the central nucleus of the amygdala contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: gp, globus...

15
The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1995, 15( 12): 8199-8213 The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the Production of Morphine Antinociception in the Rat Tail-Flick Test Barton H. Manning’ and David J. Mayer2 DeDat-tments of ‘Anatomv and 2Anesthesioloav. Medical College of Virginia, Virginia Commonwealth University, Ridhmond, Virginia 23298 Current models of endogenous pain control circuitry em- phasize neural substrates within the brainstem and spinal cord. We have recently shown, however, that the central nucleus of the amygdala (Ce) contributes to morphine-in- duced suppression of formalin-induced nociceptive behav- iors. In the four experiments reported here, we investigated the possibility that the Ce also contributes to morphine- induced suppression of simple, spinally mediated nocicep tive reflexes. Bilateral Kmethyl-D-aspartate (NMDA)-in- duced lesions of the rat Ce, but not bilateral lesions cen- tered on either the basolateral or medial amygdaloid nucle- us, abolished the antinociception produced by 2.5 mg/kg morphine sulfate in the noxious heat-evoked tail-flick test. Bilateral Ce lesions also abolished the antinociception pro- duced by 2 or 4 mg/kg morphine sulfate, but a relatively large dose of morphine sulfate (10 mglkg, s.c.) resulted in partial reinstatement of antinociception. It is unlikely that these effects were due to secondary, seizure-induced dam- age following NMDA injection (e.g., to areas outside the amygdala) since bilateral inactivation of the Ce with the lo- cal anesthetic lidocaine also reliably attenuated morphine antinociception. It is also unlikely that these effects were artifacts of lesion-induced hyperalgesia, since Ce lesions failed to result in reliable thermal hyperalgesia, even at baseline tail-flick latencies of lo-12 sec. These data are the first to provide direct evidence that systemically adminis- tered morphine requires the integrity of a forebrain area in order to suppress spinally mediated nociceptive reflexes. It is argued that the present results, together with recent evidence linking the Ce to the production of several forms of conditioned and unconditioned environmentally induced antinociception, warrant incorporation of the Ce into cur- rent models of endogenous pain control circuitry. [Key words: pain, antinociception, morphine, tail flick, amygdala, central nucleus, lesion, NMDA, lidocaine] Morphine remainsone of the most powerful analgesic agents. The CNS substrates underlying morphine’santinociceptive ef- fects have been extensively studied in recent years. Although Received May 4, 1995; revised Aug. 11, 1995; accepted Aug. 17, 1995. This research was supported by PHS Grant NS-24009. We thank Leslie Ken- iston and Milton Cook for assistance with animal testing, histology, and the preparation of figures. We also thank Drs. J. L. Dupree and J. W. Bigbee for assistance with photography, and Drs. J. Mao, D. D. Price and S. P. Welch for helpful comments and advice. Correspondence should be addressed to Barton H. Manning, Department of Anatomy, Virginia Commonwealth University, Box 980709, Richmond, VA 23298.0709. Copyright 0 1995 Society for Neuroscience 0270.6474/95/158199-15$05.00/O severaldifferent sitesof action have been suggested (see Yaksh et al., 1988) the midbrain periaqueductal gray matter (Yaksh et al., 1976; Manning et al., 1994) and spinal cord (Yaksh and Rudy, 1977) are amongthe more sensitiveto direct application of morphine. Furthermore, other neuroanatomical mediatorsof morphineantinociception(MA) havebeenestablished, including circuitry within the rostra1 ventromedial medulla (seeFields et al., 1991), and serotonergic and noradrenergic projections (Yaksh, 1979; Jensen and Yaksh, 1986b)descending through the dorsolateral funiculus (DLF; Basbaum et al., 1977; Hayes et al., 1978) to the spinal cord dorsal horn. Based on this and other evidence, a brainstemand spinal cord model of descending an- tinociceptive circuitry has been proposed (Mayer and Price, 1976;Basbaum and Fields, 1978, 1984; Mayer, 1979) and wide- ly acceptedas at least a partial basisfor antinociceptive effects of morphine,aswell asthe antinociceptive effects of some forms of focal brain stimulation and stress (seeBasbaumand Fields, 1984; Mayer and Manning, 1995). Details concerningthe microcircuitry and neurotransmitters of endogeneous pain control systems have been extensively inves- tigated (see Fields et al., 1991; Proudfit and Yeomans, 1995). The possible contribution of forebrain areas to the production of MA, however, has met with comparatively little research inter- est. The few studiesconcerning this issue have suggested the involvement of such areas as the arcuate nucleusof the hypo- thalamus (Tsengand Wang, 1992) and areatempestas (d’Amore et al., 1991),but potential interactions of these areas with critical brainstem and spinal cord substrates have not been fully ex- plored. Several experimenters haverecently used the rat paw formalin test to investigate forebrain involvement in the production of MA. The formalin test involves measuring behaviors indicative of prolonged nociception elicited by subcutaneous injection of dilute formalin into a paw (Dubuisson and Dennis, 1977). Sev- eral forebrain regions, including the posteriorhypothalamic area, evoke antinociception in the formalin test upon direct applica- tion of morphine (Manning et al., 1994). Furthermore, the sup- pression of formalin-induced nociceptive behaviorsby systemi- cally administeredmorphine can be strongly attenuatedeither by decerebration(Matthies and Franklin, 1992) or by bilateral excitotoxin-induced lesions of the central nucleusof the amyg- dala (Ce; Manning and Mayer, 1995). Although the extent of Ce involvement in the production of MA may be limited to prolonged nociceptive conditionsreflect- ed by the formalin test, it is possible that the Ce plays a general role in pain control mechanisms. It is known, for example, that the Ce entertains reciprocal connections with the PAG (Hopkins

Upload: others

Post on 13-Oct-2020

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1995, 15( 12): 8199-8213

The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the Production of Morphine Antinociception in the Rat Tail-Flick Test

Barton H. Manning’ and David J. Mayer2

DeDat-tments of ‘Anatomv and 2Anesthesioloav. Medical College of Virginia, Virginia Commonwealth University, Ridhmond, Virginia 23298

Current models of endogenous pain control circuitry em- phasize neural substrates within the brainstem and spinal cord. We have recently shown, however, that the central nucleus of the amygdala (Ce) contributes to morphine-in- duced suppression of formalin-induced nociceptive behav- iors. In the four experiments reported here, we investigated the possibility that the Ce also contributes to morphine- induced suppression of simple, spinally mediated nocicep tive reflexes. Bilateral Kmethyl-D-aspartate (NMDA)-in- duced lesions of the rat Ce, but not bilateral lesions cen- tered on either the basolateral or medial amygdaloid nucle- us, abolished the antinociception produced by 2.5 mg/kg morphine sulfate in the noxious heat-evoked tail-flick test. Bilateral Ce lesions also abolished the antinociception pro- duced by 2 or 4 mg/kg morphine sulfate, but a relatively large dose of morphine sulfate (10 mglkg, s.c.) resulted in partial reinstatement of antinociception. It is unlikely that these effects were due to secondary, seizure-induced dam- age following NMDA injection (e.g., to areas outside the amygdala) since bilateral inactivation of the Ce with the lo- cal anesthetic lidocaine also reliably attenuated morphine antinociception. It is also unlikely that these effects were artifacts of lesion-induced hyperalgesia, since Ce lesions failed to result in reliable thermal hyperalgesia, even at baseline tail-flick latencies of lo-12 sec. These data are the first to provide direct evidence that systemically adminis- tered morphine requires the integrity of a forebrain area in order to suppress spinally mediated nociceptive reflexes. It is argued that the present results, together with recent evidence linking the Ce to the production of several forms of conditioned and unconditioned environmentally induced antinociception, warrant incorporation of the Ce into cur- rent models of endogenous pain control circuitry.

[Key words: pain, antinociception, morphine, tail flick, amygdala, central nucleus, lesion, NMDA, lidocaine]

Morphine remains one of the most powerful analgesic agents. The CNS substrates underlying morphine’s antinociceptive ef- fects have been extensively studied in recent years. Although

Received May 4, 1995; revised Aug. 11, 1995; accepted Aug. 17, 1995.

This research was supported by PHS Grant NS-24009. We thank Leslie Ken- iston and Milton Cook for assistance with animal testing, histology, and the preparation of figures. We also thank Drs. J. L. Dupree and J. W. Bigbee for assistance with photography, and Drs. J. Mao, D. D. Price and S. P. Welch for helpful comments and advice.

Correspondence should be addressed to Barton H. Manning, Department of Anatomy, Virginia Commonwealth University, Box 980709, Richmond, VA 23298.0709.

Copyright 0 1995 Society for Neuroscience 0270.6474/95/158199-15$05.00/O

several different sites of action have been suggested (see Yaksh et al., 1988) the midbrain periaqueductal gray matter (Yaksh et al., 1976; Manning et al., 1994) and spinal cord (Yaksh and Rudy, 1977) are among the more sensitive to direct application of morphine. Furthermore, other neuroanatomical mediators of morphine antinociception (MA) have been established, including circuitry within the rostra1 ventromedial medulla (see Fields et al., 1991), and serotonergic and noradrenergic projections (Yaksh, 1979; Jensen and Yaksh, 1986b) descending through the dorsolateral funiculus (DLF; Basbaum et al., 1977; Hayes et al., 1978) to the spinal cord dorsal horn. Based on this and other evidence, a brainstem and spinal cord model of descending an- tinociceptive circuitry has been proposed (Mayer and Price, 1976; Basbaum and Fields, 1978, 1984; Mayer, 1979) and wide- ly accepted as at least a partial basis for antinociceptive effects of morphine, as well as the antinociceptive effects of some forms of focal brain stimulation and stress (see Basbaum and Fields, 1984; Mayer and Manning, 1995).

