reviewarticle - wordpress.com › 2014 › 03 › article-evidence-based.… · 4...

17
Hindawi Publishing Corporation Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine Article ID 345835 Review Article Are There Benefits from Teaching Yoga at Schools? A Systematic Review of Randomized Control Trials of Yoga-Based Interventions C. Ferreira-Vorkapic, 1,2,3 J. M. Feitoza, 2 M. Marchioro, 1 J. Simões, 1 E. Kozasa, 4,5 and S. Telles 6 1 Department of Physiology, Laboratory of Neurophysiology, Federal University of Sergipe (UFS), Avenida Marechal Rondon, s/n, Jardim Rosa Elze, S˜ ao Crist´ ov˜ ao, 49100-000 Aracaju, SE, Brazil 2 Department of Psychology, FASE\UNESA, Aracaju, SE, Brazil 3 Trika Research Center, Loei, ailand 4 Hospital Israelita Albert Einstein, S˜ ao Paulo, Brazil 5 Department of Psychobiology, Federal University of S˜ ao Paulo, S˜ ao Paulo, Brazil 6 Indian Council of Medical Research Center for Advanced Research in Yoga and Patanjali Research Foundation, Bengaluru, India Correspondence should be addressed to C. Ferreira-Vorkapic; [email protected] Received 1 March 2015; Revised 21 June 2015; Accepted 25 June 2015 Academic Editor: Vernon A. Barnes Copyright © C. Ferreira-Vorkapic et al. is is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Introduction. Yoga is a holistic system of varied mind-body practices that can be used to improve mental and physical health and it has been utilized in a variety of contexts and situations. Educators and schools are looking to include yoga as a cost-effective, evidence-based component of urgently needed wellness programs for their students. Objectives. e primary goal of this study was to systematically examine the available literature for yoga interventions exclusively in school settings, exploring the evidence of yoga- based interventions on academic, cognitive, and psychosocial benefits. Methods. An extensive search was conducted for studies published between 1980 and October 31, 2014 (PubMed, PsycInfo, Embase, ISI, and the Cochrane Library). Effect size analysis, through standardized mean difference and Hedges’g, allowed for the comparison between experimental conditions. Results and Conclusions. Nine randomized control trials met criteria for inclusion in this review. Effect size was found for mood indicators, tension and anxiety in the POMS scale, self-esteem, and memory when the yoga groups were compared to control. Future research requires greater standardization and suitability of yoga interventions for children. 1. Introduction Yoga is an ancient mind-body practice which originated in India more than 2.000 years ago and is described system- atically early on (Patanjali’s Yoga Sutras, circa 900 B.C.). Although, according to traditional scriptures, its ultimate goal is to achieve a unified state of consciousness and self- realization, yoga may be used to improve overall health and well-being [1]. Yoga involves different techniques such as physical postures (asanas), controlled breathing (pranaya- mas), deep relaxation (yoganidra), and meditation [1]. ese techniques seem to have specific influences on one’s psycho- logical state [2, 3] and the research on the psychophysiological benefits of yoga and meditation on adults has demonstrated improvements in emotional self-regulation with consequent reductions in depression, stress, anxiety levels [211] and posttraumatic disorder [12] as well as improvements in mood [13], quality of life, and well-being [1416]. However, in spite of the positive effects of yoga on mental health, this practice is not just limited to therapeutic use and has been utilized in a variety of situations and contexts, including educational and school settings, where teaching students about wellness and health accompanies the primary aim of academic instruction [17]. According to United Nations [18], children and adoles- cents around the world spend an average of 10 to 15 years

Upload: others

Post on 28-Jan-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • Hindawi Publishing CorporationEvidence-Based Complementary and Alternative MedicineArticle ID 345835

    Review ArticleAre There Benefits from Teaching Yoga at Schools? A SystematicReview of Randomized Control Trials of Yoga-BasedInterventions

    C. Ferreira-Vorkapic,1,2,3 J. M. Feitoza,2 M. Marchioro,1 J. Simões,1 E. Kozasa,4,5 and S.Telles6

    1Department of Physiology, Laboratory of Neurophysiology, Federal University of Sergipe (UFS), Avenida Marechal Rondon, s/n,Jardim Rosa Elze, São Cristóvão, 49100-000 Aracaju, SE, Brazil2Department of Psychology, FASE\UNESA, Aracaju, SE, Brazil3Trika Research Center, Loei, Thailand4Hospital Israelita Albert Einstein, São Paulo, Brazil5Department of Psychobiology, Federal University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil6Indian Council of Medical Research Center for Advanced Research in Yoga and Patanjali Research Foundation, Bengaluru, India

    Correspondence should be addressed to C. Ferreira-Vorkapic; [email protected]

    Received 1 March 2015; Revised 21 June 2015; Accepted 25 June 2015

    Academic Editor: Vernon A. Barnes

    Copyright © C. Ferreira-Vorkapic et al.This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License,which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

    Introduction. Yoga is a holistic system of varied mind-body practices that can be used to improve mental and physical health andit has been utilized in a variety of contexts and situations. Educators and schools are looking to include yoga as a cost-effective,evidence-based component of urgently neededwellness programs for their students.Objectives.Theprimary goal of this studywas tosystematically examine the available literature for yoga interventions exclusively in school settings, exploring the evidence of yoga-based interventions on academic, cognitive, and psychosocial benefits. Methods. An extensive search was conducted for studiespublished between 1980 and October 31, 2014 (PubMed, PsycInfo, Embase, ISI, and the Cochrane Library). Effect size analysis,through standardized mean difference and Hedges’g, allowed for the comparison between experimental conditions. Results andConclusions. Nine randomized control trials met criteria for inclusion in this review. Effect size was found for mood indicators,tension and anxiety in the POMS scale, self-esteem, and memory when the yoga groups were compared to control. Future researchrequires greater standardization and suitability of yoga interventions for children.

    1. Introduction

    Yoga is an ancient mind-body practice which originated inIndia more than 2.000 years ago and is described system-atically early on (Patanjali’s Yoga Sutras, circa 900 B.C.).Although, according to traditional scriptures, its ultimategoal is to achieve a unified state of consciousness and self-realization, yoga may be used to improve overall health andwell-being [1]. Yoga involves different techniques such asphysical postures (asanas), controlled breathing (pranaya-mas), deep relaxation (yoganidra), and meditation [1]. Thesetechniques seem to have specific influences on one’s psycho-logical state [2, 3] and the research on the psychophysiological

    benefits of yoga and meditation on adults has demonstratedimprovements in emotional self-regulation with consequentreductions in depression, stress, anxiety levels [2–11] andposttraumatic disorder [12] as well as improvements in mood[13], quality of life, and well-being [14–16]. However, in spiteof the positive effects of yoga on mental health, this practiceis not just limited to therapeutic use and has been utilized in avariety of situations and contexts, including educational andschool settings, where teaching students about wellness andhealth accompanies the primary aim of academic instruction[17].

    According to United Nations [18], children and adoles-cents around the world spend an average of 10 to 15 years

  • 2 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine

    at school. As a result, schools hold the potential to teachabout healthy habits from an early age and promote children’shealth and well-being. For children who have to deal withstressors, anxiety, traumas, abuse, learning disabilities, andeven bullying, the discipline developed by practicing con-templative techniques could be the difference between failureand success, both in their professional and personal life.[19]. Furthermore, according to Noggle et al. [20], the age ofonset of most mental health disorders in adults occurs duringchildhood and adolescence, with around 7.5% of adolescentsmeeting DSM-IV-TR criteria for one or more mental healthconditions.

    The solution for dealingwith stressors, anxiety, and learn-ing disabilities certainly depends on many factors; however,evidence suggests that some of these problems may be easedby mind-body practices, which have been shown to redirectattention, improve concentration, increase self-control, andprovide people with reliable and healthy coping mechanisms[19]. Yet, the efficacy of such practices among children isunclear and evidence is insufficient. A review conductedby Galantino et al. [21] found that there was evidence forthe benefit of yoga in the pediatric population in physicalrehabilitation, but a recent meta-analysis concluded that thedata on the clinical applications of yoga among the childrenare uncertain [22]. Authors state that while most studies weresuggestive of benefits, results were based on low quality trials.

    Studies utilizing yoga in school settings have been shownto benefit children and adolescents [17]. According to Khalsaet al. [23], a yoga program might help children recover theirself-esteem and confidence, restore their mental health, pro-mote positive attitudes, improve concentration, and reducestress and anxiety. Unfortunately, traditional curricula focusprimarily on intellectual development, and schools have pro-gressively been losing the capacity to adopt health-focusedprograms. The ability to cope with stress and anxiety (due topsychosocial demands) and to maintain physical and mentalhealth is priceless in any spheres of an individual’s life,including education. Students must be healthy in order tolearn, and academic accomplishment has been shown to berelated to health status. Consequently, there is urgent needto develop and investigate cost-effective and evidence-basedwellness programs that can be delivered in school settings.

    Therefore, the primary goal of this study was to systemat-ically examine the available literature on yoga interventionsexclusively in school settings. The objective of this reportwas to reviewmethodological quality among selected studies,exploring the evidence of yoga-based interventions regardingacademic, cognitive, and psychosocial benefits, and to con-tribute to the study of low-cost, health-focused alternativeprograms for children and adolescents in school settings.

    2. Methods

    2.1. Searching. Studies were identified by searching PubMed,PsycInfo, Embase, ISI, and the Cochrane Library. A widesearch was conducted for studies published between 1980 andOctober 31, 2014, using the following terms or key words:

    yoga, school, education, and children alone and in combina-tion with additional terms such as program, intervention, andyoga-based. A manual evaluation of reference lists of relevantstudies and reviews was also conducted. All articles relatedto the subject yoga at schools were selected for additionalexamination.

    2.2. Selection. Peer-reviewed, published manuscripts wereconsidered. Studieswere selected if (1) they included a yoga oryoga-based intervention, (2) the intervention was restrictedto school settings (integrated into the school schedule or afterclass), (3) they included children and adolescents (ages 5–18),(4) they included an evaluation of anxiety, depression, stress,or other psychological measures such as mood indicators,self-esteem, confidence, and quality of life at both preinter-vention and postintervention, (5) they included the assess-ment of academic or cognitive performance as a consequenceof the yoga intervention (pre- and postintervention), (6)the research designs were pilot studies, quasi-experimentaldesigns, or randomized designs and included control groupswith no interventions or an active control (comparativeintervention), and (7) they were written in English. Exclusioncriteria comprised (1) studies that utilized only meditationor relaxation techniques without the physical componentssuch as postures (asana) or controlled breathing (pranayama)(MBSR based programs usually have yoga as part of thepractice but only MBSR studies in which yoga is highlightedor is the main component of the program were included inthis review whose intention is to cover primarily yoga), (2)yoga-based programs for children with learning disabilitiesor any diagnosed mental disorder, and (3) dissertations andconference presentations.

