maximum voluntary isometric pinch contraction and.10

Upload: happystars

Post on 02-Jun-2018

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/10/2019 Maximum Voluntary Isometric Pinch Contraction and.10

    1/8

    Maximum voluntary isometric pinch contraction andforce-matching from the fourth to the eighth decades of lifeTrenah L. Herring-Marler, Waneen W. Spirduso, Richard T. Eakinand Lawrence D. Abraham

    Understanding the effects of age and gender on pinch

    strength, variability, and accuracy and how ones hand

    function changes with age better enables those in the

    preventative and rehabilitative fields to combat these

    losses. The present study examined fine motor maximum

    pinch strength [maximum voluntary isometric contraction

    (MVIC)] as well as the ability to maintain 5% MVIC

    accurately and consistently in five decades. One hundred

    adults in five groups, 20 in each decade of life from 30 to 79

    years old, were nonrandomly recruited from the community.

    A two-way analysis of variance applied to MVIC, and a

    two-way multivariate analysis of variance applied to the

    variability (coefficient of variability) and accuracy (root

    mean square error), plus correlation and regression

    analyses, were used to determine decade and gendereffects on pinch force. The task involved using isometric

    pinch control of a computer cursor to match a 5% of MVICforce level represented by a horizontal line. MVIC and

    force-matching steadiness and accuracy across all ageswere not significantly different until the eighth decade

    (P< 0.01). Men were stronger (P< 0.001) but performedlow-level force-matching with greater error (P< 0.001) than

    women. Strength was not correlated with steadiness but

    was weakly correlated with accuracy (r=0.293, P 60 years) in the ability

    to produce single-finger and multifinger force (13 and 48%

    lower, respectively) as compared with their gender-

    matched younger (mean 21.9 years) counterparts. De

    Serres and Fang (2004) reported a mean maximal pinch

    force of the dominant hand of 12 young adults (2228

    years) at 60.2 N, compared with 44.2 N for 12 older adults

    (6775 years), a 26.6% reduction. Similar results were

    found with older participants (mean 70.5 years), showing

    27.3% less pinch-grip strength than young adults (Ranga-

    nathan et al., 2001). Sperling (1980) reported an averagedrop of 14% in maximal finger pinch force in 70-year-old

    men and women when compared with young adults

    (2030 years). Similarly, Puh (2010), the only researcher

    to compare pinch-grip strength across four age groups

    (2034; 3549; 5064; 6579 years), found the fingertip

    pinch of the oldest group to be 19% less than that of theyoungest group. However, the strongest age group was the

    3549-year-old group, not the 2034-year-old group.

    Force variability

    Age also affects force variability during gross motor

    submaximal contractions (Tracy and Enoka, 2002) and

    Original article 159

    0342-5282 c 2014 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins DOI: 10.1097/M RR.0b013e32836061ee

    Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 8/10/2019 Maximum Voluntary Isometric Pinch Contraction and.10

    2/8

    is associated with increases in force variability during fine

    motor force-matching tasks among old adults (Galganskiet al., 1993;Laidlaw et al., 2000;Ranganathan et al., 2001;

    Vaillancourt et al., 2003; De Serres and Fang, 2004). In a510% maximum voluntary isometric contraction (MVIC)

    matching pinch task, force fluctuations were significantlygreater in amplitude among older participants (6579

    years) than in younger participants (2035 years) (Ranga-nathan et al., 2001). Furthermore, healthy older adults

    (Galganski et al., 1993) and women (Ranganathan et al.,

    2001) showed a decreased ability to maintain steady

    submaximal forces in the hand compared with that of

    young adults (2035 years) and men, respectively.

    Vaillancourt et al. (2003) found an increased coefficient

    of variability (CV) of force with increased age at all

    force levels [5, 10, 20, 40% maximum voluntary contrac-

    tion (MVC)/MVIC]; however, the greatest age differential

    in variability occurred at the 5 and 10% MVC force levels.

    AccuracyIn addition to decreases in strength and increases in

    variability, aging decreases fine motor finger force

    regulation, especially at low forces (Shinohara et al.,

    2003a) and more so for those in their 80s than those in

    their 70s (Hackel et al., 1992). Other researchers have

    substantiated these reports with findings of larger force-

    matching errors at lower force levels for older adults

    (Galganski et al., 1993; Visser et al., 2003; Sosnoff and

    Newell, 2006). De Serres and Fang (2004) found near-

    significant differences (P< 0.068) in relative force error

    between young and elderly groups (mean age 25.3 and

    71.5 years, respectively) in force-reproduction tasks

    without visual feedback at 5% maximal pinch force(termed MVIC in the present study) but not at the 20

    and 40% MVIC force-matching levels.

