the factor structures of ability and trait emotional

86
UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones 8-1-2020 The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional Intelligences The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional Intelligences Relative to General Intelligence and Personality Relative to General Intelligence and Personality Nicholas Carfagno Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalscholarship.unlv.edu/thesesdissertations Part of the Psychology Commons Repository Citation Repository Citation Carfagno, Nicholas, "The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional Intelligences Relative to General Intelligence and Personality" (2020). UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones. 3987. http://dx.doi.org/10.34917/22086615 This Dissertation is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It has been brought to you by Digital Scholarship@UNLV with permission from the rights-holder(s). You are free to use this Dissertation in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/or on the work itself. This Dissertation has been accepted for inclusion in UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Digital Scholarship@UNLV. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Upload: others

Post on 23-Mar-2022

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones

8-1-2020

The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional Intelligences The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional Intelligences

Relative to General Intelligence and Personality Relative to General Intelligence and Personality

Nicholas Carfagno

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalscholarship.unlv.edu/thesesdissertations

Part of the Psychology Commons

Repository Citation Repository Citation Carfagno, Nicholas, "The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional Intelligences Relative to General Intelligence and Personality" (2020). UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones. 3987. http://dx.doi.org/10.34917/22086615

This Dissertation is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It has been brought to you by Digital Scholarship@UNLV with permission from the rights-holder(s). You are free to use this Dissertation in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/or on the work itself. This Dissertation has been accepted for inclusion in UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Digital Scholarship@UNLV. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Page 2: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

THE FACTOR STRUCTURES OF ABILITY AND TRAIT EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCES

RELATIVE TO GENERAL INTELLIGENCE AND PERSONALITY

By

Nicholas Carfagno

Bachelor of Arts – Psychology University of California, Santa Cruz

2013

Master of Arts - Psychology University of Nevada, Las Vegas

2017

A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the

Doctor of Philosophy - Psychology

Psychology Department College of Liberal Arts The Graduate College

University of Nevada, Las Vegas August 2020

Page 3: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

ii

Dissertation Approval

The Graduate College The University of Nevada, Las Vegas

July 17, 2020

This dissertation prepared by

Nicholas Carfagno

entitled

The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional Intelligences Relative to General Intelligence and Personality is approved in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Doctor of Philosophy - Psychology Psychology Department

Stephen Benning, Ph.D. Kathryn Hausbeck Korgan, Ph.D. Examination Committee Chair Graduate College Dean Michelle Paul, Ph.D. Examination Committee Member Kimberly Barchard, Ph.D. Examination Committee Member Harsha Perera, Ph.D. Graduate College Faculty Representative

Page 4: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Abstract iii

List of Figures iv

Chapter 1: Literature Review 1

Chapter 2: Current Studies 13

Chapter 3: Method 14

Chapter 4: Data Analysis 22

Chapter 5: Discussion 34

Appendix A: R Studio Syntax 40

Appendix B: GSCI Assessment 44

Appendix C: Additional Fit Statistics 56

References 57

Curriculum Vitae 73

Page 5: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

ABSTRACT

Emotional Intelligence is a popular term used to describe one’s experience with their own

emotions and awareness of others’. Although emotional intelligence is thought to be a helpful

predictor of various variables such as work performance and wellbeing, there is disagreement

within the field of the precise definition and method of measurement. The current study

examines two of the most popular models of emotional intelligence, ability and trait models, to

determine if they are the same construct or two different constructs that share a moniker.

Additionally, both ability and trait emotional intelligences’ factor structures are examined

through hierarchical and bifactor models to determine the best fitting model for each. Finally,

ability emotional intelligence is explored relative to how it fits with current understandings of

cognitive ability and trait emotional intelligence is explored relative to how it fits with current

understandings of normal-range personality.

Page 6: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

iv

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. Hierarchical and bifactor models of the MSCEIT 25

Figure 2. Hierarchical and bifactor models of the TEIQue 26

Figure 3. Hierarchical and bifactor models of general intelligence 27

Figure 4. Hierarchical and bifactor models of general personality 28

Figure 5. Hierarchical models of ability EI and general intelligence 30

Figure 6. Scatterplot of the correlation between ability EI and trait EI 32

Page 7: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

1

CHAPTER 1: LITERATURE REVIEW

Emotional intelligence (EI) represents an individual’s experience with their own

emotions and awareness of others’ (Fernandez et al., 2012); it is a predictor of life satisfaction

(Urquijo et al., 2016), academic performance (Fernandez et al., 2012), and career adaptability

(Coetzee & Harry, 2014), among other variables. Despite EI’s apparent utility, dominant

researchers of the field disagree in regards to both its definition and method of measurement.

Until a relative consensus is formed, it is difficult to determine a standardized utility for

measuring EI. This dissertation will address the relationship between two of the most researched

forms of EI: ability EI and trait EI. Ability EI is defined as a mental ability that must be tested,

and trait emotional intelligence is defined as a facet of personality that can be assessed through

self-report (Cherniss, 2010).

Previous research suggests that ability and trait models of EI vary from being orthogonal

constructs to displaying a weak relationship with each other (r = .18; Brannick et al., 2009; Di

Fabio & Saklofske, 2014; Petrides, 2011; van der Linden et al., 2017). Any correlation between

the two models is likely due to the relationship between mental abilities (such as crystallized and

fluid intelligences) and a general factor of personality (van der Linden et al., 2017). Moreover,

substantial disagreements about the structure of emotional intelligence within each model have

plagued the field (MacCann et al., 2014; Van der Linden et al., 2012; van der Linden et al.,

2017). I aim to bridge the divide within the EI literature by examining both ability EI and trait EI

with their commonly associated constructs of general intelligence and normal-range personality

within the same study. Each construct’s factor structure will be examined to determine which

model has the best fit. The sample used in this study will consist of a greater range of intellectual

functioning than samples typically used when examining ability EI; as a result, my findings will

Page 8: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

2

be more generalizable relative to many previous studies. In summary, this dissertation will

address the following questions: What is the factor structure of ability and trait emotional

intelligences? Is ability EI a stratum II broad ability in the hierarchical model of general

intelligence and is trait EI truly synonymous with a general factor of personality? Lastly, will

ability EI and trait EI be correlated after controlling for general intelligence and personality?

Ability Model Emotional Intelligence

The ability model of EI was originally outlined by Mayer, DiPaolo, and Salovey (1990)

and was more concretely defined by Mayer and Salovey (1997, pp. 3–34). Ability EI is defined

as the ability to recognize emotions, to effectively use emotions, perceive the meanings

associated with particular emotions, and to effectively regulate emotions (Mayer & Salovey,

1997). More recently, ability EI has been further described as an intelligence that is associated

with efficient problem solving and quality of social relationships (Mayer et al., 2008). Ability EI

was created from a theoretical standpoint of a four-branch model consisting of the above

definition, with each branch contributing to an overall global factor of EI (Mayer & Salovey,

1997; Salovey & Grewal, 2005). The branches have been designated as a) managing emotions so

as to attain specific goals, b) understanding emotions, emotional language, and the signals

conveyed by emotions, c) using emotions to facilitate thinking, and d) perceiving emotions

accurately in oneself and others (Mayer et al., 2008). Ability EI is typically measured by the

Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT; Brannick, Wahi, & Goldin, 2011;

Maul, 2012). Not only was the MSCEIT created by the researchers responsible for one of the

modern definitions of ability EI, it also represents one of the most examined assessments for any

form of EI (Kong, 2014).

Page 9: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

3

However, the MSCEIT’s factor structure has been the subject of many psychometric

critiques. Gardner and Qualter (2011) observed very similar scores and high overlapping

variance between emotion facilitation and perceiving emotions via confirmatory factor analysis

(CFA). As a result, they suggest that these two factors of ability EI be collapsed into one factor,

yielding a three-factor model consisting of emotion perception, emotion understanding, and

emotion management (Rossen et al., 2008). Fan et al. (2010) also concluded that a three-factor

model better fits the MSCEIT after conducting a review of CFAs of 18 different studies using the

measure; they also found emotion facilitation and perceiving emotions to be highly correlated (r

= .90). The authors of the MSCEIT have acknowledged these findings and attribute them to a

potential issue with the test design or unintended participant response styles. Regardless, they

have chosen to maintain a four-factor model based on theory and emotion facilitation’s

association with the global EI score (Mayer et al., 2016).

Though most of the literature surrounding the factor structure has modeled the MSCEIT’s

structure at the subtest level, Maul (2012a) demonstrated that a finer-grained consideration of the

MSCEIT’s structure at the item-level and recommended either a unidimensional or two-factor

structure of the instrument. Maul also points out that the MSCEIT is especially difficult to model

from a sub test level due to its different item types, which causes some of the variance observed

to result from the different formats of different items. Additionally, not all items are independent

of each other. Several groups of items are dependent on a test-taker’s interpretation of a single

vignette or image. Although an item-level analysis would help rectify some of these difficulties

in measurement, these analyses require large sample sizes (N = 758 in that study) and item-level

data that the test publisher does not provide. This renders verification of Maul’s results

challenging in the scope of this dissertation.

Page 10: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

4

The MSCEIT has been found to be associated with both increased adaptive and decreased

maladaptive emotional functioning but only reduced maladaptive social functioning. First, the

MSCEIT is associated with wellbeing. Sanchez-Alvarez, Extremera, and Fernández-Berrocal

(2016) conducted a meta-analysis of three studies and found a correlation of .22, while Brackett

and Mayer (2003), which was not included in the meta-analysis for unknown reasons, reported a

correlation of .28. The MSCEIT is negatively correlated with assessments that measure the

presence of overall depressive and anxious symptomatology (r = -.27; Cejudo, 2016; r = -.31;

Brackett & Salovey, 2006). Additionally, the MSCEIT is associated with less aggressive

behaviors, r = -.14 (Megías et al., 2018), and less interpersonal conflict, r = -.45, but not more

positive interpersonal experiences (Lopes et al., 2003). Thus, high scores on the MSCEIT may be

associated with better emotional health, but they are not as strongly associated with positive

social relationships as the test’s authors suggest they should be.

Although the authors of the MSCEIT suggest that the MSCEIT measures one’s ability to

use feelings to enhance thought (Mayer et al., 2002), the MSCEIT is in fact more strongly related

to one’s overall intelligence rather than its application measured through real world achievement.

Kong (2014) conducted a meta-analysis of 46 publications, consisting of 53 study samples,

examining the relation of the MSCEIT (and its predecessor, the MEIS) with cognitive ability.

Kong reported a correlation of the MSCEIT/MEIS of .30 with general intelligence. Correlations

of .26 (Kong, 2014) and .23 (Pardeller et al., 2017) were found for the MSCEIT and verbal

intelligence measures. Finally, a correlation of .23 was found with nonverbal intelligence

measures (Kong, 2014). In regards to the application of intelligence, Brackett and Mayer (2003)

found that the MSCEIT correlated with high school GPA .21, college GPA .16, and Verbal SAT

scores .32. If the MSCEIT did truly measure one’s ability to enhance thought through the use of

Page 11: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

5

emotion, there should be a greater correlation between the MSCEIT and GPA rather than the

MSCEIT and general intelligence. The only instance in which the authors’ suggestion appears to

be true is in regards to verbal tasks. The small to moderate correlations suggest that ability EI is a

separate, but related construct to g.