Details concerning the microcircuitry and neurotransmitters of endogeneous pain control systems have been extensively inves- tigated (see Fields et al., 1991; Proudfit and Yeomans, 1995). The possible contribution of forebrain areas to the production of MA, however, has met with comparatively little research inter- est. The few studies concerning this issue have suggested the involvement of such areas as the arcuate nucleus of the hypo- thalamus (Tseng and Wang, 1992) and area tempestas (d’Amore et al., 1991), but potential interactions of these areas with critical brainstem and spinal cord substrates have not been fully ex- plored.

Several experimenters have recently used the rat paw formalin test to investigate forebrain involvement in the production of MA. The formalin test involves measuring behaviors indicative of prolonged nociception elicited by subcutaneous injection of dilute formalin into a paw (Dubuisson and Dennis, 1977). Sev- eral forebrain regions, including the posterior hypothalamic area, evoke antinociception in the formalin test upon direct applica- tion of morphine (Manning et al., 1994). Furthermore, the sup- pression of formalin-induced nociceptive behaviors by systemi- cally administered morphine can be strongly attenuated either by decerebration (Matthies and Franklin, 1992) or by bilateral excitotoxin-induced lesions of the central nucleus of the amyg- dala (Ce; Manning and Mayer, 1995).

Although the extent of Ce involvement in the production of MA may be limited to prolonged nociceptive conditions reflect- ed by the formalin test, it is possible that the Ce plays a general role in pain control mechanisms. It is known, for example, that the Ce entertains reciprocal connections with the PAG (Hopkins

Page 2: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

8200 Manning and Mayer - Central Nucleus Contributes to Morphine Antinociception

Ce Sham Lesion Ce Lesion

Figure 1. Photomicrographs of coronal sections through the Ce. Al-A3. Progressively higher magnifications showing a portion of Ce infused with 0.1 M PBS corresponding to a rat in the Ce sham-lesion group of Experiment 1. Note the healthy neuronal cell profiles. BI-B3, Progressively higher magnifications showing a portion of Ce infused with NMDA (0.25 M, pH 7.4) corresponding to a rat in the Ce lesion group of Experiment 1. Note the marked decrease in neuronal cell profiles and proliferation of smaller glial cells. Cresyl violet stain. Scale bars: Al and BI, 400 pm; A2 and B2, 200 pm; A3 and B3, 100 km.

and Holstege, 1978; Rizvi et al., 1991), an area well known for measured on assays involving both brief (Helmstetter and Bell- its ability to evoke suppression of spinally mediated nociceptive gowan, 1993; Fox and Sorenson, 1994) and prolonged (Helm- reflexes (Mayer et al., 1971; Yaksh et al., 1988). Furthermore, stetter, 1992) nociception. bilateral lesions of the amygdala (including the Ce) attenuate In the following series of experiments, we investigated the several forms of environmentally induced antinociception as possibility that the Ce contributes to morphine-induced suppres-

Page 3: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1995, 15(12) 8201

-2.56

-2.80

-3.30

-3.80

sion of the noxious heat-evoked, spinally mediated tail-flick (TF) reflex. We bilaterally inactivated neurons within the rat Ce in two ways: (1) through focal infusion of a small volume of the excitotoxin N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA), which destroys neu- ronal somata within a brain region but not fibers of passage (Hastings et al., 1985; Winn et al., 1990, 1991), and (2) through focal infusion of a small volume of the local anesthetic lido- Caine, which produces temporary, nonspecific, neuronal block- ade (Sandktihler et al., 1987; Martin, 1991). The antinociceptive effects of several doses of systemically administered morphine were then tested in awake, Ce-inactivated rats.

Experiment 1 The purpose of this experiment was to test the ability of a single dose of morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/kg) to suppress the TF reflex

Figure 2. Histological results of Ex- periment 1 (bilateral Ce lesions). Rep- resentations of six coronal sections through the rat forebrain are shown in sequence from anterior to posterior. Millimeters posterior to bregma are given to the left. The closed curves il- lustrate the borders of Ce lesions (n = 6), as determined by the extent of neu- ronal cell loss and gliosis. Lesion area common to all six rats is shown as dark shading in each hemisphere. Note that over 80% of the Ce was bilaterally damaged in all six rats. Adapted from Paxinos and Watson (1986). Amygda- loid areas: Ce, central nucleus; BLA, basolateral nucleus, anterior; BLV, ba- solateral nucleus, ventral; BSTIA, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, intra- amygdaloid division; Me, medial nu- cleus; BM, basomedial nucleus; Co, cortical amygdaloid nuclei. Extra- amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen.

in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the Ce. In addition to a Ce sham-lesion control group, treatment groups of rats with bilateral lesions or bilateral sham lesions centered on the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BL) or medial nucleus of the amygdala (Me) were also prepared and tested.

Materials and Methods

Subjects. Experimentally naive, male Sprague-Dawley rats (Hilltop, Scottdale, PA) weighing 30&325 gm at the time of surgery were used. Animals were housed individually in cages on a 12: 12 light/dark sched- ule (lights on at 7:00 A.M.) with food and water available ad libitum. All experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Virginia Commonwealth University.

Experimental design. Three sets of lesion animals (Ce-lesion, BL- lesion, and Me-lesion) and three sets of sham-lesion animals (Ce-, BL-, and Me-sham lesion) were surgically prepared for this experiment. An-

Page 4: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

8202 Manning and Mayer - Central Nucleus Contributes to Morphine Antinociception

+ Ce Lesion/Morphine Sulfate (2.5 mglkg) e Ce Lesion/Saline

"1 --t Ce Sham Lesion/Morphine Sulfate (2.5 mglkg) -0I Ce Sham Lesion/Saline

T

I SYSTEMIC INJECTION

2 ti II, I I I I I I I I I I -15 0 10 20 30 40 50

Time (Minutes)

Figure 3. Mean TFLs of Ce-treated rats (n = 6/group) in Experiment 1. Error bars indicate SEM. Baseline (i.e., systemic saline) TF latencies of Ce lesion rats were not reliably different from those of Ce sham- lesion rats (protected t test, P > 0.05). In Ce sham-lesion rats, admin- istration of morphine sulfate (2.5 mglkg, s.c.) resulted in reliable ele- - - vation of TF laiencies as compared with saline (protected t test, P < O.Ol), indicating the production of reliable antinociception. In Ce lesion rats, however, the same dose of morphine was ineffective in producing reliable antinociception (protected t test, P < 0.01).

imals were allowed to recover for at least 7 d before nociceptive testing began. If, however, an animal had not regained 100% of its preoperative body weight by postsurgical day 10, it was not included in the study. Each animal received two nociceptive test sessions, with each test ses- sion separated by 7 d. Before each session, the animal received a sub- cutaneous injection of either morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/kg) or saline vehicle. Beginning 15 min after the injection, TF latencies (TFLs; pro- cedure described below) were obtained at 10 min intervals for 50 min. The order of systemic drug administration was counterbalanced within each group.

The dose of 2.5 mg/kg morphine sulfate was chosen for this initial experiment because in normal rats it produces reliable, yet submaximal, suppression of the TF reflex when baseline TFLs of 34 set are used. It was hoped that by using this dose, quantitative differences between the Ce lesion group and the various control groups would be readily detectable.

Surgery. Rats were anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (48 mg/ kg, i.p.). Using standard stereotaxic equipment, stainless steel injection cannulas (30 gauge) were lowered bilaterally into either the Ce, BL, or Me, based on coordinates provided by the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1986; Ce coordinates: AP -2.5 mm posterior to bregma, lateral i 4.4 mm, ventral 8.3 mm; BL coordinates: AP -2.5 mm, L 2 5.2, V - 8.3; Me coordinates: AP -2.5 mm, L -3.2, V -9.4). The cannulas were connected, via polyethylene tubing, to syringes mounted on an infusion pump (Harvard Apparatus, South Natick, MA). After 5 min, either N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA; 0.25 M in sterile, isotonic 0.1 M PBS, pH 7.4) or vehicle alone was slowly infused into the target site over 5 min such that the final volume of injection was 150 nl. The cannulas were left in place an additional 5 min to allow sufficient time for ab- sorption. The cannulas were then removed, and the scalp wound closed with wound clips. Each rat received an injection of diazepam (1 mg, i.p.) and rats treated with NMDA were monitored for early signs of seizure activity until they awoke from anesthesia. If an NMDA-treated

rat showed any signs of seizure activity, an additional injection of di- azepam (1 mg, i.p.) was administered.

