neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

108
Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential regularities in changeable environments. A Dissertation Presented to The Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences Brandeis University Psychology Robert Sekuler, Department of Psychology, Advisor In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy by Abigail LaBombard Noyce February, 2014

Upload: others

Post on 18-Dec-2021

11 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

Neural processes for learning andmonitoring sequential regularities in

changeable environments.

A Dissertation

Presented to

The Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences

Brandeis University

Psychology

Robert Sekuler, Department of Psychology, Advisor

In Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements for the Degree

Doctor of Philosophy

by

Abigail LaBombard Noyce

February, 2014

Page 2: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

The signed version of this form is on file in the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences.

This dissertation, directed and approved by Abigail LaBombard Noyce’s committee, has been

accepted and approved by the Graduate Faculty of Brandeis University in partial fulfillment of the

requirements for the degree of:

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

Malcolm Watson, Dean of Arts and Sciences

Dissertation Committee:

Robert Sekuler, Department of Psychology, Chair

Angela Gutchess, Department of Psychology

Paul DiZio, Department of Psychology

Marc Howard, Department of Psychology, Boston University

Page 3: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

©Copyright by

Abigail LaBombard Noyce

2014

Page 4: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

Acknowledgments

First, I need to thank my advisor, Robert Sekuler, for my education in psychophysics, cognitive

research, academic writing, and being a scientist. His curiosity, enthusiasm, and high standards

have shaped this dissertation and my entire graduate career. Without his guidance and support this

document would not have been possible.

My committee members, Paul DiZio, Angela Gutchess, and Marc Howard, have been generous

with their time, their expertise, and their enthusiasm for my work. I am grateful for all of these

things, particularly that last.

My vision lab colleagues, past and present, have celebrated victories and commiserated over

defeats. Lisa Payne, Sujala Maharjan, Yile Sun, Avigael Aizenman, Chad Dube, Nichola Cohen,

Sylvia Guillory, Heather Sternshein, Jessica Maryott, and Kristina Visscher were there when I

needed them. A particular thank you to my undergraduate RAs: Erika Bell, Stephanie Bond, Brian

Dorfman, Arielle Keller, Rebecca Meyer.

At the University of New Hampshire, William Stine and Jill McGaughy helped me discover

that I like research, and Anne Gilman treated me like a grown-up scientist before anybody else did.

More recently, my ITFF colleagues have provided cheerleading, wisdom, and hilarity.

I am blessed with the most excellent family, by birth, by marriage, and by choice. My parents

and siblings, my in-laws, and my friends have been understanding, interested, excited, and proud.

There are too many of them to list, but I am deeply grateful. Finally, I need to thank my beloved

iv

Page 5: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

husband Daniel Noe, who has been picking up my slack for the last half-decade, and who has

always known I could do this.

v

Page 6: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

AbstractNeural processes for learning and monitoring sequential regularities in

changeable environments.A dissertation presented to the Faculty of

the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences ofBrandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts

by Abigail LaBombard Noyce

The world is largely stable and predictable. Humans and other organisms are sensitive to that

stability, and use it to support cognitive processes. This work consists of a series of studies that

explore how humans learn about such stability, use that information to generate predictions about

forthcoming sensory input, and detect when such predictions are inadequate. First, I present a

modeling study that quantified the distributions of errors that people make on a complex, visuomotor

sequence learning task, and examine the serial position dynamics of several parameters describing

short-term visual memory. Both precision and capacity for these sequences increases with familiarity,

and the worst-represented items show the largest increases. Next, I present an experiment that used

the same task to understand the effects of deviant items within familiar sequences. By measuring

ERPs to new, familiar, and deviant items, I dissociate the neural activity associated with detecting a

deviant from that associated with encoding task-relevant stimulus characteristics. Finally, I present

an experiment investigating the role of prediction in a task that is stochastic, rather than sequential,

and in which deviant events occurred among the distractors rather than among the task-relevant

stimuli. Unexpected events among the distractors seem to obligatorily attract attention, enhancing

or impairing performance. Further, I show that the individual differences in the neural response to

such unexpected events is predicted by temperament. Together, these studies illuminate how the

brain learns about predictability in a range of settings, and leverages such predictability to facilitate

cognition.

vi

Page 7: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

Contents

Abstract vi

1 Introduction 11.1 Sequences in short-term memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21.2 Dissociating new and deviant events in a dynamic environment . . . . . . . . . . . 31.3 Neural and behavioral effects of implicit prediction monitoring . . . . . . . . . . . 6

2 Dynamic reallocation of VSTM resources 82.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82.2 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112.3 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 202.4 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41

3 P300 to violations of newly learned predictions 453.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 453.2 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 473.3 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 553.4 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61

4 Leaky ignoring in the flanker task 654.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 654.2 Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 674.3 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 744.4 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84

5 Conclusion 875.1 Further evidence for human sensitivity to environmental structure and predictability 885.2 Future work . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89

References 93

vii

Page 8: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

Chapter 1

Introduction

The nervous systems of humans (and many other organisms) are exquisitely tuned to patterns and

structure in sensory inputs. This tuning is generally adaptive , as the world contains a great deal of

sequential regularity—cases where one sensory event reliably predicts a following event. These

regularities extend from simple, predictable inputs, such as background noise or the sensation

produced by a surface on which one is sitting, all the way to elaborately structured regularities such

as the syntax of language, or the key of a piece of music.

The brain uses these regularities to increase the efficiency and efficacy of perceptual and

cognitive processes. Reports of visual stimuli held in short-term memory are biased towards the

average or most-common stimulus characteristics (Dube & Sekuler, 2012; Huang & Sekuler, 2010a,

2010b; Payne, Guillory, & Sekuler, 2013). Such bias occurs even when the characteristics of a

currently-visible stimulus must be reported (Chalk, Seitz, & Series, 2010). In tasks involving natural

scenes, perception is biased towards stimuli that are consistent with the surrounding context, as

seen by facilitated speed and accuracy of identifying such stimuli (e.g. Bar, 2004; Biederman,

Mezzanotte, & Rabinowitz, 1982; Kersten, Mamassian, & Yuille, 2004; Gronau, Neta, & Bar, 2008;

Mandler, 1980), and in tasks involving faces and attributes, people are faster and more accurate when

1

Page 9: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

the physical features of the face are congruent with the trait being described (Cassidy, Zebrowitz,

& Gutchess, 2012). Cross-modally, visual input can impair or enhance speech perception, as in

the McGurk effect, or when auditory input is embedded in noise (Guttman, Gilroy, & Blake, 2005;

Zion Golumbic, Cogan, Schroeder, & Poeppel, 2013). Perception is also facilitated by occurring

at a predictable time. The brain appears to modulate the signal-to-noise gain of the visual system

when the timing of upcoming stimuli is known (Cravo, Rohenkohl, Wyart, & Nobre, 2013).

Regularities are also able to guide attention. It is well documented that attentional cueing can

facilitate detection and processing of cued stimuli (e.g Posner, 1980; Posner, Snyder, & Davidson,

1980; Huang & Sekuler, 2010a, 2010b). Such cueing depends on the expectation that cues are

predictive, which allows the subject to allocate attention appropriately (C. Summerfield & Egner,

2009). However, such attentional shifting does not require explicit cues. Structured scenes enable

the allocation of attention to appropriate objects (J. J. Summerfield, Lepsien, Gitelman, Mesulam,

& Nobre, 2006), and even when temporal structure is irrelevant, it triggers spatial and featural

attentional capture (Le Pelley, Vadillo, & Luque, 2013; Zhao, Al-Aidroos, & Turk-Browne, 2013).

Anticipation and predictions derived from such regularities also manifest in anticipatory eye

movements (Barnes, 2008; Burke & Barnes, 2007; Elsner, Falck-Ytter, & Gredeback, 2012; Kowler,

1989; Maryott, Noyce, & Sekuler, 2011; Rotman, Troje, Johansson, & Flanagan, 2006) and motor

control schema (Cohn, DiZio, & Lackner, 2000; Hu, 2010; Pigeon, Bortolami, DiZio, & Lackner,

2003). The studies that I report in this dissertation improve our understanding of the neural

mechanisms that underly anticipatory and predictive effects on cognition.

1.1 Sequences in short-term memory

Cognitive skills ranging from language and navigation to visually guided manipulation of tools and

devices depend upon an ability to represent the sequential order of events. In particular, humans

2

Page 10: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

frequently need to maintain a sequence of events in short-term memory for subsequent analysis

or reproduction. Previous work with serial recall tasks has demonstrated that sequences held in

memory are susceptible to strong primacy and recency effects (e.g. Agam, Bullock, & Sekuler,

2005; Kahana & Jacobs, 2000; Lashley, 1951; Maryott et al., 2011), and may depend on chunking

adjacent items within a sequence (Farrell, Hurlstone, & Lewandowsky, 2013; Hitch, Fastame,

& Flude, 2005; Maryott & Sekuler, 2009). Although much of the classic work on learning and

short-term memory for sequences has used lists of verbal stimuli (e.g. Ebbinghaus, 1913; Hebb,

1961; Hitch et al., 2005; Howard & Kahana, 1999; Kahana & Jacobs, 2000), studying short-term

memory for visual stimuli has some pronounced advantages. Verbal (and verbalizable) stimuli

can be tightly compressed in short-term memory; further, subjects can actively rehearse a verbal

representation, refreshing it over the course of retention with very little loss of fidelity (Ricker &

Cowan, 2010).

The graded fidelity with which subjects can reproduce previously-seen visual stimuli is ideal for

testing the relationships between precision and capacity limits in short-term memory for sequences

of visual stimuli. The study presented in Chapter 2 investigates how familiarity changes the precision

and effective capacity of short-term memory. To do this, I quantified the distributions of errors that

people make on a visuomotor sequence learning task.

1.2 Dissociating new and deviant events in a dynamic environ-

ment

As described above, humans use sequential regularities in the environment to support a wide range

of cognitive processes. These effects depend upon the brain’s ability to work in a predictive,

anticipatory way, using context, prior knowledge, and previous experiences to build expectations

3

Page 11: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

about forthcoming sensory inputs. However, to navigate and manage a dynamic environment, people

cannot blindly follow those expectations. Otherwise, we may step onto a non-existent stair, or walk

into a previously-safe location only to be attacked by an unexpected foe. Monitoring mechanisms

are required to detect events that deviate from the previously established regularities, allowing

people to change planned behaviors and learn about the new state of the world.

Monitoring processes are evident in tasks with an explicitly sequential structure, such as the

serial reaction time task (Nissen & Bullemer, 1987). In this task, various stimuli on the screen

cue subjects to press a particular key, and the sequence of stimuli (and thus key presses) repeats

throughout the course of the experiment. When a stimulus that is not part of the usual sequence

occurs, people are slower and less accurate to respond to it (Ferdinand, Mecklinger, & Kray, 2008;

Gobel, Sanchez, & Reber, 2011; Russeler & Rosler, 2000). These tasks do not require explicit

memory and encoding for the sequence of stimuli, and moreover, the sequence remains stable over

the course of a relatively-long experimental block.

When a subject’s expectation is specifically about a reward value, the relationship between

expectation and actual reward is coded by dopaminergic cells in the ventral tegmental area (Schultz,

2006; Schultz, Dayan, & Montague, 1997). Rewards that are lower than the predicted reward

lead to a decrease in dopamine release; rewards that are greater than the predicted reward lead

to an increase (Schultz et al., 1997). This dopaminergic signal is generally thought to be a key

driver of learning from outcomes that are better or worse than expected (e.g. Schoenbaum, Esber,

& Iordanova, 2013; Takahashi et al., 2009, 2011), but it’s not known whether it is appropriate

to extrapolate such mechanisms to predictions that are not explicitly about reward. (See Kakade

and Dayan (2002) for an argument that novelty is intrinsically rewarding, leading to dopaminergic

spiking, and Laurent (2008) for a counter.)

One area involved in prediction monitoring regardless of the presence of reward is the anterior

cingulate cortex (Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, 2004; Kennerley, Behrens, & Wallis, 2011). The

4

Page 12: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

anterior cingulate has strong links to frontal and prefrontal cortical areas, to sensory cortices, and

to the limbic system (Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Nieuwenhuis, 2004). Its activation to

new information in the environment is accompanied by the P3 ERP component, a positive-going

deflection over central electrodes from 300–500 ms after such information is presented (Crottaz-

Herbette & Menon, 2006; Linden, 2005). ACC activation (and the P3) have been observed in a

wide range of cognitive contexts, and are theorized to reflect a cognitive control process that is

responsible for developing and adjusting the working memory representation of the context, task set

or forthcoming actions (Goldstein, Spencer, & Donchin, 2002; Crottaz-Herbette & Menon, 2006;

Nieuwenhuis, Aston-Jones, & Cohen, 2005; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004). A similar theory is proposed

by Holroyd and Yeung (2012), who assert that the ACC’s primary role is to hold and monitor the

options available for behavior. The P3 has at least two subcomponents: the Novelty P3, elicited by

stimuli that violate expectations, and the P300, elicited by task-relevant stimuli (Goldstein et al.,

2002; Linden, 2005; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005; Polich, 2007).

Prediction monitoring processes’ main role may be to guide learning (Maier & Steinhauser,

2013). Pearce and Hall (1980) argue that classical conditioning depends on the unconditioned

stimulus being unpredictable, while Wills, Lavric, Croft, and Hodgson (2007) demonstrate that

associative learning occurs most rapidly for cues that remain predictive, but whose outcome changes

and Ouden, Friston, Daw, McIntosh, and Stephan (2009) show that when auditory distractors predict

visual distractors, the neural response to such predicted events decreases across trials. Similarly, in

motor control tasks, subjects learn from the mismatch between the expected motor output and the

movement that actually occurs (Cohn et al., 2000; Lackner & DiZio, 2005), but not from explicit

knowledge about the state of the world (Hu, 2010).

The experiment reported in Chapter 3 investigates the neural processes that support (i) explicit

sequential encoding and (ii) monitoring such sequences for deviant events. Using the visuomotor

sequence learning task described in Chapter 2, I measured ERPs to new, familiar, and deviant

5

Page 13: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

sequence items. By comparing ERPs to new and to deviant items, I dissociated the neural activity

related to triggering a prediction-monitoring mechanism from that related to encoding a new

stimulus exemplar. I hypothesized that both new and deviant items would enhance a P3 ERP, but

that new items would elicit only an enhanced P300, while deviant items would also elicit a Novelty

P3. These results show that the theoretical structure surrounding P3 generation is also applicable to

dynamic environments where governing regularities are frequently changing.

1.3 Neural and behavioral effects of implicit prediction moni-

toring

While the task in Chapters 2 and 3 has sequential structure that subjects are explicitly aware of,

predictive and monitoring processes occur even when subjects are not attending to the governing

regularity. In Chapter 4, I looked for behavioral and neural evidence that people are sensitive to

regularities in peripheral stimuli that they are actively attempting to ignore.

Previous work in associative-learning contexts demonstrated that unexpected outcomes trigger

the allocation of attention (Wills et al., 2007). Further, deviant events in a stream of unattended

auditory stimuli capture attention, leading to poorer detection of targets in a simultaneously-

presented stimulus stream (Schroger, 1996), and deviant tones (Escera, Alho, Winkler, & Naatanen,

1998; Nostl, Marsh, & Sorqvist, 2012; Parmentier, Elford, Escera, Andres, & San Miguel, 2008) and

vibro-tactile stimuli (Parmentier, Ljungberg, Elsley, & Lindkvist, 2011; Ljungberg & Parmentier,

2012) preceding a visual target delay target detection.

The mismatch negativity (MMN) is a negative-going deflection in the ERP elicited by a stimulus

that violates some governing regularity. While it is historically studied in the auditory domain, the

existence of a visual mismatch negativity (vMMN) has recently been established (Czigler, 2007;

6

Page 14: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

Kimura, Schroger, & Czigler, 2011; Stefanics, Kimura, & Czigler, 2011). The MMN is believed

to be generated by interactions between a predictive signal from higher-order (likely prefrontal)

areas and the bottom-up input reaching sensory cortex (Garrido, Kilner, Stephan, & Friston, 2009;

Wacongne, Changeux, & Dehaene, 2012; Wacongne et al., 2011; Winkler, 2007).

To investigate the potential attentional capture of deviant events in a task that requires stimulus

discrimination rather than mere target detection, I drew upon the Eriksen flanker task, an experi-

mental paradigm that was designed to elicit interactions between multiple, conflicting sources of

visual information (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). A single target, which can generally be in one of

two states, is accompanied by flanking distractors, which can match or differ from the target. The

congruency between the distractors and the target influences the accuracy and reaction time with

which subjects can report the state of the target (e.g Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974; Ridderinkhof, Band,

& Logan, 1999).

The experiment presented in Chapter 4 examined how predictability of distractors influenced

cognitive processing in the flanker task, and the relationship between such influences and the ERPs

elicited by onset of standard and deviant distractors.

7

Page 15: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

Chapter 2

Dynamic reallocation of VSTM resources

2.1 Introduction

Holding visual information in short-term memory is critical in many daily tasks. As researchers

have attempted to characterize the structure of visual short-term memory (VSTM), it has become

necessary to understand how resources are allocated among multiple items that are held simultane-

ously in memory. Here, I investigate how such allocation changes as people gain experience with

the items. Real-life use of visual short-term memory almost always interacts with learning and

long-term memory.

Recent work attempting to quantify the allocation of VSTM resources among items has drawn

on signal-detection approaches to understanding cognition. This approach assumes that the rep-

resentations of items in memory are noisy, or otherwise imperfect, and attempts to characterize

that imperfection. The most common method has been to ask subjects to reproduce from memory

some characteristic of an item, measure the degree to which their reproduction differs from the

initial stimulus, and use the resulting distribution of errors as a proxy for the noise of the memory

representation (e.g. Wilken & Ma, 2004; Zhang & Luck, 2008).

8

Page 16: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

A family of related models has been proposed to characterize such error distributions. The

earliest had two parameters: one describing the probability that subjects responded randomly,

presumably due to guessing, rather than by reporting the probed item; the other describing the

spread of responses around the true value (Zhang & Luck, 2008, 2009, 2011). Another group

extended this basic model to include the possibility that subjects report the identity of a non-probed

item from the memory set, leading to a third parameter: the probability of a non-target report (Bays,

Catalao, & Husain, 2009; Bays, Gorgoraptis, Wee, Marshall, & Husain, 2011; Bays, Wu, & Husain,

2011; Gorgoraptis, Catalao, Bays, & Husain, 2011; Zokaei, Gorgoraptis, Bahrami, Bays, & Husain,

2011). Finally, recent work has moved towards fitting the error distributions with a model in which

the amount of resources allocated to each object may vary from trial to trial (van den Berg, Shin,

Chou, George, & Ma, 2012; Fougnie, Suchow, & Alvarez, 2012).

While the majority of these experiments have used “snapshot” displays, in which an array

of items are briefly presented and then one item is probed, a handful of notable exceptions have

controlled for the possibility that this approach may confound encoding bottlenecks, such as

perceptual crowding, with limits on VSTM capacity (Bays et al., 2009). For example, Emrich

and Ferber (2012) and Gorgoraptis et al. (2011) both showed that inter-item confusion (i.e. non-

target reports) increases when items are shown in succession, but precision and the rate of random

responses remain constant. However, both of these studies used arrays of static stimuli and compared

a “snapshot” display to a series of two or three displays each containing a subset of the items. The

static nature of their stimuli leave open questions about VSTM structure for dynamic stimuli.

