potential educational developments involving neuroscience

25
www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk 1 Potential educational developments involving neuroscience that may arrive by 2025 Paul Howard-Jones December 2008 Introduction Educational innovation involving valid neuroscientific concepts is a relatively new phenomenon and the challenges involved are considerable, but it can be expected that progress in this area will accelerate with the growth of scientific understanding of the brain and mind. This report attempts to identify where changes are likely to occur by 2025. It deals first with those changes that are probable through neuroscience and education working together, reviews educational issues associated with neuroscience that may arise even in the absence of such positive collaboration, and then briefly considers the effect of such changes on the professional development of teachers. While these first three sections deal with what may happen by 2025, the rest of the report is concerned with what is improbable by 2025. This includes a section on advances that may occur in the far distant future but, based on our current state of knowledge, appear unlikely to occur by 2025. The final section reviews the many neuromyths in education. This is included because such concepts are known to influence educators‟ expectations of what to expect from neuroscience in the coming years but, lacking a valid scientific basis, are not likely to give rise to future innovation. Keywords: brain, education, innovation, neuroscience Probable educational advances involving neuroscience Some insights regarding brain function tend to resonate with existing educational attitudes and concepts, and may be helpful in strengthening and consolidating existing practice. Examples of such insights arise from research on

Upload: others

Post on 09-Feb-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

1

Potential educational developments involving

neuroscience that may arrive by 2025

Paul Howard-Jones December 2008

Introduction

Educational innovation involving valid neuroscientific concepts is a relatively new

phenomenon and the challenges involved are considerable, but it can be expected that

progress in this area will accelerate with the growth of scientific understanding of the

brain and mind. This report attempts to identify where changes are likely to occur by

2025. It deals first with those changes that are probable through neuroscience and

education working together, reviews educational issues associated with neuroscience

that may arise even in the absence of such positive collaboration, and then briefly

considers the effect of such changes on the professional development of teachers.

While these first three sections deal with what may happen by 2025, the rest of the

report is concerned with what is improbable by 2025. This includes a section on

advances that may occur in the far distant future but, based on our current state of

knowledge, appear unlikely to occur by 2025. The final section reviews the many

neuromyths in education. This is included because such concepts are known to influence

educators‟ expectations of what to expect from neuroscience in the coming years but,

lacking a valid scientific basis, are not likely to give rise to future innovation.

Keywords: brain, education, innovation, neuroscience

Probable educational advances involving neuroscience

Some insights regarding brain function tend to resonate with existing educational

attitudes and concepts, and may be helpful in strengthening and consolidating existing

practice. Examples of such insights arise from research on

Page 2: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

2

Brain plasticity. This emphasises the extent to which the structure and

function of the brain can respond to environmental influences including

education. Such studies tend to emphasise the general importance of

educational influence on neurocognitive development, and will always find

favour amongst those who feel passionate about the value and promise of

education. (eg Immordino-Yang presented two complimentary case studies

of boys who had undergone the surgical removal of an entire brain

hemisphere (Immordino-Yang, 2007). Both were able to develop language

and social skills far beyond expectations, by developing individual

processing strategies that exploited the functionality associated with the

remaining hemisphere).

The role of phonological processing skills which includes an understanding

of reading processes and reading difficulties. Studies have linked dyslexia

to reduced functioning in areas of the brain associated with phonological

processing, and have demonstrated that both the reading difficulties and

thus reduced functioning are amenable to remediation using approaches

that emphasise sound-spelling relationships (Shaywitz et al, 2004).1

Ongoing research continues to emphasise the importance of modern

“phonics” approaches in the classroom.

Creativity that shows that the inclusion of remotely associated concepts

increases activity in brain regions linked to creative effort, supporting the

use of such strategies as a means to foster creativity (Howard-Jones et al,

2005).

Visualisation that shows visualising an object recruits most of the brain

areas activated by actually seeing it (Kosslyn, 2005), supporting the use of

visualisation as a learning tool.

However, although such neuroscientific studies may play a vital role in consolidating

existing educational attitudes in some areas, perhaps the more salient influence of

neuroscience in education will arise from more counter-intuitive findings. That is, it may

be the educationally-relevant findings about brain function that are more surprising in

their content and implications that stimulate the more dramatic changes in educational

thinking and practice.

By definition, however, it is difficult to predict surprises. Yet, there are some areas of

neuroscience research where results already appear to challenge the types of

assumptions teachers work with, and may soon give rise to new directions. These areas

will, therefore, now be given particular focus.

Early Numeracy

The acquisition of much formal mathematics relies on our ability to learn rules and

procedures. This has been demonstrated by a neuroimaging study involving adults who

were asked to calculate answers exactly. Researchers observed increased activity in

areas of the brain involved in word association and language activity, the left frontal and

angular gyri (Dehaene et al, 1999), as these adults pursued mathematical procedures by

following formal mathematical steps that could be linguistically encoded. However, when

the same individuals attempted to estimate answers, bilateral activity in the intraparietal

sulci was linked to our more ancient and language-independent ability to approximate.

Such an ability appears early in development. Even at six months, most of us can

approximately differentiate between large numbers of items for ratios of between 1:2

and 2:3 (Starkey and Cooper, 1980) and it seems that we share this approximate

1 I have included dyslexia in this list because present understanding in neuroscience

tends to support much existing practice, although it could be argued that this is due

to existing practice already having been influenced by cognitive psychology and

cognitive neuroscience.

Page 3: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

3

number sense with other animals (Boysen and Capaldi, 1993). Such early mathematical

ability is likely to have a critical role in „bootstrapping‟ our capacity to formally grasp

exact differences and procedures (credited to Spelke and Carey in Johnson, 2004; see

also Carey, 2004), and should provide an improved basis for developing initial

mathematical understanding of number and for remediating pupils‟ difficulties in

achieving this. For example, dyscalculia has been linked to a deficit in our

„premathematical‟ estimation abilities (Butterworth, 2008) and a study of low birth-

weight adolescents with numerical difficulties revealed less gray matter in an area of the

intra-parietal sulcus (Isaacs et al, 2001). Further research is needed to confirm the

direction of cause and effect in such studies, but insights from brain imaging research

are contributing to models of mathematical development useful in developing

interventions. For example, in one intervention based on these concepts, it was

demonstrated that children with dyscalculia showed considerable improvements in a

broad range of calculation abilities when basic numerical and conceptual knowledge were

focused upon at an early stage of mathematics education (Kaufmann et al, 2003).

Dehaene has also used his own findings to develop educational software aimed at

remediating dyscalculia. The software is based on the hypothesis that dyscalculia derives

from a core deficit in number sense, or in relating number sense to numerical symbols

(Wilson et al, 2006)

Another example of the potential influence of neuroscience on mathematics education

comes from research into the role of fingers in early mathematical development. Since

one might consider mathematics as a cognitive ability, the use of fingers to support

calculation can be looked down upon as evidence of mathematical weakness. However,

finger gnosis (being able to differentiate between different fingers in response to, say,

one or more being touched) has been identified as a strong predictor of mathematical

ability (Noel, 2005). Links between finger discrimination and mathematical ability have

been studied in both children and adults. A functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

(fMRI) study has shown that, although behavioural outcomes can be the same, the

activities produced when fingers are involved in approximating tasks vary with age

(Kaufmann et al, 2008), suggesting their contribution varies with development. Eight

year-old children produce an increase in activity in the intraprietal sulci when fingers are

involved, but not adults. Kaufmann suggests fingers represent concrete embodied tokens

involved in the estimation of number magnitude – an intimate involvement with our

basic “number sense”. On this basis, children should not be discouraged from using

fingers and teachers may be able to exploit their natural role more fully (Kaufmann,

2008). For example, in one intervention based on such ideas, new arrivals at three

Belgian schools were identified as having poor finger gnosis and some of them received

two-weekly 30 minute sessions of finger training for eight weeks (Gracia-Bafalluy and

Noel, 2008). After training, these children were significantly better at quantification tasks

than those who had not received the training.

Research on the relationship between our “animal number sense” and the early learning

of mathematical concepts has only just begun. However, the concepts emerging are

sufficiently well established and different to previous ideas of mathematical development

to suggest the potential for improved educational practice in primary school classrooms

in the next 1-2 decades.