Nociceptive testing. Ambient temperature of the test room was main- tained at 24 -+ 05°C at all times. Each rat was positioned in a well ventilated, tube-shaped plastic restrainer (internal diameter, 6.5 cm; in- ternal length, 25 cm), to which the rat was habituated for 1 hr on each of 3 d immediately prior to testing. The TFL was defined as the time from the onset of radiant heat to withdrawal of the tail (D’Amour and Smith, 1941). The radiant heat source came from a projection bulb underneath a 3 mm thick glass plate. The bulb was aimed at the caudal end (4-10 cm from the tail tip) of the rat’s tail. The intensity of heat emitted by the bulb was adjusted using a rheostat to result in baseline (i.e., systemic saline) TFLs of 3.04.0 sec. An automatic cutoff time of 8 set was preset to minimize possible tissue damage caused by over- stimulation in the absence of a withdrawal response. For each time point tested, three TF trials were made with (1) a 1 min intertrial interval (Depaulis et al., 1987) and (2) changes in the position of the tail re- ceiving heat stimulation on each trial. The mean TFL from the three trials was used as the nociceptive score for that time point. Testing was performed blind as to which treatment group the subject belonged.

Histology. After completion of the second TF test session, rats were overdosed with sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg) and perfused through the heart with buffered 0.9% saline followed by 10% formalin. The brains were removed and stored overnight in a buffered 10% formalin/ 10% sucrose solution. The following morning the brains were quickly frozen and sliced at -25°C. Coronal sections (20 p,m thick) were taken starting at the crossing of the anterior commissure and ending at the caudal limit of the PAG, and stained with cresyl violet. Brains infused with NMDA were inspected with a light microscope for gliosis and neuronal cell loss as compared with brains infused with PBS (Hastings et al., 1985; Winn et al., 1990, 1991). Since the Ce projects to the ventrolateral PAG (Hopkins and Holstege, 1978; Beitz, 1982; Rizvi et al., 1991), and the ventrolateral PAG is an important site of morphine action in the TF test (e.g., Jensen and Yaksh, 1986a), the brains of lesion rats were also inspected for signs of transsynaptic neuronal cell loss in the PAG as compared with sham-lesion rats. Rat subjects were included for statistical analysis if the nucleus targeted for destruction was dam- aged by at least 80% in each hemisphere. In the case of BL and Me lesion rats, a damage level of 20% or less bilaterally to the Ce was permitted. Lesion mapping was performed blind to the behavioral re- sults.

Statistical analyses. TFLs collected from Ce-treated rats (lesion and sham lesion), BL-treated rats (lesion and sham lesion), and Me-treated rats (lesion and sham lesion) were analyzed in separate three-factor ANOVAs (lesion X systemic drug treatment X time), with systemic drug treatment and time analyzed as repeated measures. Multiple pair- wise comparisons were made using protected t tests.

Results

Recovery from surgery In the present experiment, as well as in Experiments 3 and 4 (see below), most rats treated with intra-amygdaloid NMDA ap- peared to recover well from surgery, with an average of one in 20 prepared rats failing to regain 100% preoperative body weight by postsurgical day 10. All rats receiving intra-amyg- daloid infusion of PBS regained 100% body weight before post- surgical day 7.

Histology

Bilateral intra-Ce infusion of excitotoxic levels of NMDA pro- duced lesions with a pattern of neuronal cell loss and prolifer- ation of small glial cells (Fig. 1). In all cases, the lesion had clearly definable borders, with substantial loss of neuronal cell profiles and proliferation of glial elements on one side of the border, and healthy neuronal cell profiles on the other side of the border. Infusion of NMDA into the BL or Me was also extremely effective in producing neuronal cell loss, with prolif- eration of glial cells being even more pronounced than that ob- served after NMDA infusion into the Ce. Lesions corresponding to rats included in the Ce lesion group (n = 6), BL lesion group

Page 5: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1995, 75(12) 8203

-1.80 mm

-2.56

-2.80

-3.80

Figure 4. Histological results of Ex- pediment I (bilateral BL lesions). Rep- resentations of six coronal sections through the rat forebrain are shown in sequence from anterior to posterior. Mil- limeters posterior to bregma are given to the I#. The closed curves illustrate the borders of BL lesions (n = 6), as determined by the extent of neuronal cell loss and gliosis. Lesion area com- mon to all six rats is shown as light shading in each hemisphere. Adapted from Paxinos and Watson (1986). Amygdaloid areas: Ce, central nucleus; BLA, basolateral nucleus, anterior; BLV, basolateral nucleus, ventral; BSTIA, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, intra- amygdaloid division; Me, medial nucle- us; BM, basomedial nucleus; Co, cort- cal amygdaloid nuclei. Extra-amygda- loid- areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen.

(n = 6) and Me lesion group (n = 8) are outlined in Figures 2, 4, and 6, respectively. The shading in each of these figures indicates damaged tissue common to all rats in the group.

Ce-treated ruts. Of the 14 rats initially prepared for the Ce- lesion group, six met the strict inclusion criterion of 80% bilat- eral Ce damage. Cases were rejected on the basis of insufficient bilateral Ce damage or sufficient damage to the Ce in one hemi- sphere only. In four of the six Ce lesion cases, neuronal cell loss was evident in all portions of the Ce (including the medial and lateral subdivisions) in one hemisphere, and in 85-90% of the Ce in the other hemisphere (Fig. 2). In the other two cases, neuronal cell loss was evident in 85-90% of the Ce bilaterally. In all six cases, neuronal damage extended dorsally into portions of overlying globus pallidus and caudate-putamen. Furthermore, in all six cases, there was substantial bilateral damage to the BL

and portions of the lateral nucleus in addition to varying amounts of bilateral damage to the intra-amygdaloid division of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. The posterodorsal sub- division of the Me was damaged bilaterally by approximately 50% in three cases, with the other three cases showing substan- tial Me damage in one hemisphere but not the other.

BL-treated rats. Six out of 14 prepared rats met the inclusion criterion for the BL lesion group. In all six BL lesion cases, there was substantial bilateral neuronal cell loss in at least 80% of the BL, with the ventral subdivision of the BL being most frequently spared by the NMDA treatment (Fig. 4). In all cases, a large percentage of the lateral nucleus and smaller percentages of adjacent piriform cortex and overlying caudate-putamen and globus pallidus were damaged bilaterally. Furthermore, damage extended bilaterally into the lateral portion of the Ce (CeL) in

Page 6: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

8204 Manning and Mayer - Central Nucleus Contributes to Morphine Antinociception

e BL Lesion/Morphine Sulfate (2.5 mglkg) 8

1 e BL Lesion/Saline --t BL Sham Lesion/Morphine Sulfate (2.5 mglkg) e BL Sham Lesion/Saline

T I

SYSTEMIC INJECTION

2 N J-4, I I I I I I I I I I -15 0 10 20 30 40 50

Time (Minutes)

Figure 5. Mean TF latencies of BL-treated rats (n = 6/group) in Ex- periment 1. Error bars indicate SEM. Baseline (i.e., systemic saline) TF latencies of BL lesion rats were not reliably different from those of BL sham-lesion rats (protected t test, P > 0.05). Unlike the effect observed in rats with bilateral Ce lesions (Fig. 3), the ability of morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/kg, s.c.) to produce antinociception was not compromised by bilateral lesions of the BL (protected t test, P > 0.05).

all cases, although in five cases the damage was very slight. In the other case the Ce was damaged by approximately 15% in one hemisphere, while the Ce in the other hemisphere remained intact except for slight damage around,the border of the CeL.

Me-treated ruts. Eight out of 14 prepared rats met the inclu- sion criterion for the Me lesion group. In all eight Me lesion cases, there was substantial bilateral neuronal cell loss in at least 80% of the Me with dorsal portions of the Me being the most frequently spared (Fig. 6). In all cases, there was substantial damage to portions of the cortical amygdaloid nuclei and ba- somedial amygdaloid nucleus. Furthermore, in one case there was bilateral neuronal cell loss in the medial subdivision of the Ce (CeM). This damage, however, did not exceed 20% of the total area of that nucleus. In another five cases, there was slight damage around the medial border of the CeM in one hemisphere with the Ce in the other hemisphere left completely intact.

The histological results depicted in Figures 2, 4, and 6 are combined in Figure 8 to show lesion area common to all rats in the Ce lesion group (dark shading), and how this area relates to lesion area common to rats in the Me and BL groups (light shading). Note that while the bilateral Ce lesions also consis- tently resulted in substantial neuronal cell loss in the BL, Me, and lateral amygdaloid nucleus, this damage was largely con- trolled for in the BL and Me lesion groups.

No lesion rats showed signs of neuronal cell loss or gliosis when examined along the rostral-caudal extent of the PAG.

Nociceptive scores Ce-treated ruts. TFLs of Ce-treated rats (lesion and sham lesion) are shown in Figure 3. An ANOVA revealed reliable interactions

between lesion and systemic drug treatment [F( 1,10) = 12.30, P < 0.011, and systemic drug treatment and time [F(5,.50) = 3.68, P < 0.011. Further analysis of the lesion X systemic drug treatment interaction showed that morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/kg) produced reliably higher TFLs than saline in Ce sham-lesion rats (Fig. 3; protected t test, P < O.Ol), indicating reliable antinoci- ception had occurred. Morphine, however, failed to produce re- liably higher TFLs than saline in Ce lesion rats (Fig. 3; protected t test, P > 0.05). Finally, the TFLs of Ce lesion rats treated with saline were not reliably different from TFLs displayed by Ce sham-lesion rats treated with saline (Fig. 3; protected t test, P > 0.05).