    Gender and strength

    Puh (2010) reported, along with seven other research

    groups, that womens grip strength ranged from 57 to 65%

    that of men and that womens fingertip-pinch strength

    reached 73% that of men. Using single-finger and four-

    finger tasks, Oliveira et al. (2008) and Shinohara et al.

    (2003a)investigated how the gender of participants affects

    MVIC. Elderly women showed greater force deficits

    (51%; Oliveira et al., 2008: four-finger task and 35.7%;Shinohara et al., 2003b) when compared with young

    females, whereas older men showed 45% Oliveira et al.,

    2008 and 28.2% less force production (Shinohara

    et al., 2003b) than younger men. Elderly womens proximal

    phalangeal press (intrinsic musculature) produced the

    largest age-related force deficit of 45.2%, and mens distal

    phalangeal press (extrinsic musculature) recorded the least

    amount of force deficit at 23.1%, emphasizing differences

    between sexes as well as between tasks that involve

    extrinsic and intrinsic musculature tasks (Shinohara et al.,

    2003b). Ranganathan et al. (2001) found that the pinch

    strength of 70-year-old men was 14.2% less than that of

    20-year-old men, but the pinch strength of 70-year-old

    women was 32.5% less than that of young women, which wasmore than double the pinch strength difference in men.

    Gender in force variability and accuracy

    Men and womens hand function decreases with age asmeasured by the Jebson Test of Hand Function (Hackel

    et al., 1992;Peolssonet al., 2001;Ranganathanet al., 2001).However, gender effects in controlling submaximal forces

    in fine motor tasks have been mixed. The ability of older

    women to maintain a steady submaximal pinch force has

    been reported to be less than that of men (Hackel et al.,

    1992). However,Shimet al. (2004)did not find any gender

    effects for a multifinger task at 10% MVIC.Shinoharaet al.

    (2003b) investigated how the gender of a participant

    influenced finger coordination in submaximal ramped

    force-matching, finding among elderly (7095 years old)

    participants significant differences in the ability to match

    a target force, with men having greater variability than

    women (P< 0.01).

    Relationships among maximum voluntary isometric

    contraction, force variability, and accuracy

    Few researchers have correlated fine motor strength

    measures to force variability or accuracy measures. In one

    study, older womens peg-placing time significantly corre-

    lated negatively with their index-thumb pinch strength

    (r= 0.693; Ranganathan et al., 2001). Olafsdottir et al.

    (2008) found high correlations between error measure-

    ments and force. Accuracy of performance was associated

    with increases in the force-stabilization synergies, which

    the researchers attributed to the hypothesis that

    synergies lead to more reproducible performance.

    Understanding the effects of age and gender on pinch

    strength, variability, and accuracy, and the manner in

    which ones hand function changes with age better

    enables those in the preventative and rehabilitative fields

    to combat these losses. The present study examined fine

    motor maximum pinch strength (MVIC) as well as theability to maintain 5% MVIC accurately and consistently

    in five decades, from 30 to 80 years of age.

    The researchers hypothesized that (a) 10-year age groups(decades) would differ in MVIC, variability, and accuracy;

    (b) mens MVIC pinch strength would be higher thanwomens; (c) the interaction of decade and gender would

    not be significant for MVIC pinch; and (d) genderdifferences would not be significant for variability and

    accuracy. It was also hypothesized that MVIC would becorrelated with submaximal force variability (CV) and

    accuracy [root mean square error (RMSE)].

    MethodsParticipants

    One hundred adults in five groups, 20 in each decade of

    life from 30 to 79 years, participated voluntarily in the

    160 International Journal of Rehabilitation Research 2014, Vol 37 No 2

    Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/10/2019 Maximum Voluntary Isometric Pinch Contraction and.10

    3/8

    study. Each decade group contained 10 men and 10

    women. On the basis of a P level of 0.05 or higher,and power levels equal to or greater than 0.950, we

    used Cohens (1988) interpretation of effect sizes todetermine meaningful differences. Participant selection,

    regardless of ethnic background or socioeconomic status,was based on the participants self-reported absence of

    both neurological symptoms and past or current relatedmedical treatment. Participants were excluded if deemed

    unable to comprehend or sufficiently perform practice

    trials or if they presented with any other marked deficit

    (e.g. vision impairments, orthopedic injury to tested

    upper extremity) affecting their ability to complete the

    task. Participants were allowed to use corrective lenses.

    Participants taking medications that could interfere with

    hand function, cognition, or visual acuity were also

    excluded (e.g. lithium, baclofen).