Adolescence

It may be natural to consider that a teenager is essentially a young adult, with a fully

formed brain but lacking the social experiences of his/her elders. However, scientific

investigation has revealed a very different picture, with frontal and parietal regions still

undergoing radical structural changes until the late teens, relative to other areas of the

brain which appear more fully developed. Synaptic pruning (the cutting back of neural

connections) and myelination (improving the efficiency of neural connections) also

continue throughout adolescence (Sowell et al, 2003) in frontal regions. Such change in

Page 4: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

4

specific regions suggests the teenage brain may be less ready than an adult brain to

carry out a range of specific processes, including directing attention, planning future

tasks, inhibiting inappropriate behaviour, multitasking, and a variety of socially-

orientated tasks. Although more research is needed, some psychological research backs

this up, even showing a “pubertal dip” in some areas of performance, such as matching

pictures of facial expressions to descriptors. In this task, 11-12 year olds perform worse

than younger children (McGivern et al, 2002). Discontinuities have also been shown in

abilities underlying social communication, such as taking on the viewpoint of another

person, or so-called „perspective-taking‟ (Blakemore and Choudhury, 2006; Choudhury

et al, 2006). Some parts of what might be described as a “social brain” network are also

activated differently in teenagers compared with adults when thinking about intentions

(Catherine et al, 2008) and brain regions responsible for the control of impulses appear

less well functionally connected in adolescents‟ than in adults‟ brains (Steven et al,

2007). Teenagers also appear to activate different areas of the brain from adults when

learning algebraic equations, with this difference associated with a more robust process

of long-term storage than that used by adults (Luna, 2004; Qin et al, 2004).

Adolescents, then, are not simply older children or younger adults, and cognitive

development cannot be expected to proceed in a continuous linear manner. Apart from

explaining some of the difficulties teenagers experience, such changes also suggest how

and why adolescence can be a potentially sensitive period for learning, within and

beyond academic contexts. For example, teenagers often tend to perceive risks as

smaller and more controllable than adults, and they are generally more vulnerable than

adults or children to a range of activities which are inappropriately risky, such as

gambling and drug taking. Appropriate decision making appears to require a balanced

engagement between harm-avoidance and reward orientating processes that is

regulated by processes within the prefrontal cortex, where teenage development may

lag (Ernst et al, 2005). Imaging studies comparing adults and adolescents show reduced

activity in these prefrontal areas when making risk-based decisions (Bjork et al, 2007;

Eshel et al, 2007), and this reduced activity correlates with greater risk-taking

performance (Eshel et al, 2007). Such studies provide new insights into how adolescent

risk taking may be linked to neuro-maturational events and these insights may influence

educational perspectives on teenage behaviour, helping to understand a potentially

problematic, and sometimes even dangerous, period of children‟s development (Baird et

al, 2005).

It seems likely that these and future findings from neuroscience may generate new

educational approaches in future years (eg strategies that take a more informed account

of the temporary lagging of cognitive function in some areas). Findings on the adolescent

brain are potentially illuminating and should stimulate educational changes, although the

rate at which these will occur is difficult to predict. Paus believes that “the time is right

for evidence-based, large-scale studies of interventions aimed at facilitating youth

development. Neuroimaging-based approaches hold considerable promise, providing

both the evidence as well as novel insights about the role of the environment in shaping

the adolescent brain” (Paus, 2008). However, do such beliefs reflect an awareness of the

considerable ethical, social and political issues that would be involved with such studies?

Neuroimaging studies of interventions aimed at remediation of difficulties associated with

dyslexia have already been carried out, but remediation of character (one of the five “c”s

of positive youth development (Lerner, 2005)) would take neuroethical dilemmas to a

new level.

Motivation

A burgeoning number of findings from neuroscience has supported some fresh

educational thinking about motivation, including the type of intense engagement

provided by computer gaming (Gee, 2003). Previous explanations of gaming motivation

involve issues of fantasy, challenge and curiosity (Malone, 1981) but these appear

inadequate in explaining the attraction of some traditional games such as “snakes and

Page 5: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

5

ladders” and “bingo”, or simple (but popular) computer games such as Tetris. This

attraction may be due more to elements of chance-based uncertainty. The attraction of

uncertainty is now gaining closer neuroscientific investigation, but it is a phenomenon

well established by psychological experimentation (Atkinson, 1957) which has shown

moderate risk taking (50% chance) heightens motivation.

Recent neuropsychological understanding of reward2 involves consideration of „wanting‟

and „liking‟ as two dissociable components, with the wanting of a reward being coded by

levels of dopamine release in mid brain areas (Berridge and Robinson, 2003). The

predictability of an outcome has been shown to influence this activity. In primates, it has

been shown that maximum dopamine is released when the likelihood of receiving reward

for success is about half way between totally unexpected and completely predictable, ie

50% likely (Fiorillo et al, 2003). Dopamine levels in this area of the human brain have

been linked to our motivation to pursue a variety of pleasures, including sex, food,

gambling (Elliot et al, 2000) and computer gaming (Koepp et al, 1988). The link between

the predictability of an outcome and mid-brain dopamine activity is, therefore, helpful in

explaining why humans are so attracted to activities involving elements of chance

(Shizgal and Arvanitogiannis, 2003). Activity in this area has been studied non-invasively

in humans during gaming using fMRI. These fMRI studies have shown that patterns of

dopamine activity are predicted less by reward in „real‟ absolute terms and seem more to

do with winning the particular game being played. Activity can increase with reward size

(Knutson et al, 2001) but, rather than being proportional to absolute monetary reward,

activation peaks at the same level for the best available outcome in different games

(Nieuwenhuis et al, 2005). The complex relationship between reward and motivation is

thus strongly mediated by context.

When uncertainty is encountered in more real world instances, there are potentially

more complex effects of context created by the social environment. These are illustrated

by the way our natural attraction to uncertainty falls off when the task is perceived as

educational. Students generally prefer low levels of academic uncertainty and choose

problems well below moderate (<50%) challenge (Clifford, 1988; Harter, 1978) unless

these are presented as games, when students will take greater risks (Clifford and Chou,

1991). This may suggest that individuals can be deterred from tackling academic tasks

with higher levels of uncertainty due to the implications of failure for social status and

esteem. In research involving classroom-based applications, these concepts provide a

means to understand how learning games with elements of pure chance can increase

uncertainty without impacting negatively on self-esteem, thereby raising motivation

(Howard-Jones and Demetriou, in press - now published on-line). Neurocomputational

modelling is now providing the tools to study the reward system in more detail (Elliott

and Deakin, 2008).These techniques allow estimation of how dopaminergic activity in the

reward system varies with the progress of a game. Such activity also mediates attention

in the short term, and these models can predict when declarative learning (the type of

greatest interest to educators) will occur during an educational game (Howard-Jones et

al, 2009).

Neuroscience is providing concepts that are proving useful in understanding learning

games and motivation in the classroom, particularly amongst males (Hoeft et al, 2008).

However, introducing chance-based uncertainty into learning can conflict with the

principle of reward consistency that is traditionally valued by education (OfSTED., 2001).

Our increasing understanding of this region of the brain has, therefore, the potential to

prompt a significant departure from present educational thinking.

Early screening for some developmental disorders

2 Note that reward is being used here in the psychological sense, i.e. as a process, or set

of processes, by which behaviour is reinforced.

Page 6: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

6

Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) refer to a set of distinct electrical signals emitted by the

brain and detectable using a non-invasive technique involving the attachment of

electrodes to the scalp. Some ERP waveforms of newborn infants have been identified

that can differentiate between children who will later, at eight years old, be poor readers

or be dyslexic (Molfese, 2000). Measurement of ERPs has been shown as an effective

method of predicting dyslexia in new-borns with and without a family history of dyslexia

(Guttorm et al DATE) and such techniques could form the basis of very early screening,

so that children at risk of dyslexia are able to benefit as quickly as possible from suitable

intervention(s). See also discussion by Friedrich of neural markers and specific language

impairment (Friedrich, 2008). Such techniques and possibilities are not limited to

literacy. Another type of ERP has been identified that is sensitive to children‟s response

to numerical distance (Szucs et al, 2007) that may be a helpful neural marker for

magnitude processing in infancy. This signal may provide an early indicator of later

educational risk in respect of mathematics.

The use of neural markers to provide very early detection of educational risk is an area

identified by Goswami where a neuroscience approach may provide particular promise

for education (Goswami, 2008).