BL-treated ruts. TFLs of BL-treated rats (lesion and sham lesion) are shown in Figure 5. An ANOVA revealed a reliable interaction between systemic drug treatment and time [F(5,50) = 6.01, P < O.OOl]. There was, however, no reliable main effect of lesion [F(l,lO) = 0.895, P > 0.051, indicating that TFLs of BL lesion and BL sham-lesion rats were not reliably different overall. Further analysis of the systemic drug treatment X time interaction showed that morphine produced reliably higher TFLs than saline (indicating reliable antinociception) in BL-treated rats at all time points (Fig. 5; protected t tests, P < 0.01) except the last, at 65 min after morphine injection (Fig. 5; protected t test, P > 0.05).

Me-treated ruts. TFLs of Me-treated rats (lesion and sham lesion) are shown in Figure 7. An ANOVA revealed a reliable three-way interaction between lesion, systemic drug treatment, and time [F(5,70) = 2.76, P < 0.021. Morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/ kg) produced reliably higher TFLs than saline in Me sham-lesion rats at all time points tested except the first (Fig. 7; protected t tests, P < 0.01 at all time points), indicating the production of reliable antinociception. Morphine also produced reliably higher TFLs than saline in Me lesion rats at all time points tested (Fig. 7; protected t tests, P < 0.05), indicating reliable antinociception was also produced in Me lesion rats. TFLs did not differ at any time point between Me sham-lesion rats treated with morphine and Me lesion rats treated with morphine (Fig. 7; protected t tests, P > 0.05 at all time points). Finally, no reliable differences in baseline (i.e., systemic saline) TFLs were detected between Me lesion and Me sham-lesion rats (Fig. 7; protected t tests, P > 0.05 at all time points), except at 15 min after saline injection, when Me sham-lesion rats showed reliably higher TFLs than Me lesion rats (Fig. 7; protected t test, P < 0.01).

Experiment 2

The results of Experiment 1 suggest that bilateral NMDA-in- duced lesions of the Ce, but not bilateral lesions of the BL or Me, attenuate the antinociceptive effects of systemically admin- istered morphine in the TF test. It should be pointed out, how- ever, that intracerebral infusion of neurotoxic levels of excitatory amino acids such as NMDA, ibotenate, or kainate has been re- ported to result in seizure activity (e.g., Winn et al., 1991). Fur- thermore, in the case of kainate at least, such seizure activity may result in secondary neuronal damage in brain areas remote from the site of injection (e.g., Ben-Ari et al., 1979; Kiihler and Schwartz, 1983). In our experience, intra-amygdaloid microin- jection of buffered NMDA does not produce strong seizure ac- tivity, and early signs of such activity can be easily controlled by diazepam (see Experiment 1, Materials and Methods). Fur- thermore, the histological results of Experiment 1 revealed no obvious signs of neuronal damage to PAG neurons in Ce lesion rats. Nevertheless, the possibility of seizure-induced toxic effects

Page 7: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1995, 15(12) 8205

-2.56

-2.80

-3.30

-3.80

to areas outside the amygdala cannot be completely discounted. Accordingly, we sought to determine whether bilateral intra-Ce microinjections of the local anesthetic lidocaine would also re- sult in attenuation of MA in the TF test. Lidocaine produces temporary, nonspecific, neuronal blockade within an injection area, including blockade of fibers of passage (e.g., Sandkiihler et al., 1987; Martin, 1991). It would not, however, be expected to produce neuronal blockade in brain areas remote from the site of injection.

Materials and Methods Surgery. Rats were anesthetized as described in Experiment 1. Stainless steel guide cannulas (23 gauge) were stereotaxically placed bilaterally 2 mm above the Ce according to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1986; coordinates: AP -2.5 mm posterior to bregma, lateral ? 4.4 mm, ven- tral 6.3 mm). A stainless steel stylet (00 gauge), extending just past the

Figure 6. Histological results of Ex- periment 1 (bilateral Me lesions). Rep- resentations of six coronal sections through the rat forebrain are shown in sequence from anterior to posterior. Millimeters posterior to bregma are given to the left. The closed curves il- lustrate the borders of Me lesions (n = S), as determined by the extent of neu- ronal cell loss and gliosis. Lesion area common to all eight rats is shown as light shading in each hemisphere. Adapted from Paxinos and Watson (1986). Amygdaloid areas: Ce, central nucleus; BLA, basolateral nucleus, an- terior; BLV, basolateral nucleus, ven- tral; BSTIA, bed nucleus of the stria ter- minalis, intra-amygdaloid division; Me, medial nucleus; BM, basomedial nucle- us; Co, cortical amygdaloid nuclei. Ex- trayamygdaloid areas: GP, globus pal- lidus; CP, caudate-putamen.

tip of the guide cannula, kept it free of debris during the surgical re- covery period. The recovery period lasted 7 d.

Experimental design. After surgery, animals were randomly chosen for inclusion in either an intra-Ce lidocaine treatment group or an intra- Ce saline treatment group. Each of these groups in turn received two TF sessions spaced 7 d apart: (1) a session beginning with a subcuta- neous injection of morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/kg), and (2) a session be- ginning with a subcutaneous injection of saline vehicle. Each test ses- sion began with the subcutaneous injection, whereupon the rat imme- diately started the microinjection procedure. Each stylet was removed from its respective guide cannula, and replaced with a 30 gauge inner cannula extending 2 mm beyond the tip of the guide cannula. Beginning at 5 min after subcutaneous injection, the intra-Ce injectate (either 2.0% lidocaine HCl in sterile, isotonic 0.1 M PBS, pH 7.4, or 0.1 M PBS alone) was slowly infused over 5 min such that the final volume of injection was 250 nl bilaterally. The inner cannulas were left in place for an additional two min to allow sufficient time for absorption of injectate into local tissue. The rat was then immediately placed in the

Page 8: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

8206 Manning and Mayer - Central Nucleus Contributes to Morphine AntinocicepQon

-C Me Lesion/Morphine Sulfate (2.5 mglkg) e Me Lesion/Saline + Me Sham Lesion/Morphine Sulfate (2.5 mglkg) + Me Sham Lesion/Saline

T T

VSTEMld IJECTION

AlI I I I I I I I I,, -15 0 10 20 30 40 50

Time (Minutes)

Figure 7. Mean TF latencies of Me-lesion (n = 8) and Me sham- lesion (n = 7) rats in Experiment 1. Error bars indicate SEM. Baseline (i.e., systemic saline) TF latencies of Me lesion rats were not different from those of Me sham-lesion rats (protected t test, P > 0.05). Unlike the effect observed in rats with bilateral Ce lesions (Fig. 3), the ability of morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/kg, s.c.) to produce antinociception was not compromised by bilateral lesions of the Me (protected t test, P > 0.05).

plastic restraint tube (see Experiment 1). TF testing began 15 min after subcutaneous drug injection. TFLs were obtained at 10 min intervals for the next 50 min (see Experiment 1, Materials and Methods, Noci- ceptive testing). The order of systemic drug administration was coun- terbalanced within each group.

After completion of the second TF session, rats were perfused through the heart and their brains removed and stored in sucrose/for- malin as described in Experiment 1. The brains were quickly frozen and sliced at -25°C. Coronal sections (20 p,rn thick) were taken through the amygdala and stained with cresyl violet. The tips of the inner can- nulas were determined bilaterally with the aid of a light microscope. Only rat subjects with bilateral cannula placements within the borders of the Ce were included for statistical analyses.

Statistical analyses. TFLs collected from Ce-treated rats were ana- lyzed in a three-factor ANOVA (in&a-Ce drug treatment X systemic drug treatment X time). Multiple pairwise comparisons were made us- ing protected t tests. Systemic drug treatment and time were analyzed as repeated measures.

Results Histology The bilateral placements of inner cannula tips corresponding to intra-amygdaloid lidocaine-treated rats (n = 7) are shown in Figure 9. Note that the cannula placements of these rats, as well as those corresponding to the intra-amygdaloid saline treated rats (n = 7; not shown in Fig. 9), were bilaterally within the borders of the Ce.

Nociceptive scores

Figure 10 shows the TFLs of rats in Experiment 2. An ANOVA revealed a reliable interaction between intra-Ce drug treatment and systemic drug treatment [F(1,12) = 5.015, P < 0.051, but

no reliable main effect of time [F(5,60) = 1.39, P = 0.241. Further analysis of the reliable interaction showed that there was no reliable difference between TFLs of systemic saline-treated rats simultaneously treated with intra-Ce lidocaine and systemic saline-treated rats simultaneously treated with intra-Ce saline (protected t test, P > 0.05). In rats treated with intra-Ce saline, morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/kg) produced a reliable elevation in TFLs as compared with systemic saline (Fig. 10; protected t test, P < O.Ol), indicating reliable antinociception had occurred. The same dose of morphine produced reliably less antinociception, however, in rats treated with intra-Ce lidocaine (Fig. 10; pro- tected t test, P < 0.01). The blockade of MA by intra-Ce lido- Caine was not complete, however, since morphine still retained reliable antinociceptive effects as compared with saline in lido- Caine-treated rats (protected t test, P < 0.05).

Experiment 3 The results of Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that blockade of neural activity within the Ce, using either a somata-selective excitotoxin or a nonspecific local anesthetic, abolishes the anti- nociceptive effects of 2.5 mg/kg morphine sulfate in the TF test. Experiment 3 was designed, therefore, to determine whether this effect is generalizable to other doses of systemic morphine.