    Authors screened abstracts to identify articles that meetinclusion criteria. Potential articles were then evaluatedfor inclusion. To conduct the study, all data was collectedand evaluated in terms of selection criteria, procedure,participants, intervention, methodology, assessment tools,and outcomes. Follow-up and results were also assessed. Toevaluate or discuss non-RCTs was not part of the review’sscope. Other reviews have mentioned non-RCTs, includingmethodology and possible bias [17]. The review has beenprepared using preferred reporting criteria for systematicreview guidelines (PRISMA) [24].

    The quality and reliability of the randomized controltrials (RCTs) were evaluated according to the evidence levelsrecommended by the Oxford Center for Evidence-BasedMedicine [25]. Table 1 shows the classification of scientificevidence in systematic literature reviews. Four authors con-ducted the literature searching (C. Ferreira-Vorkapic, M.Marchioro, and S. Telles) and study selection (C. Ferreira-Vorkapic, M. Marchioro, and E. Kozasa).

    After the first appraisal, another examiner (J. Simões)evaluated the selected RCTs and kappa coefficient was cal-culated in order to estimate reliability of data collectionmethods. The observed kappa score of 𝑟 = 0.9 suggests greatconsistency of agreement between investigators.

    For the effect size calculation, the means and standarddeviations (postintervention) from experimental and control

  • Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 3

    Study extraction flow diagram

    Records identified through database searching

    Additional records identified

    Total records

    Studies excluded— special issues or

    reviews

    Studies excluded due to interventions or

    outcomes

    Full-text articles assessed for eligibility

    Full-text articles excluded—uncontrolled and not

    randomized or not randomized

    Studies included in qualitative synthesis

    Studies included in quantitative synthesis

    (meta-analysis)

    Studies excluded—dissertations

    Studies excluded—

    Full-text articles Full-text articles Full-text articlesexcluded—qualitative

    study excluded—

    insufficient data excluded—not

    randomized trials

    (n = 15) (n = 33)

    (n = 26)

    (n = 48)

    (n = 8)

    (n = 8)

    (n = 9)

    (n = 9)

    (n = 3) (n = 3) (n = 3)

    (n = 7) (n = 5)(n = 2)

    Iden

    tifica

    tion

    Scre

    enin

    gEl

    igib

    ility

    Inclu

    ded

    through other sources∗

    unpublished∗

    Figure 1: Criteria for inclusion in the review. ∗Articles sent to the authors by other researchers.

    Study or subgroup Mean SD Total Mean SD Total Weight

    Noggle et al., 2012 35.42 6.27 36 40.72 9.51 15 9.9%Khalsa et al., 2012 5.2 70 1.69 5.8 34 22.1%Hagins et al., 2013 82.21 5.2 15 83.68 2.28 15 7.3%Telles et al., 2013 14.96 2.41 49 14.94 3.09 49 24.3%Verma et al., 2014 6.01 1.49 37 5.62 1.8 34 17.4%Sarokte and Rao, 2014 14.97 6.58 30 11.4 6.56 30 14.3%Haden et al., 2014 24.93 1.96 14 21.58 2.12 12 4.7%

    Total (95% CI) 251 189 100.0%

    Std. mean difference Std. mean differenceIV, fixed, 95% CI IV, fixed, 95% CI

    0 1 2Favours yoga Favours control

    Favours yoga Favours control

    −0.97

    Heterogeneity:𝜒2 = 28.40, df = 6 (P < 0.0001); I2 = 79%Test of overall effect: Z = 0.11 (P = 0.91)

    −2 −1

    0.01 [−0.39, 0.40]0.23 [−0.23, 0.70]0.54 [0.02, 1.05]1.59 [0.69, 2.50]

    −0.71 [−1.33, −0.09]−0.49 [−0.90, −0.07]−0.36 [−1.08, 0.37]

    −0.01 [−0.21, 0.18]

    Figure 2: Plot of the general effect size in the selected studies.

    groups were obtained directly from the studies. The com-parison between experimental conditions was carried outafter the analysis of the pooled effect size by the genericinverse variance method (random effect model) throughstandardized mean difference and Hedges’ g. Hedges’ g effect

    is the difference between the two means (postinterventionfor the experimental and the control groups) divided by thepooled standard deviation. Ninety-five percent confidenceintervals were computed for all variables.

  • 4 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine

    Study or subgroup Mean SD Total Mean SD Total Weight

    −3.55 15.1 67 1.27 19.4 34 69.0%38.4 16.9 36 51.2 20.1 15 31.0%

    Total (95% CI) 103 49 100.0%

    Yoga Control Std. mean differenceIV, fixed, 95% CI

    Std. mean differenceIV, fixed, 95% CI

    0 1 2−2 −1Favours yoga Favours control

    Heterogeneity:𝜒2 = 1.21, df = 1 (P = 0.27); I2 = 17%Test of overall effect: Z = 2.37 (P = 0.02)

    −0.29 [−0.70, 0.13]−0.71 [−1.32, −0.09]−0.42 [−0.76, −0.07]

    Noggle et al., 2012Khalsa et al., 2012

    Figure 3: Profile of Mood States (POMS) general score effect size.

    Study or subgroupMean SD Total Mean SD Total

    Weight

    −0.48 3.8 67 −0.03 4.7 34 70.1%5.1 3.6 36 9.3 5.8 15 29.9%

    Total (95% CI) 103 49 100.0%

    Yoga Control Std. mean difference Std. mean differenceIV, fixed, 95% CI IV, fixed, 95% CI

    0 1 2−2 −1Favours yoga Favours control

    Heterogeneity:𝜒2 = 4.79, df = 1 (P = 0.03); I2 = 79%Test of overall effect: Z = 2.04 (P = 0.04)

    −0.11 [−0.52, 0.30]−0.95 [−1.58, −0.32]−0.036 [−0.71, −0.01]

    Noggle et al., 2012Khalsa et al., 2012

    Figure 4: Profile of Mood States (POMS): subitems tension and anxiety effect size.

    Table 1: Classification of scientific evidence in systematic literaturereviews according to the evidence levels recommended by theOxford Center for Evidence-Based Medicine.

    Level of evidence Grading criteria

    1a Systematic reviews of RCTs includingmeta-analysis.

    1b Individual RCT with narrow confidenceinterval.1c Case of series “all or nothing.”2a Systematic review of cohort studies.

    2b Individual cohort study and low qualityRCT.2c Outcome research study.3a Systematic review of case control studies.3b Individual case control study.

    4 Case series, poor quality cohort, and casecontrol studies.5 Expert’s opinion.Adapted from levels of evidence of the Oxford Center for Evidence-BasedMedicine [62].

    Two studies [19, 26] were excluded from the effect sizeanalysis due to insufficient data (mean values not provided).

    3. Results

    3.1. Description of Studies. Of the 48 studies identified, 9randomized control trials met criteria for inclusion in thisreview, as illustrated by Figure 1 (PRISMA Guidelines StudyExtraction) [24]. Table 2 shows the PRISMA 2009 checklist.Selected randomized control trials (RCTs) are displayed inTable 3.

    Generally, the RCTs had low AHRQ evidence scores,with most studies receiving a score of 2b due to the lack

    of blindness and follow-up, reflecting the reduced quality ofreporting in these studies.

    All results and conditions of the studies are summarizedin Table 3.

    Due to the limited number of RCTs and the greatheterogeneity of the variables (ununiform constructs), theanalysis of the effect size of specific measures could only beperformed on studies that observed the same variables suchas mood, tension, anxiety [20, 23], self-esteem [23, 27], andmemory [28, 29] when comparing the yoga to control groups.

    After an overall effect size calculation of each study(except for [19, 26]), the effect sizes obtained from similarmeasures were grouped together: mood, tension, anxiety,self-esteem, and memory. Figure 2 shows the forest plot ofthe general effect size in the selected studies.The general plotshows divided results with half of the studies favoring yogaand the other half favoring control. 𝑃 value for overall effectis not significant (𝑃 = 0.91), but this is probably due toheterogeneity of the variables.

    Effect size from mood state indicators (POMS) wascalculated fromKhalsa et al., 2012 [23], andNoggle et al., 2012[20]. Results indicate that the yoga group showed significantlybetter scores in the postintervention condition (𝑃 = 0.02)(Figure 3). The same scale shows a second significant effectfor the subitems tension and anxiety also after the yogapractice (𝑃 = 0.04) (Figure 4).

    A third comparison was carried out for the variable self-esteem [23, 27]. Results show greater self-esteem perceptionin the postintervention condition for the yoga group (𝑃 =0.04) (Figure 5).

    Effect size for memory was also analyzed in [28] andVerma et al., 2014 [29]. Both studies utilized memory assess-ment tools, but Sarokte and Rao [28] used two differentinstruments. The results show increased memory perfor-mance for the yoga group (𝑃 < 0.00001) (Figure 6).

  • Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 5

    Table2:PR

    ISMA2009

    checklist.

    Section/topic

    #Ch

    ecklist

    item

    Repo

    rted

    onpage

    #Title

    Title

    1Are

    ThereB

    enefitsfro

    mTeaching

    Yoga

    atScho

    ols?ASyste

    maticRe

    view

    ofRa

    ndom

    ized

    Con

    trolT

    rialsof

    Yoga-Based

    Interventio

    ns.

    1Ab

    stract

    Structured

    summary

    2

    Background

    .Yogaisa

    holistic

    syste

    mof

    varie

    dmind-bo

    dypractic

    esthatcanbe

    used

    toim

    provem

    entaland

    physicalhealth.D

    ueto

    the

    well-k

    nownresto

    rativ

    eeffectso

    fyogao

    nmentalh

    ealth

    ,ith

    asbeen

    utilizedin

    avarietyof

    contextsandsituatio

    nsbeyond

    thes

    tand

    ard

    therapypractic

    e.Ed

    ucatorsa

    ndscho

    olsinparticular

    areloo

    king

    toinclu

    deyoga

    asac

    ost-e

    ffective,evidence-based

    compo

    nent

    ofurgently

    needed

    wellnessp

    rogram

    sfor

    theirstudents.Objectives.Th

    eprim

    arygoalof

    thisstu

    dywas

    tosyste

    maticallyexam

    inethe

    availableliterature

    fory

    ogainterventions

    exclu

    sively

    inscho

    olsetting

    s.Th

    eobjectiv

    eofthisreportw

    asto

    explorethe

    evidence

    ofyoga-based

    interventio

    nson

    academ

    ic,cognitiv

    e,andpsycho

    socialbenefits.Methods.Studies

    wereidentified

    bysearchingPu

    bMed,PsycInfo,Em

    base,ISI,and

    the

    CochraneL

    ibrary.A

    nextensives

    earchwas

    cond

    uctedforstudies

    publish

    edbetween1980

    andOctob

    er31,2014,usingthefollowingterm

    sor

    keywords,yoga,school,edu

    catio

    n,andchild

    ren,alon

    eand

    incombinatio

    nwith

    additio

    naltermssuchas

    program,intervention,and

    yoga-based.R

    esultsandCo

    nclusio

    ns.Forty-eight

    studies

    wereidentified

    andnine

    rand

    omized

    controltria

    lsmetcriteria

    forinclusio

    nin

    this

    review

    .Alth

    ough

    mosto

    fthe

    studies

    werec

    lassified

    as2b,according

    toAHRQ

    evidence

    levelcriteria,w

    hich

    means

    lowqu

    ality

    rand

    omized

    controltria

    ls,thisreview

    suggestsbeneficialeffectso

    fyoga-basedinterventio

    nsatscho

    olon

    both

    psycho

    logicaland

    cogn

    itive

    functio

    ns.