Further, none of the previous work with this family of models has investigated the effects of

learning on VSTM representations of information. It is well-established that memory accuracy

improves as items become more familiar (e.g. Ebbinghaus, 1913; Hebb, 1961; Howard & Kahana,

1999; Maryott et al., 2011), presumably due to increased support from long-term memory. However,

it is unknown how such support changes the distribution of errors, or the probability that a non-target

9

Page 17: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

is reported.

Sekuler, Siddiqui, Goyal, and Rajan (2003) and Agam et al. (2005) established a VSTM memory

task with the dynamic properties that have been lacking in the previous modeling work. In their task,

subjects observed and reproduced motion trajectories enacted by a disk on a computer screen. The

stimuli themselves comprised position changes over time, and each segment of a motion trajectory

was linked with its successor. Agam et al. put forth a single, scalar measure of reproduction accuracy

that has thus far been used to describe memory performance in this task. Several following papers

have used that measure to characterize the effects of learning on such performance (Agam, Galperin,

Gold, & Sekuler, 2007; Agam, Huang, & Sekuler, 2010; Maryott et al., 2011). However, the body

of VSTM modeling work described above has made it clear that memory errors may arise from

multiple sources: An item may be successfully retrieved from memory, but with added noise; an

item may fail to be encoded into memory, or fail to be retrieved; or an item may be mis-bound with

its location in the stimulus set, leading to non-target reports. The measure used in these previous

studies does not distinguish between these sources of recall errors.

Here, I characterize familiarity-induced changes in VSTM representations by fitting four related

VSTM models to responses generated on the visuomotor sequence learning task introduced by

Agam et al. (2005). I fit each model to errors generated at each level of increasing familiarity in

order to assess how model parameters changed as subjects learned a sequence. Using the resulting

serial position dynamics, I then characterized the effects of learning on VSTM characteristics such

as precision and number of items stored. The results presented in this chapter show that VSTM has

a remarkable ability to reallocate memory resources over the course of learning.

10

Page 18: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

2.2 Methods

To investigate the effects of learning on visual short-term memory, I fit several models to data from

a motion trajectory learning task. These data were previously collected by myself and others to

characterize the effects of learning on memory for non-verbal sequences, using a single measure

of accuracy (mean absolute error; Agam et al., 2005), and to investigate neural correlates of such

learning (see Agam et al., 2007; Maryott et al., 2011; Noyce & Sekuler, in revisions). All subjects

(N = 64) were young adults recruited from the Brandeis University community. Re-analyzing this

data with multiple parameters, as I have done here, leads to new insights into the effects of learning

on memory for these sequences.

2.2.1 Experimental task and accuracy measures

The data were collected from a task in which subjects observed and reproduced psuedo-random

motion trajectories. Each trajectory was presented multiple times in succession, and each subject

saw approximately 75 such trajectories. The details of the task varied slightly between experiments;

Table 2.1 summarizes the relevant details for each dataset.

Figure 2.1 illustrates the sequence of events within one presentation of a trajectory. On each

presentation, a small disk traversed a path comprising either five or six connected linear motion

segments. After each segment, the disk paused briefly before resuming its motion in a changed

direction. The disk then disappeared from view. After a retention interval, a second disk appeared

at a random point within the central portion of the display, cueing the subject to move a handheld

stylus over the surface of a graphics tablet in order to reproduce from memory the sequence of

disk motions that had just been seen. During the reproduction, the disk’s motion was yoked to the

movement of the stylus’ tip on the graphics tablet. No other feedback was provided. Note that

neither the stimulus nor the reproduction disk left a visible trail while moving across the computer

11

Page 19: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Stimulus Cue Stimulus Trajectory Retention Interval Reproduction Cue Reproduction

0.60 s 4.50 s 3.75 s

Figure 2.1: One presentation of a motion sequence stimulus. At the start of each presentation, adisk appeared at the center of the display before beginning to move in a series of either five or sixconnected linear segments without leaving any visible trail. After enacting the movements, the diskdisappeared from view. After a retention interval, a second disk appeared, signaling the subjectto begin reproducing from memory the path that had been traveled by the first disk. Each trialconsisted of either four or five such presentations.

display. To obtain a complete representation of the disk’s path, subjects would have to extract it

from the disk’s motions, and maintain it in short-term memory until reproduction was required

(Geisler, Albrecht, Crane, & Stern, 2001; Jancke, 2000).

Each trial’s quasi-random sequence of motion segments was generated by the algorithm de-

scribed by Agam et al. (2005). The direction of a sequence’s initial motion was chosen randomly,

and the magnitude of the direction change at each “corner” of the trajectory was between 30 and

150. Changes in direction could be clockwise or counter-clockwise, with equal probability. The

motions comprising a sequence were constrained by several additional rules: Motion segments were

not permitted to intersect, could not come within one-half a segment’s length of intersecting, nor

could they extend beyond the boundaries of the visible display area.

The fidelity of each reproduction was quantified offline by means of an algorithm that used

the subject’s pauses and direction changes to divide the reproduction into segments (Agam et al.,

2005; Maryott & Sekuler, 2009; Maryott et al., 2011). After segmentation, the algorithm estimated

the direction of each such segment by fitting a line to its beginning and end points. Reproduction

accuracy was then quantified by directional error: the angular difference between the direction of a

12

Page 20: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Table 2.1: Details and sources of the datasets included in these analyses.

Subjects Trials Published in

5 segments and4 presentations

12 76 Agam et al. (2007)

12 88 Noyce and Sekuler (in revisions); presented in Chapter 3

9 88 Noyce and Sekuler (unpublished)

6 segments and5 presentations

12 76 Agam et al. (2007)

8 72 Maryott et al. (2011)

11 72 Maryott et al. (2011)

motion segment in the reproduction and the direction of the corresponding segment in the stimulus

model. (See Agam et al. (2005) for a comparison of scalar measures for accuracy on this task.)

On every trial, a unique trajectory was presented either four or five times (see Table 2.1),

with subjects reproducing the sequence after each such presentation. This allowed me to measure

familiarity-induced changes in the quality of the reproduction.

2.2.2 Datasets

Table 2.1 identifies the sources and details of the behavioral data used to fit a family of four related

models. Four of the datasets were previously published using mean absolute direction error to

characterize learning. Each dataset is from an experiment with between 8 and 12 subjects and

approximately 75 trials per subject. Three datasets are from experiments using five-segment motion

trajectories with each trajectory repeated four times; three are from experiments using six-segment

trajectories with each trajectory repeated five times.

The precise size and timing of stimuli varied somewhat across the six studies. Each segment

of a trajectory took 500-525 ms; pauses at the corners of segments lasted 225-450 ms. Stimuli

were thus 3400-5400 ms in length. During one experiment, subjects were instructed to track the

13

Page 21: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

stimulus disc with their eyes (Maryott et al., 2011, Experiment 1), in two others they were instructed

to maintain fixation at the center of the screen (Noyce & Sekuler, in revisions; Noyce & Sekuler,

unpublished), and in the remaining three they were given no instructions about eye movements

(Agam et al., 2007, Experiments 1 and 2; Maryott et al., 2011, Experiment 2). Depending on

the eye movement instructions, the size of the stimulus varied, such that each segment traversed

4.5(tracking), 1(fixation), or 1.5(free eye movements) visual angle.

2.2.3 Models

I fit four members of a family of VSTM models to data collected from a visual sequence-learning

task, and assessed the ways that the models’ parameters varied with serial position and repeated

presentation.

Model 0 : Slots Mixture Model

The simplest class of models I considered was based on the one described by Wilken and Ma (2004)

and Zhang and Luck (2008). Here, the probability that subjects make a response error x is given by:

P (x) = (1− g)×M(0,SD)(x)︸ ︷︷ ︸responses from memory

+ g × U(x)︸ ︷︷ ︸guess responses

where M(µ,σ)(x) is the probability density function of the von Mises distribution with mean µ and

standard deviation σ, as evaluated at x and U(x) is the probability density function of a uniform

distribution, evaluated at x.

This model predicts a distribution of response errors that comprises the sum of a von Mises

(circular Gaussian) distribution with standard deviation SD and mean zero, and a uniform dis-

tribution. g specifies the proportion of the distribution that is from the uniform component; the

14

Page 22: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

remainder is from the von Mises component. In other words, SD relates to the precision of an item’s

representation in working memory, and g relates to the probability that the item is in memory at all.

Model 0 required each of these parameters to be fixed across serial positions, to allow testing

of the hypothesis that VSTM for items does not vary with their serial position. Zhang and Luck

(2008, 2009, 2011)’s conceptualization of VSTM as a set of two to four slots specifies that memory

resources are allocated in fixed quanta, and cannot accommodate effects of serial position on

memory accuracy.

Model 1: Standard Mixture Model

Model 1 allows both SD and g to vary across serial positions. The probability of a response error x

is given by the same equation as in Model 0, except that this distribution is computed independently

for each serial position on each repeated presentation. Previous analyses of these data have shown

strong serial-position effects on accuracy, and so this and all subsequent models allow parameters

to vary with serial position.

Model 2: Standard Mixture Model With Swaps

The third model in the family was described by Bays et al. (2009). This model extends the Standard

Mixture Model to account for the possibility that subjects report non-target items from a stimulus

display, rather than making random errors. Remember that “random” here means that subjects

selected a response without being affected by the item they’re supposed to be reproducing, nor by

any other items in the stimulus set. In Model 2, the probability that subjects make a response error

15

Page 23: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

x is given by:

P (x) = (1− g −B)×M(0,SD)(x)︸ ︷︷ ︸responses from memory

+ g × U(x)︸ ︷︷ ︸guess responses

+D∑i=1

(B

D×M(di,sd)(x)

)︸ ︷︷ ︸

inter-item confusion responses

with distribution terms as above; di refers to the ith other segment in the trajectory, of D total other

segments. (These are equivalent to “distractors” in the snapshot displays.)

This model incorporates information about the other segments in the trajectory, and produces

a distribution of response errors that comprises the sum of a uniform distribution with several

Gaussian distributions. With probability B, responses are drawn from a von Mises distribution

centered on one of the other segments in the display (each other segment is equiprobable); with

probability g, responses are drawn from the uniform distribution; with probability (1 − g − B),

responses are drawn from the von Mises distribution centered on the target.

Note that my implementation of this model does not restrict to “true swaps” among items. That

is, I don’t restrict to cases where item A is reported as item B and item B is reported as item A. The

swaps are estimated for each serial position independently. This allows the possibility that subjects

report one item at more than one serial position.

Model 3: Variable Precision Model

The final of my family of models is based on those described by Fougnie et al. (2012) and van

den Berg et al. (2012). Instead of fixing a precision parameter, this model allows it to vary across

trials. It is plausible that the allocation of memory resources among items, and even the amount of

memory resources available to encode the set, may vary across trials. Thus, the probability of a

16

Page 24: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

response error x on the ith trial is given by:

P (x) = (1− g)×M(0,sdi)(x)︸ ︷︷ ︸responses from memory

+ g × U(x)︸ ︷︷ ︸guess responses

where sdi is drawn from the Gamma distribution with scale:

θ =2sdSD2

modeSD +√

modeSD2 + 4sdSD2

and shape:

k = 1 + modeSD +1

θ

The parameters that are fitted here are g, the probability that responses are from the uniform

distribution, and modeSD and sdSD which characterize the Gamma distribution from which

precision values are drawn on any given trial. High modeSD values characterize a distribution

“centered” around less-precise representations, while high sdSD values characterize a distribution

with a lot of variability in precision values.

This model produces a distribution of responses that comprises the sum of a uniform distribution

and many von Mises distributions with mean 0 and varying SDs. The resulting distribution is

“peakier” than that of the Standard Mixture Model.

2.2.4 Model fitting

Models were specified and fitted using the MemToolbox (Suchow, Brady, Fougnie, & Alvarez,

2013) for Matlab (The MathWorks, In., Natick, MA). Within each dataset, I fitted each model

to the distribution of responses produced at each serial position on each repeated presentation.

For example, for the five-segment datasets, I fit each model independently to five segment serial

positions on each of four repeated presentations. The model fitting protocol used an iterative

17

Page 25: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

maximum likelihood procedure, which started three searches at different locations within the

parameter space, adjusted the parameters until a local maximum was reached, and ran until the three

searches converged. Further, I took advantage of MemToolbox’s hierarchical fitting abilities, which

constrain the parameters by requiring that each fitted parameter be normally distributed across

the subjects in each dataset. This helps keep the fitting procedure from converging on extremely

unlikely values, a trait that was particularly important because of the noisiness of these data, and

because of the correlations among model parameters.

2.2.5 Measures

After fitted model parameters were calculated, I used two measures to summarize familiarity’s

influence on the representation of items in VSTM. First, to characterize the overall trend in each

parameter, I took its mean fitted value across serial positions, on each repeated presentation.

Second, to characterize the magnitude of serial position effects, I took the standard deviation of the

parameter’s values at each serial position.

To test the significance of these summary measures’ changes across presentations, I used a

permutation test. On each of 10,000 iterations, each subject’s fitted parameter values were randomly

allocated among segments and presentations. The two summary measures described above were

calculated subjectwise for each simulated presentation. The slope of the best-fitting line over serial

positions was calculated, giving a distribution of 10,000 such slopes for each measure and parameter.

Familiarity-induced changes in parameter values were then tested against these distributions.

I used two measures to compare models. First, the Akaike information criterion (AIC), given by

AIC = 2k − 2 ln(L)

where k is the number of parameters in the model, and L is the likelihood of the fitted model.

18

Page 26: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Because the models were fitted using maximum likelihood estimation, AIC is maximized in the

fitting process. I therefore also compared models using a statistic that will behave somewhat

differently, the Kuiper statistic (Kuiper, 1960). Unlike measures such as likelihood and root

mean squared error, the Kuiper statistic is intended for comparing continuous, rather than discrete,

distributions. It is related to the better-known Kolmogorov-Smirnov (K-S) measure, but is more

sensitive at the tails of the distribution and is cyclicly invariant (and so better suited for assessing

probability distributions in circular space). The Kuiper statistic consists of the empirically-obtained

cumulative distribution function’s maximum distance above the model cumulative distribution

function, added to the empirical cumulative distribution function’s maximum distance below the

model cumulative distribution function.

Kuiper = max(cdfmodel − cdfdata) + max(cdfdata − cdfmodel)

For each pairwise comparison between models, I bootstrapped a distribution of mean difference

in Kuiper statistics and mean difference in AIC. For each subject, I averaged the probability density

functions of the two models being compared, and used the resulting distribution to generate two

samples of trials. I calculated the Kuiper statistic and AIC for each sample, took the differences,

and averaged those differences across my 64 subjects. I thus derived a distribution of 500 such

differences that arose when the null hypothesis was true and the samples were drawn from the

same distribution. Comparing my empirically-obtained Kuiper statistics and AIC differences to this

distribution allowed me to obtain p-values.

19

Page 27: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Table 2.2: Results of repeated-measures ANOVAs on mean absolute directional error, with factorspresentation and serial position.

five-segment data six-segment data

parameter source df1, df2 F partial η2 df1, df2 F partial η2

accuracy

presentation 3, 96 77.435∗∗∗ .708 4, 120 156.237∗∗∗ .839

serial position 4, 128 44.036∗∗∗ .579 5, 150 57.457∗∗∗ .657

pres. × serial pos. 12, 384 5.655∗∗∗ .150 20, 600 15.708∗∗∗ .344

∼p < .1. ∗p < .05. ∗∗p < .01. ∗∗∗p < .001.

2.3 Results

In this section, I first present the actual accuracy data, then fitted parameters from each model, and

finally comparisons between the four models I tested.

2.3.1 Accuracy

Figures 2.2 and 2.3 show the distributions of errors that subjects make while performing this task, for

the five-segment and six-segment data. Subjects’ mean absolute directional error on each segment

was 21.66 (SD = 6.46).

Separate two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs on the five-segment and six-segment data

confirmed a significant main effect of presentation (p < .001), a significant main effect of serial

position (p < .001), and a significant interaction between the two (p < .001). The full results are

shown in Table 2.2.

20

Page 28: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Segment serial position

Presentation 40.2

0

Presentation 10.2

0

0.2

0

Presentation 30.2

0

0 180 0 180 0 180 0 180 0

1 2 3 4 5

Presentation 2

Prop

ortio

n of

erro

rs

Figure 2.2: Histograms showing the distributions of errors generated by 33 subjects on the five-segment task. Each row corresponds to a presentation; each column corresponds to a serial position.Distributions generated on early presentations are short, squat, and kurtotic, especially at thepenultimate serial position; distributions generated on late presentations are tall and narrow.

21

Page 29: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Segment serial position

Presentation 40.2

0

Presentation 10.2

0

0.2

0

Presentation 30.2

0

0 180 0 180 0 180 0 180 0

1 2 3 4 5

Presentation 2

Prop

ortio

n of

erro

rs

6

0.2

0

Presentation 5

180 0

Figure 2.3: Histograms showing the distributions of errors generated by 31 subjects on the six-segment task. Each row corresponds to a presentation; each column corresponds to a serial position.Distributions generated on early presentations on short, squat, and kurtotic, especially at thepenultimate serial position; distributions generated on later presentations are tall and narrow.

22

Page 30: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

0

0.1

0.2

Fitte

d g

valu

es

10

20

30

40

Fitte

d SD

val

ue

-4 -3 -2 last-1 -4 -3 -2 last-1-5

Segment serial position

-4 -3 -2 last-1 -4 -3 -2 last-1-5

5 segments over 4 presentations 6 segments over 5 presentations

12

3

4

12

3

4

Standard0

1 2 3

4 5

Figure 2.4: Model 0: Standard Mixture Model with no serial position variability. Fitted SD(top) and g (bottom) values from the standard mixture model, by segment serial position andrepeated presentation. Note that the fitted g values for 5-segment sequences are nearly identical onpresentations three and four, making the plotted lines overlap to the point where presentation fourappears to be missing. Error bars are repeated-measures standard error (Loftus & Masson, 1994;Cousineau, 2005; Morey, 2008).

23

Page 31: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCESPr

opor

tion

of T

rials

0

0.3

0

0.3

0

0.3

0

0.3

Segment Serial Position

1 2 3 4 5

Directional error

0 0 0 0 0180 180 180 180 180-180 -180 -180 -180 -180

Presentation 1

Presentation 2

Presentation 3

Presentation 4

Figure 2.5: Model 0: Error distributions and fitted model predictions for a representative subject.Panels are arranged in rows by presentation and in columns by serial position.

2.3.2 Model 0: Standard Mixture Model with no serial position variability

Figure 2.4 shows the fitted SD and g values from Model 0 (Standard Mixture Model with no

serial position variability), across segment serial position and repeated presentation. In each panel,

five-segment sequences are shown on the left and six-segment sequences are shown on the right.

Note that for both parameters and both sequence lengths, the fitted values are higher (reflecting

worse memory performance) on the first presentation than on subsequent presentations.

Figure 2.5 shows the error distributions generated by a representative subject who was perform-

24

Page 32: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Table 2.3: Results of repeated-measures ANOVAs on each parameter of Model 0, with factorpresentation.

five-segment data six-segment data

parameter source df1, df2 F partial η2 df1, df2 F partial η2

SD presentation 3, 96 10.270∗∗∗ .243 4, 120 12.684∗∗∗ .297

g presentation 3, 96 5.132∗∗ .138 4,120 15.395∗∗∗ .339

∗p < .05. ∗∗p < .01. ∗∗∗p < .001.

ing the five-segment version of the task. Overlain on the histograms are the distributions predicted

by this subject’s fitted Model 0 parameters.

Separate one-way repeated-measures ANOVAs on the five-segment and six-segment data

confirmed a significant main effect of presentation (p < .003 for both SD and g). The full results

are shown in Table 2.3.