Materials and Methods Experiment 3 was identical to Experiment 1 except for the following: (1) only Ce-treated (sham lesion and lesion) rats were utilized, with each rat receiving four TF sessions (each separated by 7 d) under four different systemic drug treatments (0, 2, 4, and 10 mg/kg morphine sulfate). Drug treatments were administered in a counterbalanced order within the Ce lesion and Ce sham-lesion groups. A stricter histological inclusion criterion of 90% bilateral damage was employed for Ce lesion rats since higher doses of morphine were being employed in this ex- periment as compared with the dose employed in Experiment 1.

Statistical analyses. TFLs collected from Ce-treated rats (lesion and sham lesion) were analyzed in a three-factor ANOVA (lesion X sys- temic drug dose X time), with systemic drug dose and time analyzed as repeated measures. Multiple pairwise comparisons were made using protected t tests.

Results Histology As observed in Experiment 1, all lesions had clearly definable borders. Lesions corresponding to rats in the Ce lesion group (n = 7) are outlined in Figure 11. The dark shading in the figure indicates damaged tissue common to all rats in the group.

Of the 20 rats prepared for the Ce-lesion group in Experiment 3, seven met the strict inclusion criterion of 90% bilateral Ce damage. Once again cases were rejected on the basis of insuf- ficient bilateral Ce damage or sufficient damage to the Ce in only one hemisphere. In four of the seven Ce lesion cases, sub- stantial neuronal cell loss was evident in all portions of the Ce (including the medial and lateral subdivisions) in both hemi- spheres. In another two cases, neuronal cell loss was evident in all portions of the Ce (including the medial and lateral subdi- visions) in one hemisphere, and over 90% of the Ce in the other hemisphere. In the remaining case, neuronal cell loss was evi- dent in over 90% of the Ce bilaterally. In all seven cases, neu- ronal damage extended dorsally into portions of overlying glo- bus pallidus and caudate-putamen. Furthermore, in all seven cases there was substantial bilateral damage to the BL and por- tions of the lateral nucleus, in addition to varying amounts of bilateral damage to the intra-amygdaloid division of the bed nu- cleus of the stria terminalis, the Me and the basomedial amyg- daloid nucleus.

Page 9: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1995, 15(12) 8207

-2.56

-2.80

-3.30

-3.80

No lesion rats showed any signs of neuronal cell loss or gli- osis when examined along the rostral-caudal extent of the PAG.

Nociceptive scores

TFLs of Ce-treated rats (lesion and sham lesion) are shown in Figure 12. An ANOVA revealed reliable interactions between lesion and systemic drug dose [F(3,42) = 11.62, P < O.OOOl], lesion and time [F(5,70) = 3.51, P < 0.011 and systemic drug dose and time [F(15,210) = 3.28, P < O.OOOl]. Further analysis of the lesion X systemic drug dose interaction showed that mor- phine sulfate (2, 4 and 10 mg/kg) produced dose-dependent an- tinociception in Ce sham-lesion rats (Fig. 12A; protected t tests, P < 0.01). The two lower doses of morphine sulfate (2 and 4 mg/kg), however, failed to produce reliably higher TFLs than saline in Ce lesion rats (Fig. 12B; protected t tests, P > 0.05).

Figure 8. Combined histological re- sults of Experiment 1 (bilateral Me, Ce, and BL lesions). The histological re- sults depicted in Figures 2, 4, and 6 are combined into a composite representa- tion of the bilateral lesions that resulted in reliable attenuation of morphine an- tinociception (Ce lesions; see Fig. 3) and the bilateral lesions that failed to attenuate morphine antinociception (BL and Me lesions; see Figs. 5 and 7, respectively). Millimeters posterior to bregma are given to the left. The bilat- eral dark shiding indicates lesion area common to all rats in the Ce lesion group of Experiment 1. Lesion area common to rats in the BL and Me le- sion groups is shown as light shading in each hemisphere. Note that while bi- lateral Ce lesions also consistently re- sulted in substantial neuronal cell loss in the BL, Me and lateral amygdaloid nucleus, this damage was largely con- trolled for in the BL and Me lesion groups. Adapted from Paxinos and Watson (1986). Amygdaloid areas: Ce, central nucleus; BU, basolateral nucle- us, anterior; BLV, basolateral nucleus, ventral; BSTIA, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, intra-amygdaloid division; Me, medial nucleus; BM, basomedial nucleus; Co, cortical amygdaloid nu- clei: Extra-amygdaloid areas: GP, glo- bus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen.

The highest dose of morphine sulfate (10 mg/kg) did produce reliable antinociception in Ce lesion rats (Fig. 12B; protected t test, P < 0.05) but this effect was reliably lower than that pro- duced by 10 mg/kg morphine sulfate in Ce sham-lesion rats (Fig. 12A; protected t test, P < 0.05). Finally, as was the case in Experiment 1, the TFLs of Ce lesion rats treated with saline were not reliably different from TFLs displayed by Ce sham- lesion rats treated with saline (Fig. 12; protected t test, P > 0.05).

Experiment 4

The results of Experiments l-3 suggest that the Ce contributes to the production of MA in the TF test. It could be argued, however, that blockade of neural activity within the Ce increases a rat’s sensitivity to noxious stimulation (i.e., produces hyper-

Page 10: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

8208 Manning and Mayer * Central Nucleus Contributes to Morphine Antinociception

Figure 9. Histological results of Ex- periment 2 (bilateral lidocaine injec- tions into the Ce). Representations of four coronal sections through the rat amygdala are shown in sequence from anterior to posterior. Millimeters pos- terior to bregma are given to the left. The solid circles in each hemisphere show the approximate positions of the cannula tips corresponding to rats in the intra-Ce lidocaine treatment group (n = 7). Adapted from Paxinoi and Watson (1986). Amygdaloid areas: Ce, central nucleus; BLA, basolateral nucle- us, anterior; BLV, basolateral nucleus, ventral; BSTIA, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, i&a-amygdaloid division; Me, medial nucleus; BM, basomedial nucleus; Co, cortical amygdaloid nu- clei. Extra-amygdaloid areas: GP, glo- bus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen.

-2.12

-2.30

-2.56

-2.80

algesia) independent of a disruption of pain control circuitry. This increase in sensitivity, in turn, could result in a rightward shift in the morphine dose-response curve. While Ce blockade appeared to have had no hyperalgesic effect on baseline TFLs in Experiments l-3 (Figs. 3, 10, 12), this possibility cannot be entirely discounted since baseline TFLs of 3-4 set are n@ par- ticularly sensitive for detection of hyperalgesia (Hargreaves et al., 1988). It remains possible, then, that the effects observed in Experiments l-3 reflect a hyperalgesic effect of Ce lesions, and not a disruption of antinociceptive mechanisms. To thoroughly investigate this possibility, another set of Ce-treated rats (lesion and sham lesion) was prepared and tested for nociceptive reac- tivity with the heat source adjusted to result in baseline TFLs of 10-12 sec. Similar latencies have been used for sensitive detec- tion of hyperalgesia using the paw-withdrawal test (Hargreaves et al., 1988; Mao et al., 1994). All rats received TF test sessions on each of 6 weeks following surgery to test for possible time- dependent effects of Ce lesions on baseline TFLs.

Materials and Methods Experimental design. One set of Ce-treated (lesion and sham lesion) rats was surgically prepared as described in Experiment 1. Animals were allowed to recover for 7 d before nociceptive testing began. The general procedure of TF testing applied to these rats was similar to the procedure described in Experiment 1, with the following differences: (1) each animal received six nociceptive test sessions (see below), with each test session separated by 7 d; (2) each of these test sessions, in turn, consisted of three ‘IF trials only, the results of which were aver- aged to result in a single nociceptive score for that session; (3) the heat

source was adjusted to result in baseline TFLs of 10-12 sec. Histolog- ical procedures and lesion mapping were similar to those described in Experiment 1, with an inclusion criterion of 80% bilateral Ce damage applied to Ce lesion rats.

Nociceptive testing. Nociceptive test procedures were similar to those described in Experiments 1 and 2, with the following differences. Once again, each rat was positioned in a plastic restrainer, to which the rat was habituated for 1 hr on each of 3 d prior to testing. The radiant heat source (described in Experiment 1) was aimed at the caudal end (4-10 cm from the tail tip) of the rat’s tail and adjusted by a rheostat to result in baseline TFLs of IO-12 sec. An automatic cutoff time of 15 set was preset to minimize possible tissue damage caused by overstimulation in the absence of a withdrawal response. For each test session, three TP trials were made with a 3 min intertrial interval, and with changes in the position of the tail receiving heat stimulation on each trial. The mean TPL from the three trials was used as the nociceptive score for that test session.

Statistical analyses. TFLs collected from Ce-treated rats were ana- lyzed in a two-factor ANOVA (lesion X week), with week analyzed. as a repeated measure. Multiple pairwise comparisons were then made using protected t tests.

Results Histology Of the 20 rats prepared for the Ce lesion group in Experiment 4, nine met the inclusion criterion of 80% bilateral Ce damage. The lesions of these nine rats were similar in size to those shown in Figure 2.

Nociceptive scores

TFLs were consistently lower in Ce lesion rats (n = 9) as com- pared with Ce sham-lesion rats (IZ = 11) at all weeks tested (Fig.