    Effectsizew

    asfoun

    dform

    oodindicators,tensio

    nandanxietyin

    theP

    OMSscale,self-esteem

    ,and

    mem

    ory.Fu

    ture

    research

    requ

    ires

    greaterstand

    ardizatio

    nandmustd

    ealw

    iththep

    roblem

    ofapprop

    riateness:w

    hattypeo

    fyoga-basedinterventio

    nismostsuitablefor

    child

    renandatwhatfrequ

    ency

    anddu

    ratio

    nandob

    served

    varia

    bles.

    1

    Intro

    duction

    Ratio

    nale

    3

    Yoga

    isah

    olisticsyste

    mof

    varie

    dmind-bo

    dypractic

    esthatcanbe

    used

    toim

    provem

    entaland

    physicalhealth.D

    ueto

    thew

    ell-kno

    wn

    resto

    rativ

    eeffectso

    fyogao

    nmentalh

    ealth

    ,ith

    asbeen

    utilizedin

    avarietyof

    contextsandsituatio

    nsbeyond

    thes

    tand

    ardtherapypractic

    e.Ed

    ucatorsa

    ndscho

    olsinparticular

    areloo

    king

    toinclu

    deyoga

    asac

    ost-e

    ffective,evidence-based

    compo

    nent

    ofurgentlyneeded

    wellness

    programsfor

    theirstudents.How

    ever,there

    isno

    critically

    appraisedevidence

    such

    assyste

    maticreview

    sonpo

    tentialbenefitsof

    yoga-based

    interventio

    nsin

    scho

    olsetting

    s.

    2

    Objectiv

    es4

    Theo

    bjectiv

    eofthisreportw

    asto

    review

    metho

    dologicalqualityam

    ongselected

    studies,explorin

    gthee

    videnceo

    fyoga-based

    interventio

    nsregardingacadem

    ic,cognitiv

    e,andpsycho

    socialbenefits,andto

    contrib

    utetothes

    tudy

    oflow-cost,health-fo

    cused

    alternativep

    rogram

    sfor

    child

    renandadolescentsinscho

    olsetting

    s.2

    Methods

    Eligibility

    criteria

    5

    Peer-reviewed,pub

    lishedmanuscriptswerec

    onsid

    ered.Studies

    weres

    electedif(1)theyinclu

    deday

    ogao

    ryoga-basedinterventio

    n,(2)the

    interventio

    nwas

    restric

    tedto

    scho

    olsetting

    s(integrated

    into

    thes

    choo

    lscheduleo

    rafte

    rclass),(3)theyinclu

    dedchild

    renandadolescents

    (ages5

    –18),(4)

    they

    inclu

    dedan

    evaluatio

    nof

    anxiety,depressio

    n,stress,oro

    ther

    psycho

    logicalm

    easuressuchas

    moo

    dindicators,

    self-esteem

    ,con

    fidence,and

    quality

    oflifea

    tbothpreinterventionandpo

    stinterventio

    n,(5)theyinclu

    dedthea

    ssessm

    ento

    facademicor

    cogn

    itive

    perfo

    rmance

    asac

    onsequ

    ence

    ofthey

    ogaintervention(pre-a

    ndpo

    stinterventio

    n),(6)

    ther

    esearchdesig

    nswerep

    ilotstudies,

    quasi-e

    xperim

    entald

    esigns,orrando

    mized

    desig

    nsandinclu

    dedcontrolgroup

    swith

    nointerventio

    nsor

    anactiv

    econ

    trol(comparativ

    einterventio

    n),and

    (7)theywerew

    rittenin

    English

    .Exclusio

    ncriteria

    comprise

    d(1)studies

    thatutilizedon

    lymeditatio

    nor

    relaxatio

    ntechniqu

    eswith

    outthe

    physicalcompo

    nentssuchas

    postu

    res(asana)

    orcontrolledbreathing(pranayama)

    (MBS

    Rbasedprogramsu

    sually

    have

    yoga

    aspartof

    thep

    ractice,bu

    tonlyMBS

    Rstu

    dies

    inwhich

    yoga

    ishigh

    lighted

    oristhem

    aincompo

    nent

    ofthep

    rogram

    were

    inclu

    dedin

    thisreview

    ,who

    seintentionisto

    coverp

    rimarily

    yoga),(2)y

    oga-basedprogramsfor

    child

    renwith

    learning

    disabilitieso

    rany

    diagno

    sedmentald

    isorder,and

    (3)d

    issertatio

    nsandconference

    presentatio

    ns.

    5

  • 6 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine

    Table2:Con

    tinued.

    Section/topic

    #Ch

    ecklist

    item

    Repo

    rted

    onpage

    #

    Inform

    ation

    sources

    6

    Literature

    searches

    werec

    ondu

    cted

    inPu

    bMed,PsycInfo,Em

    base,ISI,and

    theC

    ochraneL

    ibrary

    (1980–2014).A

    nextensives

    earchwas

    cond

    uctedforstudies

    publish

    edbetween1980

    andOctob

    er31,2014,usingthefollowingterm

    sork

    eywords,yoga,school,edu

    catio

    n,and

    child

    ren,alon

    eand

    incombinatio

    nwith

    additio

    naltermssuchas

    program,intervention,andyoga-based.O

    ngoing

    registe

    redclinicaltria

    lsweren

    otsearched.Th

    edetailsforthe

    fullsearch

    strategy

    werelisted

    inafl

    owdiagram,assho

    wnin

    Figure

    1.

    5

    Search

    7Pu

    bMed,E

    MBA

    SE,and

    theC

    ochraneC

    entralRe

    giste

    rofC

    ontro

    lledTrials(C

    ENTR

    AL)

    intheC

    ochraneL

    ibrary

    5

    Stud

    yselection

    8Fo

    urauthorsc

    ondu

    cted

    theliterature

    searching(C

    .Ferreira

    -Vorkapic,M.M

    archioro,and

    S.Telles)andstu

    dyselection(C

    .Ferreira-Vorkapic,M.M

    archioro,and

    E.Ko

    zasa).

    5

    Datac

    ollection

    process

    9

    Authors(C.

    Ferreira-Vorkapic,M.M

    archioro,S.Telles,andE.

    Kozasa)screenedabstractstoidentifyarticlesthatm

    eetinclusio

    ncriteria

    .Po

    tentialarticlesw

    erethenevaluatedforinclusio

    n.To

    cond

    uctthe

    study,alldatawas

    collected

    andevaluatedin

    term

    sofsele

    ctioncriteria

    ,procedure,participants,

    interventio

    n,metho

    dology,assessm

    enttoo

    ls,andou

    tcom

    es.Follow-upandresults

    werea

    lsoassessed.Th

    ereview

    hasb

    eenprepared

    usingpreferredrepo

    rtingcriteria

    forsystematicreview

    guidelines

    (PRISM

    A).J.M.Feitoza

    hasp

    erform

    edallthe

    statistics(effectsize).For

    thee

    ffectsiz

    ecalculatio

    n,them

    eans

    andsta

    ndarddeviations

    (postin

    terventio

    n)fro

    mexperim

    entaland

    control

    grou

    pswereo

    btaineddirectlyfro

    mthes

    tudies.

    5

    Dataitems

    10Yo

    ga,schoo

    l,education,

    andchild

    ren.

    5

    Risk

    ofbias

    inindividu

    alstu

    dies

    11Afterthe

    firstappraisal,anothere

    xaminer

    (J.Simões)evaluatedthes

    electedRC

    Tsandak

    appa

    coeffi

    cientw

    ascalculated

    inordertoestim

    ate

    reliabilityof

    datacollectionmetho

    ds.Th

    eobservedkapp

    ascore

    of𝑟=0.9suggestsgreatcon

    sistencyof

    agreem

    entb

    etweeninvestigators.

    6

    Effectsize

    calculation

    12

    Thec

    omparis

    onbetweenexperim

    entalcon

    ditio

    nswas

    carriedou

    tafte

    rthe

    analysisof

    thep

    ooledeffectsizeb

    ytheg

    enericinversev

    ariance

    metho

    d(rando

    meffectm

    odel)throu

    ghsta

    ndardizedmeandifferencea

    ndHedges’g.Ninety-fivep

    ercent

    confi

    denceintervalswere

    compu

    tedfora

    llvaria

    bles.Twostu

    dies

    [19,26]

    weree

    xcludedfro

    mthee

    ffectsiz

    eanalysis

    duetoinsufficientd

    ata(

    meanvalues

    not

    provided).Th

    eanalysis

    ofthee

    ffectsiz

    eofspecific

    measuresc

    ould

    onlybe

    perfo

    rmed

    onstu

    dies

    thatob

    served

    thes

    amev

    ariables

    such

    asmoo

    d,tensionandanxiety[20,23],self-esteem

    [23,27],andmem

    ory[28,29]w

    hencomparin

    gthey

    ogatocontrolgroup

    s.

    6

    Summary

    measures

    13

    Theq

    ualityandreliabilityof

    ther

    ando

    mized

    controltria

    ls(RCT

    s)weree

    valuated

    accordingto

    thee

    videncelevelsrecom

    mendedby

    the

    OxfordCenterfor

    Evidence-Based

    Medicine(AHRQ

    ,2002).Th

    eitemsincludedstu

    dyqu

    estio

    n,stu

    dypo

    pulation,rand

    omization,blinding

    ,interventio

    ns,outcomes,statistic

    alanalysis,

    results,disc

    ussio

    n,andfund

    ingsource.Th

    equalityof

    allthe

    inclu

    dedtrialswas

    categoriz

    edinto

    levelsof

    evidence

    varyingfro

    m1to5,accordingto

    low,

    uncle

    ar,orh

    ighris

    kof

    bias.Th

    equalityandreliabilityof

    ther

    ando

    mized

    controltria

    ls(RCT

    s)weree

    valuated

    accordingto

    thee

    videncelevelsrecom

    mendedby

    theO

    xfordCenterfor

    Evidence-Based

    Medicine.