Figure 2.6 summarizes the change in each parameter across repeated presentations. On average,

SD decreases across presentation (p < .001), suggesting that subjects represent each item more

precisely as items become more familiar. Similarly, on average, g decreases across presentations

(p < .001), suggesting that increased familiarity supports subjects in successfully forming a

representation of each item.

2.3.3 Model 1: Standard Mixture Model

Figure 2.7 shows the fitted SD and g values from Model 1 (Standard Mixture Model), across

segment serial position and repeated presentation. In each panel, five-segment sequences are shown

on the left and six-segment sequences are shown on the right. Note that for both parameters and

both sequence lengths, fitted values are higher (reflecting worse memory performance) on the first

presentation than on subsequent presentations. Further, the shapes of the first presentation curves

25

Page 33: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

-1.6

-0.7

Mea

n fit

ted

g (lo

g sc

ale)

5 segments6 segments

1.0

1.5

Fitte

d SD

val

ue (l

og s

cale

)

1 2 3 4 5

1 2 3 4 5

Repeated presentation

Standard0

30

20

10

0.20

0.10

0.05

5 segments6 segments

Figure 2.6: Effects of repeated presentations on the fitted parameters of Model 0 (Standard MixtureModel with no serial position variability). (top) Mean SD significantly decreases over repeatedpresentations for 5-segment (blue) and 6-segment (green) stimuli (p < .001). (bottom) Mean gsignificantly decreases over repeated presentations (p < .001).

for SD differ substantially from the those for g, with SD showing a strong primacy effect through

the penultimate item, while g shows only a 1-item primacy effect.

Figure 2.8 shows the error distributions generated by a representative subject who was perform-

ing the five-segment version of the task. Overlain on the histograms are the distributions predicted

by this subject’s fitted Model 1 parameters.

Separate two-way repeated measures ANOVAs on the five-segment and six-segment data

separately confirmed a significant main effect of presentation (p < .001) and a significant main

effect of serial position (p < .001) for both parameters, and a significant presentation by serial

26

Page 34: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

10

20

30

40

Fitte

d sd

val

ue

-4 -3 -2 last-1 -4 -3 -2 last-1-5

Segment serial position

0

0.1

0.2

Fitte

d g

valu

e

-4 -3 -2 last-1 -4 -3 -2 last-1-5

5 segments over 4 presentations 6 segments over 5 presentations

12

3

4

12

3

4

12

3

4

5

12

3

4

5

Standard

Figure 2.7: Model 1: Standard Mixture Model. Fitted SD (top) and g (bottom) values from thestandard mixture model, by segment serial position and repeated presentation.

position interaction for SD (p < .01). The full results are shown in Table 2.4.

Figure 2.9 summarizes the change in each parameter across repeated presentations. On average,

SD decreases across presentations (p < .001), suggesting that subjects represent each item more

precisely as items become more familiar. Further, the inter-item variation in SD also decreases as

items become more familiar (p < .001), suggesting that the learning process enables subjects to

more evenly allocate resources among items in a sequence. Similarly, on average, g decreases across

27

Page 35: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCESPr

opor

tion

of T

rials

0

0.3

0

0.3

0

0.3

0

0.3

Segment Serial Position

1 2 3 4 5

Directional error

0 0 0 0 0180 180 180 180 180-180 -180 -180 -180 -180

Presentation 1

Presentation 2

Presentation 3

Presentation 4

Figure 2.8: Model 1: Error distributions and fitted model predictions for a representative subject.Panels are arranged in rows by presentation and in columns by serial position.

28

Page 36: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Table 2.4: Results of repeated-measures ANOVAs on each parameter of Model 1, with factorspresentation and serial position.

five-segment data six-segment data

parameter source df1, df2 F partial η2 df1, df2 F partial η2

SD

presentation 3, 96 38.131∗∗∗ .544 4, 120 64.438∗∗∗ .682

serial position 4, 128 21.169∗∗∗ .398 5, 150 24.223∗∗∗ .447

pres. × serial pos. 12, 384 2.833∗∗ .081 20, 600 6.456∗∗∗ .177

g

presentation 3,96 27.084∗∗∗ .458 4,120 44.223∗∗∗ .600

serial position 4,128 15.484∗∗∗ .587 5, 150 13.946∗∗∗ .317

pres. × serial pos. 12, 384 1.457 .044 20, 600 2.475∗∗ .076

∼p < .1. ∗p < .05. ∗∗p < .01. ∗∗∗p < .001.

presentations (p < .001), suggesting that increased familiarity supports subjects in successfully

forming a representation of each item, and the inter-item variation in g also decreases (p < .001),

further supporting the hypothesis that learning enables subjects to allocate resources more evenly.

2.3.4 Model 2: Standard Mixture Model With Swaps

Figure 2.10 shows the fitted SD, g, and B values from Model 2 (the model with inter-item swaps),

across segment serial position and repeated presentation. Five-segment sequences are shown on

the left of each panel; six-segment sequences are shown on the right. The fitted values of SD and

g are higher (reflecting worse memory performance) on the first presentation than on subsequent

presentations, and the curves for these parameters are extremely similar to those from Model 1.

B (probability of inter-item confusions) accounts for less than 1% of all responses, and shows no

consistent patterns across serial position or repeated presentation.

Figure 2.11 shows the error distributions generated by a representative subject who was perform-

ing the five-segment version of the task. Overlain on the histograms are the distributions predicted

29

Page 37: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

-1.6

-0.9

Seria

l pos

ition

effe

cts

on g

(log

sca

le)

-1.6

-0.8

Mea

n fit

ted

g (lo

g sc

ale)

0.3

1.0

Seria

l pos

ition

effe

cts

on s

d (lo

g sc

ale)

1.2

1.5

Mea

n fit

ted

sd (l

og s

cale

)

5 segments6 segments

5 segments6 segments

5 segments6 segments

5 segments6 segments

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

Repeated presentation Repeated presentation

Standard

20

25

3010

7

5

2

0.15

0.10

0.05

0.12

0.10

0.07

0.05

Friday, October 4, 13

Figure 2.9: Effects of repeated presentations on the fitted parameters of Model 1 (Standard MixtureModel). (A) Mean SD decreases over repeated presentations for 5-segment (blue) and 6-segment(green) stimuli. (B) Variability in SD across serial positions decreases over repeated presentations.(C) Mean g decreases over repeated presentations. (D) Variability in g across serial positionsdecreases over repeated presentations.

by this subject’s fitted Model 2 parameters. These distributions appear virtually identical to those

predicted by Model 1 because the contribution of B is very small.

Separate two-way repeated measures ANOVAs on the five-segment and six-segment data

confirmed a significant main effect of presentation (p < .001) and a significant main effect of serial

position (p < .001) for both SD and g, and a significant presentation by serial position interaction

for SD (p < .001). Neither main effect, nor the interaction, was significant for B. The full results

30

Page 38: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

0

0.1

0.2

Fitte

d B

valu

es

0

0.1

0.2

Fitte

d g

valu

e

10

20

30

40

Fitte

d sd

val

ue

-4 -3 -2 last-1 -4 -3 -2 last-1-5

Segment serial position

-4 -3 -2 last-1 -4 -3 -2 last-1-5

5 segments over 4 presentations 6 segments over 5 presentations

12

3

4

12

3

4

12

3

4

5

12

3

4

5

Swap

-4 -3 -2 last-1-5-4 -3 -2 last-1

12

3

4

12

3

4

5

Figure 2.10: Model 2: Fitted SD (top), g (middle), and B (bottom) values from the model withinter-item confusions, by segment serial position and repeated presentation.

31

Page 39: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCESPr

opor

tion

of T

rials

0

0.3

0

0.3

0

0.3

0

0.3

Segment Serial Position

1 2 3 4 5

Directional error

0 0 0 0 0180 180 180 180 180-180 -180 -180 -180 -180

Presentation 1

Presentation 2

Presentation 3

Presentation 4

Figure 2.11: Model 2: Error distributions and fitted model predictions for a representative subject.Panels are arranged in rows by presentation and in columns by serial position.

32

Page 40: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Table 2.5: Results of repeated-measures ANOVAs on each parameter of Model 2, with factors serialposition and presentation.

five-segment data six-segment data

parameter source df1, df2 F partial η2 df1, df2 F partial η2

SD

presentation 3, 96 37.866∗∗∗ .542 4, 120 59.317∗∗∗ .664

serial position 4, 128 21.869∗∗∗ .406 5, 150 24.439∗∗∗ .449

pres. × serial pos. 12, 384 2.948∗∗∗ .084 20, 600 5.823∗∗∗ .162

g

presentation 3,96 26.498∗∗∗ .435 4,120 39.566∗∗∗ .569

serial position 4,128 16.169∗∗∗ .336 5, 150 12.844∗∗∗ .300

pres. × serial pos. 12, 384 1.349 .041 20, 600 3.120∗∗∗ .094

B

presentation 3,96 .104 .003 4,120 1.619 .051

serial position 4,128 1.498 .045 5, 150 1.698 .054

pres. × serial pos. 12, 384 1.198 .036 20, 600 .743 .024

∼p < .1. ∗p < .05. ∗∗p < .01. ∗∗∗p < .001.

are shown in Table 2.4.

Figure 2.12 shows the change in each parameter across repeated presentations. SD and g show

essentially the same patterns as in Model 1 (means and variability decrease across presentations, all

ps < .001); B does not reliably change with presentation, nor does its serial position variability (all

ps > .1).

2.3.5 Model 3: Variable Precision Model

Figure 2.13 shows the fitted modeSD, sdSD, and g values from Model 3 (variable precision model

with guessing). Five-segment sequences are shown on the left of each panel, six-segment sequences

are shown on the right. The fitted values of modeSD, sdSD, and g are all higher (reflecting worse

memory performance) on the first presentation than on subsequent presentations.

33

Page 41: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

0.3

1.0

Seria

l pos

ition

effe

cts

on s

d (lo

g sc

ale)

-2.2

-1.5

Seria

l pos

ition

effe

cts

on B

(log

sca

le)

-2.4

-1.8

Mea

n fit

ted

B (lo

g sc

ale)

1.2

1.5

Mea

n fit

ted

sd (l

og s

cale

)

-1.8

-0.8

Seria

l pos

ition

effe

cts

on g

(log

sca

le)

-1.8

-0.8

Mea

n fit

ted

g (lo

g sc

ale)

5 segments6 segments

5 segments6 segments

5 segments6 segments

5 segments6 segments

5 segments6 segments

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

5 segments6 segments

Swap

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

Repeated presentation Repeated presentation

0.15

0.10

0.05

0.02

0.15

0.10

0.05

0.02

20

25

3010

7

5

0.015

0.010

0.005

0.03

0.02

0.01

2

Figure 2.12: Effects of repeated presentations on the fitted parameters of Model 2 (model withinter-item confusions). (A, C) Mean SD and g decrease over repeated presentations for 5-segment(blue) and 6-segment (green) stimuli. (B, D) Variability in SD and g across serial positions decreasesover repeated presentations. (E) B does not vary over repeated presentations. (F) Variability in Bacross serial positions does not vary with repeated presentations.

34

Page 42: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

0

5

10

Fitte

d sd

SD v

alue

10

20

30

40

Fitte

d m

odeS

D va

lue

0

0.1

0.2

Fitte

d g

valu

e

-4 -3 -2 last-1 -4 -3 -2 last-1-5

Segment serial position

-4 -3 -2 last-1 -4 -3 -2 last-1-5

5 segments over 4 presentations 6 segments over 5 presentations

12

3

4

12

3

4

VariP gamma

-4 -3 -2 last-1-5-4 -3 -2 last-1

12

3

4

12

3

4

5

1 2 3

4 5

12

3

4

5

Friday, October 4, 13

Figure 2.13: Model 3: Fitted modeSD (top), sdSD (middle), and g (bottom) values from thevariable-precision model with guessing, by segment serial position and repeated presentation.

35

Page 43: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCESPr

opor

tion

of T

rials

0

0.3

0

0.3

0

0.3

0

0.3

Segment Serial Position

1 2 3 4 5

Directional error

0 0 0 0 0180 180 180 180 180-180 -180 -180 -180 -180

Presentation 1

Presentation 2

Presentation 3

Presentation 4

Figure 2.14: Model 3: Error distributions and fitted model predictions for a representative subject.Panels are arranged in rows by presentation and in columns by serial position.

36

Page 44: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Table 2.6: Results of repeated-measures ANOVAs on each parameter of Model 3, with factorspresentation and serial position.

five-segment data six-segment data

parameter source df1, df2 F partial η2 df1, df2 F partial η2

modeSD

presentation 3, 96 6.615∗∗∗ .171 4, 120 15.299∗∗∗ .338

serial position 4, 128 6.645∗∗∗ .172 5, 150 6.003∗∗∗ .167

pres. × serial pos. 12, 384 0.936 .028 20, 600 3.288∗∗∗ .099

sdSD

presentation 3, 96 5.414∗∗ .145 4,120 6.856∗∗∗ .186

serial position 4, 128 3.021∗ .086 5, 150 2.840∗ .087

pres. × serial pos. 12, 384 0.941 .029 20, 600 1.037 .033

g

presentation 3, 96 19.409∗∗∗ .378 4,120 52.522∗∗∗ .637

serial position 4, 128 11.425∗∗∗ .263 5, 150 10.533∗∗∗ .260

pres. × serial pos. 12, 384 1.583 .047 20, 600 2.095∗∗ .065

∗p < .05. ∗∗p < .01. ∗∗∗p < .001.

Figure 2.14 shows the error distributions generated by a representative subject who was perform-

ing the five-segment version of the task. Overlain on the histograms are the distributions predicted

by this subject’s fitted Model 2 parameters. The markedly-different shapes of these distributions is

caused by the combination of multiple von Mises distributions, due to the assumption of variable

precision across trials

Separate two-way repeated measures ANOVAs on the five-segment and six-segment data

separately confirmed a significant main effect of presentation (p < .001) and a significant main

effect of serial position (p < .001) for both modeSD and g, and a significant presentation by serial

position interaction for both parameters on the six-segment data (p < .05). Neither main effect, nor

the interaction, was significant for sdSD. The full results are shown in Table 2.6.

Figure 2.15 summarizes the change in each parameter across repeated presentations. On average,

modeSD decreases across presentations (p < .001 for both 5-segment and 6-segment stimuli),

37

Page 45: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

-0.3

0.7

Seria

l pos

ition

effe

cts

on s

dSD

(log

scal

e)

-0.4

0.5

Mea

n fit

ted

sdSD

(log

sca

le)

0.4

1.0

Seria

l pos

ition

effe

cts

on m

odeS

D (lo

g sc

ale)

1.1

1.5

Mea

n fit

ted

mod

eSD

(log

scal

e)

5 segments6 segments

-1.6

-0.9

Seria

l pos

ition

effe

cts

on g

(log

sca

le)

-1.5

-0.7

Mea

n fit

ted

g (lo

g sc

ale)

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

VariP gamma

1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5

Repeated presentation Repeated presentation

5 segments6 segments

0.15

0.10

0.05

0.12

0.10

0.07

0.05

30

25

20

15

10

7

5

5 segments6 segments

5 segments6 segments

2.0

1.0

1.5

2.5

5 segments6 segments

5.0

2.0

1.05 segments6 segments

Friday, October 4, 13

Figure 2.15: Effects of repeated presentations on the fitted parameters of Model 3 (the variable-precision model with guessing). (A) Mean modeSD decreases over repeated presentations for5-segment (blue) and 6-segment (green) stimuli. (B) Variability in modeSD across serial positionsdecreases over repeated presentations. (C) Mean sdSD does not reliably change over repeatedpresentations. (D) Variability in sdSD across serial positions does not reliably change over repeatedpresentations. (E) Mean g decreases over repeated presentations. (F) Variability in g across serialpositions decreases over repeated presentations.38

Page 46: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

implying that subjects are drawing from a range of precisions centered around increasingly precise

representations. The inter-item variability in modeSD also decreases with repeated presentations.

However, while both the mean values of sdSD and the serial position effects on sdSD had negative

slopes, their decrease was less reliable (.020 < p < .071). Finally, as in Models 1 and 2 both the

average fitted values of g and the serial position variability of g decrease with repeated presentations

(p < .001), similarly to the behavior of g in Models 1 and 2.

One point which is not evident from any of the stats displayed here is that a majority of subject-

segment-presentation combinations converge at the minimum sdSD value, making them effectively

equivalent to the Model 1 (standard mixture model) fits. On average, 90.5% of fits to the five-

segment data and 87.0% of fits to the six-segment data converged on the minimum sdSD value.

To test whether such convergence depended on the presentation and segment serial position, I ran

two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs on the five-segment and six-segment data separately. There

was no significant effect of presentation, serial position, or the interaction between them (all ps

> .09). This high rate of convergence to the minimum sdSD value makes me somewhat skeptical

of Model 3’s apparent improvements.

2.3.6 Assessing model fits

I used the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Kuiper statistic to assess the fit of each model.

After computing the difference in these measures between pairs of models, I bootstrapped a

distribution of differences that occurred when data were drawn from the same generative model.

This allowed me to compute p-values for measures whose distributions are not known a priori, such

as AIC and Kuiper.

Table 2.7 shows the pairwise differences in mean Akaike information criterion (AIC) between

each of the models, and the associated p-values. As expected, the models with increasing complexity

39

Page 47: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

Table 2.7: Pairwise AIC comparison between models. Positive numbers imply a better fit (lowerAIC) for the the model in column B; negative numbers would imply a better fit for the model incolumn A.

A B difference p-value

Model 0 vs. Model 1 5.102 .500

Model 2 9.217 .224

Model 3 19.005 .032

Model 1 vs. Model 2 3.779 .596

Model 3 17.531 .024

Model 2 vs. Model 3 13.752 .076

Table 2.8: Pairwise Kuiper statistic comparison between models. Positive numbers imply a betterfit (lower Kuiper statistic) for the the model in column B; negative numbers imply a better fit forthe model in column A.

A B difference p-value

Model 0 vs. Model 1 −0.008 .524

Model 2 −0.006 .596

Model 3 0.006 .636

Model 1 vs. Model 2 0.002 .776

Model 3 0.014 .036

Model 2 vs. Model 3 0.013 .232

40

Page 48: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

do show an AIC benefit, although the only significant differences are between Model 3, the variable

precision model, and Models 0 and 1 (mean AIC is 19.005 lower in Model 3 than Model 0,

p = .032; AIC is 17.531 lower in Model 3 than Model 1, p = .024). Table 2.8 shows the pairwise

differences in mean Kuiper statistic between each of these models. Effects here are almost entirely

non-significant, with Model 0 outperforming Models 1 and 2 (although with p > .52), and Models 2

and 3 outperforming Model 1. However, the only significant effect is between Model 3, the Variable

Precision Model, and Model 1, the Standard Mixture Model (mean Kuiper statistic is 0.014 lower in

Model 3, p = .036).

2.4 Discussion

I investigated the effects of learning on visual short-term memory by using data collected from a

visuomotor sequence-learning task. Subjects viewed and reproduced sequences of motion segments

several successive times, and the accuracy with which they did so was recorded. I then fit four

related models to the distributions of response errors, and assessed how the models’ parameters

changed with increased familiarity with each sequence. Unlike previous work on the role of learning

in this task (Agam et al., 2007, 2010; Maryott et al., 2011), my model-fitting can specify the degree

to which different kinds of memory failures contribute to subjects’ performance.