Ability Emotional Intelligence and General Intelligence

In current theories, ability EI exists as a stratum II factor, while general intelligence (g) is

a stratum III; rather, ability EI is a factor that loads onto one one’s overall g (MacCann et al.,

2014; Mayer et al., 2016). General human intelligence is commonly viewed within a hierarchical

or bifactor model (Barbey, 2018; Beaujean, 2015; McGrew, 2009). Studies examining

intelligence measures often implement both models without finding significant differences

between the two (Beaujean et al., 2014; Canivez et al., 2017; Reynolds & Keith, 2017). Because

ability EI and intelligence is typically understood through a hierarchical model, I will begin with

this model to discuss both constructs.

The hierarchical model of intelligence, also referred to as the Cattell–Horn–Carroll

(CHC) model, combines the tenets of the Cattell and Horn's Gf–Gc model and the Carroll Three-

Stratum model of intelligence (McGrew, 2009). The CHC model breaks intelligence down into

three strata, the highest stratum (stratum III) refers to general intelligence. Below general

intelligence lies stratum II, which refers to broad abilities. These broad abilities include fluid

reasoning (Gf), comprehension-knowledge (Gc), short-term memory (Gsm), visual processing

(Gv), auditory processing (Ga), long-term storage and retrieval (Glr), cognitive processing speed

(Gs), decision and reaction speed (Gt), reading and writing (Grw), and quantitative knowledge

(Gq). Although currently still under investigation, the following broad abilities have been

tentatively included as part of the CHC model: general (domain specific) knowledge (Gkn),

Page 12: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

6

tactile abilities (Gh), kinesthetic abilities (Gk), olfactory abilities (Go), psychomotor abilities

(Gp), and psychomotor speed (Gps). The lowest stratum (stratum I) refers to narrow abilities that

make up different broad abilities. For example, Gs consists of perceptual speed, rate-of-test-

taking, number facility, speed of reasoning, reading speed, and writing speed.

MacCann et al. (2014) used structural equation modeling to demonstrate that ability EI

meets the correlational criteria of a stratum II broad ability. “Correlational criteria” refers to

three standards that must be met: 1) the mental abilities that make up a broad ability must form a

coherent construct, 2) a broad ability must demonstrate a positive correlation with other

previously established tests of intelligence, and 3) a broad ability must demonstrate some unique

variance relative to other broad abilities. In regards to the first criterion, the three primary mental

abilities (emotion perception, emotion understanding, and emotion management) of ability EI

form a single construct, which is represented by a global score. The second criterion has also

been met; the subtests of ability EI have an average correlation of .30 (ranging from .05 to .63)

with 15 different subtests of cognitive ability (which were primarily taken from the Educational

Testing Service Kit of Factor Referenced Cognitive Tests; MacCann et al., 2014). The third

criterion also appears to be successfully met due to the three branches of ability EI demonstrating

unique variance relative to other existing stratum II broad abilities; emotion perception, emotion

understanding, and emotion management correlate .62, .84, and .53 with Gc, while Gf-Gc

correlate .87 and Gf-Gv correlate .88 (MacCann et al., 2014). While ability EI does meet the

criteria of a CHC model, trait EI does not meet the aforementioned criterion and is better

measured through self-report.

Assuming ability EI is a stratum II ability, as suggested by MacCann et al. (2014), the

three MSCEIT branches of Emotion Perception, Emotion Understanding, and Emotion

Page 13: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

7

Management (Fan et al., 2010; Roberts et al., 2001; Rossen et al., 2008) would be considered

Stratum I abilities. A strength of viewing the branches this way is that the MSCEIT provides

formal assessments of these EI-related stratum I abilities, in contrast to the more purely

theoretical nature of most stratum I constructs. However, it is difficult to say with confidence

that the branches represent stratum I abilities unique to the MSCEIT, because these branches

appear to contain other commonly described stratum II skills within their subtests. For example,

the perceiving emotions branch has items that require test-takers to read a story and to identify

emotional content (Mayer et al., 2002). Individuals who demonstrate strengths in the stratum II

abilities Grw and Gv will read and comprehend the stories better than those who do not, which

would suggest that the Stratum II abilities of Grw and Gv would also influence an individual’s

global EI score. Even if these MSCEIT branches represent unique stratum I abilities, Maul

(2012a)’s suggested factor structure raises the question whether two or three narrow abilities

would be appropriate. Indeed, that analysis suggests even finer grained abilities below stratum I

might be necessary to understand influences on MSCEIT scores. For these reasons, I confined

my factor-level analyses to global MSCEIT scores at the stratum II level of the CHC model of

intelligence.

Trait Model Emotional Intelligence and Personality

The trait model of EI (which is alternatively labeled “emotional self-efficacy”) was

initially defined as a construct that examined behavioral dispositions and self-perceived abilities

via self-report consisting of empathy, assertiveness, self-reported social intelligence, and self-

reported ability EI (Petrides, Pita, et al., 2007; Petrides & Furnham, 2001). The Trait Emotional

Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue) is an assessment frequently used to measure the above-

described model of trait EI (Petrides, 2009; van der Linden et al., 2017). Trait EI has been

Page 14: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

8

associated with several affective variables, many of which are also correlated with personality.

TEIQue global scores correlated -.50 with mental health symptoms in a 10-study meta-analysis

(Martins et al., 2010). This moderate correlation was further supported by Cejudo (2016), who

reported a correlation of .53 between the TEIQue and the absence of psychological distress. The

absence of psychopathology symptoms is possibly explained by successful coping; the TEIQue

is moderately correlated with adaptive psychological coping, r = .44 (Mikolajczak et al., 2008).

Unlike the MSCEIT, which only consistently correlated with the absence of distressing affect,

the TEIQue is also correlated with reported feelings of happiness, (r = .70; Furnham & Petrides,

2003; r = .60; Furnham & Christoforou, 2007).

Though ability EI is thought to fall within the realm of general intelligence, trait EI is

believed to be associated with a general factor of personality as part of a hierarchical model (van

der Linden et al., 2017). The general factor of personality refers to the single highest-order factor

derived from measures of normal-range personality (Musek, 2007). The general factor of

personality is positively correlated with extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and

openness; it is negatively correlated with neuroticism (Dunkel & van der Linden, 2017).

Through the use of confirmatory factor analyses, Van Der Linden, Te Nijenhuis, and

Bakker (2010) suggest that personality exists in a hierarchical model consisting of three different

levels. This finding has also been supported through meta-analyses of the big five model of

normal-range personality (Davies et al., 2015). At the bottom level of latent variables, there are

the big five (extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness, and neuroticism). The

level above that contains “the big two:” alpha and beta. Alpha, also referred to as stability,

consists of conscientiousness, agreeableness, and neuroticism. Alpha is associated with

consistent moods, goals/motivation, and interpersonal styles. Beta, also referred to as plasticity,

Page 15: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

9

consists of extraversion and openness. Beta is associated with willingness to search for and

develop new goals and experiences (DeYoung et al., 2002; Liu & Campbell, 2017; van der

Linden et al., 2010). The highest level of the hierarchical model contains the aforementioned

general factor of personality that encompasses all of the lower-level factors (Rushton et al., 2009;

van der Linden et al., 2010). The interpretation of the general factor of personality when modeled

by the big five (Costa & McCrae, 1992) is still contested. While initially it was thought to reflect

social desirability (Arias et al., 2018; Davies et al., 2015), some current research suggests that it

better explains social effectiveness (Dunkel et al., 2016; van der Linden et al., 2016) and that

instances of wanting to appear socially desirable only affects the factor structure rather than the

validity of the general factor itself (Schermer & Holden, 2019).

Recently, trait EI has been examined within the hierarchical model of personality (Van

der Linden et al., 2012; van der Linden et al., 2017). Trait EI is the highest loading factor when

examined with big five dimensions and highly correlates with the general factor of personality (r

= .86). Due to the large amount of shared variance between trait EI and the general factor of

personality, it may be argued that the two constructs are fairly redundant (Van der Linden et al.,

2012; van der Linden et al., 2017).

Thus far, comparisons between the general factor of personality and trait EI have been

conducted with general factors produced from the big five model. Although they are all referred

to as the “general factor of personality,” the variance explained by the general factor depends on

the method of measure (Hopwood et al., 2011; Loehlin, 2012). Another popular model of

personality is the Giant Three, which is measured by the Multidimensional Personality

Questionnaire (MPQ; Tellegen & Waller, 2008). While some studies suggest that a meaningful

general factor of the MPQ exists (Rushton et al., 2009), others have had difficulty replicating the

Page 16: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

10

factor loadings and deny a robust general factor (Donnellan et al., 2012). Indeed, the correlations

between the NEO-PI-R and MPQ general factors are extremely variable, between .12 (Hopwood

et al., 2011) and -.59 (Loehlin, 2012). Loehlin (2012) suggests that this is likely a result in

different statistical methods and states that he obtained the general factor of personality directly

as the first principal factor from the intercorrelations of the scales of the inventories. In contrast,

Hopwood et al. (2011) obtained the general factors through a series of exploratory hierarchical

factor analyses starting with the lowest-order set of scales, which likely funneled out substantial

variance that was retained in Loehlin (2012). Because the relationship between the MPQ’s

general factor of personality and trait EI has not yet been explored, it is unclear whether it will be

as strong as that involving the Big Five’s general factor.

Points of Contention between Models of Emotional Intelligence

As the field continues to polarize about whether ability or trait EI represents the most

informative form of EI, researchers are becoming more willing to acknowledge that these are

likely different constructs sharing a similar title (Cherniss, 2010; Petrides, Pita, et al., 2007).

Those who favor ability EI argue that other forms of EI are not appropriate because they do not

objectively measure problem-solving skills, reasoning, or behavioral outcomes, whereas

measures of ability EI do (Pisner et al., 2017). Ability EI is also favored by neuroscientists

interested in EI due to its clearer neural correlates (Killgore & Yurgelun-Todd, 2007). Ability

EI’s apparent applicability to neuroscience results from its similarities with traditional

assessments of cognitive abilities and aforementioned more objective nature (Pisner et al., 2017).

Those who support trait EI argue that it is more important to have a measure with greater

incremental validity and ease of administration (Andrei et al., 2016). The TEIQue demonstrates

incremental predictive validity over general personality in regards to facial recognition,

Page 17: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

11

loneliness, eating disorders, well-being, emotion regulation, stress response, coping strategies,

depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and somatic complaints (Andrei et al., 2016; Jolić-

Marjanović & Altaras-Dimitrijević, 2014; Petrides, Pérez-González, et al., 2007). The MSCEIT

demonstrates incremental validity over general personality and general intelligence when

predicting academic achievement but not negative affect, positive affect, life-satisfaction, and

psychological distress (Karim & Weisz, 2010; Lanciano & Curci, 2014).

Bifactor and Hierarchical Models

The bifactor model represents an alternative factor structure to the hierarchical model.

The hierarchical model assumes that each subtest of an assessment loads onto a specific factor

and that each specific factor loads onto a higher-order general factor. Therefore, variance within

a subtest is never directly explained via the general factor. The bifactor model assumes that each

subtest loads onto both a general factor and specific factor and that both general and specific

factors exist on the same stratum (Kranzler et al., 2015; MacCann et al., 2014; Murray &

Johnson, 2013). Therefore general factors within a bifactor model are directly measured through

subtests and are not mediated by specific factors. Additionally, the general factor is partialled out

of the lower-order factors, which results in the factors being independent of each other (Arias et

al., 2018; Gignac, 2008).