Cognition and the Brain in the Curriculum: Curriculum Aims and Content

i) The influence of research on cognitive training (‘Brain training’)

There is increasing evidence to show that the cognitive training can reduce risk of

Alzheimers (Wilson et al, 2002) and in normally functioning older adults. A 5-year study

has shown that training can provide sustained improvements in a range of cognitive

functions in this age group (Ball et al, 2002).

A study by Willis et al (2006) showed sustained improvements in targeted function over

five years, following an intervention that consisted of only 10 sessions of about 60-75

minutes each. Positive effects were also been observed on daily functioning (phone,

laundry, cooking etc) (Willis et al, 2006).

There is thus good evidence to show that brain function can be trained, in the sense that

repeated practice on exercises that focus on a cognitive function, can produce

improvement in that cognitive function. There is less evidence confirming impact on

everyday functioning not specifically targeted by the training. As research continues,

however, such evidence is emerging (Mahncke et al, 2006; Willis et al, 2006) and we are

understanding more about the impact of training cognitive function on other, non-

targeted areas. Most notably, it has been convincingly shown that fluid intelligence,

which is seen as a good predictor of professional and academic achievement, can be

improved by rehearsing working memory exercises, specifically an exercise based on the

N-back test3 (Jaeggi et al, 2008). Unlike most other studies which have been undertaken

with older participants including those at risk of dementia, the average age of the

participants in this study was 25, demonstrating the relevance of this type of cognitive

training to the younger population.

In terms of developing cognitive function amongst children, it is the targeting of WM,

together with the closely allied concept of attention that has again produced the most

interesting results. In a study involving children with ADHD, training of WM was found to

successfully transfer to non-targeted areas of behaviour, producing improved complex

reasoning skills and reduced parental ratings of ADHD symptoms (Klingberg et al, 2005).

Training of visual and auditory attention has been found to benefit literacy achievement

for children with dyslexia (Chenault et al, 2004), and a study using ERPs with children

3 In the type of N-back task used in this research, participants are asked to observe a

sequence of digits or letters, and asked to recall the item that was N items back

Page 7: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

7

with specific language impairment (SLI) showed that neural mechanisms of selective

auditory attention and the associated language difficulties can be remediated through

auditory attention training (Stevens et al, 2008).

The research from science converges with other forces encouraging educators to move

further away from content towards thinking skills and, more specifically, the training of

cognitive function. Advances in technology are likely to continue improving our access to

information, with some commentators believing this will increase the need for

specialisation as it increasingly places “any human knowledge at the fingertips of any

human” (Stewart, 2008). Such advances may, therefore, make it desirable for learners

to be better at manipulating information than encoding and recalling it, with demand for

specialisation making it more difficult to predict and include the type of content that an

individual may benefit from in the initial stages of their education4. These factors may

combine with a burgeoning dialogue with neuroscience that also encourages greater

emphasis upon cognitive function generally within education – since cognitive function is

a central construct of cognitive neuroscience - and some way towards a redefinition of

the aims of education5 as an attempt to “nurture” the brain and its processes. Increasing

interest in the training of cognitive function as a means to enhance learning potential is

also reflected in current public interest in “brain training” products, although it should be

noted that no quality research exists that evaluates the claims made by the

manufacturers of these products, or even the design principles upon which they are

based. This public enthusiasm with the commercial products has already begun to

extend itself to some schools6.

ii) Teaching about the brain

There has also been a broader interest in the development of children‟s cognitive

function, in ways that include emotional aspects of behaviour. These include the

development of “Executive Function” (EF) - an umbrella term referring to the underlying

processes responsible for children‟s ability to direct, maintain and focus their attention,

manage impulses, self-regulate behaviour and emotion, plan ahead and demonstrate

flexible approaches to problem solving7. EF skills are predictive of academic achievement

(Bull et al, 2008), and social and emotional development (Hughes, 1998). For this

reason, attempts have been made to find ways of developing EF skills and some

interventions in schools have reported positive results in terms of improved behaviour

(Greenberg, 2006).

Unlike simple cognitive training, such programmes require learners to understand and

reflect upon their behaviour in terms of a set of mental processes. They are thus

delivering an explicit, if sometimes ill-defined, psychological content into the curriculum

of many schools. These programmes are becoming associated with protecting the mental

4 Memory processes will, however, still remain of key importance in education although

semantic memory (for knowing “how to”) may increase its significance relative to

declarative memory (explicit recall of facts and events) 5 An extreme view of this redefinition has been provided by Koizumi: “… education

should be designed to guide and inspire the construction of the basic architecture for

information processing in the brain by preparing and controlling the input stimuli

given to the learners.” Koizumi, H. (2004) The Concept of 'Developing the Brain': A

New Science for Learning and Education, Brain and Development, 26, 434-41. p435. 6 For example:

http://www.ltscotland.org.uk/ictineducation/gamesbasedlearning/sharingpractice/brai

ntraining/introduction.asp

7 WM is usually considered one component of EF

Page 8: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

8

health of children8 as well as improving academic standards and behaviour. They have

been explicitly linked to neuroscience by some experts (Greenberg, 2006), although (in

the opinion of the author) such links would benefit from further scientific scrutiny and

consensus. What does appear clear, however, is that the school-based curriculum

education is becoming influenced by attempts to directly attend to the development of

executive function, in order to promote emotional well-being, mental health and

academic achievement. It has been known for some time that the level of education can

influence mental health in later life, but there are now growing voices for educators to

become actively involved in fostering the mental health of their learners. A 2005 policy

paper produced by the Sainsbury foundation (together with the NHS confederation)

predicts “By 2015, mental wellbeing should be a major concern for schools, from

dedicated classroom time to the overall approach of the school towards its pupils and

staff” (SCMH, 2005, p13.

It can also be predicted that advances in neuroscientific understanding may broaden the

aims of education further still, thereby influencing the curriculum and the ways in which

it is delivered. For example, rising levels of obesity amongst children has drawn new

attention to the importance of exercise in schools, but this may gain further emphasis as

neuroscientific understanding emerges about the processes by which exercise is linked to

learning (Hillman et al, 2008). Understanding the processes by which even short bouts

of exercise improve subsequent learning (Winter et al, 2007) makes it foreseeable that

regular exercise breaks during the school day will become more popular as a means of

raising academic standards and fostering mental and physical health. Although it is

certainly not a good example of science, evidence for the likelihood of such

developments arises from the popularity of Brain Gym. The principles of Brain Gym are

unscientific and bizarre (Hyatt, 2007), but its popularity in the face of unfavourable

media exposure must surely derive, in part, from its associations with academic

achievement and neuroscience. Such associations, in the case of Brain Gym, do not

withstand scrutiny, especially in terms of neuroscience, but it is likely that the authentic

value of exercise in learning will become well understood by educators in future years. It

can be predicted that, by 2025, neuroscience will have contributed to developing

scientifically sound and educationally evaluated methods of incorporating frequent

exercise breaks into the school day and these will have become established in most

schools.

Part of the success of introducing such elements into the curriculum will depend on

ensuring learner motivation, and this may depend on learners understanding something

of neurocognitive function. Such understanding may have other benefits. It has been

reported that providing learners with a basic knowledge of the brain can, by itself,

provide significant help in improving self-image and academic achievement (Blackwell et

al, 2007). In this study with adolescents, researchers informed learners about the

structure and function of their brain, how learning changes the brain by producing new

neuronal connections and about brain plasticity, and provided the clear message that the

pupils themselves were in charge of this process. This promoted a positive change in

classroom motivation. Grades for the control group, who had not received the

intervention, continued downward while this trend was reversed for the intervention

group.

8 Programmes through the curriculum can promote mental health … important

characteristics of such programmes include those that enable children to correctly

identify and regulate one‟s feelings...” DfEE. (2001) "Promoting Children's Mental

Health within Early Years and School Settings." (Nottingham, Department for

Education and Employment), p10

Page 9: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

9

Neuroscience-related issues arriving without invitation

The potential changes so far discussed are expected come about through initiatives

involving educators, and their final form is likely to be mediated by educational

understanding, sensitivities and opinions. Some influences involving neuroscience,

however, may arrive without invitation.