    DesignThis study used a quasi-experimental between-model

    design in which the independent variables (between

    factors) were age (decade) (3079 years) and gender

    (male/female). The dependent variables (within factors)

    were MVIC, pinch-force variability (CV), and accuracy

    (RMSE). The MVIC was defined as the highest aggregate

    preferred hand thumb and finger force level of three

    MVIC trials expressed in N. The CV (force variability)

    was calculated as the mean of the coefficient of thumb

    finger aggregate force measurements for all of a

    participants 10 individual trials. Those 10 trial-specific

    values represented an average difference between a

    measured force at each sampling and the mean forceacross all samplings scaled by that mean; that is, the SDs

    away from the participants own mean divided by the

    participants own mean. Thus, CV= sum (SD/m)/10.

    The dependent variable used to indirectly assess accuracy

    was a RMSE derived from the individual force error of

    each sampling instance corresponding to the static target

    level, which was scaled to 5% of the persons MVIC.Thus, for each sampling, the error e was determined by

    e fcj j5 % MVIC;

    where fc

    is the force level of the cursor, determined

    by the sum of the finger and thumb forces. Thedependent variables then were determined as the root

    mean square of these error values, that is,

    RMSE

    ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiPNi1

    e2i

    N

    vuuut ;

    whose mean over a grouping is

    RMSEM

    XM

    i1

    RMSE i

    and the CV of force, that is,

    CV

    ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiPNi1

    fcfc 2

    i=N

    s

    fc;

    whose mean over a grouping is

    CVMXMi1

    CV i:

    In these expressionsNis the number of samplings within

    the trial segment with the summation running overall

    sampling instances in the interval being evaluated and

    M is the number of trials or trial segments over which

    the variable mean is taken.

    Instrumentation and procedures

    This study used the Manual Force Quantification System

    (MFQS) (Fig. 1a and b), controlled by LabVIEW software

    (LabVIEW 6.1; National Instruments, Austin, Texas,

    USA), which quantifies low-level isometric force control

    during a precision pinch task of the thumb and index

    finger (Spirdusoet al., 2005a).Spirduso et al. (2005a)can

    also be referenced for instrumentation, procedures, and

    data acquisition.

    After a demonstration, the participants provided three

    maximum pinch forces for a duration of 6 s each with a

    1-min rest between them. The participants performed 10

    force-matching (Fig. 2) trials at 5% MVIC for a test time

    duration scaled to their MVIC, between 12 and 18s.

    These were the last 10 trials of a battery of three tests:

    tracing (20 trials), tracking (20 trials) and force-matching

    (10 trials). The protocol was similar to that described

    bySpirduso et al. (2005b), and the details and outcomes

    of these other results (tracing and tracking) are presented

    in separate studies. No static force-matching elements

    were included in the 40 prior task trials, thus the

    participants gained no specific force-matching practiceby performing the prior tasks. Five of the 100 participants

    used their left hand as their preferred hand (Fig. 2).

    Analysis

    Statistical analyses included descriptive statistics. A mod-ified z-score method of finding outliers (Shiffler, 1988) was

    used. Trial data that were above an absolute value of 2.85

    (maximumz-score for an Nof 10) were removed from the

    data analysis but saved for reference. Means and SDs were

    recalculated based on the remaining trials.

    A simple analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to analyze

    MVIC, and two-way MANOVA (decade and gender) with

    Bonferroni correction was carried out to analyze RMSEand

    CVin force-matching. Main effects and interactions were

    tested. Post-hoc, pair-wise comparisons were carried out for

    decades in MVIC, RMSE, and CV. Levenes test of

    MVIC pinch and force-matching Herring-Marleret al. 161

    Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/10/2019 Maximum Voluntary Isometric Pinch Contraction and.10

    4/8

    homogeneity, Pearsons product-moment correlation, and

    regression analyses were performed.

    ResultsMaximum voluntary isometric contraction force

    The main effect for decades MVIC was significant

    [F(4,90) = 4.857, P< 0.001, Z2P= 0.178] for precision

    pinch peak force (Fig. 3). Post-hoc, pair-wise comparisons

    revealed that the maximum isometric pinch forces

    of the participants in their 70s were significantly lower

    than those of participants in their 40s (P< 0.01),

    50s (P< 0.005), and 60s (P< 0.01). Although the 30s

    had higher MVIC values than the 70s, the difference

    between the groups was not statistically significant

    (P= 0.09).

    The main effect for gender was significant with a large

    effect size for maximal pinch force [F(1,90) = 81.09,P< 0.001, Z2P= 0.474]. The MVIC of women was 29.9%less than that of men overall (Table 1). Gender did not

    interact with decade in MVIC.