    Table1

    show

    sthe

    classificatio

    nof

    scientifice

    videnceinsyste

    maticliteraturer

    eviews

    6

  • Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 7

    Table2:Con

    tinued.

    Section/topic

    #Ch

    ecklist

    item

    Repo

    rted

    onpage

    #Results

    Stud

    yselection

    14

    Aflo

    wchartd

    epictedthes

    earchprocessa

    ndstu

    dyselection(Figure1).Ofthe

    48stu

    dies

    identifi

    ed,9

    rand

    omized

    controltria

    lsmetcriteria

    forinclusio

    nin

    thisreview

    .Generally,

    theR

    CTsh

    adlowAHRQ

    evidence

    scores,w

    ithmoststudies

    receivingas

    core

    of2b

    duetothelackof

    blindn

    essa

    ndfollo

    w-up,reflectingther

    egular

    quality

    ofrepo

    rtingin

    theses

    tudies.A

    llresults

    andcond

    ition

    softhe

    studies

    ares

    ummarized

    inTable3

    .

    6

    Stud

    ycharacteris

    tics

    15

    Them

    etho

    dologicalqualityof

    mostincludedtrialswas

    generally

    redu

    ced.Th

    edetailsarea

    ssho

    wnin

    Table3

    .Ther

    ando

    mized

    allocatio

    nof

    participantswas

    mentio

    nedin

    alltria

    ls.Blinding

    inform

    ation,

    however,w

    asinsufficientd

    ueto

    then

    atureo

    fthe

    study

    (inyoga

    trials

    practitionerb

    linding

    isim

    possible).Interventio

    nsinclu

    dedyoga

    oryoga-based

    programsc

    ondu

    cted

    exclu

    sively

    inscho

    olsetting

    sfor

    child

    renrang

    ingfro

    m7to

    17years.Com

    paris

    ongrou

    psinclu

    dedph

    ysicaleducation,

    Ayurvedictreatment,andcontrol.Cognitiv

    eand

    psycho

    logicalfun

    ctions

    werea

    ssessedandthetotaltre

    atmentd

    urationrang

    edfro

    m8to

    18weeks.

    6

    Risk

    ofbias

    with

    instu

    dies

    16Th

    enum

    bero

    ftria

    lswas

    toosm

    alltocond

    uctany

    sufficientadd

    ition

    alanalysisof

    publicationbias.

    6

    Effectsize

    results

    17

    Afte

    ranoveralleffectsiz

    ecalculatio

    nof

    each

    study

    (exceptfor

    Ramadossa

    ndBo

    se,2010[19],and

    White,2012[26]),thee

    ffectsiz

    esob

    tained

    from

    similarm

    easuresw

    ereg

    roup

    edtogether:m

    ood,tension,

    anxiety,self-esteem

    ,and

    mem

    ory.Figure

    2show

    sthe

    forestplot

    oftheg

    eneraleffectsizeinthes

    elected

    studies.Th

    egeneralplot

    show

    sdivided

    results

    with

    halfof

    thes

    tudies

    favorin

    gyoga

    andtheo

    ther

    half

    favorin

    gcontrol.𝑃valuefor

    overalleffectisno

    tsignificant(𝑝=0.91),bu

    tthisisp

    robablydu

    etoheterogeneity

    ofthev

    ariables.

    Effectsizefrom

    moo

    dsta

    teindicators(POMS)

    was

    calculated

    from

    Khalsa

    etal.,2012

    [23],and

    Nogglee

    tal.,2012

    [20].R

    esultsindicatethat

    they

    ogag

    roup

    show

    edsig

    nificantly

    bette

    rscoresinthep

    ostin

    terventio

    ncond

    ition

    (𝑝=0.02)(Figure

    3).Th

    esam

    escaleshow

    sasecond

    significanteffectforthe

    subitemstensio

    nandanxietyalso

    after

    they

    ogap

    ractice(𝑝=0.04)(Figure

    4).

    Athird

    comparis

    onwas

    carriedou

    tfor

    thev

    ariables

    elf-este

    em[23,27].Re

    sults

    show

    greaterself-este

    emperceptio

    nin

    thep

    ostin

    terventio

    ncond

    ition

    forthe

    yoga

    grou

    p(𝑝=0.04)(Figure

    5).

    Effectsizefor

    mem

    oryw

    asalso

    analyzed

    inSaroktea

    ndRa

    o2013,[28]and

    Verm

    aetal.,2014

    [29].B

    othstu

    dies

    utilizedmem

    orya

    ssessm

    ent

    tools,bu

    tSarokteandRa

    o2013,[28]u

    sedtwodifferent

    instr

    uments.

    Ther

    esultsshow

    increasedmem

    oryperfo

    rmance

    forthe

    yoga

    grou

    p(𝑝<0.00001)(Figure

    6).

    7

    Synthesis

    ofresults

    18

    Regardingthee

    ffectso

    fyogao

    npsycho

    logicalw

    ell-being

    ,ofthe

    sixstu

    dies,three

    ofthem

    supp

    ortthe

    benefitso

    fyogao

    ryoga-based

    programsfor

    child

    renin

    scho

    olsetting

    s.Khalsa

    etal.[23]o

    bservedthatyoga

    participantsshow

    edsta

    tistic

    allysig

    nificantd

    ifferenceso

    ver

    timer

    elativ

    etocontrolson

    measureso

    fanger

    controland

    fatig

    ue/in

    ertia

    .Nogglee

    tal.[20]

    also

    observed

    preventiv

    ebenefitsin

    psycho

    socialwell-being

    (anx

    ietyandnegativ

    eaffect)for

    studentse

    nrolledin

    ayogap

    rogram

    .InRa

    madossa

    ndBo

    se[19],onlythey

    oga

    grou

    pdemon

    stratedas

    light

    decrease

    instresswhilemaintaining

    self-control.In

    contrast,

    Haden

    etal.,2014

    [30]

    andWhite[26]

    observed

    asig

    nificantincreaseinperceivedstr

    essinthey

    ogag

    roup

    comparedto

    thep

    hysic

    aleducationandcontrolgroup

    s,respectiv

    ely.H

    owever,in

    White[26],bothgrou

    ps(yogaa

    ndcontrol)repo

    rted

    significantly

    greaterself-este

    emandself-regulationover

    time.Lastly,

    Haginse

    tal.[32]

    foun

    dthatyoga

    didno

    treduces

    tressreactiv

    itycomparedto

    aphysic

    aleducationcla

    sswhenstu

    dentsw

    eres

    ubmitted

    tostressor

    tasks.

    Effectsizew

    asfoun

    dform

    oodindicators,anx

    ietyandtension(POMS),self-este

    em,and

    mem

    ory.Th

    eRCT

    ofeach

    articleisdescrib

    edin

    detailin

    thetext.

    7

  • 8 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine

    Table2:Con

    tinued.

    Section/topic

    #Ch

    ecklist

    item

    Repo

    rted

    onpage

    #Discussio

    n

    Summaryof

    evidence

    19

    Alth

    ough

    mosto

    fthe

    studies

    werec

    lassified

    as2b,according

    toAHRQ

    evidence

    levelcriteria,w

    hich

    means

    lowqu

    ality

    rand

    omized

    control

    trials,

    thisreview

    show

    sbeneficialeffectsof

    yoga-based

    interventio

    nsatscho

    olon

    both

    psycho

    logicaland

    cogn

    itive

    functio

    ns(effectsiz

    ewas

    foun

    dform

    oodindicators,anx

    ietyandtension,

    self-esteem

    andmem

    ory),but

    then

    egativee

    ffectso

    fyogaw

    erea

    lsoob

    served

    insome

    ofthes

    tudies

    andmight

    beexplainedto

    somee

    xtentb

    ythea

    daptationprocessb

    ychild

    ren,

    thea

    bsence

    ofattentionalcon

    trol,andthe

    inadequacy

    ofyoga

    practic

    efor

    child

    ren.

    Future

    research

    requ

    iresg

    reater

    stand

    ardizatio

    nandmustd

    ealw

    iththep

    roblem

    ofapprop

    riateness:w

    hattypeo

    fyoga-basedinterventio

    nismostsuitablefor

    child

    renandatwhatfrequ

    ency

    anddu

    ratio

    nandob

    served

    varia

    bles.

    16

    Limitatio

    ns20

    Not

    onlyisthen

    umbero

    fRCT

    slow

    ,but

    also

    thetria

    lswereo

    freduced

    metho

    dology

    quality

    andhadris

    kof

    bias

    interm

    sofd

    esign,

    repo

    rting,andmetho

    dology.Itisc

    omprehensib

    lethatitisdifficultto

    perfo

    rmdo

    uble-blin

    ding

    studies

    with

    yoga,but

    blinding

    tothe

    outcom

    eassessorsanddataanalyzer

    couldbe

    feasibleandhasn

    otbeen

    repo

    rted.O

    nelim

    itatio

    nof

    thisreview

    isthatitwas

    notp

    ossib

    leto

    calculatethe

    effectsizeo

    fallvaria

    bles

    observed

    inthes

    elected

    studies

    duetotheirh

    eterogeneity.

    16

    Con

    clusio

    ns21

    Thisreview

    analyzed

    nine

    peer-reviewed

    RCTstu

    dies,inwhich

    yoga

    was

    taug

    htto

    child

    renin

    aschoo

    lsettin

    g.Outcomem

    easuresincluded

    psycho

    logicalw

    ell-b

    eing

    andcogn

    itive

    functio

    ns,suchas

    attentionandmem

    ory.Eff

    ectsizew

    asfoun

    dform

    oodindicators,anx

    ietyand

    tension(POMS),self-este

    em,and

    mem

    ory.Whilesupp

    ortiv

    einmanystu

    dies,the

    utilityof

    yoga

    ineducationalsettin

    gsisinconclusiv

    edue

    tothes

    malln

    umbero

    frando

    mized

    controltria

    lsin

    theliterature.E

    venthou

    ghon

    lyRC

    Tswerer

    eviewed,m

    etho

    dologicaland

    statistic

    alprob

    lemsm

    ight

    have

    contrib

    uted

    totheu

    ncertainty:inadequ

    ates

    amples

    izes,absence

    ofcontrolgroup

    s,varia

    bilityin

    thetypeo

    fyoga

    beingtaug

    ht,lon

    gdu

    ratio

    nof

    yoga

    sessions,inapp

    ropriatepsycho

    metric

    toolsfor

    child

    ren,

    andfailu

    reto

    measure

    interveningvaria

    bles

    such

    asmindfulnessandbo

    dyaw

    areness,which

    areimpo

    rtantp

    artsof

    yoga

    practic

    e.Th

    isreview

    suggestsvaluableeffectsof

    yoga-based

    interventio

    nsatscho

    olon

    both

    psycho

    logicalstatusa

    ndcogn

    itive

    functio

    nin

    somes

    tudies

    butfuturer

    esearchrequ

    iresg

    reater

    standardizationandmustd

    ealw

    iththep

    roblem

    ofapprop

    riateness;w

    hattypeo

    fyoga-basedinterventio

    nismostsuitablefor

    child

    ren,

    specifically

    interm

    softhe

    frequ

    ency

    anddu

    ratio

    n?