I found that no matter which model I fit, the value of the parameter or parameters describing the

spread of subjects’ responses decreased over repeated presentations, suggesting that the learning

process utilizes long-term memory to increase the precision of representation in memory. Further,

this decrease was not linear; rather, the decrease from the first presentation to the second was

larger than any of the following changes. Familiarity produced a noticeable increase in VSTM

precision after only a single exposure. The magnitude of the serial position effect on this parameter

also decreased across repetitions, again with large decreases from the first to second presentation,

41

Page 49: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

followed by smaller changes thereafter.

Similarly, when models included a parameter describing the probability that subjects responded

randomly rather than giving a response based on the item being probed, that parameter’s value

decreased over repeated presentations. This suggests that long-term memory resources not only

enable subjects to reproduce items more precisely, but also enable them to maintain more items in

memory. Again, the magnitude of the serial position effect decreased across repeated presentations,

and, again, both of these decreases were largest from the first to second presentation, with smaller

changes thereafter.

Previous work on this task assessed the likelihood of transpositions, operationalizing them

as cases where swapping two segments in the reproduction led to lower absolute error on both

(Agam et al., 2005). Here, I am using a very different approach to assess the frequency of swaps.

Simulations suggest that, even if each memory representation is centered on the correct segment,

the imperfections of such representations can lead to cases where swapping segments would reduce

absolute error. The analysis here only counts swaps that occur above the levels that are attributable

to imperfect representations. Unlike Agam et al. (2005)’s result, my Model 2 found very low levels

of inter-item confusions, with no systematic effects of serial position and presentations. Apparent

transpositions can result from noisy representations alone.

My results make clear that the learning induced by repeated encounters with each trajectory

changes two separate aspects of the VSTM representation of the segments in that trajectory. The

representation of each segment becomes more precise, and the probability that each segment is

successfully stored and retrieved increases. In short, familiarity is not just inducing a tradeoff

between parameters, but actually increasing the amount of information that subjects are able to

hold in VSTM and retrieve on demand. Further, these simultaneous changes are seen after even

one prior presentation – the mechanism underlying this increased capacity is capable of one-shot

learning. I should note that I am assuming that long-term memory supports increased retention

42

Page 50: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

ability, but it is also possible that the support is occurring instead (or as well) at encoding or at

retrieval. These studies do not allow me to determine at which memory stage the improvements are

occurring. This is counter to other results that propose a trade-off between precision and capacity in

VSTM (Roggeman, Klingberg, Feenstra, Compte, & Almeida, 2013). However, those authors did

not look at the effects of learning or familiarity; their subjects only saw each display one time.

One possible neural mechanism by which long-term memory processes are able to support

VSTM after previous exposures to a stimulus is chunking, in which subjects group segments into

larger items (Miller, 1956; Agam & Sekuler, 2008; Gobet & Simon, 1998). When an item is

composed of a few chunks rather than of many segments, each chunk can have more memory

resources allocated to it, possibly leading to improved precision and capacity. Maryott and Sekuler

(2009) showed that young adults perform better on trajectories whose segments are easily grouped

than they do on those whose segments are not, suggesting that a grouping or chunking process may

underly performance on this task.

Another possible mechanism underlying these changes with familiarity is that as the brain

learns about a given stimulus, it develops a sparser representation of the elements of that sequence

(Barlow, 1961; Agam et al., 2010; Karlsson & Frank, 2008). That is, the number of neurons

required to represent each element is decreased, likely through changes in neuronal tuning. As

the representation becomes sparser, there may be less overlap among the representations of the

elements of a given trajectory, so that it is easier for the brain to maintain separate representations.

This could allow both more precise representations of a direction of motion, and the maintenance of

a greater number of such representations simultaneously.

Note that familiarity-induced changes in both precision and guess rate converge on a flat serial

position curve. On successive presentations of the same stimulus sequence, the VSTM system is

reallocating memory resources such that errors are distributed more evenly among items. In other

words, the presentation-over-presentation changes in precision and guess rate are larger for items

43

Page 51: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 2. DYNAMIC REALLOCATION OF VSTM RESOURCES

that had high amounts of error on the first presentation, and lower (or even zero) for those that had

low amounts of error. Such dynamic reallocation suggests a strong preference within the system

for representing all items imperfectly, rather than trading off high precision on one item for poor

precision on another.

It also demonstrates that the VSTM system has access to a great deal of metacognitive informa-

tion about the quality of the representations in long-term memory. This information may be directly

accessible from long-term memory, or it may be accessed more indirectly. For example, the system

may attempt to predict the direction of the disk on the subsequent presentation, and use the degree

of error on each segment as a cue to allocate more memory resources to that segment. Future work

should attempt to distinguish between these possibilities.

Further, my results are inconsistent with a slots conceptualization of VSTM resource allocation

(as proposed by Zhang & Luck, 2008, 2009, 2011), in which short-term memory resources are

allocated in quanta of fixed size. Such a model is insufficient to account for the variation in precision

and guess rate that I see in my fitted model parameters. The effects of segment serial position and of

repeated presentation, along with the model-fitting comparison’s support for the variable-precision

model, implies greater flexibility in the system, and integration with long-term memory processes,

than has previously been demonstrated. In particular, the reliable familiarity-induced decrease in

guessing cannot be accounted for by a slot model. VSTM resources must be flexibly allocated to

produce the results presented in this chapter.

44

Page 52: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

Chapter 3

P300 to violations of newly learned

predictions

3.1 Introduction

The human brain frequently operates in feedforward mode, exploiting previously-experienced

regularities to build expectations for future events. This proactive operation facilitates perceptual

processing (Bar, 2009) and makes it possible for appropriate behaviors to be prepared and executed

in a timely fashion (e.g. Kowler, 1989; Maryott et al., 2011). Among the richest regularities

available to the brain are ones entailed not in single isolated events, but in event sequences. In fact,

the brain constructs and continuously updates its representation of such sequential regularities in an

obligatory and effortless manner (Johnson & Donchin, 1982; Kimura, Kondo, Ohira, & Schroger,

2011; Sternberg & McClelland, 2012).

In order to benefit fully from the advantages of feedforward operation, the brain must have

a mechanism to detect events that violate its expectations, and to thereby trigger an appropriate

response (Winkler, 2007). Such responses might include heightening attention to the unexpected

45

Page 53: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

event, modifying or delaying a prepared behavior, or updating the brain’s representation of the

regularity at hand.

Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) provide a direct measure of neural activity that is time-

locked to specific events (Luck, 2005). Their temporal precision makes ERPs a useful tool for

studying the neural reaction to individual events within a sequential structure. The P3 is one ERP

component that has been hypothesized to reflect the brain’s response to a novel or surprising stimulus

(Linden, 2005; Polich, 2007; K. C. Squires, Wickens, Squires, & Donchin, 1976). While the P3

has historically been studied in oddball paradigms (e.g Courchesne, Hillyard, & Galambos, 1975;

N. K. Squires, Squires, & Hillyard, 1975; Zuijen, Simoens, Paavilainen, Naatanen, & Tervaniemi,

2006), where a stream of stimuli contains both frequent and infrequent stimulus exemplars, it has

also been studied in sequence-learning tasks. Stimuli that deviate from an established sequential

structure elicit a larger P3 response than stimuli that conform to the sequential structure (Ferdinand et

al., 2008; Russeler & Rosler, 2000; Russeler, Hennighausen, Munte, & Rosler, 2003; Schlaghecken,

Sturmer, & Eimer, 2000). However, the studies demonstrating this P3 enhancement have required

subjects to learn only a single sequence per block of trials, allowing them to encode and maintain

one sequential regularity that remained valid for an extended period of time.

I wanted to investigate the neural mechanisms underlying sequence learning and prediction

monitoring in a more ecologically valid setting, where the governing regularities were short-lived.

To do so, I adopted the task used by Maryott et al. (2011) to investigate the behavioral response to

deviant events embedded in complex sequential structures that frequently changed. In that study,

subjects had to remember and reproduce short sequences of movements, each approximately five

seconds long. Each sequence was seen and reproduced several times, but occasionally a deviant,

unexpected item was inserted into a well-learned sequence. Subjects successfully incorporated the

deviant items into their representation of the sequence, and even showed a slight benefit for deviant

items. Note that Maryott et al.’s task differs from traditional sequence-learning paradigms such as

46

Page 54: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

the serial reaction time task (Nissen & Bullemer, 1987) in that it required that the brain frequently

update its representation of the governing sequential structure, as a new sequence began every 60-90

seconds.

To investigate whether this dynamic, changing context would alter the brain’s response to a

deviant sequence item, I recorded EEG signals from subjects who were performing a variant of the

task used by Maryott et al., and measured ERPs to new, familiar, and deviant sequence items. I

hypothesized that I would find a P3-like component in response to both new and deviant sequence

items, but that the timing and distribution would vary somewhat between the two conditions,

capturing the distinctions that have previously been found between the Novelty P3 and the task-

relevance P300 (Polich, 2007; Goldstein et al., 2002; Linden, 2005).

Here, I show that both new and deviant sequence items evoke a larger P3 than do familiar

sequence items. Deviant sequence items elicit both a P300 and a Novelty P3 relative to familiar

items, while new sequence items elicit only a P300 (and that of slightly lower amplitude). My results

show that the neural response to deviant sequence elements in a frequently updated environment

confirms that to deviant items in more stable settings.

3.2 Methods

3.2.1 Subjects

Twelve young adults (seven female, ages 19–28) participated in this experiment. All were naıve to

the task; all were right-handed.

47

Page 55: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

3.2.2 Experimental task

To induce and measure sequence learning, I asked participants to observe and reproduce pseudo-

random motion trajectories. Each trajectory was presented four successive times, and each partici-

pant saw 128 different trajectories.

On each presentation of a trajectory, a small disk traversed a path comprising five connected

linear motion segments. Figure 2.1 illustrates the sequence of events within one such presentation.

Each segment of the trajectory was 1 cm (approximately 1 visual angle) in length, and the disk

moved at a constant speed of 2 cm per second, taking 500 ms to travel the length of each segment.

After each segment, the disk paused for 400 ms before resuming its motion in a changed direction.

The yellow disk then disappeared from view. After a retention interval of 3750 ms, a second disk

appeared, cueing the subject to move a handheld stylus over the surface of a graphics tablet (31×

24 cm; Intuos 3, Wacom, Vancouver, WA) in order to reproduce from memory the sequence of

disk motions that had just been seen. During the reproduction, the disk’s motion was yoked to the

movement of the stylus’ tip on the graphics tablet. No other feedback was provided. Note that

neither the stimulus nor the reproduction disk left a visible trail while moving across the computer

display. Subjects viewed the stimuli from a distance of approximately 57 cm, and were instructed to

maintain fixation on a central cross, and refrain from blinking, while the stimulus disk was on the

screen.

Each trial’s quasi-random sequence of five motion segments was generated by the algorithm

described by Agam et al. (2005). The direction of a sequence’s initial motion was chosen randomly,

and the direction change at each corner of the trajectory was between 30 and 150. These changes in

direction could be clockwise or counter-clockwise, with equal probability. The motions comprising

any sequence were constrained by several additional rules: Motion segments were not permitted to

intersect, could not come within one-half a segment’s length of intersecting, nor could they extend

48

Page 56: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

beyond the boundaries of the display area.

3.2.3 Design and procedure

On every trial, that trial’s unique trajectory was presented four times, with participants reproducing

the sequence after each such presentation. Each set of four presentations constituted either a

Congruent trial, in which all four presentations of a sequence were identical to one another, or

a Flip trial, in which one motion segment changed direction on the sequence’s fourth (and final)

presentation. On the last (fourth) presentation of a Flip trial, the established trajectory’s final

segment was replaced by a segment in which the disk’s motion was 180 opposite. Figure 3.1

illustrates these two conditions.

I operationalized three types of events: new, familiar, and deviant. New events were motion

segments on their first presentation; familiar events were segments on their fourth presentation;

deviant events were the “flipped” final segment of the final presentation on Flip trials. See Section

3.2.6 (page 52) for further descriptions of these operationalizations. Note that new events are

congruent with the task context, and thus very similar to other events within the experiment.

Each subject completed four 45-minute experimental sessions, in which he or she observed

and reproduced 32 different trajectories four times each. In each session, the first two trials were

Congruent trials, followed by 20 Congruent and 10 Flip trials, block-randomized so that Flip

trials were more evenly distributed. Forty trials (approximately 31%) were Flip trials; the remainder

(88 trials) were Congruent trials. This approximates the ratio of Flip to Congruent trials used by

Maryott et al. (2011), in which subjects did not anticipate the flips (as demonstrated by subjects’

anticipatory eye movements). Subjects were never informed that some trials would be Flip trials.

49

Page 57: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

Presentation

1 2 3 4

1Flip

Congruent

flip

2 3 4

Figure 3.1: The sequence of presentations that made up a Congruent trial or Flip trial. In myanalyses I consider the first presentation of a stimulus to be new, the fourth presentation of aCongruent trial to be familiar, and the “flipped” segment at the end of a Flip trial to be deviant.

3.2.4 Behavioral data analysis

The fidelity of each reproduction was quantified offline by means of a two-step algorithm (Agam et

al., 2005; Maryott et al., 2011) that used pauses and direction changes to divide the reproduction

into segments. The algorithm then estimated the direction of each such segment by fitting a line

to its beginning and end points. Reproduction accuracy was quantified by directional error: the

absolute angular difference between the direction of a motion segment in the reproduction and the

direction of the corresponding segment in the stimulus model.

Note that this segmentation algorithm fails if it divides a reproduced trajectory into a number

of segments that differs from the number in the model trajectory. To increase the likelihood that

reproductions would be successfully divided into five motion segments, I instructed subjects to

try to produce the same number of segments that had been in the stimulus (five) and to, insofar as

possible, draw straight lines with corners between them. These instructions allowed the segmentation

algorithm to successfully divide over 90% of trials.

50

Page 58: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

3.2.5 Electrophysiological recording

A high-density EEG system (Electrical Geodesics, Inc., Eugene, OR) with 129 electrodes sampled

scalp electroencephalographic signals at 250 Hz using a high-impedance amplifier. Signals were

recorded for later, off-line analysis. All channels were adjusted for scalp impedance below 50 kΩ;

after 12 trials, channels were again adjusted for impedance below 50 kΩ scalp impedance before

the subject completed the final 20 trials of the session.

3.2.6 EEG analysis

EEG data were cleaned and analyzed in the EEGLAB (Delorme & Makeig, 2004) and FieldTrip

(Oostenveld, Fries, Maris, & Schoffelen, 2011) toolboxes for Matlab (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick,

MA). Data were re-referenced to the average voltage, bandpass filtered to between 0.25 and 75 Hz,

and divided into epochs for each segment and each presentation. Every such epoch extended from

200 ms before that segment’s disk motion onset to 600 ms after. Data were visually inspected for

muscle artifacts, eye movements, and bad channels; epochs containing such artifacts were rejected.

Independent components analysis was used to isolate eye blink activity, which was subtracted from

the data. Finally, data were again visually inspected for artifacts not corrected by the previous two

processes. After cleaning, data were averaged across trials and sessions to create a subject average

ERP for each combination of condition (Congruent or Flip), segment (one, two, three, four, or

five), and presentation (one, two, three, or four,).

For each of the following investigations, I used a data-driven, non-parametric clustering approach

(Maris & Oostenveld, 2007) to select time windows and electrodes for analysis. The FieldTrip

toolbox includes software implementing this approach. It first calculates Student’s t for each

electrode and time point, and identifies clusters of time- and/or space-adjacent electrodes with

|t| > tcritical. Critical t-values were selected by the experimenters according to several criteria,

51

Page 59: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

including the degrees of freedom of the comparison, the magnitude of the difference between the

conditions, and the degree of spatial and temporal specificity desired.

For each cluster, the t-scores of the member electrodes and time-points were summed, giving a

cluster score that reflected both the extent of the cluster (in space and time) and the magnitude of

the difference between the conditions. A reference distribution of test statistics was generated by

randomly permuting the data across the two conditions being compared, computing such scores

for each resulting cluster, and taking the largest such cluster score on each of 1,000 permutations.

Where cluster-wise p-values are reported, they are derived by comparing the empirically-obtained

cluster score to such a reference distribution.

To confirm that experimenter selection of tcritical is appropriate, I compared the clusters generated

by the algorithm at three different values of tcritical. Results are shown in Figure 3.2. In each panel,

blue circles denote the electrodes that are included in a statistically-significant (cluster-wise p < .05)

positive cluster; red circles denote electrodes included in a statistically significant negative cluster.

Below each electrode map is a timeline with a gray box showing the time window over which the

cluster is significant. With the least-stringent value, tcritical = 2.093, the algorithm identifies a

positive central cluster composed of 42 electrodes over a time window from 384–526 ms after disk

motion onset (Figure 3.2, upper left panel). At a moderate value, tcritical = 2.861, the algorithm

identifies a positive central cluster composed of 18 electrodes over a time window from 396–448

ms after disk motion onset (Figure 3.2, upper right panel). Finally, at tcritical = 3.883, the algorithm

identifies a positive central cluster composed of 5 electrodes over a time window of 404–412 ms

after disk motion onset (Figure 3.2, lower left panel). Adjusting the value of tcritical expands or

contracts the electrodes and time windows included in a cluster without dramatically distorting the

cluster’s position or timing.

Grand average ERPs for each comparison of interest were created by averaging across subjects

and across the electrodes identified as part of the cluster. I did three such comparisons.

52

Page 60: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

tcritical = 2.093

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

tcritical = 2.861

tcritical = 3.883

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

cluster criterion: α = .05

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

Member of a negative cluster

Member of a positive cluster

384-516 ms

cluster criterion: α = .01

396-448 msdisk motion onset

404-412 ms

cluster criterion: α = .001

disk motion onset

disk motion onset

time

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

cluster criterion: α = .05

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

Member of a negative cluster

Member of a positive cluster

384-516 ms

cluster criterion: α = .01

396-448 msdisk motion onset

404-412 ms

cluster criterion: α = .001

disk motion onset

disk motion onset

time

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

cluster criterion: α = .05

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

Member of a negative cluster

Member of a positive cluster

384-516 ms

cluster criterion: α = .01

396-448 msdisk motion onset

404-412 ms

cluster criterion: α = .001

disk motion onset

disk motion onset

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

cluster criterion: α = .05

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net128 Channel Map

Version 1.0

For questions or additional assistance please refer to the EGI Sensor Net Technical Manual or contact us at:

Electrical Geodesics, Inc.1600 Millrace Drive, Suite 307Eugene, Oregon USA 97403Phone: (541) 687-7962 Fax: (541) 687-7963Email: [email protected] or [email protected] N-PRT-LAM-4300-001

12

3

4

5

6

7

89

10

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22Fp1

23

24F3

43

44

45T3

4647

38

39

41

42

32

34

35

37

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

48

49

50 51 52P3

5354

55

56 57LM

58T5

61

COM

6364 65

66

67

68

71

72

73

74

75Oz

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

84

86

87

88

89

90

92P4

93

S/N

94

95

96T6

97

98

99

100RM

101

102

103

105106

107

108T4

110111

112

113

115

116117118

114

119120

121

122F8

124F4

125

126127

1289 99Fp2

12333F7

36C3

40

5960

69

70O1

83O2

8591

104C4

REFCz

109

11Fz

62Pz

5

Member of a negative cluster

Member of a positive cluster

384-516 ms

cluster criterion: α = .01

396-448 msdisk motion onset

404-412 ms

cluster criterion: α = .001

disk motion onset

disk motion onset

time

Figure 3.2: The electrodes identified by my clustering algorithm at three different values of tcritical.In each panel, blue circles denote the electrodes that are included in a statistically-significant(p < .05) positive cluster, while red circles denote electrodes included in a statistically-significantnegative cluster. The gray boxes on each timeline show the time window of each cluster. As clustersize decreases with higher criterion values, the position of the cluster is stable in both space andtime.