    Force-matching

    The force-matching test also resulted in age-related

    effects for force variability and accuracy. The mean

    within-participant force variability for each decade,

    produced by averaging all the individual CV values of

    the groups participants CV, revealed a main effect for

    decade [F(4,90) = 7.318, P< 0.001, Z2P= 0.240]. Post-

    hoc tests revealed that participants in their 70s were

    more variable than participants in their 30s, 40s, 50s, and

    60s (46, 48, 39, and 35% difference, respectively,P< 0.01; Fig. 4a). A main effect for accuracy (RMSE)

    was found [F(4,90) = 4.773, P< 0.005, Z2P= 0.175].

    Participants in their 70s produced significantly more

    error than the participants in their 30s and 40s (43 and

    36% at P< 0.01, respectively). Additionally, participants

    in their 70s approached significance in error differences

    from participants in their 50s (30% more error, P= 0.06);

    whereas those in their 60s were not different from those

    in their 70s (23% more error, P= 0.33;Fig. 4b).

    No gender effects were found for force variability.However, there were gender effects for accuracy

    [F(1,90) = 17.389, P< 0.001, Z2P= 0.162]. Men per-formed with higher error at RMSE = 0.073 N, whereas

    the equivalent value for women was 32% lower(RMSE = 0.50 N). No interactions were found.

    Correlation and homogeneity

    Pearsons product-moment correlation with sexes com-

    bined (N= 100) between MVIC and accuracy (RMSE)

    wasr= 0.293,P< 0.01; whereas, for CVandRMSEit was

    r= 0.783,P< 0.001. MVIC andCVwere not significantly

    correlated with each other.

    Fig. 1

    (a) Manual Force Quantification System (MFQS) and (b) force transducers. The MFQS includes a force-matching template displayed on a computerscreen, a platform, and a console (a) that supports the adjustable force transducers (b) and arm and wrist supports. Matching task is displayed oncomputer monitor.

    Fig. 2

    Start

    Stop

    Target line

    Visual cue lines

    Analyzed data

    Initial cursor position

    Force-matching task. The participants dot cursor is red until his or hercombined thumb and finger force reaches his or her 5% maximumvoluntary isometric contraction (MVIC) force level, which is the start pointon the horizontal line. Time, not the participant, moves the cursor acrossthe target line. The participant simply attempts to match a constant 5%MVIC force level denoted by the target line for the time configured foreach participant, which is a time relative to his or her own MVIC.

    162 International Journal of Rehabilitation Research 2014, Vol 37 No 2

    Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/10/2019 Maximum Voluntary Isometric Pinch Contraction and.10

    5/8

    Levenes test for homogeneity of variance was applied

    to determine how homogeneous each decades CV andRMSEwereCV (P< 0.009) andRMSE(P< 0.002). SDs

    within each decade group tended to increase as thedecades increased with the greatest diversity (SD) being

    in the 7079-year group (Table 2).

    Regression

    A regression analysis was performed to determine the

    impact of MVIC, decade, and gender on force-variability

    characteristics (CV and RMSE). The model revealed a

    significant overall effect [F(3,99) = 11.296, P< 0.001,R = 0.511] for CV and [F(3,99) = 12.631, P< 0.001,R = 0.532] forRMSE.

    MVIC, decade, and gender were all strong predictors ofCV (P< 0.05, b = 0.292; P< 0.01, b = 0.422; andP< 0.001, b = 0.317, respectively). A 4.7% of variance

    in CV was uniquely accounted for by MVIC (partcorrelation = 0.217), 17% by decade (part correlation =

    0.413), and 5.7% by gender (part correlation = 0.239).

    Decade was a strong predictor (P< 0.001,b = 0.391) of

    RMSE, whereas gender was a moderate predictor(P< 0.05, b = 0.229). Fifteen percent of RMSE var-

    iance was uniquely accounted for by decade (partcorrelation = 0.383) and 3.0% by gender (part correla-

    tion = 0.172). MVIC was not a significant predictor ofRMSE [Part correlation (otherwise known as semipartial

    correlations), as opposed to partial correlation, takes intoaccount other variables contributions and eliminates those

    contributions from the influence of the variable of interest.].

    DiscussionMaximum voluntary isometric pinch force

    The results suggest that fine motor pinch strengthdeclines with age. Declines were slightly less than

    previously reported for gross motor strength after 5070years, 15% per decade (Grabiner and Enoka, 1995) or

    11.5% per year (Spirduso et al., 2005b) with a 7%

    decrease from the 50s to the 60s and 13% from the 60s tothe 70s. Furthermore, the strength differences between

    the 70s and the 30s were similar (19% decrease; women

    27%, men 13%) to losses previously recorded in various

    fine motor strength studies involving older adults ranging

    from 60 to 75 years and younger adults from 20 to 35 years

    old, with 26% (Ranganathan et al., 2001), 30% (Oliveira

    et al., 2008), 26.6% (De Serres and Fang, 2004), and 14%

    (Sperling, 1980) strength losses.