    22

    Fund

    ing

    Fund

    ing

    22Th

    isresearch

    was

    fund

    edby

    theF

    APITE

    CAgencyun

    derP

    rocessno

    .7838.UNI321.21944

    .25062013.

    22Ad

    aptedfro

    m[24].

  • Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 9

    Table3:Selected

    rand

    omized

    controltria

    ls(RCT

    s).

    Stud

    ySample

    Program

    Interventio

    nVa

    riables

    Evaluatio

    ntools

    Results

    Evidence

    level

    Haginse

    tal.,2013∗

    [32]

    Middle

    scho

    olstu

    dents,ages

    11to

    12(𝑛=30)

    VinyasaY

    oga

    15weeks

    50minutes,three

    times

    aweek

    Yoga

    orph

    ysical

    education(c)

    Bloo

    dpressure

    (BP),

    heartrate(HR),and

    behavioralstr

    essor

    tasks(mental

    arith

    meticand

    Mirr

    orTracing

    Tasks).

    Automated

    bloo

    dpressure

    cuff,

    MentalA

    rithm

    eticTask

    (MAT

    ),andMirr

    orTracing

    Task

    (MTT

    ).

    Therew

    eren

    osig

    nificant

    differences

    betweengrou

    ps.

    2b(not

    doub

    leblinded,no

    follo

    w-up)

    Haden

    etal.,

    2014∗

    [30]

    Middle

    scho

    olstu

    dents,ages

    11to

    12(𝑛=30)

    Ashtanga

    Yoga

    12weeks

    90minutes,three

    times

    aweek

    Yoga

    orph

    ysical

    education(c)

    Emotional(affect

    andself-perceptio

    ns)

    andbehavioral

    varia

    bles

    (internalizingand

    externalizing

    prob

    lemsa

    ndaggressio

    n).

    PANAS,Ch

    ildBe

    havior

    Checklist

    (CBC

    L),R

    evise

    dParent

    Ratin

    gScalefor

    Reactiv

    eand

    Proactive

    Aggression(R-PRA

    ),andthe

    Self-Perceptio

    nProfi

    lefor

    Child

    ren(SPP

    C).

    Therew

    eren

    osig

    nificantchanges

    betweengrou

    psin

    self-repo

    rted

    positivea

    ffect,globalself-w

    orth,

    aggressio

    nindices,or

    parent

    repo

    rts

    oftheirc

    hildren’s

    externalizingand

    internalizingprob

    lems.How

    ever,

    negativ

    eaffectincreasedfortho

    sechild

    renparticipatingin

    yoga

    when

    comparedto

    theP

    Eprogram.

    2b(in

    sufficient

    blinding

    ,no

    follo

    w-up)

    Khalsa

    etal.,2012

    [23]

    Highscho

    olstu

    dents,ages

    12to

    13(𝑛=136)

    Yoga

    Ed11weeks

    30to

    40minutes,two

    tothreetim

    esaw

    eek

    Yoga

    orph

    ysical

    education(c)

    Moo

    d,anxiety,

    perceivedstr

    ess,

    resilience,andother

    mentalh

    ealth

    varia

    bles.

    TheS

    elf-Reporto

    fPersonality

    (SRP

    )versio

    nof

    theB

    ehavior

    AssessmentSurveyfor

    Child

    renVe

    rsion2(BASC

    -2),

    POMS,theR

    esilience

    Scale

    (RS),and

    theP

    erceived

    Stress

    Scale(PS

    S).

    Yoga

    participantsshow

    edstatisticallysig

    nificantd

    ifferences

    over

    timer

    elativetocontrolson

    measureso

    fanger

    controland

    fatig

    ue/in

    ertia

    .Mosto

    utcome

    measurese

    xhibitedap

    attern

    ofworsening

    inthec

    ontro

    lgroup

    over

    time,whereas

    changesinthey

    oga

    grou

    pover

    timew

    eree

    ither

    minim

    alor

    show

    edslight

    improvem

    ents.

    2b(not

    doub

    leblinded,no

    follo

    w-up)

    Nogglee

    tal.,2012

    [20]

    Highscho

    olstu

    dents,ages

    16to

    17(𝑛=51)

    KripaluYo

    ga10

    weeks

    30minutes,twoto

    threetim

    esaw

    eek

    Yoga

    orph

    ysical

    education(c)

    Psycho

    logical

    well-b

    eing

    ,moo

    dandnegativ

    eaffect

    (prim

    arymeasures),

    stress,resilience,

    anger,and

    acceptance

    (secon

    dary

    measures).

    Prim

    aryou

    tcom

    es:P

    rofileo

    fMoo

    dStates-Sho

    rtFo

    rmand

    Positivea

    ndNegativeA

    ffect

    Schedu

    leforC

    hildren.

    Second

    aryou

    tcom

    es:Stre

    ssScalea

    ndInventoryo

    fPositive

    Psycho

    logicalA

    ttitudes,

    ResilienceS

    cale,

    StateT

    rait

    Anger

    Expressio

    nInventory-2,andCh

    ildAc

    ceptance

    Mindfulness

    Measure.

    Moo

    dim

    proved

    inyoga

    and

    worsenedin

    controls.

    Negative

    affectw

    orsenedin

    controlsand

    improved

    inyoga.

    2b(not

    doub

    leblinded,no

    follo

    w-up)

  • 10 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine

    Table3:Con

    tinued.

    Stud

    ySample

    Program

    Interventio

    nVa

    riables

    Evaluatio

    ntools

    Results

    Evidence

    level

    Ramadoss

    andBo

    se,

    2010

    [19]

    Highscho

    olstu

    dents,ages

    orgrades

    not

    inform

    ed(𝑛=472)

    Niro

    gaTL

    S18

    weeks

    15minutes,from

    oneto

    fivetim

    esaw

    eek

    Yoga

    once

    aweek,yoga

    twicea

    week,

    yoga

    three

    times

    aweek,

    yoga

    five

    times

    aweek,

    oraw

    aitin

    glistgroup

    (c).

    Stressand

    self-control.

    PerceivedStressScale(PS

    S-10)

    andTang

    ney’s

    Self-Con

    trol

    Scale(TS

    CS-13).

    Theinterventiongrou

    pdemon

    strated

    aslight

    decrease

    instr

    essa

    ndmaintenance

    inself-control.In

    contrast,

    thec

    ontro

    lgrou

    p,which

    received

    nocla

    sses,

    demon

    strated

    nosig

    nificantchange

    instressandan

    onsig

    nificanttrend

    towarddeterio

    ratio

    nof

    self-control.

    2b(not

    doub

    leblinded,no

    follo

    w-up)

    White,

    2012∗

    [26]

    Elem

    entary

    scho

    olstu

    dents,ages

    9to

    11(girls)

    (𝑛=155)

    TheM

    indful

    Awareness

    forG

    irlsthrou

    ghYo

    ga8weeks

    60minutes,oncea

    week(10-minute

    homew

    orkforthe

    other

    6days

    ofthew

    eek)

    Yoga

    orno

    interventio

    n(c).

    Stress,cop

    ing

    strategies,

    self-esteem

    ,and

    mindfulness.

    TheF

    eelB

    adScale,the

    Scho

    olagers’Cop

    ingStrategies

    Inventory,andtheH

    ealth

    ySelf-Re

    gulationSubscaleof

    theM

    indful

    Thinking

    and

    Actio

    nScalefor

    Adolescents.

    Nosig

    nificantd

    ifferencesb

    etween

    grou

    pswerefou

    nd.

    Overtim

    e,theinterventiongrou

    pwas

    morelikely

    than

    thec

    ontro

    lgrou

    pto

    repo

    rtstr

    ess

    2b(not

    doub

    leblinded,no

    follo

    w-up)

    Saroktea

    ndRa

    o,2013

    [28]

    Middlea

    ndhigh

    scho

    olstu

    dents,ages

    10to

    16(𝑛=90)

    Hatha

    Yoga

    3mon

    ths

    40minutes,every

    day

    asho

    mew

    ork

    Medhya

    Rasayana

    (Ayurveda

    treatment),

    Yoga,orn

    ointerventio

    n(c)

    Executivefun

    ctions

    andmentalstatus.

    Short-term

    mem

    orytest,

    pictures,serialeffectstest,

    words,and

    minim

    entalstate

    scale.

    Group

    Bshow

    edhigh

    lysig

    nificant

    andmosteffectivec

    hanges

    inshort-term

    mem

    orytestpictures

    andseria

    lrecalleffectstestu

    sing

    mem

    oryscop

    e.Group

    Cshow

    edhigh

    lysig

    nificantand

    mosteffective

    changesw

    ithrespecttosubjectiv

    eandob

    jectivep

    aram

    etersinmini

    mentalstatusscale.

    2b(not

    doub

    leblinded,no

    follo

    w-up)

    Tellese

    tal.,

    2013

    [27]

    Elem

    entary

    andmiddle

    scho

    olstu

    dents,ages

    8to

    13(𝑛=98)

    Hatha

    Yoga

    3mon

    ths

    45minutes,five

    times

    aweek

    Yoga

    orph

    ysical

    activ

    ity(c)

    Self-esteem

    ,attention,

    and

    physicalfitness.

    Stroop

    color-wo

    rdtask

    for

    child

    ren,

    Battle’s

    self-esteem

    inventory,andtheteachers’

    ratin

    gof

    thec

    hildren’s

    obedience,academ

    icperfo

    rmance,atte

    ntion,

    punctuality,and

    behavior

    with

    friend

    sand

    teachers.

    Teachers’ratingof

    thec

    hildren’s

    behavior.

    Socialself-esteem

    :↑PA

    grou

    pStroop

    :↑bo

    thgrou

    ps(higher

    interfe

    renceintheP

    Agrou

    p).

    Total,general,andparental

    self-esteem

    improved

    inthey

    oga

    grou

    p.Bo

    thgrou

    psshow

    edan

    increase

    inBM

    Iand

    numbero

    fsit-ups.B

    alance

    worsenedin

    the

    physicalexercise

    grou

    p,whileplate

    tapp

    ingim

    proved

    inthey

    ogag

    roup

    .

    2b(not

    doub

    leblinded,no

    follo

    w-up)

    Verm

    aetal.,

    2014

    [29]

    Middlea

    ndhigh

    scho

    olstu

    dents,ages

    11to

    15(𝑛=82)

    Hatha

    Yoga

    12weeks

    45minutes,five

    times

    aweek

    Yoga

    and

    control

    grou

    psCognitiv

    efun

    ctions.

    MentalA

    bilityTestand

    Batte

    ryandMem

    oryTest.