Page 11: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

e Ce LidocainelSystemic Morphine Sulfate (2.5 mglkg) Q Ce Lidocaine/Systemic Saline -O- Ce Saline/Systemic Morphine Sulfate (2.5 mglkg) -O- Ce SalinelSvstemic Saline

1

II,, I I I I I I I I I -15 f 0 10 20 30 40 50

INTFtA-Ce INJECTION

Time (Minutes)

Figure 10. Mean TF latencies of intra-Ce lidocaine (n = 7) and intra- Ce saline (n = 7) treated rats in Experiment 2. Error bars indicate SEM. In rats treated with bilateral intra-Ce microinjections of saline, systemic administration of morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/kg, s.c.) resulted in reliable elevation of TF latencies (i.e., antinociception) as compared with sys- temic administration of saline (protected t test, P < 0.01). Morphine sulfate (2.5 mg/kg, s.c.) also produced reliable antinociception in rats treated with bilateral intra-Ce microinjections of lidocaine (protected t test, P < 0.05), but this effect was reliably lower than that produced by the same dose of morphine in rats treated with intra-Ce saline. Bi- lateral intra-Ce microinjection of lidocaine failed to alter baseline (i.e., systemic saline) TF latencies as compared with bilateral intra-Ce mi- croinjection of saline (protected t test, P > 0.05). Note that bilateral intra-Ce microinjection of lidocaine reliably attenuated MA for the en- tire duration of the 50 min test session (protected t test, P < 0.05). This duration of effect appears to be consistent with the time course of neu- ronal inactivation and hypometabolism produced by lidocaine in the cerebral cortex (Sandktihler et al., 1987) and spinal cord dorsal columns (Martin, 1991).

13). This effect, however, cannot be classified as reliable thermal hyperalgesia since there was no reliable main effect of lesion [F(1,20) = 2.41, P > 0.051, nor was there a reliable interaction between lesion and week [F(5,100) = 0.936, P > 0.051. Given that Ce lesion rats in Experiments 1 and 3 were tested at 14 weeks after surgery, it seems unlikely that the results of these experiments were artifacts of Ce lesion-induced hyperalgesia.

Discussion

The results of Experiments lil indicate that bilateral lesions of the Ce abolish the antinociceptive effects of low doses (2, 2.5, 4 mg/kg) of systemically administered morphine in the rat TF test (Figs. 3, 10, 12). Antinociception can be partially reinstated, however, if the dose of morphine sulfate is raised to 10 mg/kg (Fig. 12B). This pattern of results is similar to patterns obtained with thoracic spinal cord transections (Advokat and Burton, 1987), bilateral lesions of the spinal cord DLF (Basbaum et al., 1977), and lesions of the rostra1 ventromedial medulla (Yaksh et al., 1977): the inhibitory effects of these manipulations on MA can be overcome if the dose of morphine sulfate is raised

The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1995, 15(12) .8209

from 3-5 mg/kg to lO+ mg/kg. The apparent antinociceptive effect of 10 mg/kg in Ce lesion rats (Fig. 12B) may indeed reflect reinstatement of antinociception, perhaps through an in- creased contribution of spinal cord or peripheral (e.g., Levine and Taiwo, 1989) sites of action as the dose of systemic mor- phine is raised. Alternatively, increased motor impairments (i.e., catatonia) that accompany higher doses of morphine (De Ryck et al., 1980; Wolgin, 1985), or the few surviving Ce neurons that remained in several Ce lesion rats, may account for the apparent presence of an antinociceptive effect at this dose.

It is unlikely that a factor other than the destruction of Ce neurons was responsible for the attenuation of MA in the present experiments. First, biochemical analyses of the effects of NMDA lesions in other brain areas indicate that this neurotoxin leaves fibers of passage intact (Hastings et al., 1985; Winn et al., 1990, 1991). It is unlikely, then, that damage to fibers coursing through or around the Ce is responsible for the effects reported in the present experiments. Second, while our Ce lesions also extended into the basolateral, lateral, and medial amygdaloid nuclei, the effects of this extraneous damage were controlled for in the BL and Me lesion groups of Experiment 1 (see Fig. 8). Third, it is unlikely that neuronal damage to portions of overlying striatum and globus pallidus account for the present effects since well over 95% of both structures remained intact in Ce lesion rats. Finally, the techniques used to block Ce neuronal activity in Experiments 1 and 2 are complementary: the somata-selective lesions produced by NMDA infusion control for the blockade of fiber activity produced by lidocaine infusion, and the nonse- lective local anesthesia produced by lidocaine infusion controls for possible remote, seizure-induced damage produced by NMDA infusion.

The present experiments are the first to demonstrate that le- sions of a specific amygdaloid nucleus disrupt MA in the TF test. Several other reports, however, implicate the amygdala in MA. We have recently shown that the suppression of formalin- induced nociceptive behaviors by systemically administered morphine is strongly attenuated by bilateral excitotoxin-induced lesions of the Ce (Manning and Mayer, 1995). Furthermore, mi- croinjection of morphine into the corticomedial subdivision of the amygdala has been reported to produce antinociception in the flinch-jump (Rodgers, 1977, 1978) and hot plate (Yaksh et al., 1988) tests, and one report suggests that microinjection of morphine into the BL produces antinociception in the TF test (Helmstetter et al., 1993). While the BL and corticomedial amygdaloid subdivision may indeed elicit antinociception upon focal microinjection of morphine, the results of Experiment 1 (Fig. 8) suggest that these areas are not major contributors to the antinociceptive effects of systemic morphine, since large bi- lateral lesions of the Me (including large portions of the cortical amygdaloid nuclei; see Figs. 6, 8) or BL (Figs. 4, 8; see also Manning and Mayer, 1995) do not attenuate MA (Figs. 5, 7).

Our present and previous (Manning and Mayer, 1995) data suggest that the Ce contributes to the production of MA in both the formalin and TF tests. This pattern of results, however, con- trasts somewhat with several previously published reports. In agreement with our previous data, removal of the entire rat fore- brain (i.e., decerebration) is reported to attenuate the antinoci- ceptive effects of systemic morphine in the formalin test (Mat- thies and Franklin, 1992). Decerebration does not, however, sim- ilarly impair MA in the TF test (Proudfit and Levy, 1978; Mat- thies and Franklin, 1992). These results appear to support the hypothesis that the neural substrates underlying MA in the for-

Page 12: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

8210 Manning and Mayer - Central Nucleus Contributes to Morphine Antinociception

-2.12

-2.56

Figure 1 I. Histological results of Ex- periment 3 (bilateral Ce lesions). Rep- resentations of six coronal sections -2.80 through the rat forebrain are shown in sequence from anterior to posterior. Millimeters posterior to bregma are given to the left. The closed curves il- lustrate the borders of Ce lesions (n = 7), as determined by the extent of neu- ronal cell loss and gliosis. Lesion area common to all seven rats is shown as -3.30 dark shading in each hemisphere. Adapted from Paxinos and Watson (1986). Amygdaloid areas: Ce, central nucleus; BLA, basolateral nucleus, an- terior; BLV, basolateral nucleus, ven- tral; BSTIA, bed nucleus of the stria ter- minalis, intra-amygdaloid division; Me, -3.80 medial nucleus; BM, basomedial nucle- us; Co, cortical amygdaloid nuclei. Ex- tra-amygdaloid areas: GP, globus pal- lidus; CP, caudate-putamen.

malin test are different from those underlying MA in the TF test (e.g., Abbott and Melzack, 1982). Our data tend to suggest, how- ever, that the neural substrates underlying MA in the formalin and TF tests overlap, at least at the level of the amygdala. Clear- ly, further experimentation is necessary to determine why dis- crete bilateral removal of the Ce using an excitotoxin is effective in attenuating MA in the TF test (present results), while gross bilateral removal of the Ce via decerebration fails to have a similar effect (Proudfit and Levy, 1978; Matthies and Franklin, 1992).

It remains unclear as to which subpopulation of Ce receptors plays a role in MA. If opioid receptors within the Ce are nec- essary, then systemically administered morphine may elicit at least part of its antinociceptive effects through direct action at these receptors. Two lines of evidence, however, count against

this possibility. First, receptor autoradiographic studies have failed to detect the presence of y-opioid receptors in the Ce (McLean et al., 1986; Mansour et al., 1987). Second, intra-Ce microinjection of morphine appears to be ineffective in eliciting antinociception in the flinch-jump and TF tests (Rodgers, 1978). Although further research is necessary to determine the precise role of the Ce in MA, the possibility exists that the Ce represents a nonopioid component of endogenous antinociceptive circuitry that is activated after the occupation of opioid receptors else- where in the CNS.