    Pinch-force strength did not decline linearly across the

    decade groups, as has been seen with isometric grip

    strength, which involves multiple larger muscle

    masses. Rantanen et al. (1998) reported a clear lineardecline significantly different in every age group in a

    sample of 3680 men ranging in age from 45 to 92 years in

    grip strength, a manual isometric test requiring full force

    from both intrinsic and extrinsic muscles controlling the

    thumb and digits. Puh (2010) also found significant

    differences in isometric grip and three different pinch

    forces in four different age categories ranging from 2034

    to 6579 years; however, these were not linear. Puhs

    second age group (3549 years) and this studys second

    age group (4049 years) showed higher strength levels

    than the respective younger age groups.

    While our 4069-year-olds were statistically stronger thanthe 70-year-olds, 30-year-olds had higher MVIC values

    Fig. 3

    MVIC

    MVIC(

    N)

    120

    100

    80

    60

    40

    20

    030s 40s 50s 60s 70s

    Men Women

    Maximum voluntary isometric contraction (MVIC) force across decades.The 7079-year-olds were weaker than people in their 60s, 50s, and40s (P< 0.01), but, interestingly, not weaker than the 30s. Values are

    the decade group meansSD.

    Table 1 Maximal voluntary isometric contraction by decade and gender

    MSD

    Fourth decade(3039 years)

    Fifth decade(4049 years)

    Sixth decade(5059 years)

    Seventh decade(6069 years)

    Eighth decade(7079 years)

    Average acrossdecades

    MVIC force (N) 63.214 66.019 69.516 67.016 54.016 64.317Men MVIC (N) 73.507 78.317 78.815 80.110 63.313 74.814Women MVIC (N) 52.811 55.012 59.210 53.908 44.713 53.112

    MVIC, maximum voluntary isometric contraction.

    MVIC pinch and force-matching Herring-Marleret al. 163

    Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/10/2019 Maximum Voluntary Isometric Pinch Contraction and.10

    6/8

    than those in their 70s, but not so much so that it

    resulted in significant differences. Upon closer examina-

    tion, the 30-year-olds contained a wide distribution of

    MVIC values with two substantially low MVICs, which

    contributed toward the insignificant differences (P=

    0.09) between the 30- and 70-year-olds.

    Gender effects on maximum voluntary isometric contraction

    The significant gender main effect indicated that

    womens pinch-force averages were lower than mensthroughout the decades. Averaged over five decades, men

    were 29% stronger than women. An analysis of within-gender age differences revealed that men in their 70s

    were able to produce 87% of their MVIC compared with

    men in their 30s, whereas women in their 70s produced

    only 75% compared with women in their 30s.

    The decade gender interaction was not significant. Theobservation of lower MVIC for women is consistent

    with previous findings. This could be explained by the

    findings that women have less muscle mass than men

    (Puh, 2010), demonstrate smaller twitch forces (Doherty

    and Brown, 1997), have a longer half-relaxation time, and

    have a higher proportion of type I fibers (Hunter, 2009).

    Force variability and accuracy

    The ability of participants to sustain a steady force at a

    target level was compromised with aging. Adults in their

    70s exhibited higher force variability than those in all

    other decades. The results were similar for accuracy;

    however, the difference in the 60s and 70s approached

    significance (P= 0.06). Group mean step differences

    between decades for force variability (CV) were as

    follows: fifthfourth decade = 0.000, sixthfifth decade =

    0.001, seventhsixth decade= 0.001, and eighth

    seventh decade = 0.008 (Fig. 4a). Group mean step

    differences in force error (RMSE) were 0.006, 0.005,0.005, and 0.019 N, respectively (Fig. 4b). Therefore, the

    age differences for force-matching were also nonlinear.

    Accuracy, but not force variability, was weakly and

    positively correlated with fine motor strength, which

    suggests that the stronger the participant was the less

    accurate his or her force-matching performance was (as

    measured by RMSE). CV and RMSE were positively

    correlated with each other. Finally, the older the age

    group was the greater were the differences in MVIC,

    accuracy, and force variability among members of that

    decade.