53

Page 61: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

First, to assess the changes in neural responses as a new sequence item becomes increasingly

familiar, I collapsed across segments two, three, four, and five1 of Congruent trials and computed

subject-average ERPs for each presentation. I will refer to these four presentations as pres1,

pres2, pres3, and pres4. Because the clustering approach requires pairwise comparisons, I used the

difference between pres1 and pres2 to identify the electrodes and time window for this comparison.

As seen in Chapter 2, the encoding and maintenance of repeated presentations of a given sequence

item are supported by long-term memory. Therefore, the neural response to pres1 should reflect the

increased demands of encoding an item that has not been previously seen and thus cannot draw on

such long-term memory support.

Next, to measure the differences between neural responses to deviant sequence items and those

to familiar sequence items, I calculated ERPs to segment five of presentation four on Congruent

and Flip trials. Note that this is the segment and presentation on which the “flip” occurs on Flip

trials. I will use Con p4s5 and Flip p4s5 to denote these two segments. Here, the response to

Flip p4s5 segments should reflect both the increased encoding demands also seen in pres1 and the

prediction-monitoring processes that trigger such new encoding.

Finally, because both the comparison between new and familiar sequence items and the com-

parison between deviant and familiar sequence items incorporate an increased encoding load for

one condition (new and deviant), and a reliance on expectation for the other (familiar), I directly

compared the neural responses to new and deviant sequence items to allow me to dissociate the

ERP correlates of unexpectedness from those of mere novelty. I computed ERPs to segment five of

Flip trials on both presentation one and presentation four. I will refer to these two segments as

Flip p1s5 and Flip p4s5. On presentation one, subjects cannot predict the segment’s direction of

motion, nor do they expect to be able to predict it, while on presentation four, subjects do have

1The sudden onset of disk motion at the start of the first segment evokes a neural response that is very differentfrom that to the other segments (Agam & Sekuler, 2007).

54

Page 62: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

predictions about the disk’s motion, which are violated by the deviant stimulus.

3.3 Results

3.3.1 Behavioral accuracy

Figure 3.3A shows subjects’ reproduction accuracy on Congruent trials. I ran a 5×4 repeated-

measures ANOVA with factors segment (one, two, three, four, or five) and presentation (one,

two, three, or four). There was a main effect of segment (F(4,44) = 8.955, p = .003, partial

η2 = .449), and a segment by presentation interaction (F(12,132) = 6.310, p = .048, partial η2 =

.364). There was also a main effect of presentation (F3,33) = 38.866, p < .001, partial η2 = .779).

Follow-up analyses showed that the improvement in reproduction accuracy from presentation

one to presentation two was significant (F(1,11) = 62.654, p < .001, partial η2 = .850), but the

changes from presentation two to three and from three to four were not.

I also compared mean directional error on Flip and Congruent trials on presentation four

(that is, the presentation on which these two trial types differed). I found no significant main effect

of condition (Flip vs. Congruent, F(1,11) = 2.554, p = .138, partial η2 = .189), as shown in Figure

3.3B. These results replicate a central finding of Maryott et al. (2011) and confirm that subjects

can successfully incorporate unexpected events into their planning and execution of a reproduced

motion sequence.

3.3.2 ERPs

Figure 3.4 illustrates the changes in neural activity that accompany learning, as a stimulus goes

from being new to being familiar over the course of four successive presentations. A data-driven

clustering and permutation algorithm identified the cluster of electrodes that best captured (p = .098)

55

Page 63: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

10

20

30

40

1 2 3 4 5

Erro

r in

repr

oduc

tion

(°)

Segment serial position

1st

2nd

3rd

4th

Presentation

10

20

30

40

1 2 3 4 5

Erro

r in

repr

oduc

tion

(°)

Segment serial position

Flip

Congruent

A

B

Figure 3.3: A: Mean directional error on Congruent trials across the five segments in a motionsequence. Data are displayed separately for each of the four presentations of a sequence. Subjects’accuracy improved over repeated presentations. B: Mean directional error on Flip and Congruenttrials for the fourth (final) presentation. The two conditions do not differ significantly. Error barsare repeated-measures standard error (Morey, 2008).

the change in scalp voltage topographies between pres1 and pres2. This cluster was derived using

tcritical = 2.201, the critical t-value at α = .05, df = 11. The cluster consisted of nine electrodes

that were more positive-going on pres1 than pres2 during an interval from 200–280 ms after disk

motion onset. The inset in Figure 3.4 depicts the distribution of t-scores between ERPs to the two

presentations during that time window and the locations of the electrodes comprising this cluster;

56

Page 64: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

−200 ms −196 ms

−8

−4

0

4

8

t−scores

1 μV

100 ms

8

-8t200-280 ms

disk motion onset 200-280 ms

pres1pres2

pres3pres4

Figure 3.4: ERPs, collapsed across segments two through five, to pres1, pres2, pres3, and pres4 onCongruent trials, at a cluster of central electrodes. The inset topographical plot shows the locationsof the nine electrodes making up the significant cluster, and the distribution across the scalp of tvalues at 200–280 ms after disk motion onset. The traces show ERPs at the cluster, timelocked todisk motion onset of each segment. Error ribbons are within-subject standard error (Morey, 2008),and thus do not reflect the process or results of the permutation significance test.

57

Page 65: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

−200 ms −196 ms

−8

−4

0

4

8

t−scores

1 μV

100 ms

Flip_p4s5(deviant)

Con_p4s5(familiar)

8

-8t396-448 ms

disk motion onset 396-448 ms

Figure 3.5: ERPs to Con p4s5 (familiar) and Flip p4s5 (deviant) at a cluster of central electrodes.The inset topographical plot shows the locations of the 18 electrodes making up the significantcluster, and the distribution across the scalp of t values at 396–448 ms after disk motion onset. Thetraces show ERPs at the cluster, timelocked to disk motion onset of each segment.

the traces in the main body of the figure show ERPs at this cluster, timelocked to each segment’s

disk motion onset, for pres1, pres2, pres3, and pres4.

Figure 3.5 illustrates the differences between neural activity that accompanies familiar sequence

item and that accompanying deviant items. I used the same approach as above to identify the cluster

of electrodes that best captures (p < .001) the difference between Flip p4s5 and Con p4s5 (i.e.,

deviant and familiar sequence items). This cluster was derived using tcritical = 3.106, the critical

t-value at α = .01, df = 11. The resulting cluster consisted of 18 electrodes that were more

positive-going to the deviant item than to the familiar item from 396–448 ms after disk motion

onset. The inset in Figure 3.5 depicts the distribution of t-scores between ERPs to Flip p4s5 and to

58

Page 66: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

Con p4s5 segments during that time window and the locations of the electrodes comprising this

cluster; the traces in the main body of the figure depict ERPs at the cluster, timelocked to disk

motion onset of segment five, for Flip p4s5 (deviant) and Con p4s5 (familiar).

Figure 3.6 highlights the differences between neural response to new and to deviant sequence

items. Using the clustering algorithm described above, I identified two clusters of electrodes that

captured differences between a new item, Flip p1s5, and a deviant item in the same sequential

position, Flip p4s5. These clusters were derived using tcritical = 2.201, the critical t-value at

α = .05, df = 11. Figure 3.6A depicts the first (by time) resulting cluster (p = .025), which

consisted of 16 electrodes that were more negative-going to the deviant sequence item than to the

new item from 244–344 ms after disk motion onset for the segment. The inset of Figure 3.6A shows

the distribution of t-scores between ERPs to the two segments during that time window (negative

values imply Flip p1s5 is more positive) and the locations of the electrodes comprising this cluster;

the traces in the main body of Figure 3.6A depict ERPs at that cluster, time locked to disk motion

onset. I included traces of the ERP to the equivalent familiar segment, Con p4s5, for comparison.

The second cluster (p = .002) consisted of 26 electrodes that were more positive-going to deviant

items than to new items from 376–468 ms after disk motion onset for the segment. The inset of

Figure 3.6B depicts the distribution of t-scores between the two conditions during this time window

and the locations of the electrodes comprising this cluster. The traces in the main body of Figure

3.6B show ERPs at the cluster, time locked to disk motion onset.

Note that the two clusters depicted in Figure 3.6 likely capture two different ERP sub-components.

The enhanced positive-going response to new motion segments (Flip p1s5) relative to familiar

motion segments (Con p4s5) is only evident at the centro-parietal electrodes of the cluster shown in

Figure 3.6A. Further, it onsets earlier than the response to deviant segments (Flip p4s5), although

the response to deviant segments peaks substantially higher than that to new segments. In contrast,

the enhanced positive-going response to deviant segments is evident at both clusters. Over fronto-

59

Page 67: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

−200 ms −196 ms

−8

−4

0

4

8

t−scores

−200 ms −196 ms

−8

−4

0

4

8

t−scores

1 μV

100 ms

244-344 ms

disk motion onset

376-468 ms

244-344 ms

A 8

-8t

B

disk motion onset 376-468 ms

8

-8t

1 μV

100 ms

Flip_p1s5 (new)Flip_p4s5 (deviant)

Con_p4s5 (familiar)

Figure 3.6: ERPs to Flip p1s5 (new), Flip p4s5 (deviant), and Con p4s5 (familiar) at two clusters.The inset in each panel shows the locations of the electrodes making up each cluster and itsdistribution of t-scores; the traces show ERPs at the cluster to each segment. A: A slightly right-lateralized centro-parietal cluster composed of 16 electrodes that were more negative-going onFlip p4s5 (deviant) than on Flip p1s5 (new) from 244–344 ms after disk motion onset for thesegment. B: A fronto-central cluster composed of 26 electrodes that were more positive-going onFlip p4s5 (deviant) than on Flip p1s5 (new) from 376–468 ms after disk motion onset.

60

Page 68: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

central electrodes (Figure 3.6B), its amplitude is substantially larger than that of the ERP to new or

familiar items, although it onsets at the same time.

3.4 Discussion

I recorded high-density scalp EEG while subjects performed a visuomotor sequence-learning task,

with occasional deviant elements inserted into familiar sequences. Subjects successfully reproduced

the sequences, including the deviant items. Because the sequence changed every 60-90 seconds,

subjects had to dynamically update their representation of the relevant sequential structure. In

order to characterize the neural mechanisms that respond to unpredictability within newly-learned

sequences, I measured ERPs to novel, familiar, and deviant sequence items. As compared to familiar,

well-predicted sequence items, both new and deviant items evoked an enhanced centralized P3, as

identified by a data-driven, bottom-up analysis approach. For new elements, this P3 was consistently

timed and over fronto-central electrodes, while the deviant elements evoked a P3 over fronto-central

electrodes as well as a slightly later positivity over centro-parietal electrodes.

These results fill an important gap in the previous work on the neural response to sequence

deviants, most of which has been done using a serial reaction time task (SRTT). In the SRTT,

subjects make key presses to a stream of letters or other stimuli (Nissen & Bullemer, 1987). When

a repeating sequence of letters is embedded within the stream, subjects respond more quickly

to those items, and some subjects develop explicit knowledge of the sequence’s presence and

structure. Subjects who have such explicit knowledge show an enhanced P3 to letters that violate

the established sequence, but subjects without such knowledge do not (Ferdinand et al., 2008;

Schlaghecken et al., 2000). Similarly, subjects who are instructed that an underlying sequence exists

show an enhanced P3 (Russeler et al., 2003).

Russeler and Rosler (2000) created an SRTT variant in which multiple stimuli mapped onto each

61

Page 69: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

response key, so that they could separate the neural response to perceptual deviants (wrong letter

but same response) from that to perceptual + motor deviants (wrong letter AND different keypress).

They found an enhanced P3 only to the latter category of sequence deviants, and concluded that

the P3 effect reflects the need to change or update a response rather than the mere detection of

an unexpected stimulus. In my task, participants are explicitly aware of the sequential structure,

as their task is to memorize and reproduce it. Thus, deviant sequence items in my study require

participants to change a planned motor output as well as to encode information about the new

event. My findings of P3 enhancement to deviant sequence items is consistent with Russeler and

Rosler’s hypothesis. Interestingly, de Bruijn, Schubotz, and Ullsperger (2007) showed participants

slideshows of everyday actions, with or without execution errors, and found an enhanced P300 for

observed errors relative to correct executions. In that task, subjects are presumably drawing on their

previous knowledge about the sequential structure of the familiar, everyday actions; observed errors

deviate from this structure but do not require a response from the participant.

Two distinct subcomponents with very similar timing have ben identified in the P300 literature

(Polich, 2007; Linden, 2005; Goldstein et al., 2002; Spencer, Dien, & Donchin, 2001; N. K. Squires

et al., 1975). The slightly earlier subcomponent, the Novelty P3, is centered over fronto-central

electrodes, and is described as elicited by deviant stimuli that are salient, low-probability, and

do not require a response (Goldstein et al., 2002). The second subcomponent, the P300, is more

posterior, centered over centro-parietal electrodes. The P300 is elicited by task-relevant stimuli such

as targets. Deviant sequence elements in the SRTT enhance the P300 (also sometimes called P3b)

subcomponent (Ferdinand et al., 2008; Russeler et al., 2003; Schlaghecken et al., 2000).

My results support the presence of separable P3 subcomponents using a clustering approach

to separate ERPs to novel from deviant items. One, a centro-parietal ERP peaking around 400 ms

after disk motion onset, is enhanced by new and deviant items in comparison to familiar ones. This

response appears to be parallel to the P300, as both new and deviant sequence items are task-relevant.

62

Page 70: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

This activity most likely reflects the perception and encoding of a task-relevant stimulus (which

subjects must do for both new and deviant items). The second subcomponent is a fronto-central

ERP, also peaking about 400 ms after disk motion onset. At these electrodes, the ERP to novel and

familiar sequence items are indistinguishable, but the P3-like peak to deviant sequence items is

substantially enhanced. This response likely parallels the P3a or Novelty P3 subcomponent and

suggests that “novel” may be better characterized as “expectation-violating” (Vossel, Weidner, Thiel,

& Fink, 2009; Yu & Dayan, 2005). This ERP may specifically reflect inhibition or rejection of a

perceptual representation and motor plan that are actively stored in working memory.

Differences between the neural responses to new and deviant stimuli highlight the importance

of prediction-monitoring in cognition. On the surface, new and deviant items are quite similar:

both require the subject to perceive and encode a direction of motion that has not previously been

observed, in order to reproduce it from memory. The difference is in the context: When viewing a

deviant sequence item, participants have strong predictions about the disk’s direction of motion;

when viewing a new item, they have no such predictions. The differences in neural response between

these two conditions therefore reflects the effects of these predictions on perceiving and encoding

the element.

Although my results contribute to the understanding of the relationship between sequential

structure, unpredictability, and the P3, the present study has two important limitations. First, because

participants were explicitly aware of both the sequential structure of the task and the points at

which new trials (and thus new sequences) began, these findings cannot be extended to explain the

mechanisms by which people come to identify sequential structure in continuous streams of sensory

input, nor to explain the processes by which people identify changes in that structure. Second,

because the deviant items in this paradigm always differed from the familiar items by 180, I cannot

say anything about the effect of the magnitude of prediction violations. Both of these questions

will need to be investigated before the relationship between expected unpredictability, unexpected

63

Page 71: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 3. P300 TO VIOLATIONS OF NEWLY LEARNED PREDICTIONS

unpredictability, and the subcomponents of the P3 can be fully described.

In summary, I have shown that, when learning motion sequences, people show distinct neural

responses to new stimuli, familiar stimuli, and stimuli that deviate from the governing sequence.

The neural responses to a deviant sequence item are different than those to a new sequence item,

further supporting the hypothesis that identifying prediction errors is a cognitive process. Finally,

my results extend previous work on monitoring sequential regularities and show that the neural

mechanisms involved are similar when the sequential structure is dynamic and when it is stable over

time.

64

Page 72: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

Chapter 4

Leaky ignoring in the flanker task

4.1 Introduction

The human brain is sensitive to events that violate its explicit and implicit predictions about

forthcoming sensory inputs (Ferdinand et al., 2008; Nassar et al., 2012; Ouden et al., 2009; Schultz

et al., 1997). As I showed in Chapter 3, humans detect such prediction-violating events and

incorporate them into working memory representations in a task that requires explicit reproduction

of visuomotor sequences. In this chapter, I directly investigate the effect of such events on attention.

In associative learning or other explicitly predictive tasks, prediction-violating events trigger the

allocation of attention (Wills et al., 2007). Further, as is familiar from everyday experience, when

an unattended stimulus stream contains an event that is unexpected, that event becomes distracting,

impairing performance on the attended task (e.g. Escera et al., 1998; Parmentier et al., 2008;

Schroger, 1996).

The Eriksen flanker task is frequently used to investigate the cognitive processes involved in

response conflict and spatial attention (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974; Ridderinkhof et al., 1999). In

this paradigm, a central target is surrounded by flanking distractors which may be congruent with

65

Page 73: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

(i.e., match) the target or be incongruent with it. The classic finding from this task is that, despite

subjects’ attempts to ignore the flanking distractors, flankers that are incongruent with the central

target interfere with processing, leading to reduced speed and accuracy on those trials.

I modified the flanker task to incorporate unexpected events among the unattended flankers.

I hypothesized that the behavioral effect of the flankers should be magnified on trials with those

unexpected flanker events, due to unexpected events’ obligatory attraction of attentional resources.

To characterize the effects of unexpected flankers, I compared ERPs to the onset of stimuli

containing expected and unexpected flankers. The visual mismatch negativity (vMMN) is an early,

negative-going deflection in the ERP that occurs in response to occasional deviant elements within

a sequence of visual stimuli that obey some regularity (Czigler, 2007; Pazo-Alvarez, Cadaveira,

& Amenedo, 2003). It is parallel to the well-established auditory MMN, which is theorized to

be generated in auditory cortex when a predictive signal from prefrontal areas is disconfirmed by

incoming sensory information (Garrido et al., 2009; Wacongne et al., 2011, 2012). The auditory

and visual MMNs do not depend on attention, and arise even when subjects are attending to other

stimulus streams, such as a listening task in one ear with a sequence of tones in the other, or a

visuomotor task w/ irrelevant stimuli in the periphery (Naatanen, Paavilainen, Titinen, Jiang, &

Alho, 1993; Stefanics et al., 2011; Winkler, Teder-Salejarvi, Horvath, Naatanen, & Sussman, 2003).

There is some evidence that the MMN reflects characteristics of stimulus processing (Kimura,

Kondo, et al., 2011; Naatanen, Paavilainen, Rinne, & Alho, 2007; Tales, Troscianko, Wilcock,

Newton, & Butler, 2002). I measured the vMMN elicited by unexpected flankers in the Eriksen

flanker task, and tested its relationship to behavioral evidence of oddball flanker interference.

I also measured individual differences in task performance and neural activity. The processes

involved in this task are likely influenced by individual differences in emotional, motor, and

attentional reactivity (Rothbart, 2007). I hypothesized that individual differences in temperament—

particularly the factors relating to perceptual sensitivity and attentional control—would predict

66

Page 74: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

individual susceptibility to unexpected flankers.

Finally, I investigated the mechanisms linking the neural response to a prediction error with

changes in behavior. Error-induced behavior changes are theorized to rely on on the low-level

motivational systems that drive organisms to avoid aversive experiences (Hajcak & Foti, 2008;

Holroyd & Coles, 2002; Ridderinkhof, Ramautar, & Wijnen, 2009), and responses to prediction

errors may be similarly organized. A recent study found that the human startle response is slightly

potentiated after an incorrect response (Hajcak & Foti, 2008). I thus attempted to replicate their

finding, and investigated whether similar startle potentiation accompanies unexpected flanker events.