In addition to its putative role in MA, several other lines of evidence suggest an involvement of the amygdala in antinoci- ceptive processes. Bilateral microinjection of carbachol into var- ious amygdaloid nuclei, including the Ce, has been reported to elicit antinociception in the TF test (Klamt and Prado, 1991;

Page 13: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1995, 15(12) 8211

e Saline

A b Sham - 2.0 r@kg morphine sulfate + 4.0 mkg morphine sulfate

Lesion Rats + 10.0 tngkg morphine sulfate

SYSTEMIC NJECTION

-15 0 10 20 30 40 50

B Ce Lesion z i.?Glkg morphine sulfate

a Rats + 4.0 mglkg morphine sulfate e 10.0 mglkg morphine sulfate

T T T

-15 0 IO 20 30 40 50

Time (Minutes)

Figure 12. Mean TF latencies of Ce sham-lesion (A; n = 8) and Ce lesion (B; n = 7) rats in Experiment 3. Error bars indicate SEM. Baseline (i.e., systemic saline) TF latencies of Ce lesion rats were not reliably dif- ferent from those of Ce sham-lesion rats (protected t test, P > 0.05). In Ce sham-lesion rats, morphine sulfate (2, 4 and 10 mg/kg, s.c.) produced dose- dependent antinociception (see A and Experiment 3, Results). In Ce lesion rats, however, 2 and 4 mgkg morphine sulfate failed to produce reliable antinociception (B; protected t test, P > 0.05). A dose of 10 mgkg pro- duced reliable antinociception in Ce lesion rats (B; protected t test, P < 0.05), but the antinociception was reliably less than that produced by the same dose in Ce sham-lesion rats (protected t test, P < 0.05).

+ Ce Lesion e Ce Sham Lesion

SURGERY 1

ti I I I , I I

0 1 2 3 4 5 6

Week

Figure 13. Mean TF latencies of Ce sham-lesion (n = 11) and Ce lesion (n = 9) rats in Experiment 4. Error bars indicate SEM. The heat source was adjusted in this experiment to result in baseline TF latencies of lo-12 sec. This was done in order to better detect a possible hyper- algesic effect of Ce lesions. Although the Ce lesion group displayed consistently lower mean TF latencies than the Ce sham-lesion group, TF latencies of Ce lesion rats were not reliably different from those of Ce sham-lesion rats (protected t test, P > 0.05). These results suggest that bilateral Ce lesions fail to result in reliable thermal hyperalgesia.

Oliveira and Prado, 1994) while bilateral intra-Ce microinjec- tion of the enkephalinase inhibitor SCH-32615 (Al-Rodhan et al., 1990) or neurotensin (Kalivas et al., 1982) elicits antinoci- ception in the hot plate test. Several recent reports also implicate the amygdala in the expression of several forms of environmen- tally induced antinociception. Bilateral lesions of the amygdala (including the Ce) abolish antinociception produced by reexpo- sure to cues previously associated with noxious footshock (i.e., classically conditioned antinociception; Helmstetter, 1992; Helmstetter and Bellgowan, 1993; Watkins et al., 1993). Lesions of the Ce also impair some unconditioned forms of environ- mentally induced antinociception in the TF test. The antinoci- ception produced by exposure to a natural predator (Fox and Sorenson, 1994), as well as the antinociceptions produced by exposure to brief (Fox and Sorenson, 1994) or prolonged (Werka and Marek, 1990; Werka, 1994) footshock, are all attenuated or abolished by bilateral Ce lesions.

The amygdala, PAG, and hypothalamus have been proposed to be components of an integrated defense system (Adams, 1979; Graeff, 1988; LeDoux et al., 1988; Fanselow, 1991; Helmstetter, 1992). Furthermore, the amygdala (including the Ce) has been repeatedly implicated in the mediation of both conditioned and innate fear-related processes (see Davis, 1992, for a review). A role for the Ce in antinociceptive processes (including MA) is not inconsistent with its proposed role in fear and defense reactions; indeed, classically conditioned antinoci- ception is considered to be an index of fear by some investi- gators (Lichtman and Fanselow, 1991; Helmstetter, 1992). It

Page 14: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

8212 Manning and Mayer + Central Nucleus Contributes to Morphine Antinociception

may also be relevant that the Ce receives nociceptive signals from the parabrachial area via the spino-ponto-amygdaloid no- ciceptive pathway (Bernard et al., 1992). These signals have been hypothesized to trigger affective-motivational responses to noxious stimulation (Bernard and Besson, 1990; Bernard et al., 1992), responses that may, under certain circumstances, include fear and antinociception. It may be useful, then, to view the amygdala as part of a highly complex and coordinated defense system of which nociception, fear, antinociception (both condi- tioned and unconditioned) and various autonomic concomitants are all components.

The present results are the first to demonstrate, using lesioning techniques, that relatively low doses of systemically adminis- tered morphine require the integrity of a specific forebrain area to suppress nociceptive signals at the level of the spinal cord. While microinjections of various opioid and nonopioid com- pounds into a wide range of brainstem and forebrain areas have been shown to elicit antinociception, only lesions of the spinal cord DLF (e.g., Basbaum et al., 1977; Hayes et al., 1978; Barton et al., 1980) and medullary nucleus raphe magnus (e.g., Proudfit and Anderson, 1975; Cannon et al., 1983) have consistently re- sulted in attenuation of the antinociceptive effects of systemic morphine. Accordingly, a large portion of the research into su- praspinal pain control systems has focussed on medullospinal systems (see Fields et al., 1991). Given, however, that there is now mounting evidence implicating the amygdala in the pro- duction of MA, as well as several forms of conditioned and unconditioned environmentally induced antinociception, it may be appropriate to incorporate the amygdala into current models of endogenous pain control circuitry. At the very least, further experimentation is necessary to determine how the Ce interacts with previously established pain control circuitry to generate the full antinociceptive effect of systemic morphine.

References Abbott FV, Melzack R (1982) Brainstem lesions dissociate neural

mechanisms of morphine analgesia in different kinds of pain. Brain Res 251:149-l%.

Adams DB (1979) Brain mechanisms for offense, defense and sub- mission. Behav Brain Sci 2:201-241.

Advokat C, Burton P (1987) Antinociceptive effect of systemic and intrathecal morphine in spinally transected rats. Em J Pharmacol 139: 335-343.

Al-Rodhan N, Chipkin R, Yaksh TL (1990) The antinociceptive effects of SCH-32615, a neutral endopeptidase (enkephalinase) inhibitor, mi- croinjected into the periaqueductal, ventral medulla and amygdala. Brain Res 520: 123-130.

Barton C, Basbaum AI, Fields HL (1980) Dissociation of supraspinal and spinal actions of morphine: a quantitative evaluation. Brain Res 188:487-498.

Basbaum AI, Fields HL (1978) Endogenous pain control mechanisms: review and hypothesis. Ann Neurol 4:451-462.

Basbaum AI, Fields HL (1984) Endogenous pain control systems: brainstem spinal pathways and endorphin circuitry. Annu Rev Neu- rosci 7:309-338.

Basbaum AI, Marley NJE, O’Keefe J, Clanton CH (1977) Reversal of morphine and stimulus-produced analgesia by subtotal spinal cord lesions. Pain 3:43-56.

Beitz AJ (1982) The organization of afferent projections to the mid- brain periaqueductal gray of the rat. Neuroscience 7:133-159.

Ben-Ari Y, Tremblay E, Ottersen OP, Naquet R (1979) Evidence sug- gesting secondary epileptogenic lesions after kainic acid: pretreat- ment with diazepam reduces distant but not local brain damage. Brain Res 165:362-365.

Bernard JF, Besson JM (1990) The spino(trigemino)pontoamygdaloid pathway: electrophysiological evidence for an involvement in pain processes. J Neurophysiol 63:473-490.

Bernard JE Huang GE Besson JM (1992) Nucleus centralis of the

amygdala and the globus pallidus ventralis: electrophysiological ev- idence for an involvement in pain processes. J Neurophysiol 68:551- 569.

Cannon JT, Lewis JW, Weinberg VE, Liebeskind JC (1983) Evidence for the independence of brainstem mechanisms mediating analgesia induced by morphine and two forms of stress. Brain Res 269:231- 236.

d’Amore A, Lorenzini P, Massotti M (1991) Antinociceptive action of opiates and opioid peptides after unilateral microinjection into aren tempestus in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 259: 1308-13 15.

D’Amour FE, Smith DL (1941) A method for determining loss of pain sensation. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 72:7&79.

Davis M (1992) The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. In: The amygdala: neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction (Aggleton JP, ed), pp 255-305. New York: Wiley-Liss.

Depaulis A, Morgan MM, Liebeskind JC (1987) GABAergic modu- lation of the analgesic effects of morphine microinjected in the ven- tral periaqueductal gray matter of the rat. Brain Res 436:223-228.

De Ryck M, Schallert T, Teitelbaum P (1980) Morphine versus halo- peridol catalepsy in the rat: a behavioral analysis of postural support mechanisms. Brain Res 201:143-172.

Dubuisson D, Dennis SG (1977) The formalin test: a quantitative study of the analgesic effects of morphine, meperidine and brain stem stim- ulation in rats and cats. Pain 4:161-174.

Fanselow MS (1991) The midbrain periaqueductal gray as a coordi- nator of action in response to fear and anxiety. In: The midbrain periaqueductal gray matter: functional, anatomical, and neurochemi- cal organization (Depaulis A, Bandler R, eds), pp 151-173. New York: Plenum.

Fields HL, Heinricher MM, Mason P (1991) Neurotransmitters in no- ciceptive modulatory circuits. Annu Rev Neurosci 14:219-245.

Fox RJ, Sorenson CA (1994) Bilateral lesions of the amygdala atten- uate analgesia induced by diverse environmental challenges. Brain Res 648:215-221.

Graeff FG (1988) Animal models of aversion. In: Selected models of anxiety, depression and psychosis (Simon P, Soubrie P, Wildlocher D, eds), pp 115-l 41. Basel: Karger.