One such issue is the use of cognitive enhancers. In the US, students are increasingly

using prescription drugs in order to provide cognitive enhancement and thereby support

their studies. Usage varies widely from one university to the next, with an average figure

of 6.9% of students indulging in non-medical use of prescription stimulants (McCabe et

al, 2005). In another study, however, that surveyed 1811 students at a large

Southeastern US university, 34% reported the illegal use of ADHD stimulants (eg

methylphenidate), mostly to improve their cognitive function during periods of fatigue

and stress (DeSantis et al, 2008).

The production of new and stronger drugs for cognitive enhancement is likely to

increase, driven partly by efforts to combat the effects of Alzheimer‟s disease. One such

drug is donepezil (marketed as Aricept) that increases levels of acetylcholine (ACh). ACh

is thought to modulate the rate at which neural connections adjust themselves when

learning, with increases in ACh thus able to bring about increases in learning rate.

Donepezil reduces cholinesterase that mops up ACh, thereby increasing levels of ACh

and improving cognitive function, including memory, amongst those suffering from

Alzheimers (Roman and Rogers, 2004). The potential value of this drug for other users

was demonstrated in a study that administered donepezil to healthy young adults for

only 30 days, and revealed significant improvements in episodic memory performance

(Gron et al, 2005).

Scientists have been speaking out in positive terms about the “new enhancement

landscape” for healthy adults (Gazzaniga, 2005). In a recent article in Nature, one

group of scientists suggested that the growing demand for cognitive enhancement

should be responded to, and that the response should begin by “rejecting the idea that

enhancement is a dirty word.”(Greely et al, 2008) However, a modest UK consultation

showed more ambivalence (Horn, 2008), with concerns raised that included

possible side and long term effects including personality change

the devaluation of „normal‟ achievement and the intrinsic value of effort and

motivation in learning

inequality if such drugs are expensive

pressure to use such drugs and the exacerbation of an already over-competitive

culture.

It seems likely that the use of cognitive enhancers amongst the general UK population

will increase as public sensitivity diminishes and the drugs become more socially

acceptable. This will stimulate significant ethical debate amongst educators. Some

educational institutions may, with parental permission, choose to introduce drug testing.

Since there are few clear precedents for the issues involved with these drugs (it is

debatable whether a comparison with the use of drugs in sport is helpful) there may, for

some time, exist a diverse range of attitudes and practices amongst learners and

educational institutions in respect of cognitive enhancers. This may impede the

development of any necessary legislation.

Page 10: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

10

The influence of neuroscience on educational professional

development

Psychology returns, accompanied by some neuroscience, to initial teacher

training

Our burgeoning understanding of the human brain and mind, particularly in regard to the

issues raised above, will increase the likelihood of some elements of psychology

returning to initial teacher training. When teachers consider learning processes, they

consider mental rather than neural processes, although in the future we can expect

constructions about these mental processes to be increasingly informed by

neurobiological understanding. Neuroscience, in itself, remains largely meaningless in

educational terms, except insofar as it informs our psychological concepts about the

learner‟s mind. However, neuroscientific concepts are helpful in formulating and

communicating educationally-relevant concepts about the mind (Howard-Jones, 2008).

The emergence of neuro-educational research and new types of educational

professional

Although appearing under many different names (eg educational neuroscience,

neuroeducational research, neuroscience and education) a field of research at the

interface between neuroscience and education is becoming established. This provides

some support for the prediction made by some that, in the future, hybrid professionals

will emerge with expertise in both neuroscience and education (Pickering and Howard-

Jones, 2007; Szucs and Goswami, 2007).

Developments that may occur in the future, but not by 2025

Genetic profiling in mainstream education

The area where genetic knowledge is first likely to impact, and appears likely to do so

before 2025, is in the area of learning difficulties. Gene-based diagnoses of learning

difficulties will be able to predict general learning difficulty as well as difficulties within

specific areas such as maths (Plomin, 2008). Such very early predictions, combined with

emerging educational understanding of effective interventions, will provide the soonest

possible implementation of appropriate interventions. Genetic knowledge will provide

opportunities for new levels of personalised learning and these should ameliorate or even

prevent the manifestation of some learning difficulties.

Ultimately, genetic knowledge should allow educational programmes to be better tailored

to suit all individuals according to their genetically-informed educational profiles. It has

been suggested that, in the future, “Educogeneticists” will be able to provide informed

recommendations to schools and families about how a child‟s education may be planned

in order to optimise academic outcomes (Grigorenko, 2007). Genetics may, therefore, be

considered to have considerable educational potential beyond the early identification and

amelioration of learning difficulties. This wider application, however, will only add further

controversy to a plethora of ethical issues and questions about using genetic knowledge

in education: what may result when genetic testing proceeds without full understanding

of the educational intervention that may be needed? Who makes the decisions about

testing and interventions, and by what processes? What precautions are needed to

prevent this new educational opportunity feeding demand for genetic engineering and

eugenics?

Page 11: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

11

Since biotechnology companies are now marketing genetic tests directly to the public, it

is possible that this issue may arrive without educational invitation by 2025, as with

cognitive enhancers. One can imagine that, by 2025, all parents will have the

opportunity to independently purchase a genetic profile of their child and ask what their

school intends to do about it. However, whilst the educational application of smart pills

requires only that a bottle is opened and a pill consumed, there remains much

educational (or “edu-genetic”) research necessary in order to utilise genetic knowledge

in mainstream education. This knowledge gap and the ethical issues mentioned above

will provide some barrier to progress. Due to the potential benefits to individual learners,

public attitudes are likely to become increasingly positive and demands for edugenetic

approaches will grow, but it seems unlikely that schools will develop established

approaches to genetically-based differentiation of mainstream teaching and learning by

2025.

Brain-Computer interfaces (BCIs) in mainstream education and ‘brain reading’

Brain-computer interfaces have developed the potential to provide valuable aid to some

profoundly disabled individuals. For example, severely paralysed patients can control

prosthetic limbs and computer cursors by thought alone. Non-invasive approaches often

adapt the type of technology used by ERP and EEG measurements, using a patient‟s

electrical brain activity sensed by electrodes placed on the scalp. The EEG/ERP signal is

analysed and interpreted automatically by a computer, which then produces an

appropriate output. In this way, the user can generate some rudimentary signal to the

outside world by producing the pre-defined type of thought that the computer is

programmed to decode. For example, by imagining different body motions (eg left

versus right hand), the user can generate different EEG signals that can be translated by

the computer into responses to binary (yes/no) questions with high, but not perfect,

accuracy (Neuper et al, 2006). As illustrated in this example, there are presently

significant limitations upon the amount of information such BCI interfaces can

communicate. These limitations arise from the noisiness of the signal and the variability

of the signals produced by different individuals. Improvements in technology alone may

not be sufficient to overcome these limitations, since significant advances may require

greater neurobiological and psychological understanding of the signals themselves.

At present then, although the usefulness of non-invasive BCIs for the profoundly

disabled may be possible in the next 1-2 decades, the likelihood of the wider population

using them to communicate with everyday technology is something for the very far

distant future when both our technology and, perhaps more importantly, our

understanding of brain function is unimaginably superior than at present9.

Similar issues limiting the advances in BCI‟s over coming years will also apply to „brain

reading‟. Some experiments have shown that imaging technology can reveal socially

sensitive and relevant information, such as racial group identity and unconscious racial

attitudes. For example, white subjects with more negative evaluations of black faces

showed increased amygdalic activity in response to unfamiliar black, compared with

white, faces. There has also been some success in identifying the correlates of deception

(Nunez et al, 2005) and such knowledge may be applied in counter-terrorism efforts in

the future. Such examples, however, tend to compare a very small set of conditions, and

results could only be used to differentiate between a correspondingly small set of

possible alternatives regarding mental content (eg truth/lies). As with the notion of using

a BCI to access one‟s computer in an everyday sense, the possibility of reading the

everyday contents of a learner‟s mind will be science fiction until well beyond 2025. As

9 Invasive BCIs involve the implantation of electrodes, produce cleaner signals and

perform a little better, but clearly these are not likely to become acceptable amongst

those who suffer no serious disability and so these devices are not considered here.

