágútá.

Musa chased sheep be-in house three-three

‘Musa chased the sheep into the house in threes.’

In the interest of being concise, further examples will not be provided. However, it will be mentioned that these

samples are quite representative of similar types of sentences in the language. That is, Nupe does not allow RNCs to

occur in sentences with stative verbs or inanimate subjects. Furthermore, it seems that in addition to being plural

and unique, the verb’s object, which may or may not be animate (as illustrated by (29e) above), can not be a mass

noun. These final remarks conclude our discussion of the secondary licensing constraints governing RNCs in Nupe.

(30) below summarizes the various licensing constraints argued for in section four.

20

Page 21: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

(30) Constraints on RNC licensing (for simple non-serialized sentences)

i. If V takes an immediate object, that object must be unique and filled by a non-singular count noun.

ii. If V does not take an object, its subject must be a non-singular count noun.

iii. V must be eventive.

iv. V’s subject must be animate.

5.0 Conclusion

2 Numeral constructions such as those in (1c) seem to be common in languages which employ reduplication rather productively. To my knowledge, in addition to Nupe such numeral complexes are attested in Hebrew, Yoruba, Edo, Kikongo, Lingala, Tagalog, Georgian, Turkish, and Malagasy. See Gill (1982) for others. It is my belief that a significant percentage of the world’s languages employ numerals in this way and that the phenomenon is somehow rooted in facts surrounding reduplication, as opposed to being unique to African and Austronesian languages. In these respects, such numeral constructions are deserving of our attention.

3 Numeral stems in Nupe (e.g. gútá) are composed of a numeral root and an affix. Cardinal numerals are comprised of a prefix plus the root; ordinal numerals consist of the root plus a suffix. Thus, it is argued that in a numeral like gútá, gú is a prefix and tá is the numeral root. Evidence for this proposal comes from the fact that tá occurs in both cardinal and ordinal numerals, while gú does not (Kawu 1997); e.g. gútá (‘three’) vs. tátsi (‘third’). Thus, RNCs can be seen as types of stem-reduplicated numerals. In this way, they contrast with another intriguing numeral construction present in the language, in which the root is the target of reduplication. In such constructions, numerals such as gútá-tá act both like universal quantifiers and cardinality predicates. See Kandybowicz (2000) for a full discussion of the syntax and semantics of both stem and root reduplicated numerals in Nupe.

4 See Baker, Stewart, et al. (2000) for arguments that adverbs adjoin higher in Nupe than in Edo. Such a claim, if true, does not refute the proposals put forth in section 3, but rather provides a more precise explanation for the phemenon detailed there.

5 Serial Verb Constructions are not limited to chains of exactly two verbs occurring in series. For the sake of clarity and brevity, however, I discuss the properties of SVCs limited to just those complexes housing two verbs. These properties can be generalized to instances when more than two verbs occur serialized.

References

Baker, M.C. (1989) “Object Sharing and Projection in Serial Verb Constructions. Linguistic Inquiry 20, #4: 513-

553.

Burzio, L. (1986) Italian Syntax: A Government-Binding Approach, Dordrecht: Reidel

George, I. (1975) A Grammar of Kwa-Type Verb Serialization: Its Nature and Significance in Current Generative

Theory, Ph.D dissertation, UCLA.

Gill, D. (1982) Distributive Numerals, Ph.D dissertation, UCLA.

21

Page 22: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

I have argued that adverbs in Nupe admit of a type distinction similar to the one found in Edo. Using this finding

as grounds for a taxonomic division of adverbials in Nupe, the question regarding the modificational status of the

Reiterated Numeral Construction gives rise to an intriguing answer. Contrary to the standard role of the numeral as

adjectival modifier (semantically speaking), when duplicated and situated adjacent to the original, the resulting

numerals together as a morphosyntactic unit take on the modificatory role of adverb as predicate of an event. Thus,

in sentence (31):

(31) Musa dà tákùnzì gwa ya gúni-gúni.

Musa pushed rocks hand fell two-two

‘Musa pushed the rocks down in twos (pairs).’

the reiterated numeral complex modifies the event of rock pushing, not the substance rocks, following Parsons’

account of adverbs as predicates of events denoted by verbs (Parsons 1990). I take this finding to be of considerable

significance for two reasons: 1) it cuts across the generalization that numerals are strictly nominal/adjectival in

nature, and 2) it suggests that the fundamental means by which adverbs are classified is not based solely on the

Kandybowicz, J. (2000) “Deviant Numeral Constructions in Nupe”, B.A. honors thesis, Rutgers University.

Kawu, A. (1997) “Nupe Reduplication”, ms. Rutgers University.

Parsons, T. (1990) Events in the Semantics of English: A Study in Subatomic Semantics, Cambridge, Mass.: MIT

Press.

Ross, J. (1967) Constraints on Variables in Syntax, Ph.D dissertation, MIT. Cambridge, Mass. Published in 1986

as Infinite Syntax. Norwood, N.J: Ablex.

Stewart, O.T. (1996) “Adverb Placement and the Structure of the Serial Verb Construction,” Proceedings of NELS

26, 409-423, MIT/Harvard GLSA.

Stewart, O.T. (1998) The Serial Verb Construction Parameter, Ph.D dissertation, McGill University.

Stewart, O.T., Baker, M.C., Kawu, A. “A Comparative Analysis of Serial Verb Constructions in Kwa Languages.”

Talk presented at the 31st Annual Conference on African Linguistics, March 3 2000, Boston University.

22

Page 23: The Syntax of the Reiterated Numeral Construction1resnik/prize99/papers/kandybowicz/RNC-E…  · Web viewIn light of the fact that the presence of a copied numeral gives rise to

adverb’s semantic label (e.g. manner, benefactive, comitative), but rather the conjunction of the adverb’s semantic

attributes with its distributional properties.

Notes

23