Bifactor models have a number of interpretive advantages over hierarchical models,

though they may be less able to detect poorly fitting models because they fit large numbers of

parameters. Group factor scores from bifactor models are readily interpretable due to the group

factors being independent of the general factor, which allows for parsing out independent

estimates of test variability due to general and group factors. Separating general and group

factors also reduces multicollinearity among factors in multivariate analyses (Kranzler et al.,

Page 18: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

12

2015). Without having a factor structure forced into a hierarchy, bifactor models typically

provides a better fitting model. This is because forcing a factor structure requires proportionality

constraints such that the ratio of general and group factor variance is the same for each indicator

within the group factor;the greater the violation of these constraints, the worse fit statistics the

model will have (Gignac, 2016). Even with a likely better fit, this does not imply that the bifactor

model is always advantageous. When using the bifactor model, more parameters must be

estimated, which reduces the overall precision of estimations made. While hierarchical models

are more sensitive to misspecifications and are less likely to accept incorrect models compared to

bifactor models (Murray & Johnson, 2013; Preacher et al., 2013). Fit statistics for cognitive

ability measures, which includes the MSCEIT, computed as part of a CFA tend to favor bifactor

models even when the construct being examined is truly a higher order model in simulation

studies (Morgan et al., 2015). Thus, researchers should not base their models solely on fit

statistics but instead also be cognizant of the theory that the cognitive assessment is grounded in.

Though it must be cautioned that given the complex nature of psychological assessment, it is

unreasonable to assume that the proportionality constraints placed by researchers accurately

reflect the true factor structure of an assessment (Gignac, 2016).

Alternative Models and Other Measurements of Emotional Intelligence

Other models of emotional intelligence combine both self-report and ability, but tend to

use either the term trait model or the term ability model. For example, some examine ability,

personality, motivation, and empathy in a self-report format (van Zyl & de Bruin, 2012;

Whitman et al., 2008). I view the constructs of ability EI and trait EI as two distinct entities that

have differing methods of measurement, and this dissertation aims to examine both as such.

Page 19: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

13

CHAPTER 2: CURRENT STUDY

The present study seeks to compare the different theories and methods of measures of EI

in an undergraduate sample. Specifically I will contrast the ability model of EI, measured by the

Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT), and the trait model of EI,

measured by the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue). Three specific questions

will be addressed. First, does a hierarchical or bifactor model best represent ability EI, trait EI,

general intelligence, and general personality? Second, in reference to the first question, what is

the factor structure of each construct? Third, are the global scores of ability EI and trait EI

correlated with each other and with general factors of intelligence and personality?

Hypotheses

I predict that although ability EI and trait EI share the moniker of emotional intelligence,

that they in fact represent different constructs that are products of their tools of measurement. I

also predict that ability EI, trait EI, general intelligence, and general personality will best fit a

hierarchical model. Due to ability EI’s similarity in measurement to traditional intelligences, I

believe that it will load as a stratum II broad ability within the CHC model of general

intelligence. Due to trait EI’s similarity in measurement to personality, and its previous

association with the general factor of personality, I believe that trait EI will represent an

analogous construct to the general factor of personality regardless of the factor structure used

(van der Linden et al., 2017).

Page 20: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

14

CHAPTER 3: METHOD

Participants

Participants consisted of 207 (68% female) undergraduate students recruited from

University of Nevada, Las Vegas via the Sona system. This desired sample size was determined

by using a normal bivariate model in G*Power 3.1.9.2 (Faul et al., 2007). It has 80% power to

detect two-tailed correlations of .25 at an α level of .005, which provides a higher evidentiary

value than a typical α level of .05 (Benjamin et al., 2018). A predicted correlation of .25 was

used because it has been identified to be the average correlation among variables in personality

research (Gignac & Szodorai, 2016). The mean participant age was 19.3 years (SD = 3.14 years).

In total, 41.6% of participants were White, 14.0% were Black, 25.8% were Asian, 2.5% were

Native American or Native Alaskan, and 14.5% identified as some other racial group; 19.8% of

participants identified as Hispanic.

Measures

Demographics

The demographic questionnaire was a self-report measure consisting of 61 items. This

measure inquired about the participant’s age, gender, race/ethnicity, marital status, family

history, educational history, medical/psychological history, substance use, and criminal activity.

Emotional Intelligence

Two measures of emotional intelligence were used in this dissertation. The MSCEIT

(Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, & Sitarenios, 2003) was used to measure ability emotional

intelligence, and the TEIQue (Petrides, 2009) was used to measure trait emotional intelligence.

Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). The MSCEIT is a

141 item, electronically administered assessment of ability EI. It contains four branches that are

Page 21: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

15

believed to be components of EI: perceiving emotions, using emotions to facilitate thought,

understanding emotions, and managing emotions. Items on the MSCEIT involve multiple choice

and rating scale responses to stimuli, such as anecdotes and the facial expressions.

Each item is scored with reference to the consensus of either experts in emotion research

or lay community members without this expertise. Because measures of traditional intelligence

are scored in a manner closer to the expert consensus method and the authors recommend it due

better inter-rater agreement (Mayer et al., 2003), I will use expert consensus scores from the

MSCEIT. Expert consensus scoring more closely aligns with traditional intelligence measures

because items are created with the intent that some answers are more correct than others, which

are determined by an individual who specializes in the associated field. Nevertheless, across all

five item responses to the 141 items, expert and lay consensus scores had an agreement rate of r

= .91. Furthermore, expert and lay consensus branch, area, and total scores correlate between .96

and .98. The MSCEIT contains a full test split-half reliability of r = .91 for expert consensus

scores (Mayer et al., 2003).

Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue). The TEIQue is 153-item self-

report measure of trait EI. It consists of the following four factors and 15 facets: 1) Well-being

examines positive affect and self-esteem [Trait optimism, Trait happiness, Self-esteem], 2)

Sociability [Emotion management of others, Assertiveness, Social awareness] examines one’s

effectiveness in affecting others’ emotions, assertiveness, and social skills, 3) Emotionality [Trait

empathy, Emotion perception of self and others, Emotion expression, Relationships] examines

perspective taking, awareness of own and others’ emotions, communicating emotions, and

presence of fulfilling relationships, 4) Self-control [Emotion regulation, Impulsiveness, Stress

management] examines control over one’s own emotions, resistance of urges, and stress

Page 22: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

16

regulation. The last two facets do not load onto any factors of trait EI, but they contribute to

one’s overall trait EI: Self-motivation, which examines one’s unwillingness to give up, and

Adaptability, flexibility in new situations (Andrei et al., 2016). Facets typically range in internal

consistency (as calculated by Cronbach alpha) from .68 to .87, with an Cronbach alpha’s of .89

for total EI scores (Petrides, 2009).

Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire - Brief Form (MPQ-BF). The MPQ-BF

is a 155-item self-report measure of normal-range personality. It consists of three higher order

factors and 11 primary trait scales. The inclusion of primary trait scales is an advantage over the

big five models of personality, in which the lowest level factors are facets. The inclusion of

primary trait scales will allow for interpretations with greater specificity (Miller et al., 2011).

The first higher order factor, Positive Emotional Temperament (PEM), refers to one’s

disposition towards the experience of positive emotions. PEM consists of several subscales:

Well-Being (feelings of happiness and optimism), Social Potency (preferring attention and

socially dominant roles), Achievement (hardworking and ambitious attitude), and Social

Closeness (feeling close and affectionate towards many friends). The second high order factor,

NEM, refers to one’s disposition towards the experience of negative emotions. Negative

Emotional Temperament (NEM) consists of the following sub-scales: Stress Reaction (proneness

to anxiety, stress, and becoming easily upset), Alienation (feelings of suspicion and being

betrayed by others), and Aggression (experiencing enjoyment when observing violence and

willingness to mistreat others for personal gain. The final higher-order factor, Constraint (CON),

refers to one’s ability to inhibit behavior and plan before acting. CON consists of the following

subscales: Harm Avoidance (preference for safe, non-thrilling activities), Control (preference for

caution, control, and planning), and Traditionalism (preference for cultural and religious norms).

Page 23: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

17

The MPQ-BF also examines the primary trait of absorption, which assesses heightened

responsiveness to sensory experiences and vividness of imaginings. Absorption cross loads

approximately equally onto PEM and NEM (Benning & Freeman, 2017; Eigenhuis et al., 2017;

C. J. Patrick et al., 2002)

The primary trait scales of the MPQ-BF maintain good internal consistency with

Cronbach’s α between .74 and .84 (C. J. Patrick et al., 2002). The MPQ also includes three

validity scales: Variable Response Inconsistency (VRIN), True Response Inconsistency (TRIN),

and Unlikely Virtues. While the first two validity scales act to identify inconsistent responding,

Unlikely Virtues aims to detect impression management and those who are attempting to appear

socially desirable (Benning & Freeman, 2017; C. J. Patrick et al., 2002). This is particularly

advantageous considering Schermer and Holdern (2019)’s warning that trying to appear socially

desirable affects the factor structure of models that include a general factor of personality. Most

other normal-range personality measures do not include a social desirability scale; those that do

contain a social desirability scale, such as the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, are older

measures and are limited to fewer primary trait scales (Sato, 2005). As a result, the MPQ-BF

presents itself as the most appropriate normal-range personality measure for this dissertation.

Fluid intelligence. Fluid intelligence refers to one’s ability to learn and manipulate new

information; processes involved include problem solving and reasoning (Hülür et al., 2018;

Takaiwa et al., 2018). To measure fluid intelligence, I used select subtests of the International

Cognitive Ability Resource (ICAR). The ICAR is an open-access assessment of general

intelligence that was collaboratively created by several researchers. Three subtests of the ICAR

were used to measure fluid intelligence. Three-dimensional Rotation consists of 24 items that

asks participants which of the six answer choices is a possible rotation of the target stimuli cube;

Page 24: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

18

all cubes contain figures on the three visible sides. Participants could also choose “None of the

cubes could be a rotation” and “I do not know the solution.” Letter and Number Series consists

of nine items that provide five numbers or letters and asks participants which letter or number

comes next in the series; six number or letter answer choices are provided along with “None of

these” and “I don’t know.” Matrix Reasoning consists of 11 items, each of which provide a 3 x 3

grid consisting of eight figures with a missing figure in the bottom-right corner. Participants

were asked to choose one of six answer choices that provide the figure that would complete the

pattern established in the grid (Condon & Revelle, 2014).

Crystallized intelligence. Crystallized intelligence refers to one’s ability to recall

information such as facts and vocabulary (Hülür et al., 2018; Takaiwa et al., 2018). To measure

crystallized intelligence, I used three assessments. First, the Shipley Institute of Living Scale:

Vocabulary, which is a 40-item, multiple-choice assessment of one’s spelling ability. Individuals

are given 10 minutes to complete the assessment. It is used to assess crystallized intelligence in

adults (Shipley, 1940).

Secondly, I used certain items of the Verbal Reasoning subtest of the ICAR. This subtest

contains questions regarding factual information with the options of eight responses. Some of

these items ask the test-taker to solve mathematical word problems; these items were excluded.

This left seven factual information questions for participants to answer.

The final measure of crystallized intelligence is the Grammatical Sentences of

Crystalized Intelligence (GSCI). The GSCI is a 30-item measure that asks individuals to identify

a possible grammatical error within a short passage. Each passage contains four underlined

portions. Individuals must identify which of the four underlined portions contains an error. If

none of the underlined portions contain an error, they must specify so. This assessment was

Page 25: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

19

created specifically for this dissertation due to the ICAR’s plethora of fluid intelligence

subscales, but minimal crystalized intelligence measures.

Because not all 30 items were likely to be acceptable measures of this construct, I

determined prior to the study that I would prune the final set of items to analyze. I used a parallel

component analysis of the tetrachoric correlation matrix with 1000 replications using the

fa.parallel function in the psych R package to determine whether multiple factors have

eigenvalues greater than the 95th percentile of resampled data. The parallel analysis indicated that

three factors should be extracted, so I subjected these data to a Schmid-Leiman exploratory

bifactor analysis involving promax rotation using the mirt function in the mirt R package. After

inspecting the rotated factor loadings, I concluded that there was only one interpretable factor.