Page 12: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

12

Farah suggests (p1126) in relation to brain-reading, “even a major leap in the signal-to-

noise ratio of functional brain imaging would leave us with gigabytes of more accurate

physiological data whose psychological meaning would be obscure.” (Farah, 2002)

Improbable educational advances involving neuroscience

There are considerable differences between the views of educators and scientists about

how neuroscience is relevant to education, although consensus is emerging in both areas

that the relevance exists. Perhaps unsurprisingly, educators associate neuroscience

chiefly with those brain-based concepts already commonly found in schools (Pickering

and Howard-Jones, 2007). Unfortunately, these concepts are often not well supported by

existing science, yet educational expectations of how neuroscience may influence

education in the future are likely to be strongly influenced by these neuromyths. This

was illustrated in 2000 when scientists Uta Frith and colleague Sarah-Jayne Blakemore

were commissioned by the Teaching and Learning Research Programme (TLRP) to carry

out a review of neuroscientific findings that may be of relevance to educators

(Blakemore and Frith, 2000). This review attacked a number of unscientific myths and

highlighted some scientific areas of potential interest to educators. In January 2001, to

promote further discussion about a possible research agenda, the TLRP wrote to

scientific and educational institutions, asking for comments on the report by Blakemore

and Frith. Respondents were particularly asked to provide “identification of key research

questions, … their priority … and estimate of their tractability (in terms of return on

research effort)”. While scientists indicated areas such as learning disorders, memory

and plasticity, educational respondents identified areas such as multiple intelligences and

learning styles, which are problematic as robust and well-defined scientific concepts

suitable for orientating neurocognitive research. The report on the consultation

concluded that no collaborative research agenda had yet emerged (Des Forges, 2001).

Indeed, perhaps the most immediate benefit of the increased dialogue between

neuroscience and education has been to highlight the large number of neuromyths that

have developed within education. Some of the most prominent will now be listed below,

none of which are likely to receive support from neuroscience in the coming years and

should become less prominent by 2025, despite the hopes of many educators.

Multiple Intelligences (MI) Theory

Gardner‟s MI theory proposed that, rather than a single all-purpose intelligence, it is

more useful to describe an individual as possessing a small number of relatively

independent intelligences (Gardner, 1983). Possible candidates for these intelligences

include linguistic, musical, logical-mathematical, spatial, bodily-kinaesthetic,

intrapersonal sense of self, interpersonal and Gardner has later proposed other

possibilities such as naturalistic and existential intelligence (Gardner, 1999). MI theory is

in direct opposition to the idea of a unitary general intelligence factor „g‟, reflecting

overall brain efficiency and the close interconnection of our mental skills. MI theory

resonates with many educators, who see it as a robust argument against IQ-based

education.

In a critical review of the evidence for MI theory, Waterhouse examined the empirical

scientific evidence (Waterhouse, 2006). MI theory claims to be drawn from a wide range

of disciplines including neuroscience. Indeed, Gardner has claimed “accumulating

neurological evidence is amazingly supportive of the general thrust of MI theory”.

However, Waterhouse points out that the general processing complexity of the brain

makes it unlikely that anything resembling MI theory will ever emerge from

neuroscience. Cognitive neuroscience is exploring the brain in terms of processes (vision,

Page 13: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

13

hearing, smell, etc) but not in terms of seeing intelligence, auditory intelligence or

smelling intelligence. In the realm of neuroscience, it neither appears accurate or useful

to reduce the vast range of complex individual differences at neural and cognitive levels

to any limited number of capabilities.

Despite the absence of MI theory in the neuroscience literature, teachers heavily

associate MI theory with neuroscience. (To confirm this, the author returned to the data

collected from the 150 UK teachers who were asked the question “Please list any ideas

that you have heard of in which the brain is linked to education” (Pickering and Howard-

Jones, 2007). Of those teachers who responded to this question (121), most listed no

more than 2-3 ideas. Of these, MI theory occurred 17 times (14%)).

Thus, in educational terms, MI theory appears like a liberator – providing teachers with

the „scientific‟ license to celebrate diversity. In terms of the science, however, it seems

an unhelpful simplification as no clearly delineated, limited set of capabilities arises from

either the biological or psychological research.

Learning Styles

In educational terms, an individual‟s learning style can be considered as a set of learner

characteristics that influence their response to different teaching approaches. A survey in

2004 identified 71 different models of learning styles (Coffield et al, 2004) and our own

survey showed almost a third of UK teachers had heard of learning styles, with most of

those who used this approach reporting it as effective (Pickering and Howard-Jones,

2007). As with MI theory, which is also often interpreted by educators as a means to

identify preferred modes of learning, the promotion of learning styles has benefited from

a strong association with neuroscience. Many learning style models have a distinctly

biological justification, with one of their major proponents, Rita Dunn, commenting that

“at least three fifths of style is biologically imposed” (Dunn et al, 1990).

Very many educational projects have pursued improvement through tailoring

programmes to meet individual learning styles but, as yet, there is no convincing

evidence that any benefit arises. A review of such studies, concluded that matching

instruction to meet an individual‟s sensory strengths appears no more effective than

designing content-appropriate forms of education and instruction (Coffield et al, 2004).

Perhaps the best known inventory of learning styles within education is the one

categorising individuals in terms of their preferred sense modality for receiving,

processing and communicating information: visual, auditory or kinaesthetic (VAK). In a

laboratory study of memory performance, participants‟ own self assessment of their VAK

learning style was shown to be out of line with more objective measures, and memory

scores in different modalities appeared unrelated to any measure of dominant learning

style (Kratzig and Arbuthnott, 2006). There was, instead, evidence that participants‟

self-rating as kinaesthetic learners was related to visual performance, that they were

self-rating their learning styles in ways possibly promoted by the inventory itself, and

objective evidence from memory testing that suggested visual and kinaesthetic/tactile

tasks were tapping the same underlying memory process. The authors concluded that

educators‟ attempts to focus on learning styles were “wasted effort”.

In approaches such as VAK, the implicit assumption appears to be that, since different

modalities are processed independently in different parts of the brain, differences in the

efficiency of these parts results in a clear modality-based method of classifying how

learners are able to process information most efficiently. However, as pointed out by

Geake, this flies in the face of what we know about the interconnectivity of the

brain(Geake, 2008). Geake refers to a recent piece of experimental research in which

five year olds showed themselves able to distinguish between groups of dots even when

the numbers were too large for counting (Gilmore et al, 2007). They were then asked to

repeat the task in auditory mode by counting clicks, and reproduced almost identical

Page 14: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

14

levels of accuracy. Geake points out that this is because input modalities in the brain are

very interlinked. As yet, no evidence arising from neuroscience, or any other science,

supports the educational usefulness of categorising learners in terms of their sensory

modality or any other type of learning style. In the meantime, educators continue to be

drawn to VAK as means to implement a type of differentiation between learners.

Learning styles based on these ideas are likely to diminish in their popularity as

awareness grows of their ineffectiveness and lack of scientific basis. By 2025, one can be

hopeful that differentiation of learners may be informed by a better understanding of the

development of literacy, mathematical and other skills, and this understanding will

undoubtedly be informed by insights from cognitive neuroscience. More speculatively, it

is also possible in the more distant future (but not by 2025 – see above) of genetically-

informed profiling of individual learners.

Left-Brain Right Brain

Another popular way of categorising learning style is in terms of “left-brain right-brain”

theory(Springer and Deutsch, 1989). According to this theory, learners‟ dispositions

arise from the extent to which they are left or right brain dominant. It is true that some

tasks can be associated with extra activity that is predominantly in one hemisphere or

the other. For example, language is considered to be left lateralised. However, no part of

the brain is ever normally inactive in the sense that no blood flow is occurring.

Furthermore, performance in most everyday tasks, including learning tasks, requires

both hemispheres to work together in a sophisticated parallel fashion. The division of

people into left-brained and right-brained takes the misunderstanding one stage further.

There is no reliable evidence that such categorisation is helpful for teaching and learning.

Educational Kinesiology (Brain Gym)

Educational kinesiology (or Edu-K, also often sold under the brand name of Brain Gym)

was developed by Paul and Gail Dennison as a means to „balance‟ the hemispheres of

the brain so they work in an integrated fashion and thus improve learning (Dennison,

1981). Whatever the flaws in its theoretical basis (which are many and fatal), there is a

lack of published research in high quality journals to make claims about the practical

effectiveness of programmes such as Brain Gym to raise achievement. Of the studies

published elsewhere, the lack of information about the exercises undertaken and/or the

insufficient or inappropriate analysis of the results undermine their credibility (Hyatt,

2007). However, it may also be that programmes such as Brain Gym are contributing to

learning, but for entirely different reasons than those used to promote them. As

discussed above, there is an emerging body of multidisciplinary research supporting the

beneficial effect of aerobic exercise on selective aspects of brain function of importance

to education (Hillman et al, 2008). However, these advantages appear linked to the

aerobic nature of the exercise, which is low in Brain Gym.