    Fig. 4

    0.04

    0.035

    0.16

    0.14

    0.12

    0.1

    0.08

    (a) (b)

    0.06

    0.04

    0.02

    0

    CV

    CV

    RMSE

    RMSE

    0.03

    0.02

    0.015

    0.01

    0.005

    030s 40s 50s 60s 70s 30s 40s 50s 60s 70s

    0.025

    Men Women

    Force-matching. (a) Force variability [coefficient of variability (CV)] across decades, (b) accuracy [root mean square error (RMSE)] across decades.Arrows, significant difference; double hash intercepts, near differences. The 7079-year-olds were significantly different from the 30s, 40s, 50s, and60s (P< 0.001) for CV but the 70s were not different from the 50s (P= 0.06) or 60s (P= 0.32) for RMSE. Values are decade group meansSD.

    Table 2 Force variability (coefficient of variation) and accuracy (root mean square error) by decade and gender

    MSD

    Fourth decade(3039 years)

    Fifth decade(4049 years)

    Sixth decade(5059 years)

    Seventh decade(6069 years)

    Eighth decade(7079 years)

    Average acrossdecades

    Force variability (CV) 0.0130.004 0.0130.005 0.0140.005 0.0150.006 0.0230.010 0.0160.006Men (CV) 0.0130.004 0.0120.003 0.0150.007 0.0160.007 0.0250.007 0.0160.006Women (CV) 0.0120.004 0.0130.006 0.0120.003 0.0140.006 0.0230.012 0.0130.005Accuracy (RMSE) 0.0480.015 0.0540.025 0.0590.034 0.0650.028 0.0840.042 0.0620.032Men (RMSE) 0.0550.015 0.0560.017 0.0740.042 0.0750.022 0.1070.042 0.0730.034Women (RMSE) 0.0410.012 0.0520.032 0.0450.015 0.0540.030 0.0600.028 0.0500.024

    CV, coefficient of variability; RMSE, root mean square error.

    164 International Journal of Rehabilitation Research 2014, Vol 37 No 2

    Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/10/2019 Maximum Voluntary Isometric Pinch Contraction and.10

    7/8

    Several factors have been proposed as explanations for

    greater force variability and lower accuracy in the forcecontrol of older adults: lower levels of maximum strength,

    enhanced neural noise, and tactile and proprioceptiveeffects.

    Low force levelsIt is well documented that controlling very low levels (5%

    MVIC) of force is more difficult than controlling higher

    levels (Slifkin and Newell, 1999; Varadhan et al., 2010).

    Also, in older participants who have significantly lower

    MVICs, the requirement of extremely low force levels

    (5% MVIC) could account for the greater difficulty that

    older adults have in the force-matching task. Producing

    5% of a very low MVIC could possibly be more

    challenging than producing 5% of a moderate MVIC

    (Sosnoff and Newell, 2006).

    Enhanced neural noise

    Neuronal noise is a term commonly used to describe the

    continuous and frequent random synaptic inputs within

    complex neural networks that affect individual neurons,

    creating a noisy synaptic input state for any given

    neuron. The noisier the neural state is the higher the

    signal (motor command) has to be to be detected.

    Neuronal and synaptic noise has been shown to increase

    with age, resulting in lower signal-to-noise ratios, which

    in turn results in older adults greater error fluctuations

    during voluntary force production (Jones et al.,

    2002; Smits-Engelsman et al., 2003; Christou and Tracy,

    2006;John et al., 2009).Pascoet al. (2011)suggested that

    age-related changes in the integration of synaptic inputreduce the ability of older adults to modulate discharge

    characteristics of motor units at low sustained isometric

    force levels (5% MVC) of the biceps brachii.

    Tactile and proprioceptive mechanisms

    A third factor that may explain age differences in

    variability and accuracy is the contribution of tactile and

    proprioceptive mechanisms. Neurologists and physical

    therapists often use weighted spoons, wrist or ankle cuffs,or vests to assist their patients in producing steadier and

    smoother movements and hence prevent food and drink

    spills. Weighting enhances sensory information fromreceptors for increased proprioception (muscle spindles

    and Golgi tendon organs), thus assisting patients by

    providing more feedback about their location in space

    and allowing them to better feel the force they are

    producing. In addition, pressure (force) is sensed through

    pacinian corpuscles, which enhances the development of

    motor memory for the task at hand (Fix, 2002). A similar

    phenomenon could be occurring with the force-matching

    task. Researchers suggest that 5% MVIC of a weaker

    person could result in a more unloaded condition,

    feasibly offering significantly reduced initial sensory feed-

    forward proprioception information as well as providing

    less proprioception and kinesthesia for the participant

    (Robles-De-La-Torre and Hayward, 2001).