The first factor comprised items indexing a variety of grammatical issues. The second factor

comprised items that were nearly always incorrectly answered in our sample, marking it as a

difficulty factor. The third factor comprised all the items with “No error” as the correct response.

Because only one factor emerged, I retained all items with a loading of >|.45| on the first factor

and <|.25| on the other two factors, which retained nine items to use in my subsequent analyses.

Procedures

Participants read and signed a consent form before being allowed to participate in the

study. Participants were seated in a room containing five desktop computers, three on one side of

the room and two on the other, with computer towers between each participant to ensure privacy

among adjacent participants. Over the course of approximately two hours, participants completed

the MSCEIT, TEIQue, MPQ-BF, and ICAR. Demographic information was collected along with

two other assessments that will not be included in this dissertation. All assessments were

administered electronically, and a research assistant was present in the testing room to answer

Page 26: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

20

any potential questions regarding the experiment. Participants received course credit for

engaging in the study after being debriefed about its purpose.

Data Analysis

A stepwise data analysis plan was used in which a series of models were compared. The

first research question addressed whether a hierarchical or bifactor model best represents the

factor structures described above of ability EI, trait EI, general intelligence, and normative

personality. Each structure was modeled using confirmatory analysis via the sem command in the

lavaan package in R.

For each model, I reported χ2, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and

standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) values as measures of absolute fit; Tucker-

Lewis Index (TLI) values as a measure of relative fit; and Akaike and Bayesian Information

Criteria (AIC and BIC) as measures of the relative likelihoods of each model. Following Hu and

Bentler (1999), I aimed to consider well-fitting models with RMSEA values close to or below

.06, SRMR values close to or below .08, and TLI values close to or exceeding .95. I modified ill-

fitting models only with paths that were both theoretically sensible and had a basis in prior

studies. In each pair of models, the model with the lower value of AIC and BIC was selected to

answer subsequent research questions. One univariate outlier that was greater than three standard

deviations from the mean were excluded from the MSCEIT’s Managing branch. I used a

Mahalanobis distance test to identify multivariate outliers. Although the MSCEIT contained

three, the TEIQue contained four, and the general intelligence measures contained three, the

exclusion of these outliers did not significantly change any of the fit statistics or allow models

that did not initially converge to converge. As a result, I did not remove the multivariate outliers

from analyses.

Page 27: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

21

The second research question aimed to determine whether ability EI is a second-order

factor of general intelligence or a separate construct, as well as determining whether trait EI is a

second-order factor of the general factor of personality or a separate construct. The best-fitting

models from the first research question was used in these analyses. Structural equation models

were used to model each EI as either a second-order factor beneath the general factor and as a

separate construct to determine the best fit.

The third study question aimed to answer whether ability and trait EI are correlated. I

accomplished this by using the factor scores extracted from the best fitting ability and trait EI

models determined by the second research question.

Page 28: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

22

CHAPTER 4: RESULTS

Aim 1

All model fit and comparison statistics for Aim 1 are summarized in Table 1. In general,

the MSCEIT and intelligence measures had superior fits to the TEIQue and MPQ-BF.

Table 1

Fit Statistics for Hierarchical and Bifactor Models of Each Measure

Model RMSEA 90% RMSEA CI SRMR TLI AIC BIC MSCEIT

Hierarchical .045 .000-.081 .051 .935 -3663 -3580 Bifactor N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A

TEIQue Hierarchical .121 .108-.135 .090 .820 7359 7472 Bifactor .112 .098-.126 .074 .828 7305 7448

Intelligence Hierarchical .036 .000-.116 .039 .978 -694 -658 Bifactor .081 .000-.221 .020 .884 -691 -644

MPQ-BF Hierarchical .092 .072-.113 .088 .745 -414 -327 Bifactor .081 .058-.105 .081 .802 -432 -322

MSCEIT

When examining the MSCEIT, I encountered errors regarding inability to invert the

matrix for both the hierarchical and bifactor models; this was most likely due to having latent

variables with only two indicators. As a result, I constrained the Faces and Pictures subtests to

load equally on Perceiving Emotions and the Changes and Blends subtests to load equally onto

Understanding Emotions. These constraints allowed the hierarchical model to converge

Page 29: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

23

successfully, but the bifactor model was still unable to invert its matrix. To attempt to create a

bifactor model that converged, I specified starting values for each factor. Unfortunately, even

with the specifications the model could not converge and fit statistics were unable to be

calculated (see Figure 2).

TEIQue

The hierarchical factor model of the TEIQue produced no errors when fitting the model,

whereas the bifactor model had negative variance in the Emotion Regulation facet (see Figure 2).

Although that variance had a small and insignificant z score (z = -0.125, p = .901), it had a very

large standard error of 50.9; the next largest standard error in the model was more than two

orders of magnitude lower at 0.097, even though all indicators were scored on the same scale. As

a result, I did not interpret this negative variance as being indistinguishable from zero due to the

uncertainty caused by the enormous standard error. This led me to conclude that even though the

bifactor model had lower AIC and BIC values, the hierarchical model was the best model when

considering the instability of the bifactor model’s parameters and was used in subsequent

analyses.

General Intelligence

Initially, errors inverting the matrix were encountered when modeling general

intelligence with hierarchical and bifactor structures. This was likely due to the elimination of

the verbal reasoning subtest (which had a Cronbach’s α of .29, even after screening for outliers),

leaving Gc with only two observed variables (Little et al., 1999). To remedy this problem, I

constrained the Shipley and the GSCI to have equal loadings on Gc. This modification allowed

both models to converge successfully without any errors (see Figure 3). Not only did the

hierarchical model produce a RMSEA and a TLI closer to what I had previously indicated would

Page 30: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

24

be a “good model,” it also produced a lower BIC than the bifactor model. Therefore, the

hierarchical factor structure of general intelligence was the better fitting model and was used in

subsequent analyses. Crystallized and fluid intelligences had essentially equal and strong

loadings on the general intelligence factor.

MPQ-BF

The hierarchical model of the MPQ-BF produced variances for PEM, NEM, and CON

greater than 1. This prevented the standardized parameters of the GFP model from being

calculated. The bifactor model of the MPQ-BF initially produced a negative residual variance for

the Traditionalism facet. Due to this residual’s low and insignificant z score (z = -0.122, p =

.903), as well as its interpretable standard error of 0.515 (which was within two orders of

magnitude of the next largest standard error at 0.016), I determined that this variance was

indistinguishable from zero. As a result, I constrained the residual variance of Traditionalism to

zero. After this constraint, the bifactor model ran without any errors (see Figure 4). The bifactor

model was the best model of the MPQ-BF based on BIC and having no inadmissible parameter

estimates, as was the case in the hierarchical model. In this model, Control (-), Harm Avoidance

(-), and Aggression were the strongest (albeit moderate) markers of the general factor of

personality.

Page 31: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

25

Figure 1. Hierarchical (top panel) and bifactor (bottom panel) models of the MSCEIT. Prc = Perceiving emotions, Und = Understanding emotions, Mng = Managing emotions, Fcs = Faces, Pct = Pictures, Chn = Changes, Bln = Blends, Fcl = Facilitation, Sns = Sensations, E_M = Emotional management, S_M = Social management, AEI = Ability emotional intelligence

Page 32: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

26

Figure 2. Hierarchical (top panel) and bifactor (bottom panel) models of the TEIQue. WlB = Well-being, Scb = Sociability, Emt = Emotionality, SlC = Self-Control, Opt = Trait Optimism, Hpp = Trait happiness, SlE = Self-esteem, EmM = Emotion management, Ass = Assertiveness, ScA = Social awareness, Emp = Trait empathy, Emp = Emotion perception, ExE = Emotion expression, Rlt = Relationships, EmR = Emotion regulation, ImC = Impulse control, StM = Stress management, Mtv = Motivation, Adp = Adaptability, TEI = Trait Emotional Intelligence

Page 33: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

27

Figure 3. Hierarchical (top panel) and bifactor (bottom panel) models of general intelligence. Gf = Fluid intelligence, Gc = Crystallized intelligence, TD_ = Three-dimensional rotation, LNS = Letter and number series, M_R = Matrix reasoning, Shp = Shipley Institute of Living Scale: Vocabulary, GSC = Grammatical Sentences of Crystalized Intelligence, IQ = General Intelligence.

Page 34: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

28

Figure 4. Hierarchical (top panel) and bifactor (bottom panel) models of general personality PEM = Positive emotionality, NEM = Negative emotionality, CON = Constraint, W_B = Wellbeing, S_P = Social potency, Ach = Achievement, S_C = Social closeness, Abs = Absorption, S_R = Stress reaction, Ain = Alienation, Agg = Aggression, Cnt = Control, H_A = Harm avoidance, Trd = Traditionalism.

Page 35: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

29

Aim 2

All model fit and comparison statistics for Aim 2 are summarized in Table 2. None of

these models had good absolute levels of fit, which likely reflects a number of unmodeled

potential cross loadings among indicators across factors.

Table 2

Fit Statistics for Models Assessing Relationships among Conceptually Similar General Factors

Model RMSEA 90% RMSEA CI SRMR TLI AIC BIC INTELLIGENCE AND MSCEIT

Separate Factors .160 .134-.187 .184 .710 494 548 Second-Order Factor .132 .106-.160 .133 .803 461 514

MPQ AND TEIQUE Separate Factors N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A Second-Order Factor N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A

General Intelligence and the MSCEIT

Continuing to use the constraints reported for the hierarchical models of general

intelligence and the MSCEIT allowed both the general intelligence and ability EI as separate

general factors model and the second-order factor model successfully converge without errors.

The factor scores of perceiving emotions, understanding emotions, and managing emotions from

their individual models were saved and used in the current models to decrease the number of

parameters that were required to be estimated (see Figure 5). Compared to the separate general

factors model, the ability EI as a second-order factor model had a higher TLI value and lower

SRMR, AIC, and BIC values. In this model, ability EI had a moderate loading on the general

intelligence factor and was substantially smaller than those for crystallized and fluid intelligence.

Page 36: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

30

Page 37: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

31

Figure 5. Hierarchical models of ability EI and general intelligence. Prc = Perceiving emotions, Und = Understanding emotions, Mng = Managing emotions, Fcs = Faces, Pct = Pictures, Chn = Changes, Bln = Blends, Fcl = Facilitation, Sns = Sensations, E_M = Emotional management, S_M = Social management, AEI = Ability emotional intelligence, Gf = Fluid intelligence, Gc = Crystallized intelligence, TD_ = Three-dimensional rotation, LNS = Letter and number series, M_R = Matrix reasoning, Shp = Shipley Institute of Living Scale: Vocabulary, GSC = Grammatical Sentences of Crystalized Intelligence, IQ = General Intelligence.

Page 38: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

32

MPQ-BF and TEIQue

Despite continuing to constrain the traditionalism facet to zero, neither model could be

identified. The GFP and trait EI as separate general factors model produced an error that its

matrix could not be inverted, while the second-order factor model indicated that no solution

could be found. Neither error messages nor models indicated reasonable modifications that could

be used to create a viable factor structure; therefore, the relationship between the GFP and trait

EI in Aim 3 was examined via correlating the general factors from their separate models.

Aim 3

To examine the relationship between ability EI and trait EI, I correlated both general

factors from their best fitting models in Aim 1. The two demonstrated a very small and

insignificant correlation, r(205) = .0006, p = .994 (see Figure 6). For the sake of thoroughness, I

examined the correlations of the ability general factors with one another and the trait general

factors. General intelligence correlated moderately with MSCEIT ability EI, r(205) = .553, p <

.001, but the MPQ-BF’s general factor did not correlate with TEIQue trait EI, r(205) = -.0025, p

= .971.