Implicit learning

Work with artificial grammars, in which participants are able to acquire grammatical

rules by observing examples of artificial language, demonstrates our ability to learn

implicitly, ie without being able to report explicitly what has been learnt. Such

experiments have contributed to enthusiastic calls for more educational focus on implicit

learning (Claxton, 1998). However, there are considerable barriers to the practical

application of such ideas, making their usefulness to education questionable and causing

some scientific authorities to label them a new source of neuromyth (Goswami, 2004). A

non-specialist interpretation of the phenomenon of implicit learning might involve ideas

about absorbing information and concepts from the environment without attending to

them, but such ideas have no scientific basis. For example, in the artificial grammar

scenario, formal rules may be acquired without the learner consciously formulating

Page 15: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

15

them, but the learner must pay considerable attention to the examples of artificial

language in order to facilitate this. In a more real world context, we may also implicitly

develop understanding about, for example, the motivations of people around us, without

being able to articulate how we have achieved this. Again, however, this is only possible

by paying attention to the behaviour of those people. “Implicit learning” does not equate

to “learning without attention”, and it seems unlikely that such concepts will become

usefully applied in education in the coming years.

A brain-based science of learning?

Although researchers at the interface of neuroscience and education have done much to

counter the neuromyths prevailing in schools, they may also be guilty of inadvertently

creating one. There has been much enthusiasm amongst policy makers for the creation

of a “new” science of learning (OECD, 2002; OECD, 2007). This may be because

neuroscience seems a more secure basis for learning theory, with its images of blood

flow appearing more concrete than abstract psychological concepts. Indeed, it has been

experimentally determined that including references to the brain (even irrelevant ones)

increases the satisfaction of a reader (Weisberg et al, 2008). However, as with social and

experiential perspectives on learning, the biological perspective is, on its own, limited in

terms of what it can tell us. A science of teaching and learning which is chiefly based

upon the brain is unlikely to develop in the foreseeable future, because neuroscientific

perspectives struggle with many concepts (such as meaning and autonomy) that are

central to educational aims and understanding (Howard-Jones, in press). On the other

hand, this brief review has emphasised that greater inclusion of biological perspectives in

educational thinking, alongside other perspectives, is increasingly desirable and

probable.

Summary

It is anticipated that the following educational developments involving neuroscience may

arrive by 2025:

New educational approaches will become established for the teaching and

learning of mathematics in the early years, as a result of insights from cognitive

neuroscience.

Adolescents will become recognised as a more distinct group of learners and

educational approaches will be developed that are better tailored to meet their

social, emotional and educational needs

A new understanding of motivation will be developed and new approaches to

engaging learners will become established (eg in areas involving the use of

games) informed by insights into the brain‟s reward system

Early screening will be available for a range of learning disorders, using neural

markers and genetic testing.

Attendance to the training of some targeted cognitive functions, including working

memory, will feature across year groups in the National Curriculum

The reflective understanding and development of executive function will feature in

the National Curriculum for young learners

Understanding of mental health issues will become a stronger feature of the

curriculum, as the aims of education become broader. This will include a basic

understanding of brain function, with the associated academic benefits that such

an understanding may bring.

Exercise breaks will become a feature of the curriculum, as the link between

exercise and academic achievement becomes clearer, and the UK struggles with

increasing levels of obesity.

Page 16: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

16

The use of drugs to enhance cognitive function will become commonplace,

remaining chiefly unchecked by legislation as the government remains unwilling

to intervene in the absence of clear public consensus. The attitudes and practices

amongst different groups of learners and educational institutions will diverge.

Psychology, and some neuroscience, will become an established feature of

teachers‟ professional development and training.

There will be no brain-based science of learning that is meaningful in educational

terms, but a new field of neuro-educational research will become established,

together with the development of professionals trained in both education and the

relevant natural sciences (eg cognitive neuroscience, genetics). Biological

perspectives will become an increasingly important component of educational

understanding, practice and policy making.

Page 17: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

17

References

Atkinson, J.W. (1957) Motivational Determinants of Risk Taking Behaviour. Psychological

Review, 64, pp.359-72.

Baird, A, Fugelsang, J. and Bennett, C. (2005) "What Were You Thinking"? A Neural

Signature Associated with Reasoning in Adolescence. Journal of Cognitive

Neuroscience, S, pp.193-93.

Ball, K., Berch, D.B., Helmers, K.F., Jobe, J.B., Leveck, M.D., Marsiske, M., Morris, J.N.,

Rebok, G.W., Smith, D.M., Tennstedt, S.L., Unverzagt, F.W. and Willis, S.L.

(2002) Effects of Cognitive Training Interventions with Older Adults:A

Randomized Controlled Trial. Journal of American Medical Association, 288 (18),

pp.2271-81.

Berridge, K.C. and Robinson, T.E. (2003) Parsing Reward. Trends in Neurosciences, 26

(9), pp.507-13.

Bjork, J.M., Smith, A.R., Danube, C.L. and Hommer, D.W. (2007) Developmental

Differences in Posterior Mesofrontal Cortex Recruitment by Risky Rewards.

Journal of Neuroscience, 27 (18), pp.4839-49.

Blackwell, L.S., Trzesniewski, K.H. and Dweck, C.S. (2007) Implicit Theories of

Intelligence Predict Achievement across an Adolescent Transition: A Longitudinal

Study and an Intervention. Child Development, 78 (1), pp.246-63.

Blakemore, S.J. and Choudhury, S. (2006) Development of the Adolescent Brain:

Implications for Executive Function and Social Cognition. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 47 (3), pp.296-312.

Boysen, S.T. and Capaldi, E.J. (1993) The Development of Numerical Competence:

Animal and Human Models. Hillsdale, N.J., Erlbaum.

Bull, R., Espy, K.A., and Wiebe, S.A. (2008) Short-Term Memory, Working Memory, and

Executive Functioning in Preschoolers: Longitudinal Predictors of Mathematical

Achievement at Age 7 Years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33 (3), pp.205-

28.

Butterworth, B. (2008) Dyscalculia. Mental Capital and Wellbeing, State-of-Science

Reviews. London, Government Office for Science.

Carey, S. (2004) Bootstrapping and the Origins of Concepts. Daedalus, pp.59-68.

Page 18: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

18

Catherine, S., Burnett, S. and Blakemore, S. (2008) Neuroscience of Social Cognition in

Teenagers: Implications for Inclusion in Society. Mental Capital and Wellbeing,

State-of-Science Reviews. London, Government Office for Science.

Chenault, B., Thomson, J., Abbott, R.D. and Berninger, V.W. (2004) Effects of Prior

Attention Training on Child Dyslexics' Response to Composition Instruction. 24th

Annual Meeting of the National-Academy-of-Neuropsychology. Seattle, WA.,

Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc Inc., pp.243-60).

Choudhury, S., Blakemore, S.J. and Charman, T. (2006) Social Cognitive Development

During Adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, pp.165-74.

Claxton, G. (1998) Knowing without Knowing Why. The Psychologist, 11 (5), pp.217-20.

Clifford, M.M. (1988) Failure Tolerance and Academic Risk-Taking in Ten- to Twelve-

Year-Old Students. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, pp.15-27.

Clifford, M.M. and Chou, F.C. (1991) Effects of Payoff and Task Context on Academic

Risk Taking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83 (4), pp.499-507.

Coffield, F., Moseley, D., GHall, E. and Ecclestone, K. (2004) Learning Styles and

Pedagogy in Post-16 Learning: A Systematic and Critical Review (Report No.

041543). London, Learning and Skills Research Centre.

Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R. and Tsivkin, S. (1999) Sources of

Mathematical Thinking: Behavioral and Brain-Imaging Evidence, Science, 284,

pp.970-74.

Dennison, P.E. (1981) Switching On: A Guide to Edu-Kinesthetics. Ventura, California

Edu-Kinesthetics.

DeSantis, A.D., Webb, E.M. and Noar, S.M. (2008) Illicit Use of Prescription ADHD

Medications on a College Campus: A Multimethodological Approach. Journal of

American College Health, 56 (3), pp.315-23.