    Sign

    ificant

    improvem

    entw

    asob

    served

    inmeasureso

    fmental

    abilityandmem

    oryin

    experim

    ental

    grou

    p

    2b(not

    doub

    leblinded,no

    follo

    w-up)

    Negativee

    ffectso

    nstr

    essa

    ndaffecto

    rnosig

    nificance

    betweengrou

    ps.

  • Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 11

    Study or subgroup Mean SD Total Mean SD Total Weight

    −0.01 5.2 70 0.6 6.6 30 47.3%Telles et al., 2014 33.51 6.46 49 36.98 4.82 49 52.7%Total (95% CI) 119 79 100.0%

    Yoga Control Std. mean difference Std. mean differenceIV, fixed, 95% CI IV, fixed, 95% CI

    0 1 2−2 −1Favours yoga Favours control

    Heterogeneity:𝜒 2= 2.73, (P = 0.10); I2 = 63%Test of overall effect: Z = 2.46 (P = 0.01)

    −0.11 [−0.54, 0.32]−0.60 [−1.01, −0.20]−0.37 [−0.66, −0.07]

    Khalsa et al., 2012

    df = 1

    Figure 5: Self-esteem effect size.

    Study or subgroup Mean SD Total Mean SD Total Weight

    Sarokte and Rao, 2014 20.73 3.02 30 22.63 2.54 30 32.9%

    Sarokte and Rao, 2014 12.13 3.09 30 17.07 2.79 30 25.5% 0.6000

    5.57 2.39 37 6.83 2.73 37 41.7% 0.4600

    Total (95% CI) 97 97 100.0%

    Yoga Control Std. mean difference Std. mean differenceIV, fixed, 95% CI IV, fixed, 95% CI

    0 1 2Favours yoga Favours control

    −2 −1Heterogeneity: 𝜒2 = 9.96, (P < 0.007); I 2= 80%Test of overall effect: Z = 5.55 (P < 0.00001)

    −0.67 [−1.19, −0.15]

    −1.66 [−2.25, −1.06]

    −0.49 [−0.95, −0.02]

    −0.85 [−1.14, −0.55]Verma et al., 2014

    df = 2

    Figure 6: Memory effect size.

    Since the number of selected RCTs is reduced and otherinteresting results were observed during the review process(that could not provide for an effect size calculation), suchas negative effects or different variables, the studies areindividually described in the following sections in order toprovide details, similarities, and differences between them.

    3.2. Outcome Analysis

    3.2.1. Yoga and Psychological Well-Being. Six RCT studiesobserved the effects of a yoga program on psychological well-being in school settings.

    In [30], authors sought to determine the effects of yogaon children’s emotional and behavioral variables using acomparison group consisting of physical education (PE)classes. Thirty middle school children were randomized toparticipate in either a school-based ashtanga yoga programor PE class three times a week for 12 weeks. Yoga classes con-sisted of opening rituals (3–7min), asana practice (30min),seated meditation (2–5min), and guided relaxation (4min).

    Emotional and behavioral functioning was measuredbefore and after intervention utilizing the following tools:Positive and Negative Affect Scales (PANAS), the ChildBehavior Checklist (CBCL), the Revised Parent Rating Scalefor Reactive and Proactive Aggression (R-PRA), and the Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC). Results show nosignificant changes between groups in self-reported posi-tive affect, global self-worth, aggression indices, or parents’reports of their children’s externalizing and internalizingproblems. However, negative affect increased for those chil-dren participating in yoga when compared to the PE pro-gram. Authors offer a few explanations for these findings: (1)the first contact with yoga may be demanding for children

    at this age (sixth grade) and as a result may increase stresslevels in the short term; (2) the “dose” and type of yoga maynot have been satisfactory; (3) one of the outcomes of yogapractice may be greater self-awareness and mindfulness andthese variables were not assessed in the current study; and (4)the sample size was small.

    Khalsa et al., 2012 [23], evaluated mental health bene-fits of yoga for adolescents in secondary school. Studentswere randomly assigned either to regular physical educationclasses (PE) or to 11 weeks of yoga practice based on the YogaEd program. Typical 30-minute Yoga Ed sessions included5-minute initial relaxation and breathing techniques, 5-minute warm-up, 15min of yoga poses, and 5-minute closingrelaxation. Each session also had a theme that was discussedby the instructor, such as yoga philosophy and method-ology, nonviolence, mind-body interactions and awareness,body systems, stress management, emotional intelligence,and similar topics. Psychological well-being was assessedusing the Self-Report of Personality (SRP) version of theBehavior Assessment Survey for Children Version 2 (BASC-2), the Profile of Mood States-Short Form (POMS-SF), theResilience Scale (RS), the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), and theInventory of Positive Psychological Attitudes-32R (IPPA).

    Outcome measures revealed that yoga participantsshowed statistically significant differences over time relativeto controls on measures of anger control and fatigue/inertia.Most outcome measures exhibited a pattern of worseningin the control group over time, whereas changes in theyoga group over time either were minimal or showed slightimprovements. Authors explain that while statisticallysignificant differences between groups were found for onlya few outcome measures, each of these favored the yogagroup. Onmost measures, findings suggested relatively smallpositive effects in the yoga group but marked declines in the

  • 12 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine

    control group. A few study limitations such as inadequatepsychometric tools and short duration of the program mightbe responsible for the observed outcomes.

    Noggle et al. [20] observed the effects of a yoga programonpsychologicalwell-being, psychological attitudes, and self-regulatory skills on 51 high school students.The overall scopeof this study was to evaluate the psychological benefits ofa yoga program conducted within the school curriculumfor adolescents and compare them to the regular physicaleducation classes (PE). This differed from Khalsa et al.,2012 [23], as Noggle et al. [20] focused on scales developedfor normative adolescent populations and based their inter-vention on a different yoga style (Kripalu Yoga). Studentsparticipated in both interventions two to three times a weekfor 30minutes. Yoga practice consisted of 5-minute centeringand breathing exercises, 5-minute warm-up, 15 minutes ofyoga postures/exercises, and 5-minute closing relaxation.The authors stated that the yoga intervention developedfor this study might become a standardized Kripalu-basedprogram for use in research within the school setting. After10 weeks of program, students attending physical educationclasses showed decreases in primary outcomes (Profile ofMood States-Short Form and Positive and Negative AffectSchedule for Children). Yoga students were maintained orimproved. Totalmooddisturbance improved in yoga studentsand worsened in controls as did Profile of Mood States-Short Form (POMS-SF) Tension-Anxiety subscale. Althoughpositive affect remained unchanged in both, negative affectsignificantly worsened in controls (PE), while it improved inyoga students. Secondary outcomes, such as the ResilienceScale, the State Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2, and theChild Acceptance Mindfulness Measure were not significant.While the study has specific limitations regarding the real-world applicability of an education setting (sample size,group-randomized trial, observer bias, and minor irregulari-ties), results suggest preventive benefits in psychosocial well-being from the Kripalu yoga program at school.

    Ramadoss and Bose, 2010 [19], observed the effects of ayoga-based transformative life skills program (TLS) on stresslevels and self-control of 387 high school students. The con-trol group did not receive the TLS protocol, while among theintervention groups students received one, two, or five classesperweek. Each yoga session lasted 15minutes and included anorienting opening bell, focused breathing (pranayama), silentsittingmeditation, sun salutations, posture (asana), rhythmicbreathing, silent sitting, and a closing bell.Outcomemeasureswere stress and self-control, evaluated by the Perceived StressScale and Tangney’s Self-Control Scale. Results show thatthe intervention group demonstrated a slight decrease instress and maintenance in self-control. In contrast, in thecontrol group, there was no significant change in stress and anonsignificant trend toward deterioration of self-control.Thestudy indicates an improvement in stress, self-control, andself-awareness after 18 weeks of a yoga program, comparedto a control group. Although the study has specific strengthssuch as qualitative feedback from students and teachers,an appropriate sample, and a condensed and effective yogapractice (which might have been the reason for the positiveoutcomes), however, blinding protocol was not clear and

    authors could have explored other psychological variables,such as anxiety anddepression, since the studywas conductedwith vulnerable teens.

    White [26] investigated the efficacy of eight-week mind-fulness training through yoga with 155 middle school girls(mean age of 9.9 years) on stress and coping abilities, self-esteem, and self-regulation. A randomized group designrandomly assigned two public schools to either the inter-vention or wait-list control group. The experimental groupmet one day per week, for one hour, and completed 10minutes of yoga homework six days a week. The MindfulAwareness for Girls through Yoga program, utilized in thisstudy, was adapted from Kabat-Zinn’s Mindfulness BasedStress Reduction (MBSR) [31] and focused on the yogaportion. The 10-minute practice comprised ringing tingshabells, breathing\sitting meditation, yoga warm-up, postures,and comments. Psychometric tools consisted of the FeelBad Scale, the Schoolagers’ Coping Strategies Inventory, theGlobal Self-Worth Subscale of the Self-Perception Profile forChildren, and the Healthy Self-Regulation Subscale of theMindful Thinking and Action Scale for Adolescents.

    Authors reported no significant differences betweengroups. In addition, over time, the intervention group wasmore likely than the control group to report higher perceivedstress scores (although no increase in the frequency ofstressors was found) and greater frequency of coping withstress. As the authors state, as self-awareness progresseswith yoga practice, children may become aware of difficultemotions. This may lead to increased perceived stress at firstbut subsequently may result in better means to cope withsuch feelings. Both groups reported significantly greater self-esteem and self-regulation. According to the authors, thenegative outcomes regarding perceived stress might be dueto (1) use of inadequate psychometric tools, (2) the fact thatawareness of stress might have facilitated coping and that thisincreased awareness of stress might also have precipitatedmore stress, and (3) the fact that increase in stress mightbe transient and part of the process of becoming mindful.Study limitations include a homogeneous sample (only girls),the quality of intervention (due to a large sample size), and,especially, the fact that a greater part of the practice was doneas homework and children were unattended during most ofthe time.

    Lastly, in a 15-week study, Hagins et al. [32] examined theeffects of yoga compared to a physical education class (PE)on physiological responses [based on the blood pressure (BP)and heart rate (HR)] to behavioral stressor tasks such asMen-tal Arithmetic Task (MAT) and Mirror Tracing Task (MTT)on 30middle school students. After initial screening, studentswere randomly assigned to either PE classes or yoga course.During 15 weeks, students participated in yoga program orPE classes. Both interventions occurred three times a weekand each session lasted approximately 50 minutes. The yogaclass consisted of an opening ritual (centering and consciousbreathing), asana practice (posture), seated meditation, andguided relaxation.