Page 39: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

33

Figure 6. Scatterplot of the correlation between ability EI and trait EI. AEI = ability EI, TEI = trait EI.

−2 −1 0 1

−2

−1

01

Only_EI_Fscores$AEI

Only_EI_Fscores$TEI

Page 40: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

34

CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION

This study examined the factor structures of the MSCEIT, general intelligence as

measured through a number of assessments, the TEIQue, and the MPQ-BF. In accordance to my

first hypothesis, Ability EI, general intelligence, and trait EI all were best described by

hierarchical structures. Contrary to this hypothesis, the bifactor model fit best when examining

personality. The first half of my second hypothesis, which stated ability EI is a second-order

factor of general intelligence, was found to be correct. The second half that stated trait EI and the

GFP, as measured by the MPQ-BF, are analogous constructs was incorrect; in fact, they appear

to be separate and unrelated constructs. Finally, ability EI and trait EI were not significantly

related, which confirmed my final hypothesis that ability EI and trait EI are completely separate

constructs that share only a moniker.

Models of Abilities and Traits

Abilities

Many contemporary models of general intelligence approach the construct via a bifactor

model because of the tendency to yield a better statistical fit (Schult & Sparfeldt, 2016). Despite

a usually better fit, several authors caution against assuming general intelligence is best

represented by a bifactor because of the overall statistical bias towards bifactor model fit indices

(Morgan et al., 2015; Murray & Johnson, 2013). Instead, it is important to consider both

statistical fit and construct conceptualization when choosing a preferred model (Morgan et al.,

2015). Not only does the hierarchical model of general intelligence appeal more to a layperson’s

intuition, it was also a better statistical fit when examining my data. A possible explanation for

the better fit is the limited number of indicator variables used in the study, but in combination

with construct conceptualization, it is the best observed model for this data.

Page 41: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

35

Similarly, the hierarchical model of the MSCEIT yielded a relatively good fit, whereas I

was unable to find a solution that allowed the bifactor model to converge. Such difficulties with

the bifactor model of the MSCEIT were also observed by Evans et al. (2020), who were also

unable to find a solution. MacCann et al. (2014) was able to find a bifactor MSCEIT solution,

but only after constraining indicator variables to have equal loadings, therefore making the

bifactor model indistinguishable from the hierarchical model.

Both Evans et al. (2020) and MacCann et al. (2014) suggest ability EI is a second-order

factor of general intelligence. I wanted to test these findings due to neither study examining

whether ability EI would be a better statistical fit as a separate but related construct. Though

neither the second-order factor model nor separate factors model had a good overall fit, the

nested model had a better fit than the separate model based on both absolute and relative fit

statistics. Within the second-order model, ability EI had a moderate loading onto general

intelligence. This finding does not suggest that ability EI should be included in test batteries

assessing general intelligence. Ability EI should instead be conceptualized similarly to other

second-order factors that are not typically included on cognitive intelligence batteries, such as

Ga, Gt, Glr, Gs, Grw, and Gq. Just like ability EI, these factors tend to have weaker loadings on

general intelligence relative to Gc and Gf (MacCann et al., 2014).

Traits

The relationship between the GFP and trait EI is somewhat unclear. The GFP, as modeled

by the MPQ-BF, appears to be unrelated to trait EI. The GFP in this study must be interpreted

with caution. While I had initially chosen the MPQ-BF as a normal-range personality measure

due to it containing an unlikely virtues scale, which would have been used if I was unable to

extract a general factor, I had not considered that its factor scores of PEM, NEM, and CON were

Page 42: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

36

created to be independent of each other. Unlike Donnellan et al. (2012), I was able to extract a

general factor from the MPQ-BF. However, this general factor resembles trait externalizing due

to the high loadings of aggression, control, and harm avoidance (Blonigen et al., 2016; Hopwood

& Grilo, 2010) rather than traditional GFPs that are associated with social effectiveness and

social desirability (Arias et al., 2018; Davies et al., 2015; van der Linden et al., 2016). My

findings support the conclusions made by Hopwood et al. (2011) that the GFP is dependent on

which personality inventory is used to obtain it, and that general factors between personality

assessments typically have a weak to non-existent correlation. The TEIQue is typically examined

through a hierarchical model (Petrides, 2009), and my results further support this choice. While

the hierarchical model did not indicate a “good fit” as specified in my methods section, it

successfully converged without any psychometric concerns, whereas the bifactor model

contained unresolvable Heywood cases that rendered it uninterpretable.

Contrary to my hypothesis, trait EI and the GFP did not represent analogous constructs;

instead, they were completely uncorrelated. This is in stark contrast to Van Der Linden et al.'s

(2017) meta-analysis, which reported a correlation of .86 between trait EI and the GFP. A major

difference between the current study and the meta-analysis is that the meta-analysis calculated

the GFP via the big two: alpha and beta. Within this study, the observed GFP created by the

MPQ-BF resembled trait externalizing (Hopwood & Grilo, 2010). Externalizing behaviors are

negatively correlated with alpha and positively correlated with beta (DeYoung et al., 2008);

given the very high correlation between trait EI and the GFP calculated from a big two model, it

is understandable that trait EI would be uncorrelated with a GFP that resembles trait

externalizing. As a result, my findings regarding trait EI and personality may actually offer

Page 43: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

37

discriminant validity; thus, only the GFP as calculated by the big five and big two is similar to

trait EI.

The Generality of Emotional Intelligence

In regards to my final hypothesis, my findings support that ability EI and trait EI are

completely separate constructs. In fact, they are a prime example of the jingle fallacy, leading

individuals to assume that they measure the same construct because they both share the title of

emotional intelligence (Gonzalez et al., 2020). Simply placing the “ability” or “trait” descriptor

before the term “EI” is likely to continue the conceptual confusion that has roiled these

literatures. Therefore, I propose that ability EI continue to use the title EI due to it being

measured similarly to traditional intelligences, while trait EI as measured by the TEIQue solely

use its alternative name of “emotional self-efficacy” (Petrides, Pita, et al., 2007).

Limitations and Future Directions

While it would have been ideal to examine the plethora of different EI assessments and

models beyond an undergraduate sample, the scope of this dissertation did not allow for it due to

funding and time constraints. The Situational Test of Emotion Management (STEM) and

Situational Test of Emotional Understanding (STEU) are also measures of ability EI. The STEM

assesses one’s ability to manage emotions, whereas the STEU measures one’s ability to

understand emotions (MacCann et al., 2011). These measures were created due to concerns of

the factor structure of the MSCEIT and the desire to increase the availability of ability EI

measures (Ferguson & Austin, 2011; Libbrecht & Lievens, 2012). These measures are somewhat

commonly used and have been utilized by some medical schools as part of their selection process

(De Leng et al., 2017). The STEM and the STEU were not used in this dissertation because

according to the ability EI model from which these assessments were created, there are four

Page 44: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

38

branches, which have been commonly collapsed into three through psychometric rigor in recent

research (MacCann et al., 2014). The STEM and the STEU only contain two of the branches,

managing emotions and understanding emotions. I believed that only two branches would not

thoroughly address the research questions posed in this study.

The Emotional Quotient Inventory 2.0 (EQ-I 2.0) is a self-report measure, but the items

attempt to examine features that are more closely aligned with the ability model (Van Zyl, 2016;

Petrides, 2009, pp. 85–101). The Schutte Self Report Emotional Intelligence Test (SSEIT) is

another popular self-report measure that uses the same model of EI as the MSCEIT (Schutte et

al., 1998). Although the measure was designed based on the model of EI used to develop the

MSCEIT, the two measures only correlate .14 (Schutte et al., 1998).

This study omitted the competency model of EI, which accounts for both EI and social

intelligence (Kunnanatt, 2008). As defined by this model, EI is associated with self-awareness of

one’s internal states and preferences as well as the self-management of internal states and

impulses (Kunnanatt, 2008). The competency model is measured by the Emotional and Social

Competence Inventory (ESCI; Kunnanatt, 2008). It aims to measure EI via a “behavioral

approach” and makes use of several raters (i.e. supervisors, peers, etc.), including the individual

being assessed. The ESCI is associated with performance in certain environments, such as at

one’s occupation, and aids in identifying strengths and weaknesses (Boyatzis, 2009; Kunnanatt,

2008).

Lastly, obtaining a GFP with a personality measure other than the MPQ-BF, which was

originally designed to have three orthogonal factors (C. Patrick & Kramer, 2017), would likely

allow for a more cohesive and meaningful general factor that could be examined relative to trait

EI.

Page 45: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

39

Conclusions

In summary, ability EI should be considered a second-order factor of general intelligence.

Therefore, some of the variance observed in ability EI is explained by general intelligence. The

GFP, as measured by the MPQ-BF, and trait EI are best described as unrelated constructs that do

not share or explain variance within one another. However, the radically different natures of

GFPs obtained different personality measures call into question whether it is even meaningful to

posit relationships between the GFP and trait EI measures like the TEIQue. Indeed, the term GFP

shares similar issues with the term EI; it is a moniker shared by many different constructs and is

dependent on the tool of measurement.

Page 46: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

40

APPENDIX A: R SYNTAX

############ # Figure 1 # ############ # Hierarchical MSCEIT model <- ' # latent variable definitions #Perceiving =~ Faces + Pictures #Understanding =~ Changes + Blends Perceiving =~ Faces + equal("Perceiving=~Faces")*Pictures Understanding =~ Changes + equal("Understanding=~Changes")*Blends Managing =~ Emotion_Management + Social_Management + Facilitation + Sensations AEI =~ Managing + Perceiving + Understanding ' library(semPlot) fit <- sem(model, data=st025_MSCEITQs, std.lv=TRUE) semPaths(fit, whatLabels = "std", layout = 'tree3', edge.label.cex=1, title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE) fitmeasures(fit) # Bifactor MSCEIT model <- ' # latent variable definitions Perceiving =~ Faces + equal("Perceiving=~Faces")*Pictures Understanding =~ Changes + equal("Understanding=~Changes")*Blends Managing =~ Emotion_Management + Social_Management + Facilitation + Sensations AEI =~ Faces + Pictures + Changes + Blends + Emotion_Management + Social_Management + Facilitation + Sensations ' fit <- sem(model, data=st025_MSCEITQs, orthogonal=TRUE, std.lv=TRUE) semPaths(fit, layout = 'tree3', edge.label.cex=1, bifactor = 'AEI', title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE, exoCov = FALSE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE) fitmeasures(fit) corr.test(subset(st025_MSCEITQs, select= Faces:Social_Management)) describe(subset(st025_MSCEITQs, select=c(Emotion_Management,Social_Management,Facilitation,Sensations))) subset(st025_MSCEITQs, FirstName == 1183, select=c(Emotion_Management,Social_Management,Facilitation,Sensations)) ############ # Figure 2 # ############