Dunn, R., Sklar, R.I., Beaudry, J. and Bruno, J. (1990) Effects of Mismatching Students'

Hemispheric Preferences on Mathematics Scores. Journal of Educational Research

and Extension, 83 (5), pp.283-88.

Elliot, R., Friston, K.J, and D., Raymond J. (2000) Dissociable Neural Responses in

Human Reward Systems. Journal of Neuroscience, 20 (16), pp.6159-65.

Elliott, R. and Deakin, B. (2008) "Neuroscience of Human Reward." Mental Capital and

Wellbeing, State-of-Science Reviews. London, Government Office for Science.

Ernst, M., Pine, D.S. and Hardin, M. (2005) Triadic Model of the Neurobiology of

Motivated Behavior in Adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 36, pp.299-312.

Page 19: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

19

Eshel, N., Nelson, E.E., Blair, R.J., Pine, D.S. and Ernst, M. (2007) Neural Substrates of

Choice Selection in Adults and Adolescents: Development of the Ventrolateral

Prefrontal and Anterior Cingulate Cortices. Neuropsychologia, 45 (6), pp.1270-79.

Farah, M.J. (2002) Emerging Ethical Issues in Neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 5

(11), pp.1123-29.

Fiorillo, C.D., Tobler, P.N. and Schultz, W. (2003) Discrete Coding of Reward Probability

and Uncertainty by Dopamine Neurons. Science, 299, pp.1898-902.

Friedrich, M. (2008) "Early Neural Markers of Language Learning Difficulty in German."

Mental Capital and Wellbeing, State-of-Science Reviews. London, Government

Office for Science.

Gardner, H. (1983) Frames of the Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. New York,

Basic Books.

Gardner, H. (1999) Intelligence Reframed. New York, Basic Books.

Gazzaniga, M.S. (2005) Smarter on Drugs. Scientific American: Mind, 32-37.

Geake, J.G. (2008) Neuromythologies in Education. Educational Research, 50 (2).

Gee, J.P. (2003) What Video Games Have to Teach Us About Learning and Literacy.

London, Palgrave Macmillan.

Gilmore, C.K., McCarthy, S.E. and Spike, E. (2007) Symbolic Arithmetic without

Instruction, Nature, 447, pp.589-91.

Goswami, U. (2004) Neuroscience and Education. British Journal of Educational

Psychology, 74, pp.1-14.

Goswami, U. (2008) Neuroscience in Education. Mental Capital and Wellbeing, State-of-

Science Reviews. London, Government Office for Science.

Gracia-Bafalluy, M. and Noel, M.-P. (2008) Does Finger Training Increase Young

Children's Numerical Performance? Cortex, 44, pp.368-75.

Greely, H, Sahakian, B., Harris, J., Kessler, R.C., Gazzaniga, M.S., Campbell, P. and

Farah, M.J. (2008) Towards Responsible Use of Cognitive-Enhancing Drugs by the

Healthy. Nature, 456, pp.702-05.

Greenberg, M.T. (2006) Promoting Resilience in Children and Youth - Preventive

Interventions and Their Interface with Neuroscience. In: Lester (ed) Conference

on Resilience in Children, Arlington, VA., Blackwell Publishing, pp.139-50.

Grigorenko, E.L. (2007) How Can Genomics Inform Education? Mind, Brain and

Education, 1 (1), pp.20-27.

Page 20: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

20

Gron, G., Kirstein, M., Thielscher, A., Riepe, M.W. and Spitzer, M. (2005) Cholinergic

Enhancement of Episodic Memory in Healthy Young Adults. Psychopharmacology,

182 (1), pp.170-79.

Guttorm, T.K., Leppanen, P.H.T., Poikeus, A.-M., Eklund, K.M., Lyytinen, P. and

Lyytinen, H. (YEAR) Brain Event-Related Potentials (Erps) Measured at Birth

Predict Later Language Development in Children with and without Familial Risk for

Dyslexia. Cortex, 41, pp.291-303.

Harter, S. (1978) Pleasure Derived from Cognitive Challenge and Mastery. Child

Development, 45, pp.661-69.

Hillman, C.H., Erickson, K.I. and Framer, A.F. (2008) Be Smart, Exercise Your Heart:

Exercise Effects on Brain and Cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, pp.58-

65.

Hoeft, F., Watson, C.L., Kesler, S.R., Bettinger, K.E. and Reiss, A.L. (2008) Gender

Differences in the Mesocorticolimbic System During Computer Game-Play. Journal

of Psychiatric Research, 42, pp.253-58.

Horn, G. (2008) Brain Science, Addiction and Drugs. London, Academy of Medical

Sciences.

Howard-Jones, P.A. (in press) Philosophical Challenges for Researchers at the Interface

between Neuroscience and Education. Journal of Philosophy of Education.

Howard-Jones, P.A. (2008) Fostering Creative Thinking: Co-Constructed Insights from

Neuroscience and Education. Bristol, Escalate.

Howard-Jones, P.A., Blakemore, S.J., Samuel, E., Summers, I.R., and Claxton, G. (2005)

Semantic Divergence and Creative Story Generation: An fMRI Investigation.

Cognitive Brain Research, 25, pp.240-50.

Howard-Jones, P.A., Bogacz, R., Demetriou, S., Leonards, U. and Yoo, J. (2009) From

Gaming to Learning: A Reward-Based Model of Decision-Making Predicts

Declarative Memory Performance in a Learning Game. British Psychological

Society Annual Conference, Brighton.

Howard-Jones, P.A. and Demetriou, S. (in press - now published on-line WHEN)

Uncertainty and Engagement with Learning Games, Instructional Science.

Hughes, C. (1998) Executive Function in Preschoolers: Links with Theory of Mind and

Verbal Ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, pp.233-53.

Hyatt, K.J. (2007) Brain Gym: Building Stronger Brains or Wishful Thinking? Remedial

and Special Education, 28 (2), pp.117-24.

Page 21: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

21

Immordino-Yang, M.H. (2007) A Tale of Two Cases: Lessons for Education from the

Study of Two Boys Living with Half Their Brains. Mind, Brain and Education, 1 (2),

pp.66-83.

Isaacs, E.B., Edmonds, C.J., Lucas, A. and Gadian, D.G. (2001) Calculation Difficulties in

Children of Very Low Birthweight - a Neural Correlate. Brain, 124, pp.1701-07.

Jaeggi, S.M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J. and Perrig, W.J. (2008) Improving Fluid

Intelligence with Training on Working Memory. Proceedings of the National

Academy of Sciences (USA), 105 (19), pp.6829-33.

Johnson, M.H. (2004) Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience: An Introduction. 2nd

edition.

Kaufmann, L. (2008) Dyscalculia: Neuroscience and Education. Educational Research, 50

(2).

Kaufmann, L., Handl, P. and Thony, B. (2003) Evaluation of a Numeracy Intervention

Program Focusing on Basic Numerical Knowledge and Conceptual Knowledge:A

Pilot Study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36 (6), pp.564-73.

Kaufmann, L., Vogel, S.E., Wood, G., Kremser, C., Schocke, M., Zimmerhackl, L.-B. and

Koten, J.W. (2008) A Developmental fMRI Study of Nonsymbolic Numerical and

Spatial Processing. Cortex, 44, pp.376-85.

Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P.J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrom, K.,

Gillberg, C.G., Forssberg, H. and Westerberg, H. (2005) Computerized Training of

Working Memory in Children with ADHD - a Randomized, Controlled Trial. Journal

of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44 (2), pp.177-86.

Knutson, B., Adams, C.M., Fong, G.W. and Hommer, D. (2001) Anticipation of Monetary

Reward Selectively Recruits Nucleus Accumbens. Journal of Neuroscience, 21,

RC159, pp.1-5.

Koepp, M.J., Gunn, R.N., Lawrence, A.D., Cunningham, V.J., Dagher, A., Jones, T.,

Brooks, D.J., Bench, C.J. and Grasby, P.M. (1988) Evidence for Striatal Dopamine

Release During a Video Game. Nature, 393, pp.266-68.

Kosslyn, S.M. (2005) Mental Images and the Brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22 (3-

4), pp.333-47.

Kratzig, G.P. and Arbuthnott, K.D. (2006) Perceptual Learning Style and Learning

Proficiency: A Test of the Hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 (1),

pp.238-46.