4.2 Methods

4.2.1 Subjects

Twenty members of the Brandeis University community (15 female, age range 18-21) participated

in this study. All were right-handed (mean score on the revised Edinburgh Handedness Inventory

89.49, SD = 12.85). Two other subjects completed one experimental session but did not return for

the second; their data are not reported here.

4.2.2 Experimental task

I designed a modified Eriksen flankers task using chevron stimuli (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974;

Ridderinkhof et al., 1999). The basic trial structure is shown schematically in Figure 4.1. On each

trial, subjects were presented with an array of five chevrons that were displayed for 50 ms and were

not masked upon offset. They were instructed to report whether the central chevron was pointing

to the left or to the right. I will refer to this central chevron as the target, and the two chevrons on

each side of it as the flankers. The four flankers were always consistently oriented, and the central

67

Page 75: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

> > < > >+

50 ms

Stimulus Response

< 2000 ms

A

A

> > < > >+

50 ms

Stimulus Response

“L” or “R”keypress

< 2000 ms

350 ms

Startle Probe

> > < > >+

50 ms

Stimulus Response

< 2000 ms

B

105 dB50 ms

Startle Probe

300 ms

“L” or “R”keypress

“L” or “R”keypress

105 dB50 ms

Figure 4.1: The sequence of events within a trial with no startle probe. After fixation, the flankerstimulus appeared for 50 ms, after which participants had two seconds to report the direction of thecenter chevron.

chevron’s orientation was equiprobably congruent or incongruent with its flankers. After a subject’s

response, a fixation cross was displayed for an inter-trial interval of 1000 ms before the next trial

display appeared.

Subjects viewed the display from a difference of approximately 57 cm. Each chevron subtended

approximately 1.40 visual angle, and the full array extended to an eccentricity of 4.67.

In order to maintain more-consistent error levels across subjects and conditions, subjects received

feedback about their performance after every thirty trials (after Hajcak & Foti, 2008). If the subject

had responded correctly on between 75% and 90% of those trials, the feedback was “You’re doing

great!” If their accuracy was lower than 75%, the feedback instructed them to increase their

accuracy; if it was above 90%, the feedback instructed them to respond more quickly.

Trial types were distributed among four conditions in a two-by-two design, as shown in Figure

4.2. The first factor governed the direction of the four flanker chevrons. On ninety percent of trials,

the flanker chevrons pointed in one direction (the Standard direction) and on ten percent of trials

they pointed the other (the Oddball direction). The second factor governed the relationship between

68

Page 76: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

> > > > >< < < < <

< < > < < > > < > >

90% left flankers

Standard Oddball

Congruent

Incongruent

45%

45%

5%

5%

< < < < <

< < > < <> > < > >

> > > > >

90% right flankers

Standard Oddball

Congruent

Incongruent

45%

45%

5%

5%

Figure 4.2: Diagram of the two-by-two trial design. One factor, flanker direction, governedthe direction of the four flanker chevrons; the second, congruency, governed the relationshipbetween the central target and its flankers. Whether the Standard flanker direction was left orright was counterbalanced within subjects. 90% of trials incorporated the Standard flankers(45% Congruent and 45% Incongruent); 10% of trials incorporated the Oddball flankers (5%Congruent and 5% Incongruent).

the central target and the flankers. On half of trials, the target was Congruent with the flankers, and

on half it was Incongruent. Note that left-facing and right-facing target were equally frequent, and

the directions comprising Standard and Oddball flankers were counterbalanced within subjects.

4.2.3 Startle probes

In order to elicit startle responses, occasional trials included a 105 dB burst of white noise lasting

50 ms (Blumenthal et al., 2005; Hajcak & Foti, 2008). These startle probes were presented through

a single speaker resting on the display monitor. As one aim of this study was to compare response-

induced to stimulus-induced startle priming, participants completed four blocks of trials. Two of

these blocks included stimulus-aligned startle probes; the other two included response-aligned

69

Page 77: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

> > < > >+

50 ms

Stimulus Response

< 2000 ms

A

A

> > < > >+

50 ms

Stimulus Response

“L” or “R”keypress

< 2000 ms

350 ms

Startle Probe

> > < > >+

50 ms

Stimulus Response

< 2000 ms

B

105 dB50 ms

Startle Probe

300 ms

“L” or “R”keypress

“L” or “R”keypress

105 dB50 ms

Figure 4.3: A: The sequence of events within one trial with a stimulus-aligned startle probe. The50 ms noise burst played 350 ms after stimulus onset. See page 71 for a description of the rulesgoverning whether startle probes occurred. B: The sequence of events within one trial with aresponse-aligned startle probe. Subjects had to respond within two seconds of stimulus onset; thenoise burst played 300 ms after their response.

70

Page 78: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

startle probes. Block order was counter-balanced across participants using all possible orders.

In the stimulus-aligned condition, startle probes occurred 350 ms after stimulus onset and were

contingent on the flanker direction of the stimulus displayed on that trial (Figure 4.3A). In these

blocks, startle probes occurred on 50% of Oddball trials, 50% of Standard trials immediately

following an Oddball trial, and 4% of all other Standard trials. No explicit constraint on was placed

on startle probes on successive trials. The two separate rules for Standard trials control for the

predictability of the Oddball-contingent startle probes. On average, blocks with stimulus-aligned

startle probes featured 64 probes per 510-trial block.

In the response-aligned condition, startle probes occurred 300 ms after the subject’s response,

and were contingent on whether the subject responded correctly (Figure 4.3B). In these blocks,

startle probes occurred on 50% of error trials, on 50% of correct trials following an error trial, and

on 4% of all other correct trials. On average, blocks with response-aligned startle probes featured

73 probes per 510-trial block.

To assess whether unexpected stimuli and unexpected responses similarly recruit defensive

alerting mechanisms, I measured the eye blink response to startle probes following Oddball

and Standard stimuli, and the response to probes following Error and Correct responses. The

magnitude of subjects’ eye-blink responses was measured via electromyography (EMG), using a

subset of the electrodes on the EEG cap. Page 74 describes details of the recording and analysis.

4.2.4 Procedures and analyses

Each recording block comprised 510 trials, with the first thirty discarded as practice. Each subject

completed two blocks with stimulus-aligned startle probes (one with left-facing flankers as the

Standard direction, the other with right-facing flankers) and two blocks with response-aligned

startle probes. These four blocks were completed in two separate recording sessions; the order of

71

Page 79: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

blocks was counterbalanced across subjects using all possible orders. By the end of the experiment,

each subject had completed 2040 trials, 1920 of which were included for analysis.

Subjects filled out an anonymous questionnaire after each recording session, confirming that

they got reasonable amounts of sleep, were not under the influence of any psychoactive substances,

and had no medical history, such as a head injury or neurological diagnosis, that should lead me to

exclude their data.

Behavioral measures

Subjects’ reaction times and responses were recorded from each trial and analyzed. I computed

accuracy for each of Congruent Standard, Incongruent Standard, Congruent Oddball, and

Incongruent Oddball conditions. I also computed decile reaction times and accuracy for all

four conditions, computed ex-Gaussian fits to reaction time distributions (Balota, Yap, Cortese, &

Watson, 2008), and measured post-error slowing—the difference in reaction time between the trial

following an error and the trial preceding an error (Dutilh et al., 2012).

After the end of their final experimental session, subjects completed the Adult Temperament

Questionnaire Short Form (ATQ). This instrument has 77 items which form several self-report

scales describing temperament factors (Evans & Rothbart, 2007). I selected two factors a priori for

testing: Attentional Control and Orienting Sensitivity. Attentional Control refers to the capacity to

focus attention, and to shift attention as desired. “It’s often hard for me to alternate between two

different tasks,” is an example of a reverse-scored Attentional Control item. Orienting Sensitivity

refers to awareness of low-intensity environmental and self-generated stimuli and experiences. “I

often notice visual details in the environment,” is an example of an Orienting Sensitivity item.

I hypothesized that the Attentional Control would account for some variability in people’s task

performance, while Orienting Sensitivity would account for variability in startle responses.

72

Page 80: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

EEG recording and measures

A high-density EEG system (Electrical Geodesics, Inc., Eugene, OR) with 129 electrodes sampled

scalp electroencephalographic signals at 250 Hz using a high-impedance amplifier. Signals were

recorded for later, off-line analysis. All channels were adjusted for scalp impedance below 50 kΩ

impedance; after one experimental block, channels were returned to at most 50 kΩ scalp impedance

before the subject completed the session.

After recording, EEG data were preprocessed using the EEGLAB Matlab toolbox (Delorme &

Makeig, 2004). Continuous EEG signals were bandpass filtered to between 0.25 and 100 Hz using

a first-order Butterworth filter. A 60 Hz notch filter was also applied to the continuous data, to

reduce line electrical noise. Then, data were broken into epochs time-locked to stimulus onset and

subject responses. Stimulus-locked epochs comprised a window from 200 ms preceding stimulus

onset to 500 ms ms after; response-locked epochs comprised 200 ms preceding keypress responses

to 500 ms after. Finally, data were cleaned using the process described in Section 3.2.6 (page 51).

Using the clustering analysis described in Section 3.2.6 (page 52), I ran two comparisons. First,

I identified time windows and electrodes that captured the differences between Standard and

Oddball stimuli, and computed the ERPs to stimulus onset at those electrodes. The mean difference

between the two ERPs during the significant time window was my measure of vMMN magnitude.

To verify that the clustering algorithm identifies valid ERP components, I also compared the

ERPs elicited by Correct and Incorrect responses. The error response negativity (ERN) is a

negative-going deflection in the ERP that occurs very shortly after incorrect responses. I identified

time windows and electrodes that captured the differences between Correct and Error responses,

and computed ERPs to subject responses at those electrodes. The mean difference between the two

ERPs during the significant time window was my measure of ERN magnitude.

73

Page 81: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

EMG recording and measures

EMG data were recorded simultaneously with the EEG data, using electrodes 14, 21, 126, and

127 of the sensor net. These electrodes are positioned directly above and below the eyes, and

are designed to capture the vertical/blink component of eye-movement EMG. Using a 4th-order

Butterworth filter, the data were bandpass filtered to 28–100 Hz and broken into epochs from 200

ms preceding each startle probe to 350 ms after its onset. Epoched data were rectified, low-pass

filtered at 50 Hz and visually inspected for non-blink artifacts (Blumenthal et al., 2005; Hajcak

& Foti, 2008). Artifact-contaminated epochs were removed from the processing stream. Cleaned

data were baseline-corrected using the 200 ms immediately preceding each startle probe, and blink

magnitude was operationalized as the maximum voltage within a time window from 20–150 ms

after a startle probe.

4.3 Results

4.3.1 Behavioral measures

Figure 4.4 shows the effects of congruency and expectedness on subjects’ speed and accuracy

in the flankers task. On trials in which the flankers were in the Standard condition (presented

in blue in Figure 4.4), subjects were faster and more accurate on Congruent trials (proportion

correct 0.93, SD = 0.04; median correct reaction time 356 ms, SD = 33) than on Incongruent

trials (proportion correct 0.86, SD = 0.05; median correct reaction time 378 ms, SD = 37), with the

largest accuracy differences occurring on trials where subjects responded quickly. This effect was

exaggerated when the flankers were in the Oddball condition (presented in green in Figure 4.4).

On Oddball Congruent trials, subjects were faster and more accurate (proportion correct 0.95, SD

= 0.04; median correct reaction time 343 ms, SD = 31) than on Standard Congruent trials, and on

74

Page 82: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

0.25

0.50

0.75

1.00

0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7

Prop

ortio

n co

rrect

Reaction time (s)

Oddball Congruent

Standard Congruent

Standard Incongruent

Oddball Incongruent

0.50

0.75

1.00

Congruent Incongruent

Prop

ortio

n co

rrect

StandardsOddballs

0.30

0.35

0.40

0.45

Congruent Incongruent

Med

ian

reac

tion

time

(s)

Figure 4.4: (A) Reaction time and proportion correct for each decile of Standard (blue) and Oddball(green) trials, when the flankers are Congruent (solid lines) and Incongruent (dashed lines). (B)Proportion correct (left) and median reaction times (right) for each condition, summarizing theabove. Error bars are repeated-measures standard error.

75

Page 83: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

Table 4.1: ANOVA results for ex-Gaussian fits to RT distributions.

parameter source df1, df2 F partial η2

µ

Flanker 1,19 10.976∗∗ .366

Congruency 1,19 0.015 .015

Flanker × Congruency 1,19 2.202 .104

σ

Flanker 1,19 0.848 .043

Congruency 1,19 25.323∗∗∗ .571

Flanker × Congruency 1,19 3.343 .150

τ

Flanker 1, 19 11.425∗∗ .376

Congruency 1,19 0.094 .005

Flanker × Congruency 1,19 0.004 .000

∗p < .05. ∗∗p < .01. ∗∗∗p < .001.

Oddball Incongruent trials, subjects were slowest and least accurate (proportion correct 0.59, SD

= 0.16; median correct reaction time 427 ms, SD = 47).

These results were confirmed by two-way repeated measures ANOVAs with factors congruency

and flanker direction. There was a main effect of congruency on proportion correct (F(1,19) =

94.211, p < .001), a main effect of flanker direction on proportion correct (F(1,19) = 80.019,

p < .001), and a congruency × flanker direction interaction (F(1,19) = 101.809, p < .001).

There was a significant main effect of congruency on median correct reaction time (F(1,19) =

92.244, p < .001), no main effect of flanker direction on median correct reaction time (F(1,19) =

0.026, p = .875), but a significant congruency × flanker direction interaction (F(1,19) = 30.917,

p < .001).

To further characterize the reaction times, I fit ex-Gaussian curves to the reaction time distribu-

tion generated by each subject on each condition. These curves have three parameters: τ , which

characterizes the positive tail of the distribution (the exponential component); µ, the mean of the

76

Page 84: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

0

0.2

0.4

Congruent Incongruent

τ (s)

0

0.5

1.0

Congruent Incongruent

µ (s

)

0

0.1

Congruent Incongruent

σ (s

)

StandardsOddballs

Figure 4.5: Ex-Gaussian fits to reaction time distributions for each stimulus condition. A: Oddballflanker directions increased µ relative to Standard. B: Incongruent trials had higher σ thanCongruent trials. C: Standard flanker directions increased τ relative to Oddball trials.

77

Page 85: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

Gaussian component, and σ, the standard deviation of the Gaussian component. Figure 4.5 shows

the results. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs on each parameter confirmed the effects. There

was a main effect of flanker on µ (p = .037), with unexpected flanker directions shifting the entire

reaction time distribution slower relative to expected flanker directions, and on τ , with unexpected

flanker directions actually decreasing the skewness of the RT distribution. (While this effect seems

paradoxical at first, it is due to the two-second timeout for subjects’ responses. With such a limit, as

the distribution shifts right, it must become less skewed.) There was a main effect of congruency on

σ, suggesting that incongruent stimuli increased the variability of subjects’ reaction times relative

to congruent stimuli. Complete ANOVA results are shown in Table 4.1.

There was substantial post-error slowing in this experiment. On average, subjects responded

30 ms more slowly (SD = 24 ms) on the trial following an error than on the trial preceding one. I

ran a three-way repeated-measures ANOVA on this post-error slowing with factors congruency,

flanker direction, and startle probe timing. There was a marginal main effect of startle probe

timing, with subjects exhibiting less post-error slowing on blocks with with stimulus-contingent

startle (26 ms, SD = 18 ms) than on blocks with response-contingent-startle (42 ms, SD = 38 ms;

F(1,19) = 4.031, p = .059). There was no main effect of congruency or flanker direction (p > .5)

and no significant interactions (all ps > .08).

On average, subjects scored near the center of the temperament self-report range from the ATQ.

The mean Perceptual Sensitivity score was 5.00 (SD = 0.86), and the mean Attentional Control

score was 4.03 (SD = 0.91). There was a non-significant negative correlation between Attentional

Control and proportion correct (r = −0.291, p = .213),

78

Page 86: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK−200 ms 0 ms

−8

−4

0

4

8

t−scores

-2

0

2

-200 -100 0 100 200 300 400 500

ampl

itude

(μV)

Time (ms)

StandardOddball

-0.5

0

0.5

-200 -100 0 100 200 300 400 500

Diffe

renc

e (µ

V)

Time (ms)

t

-8

8

0

A

B

C

Figure 4.6: ERPs to Standard and Oddball flanker directions at a cluster of posterior electrodes.A: Topographical plot showing the locations of the 16 electrodes making up the cluster, and thedistribution across the scalp of t-values at 180–320 ms after stimulus onset. B: Traces show ERPstime locked to stimulus onset for Standard (shown in blue) and Oddball (green) flankers, at thecluster of electrodes shown in panel A. C: Trace depicts the difference between the two traces shownin panel B. Negative values occurred when ERPs to Oddball flankers were more negative-goingthan those to Standard flankers. Error ribbons are within-subject standard error, and thus do notreflect the process or the results of the permutation significance test.

79

Page 87: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

4.3.2 ERPs

Standard vs. Oddball

Figure 4.6 illustrates the neural activity that accompanied the presentation of Standard and Oddball

flanker directions. A data-driven clustering and permutation analysis (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007;

see page 52) identified the cluster of electrodes that best captured (p < .001) the difference in

scalp voltage topographies between Standard and Oddball flankers. This cluster was derived with

tcritical = 3.883, the critical t-value at α = .001 and df = 19. The cluster was composed of 16

posterior electrodes that were more negative-going on Oddball than on Standard trials during an

interval from 180–320 ms after stimulus onset. Figure 4.6A depicts the locations of the electrodes

making up the cluster and the distribution of t-scores between ERPs to the two flanker directions

during that time window. The traces in Figure 4.6B show ERPs at the cluster, time locked to

stimulus onset, to Standard (blue) and Oddball (green) flankers. Figure 4.6C shows the difference

between the two traces in Figure 4.6B. Negative values reflect Oddball-evoked ERPs that were

more negative-going than Standard-evoked ERPs.

I tested whether vMMN magnitude reflected subjects’ susceptibility to interference from un-

expected flanker directions. To account for differences in neural function and anatomy that are

unrelated to such susceptibility, I partialled out each subjects’ N2 magnitude to Standard flankers.

N2 magnitude was operationalized as each subject’s mean amplitude of the ERP from 180–220 ms

after stimulus onset on Standard trials. The residual vMMN values were marginally correlated

with Attentional Control score (r = −0.439, p = .053), suggesting that subjects who scored

higher on Attentional Control tended to have larger (negative) MMNs. They were also correlated

with Orienting Sensitivity score (r = 0.464, p = .039), such that subjects who scored higher on

Orienting Sensitivity tended to have smaller vMMNs. Figure 4.7 illustrates the relationship between

these three measures. Combining Attentional Control and Orienting Sensitivity in a multiple linear

80

Page 88: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

-0.3

-0.2

-0.1

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

3 4 5 6 7

N2-c

orre

cted

vM

MN

Orienting Sensitivity Score

Bottom quartile

Top quartile

Attentional Control Score

r2 = .35p = .026

Figure 4.7: A multiple linear regression on Attentional Control and Orienting Sensitivity scorespredicts inter-individual differences in N2-corrected vMMN magnitude. Data points are plottedaccording to Orienting Sensitivity score and vMMN magnitude; note that because vMMN isnegative, lower values here correspond to larger vMMNs. The two fit lines show the relationshipbetween Orienting Sensitivity and vMMN magnitude at different ranges on the Attentional Controlfactor.

regression significantly predicted N2-corrected vMMN magnitude (R2 = 0.35, p = .026).