Page 47: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

41

# Hierarchical TEIQue model <- ' # latent variable definitions WellBeing =~ Optimism + Happiness + SelfEsteem Sociability =~ EmotionManagement + Assertiveness + SocialAwareness Emotionality =~ Empathy + EmotionPerceptionIndex + EmotionExpression + Relationships SelfControl =~ EmotionRegulation + ImpulseControl + StressManagement EI =~ WellBeing + Sociability + Emotionality + SelfControl + Motivation + Adaptability ' library(semPlot) fit <- sem(model, data=TEIQue, std.lv=TRUE) semPaths(fit, whatLabels = "std", layout = 'tree3', edge.label.cex=1, title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE) fitmeasures(fit) # Bifcator TEIQue model <- ' # latent variable definitions WellBeing =~ Optimism + Happiness + SelfEsteem Sociability =~ EmotionManagement + Assertiveness + SocialAwareness Emotionality =~ Empathy + EmotionPerceptionIndex + EmotionExpression + Relationships SelfControl =~ EmotionRegulation + ImpulseControl + StressManagement EI =~ Optimism + Happiness + SelfEsteem + EmotionManagement + Assertiveness + SocialAwareness + Empathy + EmotionPerceptionIndex + EmotionExpression + Relationships + EmotionRegulation + ImpulseControl + StressManagement + Motivation + Adaptability #Fix negative variance issues test #EmotionRegulation ~~ 0*EmotionRegulation #Happiness ~~ 0*Happiness ' library(semPlot) fit <- sem(model, data=TEIQue, orthogonal=TRUE, std.lv=TRUE) semPaths(fit, whatLabels = "std", edge.label.cex=1, layout = 'tree2', bifactor = 'EI', title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE, exoCov = FALSE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE) fitmeasures(fit) ############ # Figure 3 # ############ # Hierarchical IQ model <- ' # latent variable definitions Gf =~ ThreeD_Rotation + Matrix_Reasoning + LNS

Page 48: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

42

Gc =~ GSCI + equal("Gc=~GSCI")*Shipley IQ =~ Gf + Gc ' fit <- sem(model, data=Total_IQ, std.lv=TRUE) semPaths(fit, whatLabels = "std", layout = 'tree3', edge.label.cex=1, title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE) fitmeasures(fit) # Bifactor IQ model <- ' # latent variable definitions Gf =~ ThreeD_Rotation + Matrix_Reasoning + LNS Gc =~ GSCI + equal("Gc=~GSCI")*Shipley IQ =~ ThreeD_Rotation + Matrix_Reasoning + LNS + GSCI + Shipley ' fit <- sem(model, data=Total_IQ, orthogonal=TRUE, std.lv=TRUE) semPaths(fit, layout = 'tree3', edge.label.cex=1, bifactor = 'IQ', whatLabels = "std", title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE, exoCov = FALSE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE) fitmeasures(fit) ############ # Figure 4 # ############ # Hierarchical MPQ model <- ' # latent variable definitions PEM =~ Well_Being + Social_Potency + Achievement + Social_Closeness + Absorption NEM =~ Stress_Reaction + Alienation + Aggression + Absorption CON =~ Control + Harm_Avoidance + Traditionalism GFP =~ PEM + NEM + CON ' fit <- sem(model, data=MPQfullscales) semPaths(fit, whatLabels = "std", edge.label.cex=1, layout = 'tree3', title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE) fitmeasures(fit) # Bifactor MPQ model <- ' # latent variable definitions GFP =~ Well_Being + Social_Potency + Achievement + Social_Closeness + Absorption + Stress_Reaction + Alienation + Aggression + Control + Harm_Avoidance + Traditionalism PEM =~ Well_Being + Social_Potency + Achievement + Social_Closeness + Absorption NEM =~ Stress_Reaction + Alienation + Aggression + Absorption CON =~ Control + Harm_Avoidance + Traditionalism

Page 49: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

43

#Fix traditionalism to 0 due to non-significant negative variance Traditionalism ~~ 0*Traditionalism ' fit <- sem(model, data=MPQfullscales, orthogonal=TRUE) semPaths(fit, whatLabels = "std", layout = 'tree3', edge.label.cex=1, bifactor = 'GFP', title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE, exoCov = FALSE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE, modindices = TRUE) fitmeasures(fit) ############ # Figure 5 # ############ #Ability EI as a Second-Order Factor model <- ' # latent variable definitions Gf =~ ThreeD_Rotation + Matrix_Reasoning + LNS Gc =~ GSCI + equal("Gc=~GSCI")*Shipley AEI =~ Perceiving + Understanding + Managing IQ =~ Gf + equal("IQ=~Gf")*Gc + AEI ' fit <- sem(model, data=GMSCEIT, std.lv=TRUE) semPaths(fit, whatLabels = "std", layout = 'tree3', edge.label.cex=1, title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE) fitmeasures(fit) # Ability EI as a Separate Factor model <- ' # latent variable definitions Gf =~ ThreeD_Rotation + Matrix_Reasoning + LNS Gc =~ GSCI + equal("Gc=~GSCI")*Shipley IQ =~ Gf + equal("IQ=~Gf")*Gc AEI =~ Perceiving + Understanding + Managing IQ ~~ AEI ' fit <- sem(model, data=GMSCEIT, std.lv=TRUE) semPaths(fit, whatLabels = "std", layout = 'tree', edge.label.cex=1, title = FALSE, curvePivot = TRUE) #Model Summary Statistics summary(fit, standardized=TRUE) fitmeasures(fit)

Page 50: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

44

APPENDIX B: GSCI ASSESMENT

Instructions Each passage has four underlined portions. These underlined portions may or may not contain an error (there is only one possible error per question). If you believe the first underlined portion has an error, then answer "1st blank". If the second underlined portion contains an error, then answer "2nd blank", etc. If you do not believe there are any errors in a passage, then answer "No error".

Q1 The otter, blue jay, and weasel were walking among the river when they saw the kitten sitting underneath the tree.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q2 The kitten stood up and became excited when his friends came towards him, and he thought that he could finally catch the dastardly beetle.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 51: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

45

Q3 The kitten and his friends strategized a way that they could trap the beetle using sticks and tree leafs. To their displeasure, they were not successful.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q4 The woman, dressed in all black clothing, was mourning the loss of her husband on a cold and rainy friday night.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q5 An extraordinary discovery was made by accident when the chemist made a mistake and dropped a glass flask coated in plastic cellulose nitrate.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 52: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

46

Q6 Nina Simone was not only a well known artist but an activist for civil rights during the 1950s and 1960s.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q7 In 2018 the sleek modern hotel tower replaced the old and dusty brown casino that overlooked the Las Vegas Strip.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 53: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

47

Q8 Neither the hockey announcer or the referee could be heard over the angry shouting of the team’s fans in the arena.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q9 The brand new parking garage was convenient but over 300 residents were forced to move when they demolished the old apartments.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q10 The poet admired the woman from afar and was inspired to write when they felt the pain of unrequited love.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 54: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

48

Q11 Just between you and I, there is something wrong with our boss. She looks tired and yells a lot now.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q12 I felt like I needed a change. I wanted to shake it up, to get a thrill which only comes from something different.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q13 If only he were able to feel love, compassion, or remorse. Any of these emotions would make him seem human.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 55: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

49

Q14 If I had a dollar for every silly thing I heard or read, I would be rich like you, too.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q15 This town has such a low crime rate that, for all intense and purposes, one could bike to a convenience store in the middle of the night and not worry.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 56: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

50

Q16 The three best things about going to Costa Rica was the hikes, the hidden hot springs, and the food (of course).

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q17 No matter how many points your team is currently losing by, you must give your best effort because you never know how it will effect the game.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q18 The student ran home to show his parents his test score. Everyone in his family was proud of how good he had done on the exam.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 57: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

51

Q19 The coach guaranteed without a doubt that his team would eradicate the competition. He believed that they were in a league of their own.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q20 There within the large house, their hearts beat furiously. Although afraid, they’re the ones who had the best chance of completing the job.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q21 The director stated that the reason the series is so successful have been the people who work hard on set everyday.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 58: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

52

Q22 While mopping the floor after a fast-paced basketball game, the janitor found a smelly player’s sock.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q23 When the little girl brought home her straight “A” report card to her mother, she was so proud.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q24 Leading voices of the robotics division are focusing on a positive rebranding of artificial intelligence in response to public concern of the possibility of a robot uprising.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 59: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

53

Q25 What we consider “good, better, or best” is relative to the available options. Sometimes even the best option is unfavorable.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q26 The store had everything; books, shoes, candles, toys, etc. We all loved to wait outside before the store opened to get the best merchandise.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 60: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

54

Q27 The continuous thunder shook the entire neighborhood; my friends and I wondered if the dogs would ever stop barking.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q28 The weight of a thousand concerns stooped his shoulders. He could not bare to think about how he would have to address them.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Q29 Although a lot of people are terrified of anacondas for their menacingly large bodies, these reptile are actually known to be one of the least dangerous snakes.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error

Page 61: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

55

Q30 After having to tie my shoe, I noticed that it was quickly becoming dark. The only ones who hadn’t made it back to the base were Miranda and me.

o 1st blank

o 2nd blank

o 3rd blank

o 4th blank

o No error End of Block: Default Question Block

Questions retained and used in study: 1,4,6,7,8,11,16,18,28

0 5 10 15 20 25 30

01

23

Parallel Analysis Scree Plots

Factor/Component Number

eige

nval

ues

of p

rinci

pal c

ompo

nent

s an

d fa

ctor

ana

lysi

s

PC Actual Data

PC Simulated Data

PC Resampled Data

FA Actual Data

FA Simulated Data

FA Resampled Data

Page 62: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

56

APPENDIX C: ADDITIONAL FIT STATISTICS

χ2 and CFI of Each Model

Model χ2 p-value CFI MSCEIT

Hierarchical 28.323 .077 .948

Bifactor N/A N/A N/A TEIQue Hierarchical 348.060 <.001 .834

Bifactor 276.274 .<.001 .874 Intelligence

Hierarchical 5.044 .283 .991

Bifactor 2.355 .125 .988 MPQ-BF

Hierarchical 109.896 <.001 .815

Bifactor 77.888 <.001 .881 Intelligence and MSCEIT Separate Factors 67.981 <.001 .904 Second Order-Factor 67.981 <.001 .904 MPQ-BF and TEIQue Separate Factors N/A N/A N/A Second-Order Factor N/A N/A N/A

Page 63: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

57

REFERENCES

Andrei, F., Siegling, A. B., Aloe, A. M., Baldaro, B., & Petrides, K. V. (2016). The incremental

validity of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue): A systematic review

and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality Assessment, 98(3), 261–276.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2015.1084630

Arias, V. B., Jenaro, C., & Ponce, F. P. (2018). Testing the generality of the general factor of

personality: An exploratory bifactor approach. Personality and Individual Differences,

129, 17–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.02.042

Barbey, A. K. (2018). Network neuroscience theory of human intelligence. Trends in Cognitive

Sciences, 22(1), 8–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.10.001

Beaujean, A. (2015). John Carroll’s Views on Intelligence: Bi-Factor vs. Higher-Order Models.

Journal of Intelligence, 3, 121–136. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence3040121

Beaujean, A., Parkin, J., & Parker, S. (2014). Comparing Cattell–Horn–Carroll factor models:

Differences between bifactor and higher order factor models in predicting language

achievement. Psychological Assessment, 26(3), 789–805.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036745

Benjamin, D. J., Berger, J. O., Johannesson, M., Nosek, B. A., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Berk, R.,

Bollen, K. A., Brembs, B., Brown, L., Camerer, C., Cesarini, D., Chambers, C. D., Clyde,

M., Cook, T. D., Boeck, P. D., Dienes, Z., Dreber, A., Easwaran, K., Efferson, C., …

Johnson, V. E. (2018). Redefine statistical significance. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(1),

6–10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0189-z

Benning, S. D., & Freeman, A. J. (2017). The Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire’s

inconsistency scales identify invalid profiles through internal statistics and external

Page 64: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

58

correlates. Psychological Assessment, 29(12), 1531–1536.

https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000452

Blonigen, D. M., Bui, L., Britt, J. Y., Thomas, K. M., & Timko, C. (2016). Internalizing and

externalizing personality subtypes predict differences in functioning and outcomes

among veterans in residential substance use disorder treatment. Psychological

Assessment, 28(10), 1186–1197. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000250

Boyatzis, R. E. (2009). Competencies as a behavioral approach to emotional intelligence.