    Gender effects on force variability and accuracy

    Gender differences were not significant in force variability

    (CV), but they were significant in accuracy as measured by

    RMSE. Just as Shinohara et al. (2003b)found men to be

    more variable, this study showed similar results: men

    produced greater error, RMSE = 0.073 N, than women,RMSE = 0.050 N. The differences in mens and womens

    body sizes have been attributed to more effective gross

    motor performance in men (Thomaset al., 1991), but fine

    motor tasks require different sensorimotor attributes than

    strength and power. Women have been shown to perform

    better in tasks requiring sensory discrimination (Noble,

    1978). Women also have a greater number of type 1 fibers;

    therefore, they would activate more motor units at lower

    force levels than men and, hence, would be better able to

    modify fine motor forces.

    Conclusion

    As expected, 7079-year-olds were weaker than 4069-

    year-olds, and women were weaker than men. Unique

    findings of this study were that (a) no differences were

    found in strength, steadiness, or force variability among

    the decades, up to the seventh decade (r 69 years of

    age); (b) 7079-year-olds were weaker, less steady, and

    less accurate in force-matching than their younger

    counterparts; (c) although Hackel (1992) found women

    more variable and Shim et al. (2004) reported no gender

    difference, the results of this study were in agreement

    with those ofShinoharaet al.(2003b)that men performedlow-level force-matching with greater error than women;

    (d) strength was not correlated with steadiness but was

    weakly correlated with accuracy, and steadiness and

    accuracy were strongly correlated; and (e) decade and

    gender were moderate and strong predictors of accuracy

    and steadiness, respectively.

    Clinical applications

    Fine motor strength declines are significant and can cause

    severe limitations in daily activities as adults age. Society

    has been educated and trained to address age-related gross-muscle motor losses with research, rehabilitation, and

    training, but it is clear that fine motor deficits can beseriously debilitating as well and should receive more

    attention. The results suggest that additional research

    should address the benefits of training programs designed

    to increase finger strength and dexterity. Accordingly, these

    programs should be initiated no later than a persons sixth

    decade (age 5059 years), especially in women.

    The increase in SD in MVIC, force variability, and accuracy

    within decade groups as age progresses suggest that as

    people age they become more diversified within their age

    groups. This implies that some people retain function as

    they grow old, and some do not. An important goal of aging

    MVIC pinch and force-matching Herring-Marleret al. 165

    Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/10/2019 Maximum Voluntary Isometric Pinch Contraction and.10

    8/8

    research is to determine how to increase the number of

    individuals who maintain function in their later years.

    Limitations

    A potential limitation for this study is a possible order

    effect of task administration that could have produced

    cognitive and physical fatigue. Force-matching was the

    last of three tasks the participants performed within thetest session and was the easiest, but all tasks required

    high levels of attention and information processing. Each

    test session was B3045 min long and could have

    produced some cognitive fatigue. However, given that

    the matching task included only 10 trials and required

    only 5% MVIC and that the overall duration over which

    the slowest participants were actively contracting isome-

    trically during the entire protocol was no more than

    B4 min, it is unlikely that physical fatigue, even in the

    older groups, compromised the results.

    AcknowledgementsThe authors acknowledge and express extended appre-

    ciation for the excellent help of Diana Hunter for

    consultation on design and written representation,

    Michael Mahometa from the Division of Statistics and

    Scientific Computation at The University of Texas at

    Austin for statistical consultation, and Tess Roach for

    editing services.

    Conflicts of interest

    There are no conflicts of interest.

    ReferencesChristou EA, Tracy BL (2006). Aging and variability in motor output. In: Davids K,

    Bennett S, Newell K, editors. Movement system variability. Champaign, IL:Human Kinetics; pp. 199215.

    Cohen J (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    De Serres SJ, Fang NZ (2004). The accuracy of perception of a pinch grip forcein older adults.Can J Physiol Pharmocol82:693701.

    Doherty TJ, Brown WF (1997). Age-related changes in the twitch contractileproperties of human thenar motor units. J Appl Physicol82:93101.

    Fix JD (2002). Neuroanatomy. Hagerstown, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.Francis KL, Spirduso WW (2000). Age differences in the expression of manual

    asymmetry.Exp Aging Res26:169180.Galganski ME, Fuglevand AJ, Enoka RM (1993). Reduced control of motor

    output in a human hand muscle of elderly subjects during submaximalcontractions. J Neurophysiol69:21082115.

    Grabiner MD, Enoka RM (1995). Changes in movement capabilities with aging.Exerc Sports Sci Rev23:65104.Hackel ME, Wolfe GA, Bang SM, Canfield JS (1992). Changes in hand function

    in the aging adult as determined by the Jebsen Test of Hand Function. PhysTher72:373377.