Page 22: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

22

Lerner, R.M. (2005) Promoting Positive Youth Development: Theoretical and Empirical

Bases. Washington D.C., National Research Council/Institute of Medicine.

Luna, B. (2004) Algebra and the Adolescent Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8,

pp.437-439.

Mahncke, H.W., Connor, B.B., Appelman, J., Ahsanuddin, O.N., Hardy, J.L., Wood, R.A.,

Joyce, N.M., Boniske, T., Atkins, S.M. and Merzenich, M.M. (2006) Memory

Enhancement in Healthy Older Adults Using a Brain Plasticity-Based Training

Program: A Randomized, Controlled Study. Proceedings of the National Academy

of Sciences of the United States of America, 103 (33), pp.12523-28.

Malone, T.W. (1981) Toward a Theory of Intrinsically Motivating Instruction. Cognitive

Science, 4, pp.333-39.

McCabe, S.E., Knight, J.R., Teter, C.J. and Wechser, H. (2005) Non-Medical Use of

Prescription Stimulants among US College Students: Prevalence and Correlates

from a National Survey. Addiction, 100 (1), pp.96-106.

McGivern, R.F., Andersen, J., Byrd, D., Mutter, K.L. and Reilly, J. (2002) Cognitive

Efficiency on a Match to Sample Task Decreases at the Onset of Puberty in

Children. Brain and Cognition, 50, pp.73-89.

Molfese, D.L. (2000) Predicting Dyslexia at 8 Years of Age Using Neonatal Brain

Responses. Brain and Language, 72, pp.238-45.

Neuper, C., Muller-Putz, G.R., Scherer, R. and Pfurtscheller, G. (2006) Motor Imagery

and EEG-Based Control of Spelling Devices and Neuroprostheses. Event-Related

Dynamics of Brain Oscillations. Amsterdam, Elsevier Science Bv, pp.393-409.

Nieuwenhuis, S., Heslenfeld, D.J., von Geusau, N.J.A., Mars, R.B., Holroyd, C.B. and

Yeung, N. (2005) Activity in Human Reward-Sensitive Brain Areas Is Strongly

Context Dependent. Neuroimage, 25 (4), pp.1302-09.

Noel, M.-P. (2005) Finger Gnosia: A Predictor of Numerical Abilities in Children? Child

Neuropsychology, 11, pp.413-20.

Nunez, J.M., Casey, B.J., Egner, T., Hare, T. and Hirsch, J. (2005) Intentional False

Responding Shares Neural Substrates with Response Conflict and Cognitive

Control. Neuroimage, 25, pp.267-77.

OECD (2007) Understanding the Brain: Birth of a New Learning Science. Paris, OECD.

OECD (2002) Understanding the Brain:Towards a New Learning Science. Paris, OECD

Publications.

Page 23: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

23

OfSTED (2001) Improving Attendance and Behaviour in Secondary Schools. London,

OfSTED.

Paus, T. (2008) Mapping Brain Maturation and Development of Social Cognition During

Adolescence. Mental Capital and Wellbeing, State-of-Science Reviews. London,

Government Office for Science.

Pickering, S.J., and Howard-Jones, P. (2007) Educators' Views on the Role of

Neuroscience in Education: Findings from a Study of Uk and International

Perspectives. Mind, Brain and Education, 1 (3), pp.109-13.

Plomin, R. (2008) Genetics and the Future Diagnosis of Learning Disabilities. Mental

Capital and Wellbeing, State-of-Science Reviews, London, Government Office for

Science.

Qin, Y., Carter, C.S., Silk, E.M., Stenger, V.A., Fissell, K., Goode, A. and Andersen, J.R.

(2004) The Change of the Brain Activation Patterns as Children Learn Algebra

Equation Solving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 101,

pp.5686-5691.

Roman, G.C. and Rogers, S.J. (2004) Donepezil: A Clinical Review of Current and

Emerging Indications. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 5 (1), pp.161-80.

SCMH (2005) The Future of Mental Health: A Vision for 2015. London, The Sainsbury

Centre for Mental Health.

Shaywitz, B.A., Shaywitz, S.E., Blachman, B.A., Pugh, K.R., Fullbright, R.K., Skudlarski,

P., Mencl, W.E., Constable, R.T., Holahan, J.M., Marchione, K.E., Fletcher, J.M.,

Lyon, G.R. and Gore, J.C. (2004) Development of Left Occipitotemporal Systems

for Skilled Reading in Children after a Phonologically-Based Intervention.

Biological Psychiatry, 55 (9), pp.926-33.

Shizgal, P. and Arvanitogiannis, A. (2003) Gambling on Dopamine. Science, 299,

pp.1856-58.

Springer, S.P. and Deutsch, G. (1989) Left Brain, Right Brain. New York, Freeman.

Starkey, P. and Cooper, R.G. (1980) Perception of Numbers by Human Infants, Science,

4473, pp.1033-35.

Steven, M.C., Kiehl, K.A., Pearlson, G.D. and Calhoun, V.D. (2007) Functional Neural

Networks Underlying Response Inhibition in Adolescents and Adults. Behavioural

Brain Research, 181 (1), pp.12-22.

Stevens, C., Fanning, J., Coch, D., Sanders, L. and Neuille, H. (2008) Neural

Mechanisms of Selective Auditory Attention Are Enhanced by Computerized

Page 24: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

24

Training: Electrophysiological Evidence from Language-Impaired and Typically

Developing Children. Brain Research, 1205, pp.55-69.

Stewart, W.J. (2008) Technology Futures. Mental Capital and Wellbeing, State-of-

Science Reviews. London, Government Office for Science.

Szucs, D. and Goswami, U. (2007) Educational Neuroscience: Defining a New Discipline

for the Study of Mental Representations. Mind, Brain and Education, 1 (3),

pp.114-27.

Szucs, D., Soltesz, F., Jarmi, E. and Csepe, V. (2007) The Speed of Magnitude

Processing and Executive Functions in Controlled and Automatic Number

Comparison in Children: An Electro-Encephalography Study. Behavioral and Brain

Functions, 3, pp.20.

Waterhouse, L. (2006) Multiple Intelligences, the Mozart Effect, and Emotional

Intelligence: A Critical Review. Educational Psychologist, 41 (4), pp.207-25.

Weisberg, D.S., Keil, F.C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E. and Gray, J. (2008) The Seductive

Lure of Neuroscience Explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20 (3),

pp.470-77.

Willis, S.L., Tennstedt, S.L., Marsiske, M., Ball, K., Elias, J., Mann Koepke, K., Morris,

J.N., Rebok, G.W., Unverzagt, F.W., Stoddard, A.M. and Wright, E. (2006) Long-

Term Effects of Cognitive Training on Everyday Functional Outcomes in Older

Adults. Journal of American Medical Association, 296 (23), pp.2805-14.

Wilson, A.J., Dehaene, S., Pinel, P., Revkin, S.K., Cohen, L. and Cohen, D. (2006)

Principles Underlying the Design Of "The Number Race", an Adaptive Computer

Game for Remediation of Dyscalculia. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 2 (19).

Wilson, R.S., Mendes de Leon, C.F., Barnes, L.L., Schneider, J.A., Bienias, J.L., Evans,

D.A. and Bennett, D.A. (2002) Participation in Cognitively Stimulating Activities

and Risk of Alzheimer Disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287

(6), pp.742-48.

Winter, B., Breitenstein, C., Mooren, F.C., Voelker, K., Fobker, M., Lechtermann, A.,

Krueger, K., Fromme, A., Korsukewitz, C., Floel, A. and Knecht, S. (2007) High

Impact Running Improves Learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 87,

pp.597-609.

Page 25: Potential educational developments involving neuroscience

www.beyondcurrenthorizons.org.uk

25

Commonly used abbreviations :

fMRI - functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)

OfSTED – Office for Standards in Education, Children‟s Services and Skills

ERP – Event Related Potential

EEG – Electroencephalogram

ADHD – Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

WM – Working Memory

EF – Executive Function

SLI – Specific Language Impairment

ACh – Acetylcholine

BCI – Brain Computer Interface

MI - Multiple Intelligences theory

VAK – Visual Auditory Kinaesthetic

SCMH - The Sainsbury Centre for Mental Health

This document has been commissioned as part of the UK Department for Children,

Schools and Families’ Beyond Current Horizons project, led by Futurelab. The views

expressed do not represent the policy of any Government or organisation.