Journal of Management Development, 28(9), 749–770.

https://doi.org/10.1108/02621710910987647

Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent, Discriminant, and Incremental Validity of

Competing Measures of Emotional Intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 29(9), 1147–1158. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203254596

Brannick, M. T., Wahi, M. M., Arce, M., Johnson, H.-A., Nazian, S., & Goldin, S. B. (2009).

Comparison of trait and ability measures of emotional intelligence in medical students.

Medical Education, 43(11), 1062–1068. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-

2923.2009.03430.x

Brannick, M. T., Wahi, M. M., & Goldin, S. B. (2011). Psychometrics of Mayer-Salovey-Caruso

Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) scores. Psychological Reports, 109(1), 327–337.

https://doi.org/10.2466/03.04.PR0.109.4.327-337

Canivez, G. L., Watkins, M. W., Good, R., James, K., & James, T. (2017). Construct validity of

the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Fourth UK Edition with a referred Irish

sample: Wechsler and Cattell-Horn-Carroll model comparisons with 15 subtests. The

Page 65: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

59

British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(3), 383–407.

https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12155

Cejudo, J. (2016). Relationship between emotional intelligence and mental health in school

counselors. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 14.

https://doi.org/10.14204/ejrep.38.15025

Cherniss, C. (2010). Emotional intelligence: Toward clarification of a concept. Industrial and

Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 3(2), 110–126.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1754-9434.2010.01231.x

Coetzee, M., & Harry, N. (2014). Emotional intelligence as a predictor of employees’ career

adaptability. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 84(1), 90–97.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2013.09.001

Condon, D. M., & Revelle, W. (2014). The international cognitive ability resource: Development

and initial validation of a public-domain measure. Intelligence, 43, 52–64.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.004

Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-

Factory Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional manual. Psychological Assessment

Resources.

Davies, S. E., Connelly, B. S., Ones, D. S., & Birkland, A. S. (2015). The General Factor of

Personality: The “Big One,” a self-evaluative trait, or a methodological gnat that won’t

go away? Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 13–22.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.01.006

De Leng, W. E., Stegers-Jager, K. M., Husbands, A., Dowell, J. S., Born, M. Ph., & Themmen,

A. P. N. (2017). Scoring method of a Situational Judgment Test: Influence on internal

Page 66: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

60

consistency reliability, adverse impact and correlation with personality? Advances in

Health Sciences Education, 22(2), 243–265. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-016-9720-7

DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2002). Higher-order factors of the Big Five

predict conformity: Are there neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differences,

33(4), 533–552. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00171-4

DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., Séguin, J. R., & Tremblay, R. E. (2008). Externalizing behavior

and the higher order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(4),

947–953. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013742

Di Fabio, A., & Saklofske, D. H. (2014). Promoting individual resources: The challenge of trait

emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 65, 19–23.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.026

Donnellan, M. B., Hopwood, C. J., & Wright, A. G. C. (2012). Revaluating the evidence for the

General Factor of Personality in the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire:

Concerns about Rushton and Irwing (2009). Personality and Individual Differences,

52(3), 285–289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.018

Dunkel, C. S., & van der Linden, D. (2017). The general factor of personality and character: A

reanalysis. The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human

Development, 178(6), 334–338. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2017.1381070

Dunkel, C. S., van der Linden, D., Brown, N. A., & Mathes, E. W. (2016). Self-report based

General Factor of Personality as socially-desirable responding, positive self-evaluation,

and social-effectiveness. Personality and Individual Differences, 92, 143–147.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.12.034

Page 67: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

61

Eigenhuis, A., Kamphuis, J. H., & Noordhof, A. (2017). Personality in general and clinical

samples: Measurement invariance of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire.

Psychological Assessment, 29(9), 1111–1119. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000408

Evans, T. R., Hughes, D. J., & Steptoe-Warren, G. (2020). A conceptual replication of emotional

intelligence as a second-stratum factor of intelligence. Emotion (Washington, D.C.),

20(3), 507–512. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000569

Fan, H., Jackson, T., Yang, X., Tang, W., & Zhang, J. (2010). The factor structure of the Mayer–

Salovey–Caruso emotional intelligence test V 2.0 (MSCEIT): A meta-analytic structural

equation modeling approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(7), 781–785.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.004

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical

power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior

Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146

Ferguson, F. J., & Austin, E. J. (2011). The factor structures of the STEM and the STEU.

Personality and Individual Differences, 51(6), 791–794.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.07.002

Fernandez, R., Salamonson, Y., & Griffiths, R. (2012). Emotional intelligence as a predictor of

academic performance in first‐year accelerated graduate entry nursing students. Journal

of Clinical Nursing, 21(23–24), 3485–3492. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-

2702.2012.04199.x

Furnham, A., & Christoforou, I. (2007). Personality traits, emotional intelligence and multiple

happiness. North American Journal of Psychology, 9(3), 439–462.

Page 68: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

62

Furnham, A., & Petrides, K. V. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence and happiness. Social

Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 31(8), 815–824.

https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2003.31.8.815

Gardner, K. J., & Qualter, P. (2011). Factor structure, measurement invariance and structural

invariance of the MSCEIT v2.0. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(4), 492–496.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.004

Gignac, G. E. (2008). Higher-Order Models versus Direct Hierarchical Models: G as

Superordinate or Breadth Factor? Psychology Science, 50(1), 21.

Gignac, G. E. (2016). The higher-order model imposes a proportionality constraint: That is why

the bifactor model tends to fit better. Intelligence, 55, 57–68.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.01.006

Gignac, G. E., & Szodorai, E. T. (2016). Effect size guidelines for individual differences

researchers. Personality and Individual Differences, 102, 74–78.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.069

Gonzalez, O., MacKinnon, D. P., & Muniz, F. B. (2020). Extrinsic convergent validity evidence

to prevent jingle and jangle fallacies. Multivariate Behavioral Research.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2019.1707061

Hopwood, C. J., & Grilo, C. M. (2010). Internalizing and externalizing personality dimensions

and clinical problems in adolescents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 41(4),

398–408. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-010-0175-4

Hopwood, C. J., Wright, A. G. C., & Brent Donnellan, M. (2011). Evaluating the evidence for

the general factor of personality across multiple inventories. Journal of Research in

Personality, 45(5), 468–478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.06.002

Page 69: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

63

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:

Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A

Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118

Hülür, G., Gasimova, F., Robitzsch, A., & Wilhelm, O. (2018). Change in fluid and crystallized

intelligence and student achievement: The role of intellectual engagement. Child

Development, 89(4), 1074–1087. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12791

Jolić-Marjanović, Z., & Altaras-Dimitrijević, A. (2014). Reliability, construct and criterion-

related validity of the Serbian adaptation of the Trait Emotional Intelligence

Questionnaire (TEIQue). Psihologija, 47(2), 249–262.

https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI1402249J

Karim, J., & Weisz, R. (2010). Cross-cultural research on the reliability and validity of the

Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). Cross-Cultural

Research: The Journal of Comparative Social Science, 44(4), 374–404.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397110377603

Killgore, W. D. S., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2007). Neural correlates of emotional intelligence

in adolescent children. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 140–151.

Kong, D. T. (2014). Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT/MEIS) and

overall, verbal, and nonverbal intelligence: Meta-analytic evidence and critical

contingencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 66, 171–175.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.03.028

Kranzler, J. H., Benson, N., & Floyd, R. G. (2015). Using estimated factor scores from a bifactor

analysis to examine the unique effects of the latent variables measured by the WAIS-IV

Page 70: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

64

on academic achievement. Psychological Assessment, 27(4), 1402–1416.

https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000119

Kunnanatt, J. T. (2008). Emotional intelligence: Theory and description: A competency model

for interpersonal effectiveness. Career Development International, 13(7), 614–629.

https://doi.org/10.1108/13620430810911083

Lanciano, T., & Curci, A. (2014). Incremental validity of emotional intelligence ability in

predicting academic achievement. The American Journal of Psychology, 127(4), 447–

461. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.127.4.0447

Libbrecht, N., & Lievens, F. (2012). Validity evidence for the situational judgment test paradigm

in emotional intelligence measurement. International Journal of Psychology, 47(6), 438–

447. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.682063

Little, T. D., Lindenberger, U., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1999). On selecting indicators for

multivariate measurement and modeling with latent variables: When “good” indicators

are bad and “bad” indicators are good. Psychological Methods, 4(2), 192–211.

https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.4.2.192

Liu, D., & Campbell, W. K. (2017). The Big Five personality traits, Big Two metatraits and

social media: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 70, 229–240.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2017.08.004

Loehlin, J. C. (2012). How general across inventories is a general factor of personality? Journal

of Research in Personality, 46(3), 258–263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.02.003

Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., & Straus, R. (2003). Emotional intelligence, personality, and the

perceived quality of social relationships. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(3),

641–658. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00242-8

Page 71: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

65

MacCann, C., Joseph, D. L., Newman, D. A., & Roberts, R. D. (2014). Emotional intelligence is

a second-stratum factor of intelligence: Evidence from hierarchical and bifactor models.

Emotion, 14(2), 358–374. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034755

MacCann, C., Pearce, N., & Roberts, R. D. (2011). Emotional intelligence as assessed by

situational judgment and emotion recognition tests: Building the nomological net.

Psihologijske Teme, 20(3), 393–412.

Martins, A., Ramalho, N., & Morin, E. (2010). A comprehensive meta-analysis of the

relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Personality and Individual

Differences, 49(6), 554–564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.029

Maul, A. (2012a). Examining the structure of emotional intelligence at the item level: New

perspectives, new conclusions. Cognition and Emotion, 26(3), 503–520.

https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.588690

Maul, A. (2012b). The validity of the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test

(MSCEIT) as a measure of emotional intelligence. Emotion Review, 4(4), 394–402.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912445811

Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., & Salovey, P. (2016). The ability model of emotional intelligence:

Principles and updates. Emotion Review, 8(4), 290–300.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916639667

Mayer, J. D., DiPaolo, M., & Salovey, P. (1990). Perceiving affective content in ambiguous

visual stimuli: A component of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 54(3–4), 772–781. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5403&4_29

Page 72: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

66

Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey, D. J. Sluyter,

P. Salovey (Ed), & D. J. Sluyter (Ed) (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional

intelligence: Educational implications. (1997-08644-001; pp. 3–34). Basic Books.

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). The Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional

Intelligence Test (MSCEIT); User’s Manual. Multi-Health Systems.

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2008). Emotional intelligence: New ability or

eclectic traits? American Psychologist, 63(6), 503–517. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-

066X.63.6.503

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., & Sitarenios, G. (2003). Measuring emotional

intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 3(1), 97–105.

McGrew, K. S. (2009). CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the

shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence, 37(1), 1–10.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.08.004

Megías, A., Gómez-Leal, R., Gutiérrez-Cobo, M. J., Cabello, R., & Fernández-Berrocal, P.

(2018). The relationship between aggression and ability emotional intelligence: The role

of negative affect. Psychiatry Research, 270, 1074–1081.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.05.027

Mikolajczak, M., Nelis, D., Hansenne, M., & Quoidbach, J. (2008). If you can regulate sadness,

you can probably regulate shame: Associations between trait emotional intelligence,

emotion regulation and coping efficiency across discrete emotions. Personality and

Individual Differences, 44(6), 1356–1368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.004

Miller, J. D., Gaughan, E. T., Maples, J., & Price, J. (2011). A comparison of Agreeableness

scores from the Big Five Inventory and the NEO PI-R: Consequences for the study of

Page 73: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

67

Narcissism and Psychopathy. Assessment, 18(3), 335–339.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111411671

Morgan, G. B., Hodge, K. J., Wells, K. E., & Watkins, M. W. (2015). Are Fit Indices Biased in

Favor of Bi-Factor Models in Cognitive Ability Research?: A Comparison of Fit in

Correlated Factors, Higher-Order, and Bi-Factor Models via Monte Carlo Simulations.