    The authors stated that the two behavioral stressor tasks(Mental Arithmetic Task and Mirror Tracing Task) utilizedin this investigation had been previously used successfully in

  • Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 13

    studies of stress reactivity in children. The MAT consisted ofsimple arithmetic counts during a specific period of time. Inthe MTT, participants had to trace a star using only a mirrorversion of the star for guidance. Participants had to trace thestar asmany times as possible without any errors during threeminutes. Systolic and diastolic BP and HR were obtainedduring the tests through an automated blood pressure cuff.The procedures and measures in pre- and posttests were thesame. Pretesting occurred one to twoweeks before the start ofthe intervention and posttesting occurred one to two weeksafter the final class. After data analysis, authors concluded thatthe yoga program did not reduce stress reactivity comparedto a physical education class. Furthermore, statistical analysiscomparing the first stressors (MAT versusMTT) used duringthe pretest found that there were no significant differencesin BP or HR values relative to the type of stressor used.Besides, the difficulty in finding differences in BP orHR in theparticipantsmight be due to their good health, especially aftersuch a short time intervention. The authors concluded thatthe results do not support the idea that benefits from yoga arederived from a mechanism related to increased regulation ofthe autonomic nervous system. As in other studies, the resultsobserved here may be related to the way yoga practice wasapplied and a failure to directly address the issue of reactionto perceived stress.

    3.2.2. Yoga and Cognitive Function. Three RCT studiesobserved the effects of a yoga program on different cognitivefunctions, such as attention, memory, and developmentalabilities, in school settings.

    A three-month study by Sarokte and Rao [28] observedthe effects of a yoga program and Ayurvedic medicine (Med-hya Rasayana) on mental state and cognitive functions of 90students between the ages of 10 and 16. Medhya Rasayana(which means “a preparation which prevents mental andintellectual disorders” in Sanskrit) was administered everyday in the morning and evening and students in the yogagroup were asked to perform yogic practices regularly ina specific order. The practice included postures (asanas),breathing techniques, and meditation. The frequency orduration of each individual practice is not mentioned. In thecontrol group, no intervention or treatment was given. Thegroup treated withMedhya Rasayana showed significant andmost significant changes in the objective variables measured(short-term memory test pictures and serial recall effectstest).The yoga group showed significant changes with respectto subjective and objective parameters in mini mental statusscale. The first follow-up also shows a greater positive impactin the Ayurveda group when compared to the yoga group.The authors concluded that Medhya Rasayana brings fasterimprovements inmemory when compared to yoga. However,the yoga group was asked to perform yogic practices but itis not certain if these children were really practicing yogaat home, and if so, the duration and frequency are unclear.Although the administration of the Ayurvedic drug was alsonot controlled (Medhya Rasayana was given twice a day,possibly by the parents), it is much easier to have 12-year-old boys taking medicine than performing yogic practices,

    which takes discipline, effort, and time. Yet, the study showsinteresting results, demonstrating that both interventionsmight be useful to improve cognitive functions.

    Telles et al. [27] observed the effects of yoga or physicalexercise on physical fitness, cognitive performance, self-esteem, and teacher-rated behavior and performance in 98school children between the ages of 8 and 13. After initial ran-domization and group allocation, participants were assessedfor physical fitness (Eurofit physical fitness test), performancein the Stroop task (the Stroop color-word naming task), self-esteem, and analog scales (attention, punctuality, behaviorwith friends, and behavior with teachers) rated by the teach-ers. Both groups were assessed at the end of twelve weeks.Yoga practice involved pranayamas (yoga breathing tech-niques), sithilikarana vyayama (loosening exercises), asanas(postures), chanting, and yoga relaxation techniques andlasted three months with a frequency of five times a week (45minutes per day, during school hours). Physical exercise hadthe same time and frequency and consisted of jogging in placeas well as bending and spinal twists. In this study, authorsplaced an emphasis on the differences between yoga andphysical exercise, with this difference being the importanceof awareness, relaxation, and breathing regulation in yoga.

    After testing for the differences between groups, socialself-esteem was the only variable with significant changes,increasing in the physical exercise group. pre and postvalues within each group also showed significant changes incognitive function. In the Stroop task, both groups showed anincrease in word scores, color scores, and color-word scores.The physical exercise group showed reduced interference rawscores and an increase in interference scores. In addition,both groups showed an improvement in obedience, academicperformance, attention, punctuality, behavior with friends,and behavior with teachers.

    The authors suggest that the improved scores might bedue to better aerobic fitness (observed in both groups). Also,the increase in interference T scores in the physical exercisegroup suggests reduced flexibility and ability to respond tothe task demands after this intervention. Physical activity andyoga also separately improved emotional well-being in bothgroups, but the underlying mechanisms are not clear. Thestudy has limitations such as the fact that the yoga and thephysical exercise programs had to fit in the school schedule,which could have produced differences in outcomes. Therewas also no follow-up.

    Finally, Verma et al., 2014 [29] observed significantimprovements in measures of mental ability and memory inan experimental yoga group of high school students, from11 to 15 years of age, randomly divided into a yoga and acontrol group (𝑛 = 82). All children were tested beforeand after the 12-week intervention (or control), using thetest battery of Cognition Function tests (CFTs), an Indianadaptation based on Guilford’s Structure of Intellect Model.Yoga sessions were conducted for 45min, five days a weekwithin the school setting. The practices included chanting,asanas (postures), and pranayamas (breathing techniques).Significant improvement was observed inmeasures ofmentalability and memory only in the experimental group, specifi-cally cognitive processes such as attention, perception, and

  • 14 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine

    observation. The authors suggest that the observed results(on memory scores) indicate that yoga affected only primaryprocessing of visual inputs. Yoga intervention was wellaccepted, particularly due to its short duration,making it easyto be successfully incorporated into the school curriculum.

    4. Discussion

    This review systematically examined the literature on yoga inschool settings, exploring the evidence of yoga-based inter-ventions on different psychological variables and cognitivefunctions.

    Forty-eight peer-reviewed, published studies in whichyoga was taught to school-aged children in a school settingwere identified. Inclusion criteria included only randomizedcontrol studies (i.e., the control group had no intervention oran active control) in which yoga (and not just meditation)was taught and the effects on psychological well-being orcognitive functions were analyzed. After wide qualitative andquantitative synthesis, nine studies were selected.

    Regarding the effects of yoga on psychological well-being,of the six studies, three of them support the benefits of yoga oryoga-based programs for children in school settings. Khalsaet al. [23] observed that yoga participants showed statisticallysignificant differences over time relative to controls on mea-sures of anger control and fatigue/inertia. Noggle et al. [20]also observed preventive benefits in psychosocial well-being(anxiety and negative affect) for students enrolled in a yogaprogram. In Ramadoss and Bose [19], only the yoga groupdemonstrated a slight decrease in stress while maintainingself-control.

    In contrast, Haden et al., 2014 [30] and White [26]observed a significant increase in perceived stress in theyoga group compared to the physical education and controlgroups, respectively. However, in White [26], both groups(yoga and control) reported significantly greater self-esteemand self-regulation over time. Lastly, Hagins et al. [32] foundthat yoga did not reduce stress reactivity, compared to aphysical education class, when students were submitted tostressor tasks.

    The analysis of the effect size for psychological well-beingshowed that an effect size was found for mood state indi-cators (POMS), demonstrating that the yoga group showedsignificant better scores in the postintervention condition.The same scale showed a second significant effect for thesubitems tension and anxiety. The variable self-esteem alsoshowed effect size and better results in the postinterventioncondition for the yoga group.

    The evidence for the benefits of yoga in adults, whetherhealthy or suffering from mental disorders, is significant[4, 16]. However, due to the reduced number of randomizedtrials in school settings and the conflicting findings, nodefinitive conclusions can be drawn from these studies withchildren but rather indications and suggestions based onsignificant and reliable but isolated results. The followingsubsections discuss some of the positive and negative results.

    4.1. Initial Negative Effect and Insignificant Results. The nega-tive effects of yoga observed in some of the studies heremightbe explained to some extent by (1) the adaptation process, (2)attentional control, and (3) inadequacy of yoga practice forchildren.

    4.1.1. Becoming Mindful. The practice of yoga requires effortand discipline. A child’s first contact with yoga is oftendemanding. When yoga is added to a child’s already existingacademic and extracurricular activities, the child may expe-rience higher levels of stress in the short term. According toHayes and Feldman [33], this temporary increase in stressmay also be part of the process of becoming mindful as indi-viduals begin to recognize the typical habits of the reactionto stress. In order to confirm if the initial increase in stress ispart of an adaptation process, the persistence of stress levelsshould be examined later on. Additionally, accomplishmentin yoga depends on acquired self-confidence. Benavidesand Caballero [34] have demonstrated that participants inyoga show increased self-worth but demonstrated that thisis dependent on one’s confidence. At first, initially, tryingsomething new at which a person is not skilled may increasefeelings of inadequacy. According to Kaley-Isley et al. [35],individuals progressing through the stages of change fromprecontemplative to contemplative and active often experi-encemore distress in the transitive contemplative stage wherethere is awareness of the need to change, but the person hasnot yet developed the means or the mastery to do so. Someauthors suggest that this finding would reverse with a longerdose of the intervention [35]. Kaley-Isley et al. [35] concludedthat although there are a few case reports of adverse eventsrelated to the use of yoga in adults [22], there is a need toconduct controlled studies in which systematic data could begathered regarding any adverse effects of yoga with adults,children, and adolescents.

    4.1.2. Poor Attentional Control. Yoga techniques such asbreathing and meditation require attentional control, anexecutive function that is still not mature in children andadolescents. As the frontal lobes mature [36], children’scapacity to exercise attentional control increases [37], butthe ability remains much poorer in children than in adults[38]. Paradoxically, yoga has been found to improve attentionin adults and children [8, 39–42]. Therefore, yoga practiceshould be specifically adapted to children so they can reallybenefit from the positive effects observed in adults given theirbrain maturity.

    4.1.3. Inadequacy of Practice and Methodology. The durationof yoga practice observed in some of the studies might not besuitable for children due to their inability to control attentionand reduced discipline. In studies where the yoga practice hasbeen found beneficial for the students [19, 20, 23], sessionswere short and condensed, lasting from 15 to 30 minutes.Additionally, many of these studies compared the effectsof yoga to exercise (physical education) and have observedvery similar results. A good methodological approach wouldconsider different intervention groups and a control group.

  • Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 15

    One of the outcomes of yoga practice may be greater self-awareness and mindfulness, a primary difference betweenyoga and standard physical education, and these variableswere not assessed in any of the studies. Actually, physicaleducation and yoga can be considered as complementary andit is therefore inappropriate to try and compare one as betterthan the other.

    4.2. Yoga at Schools: Why It Might Work. Although thenumber of RCT studies observing the effects of yoga onpsychological and cognitive functions in school settings isvery limited, the results seempromising. Effect sizewas foundfor mood indicators, tension and anxiety in the POMS scale,self-esteem, and memory.