Residual MMN values were also correlated with proportion correct on Oddball Congruent

trials (r = −0.448, p = .048), such that those subjects who were most accurate on these trials had

larger (negative) MMNs. However, residual MMN magnitude did not correlate significantly with

proportion correct on Oddball Incongruent trials (r = 0.194, p = .412), nor did it correlate with

any reaction time measures (all ps > .08).

Correct vs. Error

Figure 4.8 depicts the neural activity that occurs following Correct and Error responses. The same

approach as above was used to identify the cluster of electrodes that best captured (p < .001) the

difference in scalp voltage topographies following Correct and Error responses. The cluster was

81

Page 89: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK−200 ms 0 ms

−8

−4

0

4

8

t−scores

-4

0

4

-200 -100 0 100 200 300 400 500

Diffe

renc

e (µ

V)

Time (ms)

-2

0

2

-200 -100 0 100 200 300 400 500

ampl

itude

(µV)

Time (ms)

t

-8

8

0

A

B

C

CorrectError

Figure 4.8: ERPs to Correct and Error responses at a cluster of central electrodes. A: Topographicalplot showing the locations of the 33 electrodes making up the cluster, and the distribution acrossthe scalp of t-values from 100 ms before subject responses to 80 ms after. B: Traces show ERPstime locked to responses for Correct (shown in yellow) and Error (red) responses, at the cluster ofelectrodes shown in panel A. C: Trace depicts the difference between the two traces shown in panelB. Negative values occurred when ERPs to Error responses were more negative-going than thoseto Correct responses.

82

Page 90: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

0

5

10

Star

tle b

link

mag

nitud

e (µ

V)

CorrectUnpredictable

CorrectPredictable

ErrorStandardUnpredictable

StandardPredictable

Oddball0

5

10

Star

tle b

link

mag

nitud

e (µ

V)

Integral over 40 ms around peak

Figure 4.9: Magnitude of the blink EMG response to startle probes following Standard (blue)and Oddball (green) flankers, and to startle probes following Correct (yellow) and Error (red)responses.

derived using tcritical = 5.949, the critical t-value at α = .00001 and df = 19. This cluster was

composed of 33 central electrodes that were more negative-going on Error than on Correct trials,

during a window from 100 ms before participants’ responses to 80 ms after. Figure 4.8A illustrates

the distribution of t-scores between ERPs to Correct and Error responses during that time window,

and the locations of the electrodes making up this cluster. The traces in Figure 4.8B show ERPs at

the cluster, time locked to subjects’ responses, for Correct (yellow) and Error (red) trials. Figure

4.8C shows the difference between the two traces in Figure 4.8B. Negative values reflect Error

ERPs that are more negative-going than Correct ERPs.

4.3.3 Startle blink

On average, subjects’ mean startle blink magnitude was 7.676 µV (SD = 6.556). Figure 4.9 shows

blink magnitudes for each startle probe condition. Because there was no significant difference

between the two Standard conditions, nor between the two Correct conditions (p > .06), I

collapsed across Standard Unpredictable and Predictable, and across Correct Unpredictable

83

Page 91: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

and Predictable. Startle blinks were of almost identical magnitude following Oddball flankers

(7.19 µV, SD = 6.84) than following Standard flankers (7.19 µV, SD = 6.80). This difference was

not significant (paired t(19) = 0.010, p = .993). Startle blinks following Error responses (7.95 µV,

SD = 7.47) were very slightly smaller than those following Correct responses ( 8.26 µV, SD =

6.32). This difference was not significant (paired t(19) = 0.496, p = .626).

As I hypothesized, average startle blink magnitude was significantly positively correlated with

subjects’ Orienting Sensitivity score on the ATQ (r = 0.539, p = .010). However, startle blink

magnitude and startle priming were not correlated with any of the ERP measures (all ps > .1).

Interestingly, subjects’ Orienting Sensitivity scores were also significantly correlated with the effects

of Oddball flankers on exGaussian fits to reaction times. There was a significant positive correlation

between Orienting Sensitivity score and the flanker direction × congruency interaction on µ (the

mean of the Gaussian component; r = 0.482, p = .031) and a significant negative correlation

between Orienting Sensitivity score and the interaction on τ (the exponent component; r = −0.491,

p = .028). This suggests that Orienting Sensitivity captures subjects susceptibility to the Oddball

flankers, as well as their overall sensitivity to the startle probes.

4.4 Discussion

I modified the Eriksen flanker task by manipulating flanker frequency to create Standard and

Oddball flanker directions. As expected, I replicated the usual flanker congruency effect: subjects

were faster and more accurate when flankers were congruent with the target than when the flankers

were incongruent. This effect was markedly larger for Oddball flankers than for Standard flankers.

On trials with Oddball flankers that were congruent with the target, subjects were fastest and most

accurate, while on trials with Oddball flankers that were incongruent with the target, subjects were

worst.

84

Page 92: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

Several groups of researchers have proposed competing models of the interference between

flankers and targets. Yu, Dayan, and Cohen (2009) proposed a Bayesian account where subjects’

priors incorporated a bias for congruency among spatially proximate regions. White et al. (2011,

2012) proposed a diffusion model in which the spotlight of spatial attention changes size over time.

The sudden onset of the stimulus requires attention to be spread across the entire display; over

time, it contracts to encompass only the target. Neither of these models is capable of describing

the differences in my data between Standard and Oddball flankers without adding a parameter

describing subjects’ expectations about flanker directions. Such expectations facilitate suppression

of incongruent flankers when they are as predicted, but not when they are deviant. The behavioral

evidence from this experiment thus supports the hypothesis that unexpected events obligatorily

capture attention (Parmentier, Elsley, Andres, & Barcelo, 2011; Schroger, 1996), enhancing the

flanker congruency effect.

This same hypothesis was further supported by the visual mismatch negativity (vMMN) elicited

by the Oddball flankers. This ERP component is thought to reflect a potentiation of the neural

response to stimuli that do not match a predictive feedback signal sent from higher cortical areas to

the sensory cortices (Garrido et al., 2009; Wacongne et al., 2012, 2011). The presence of a vMMN

to the Oddball flankers confirm that subjects’ brains are sensitive to the regularity governing flanker

direction, despite the fact that most subjects did not express awareness of that regularity.

I assessed whether subjects’ self-reported temperament factors could predict the effects of

Oddball flankers. Two factors of temperament—Attentional Control and Orienting Sensitivity—

together predicted about a third of the variance in vMMN magnitude. It appears that high Attentional

Control ability facilitates predictive coding, leading to larger differences between Standards and

Oddballs. Similarly, high Orienting Sensitivity impairs ignoring of the Standards, reducing such

differences.

Future work should explore whether temperament predicts the relative strength of bottom-up

85

Page 93: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 4. LEAKY IGNORING IN THE FLANKER TASK

and top-down neural processes in other settings. Another area for future work is to identify traits

that predict individual differences in behavior in response to the Oddball flanker directions, as my

results only predict the magnitude of the neural responses. Finally, future work should investigate

the role that alpha-band EEG oscillations play in performing this modified flanker task. Such

oscillations are thought to gate incoming sensory information, reducing interference from incoming

sensory signals (Jones et al., 2010; Payne et al., 2013; Romei, Gross, & Thut, 2010).

My attempts to demonstrate startle potentiation to errors and to Oddball flankers did not result

in any measurable effect. It is possible that my strategy of measuring EMG using the oculomotor

channels of the EEG cap resulted in electrode placement that was not reliably precise, or that the

high-impedance amplifier was not able to capture subtle EMG effects. Further, the effect I was

attempting to replicate is quite small (Hajcak & Foti, 2008), and it may be too small to be reliably

identified.

In summary, my novel modification of the Eriksen flanker task has produced a very strong

behavioral effect, showing that Oddball flankers have an exaggerated effect on subjects responses. I

also demonstrated that the vMMN produced by such flankers varies across subjects, and that about

a third of such variation can be explained by individual differences in temperament. The human

brain is sensitive to stochastic as well as sequentially-structured regularities, and can use them to

facilitate ignoring of incongruent distractors.

86

Page 94: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

Chapter 5

Conclusion

The studies presented and discussed in the preceding chapters expand our understanding of how

humans learn to represent sequences and structure, how they use those representations to support

cognition, and how they monitor whether those representations actually predict sensory input.

In Chapter 2 I used cognitive modeling to fit the error distributions that subjects generated on a

visuomotor sequence learning task. The best-fitting parameters make it clear that subjects’ improved

performance on familiar sequences is due to increases in both the precision with which items are

represented and the probability that all items have actually entered short-term memory, as seen in

Figures 2.7, 2.10, and 2.13. I also found that serial position curves flatten as sequences become

familiar, suggesting that items that are poorly-represented on the first presentation are allocated

more VSTM resources when the sequence is presented a subsequent time (Figures 2.9, 2.12, and

2.15).

In Chapter 3 I measured neural responses to new, familiar, and deviant sequence items, and found

that the neural responses to both new and deviant items differ from those to familiar items (Figures

3.4 and 3.5). Further, one P3 subcomponent is enhanced for both new and deviant items, while

another is enhanced only for deviant items (Figure 3.6), dissociating the processes of identifying

87

Page 95: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 5. CONCLUSION

and encoding task-relevant information from the processes detecting deviance.

Chapter 4 showed that unexpected events interspersed within a stream of actively-ignored stimuli

interfere with attention and action planning (Figure 4.4), and that individual differences in the neural

responses to such events are related to temperament (Figure 4.7).

5.1 Further evidence for human sensitivity to environmental

structure and predictability

Regardless of task or setting, sequences, predictions, and prediction monitoring are greatly important

to human cognition. For example, it appears that contextual cueing for items occurs even if the

items themselves are not present. When contexts are predictive of their accompanying items,

re-presentation of the context without the items strengthens subjects’ recollection of the items on a

later memory test (Smith, Hasinski, & Sederberg, 2013). This finding extends previous work on the

role of context in item processing and recognition (Biederman et al., 1982; Kersten et al., 2004;

Gronau et al., 2008; Mandler, 1980), and shows that context alone is capable of activating an object

representation to such a degree that memory is facilitated. Note that this only occurs when contexts

are reliably predictive, showing that the system has access to information about which cues should

be attended to and which should not.

Even more interesting are cases where people fail to use prediction information to facilitate

some task. For example, in a series of studies where people could choose how to allocate time

spent studying, a substantial portion of subjects made suboptimal decisions, even with information

available about their level of mastery and what would be tested (Ariel, 2013). Further, people’s

belief about the causal structure of the world influences their predictions within a series of events.

Subjects who believe they caused an outcome assume non-temporally-dependent structure, and make

88

Page 96: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 5. CONCLUSION

optimal predictions; subjects who believe outcomes are computer-generated search for patterns,

and probability-match (Green, Benson, Kersten, & Schrater, 2010). Subjects also assume positive

recency effects for short runs of events, and negative recency effects for long runs—the classic

Gambler’s Fallacy (Scheibehenne & Studer, 2013). Finally, despite Green et al. (2010)’s results

suggesting that people seek sequential patterns and structures, non-verbalizable sequences that are

periodic are quite difficult for subjects to learn (Kalish, 2013).

5.2 Future work

Several future lines of research could extend the projects presented in the preceding pages.

5.2.1 Modeling projects

Multi-parameter models for describing distributions are a rich and valuable tool. Both the VSTM

mixture models described in Chapter 2 and the exGaussian models described in Chapter 4 enrich

our understanding of exactly how task manipulations influence people’s processing and perfor-

mance. Future work with the VSTM mixture models should explicitly test the dynamic reallocation

hypotheses by fitting parameters to errors that are produced when a segment is poorly-represented

on the first presentation and to those that are produced when a segment is well-represented. The

hypothesis predicts smaller presentation-over-presentation changes on the remaining segments of

the display when one segment is poorly represented, due to memory resources being allocated to

that segment.

Another possible line of investigation is to follow up on the apparent encoding-limited nature

of this VSTM task. Repeated presentations lead to improved performance, suggesting that more

information is encoded each time. This contradicts the claims of Vogel, Woodman, and Luck (2006),

89

Page 97: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 5. CONCLUSION

who found that visual short-term memory is equally effective whether subjects are exposed to a

stimulus for 200 ms or 500 ms. They argue that this shows that VSTM cannot be encoding-limited

at moderate display lengths, which my task has. Vogel et al. (2006) did not, however, test the effect

of multiple short presentations. Future work could compare memory performance for single short

displays, single long displays, and multiple short displays, and determine whether there is a benefit

of repeated exposure.

5.2.2 Types of predictions

Chapters 3 and 4 test two very different paradigms within the space of predictive cognition. In

Chapter 3, the predictions involved are explicitly known, are derived from a fixed sequence, and

the task is to represent the sequence itself. In Chapter 4, the predictions are not explicitly known

(by most subjects), they are stochastic rather than sequential, and they are incidental to the actual

task subjects are performing. This dissertation does not speak to the great range of situations that

are in the middle. People have varying degrees of access to information about regularities in the

world; those regularities can perfectly sequential, somewhat sequential, or entirely stochastic; and

the predictability can be central or incidental to the task people are trying to perform. One can

conceptualize a three-dimensional space defined by strictness of temporal regularity on one axis,

relevance of that regularity on a second axis, and people’s access to information about the regularity

on a third. An area of rich further exploration would be to develop a task and a set of regularities

which can be used to systematically explore the space from sequential to stochastic regularities,

applying to elements which are task-relevant, irrelevant, or distractors.

Other work has been done that measures the influence of predictability at different spaces

in this domain. The serial reaction time task (SRTT; Nissen & Bullemer, 1987) measures the

accuracy and reaction time with which subjects can make keypress responses. Various stimuli on

90

Page 98: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 5. CONCLUSION

the screen cue subjects to press particular keys, and the sequence of stimuli (and thus keypresses)

repeats throughout an experimental block. Here, the prediction is sequential, not stochastic, and and

central to the task. Subjects develop varying degrees of awareness of the sequential structure; such

awareness does not affect the facilitation of performance for trials that obey the sequence, but does

predict neural responses to trials that deviate from it (Ferdinand et al., 2008; Russeler et al., 2003;

Russeler & Rosler, 2000; Schlaghecken et al., 2000). In short, these results are in the same area of

this three-dimensional conceptual space as my results from Chapter 3.

In a very different domain, Vo and Wolfe (2012) measured ERPs to stimuli that violated scene

context rules, which is in between the explicitly sequential and the completely stochastic structures

that I created for Chapters 3 and 4. Th predictability was central to the task (identifying objects

in naturalistic scenes), and subjects were generally aware when the context rules were violated.

Their ERP results are very different from mine, but due to the differences in the tasks and stimuli,

those differences are hard to interpret. One possible explanation is that predictions derived from

temporal regularities are qualitatively different from predictions derived from scene structure and

co-occurrence rules, and that a different system is involved in monitoring in this case. Further work

should more carefully follow up on this question.

5.2.3 Neuroimaging

My work has focused on the neural processes that are easily measured by scalp EEG: the anterior

cingulate and frontal areas, as measured by the P3, and sensory cortices, as measured by the MMN.

Other brain areas are of course involved in detecting and responding to deviant events. In particular,

the hippocampus has been implicated in binding events to their spatial and temporal contexts, and in

detecting associative novelty when an event occurs outside its usual context (Kumaran & Maguire,

2006, 2007; Manns, Howard, & Eichenbaum, 2007). Investigating hippocampal contributions

91

Page 99: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

CHAPTER 5. CONCLUSION

will require the use of fMRI or MEG, as those imaging methods are more capable of measuring

the activity of subcortical structures. A question of particular interest is whether and how the

hippocampus and the anterior cingulate cortex are connected. It is not known whether hippocampal

detection of associative novelty lead to anterior cingulate activation, or vice versa.

In summary, this dissertation describes three projects that further our understanding of the pro-

cesses by which people learn sequences and regularities, use predictions based on such regularities

to support cognition, and monitor when the predictions are validated by actual events.

92

Page 100: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

References

ReferencesAgam, Y., Bullock, D., & Sekuler, R. (2005). Imitating unfamiliar sequences of connected linear

motions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 2832-2843.Agam, Y., Galperin, H., Gold, B. J., & Sekuler, R. (2007). Learning to imitate novel motion

sequences. Journal of Vision, 7(5:1), 1–17.Agam, Y., Huang, J., & Sekuler, R. (2010). Neural correlates of sequence encoding in visuomotor

learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103, 1418-1424.Agam, Y., & Sekuler, R. (2007). Interactions between working memory and visual perception: An

ERP/EEG study. Neuroimage, 36, 933–942.Agam, Y., & Sekuler, R. (2008). Geometric structure and chunking in reproduction of motion

sequences. Journal of Vision, 8(1:11), 1–12.Ariel, R. (2013). Learning what to learn: The effects of task experience on strategy shifts in

the allocation of study time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, andCognition.

Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., & Watson, J. M. (2008). Beyond mean response latency:Response time distributional analyses of semantic priming. Journal of Memory and Language,59, 495-523.

Bar, M. (2004). Visual objects in context. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 617-629.Bar, M. (2009). The proactive brain: Memory for predictions. Philosophical Transactions of the

Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1235-1243.Barlow, H. B. (1961). Possible principles underlying the transformations of sensory messages. In

W. Rosenblith (Ed.), Sensory communication (pp. 217–234). MIT Press.Barnes, G. R. (2008). Cognitive processes involved in smooth pursuit eye movements. Brain and

Cognition, 68, 309-26.Bays, P. M., Catalao, R. F. G., & Husain, M. (2009). The precision of visual working memory is

set by allocation of a shared resource. Journal of Vision, 9(10:7), 1-11.Bays, P. M., Gorgoraptis, N., Wee, N., Marshall, L., & Husain, M. (2011). Temporal dynamics of

encoding, storage, and reallocation of visual working memory. Journal of Vision, 11(10:6),1-15.

Bays, P. M., Wu, E. Y., & Husain, M. (2011). Storage and binding of object features in visualworking memory. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1622-31.

Biederman, I., Mezzanotte, R. J., & Rabinowitz, J. C. (1982). Scene perception: Detecting andjudging objects undergoing relational violations. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 143–177.

Blumenthal, T. D., Cuthbert, B. N., Filion, D. L., Hackley, S., Lipp, O. V., & Boxtel, A. van.(2005). Committee report: Guidelines for human startle eyeblink electromyographic studies.Psychophysiology, 42, 1-15.

Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulatecortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.

Burke, M. R., & Barnes, G. R. (2007). Sequence learning in two-dimensional smooth pursuit eye

93

Page 101: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

References

movements in humans. Journal of Vision, 7(1:5), 1–12.Cassidy, B. S., Zebrowitz, L. A., & Gutchess, A. H. (2012). Appearance-based inferences bias

source memory. Memory & Cognition, 40, 1214-1224.Chalk, M., Seitz, A. R., & Series, P. (2010). Rapidly learned stimulus expectations alter perception

of motion. Journal of Vision, 10(8:2), 1-18.Cohn, J. V., DiZio, P., & Lackner, J. R. (2000). Reaching during virtual rotation: Context specific

compensations for expected Coriolis forces. Journal of Neurophysiology, 83, 3230-3240.Courchesne, E., Hillyard, S. A., & Galambos, R. (1975). Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the

visual evoked potential in man. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 39,131–143.

Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftusand Masson’s method. Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42–45.

Cravo, A. M., Rohenkohl, G., Wyart, V., & Nobre, A. C. (2013). Temporal expectation enhancescontrast sensitivity by phase entrainment of low-frequency oscillations in visual cortex.Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 4002-4010.