Journal of Intelligence, 3(1), 2–20. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence3010002

Murray, A. L., & Johnson, W. (2013). The limitations of model fit in comparing the bi-factor

versus higher-order models of human cognitive ability structure. Intelligence, 41(5), 407–

422. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.004

Musek, J. (2007). A general factor of personality: Evidence for the Big One in the five-factor

model. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(6), 1213–1233.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.02.003

Pardeller, S., Frajo-Apor, B., Kemmler, G., & Hofer, A. (2017). Emotional Intelligence and

cognitive abilities—Associations and sex differences. Psychology, Health & Medicine,

22(8), 1001–1010. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2016.1255766

Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J., & Tellegen, A. (2002). Development and validation of a brief form of

the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 14(2), 150–

163. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.14.2.150

Patrick, C., & Kramer, M. (2017). Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire.

Petrides, K. V. (2009). Psychometric properties of the Trait Emotional Intelligence

Questionnaire (TEIQue). In C. Stough, D. H. Saklofske, J. D. A. Parker, C. Stough (Ed),

D. H. Saklofske (Ed), & J. D. A. Parker (Ed) (Eds.), Assessing emotional intelligence:

Page 74: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

68

Theory, research, and applications. (2009-08605-005; pp. 85–101). Springer Science +

Business Media. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88370-0_5

Petrides, K. V. (2011). Ability and trait emotional intelligence. In T. Chamorro-Premuzic, S. von

Stumm, A. Furnham, T. Chamorro-Premuzic (Ed), S. von Stumm (Ed), & A. Furnham

(Ed) (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of individual differences. (2011-23563-025;

pp. 656–678). Wiley-Blackwell.

Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric investigation

with reference to established trait taxonomies. European Journal of Personality, 15(6),

425–448. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.416

Petrides, K. V., Pérez-González, J. C., & Furnham, A. (2007). On the criterion and incremental

validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cognition and Emotion, 21(1), 26–55.

https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930601038912

Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The location of trait emotional intelligence in

personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 273–289.

https://doi.org/10.1348/000712606X120618

Pisner, D. A., Smith, R., Alkozei, A., Klimova, A., & Killgore, W. D. S. (2017). Highways of the

emotional intellect: White matter microstructural correlates of an ability-based measure

of emotional intelligence. Social Neuroscience, 12(3), 253–267.

https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1176600

Preacher, K. J., Zhang, G., Kim, C., & Mels, G. (2013). Choosing the Optimal Number of

Factors in Exploratory Factor Analysis: A Model Selection Perspective. Multivariate

Behavioral Research, 48(1), 28–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2012.710386

Page 75: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

69

Reynolds, M. R., & Keith, T. Z. (2017). Multi-group and hierarchical confirmatory factor

analysis of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fifth edition: What does it

measure? Intelligence, 62, 31–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2017.02.005

Roberts, R. D., Zeidner, M., & Matthews, G. (2001). Does emotional intelligence meet

traditional standards for an intelligence? Some new data and conclusions. Emotion, 1(3),

196–231. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.3.196

Rossen, E., Kranzler, J. H., & Algina, J. (2008). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Mayer—

Salovey—Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test V 2.0 (MSCEIT). Personality and

Individual Differences, 44(5), 1258–1269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.020

Rushton, J. P., Bons, T. A., Ando, J., Hur, Y.-M., Irwing, P., Vernon, P. A., Petrides, K. V., &

Barbaranelli, C. (2009). A general factor of personality from multitrait-multimethod data

and cross-national twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics: The Official Journal of

the International Society for Twin Studies, 12(4), 356–365.

https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.12.4.356

Salovey, P., & Grewal, D. (2005). The Science of Emotional Intelligence. Current Directions in

Psychological Science, 14(6), 281–285. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-

7214.2005.00381.x

Sánchez-Álvarez, N., Extremera, N., & Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2016). The relation between

emotional intelligence and subjective well-being: A meta-analytic investigation. The

Journal of Positive Psychology, 11(3), 276–285.

https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2015.1058968

Page 76: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

70

Sato, T. (2005). The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Brief Version: Factor Structure and

Reliability. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 139(6), 545–552.

https://doi.org/10.3200/JRLP.139.6.545-552

Schermer, J. A., & Holden, R. R. (2019). Personality facet loadings onto the general factor of

personality change when social desirability responding is considered. Personality and

Individual Differences, 147, 200–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.038

Schult, J., & Sparfeldt, J. R. (2016). Do non-g factors of cognitive ability tests align with specific

academic achievements? A combined bifactor modeling approach. Intelligence, 59, 96–

102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.08.004

Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., &

Dornheim, L. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional

intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25(2), 167–177.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00001-4

Shipley, W. C. (1940). A self-administering scale for measuring intellectual impairment and

deterioration. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 9, 371–377.

https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1940.9917704

Takaiwa, A., Kuwayama, N., Akioka, N., Kashiwazaki, D., & Kuroda, S. (2018). Discrepancy

analysis between crystallized and fluid intelligence tests: A novel method to detect mild

cognitive impairment in patients with asymptomatic carotid artery stenosis. European

Journal of Neurology, 25(2), 313–319. https://doi.org/10.1111/ene.13504

Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (2008). Exploring personality through test construction:

Development of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. In G. J. Boyle, G.

Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality theory and

Page 77: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

71

assessment, Vol 2: Personality measurement and testing. (2008-14475-013; pp. 261–

292). Sage Publications, Inc. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781849200479.n13

Urquijo, I., Extremera, N., & Villa, A. (2016). Emotional intelligence, life satisfaction, and

psychological well-being in graduates: The mediating effect of perceived stress. Applied

Research in Quality of Life, 11(4), 1241–1252. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-015-9432-

9

van der Linden, D., Dunkel, C. S., & Petrides, K. V. (2016). The General Factor of Personality

(GFP) as social effectiveness: Review of the literature. Personality and Individual

Differences, 101, 98–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.05.020

van der Linden, D., Pekaar, K. A., Bakker, A. B., Schermer, J. A., Vernon, P. A., Dunkel, C. S.,

& Petrides, K. V. (2017). Overlap between the general factor of personality and

emotional intelligence: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 143(1), 36–52.

https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000078

van der Linden, D., te Nijenhuis, J., & Bakker, A. B. (2010). The General Factor of Personality:

A meta-analysis of Big Five intercorrelations and a criterion-related validity study.

Journal of Research in Personality, 44(3), 315–327.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.03.003

Van der Linden, D., Tsaousis, I., & Petrides, K. v. (2012). Overlap between General Factors of

Personality in the Big Five, Giant Three, and trait emotional intelligence. Personality and

Individual Differences, 53(3), 175–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.03.001

Van Zyl, C. J. J. (2016). Measurement invariance across gender and ethnicity on the Emotional

Quotient Inventory 2.0. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 26(2), 141–148.

Page 78: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

72

van Zyl, C. J. J., & de Bruin, K. (2012). The relationship between mixed model emotional

intelligence and personality. South African Journal of Psychology, 42(4), 532–542.

Whitman, D. S., Van Rooy, D. L., Viswesvaran, C., & Alonso, A. (2008). The susceptibility of a

mixed model measure of emotional intelligence to faking: A Solomon four-group design.

Psychology Science, 50(1), 44–63.

Page 79: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

73

Doctoral Intern August 2019 – August 2020

Pre-Doctoral Practicum Assessment and Therapy Trainee August 2018 – May 2019

Page 80: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

74

Pre-Doctoral Practicum Therapy and Assessment Trainee August 2017 – August 2018

Clinical Doctoral Student Graduate Counselor August 2016 – May 2017 Counseling and Psychological Services at the University of Nevada, Las Vegas Therapy and Intake Supervisors: James Jobe, Ph.D.; Alexandria Moorer, M.A. • 9-month practicum at a university counseling center. A weekly caseload of approximately six

therapy clients and one intake client was maintained. Weekly individual supervision, group supervision, and multidisciplinary case rounds were conducted

• Focused on exploring and implementing cultural development models, developing an acceptance and commitment therapy orientation, treatment in a brief-intervention model, and working in an integrative healthcare institution

• Worked with culturally and economically diverse undergraduate and graduate students who presented with depressive symptoms, anxious symptoms, suicidality, relationship concerns, adjustment disorders, and academic concerns

Clinical Doctoral Student Graduate Clinician August 2015 – August 2016; The PRACTICE Clinic University of Nevada, Las Vegas August 2017 – May 2018

Page 81: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

75

Therapy Supervisors: Kristin Culbert, Ph.D.; Katherine Isaza, Psy.D. Assessment Supervisors: Stephen Benning, Ph.D.; Michelle Paul, Ph.D. • 12-month practicum placement that provides low cost mental health therapy and assessment

services to the university population and community at large. A weekly caseload of approximately five therapy clients and one assessment client is maintained, with weekly individual and group supervision. Intakes were conducted on a biweekly basis

• Explored different theoretical orientations, advocated for underserved and underrepresented populations, researched considerations when working with clients of minority status, and facilitated group therapy

• Served a wide variety of clients as a result of sliding-scale fees; clients consisted of adults who were diverse in socioeconomic status, education, and ethnicity

• • •

• •

Page 82: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

76

Interprofessional Education Day March 2014, March 2015, March 2016 University of Nevada, Las Vegas • Engaged in a professional training aimed to improve client experience by increasing the

cohesiveness and collaboration between healthcare professionals within an integrated healthcare model

• Co-created and presented a lecture regarding the role of clinical psychology within an integrated healthcare model

• Collaborated with seven healthcare graduate degree programs from the University of Nevada, Las Vegas (UNLV) participated in a series of group learning projects, simulations, and mock treatment planning

• Enrolled in a semester-long course (Integrated Behavioral Health Care) to develop and reinforce skills critical to the collaboration and consultation of healthcare professionals

The Counseling and Testing Center, University of Idaho August 2019 – December 2019

• Supervised a graduate student in their fifth year of practicum in the clinical psychology Ph.D. program at Washington State University

• Facilitated the skill development of supervisee professional identity, navigation of ethical concerns, risk management, documentation, and preparation for internship.

• Received group supervision of supervision with the training director of the Counseling and Testing Center and fellow doctoral interns.

The PRACTICE Clinic University of Nevada, Las Vegas • Supervised a graduate student who just completed their first year of practicum in the clinical

psychology Ph.D. program at UNLV • Guided supervisee in regards to growth edges, intervention planning, assisting in case

conceptualizations, exploring multicultural implications, managing client risk, discussing ethical concerns, and defining supervisee supervision goals

• Received supervision of supervision with the training director of the Practice and was concurrently enrolled in the course “Introduction to Supervision.”

• • •

Page 83: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

77

• •

o o

The PRACTICE Clinic University of Nevada, Las Vegas

Page 84: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

78

Instructor August 2016 – May 2017 University of Nevada, Las Vegas August 2018 – Present Course: General Psychology Created an introductory course for undergraduate students. Solely responsible for course content including lecture material, in-class activities, assignments, and exams. Classes operate through open discussions based on lecture material, different values and beliefs held by students are encouraged to be shared.

Page 85: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

79

Page 86: The Factor Structures of Ability and Trait Emotional

80

• • • •

• • • •