Hargreaves K, Dubner R, Brown F, Flores C, Joris J (1988) A new and sensitive method for measuring thermal nociception in cutaneous hyperalgesia. Pain 32:77-88.

Hastings MH, Winn P, Dunnett SB (1985) Neurotoxic amino acid le- sions of the lateral hypothalamus: a parametric comparison of the effects of ibotenate, N-methyl-o,L-aspartate and quisqualate in the rat. Brain Res 360:248-256.

Hayes RL, Price DD, Bennett GJ, Wilcox GL, Mayer DJ (1978) Dif- ferential effects of spinal cord lesions on narcotic and non-narcotic suppression of nociceptive reflexes: further evidence for the physio- logic multiplicity of pain modulation. Brain Res 155:91-101.

Helmstetter FJ (1992) The amygdala is essential for the expression of conditional hypoalgesia. Behav Neurosci 106:5 18-528.

Helmstetter FJ, Bellgowan PS (1993) Lesions of the amygdala block conditional hypoalgesia on the tail flick test. Brain Res 612:253-257.

Helmstetter FJ, Bellgowan PS, Tershner SA (1993) Inhibition of the tail flick reflex following microinjection of morphine into the amyg- dala. Neuroreport 4:471474.

Hopkins DA, Holstege G (1978) Amygdaloid projections to the mes- encephalon, pons and medulla oblongata in the cat. Exp Brain Res 321529-547.

Jensen TS, Yaksh TL (1986a) I. Comparison of antinociceptive action of morphine in the periaqueductal gray, medial and paramedial me- dulla in rat. Brain Res 363:99-l 13.

Jensen TS, Yaksh TL (1986b) II. Examination of spinal monoamine receptors through which brainstem opiate-sensitive systems act in the rat. Brain Res 363: 114-127.

Kalivas PW, Gau BA, Nemeroff CB, Prange AJ Jr (1982) Antinoci- ception after microinjection of neurotensin into the central amygda- loid nucleus of the rat. Brain Res 243:279-286.

Klamt JG, Prado WA (1991) Antinociception and behavioral changes induced by carbachol microinjected into identified sites of the rat brain. Brain Res 549:9-18.

Kohler C, Schwartz R (1983) Comparison of ibotenate and kainate neurotoxicity in rat brain: a histological study. Neuroscience 8:819- 835.

LeDoux JE, Iwata J, Cicchetti P, Reis DJ (1988) Different projections

Page 15: The Central Nucleus of the Amygdala Contributes to the ... · amjgdaloid areas: GP, globus pallidus; CP, caudate-putamen. in rats with bilateral, excitotoxin-induced lesions of the

The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1995, 15(12) 8213

of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral correlates of conditioned fear. J Neurosci 8:2517-2529.

Levine JD, Taiwo YO (1989) Involvement of the mu-opiate receptor in peripheral analgesia. Neuroscience 32571-575.

Lichtman AH, Fanselow MS (1991) Opioid and nonopioid conditional analgesia: the role of spinal opioid, noradrenergic and serotonergic systems. Behav Neurosci 105:6877698.

Manning BH, Mayer DJ (1995) The central nucleus of the amygdala contributes to the production of morphine antinociception in the for- malin test. Pain 63:141-152.

Manning BH, Morgan MJ, Franklin K (1994) Morphine analgesia in the formalin test: evidence for forebrain and midbrain sites of action. Neuroscience 63:289-294.

Mansour A, Khachaturian H, Lewis ME, Akil H, Watson SJ (1987) Autoradiographic differentiation of mu, delta and kappa opioid re- - _ ceptors in the rat forebrain and midbrain. J Neurosci 7:2445-2464.

Mao J. Price DD. Maver DJ (1994) Thermal hvoeralaesia in associa- tion’ with the development ‘of morphine tole&ce Tn rats: roles of excitatory amino acid receptors and protein kinase C. J Neurosci 14: 2301-2312.

Martin JH (199 1) Autoradiographic estimation of the extent of revers- ible inactivation produced by microinjection of lidocaine and mus- cimol in the rat. Neurosci Lett 127:160-164.

Matthies BK, Franklin K (1992) Formalin pain is expressed in decer- ebrate rats but not attenuated by morphine. Pain 51:199-206.

Mayer DJ (1979) Endogenous analgesia systems: neural and behavioral mechanisms. In: Advances in pain research and therapy, Vol 3 (Bon- ica JJ, Liebeskind JC, Albe-Fessard DC, eds), pp 385-410. New York: Raven.

Mayer DJ, Manning BH (1995) The role of opioid peptides in envi- ronmentally-induced analgesia. In: The pharmacology of opioid pep- tides (Tseng LF, ed), pp 345-395. Churi Switzerland! Harwood.- -

Maver DJ. Price DD (1976) Central nervous svstem mechanisms of analgesia. Pain 2:379-404:

Mayer DJ, Wolfle TL, Akil H, Carder B, Liebeskind JC (1971) Anal- gesia from electrical stimulation in the brainstem of the rat. Science 174:1351-1354.

McLean S, Rothman RB, Herkenham M (1986) Autoradiographic lo- calization of lo- and a-opiate receptors in the forebrain of the rat. Brain Res 378:49-60.

Oliveira MA, Prado WA (1994) Antinociception and behavioral man- ifestations induced by intracerebroventricular or intra-amygdaloid ad- ministration of cholinergic agonists in the rat. Pain 57:383-391.

Paxinos G, Watson C (1986) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates, Vol 2. New York: Academic.

Proudfit HK, Anderson EG (1975) Morphine analgesia: blockade by raphe magnus lesions. Brain Res 98:612-618.

Proudfit HK, Levy RA (I 978) Delimitation of neuronal substrates nec- essary for the analgesic action of baclofen and morphine. Eur J Phar- macol 47: 159-166.

Proudfit HK, Yeomans DC (1995) The modulation of nociception by enkephalin-containing neurons in the brainstem. In: The pharmacol- ogy of opioid peptides (Tseng LE ed), pp 197-217. Chur, Switzer- land: Harwood.

Rizvi TA, Ennis M, Behbehani MM, Shipley MT (1991) Connections between the central nucleus of the amygdala and the midbrain peri- aqueductal gray: topography and reciprocity. J Comp Neurol 303: 121-131.

Rodgers RJ (1977) Elevation of aversive threshold in rats by intra- amygdaloid injection of morphine sulphate. Pharmacol Biochem Be- hav 6:385-390.

Rodgers RJ (1978) Influence of intra-amygdaloid opiate injections on shock thresholds, tail-flick latencies and open field behaviour in rats. Brain Res 153:211-216.

Sandktihler J. Maisch B. Zimmermann M (1987) The use of local anaesthetic microinjections to identify central pathways: a quantita- tive evaluation of the time course and extent of the neuronal block. Exp Brain Res 68:168-178.

Tseng LE Wang Q (1992) Forebrain sites differentially sensitive to B-endorphin and morphine for analgesia and release of met-enkeph- alin in the pentobarbital-anesthesized rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 261: 1028-1036.

Watkins LR, Wiertelak EP, Maier SF (1993) The amygdala is necessary for the expression of conditioned but not unconditioned analgesia. Behav Neurosci 107:402-405.

Werka T (1994) Post-stress analgesia in rats with partial amygdala lesions. Acta Neurobiol Exp 54: 127-132.

Werka T, Marek P (1990) Post-stress analgesia after lesions to the central nucleus of the amygdala in rats. Acta Neurobiol Exp 50: 13- 22.

Winn P Clark A, Hastings M, Clark J, Latimer M, Rugg E, Brownlee B (1990) Excitotoxic lesions of the lateral hypothalamus made by N-methyl-D-aspartate in the rat: behavioural, -histological and bio- chemical analvses. EXD Brain Res 82:628-636.

Winn P, Stone TW, Labmer M, Hastings MH, Clark AJM (1991) A comparison of excitotoxic lesions of the basal forebrain by kainate, quinolinate, ibotenate, N-methyl-D-aspartate or quisqualate, and the effects on toxicity of 2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid and kynurenic acid in the rat. Br J Pharmacol 102:904-908.

Wolgin DL (1985) Forelimb placing and hopping reflexes in haloper- idol- and morphine- treated cataleptic rats. Behav Neurosci 99:423- 435.

Yaksh TL (1979) Direct evidence that spinal serotonin and noradren- aline terminals mediate the spinal antinociceptive effects of morphine in the periaqueductal gray. Brain Res 160: 180-l 85.

Yaksh TL, Rudy TA (1977) Studies on the direct spinal action of nar- cotics in the production of analgesia in the rat. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 202:41 l-428.

Yaksh TL, Yeung JC, Rudy TA (1976) Systematic examination in the rat of brain sites sensitive to the direct application of morphine: ob- servation of differential effects within the periaqueductal gray. Brain Res 114:83-103.

Yaksh TL, Plant RL, Rudy TA (1977) Studies on the antagonism by raphe lesions of the antinociceptive action of systemic morphine. Eur J Pharmacol 41:399+08.

Yaksh TL, Al-Rodhan NRE Jensen TS (1988) Sites of action of opiates in production of analgesia. In: Progress in brain research, Vol 77 (Fields HL, Besson J-M, eds), pp 371-394. New York: Elsevier.