memory rehabilitation—should we be aiming for restoration or compensation?

2
J Neurol (2006) 253 : 520–521 DOI 10.1007/s00415-006-0014-5 SHORT COMMENTARY Jonathan J. Evans Memory rehabilitation – should we be aiming for restoration or compensation? A commentary on Hildebrandt et al. “Group therapy for memory impaired patients. A partial remediation is possible.” in J Neurol (2006) 253:512–519 Memory impairment is a common consequence of neu- rological injury or disease, causes significant disability and is therefore a critical target for rehabilitation inter- vention. Whether cognitive rehabilitation should aim to reduce impairment or to compensate for the impair- ment is a question that has exercised the rehabilitation community, but in relation to most cognitive functions remains largely unresolved. For some disorders (e. g. unilateral neglect) there is evidence that interventions specifically targeted at modifying relevant cognitive sys- tems are effective [1]. However, for memory rehabilitation, there has, to date, been no substantial evidence that memory can be improved through restitution-oriented therapies and hence compensatory approaches (e. g. use of external memory aids, or application of compensatory learning strategies) are the treatment of choice [1, 2]. However, this conclusion is largely based on the absence of evi- dence rather than evidence of absence of an effect of restitution oriented therapies [2]. As a result of the paucity of evidence comparing restitution-oriented and compensation approaches to memory rehabilitation, the paper by Hildebrandt et al. is welcome. I will argue, however, that caution is required in drawing conclusions with regard to clinical practice from this study. Hildebrandt et al’s study compared three interventions for patients with organic memory impairment. One group underwent an intensive process-oriented treatment (POT) with a focus on prac- tice at learning word lists (emphasising encoding strate- gies, repetition, and rehearsal). A second group received memory strategy training (ST), including associational strategies (i. e. face-name learning), various encoding strategies and use of external aids.A third control group received a similar training to the POT group, but with lower intensity. Outcome measures were neuropsycho- logical tests of memory, attention and verbal fluency. The results showed that the low intensity treatment con- trol group did not improve significantly on any measure. Both of the high intensity treatment groups showed im- provement on a range of measures of memory function- ing, but the POT group improved on a greater number of tests including a measure of free recall and a measure of attention. There was therefore evidence of a dose-de- pendent effect for memory rehabilitation and the process-oriented treatment was, overall, more effective. Hildebrandt et al. acknowledge that their study has limitations, particularly in terms of the outcome mea- sures used. Rehabilitation is ultimately concerned with the ability of individuals to participate in valued activi- ties. Psychometric measures of cognition are useful, but limited, tools for the evaluation of cognitive rehabilita- tion. Restitution-oriented treatments may be better at improving performance on psychometric tests (which are typically close in form to training tasks) than strat- egy training. However, compensatory strategy training may be better at increasing functioning in activities of daily living than restitution oriented therapies. If this were the case, it would be reasonable to conclude that limited rehabilitation resources should be focused on compensatory strategy training. Hildebrandt et al.’s pa- per helpfully highlights that this issue is not yet resolved, and requires further systematic examination. JON 2014 Received: 27 May 2005 Accepted: 27 May 2005 Prof. Jonathan J. Evans () Academic Centre Gartnavel Royal Hospital Glasgow, G12 0XH, UK Tel.: +44-0141/211-3978 E-Mail: [email protected]

Upload: jonathan-j-evans

Post on 15-Jul-2016

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Memory rehabilitation—should we be aiming for restoration or compensation?

J Neurol (2006) 253 : 520–521DOI 10.1007/s00415-006-0014-5 SHORT COMMENTARY

Jonathan J. Evans Memory rehabilitation – should we beaiming for restoration or compensation?

A commentary on Hildebrandt et al. “Group therapyfor memory impaired patients. A partial remediationis possible.” in J Neurol (2006) 253:512–519

Memory impairment is a common consequence of neu-rological injury or disease, causes significant disabilityand is therefore a critical target for rehabilitation inter-vention. Whether cognitive rehabilitation should aim toreduce impairment or to compensate for the impair-ment is a question that has exercised the rehabilitationcommunity, but in relation to most cognitive functionsremains largely unresolved. For some disorders (e. g.unilateral neglect) there is evidence that interventionsspecifically targeted at modifying relevant cognitive sys-tems are effective [1].

However, for memory rehabilitation, there has, todate, been no substantial evidence that memory can beimproved through restitution-oriented therapies andhence compensatory approaches (e. g. use of externalmemory aids, or application of compensatory learningstrategies) are the treatment of choice [1, 2]. However,this conclusion is largely based on the absence of evi-dence rather than evidence of absence of an effect ofrestitution oriented therapies [2].

As a result of the paucity of evidence comparingrestitution-oriented and compensation approaches tomemory rehabilitation, the paper by Hildebrandt et al. iswelcome. I will argue, however, that caution is required

in drawing conclusions with regard to clinical practicefrom this study. Hildebrandt et al’s study comparedthree interventions for patients with organic memoryimpairment. One group underwent an intensiveprocess-oriented treatment (POT) with a focus on prac-tice at learning word lists (emphasising encoding strate-gies, repetition, and rehearsal). A second group receivedmemory strategy training (ST), including associationalstrategies (i. e. face-name learning), various encodingstrategies and use of external aids.A third control groupreceived a similar training to the POT group, but withlower intensity. Outcome measures were neuropsycho-logical tests of memory, attention and verbal fluency.The results showed that the low intensity treatment con-trol group did not improve significantly on any measure.Both of the high intensity treatment groups showed im-provement on a range of measures of memory function-ing, but the POT group improved on a greater number oftests including a measure of free recall and a measure ofattention. There was therefore evidence of a dose-de-pendent effect for memory rehabilitation and theprocess-oriented treatment was, overall, more effective.

Hildebrandt et al. acknowledge that their study haslimitations, particularly in terms of the outcome mea-sures used. Rehabilitation is ultimately concerned withthe ability of individuals to participate in valued activi-ties. Psychometric measures of cognition are useful, butlimited, tools for the evaluation of cognitive rehabilita-tion. Restitution-oriented treatments may be better atimproving performance on psychometric tests (whichare typically close in form to training tasks) than strat-egy training. However, compensatory strategy trainingmay be better at increasing functioning in activities ofdaily living than restitution oriented therapies. If thiswere the case, it would be reasonable to conclude thatlimited rehabilitation resources should be focused oncompensatory strategy training. Hildebrandt et al.’s pa-per helpfully highlights that this issue is not yet resolved,and requires further systematic examination.JO

N 2

014

Received: 27 May 2005Accepted: 27 May 2005

Prof. Jonathan J. Evans (�)Academic CentreGartnavel Royal HospitalGlasgow, G12 0XH, UKTel.: +44-0141/211-3978E-Mail: [email protected]

520_521_Evans_JON_2014 13.04.2006 9:08 Uhr Seite 520

Page 2: Memory rehabilitation—should we be aiming for restoration or compensation?

521

1. Robertson IR (1999) Setting goals forrehabilitation. Curr Opin Neurol 12:703–708

2. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Kalmar K,et al. (2000) Evidence based cognitiverehabilitation: recommendations forclinical practice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil81:1596–1615

References

520_521_Evans_JON_2014 13.04.2006 9:08 Uhr Seite 521