    This review identified three RCT studies that observedthe effects of yoga-based interventions on different cognitivefunctions, such as attention, memory, and developmen-tal abilities. Overall, participation in a yoga program wasassociated with improvements in subjective and objectiveparameters in mini mental status scale [28], mental ability,and memory [29] while performing the Stroop task [27].

    Lowered mood is associated with declines in cognitivefunction and, at least in adults, yoga has been reported toproduce improvements in mood [43, 44]. Additionally, thepractice of yoga emphasizes body awareness and involvesfocusing one’s attention. Indeed, it has been demonstratedthat yoga improves general attentional abilities in adults[8, 39–42]. Attentional focus is a key aspect of yoga. Itproduces similar effects as relaxation in that it tends topromote self-control, concentration, self-efficacy, and bodyawareness [45]. Studies with children suffering from ADHDhave demonstrated significant improvements pre- to posttestin different attention scales and tasks [39, 42]. Neuroimagestudies in adults demonstrate that the effect of meditation(one of yoga techniques) on gray matter was most significantin the putamen [46] and anterior cingulate cortex [47],structures involved in attention processing.

    In addition, yoga practice has been shown to reduce anxi-ety based on reductions in psychological arousal [48] (thoughthis variable was not measured), and studies with adults haveverified that anxiety affects performance on tasks requiringattention [49]. Sarang and Telles [50] speculated that anxietyreduction was the likely basis for better performance in theirstudy.

    Two of the studies reviewed here showed significantimprovements in memory tasks after a few weeks of yoga-based interventions [28, 29]. Memory improvement follow-ing the practice of breathing exercises (pranayama) [51, 52] orcontemplative techniques such as meditation [53–55], a piv-otal part of the yoga practice, has been widely demonstrated.Activation in the hippocampus, a subcortical structureknown to be critically involved in memory processes [56],has been reported during meditative states [57, 58]. A recentstudy contrasting structural MRI scans of novice meditatorsbefore and after eight weeks of ameditation training programconfirmed actual meditation-induced changes in regions ofthe left hippocampus [59]. Hippocampal differences existbetween meditators and nonmeditators or actual changes of

    the hippocampus occur due to meditation, as revealed instructural imaging studies. Studies using positron emissiontomography or functional MRI (fMRI) indicated increasedbrain activation (compared to baseline) during meditationin left and right hippocampal and parahippocampal regions[57, 58, 60, 61]. Even though these are studies conductedin adults, improvements in attentional networks and higherhippocampal activation might also explain the effects of yogaon cognitive functions in children.

    5. Conclusions

    This review analyzed nine peer-reviewed RCT studies, inwhich yoga was taught to children in a school setting.Outcome measures included psychological well-being andcognitive functions, such as attention and memory. Whilesupportive in some studies and different variables, the utilityof yoga in educational settings is uncertain due to thesmall number of randomized control trials in the literature.Even though only RCTs were reviewed, methodological andstatistical problems might have contributed to the uncer-tainty: inadequate sample sizes, absence of control groups,variability in the type of yoga being taught, long durationof yoga sessions, inappropriate psychometric tools for chil-dren, and failure to measure intervening variables such asmindfulness and body awareness, which are important partsof yoga practice. This review suggests important effects ofyoga-based interventions at school on both psychologicalstatus and cognitive function in some studies, but futureresearch requires greater standardization and must deal withthe problem of appropriateness; what type of yoga-basedintervention ismost suitable for children, specifically in termsof the frequency and duration?

    Conflict of Interests

    None of the authors have any conflict of interests regardingthe publication of this paper.

    Acknowledgments

    This research was funded by the FAPITEC Agency underProcess no. 7838.UNI321.21944.25062013. The authors wouldlike to thank Andrew Jungkuntz for English revision.

    References

    [1] S. Muktibodhananda,Hatha Yoga Pradikipa, Yoga PublicationsTrust, Rishikesh, India, 1998.

    [2] S. Telles, V. Gaur, and A. Balkrishna, “Effect of a yoga practicesession and a yoga theory session on state anxiety,” Perceptualand Motor Skills, vol. 109, no. 3, pp. 924–930, 2009.

    [3] S. Telles, N. Singh, M. Joshi, and A. Balkrishna, “Post traumaticstress symptoms and heart rate variability in Bihar floodsurvivors following yoga: a randomized controlled study,” BMCPsychiatry, vol. 10, article 18, 2010.

    [4] C. Ferreira-Vorkapic and B. Rangé, “Mente Alerta, MenteTranquila: ¿constituye el yoga una intervención terapêutica

  • 16 Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine

    consistente para los trastornos de ansiedad?” Revista Argentinade Cĺınica Psicológica, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 211–220, 2010.

    [5] G. Kirkwood, H. Rampes, V. Tuffrey, J. Richardson, and K. Pilk-ington, “Yoga for anxiety: a systematic review of the researchevidence,” British Journal of Sports Medicine, vol. 39, no. 12, pp.884–891, 2005.

    [6] C. S. M. Chong, M. Tsunaka, H. W. H. Tsang, E. P. Chan, andW.M.Cheung, “Effects of yoga on stressmanagement in healthyadults: a systematic review,” Alternative Therapies in Health andMedicine, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 32–38, 2011.

    [7] S. Telles, K. V. Naveen, and M. Dash, “Yoga reduces symptomsof distress in tsunami survivors in the Andaman Islands,”Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, vol.4, no. 4, pp. 503–509, 2007.

    [8] P. Subramanya and S. Telles, “Effect of two yoga-based relax-ation techniques on memory scores and state anxiety,” BioPsy-choSocial Medicine, vol. 3, article 8, 2009.

    [9] A. Michalsen, P. Grossman, A. Acil et al., “Rapid stressreduction and anxiolysis among distressed women as a con-sequence of a three-month intensive yoga program,” Medi-cal Science Monitor, vol. 11, no. 12, pp. CR555–CR561, 2005,http://www.medscimonit.com/download/index/idArt/438851.

    [10] J. Granath, S. Ingvarsson, U. Von Thiele, and U. Lund-berg, “Stress management: a randomized study of cognitivebehavioural therapy and yoga,” Cognitive Behaviour Therapy,vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 3–10, 2006.

    [11] E. H. Kozasa, R. F. Santos, A. D. Rueda, A. A. Benedito-Silva, F. L. De Moraes Ornellas, and J. R. Leite, “Evaluation ofSiddha Samadhi yoga for anxiety and depression symptoms: apreliminary study,” Psychological Reports, vol. 103, no. 1, pp. 271–274, 2008.

    [12] P. Cabral, H. B. Meyer, and D. Ames, “Effectiveness of yogatherapy as a complementary treatment for major psychiatricdisorders: a meta-analysis,” The Primary Care Companion forCNS Disorders, vol. 13, no. 4, 2011.

    [13] S. B. S. Khalsa, S. M. Shorter, S. Cope, G. Wyshak, and E. Sklar,“Yoga ameliorates performance anxiety and mood disturbancein young professional musicians,”Applied Psychophysiology andBiofeedback, vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 279–289, 2009.

    [14] N. K. Patel, A. H. Newstead, and R. L. Ferrer, “The effects ofyoga on physical functioning and health related quality of lifein older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis,” Journalof Alternative and Complementary Medicine, vol. 18, no. 10, pp.902–917, 2012.

    [15] K. Y. Lin, Y. T. Hu, K. J. Chang, H. F. Lin, and J. Y. Tsauo, “Effectsof yoga on psychological health, quality of life, and physicalhealth of patients with cancer: a meta-analysis,” Evidence-BasedComplementary and Alternative Medicine, vol. 2011, Article ID659876, 12 pages, 2011.

    [16] M. Balasubramaniam, S. Telles, and P. M. Doraiswamy, “Yogaon our minds: a systematic review of yoga for neuropsychiatricdisorders,” Frontiers in Psychiatry, vol. 3, article 117, 2013.

    [17] M. L. Serwacki and C. Cook-Cottone, “Yoga in the schools: asystematic review of the literature,” International Journal of YogaTherapy, vol. 22, pp. 101–109, 2012.

    [18] United Nations Development Programme, 2007,http://hdr.undp.org/en/reports/global/hdr2007-2008.

    [19] R. Ramadoss and B. Bose, “Transformative life skills: pilotstudies of a 6. yoga model for reducing perceived stressand improving self-control in vulnerable youth,” InternationalJournal of Yoga Therapy, vol. 20, pp. 75–80, 2010.

    [20] J. J. Noggle, N. J. Steiner, T. Minami, and S. B. S. Khalsa,“Benefits of yoga for psychosocial well-being in a us high schoolcurriculum: a preliminary randomized controlled trial,” Journalof Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, vol. 33, no. 3, pp.193–201, 2012.

    [21] M. L. Galantino, R. Galbavy, and L. Quinn, “Therapeutic effectsof yoga for children: a systematic review of the literature,”Pediatric Physical Therapy, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 66–80, 2008.

    [22] G. S. Birdee, G. Y. Yeh, P. M. Wayne, R. S. Phillips, R. B. Davis,and P. Gardiner, “Clinical applications of yoga for the pediatricpopulation: a systematic review,”Academic Pediatrics, vol. 9, no.4, pp. 212–220, 2009.

    [23] S. B. S. Khalsa, L. Hickey-Schultz, D. Cohen, N. Steiner, andS. Cope, “Evaluation of the mental health benefits of yoga ina secondary school: a preliminary randomized controlled trial,”Journal of Behavioral Health Services and Research, vol. 39, no.1, pp. 80–90, 2012.

    [24] D. Moher, A. Liberati, J. Tetzlaff, and D. G. Altman, “Preferredreporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: thePRISMA statement,” PLoS Medicine, vol. 6, no. 7, Article IDe1000097, 2009.

    [25] Agency for Health Care Research and Quality, “Systemsto rate the strength of scientific evidence,” EvidenceReport/Technological Assessment 47, Agency for HealthCare Research and Quality, Rockville, Md, USA, 2002,http://archive.ahrq.gov/clinic/epcsums/strengthsum.pdf.

    [26] L. S. White, “Reducing stress in school-age girls throughmindful yoga,” Journal of Pediatric Health Care, vol. 26, no. 1,pp. 45–56, 2012.

    [27] S. Telles,N. Singh,A.K. Bhardwaj, A.Kumar, andA. Balkrishna,“Effect of yoga or physical exercise on physical, cognitive andemotional measures in children: a randomized controlled trial,”Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, vol. 7, no. 1,article 37, 2013.

    [28] A. S. Sarokte and M. V. Rao, “Effects of Medhya Rasayanaand Yogic practices in improvement of short-term memoryamong school-going children,”AYU:An International QuarterlyJournal of Research in Ayurveda, vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 383–389, 2013.

    [29] A. Verma, S. U. Shete, G. S. Thakur, D. D. Kulkarni, and R. S.Bhogal, “The effect of yoga practices on cognitive developmentin rural residential school children in