    Hunter SK (2009). Sex differences and mechanisms of task-specific musclefatigue. Exerc Sport Sci Rev37:113122.

    John EB, Liu W, Gregory RW (2009). Biomechanics of muscular effort:age-related changes. Med Sci Sports Exerc41:418425.

    Jones KE, Hamilton AF, Wolpert DM (2002). Sources of signal-dependent noiseduring isometric force production. J Neurophysiol88:15331544.

    Laidlaw DH, Bilodeau M, Enoka RM (2000). Steadiness is reduced and motorunit discharge is more variable in old adults. Muscle Nerve23:600612.

    Noble CE (1978). Age, race, and sex in learning and performance of psychomotorskills. In: Osborne RT, Noble CE, Weyl N, editors. Human variation: the bio-psychology of age, race and sex. New York: Academy Press; pp. 287378.

    Olafsdottir HB, Kim SW, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2008). Anticipatory synergyadjustments in preparation to self-triggered perturbations in elderly indivi-duals. J Appl Biomech 24:175179.

    Oliveira MA, Hsu J, Park J, Clark JE, Kun Shim JK (2008). Age-related changes inmulti-finger interactions in adults during maximum voluntary finger forceproduction tasks. Hum Mov Sci27:714727.

    Pasco MA, Holmes MR, Enoka RM (2011). Discharge characteristics of bicepsbrachii motor units at recruitment when older adults sustained an isometriccontraction. J Neurophysiol105 :571581.

    Peolsson A, Hedlund R, Oberg B (2001). Intra- and inter-tester reliability andreference values for hand strength. J Rehabil Med33:3641.

    Puh U (2010). Age-related and sex-related differences in hand and pinch gripstrength in adults. Int J Rehabil Res 33:411.

    Ranganathan VK, Siemionow V, Sahgal V, Yue GH (2001). Effects of aging onhand function. J Am Geriatr Soc49:14781484.

    Rantanen T, Masaki K, Foley D, Izmirlian G, White L, Guralnik JM (1998). Gripstrength changes over 27 yr in Japanese-American men. J Appl Physiol

    85:20472053.Robles-De-La-Torre G, Hayward V (2001). Force can overcome object geometry

    in the perception of shape through active touch.Nature412 :445448.Shiffler RE (1988). Maximum z scores and outliers. Am Stat42:7980.Shim JK, Lay BS, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2004). Age-related changes in

    finger coordination in static prehension tests. J Appl Physiol97:213224.Shinohara M, Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (2003a). Age effects on force produced

    by intrinsic and extrinsic hand muscles and finger interaction duringMVC tasks. J Appl Physiol95 :13611369.

    Shinohara M, Li S, Kang N, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2003b). Effects of age andgender on finger coordination in MVC and submaximal force-matching tasks.J Appl Physiol94 :259270.

    Slifkin AB, Newell KM (1999). Noise, information transmission, and forcevariability.J Exp Psych Hum Percept Perform 25:837851.

    Smits-Engelsman BC, Gielen SC, Duysens J (2003). Effects of aging on motoroutput variability in uni- and bi-manual isometric tasks. 33rd Annual Meetingof the Society of Neuroscience; December 2003; New Orleans, LA;pp. 392393.

    Sosnoff JJ, Newell KM (2006). Visual intermittency and variability in isometricforce output. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci61:117124.

    Sperling L (1980). Evaluation of upper extremity function in 70 year old menand women. Scand J Rehabil Med12:139144.

    Spirduso WW, Francis K, Eakin T, Stanford C (2005a). Quantification of manualforce control and tremor. J Mot Behav37:197210.

    Spirduso WW, Francis K, MacRae P (2005b). Physical dimensions of aging.2nd ed. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Thomas JR, Nelson JK, Church G (1991). A developmental analysis of genderdifferences in health related physical fitness. Pediatr Exerc Sci3:2842.

    Tracy BL, Enoka RM (2002). Older adults are less steady during submaximalisometric contractions with knee extensor muscles. J Appl Physiol92:10041012.

    Vaillancourt DE, Larsson L, Newell KM (2003). Effects of aging on forcevariability, single motor unit discharge patterns, and the structure of 10, 20,

    and 40 Hz EMG activity. Neurobiol Aging 24:2535.Varadhan SK, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2010). Variance components in discreteforce production tasks. Exp Brain Res 205 :335349.

    Visser B, de Looze MP, Veeger DH, Douwes M, Groenesteijn L, de Korte E,van Dieen JH (2003). The effects of precision demands during a low intensitypinching task on muscle activation and load sharing of the fingers.J Electromyogr Kinesiol13:149157.

    166 International Journal of Rehabilitation Research 2014, Vol 37 No 2

    Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.