Crottaz-Herbette, S., & Menon, V. (2006). Where and when the anterior cingulate cortex modulatesattentional response: combined fMRI and ERP evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,18, 766–780.

Czigler, I. (2007). Visual mismatch negativity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21, 224-230.de Bruijn, E. R. A., Schubotz, R. I., & Ullsperger, M. (2007). An event-related potential study on the

observation of erroneous everyday actions. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience,7, 278–285.

Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trialEEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods,134, 9-21.

Dube, C., & Sekuler, R. (2012). On the nature of prototype effects in visual working memory formotion. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Vision Sciences Society, Naples, FL.

Dutilh, G., Ravenzwaaij, D. van, Nieuwenhuis, S., Maas, H. L. van der, Forstmann, B. U., &Wagenmakers, E.-J. . J. (2012). How to measure post-error slowing: A confound and a simplesolution. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56, 208-216.

Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York, NY:Teachers College, Columbia University.

Elsner, C., Falck-Ytter, T., & Gredeback, G. (2012). Humans anticipate the goal of other people’spoint-light actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 120:1–7.

Emrich, S. M., & Ferber, S. (2012). Competition increases binding errors in visual working memory.Journal of Vision, 12(4:12), 1-16.

Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a targetletter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149.

Escera, C., Alho, K., Winkler, I., & Naatanen, R. (1998). Neural mechanisms of involuntaryattention to acoustic novelty and change. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 590–604.

94

Page 102: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

References

Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for adult temperament. Journal ofResearch in Personality, 41, 868-888.

Farrell, S., Hurlstone, M. J., & Lewandowsky, S. (2013). Sequential dependencies in recall ofsequences: Filling in the blanks. Memory & Cognition.

Ferdinand, N. K., Mecklinger, A., & Kray, J. (2008). Error and deviance processing in implicit andexplicit sequence learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 629-642.

Fougnie, D., Suchow, J. W., & Alvarez, G. A. (2012). Variability in the quality of visual workingmemory. Nature Communications, 3, 1229:1–8.

Garrido, M. I., Kilner, J. M., Stephan, K. E., & Friston, K. J. (2009). The mismatch negativity: Areview of underlying mechanisms. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 453-463.

Geisler, W. S., Albrecht, D. G., Crane, A. M., & Stern, L. (2001). Motion direction signals in theprimary visual cortex of cat and monkey. Visual Neuroscience, 18, 501–516.

Gobel, E. W., Sanchez, D. J., & Reber, P. J. (2011). Integration of temporal and ordinal informationduring serial interception sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,Memory, and Cognition, 37, 994-1000.

Gobet, F., & Simon, H. (1998). Expert chess memory: revisiting the chunking hypothesis. Memory,6, 225–255.

Goldstein, A., Spencer, K. M., & Donchin, E. (2002). The influence of stimulus deviance andnovelty on the P300 and novelty P3. Psychophysiology, 39, 781–790.

Gorgoraptis, N., Catalao, R. F. G., Bays, P. M., & Husain, M. (2011). Dynamic updating of workingmemory resources for visual objects. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 8502-8511.

Green, C. S., Benson, C., Kersten, D., & Schrater, P. (2010). Alterations in choice behavior bymanipulations of world model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America, 107, 16401-16406.

Gronau, N., Neta, M., & Bar, M. (2008). Integrated contextual representation for objects’ identitiesand their locations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 371-388.

Guttman, S. E., Gilroy, L. A., & Blake, R. (2005). Hearing what the eyes see: Auditory encodingof visual temporal sequences. Psychological Science, 16, 228-235.

Hajcak, G., & Foti, D. (2008). Errors are aversive: Defensive motivation and the error-relatednegativity. Psychological Science, 19, 103-108.

Hebb, D. O. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal. In A. Fessard, R. Gerard,& J. Konorski (Eds.), Brain mechanisms and learning (pp. 37–46). Oxford, UK: BlackwellScientific Publications.

Hitch, G. J., Fastame, M. C., & Flude, B. (2005). How is the serial order of a verbal sequencecoded? Some comparisons between models. Memory, 13, 247-258.

Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Rein-forcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109,679-709.

Holroyd, C. B., & Yeung, N. (2012). Motivation of extended behaviors by anterior cingulate cortex.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 122–128.

95

Page 103: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

References

Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (1999). Contextual variability and serial position effects in freerecall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 923-941.

Hu, E. (2010). Context-specificity of internal model formation. Unpublished master’s thesis,Brandeis University.

Huang, J., & Sekuler, R. (2010a). Attention protects the fidelity of visual memory: Behavioral andelectrophysiological evidence. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 13461-13471.

Huang, J., & Sekuler, R. (2010b). Distortions in recall from visual memory: Two classes ofattractors at work. Journal of Vision, 10(2:24), 1-27.

Jancke, D. (2000). Orientation formed by a spot’s trajectory: a two-dimensional populationapproach in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, RC86:1–6.

Johnson, R., & Donchin, E. (1982). Sequential expectancies and decision making in a changingenvironment: An electrophysiological approach. Psychophysiology, 19, 183–200.

Jones, S. R., Kerr, C. E., Wan, Q., Pritchett, D. L., Hamalainen, M., & Moore, C. I. (2010).Cued spatial attention drives functionally relevant modulation of the mu rhythm in primarysomatosensory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 13760–13765.

Kahana, M. J., & Jacobs, J. (2000). Interresponse times in serial recall: Effects of intraserialrepetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26,1188-1197.

Kakade, S., & Dayan, P. (2002). Dopamine: Generalization and bonuses. Neural Networks, 15,549-559.

Kalish, M. L. (2013). Learning and extrapolating a periodic function. Memory & Cognition, 41,886–896.

Karlsson, M. P., & Frank, L. M. (2008). Network dynamics underlying the formation of sparse,informative representations in the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience(52), 14271–14281.

Kennerley, S. W., Behrens, T. E. J., & Wallis, J. D. (2011). Double dissociation of value computa-tions in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 1581–1589.

Kersten, D., Mamassian, P., & Yuille, A. (2004). Object perception as Bayesian inference. AnnualReview of Psychology, 55, 271-304.

Kimura, M., Kondo, H., Ohira, H., & Schroger, E. (2011). Unintentional temporal context-based prediction of emotional faces: An electrophysiological study. Cerebral Cortex, 22,1774–1785.

Kimura, M., Schroger, E., & Czigler, I. (2011). Visual mismatch negativity and its importance invisual cognitive sciences. NeuroReport, 22, 669–673.

Kowler, E. (1989). Cognitive expectations, not habits, control anticipatory smooth oculomotorpursuit. Vision Research, 29, 1049-1057.

Kuiper, N. H. (1960). Tests concerning random points on a circle. Proceedings of the KoninklijkeNederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, Series A, 63, 38–47.

Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2006). An unexpected sequence of events: Mismatch detection inthe human hippocampus. PLoS Biology, 4, 2372-2382.

Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Match-mismatch processes underlie human hippocampal

96

Page 104: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

References

responses to associative novelty. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 8517–8524.Lackner, J. R., & DiZio, P. (2005). Motor control and learning in altered dynamic environments.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 653-659.Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In Hixon Symposium, 1948

(p. 112-135).Laurent, P. A. (2008). The emergence of saliency and novelty responses from reinforcement

learning principles. Neural Networks, 21, 1493–1499.Le Pelley, M. E., Vadillo, M., & Luque, D. (2013). Learned predictiveness influences rapid

attentional capture: Evidence from the dot probe task. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1888–1900.

Linden, D. E. J. (2005). The P300: Where in the brain is it produced and what does it tell us?Neuroscientist, 11, 563–576.

Ljungberg, J. K., & Parmentier, F. B. R. (2012). Cross-modal distraction by deviance: Functionalsimilarities between the auditory and tactile modalities. Experimental Psychology, 59, 355–363.

Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 476–490.

Luck, S. J. (2005). An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.

Maier, M. E., & Steinhauser, M. (2013). Updating expected action outcome in the medial frontalcortex involves an evaluation of error type. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 15705-15709.

Mandler, G. (1980). Recognizing: The judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological review, 87,252-271.

Manns, J. R., Howard, M. W., & Eichenbaum, H. (2007, 11). Gradual changes in hippocampalactivity support remembering the order of events. Neuron, 56(3), 530-40.

Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data.Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164, 177–190.

Maryott, J., Noyce, A., & Sekuler, R. (2011). Eye movements and imitation learning: Intentionaldisruption of expectation. Journal of Vision, 11(1:7), 1-16.

Maryott, J., & Sekuler, R. (2009). Age-related changes in imitating sequences of observedmovements. Psychological Aging, 24, 476-486.

Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacityfor processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.

Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005).Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4, 61–64.

Naatanen, R., Paavilainen, P., Rinne, T., & Alho, K. (2007). The mismatch negativity (MMN)in basic research of central auditory processing: A review. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118,2544–2590.

Naatanen, R., Paavilainen, P., Titinen, H., Jiang, D., & Alho, K. (1993). Attention and mismatchnegativity. Psychophysiology, 30, 436-450.

97

Page 105: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

References

Nassar, M. R., Rumsey, K. M., Wilson, R. C., Parikh, K., Heasly, B., & Gold, J. I. (2012). Rationalregulation of learning dynamics by pupil-linked arousal systems. Nature Neuroscience, 15,1040-1046.

Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). Decision making, the P3, and the locuscoeruleus-norepinephrine system. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 510–532.

Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence fromperformance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1-32.

Nostl, A., Marsh, J. E., & Sorqvist, P. (2012). Expectations modulate the magnitude of attentionalcapture by auditory events. PLoS One, 7(11), e48569.

Noyce, A., & Sekuler, R. (in revisions). Two P3 components elicited by violations of newly-learnedpredictions.

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., & Schoffelen, J.-M. M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source softwarefor advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. ComputationalIntelligence and Neuroscience, 2011, 156869:1–9.

Ouden, H. E. M. den, Friston, K. J., Daw, N. D., McIntosh, A. R., & Stephan, K. E. (2009). A dualrole for prediction error in associative learning. Cerebral Cortex, 19(5), 1175-1185.

Parmentier, F. B. R., Elford, G., Escera, C., Andres, P., & San Miguel, I. (2008). The cognitivelocus of distraction by acoustic novelty in the cross-modal oddball task. Cognition, 106,408–432.

Parmentier, F. B. R., Elsley, J. V., Andres, P., & Barcelo, F. (2011). Why are auditory novelsdistracting? Contrasting the roles of novelty, violation of expectation and stimulus change.Cognition, 119(3), 374–380.

Parmentier, F. B. R., Ljungberg, J. K., Elsley, J. V., & Lindkvist, M. (2011). A behavioral study ofdistraction by vibrotactile novelty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perceptionand Performance, 37, 1134–1139.

Payne, L., Guillory, S., & Sekuler, R. (2013). Attention-modulated alpha-band oscillations protectagainst intrusion of irrelevant information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1463–1476.

Pazo-Alvarez, P., Cadaveira, F., & Amenedo, E. (2003). MMN in the visual modality: A review.Biological Psychology, 63, 199-236.

Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness ofconditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532-552.

Pigeon, P., Bortolami, S. B., DiZio, P., & Lackner, J. R. (2003). Coordinated turn-and-reachmovements. I. Anticipatory compensation for self-generated Coriolis and interaction torques.Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 276-289.

Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology,118, 2128-2148.

Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,32, 3-25.

Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals.

98

Page 106: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

References

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160-174.Ricker, T. J., & Cowan, N. (2010). Loss of visual working memory within seconds: The com-

bined use of refreshable and non-refreshable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1355-1368.

Ridderinkhof, K. R., Band, G. P., & Logan, G. D. (1999). A study of adaptive behavior: Effects ofage and irrelevant information on the ability to inhibit one’s actions. Acta Psychologica, 101,315-337.

Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ramautar, J. R., & Wijnen, J. G. (2009). To P(E) or not to P(E): A P3-like ERPcomponent reflecting the processing of response errors. Psychophysiology, 46, 531–538.

Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of themedial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443-447.

Roggeman, C., Klingberg, T., Feenstra, H. E. M., Compte, A., & Almeida, R. (2013). Trade-off between capacity and precision in visuospatial working memory. Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience.

Romei, V., Gross, J., & Thut, G. (2010). On the role of prestimulus alpha rhythms over occipito-parietal areas in visual input regulation: correlation or causation? Journal of Neuroscience,30, 8692–8697.

Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Temperament, development, and personality. Current Directions inPsychological Science, 16, 207–212.

Rotman, G., Troje, N. F., Johansson, R. S., & Flanagan, J. R. (2006). Eye movements whenobserving predictable and unpredictable actions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96, 1358-1369.

Russeler, J., Hennighausen, E., Munte, T. F., & Rosler, F. (2003). Differences in incidental andintentional learning of sensorimotor sequences as revealed by event-related brain potentials.Cognitive Brain Research, 15, 116–126.

Russeler, J., & Rosler, F. (2000). Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: Evidence fordistinct coding of perceptual and motor representations. Acta Psychologica, 104, 45-67.

Scheibehenne, B., & Studer, B. (2013). A hierarchical Bayesian model of the influence of runlength on sequential predictions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, epub ahead of print.

Schlaghecken, F., Sturmer, B., & Eimer, M. (2000). Chunking processes in the learning of eventsequences: Electrophysiological indicators. Memory & Cognition, 28, 821-831.

Schoenbaum, G., Esber, G. R., & Iordanova, M. D. (2013). Dopamine signals mimic rewardprediction errors. Nature Neuroscience, 16, 777–779.

Schroger, E. (1996). A neural mechanism for involuntary attention shifts to changes in auditorystimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 527–539.

Schultz, W. (2006). Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annual Review ofPsychology, 57, 87-115.

Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward.Science, 275, 1593-1599.

Sekuler, R., Siddiqui, A., Goyal, N., & Rajan, R. (2003). Reproduction of seen actions: Stimulus-selective learning. Perception, 32, 839-854.

99

Page 107: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

References

Smith, T. A., Hasinski, A. E., & Sederberg, P. B. (2013). The context repetition effect: Predictedevents are remembered better, even when they don’t happen. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: General, 142, 1298–1308.

Spencer, K. M., Dien, J., & Donchin, E. (2001). Spatiotemporal analysis of the late ERP responsesto deviant stimuli. Psychophysiology, 38, 343–358.

Squires, K. C., Wickens, C., Squires, N. K., & Donchin, E. (1976). The effect of stimulus sequenceon the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. Science, 193, 1142-1146.

Squires, N. K., Squires, K. C., & Hillyard, S. A. (1975). Two varieties of long-latency positivewaves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. Electroencephalography and ClinicalNeurophysiology, 38, 387-401.

Stefanics, G., Kimura, M., & Czigler, I. (2011). Visual mismatch negativity reveals automaticdetection of sequential regularity violation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 46:1-9.

Sternberg, D. A., & McClelland, J. L. (2012). Two mechanisms of human contingency learning.Psychological Science, 23, 59–68.

Suchow, J. W., Brady, T. F., Fougnie, D., & Alvarez, G. A. (2013). Modeling visual workingmemory with the MemToolbox. Journal of Vision, 13(10:9), 1–8.

Summerfield, C., & Egner, T. (2009). Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends inCognitive Sciences, 13, 403-409.

Summerfield, J. J., Lepsien, J., Gitelman, D. R., Mesulam, M. M., & Nobre, A. C. (2006). Orientingattention based on long-term memory experience. Neuron(6), 905–916.

Takahashi, Y. K., Roesch, M. R., Stalnaker, T. A., Haney, R. Z., Calu, D. J., Taylor, A. R., et al.(2009). The orbitofrontal cortex and ventral tegmental area are necessary for learning fromunexpected outcomes. Neuron, 62, 269-280.

Takahashi, Y. K., Roesch, M. R., Wilson, R. C., Toreson, K., O’Donnell, P., Niv, Y., et al. (2011).Expectancy-related changes in firing of dopamine neurons depend on orbitofrontal cortex.Nature Neuroscience, 14, 1590-1597.

Tales, A., Troscianko, T., Wilcock, G. K., Newton, P., & Butler, S. R. (2002). Age-related changesin the preattentional detection of visual change. NeuroReport, 13, 969-972.

van den Berg, R., Shin, H., Chou, W.-C. C., George, R., & Ma, W. J. (2012). Variability in encodingprecision accounts for visual short-term memory limitations. Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 8780-8785.

Vo, M., & Wolfe, J. (2012). Semantic and syntactic inconsistencies in scenes elicit differential ERPsignatures. Presented at the annual meeting of the Vision Sciences Society, Naples, FL.

Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2006). The time course of consolidation invisual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception andPerformance(6), 1436–1451.

Vossel, S., Weidner, R., Thiel, C. M., & Fink, G. R. (2009). What is odd in Posner’s location-cueingparadigm? Neural responses to unexpected location and feature changes compared. Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 30–41.

Wacongne, C., Changeux, J.-P. P., & Dehaene, S. (2012). A neuronal model of predictive coding

100

Page 108: Neural processes for learning and monitoring sequential

References

accounting for the mismatch negativity. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 3665-3678.Wacongne, C., Labyt, E., Wassenhove, V. van, Bekinschtein, T., Naccache, L., & Dehaene, S. (2011).

Evidence for a hierarchy of predictions and prediction errors in human cortex. Proceedingsof the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 20754–20759.

White, C. N., Brown, S., & Ratcliff, R. (2012). A test of Bayesian observer models of processingin the Eriksen flanker task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception andPerformance, 38, 489–497.

White, C. N., Ratcliff, R., & Starns, J. J. (2011). Diffusion models of the flanker task: Discreteversus gradual attentional selection. Cognitive Psychology, 63, 210–238.

Wilken, P., & Ma, W. J. (2004). A detection theory account of change detection. Journal of Vision,4, 1120-1135.

Wills, A. J., Lavric, A., Croft, G. S., & Hodgson, T. L. (2007). Predictive learning, predictionerrors, and attention: Evidence from event-related potentials and eye tracking. Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience, 19, 843-854.

Winkler, I. (2007). Interpreting the mismatch negativity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21, 147–163.Winkler, I., Teder-Salejarvi, W. A., Horvath, J., Naatanen, R., & Sussman, E. (2003). Human

auditory cortex tracks task-irrelevant sound sources. NeuroReport, 14, 2053-2056.Yu, A. J., & Dayan, P. (2005). Uncertainty, neuromodulation, and attention. Neuron, 46, 681-92.Yu, A. J., Dayan, P., & Cohen, J. D. (2009). Dynamics of attentional selection under conflict:

Toward a rational Bayesian account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perceptionand Performance, 35, 700–717.

Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2008). Discrete fixed-resolution representations in visual workingmemory. Nature, 453, 233-235.

Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2009). Sudden death and gradual decay in visual working memory.Psychological Science, 20, 423–428.

Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2011). The number and quality of representations in working memory.Psychological Science, 22, 1434-1441.

Zhao, J., Al-Aidroos, N., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2013). Attention is spontaneously biased towardregularities. Psychological Science, 24, 667–677.

Zion Golumbic, E., Cogan, G. B., Schroeder, C. E., & Poeppel, D. (2013). Visual input enhancesselective speech envelope tracking in auditory cortex at a “cocktail party”. Journal ofNeuroscience, 33, 1417-1426.

Zokaei, N., Gorgoraptis, N., Bahrami, B., Bays, P. M., & Husain, M. (2011). Precision of workingmemory for visual motion sequences and transparent motion surfaces. Journal of Vision,11(14:2), 1-18.

Zuijen, T. L. van, Simoens, V. L., Paavilainen, P., Naatanen, R., & Tervaniemi, M. (2006). Implicit,intuitive, and explicit knowledge of abstract regularities in a sound sequence: an event-relatedbrain potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1292–1303.

101