lskjgks

Upload: josh6692

Post on 14-Feb-2018

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/23/2019 lskjgks

    1/6

    Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2005, 46, 361366

    2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 9600 GarsingtonRoad, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. ISSN 0036-5564.

    BlackwellPublishing,Ltd.

    Personality and Social Sciences

    The association between implicit and explicit prejudice: the moderating

    role of motivation to control prejudiced reactions

    NAZAR AKRAMI and BO EKEHAMMAR

    Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden

    Akrami, N. & Ekehammar, B. (2005). The association between implicit and explicit prejudice: the moderating role of motivation to control

    prejudiced reactions. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology

    , 46

    , 361366.

    The main aim of the present research was to assess the relationship between implicit and explicit ethnic attitude measures and to examine the

    impact of motivation to control prejudiced reactions on this relation. Implicit ethnic prejudice was assessed by a response latency measure,

    and a self-report modern prejudice scale was used to assess explicit prejudice. The results showed that an association between implicit and

    explicit attitudes was observed only when the explicit attitude measure was corrected for motivational bias. The findings are discussed in relation

    to previous research reporting either association or dissociation between implicit and explicit attitude measures.

    Key words:

    Implicit prejudice, explicit prejudice, prejudice control.

    Nazar Akrami, Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Box 1225, SE-751 42 Uppsala, Sweden. Tel: +46 18 471 15 25; fax: +46 18 471

    21 23

    ; e-mail: [email protected]

    INTRODUCTION

    Explicit and implicit ethnic attitudes

    Greenwald and Banaji (1995) have emphasized the importance

    of the distinction between explicit and implicit measures of

    attitudes. Explicit attitudes are slow and intentional and

    operate in a conscious mode. They can be measured by tra-

    ditional self-report questionnaires. Implicit attitudes, in con-

    trast, are fast and automatic and operate without intention,

    often in an unconscious mode. They are assessed using indirect

    measures, like response latency.

    Important in the present context is the relationship

    between implicit and explicit attitudes. Implicit and explicit

    attitudes can be expected to be associated because of their

    common root in peoples personal and cultural experiences

    and socialization history (e.g., Dovidio, Kawakami, Johnson,

    Johnson & Howard, 1997) or because implicit attitudes

    are in fact internalized explicit attitudes (e.g., Brauer, Wasel

    & Niedenthal, 2000). However, Devine (1989) argued that

    because stereotypes, unlike personal beliefs, are culturally

    shared, they can be automatically activated. Thus, a person

    might hold a positive attitude toward a social group and at

    the same time automatically activate negative stereotypes

    on the mere presence of a stereotyped group member.

    Consequently, implicit measures (like response latencies) and

    explicit measures (like scores on self-report scales) of ethnic

    prejudice might be dissociated. Given this state of affairs, to

    what extent are implicit attitudes reflected in explicit attitude

    measures? This issue is addressed in the present paper.

    Since the 1970s, social psychologists have questioned the

    traditional paper-and-pencil questionnaires ability to accurately

    reflect prejudicial attitudes that people may harbor against

    minority groups (e.g., McConahay, Hardee & Batts, 1981).

    This suspicion was motivated because of the reactivity biases

    due to, for example, social desirability. Crosby, Bromley, and

    Saxe (1980), for example, concluded that anti-Black senti-

    ments are much more prevalent among White Americans

    than the survey data lead one to expect (p. 546). Further, it

    has been argued that the prevailing sociopolitical climate

    (Katz & Hass, 1988) and peoples tendency to present them-

    selves as non-prejudiced and socially or politically correct

    may prevent the expression of racial/ethnic prejudice

    openly (Crosby et al.

    , 1980; see also Akrami, Ekehammar &

    Araya, 2000; Franco & Maass, 1999; Pettigrew & Meertens,

    1995). Consequently, two types of scales measuring racial/

    ethnic prejudice have been suggested; one (old-fashioned)

    tapping direct or open, and another (modern) tapping

    covert or subtle racial/ethnic prejudice (e.g., McConahay

    1986). One example is the old-fashioned and the modern

    racism (the Modern Racism Scale) measures suggested by

    McConahay (1986). More recently, however, some researchers

    have suggested that, like traditional (i.e., old-fashioned)

    self-report measures, the modern measures might be equally

    reactive and susceptible to social desirability and self-

    presentational concerns (cf. Brauer, Wasel & Niedenthal, 2000;

    Dovidio et al

    ., 1997; Fazio, Jackson, Dunton & Williams,

    1995).

    Important in the present context is the study of Fazio et al.

    (1995) where a dissociation was found between a measure of

    explicit prejudice (the Modern Racism Scale) and a measure

    of implicit prejudice based on response latency. However,

    Fazio and his colleagues found an interaction between the

  • 7/23/2019 lskjgks

    2/6

    362

    N. Akrami and B. Ekehammar

    Scand J Psychol 46 (2005)

    2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

    participants Modern Racism Scale scores and their motivation

    to control prejudiced reactions. On this basis, they argued

    that the resulting dissociation was due to the reactivity of

    the Modern Racism Scale. Similarly, Dovidio et al

    . (1997),

    employing a response-latency-based measure of implicit atti-

    tudes, found a significant relationship between this measureand the Modern Racism Scale scores in one of their three

    experiments. Like Fazio et al

    . (1995), Dovidio et al

    . (1997)

    questioned the non-reactivity of the Modern Racism

    Scale. Regardless of the method employed for measuring

    implicit prejudice, researchers seem to agree that the lack

    of association between implicit and explicit prejudice might

    be due to the bias in the explicit measures (cf. Brauer et al.

    ,

    2000).

    Assessing implicit ethnic attitudes

    A widely used method for assessing implicit attitudes, besides

    the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee& Schwartz, 1998), is the adjective evaluation task (Fazio,

    Sanbonmatsu, Powell & Kardes, 1986). Like the IAT, a typ-

    ical feature of this response-latency-based technique is that

    participants are first exposed to a prime (e.g., a facial photograph

    of a European-American or an African-American) and then

    a target word (e.g., either a positive or a negative adjective).

    Participants are asked to indicate the valence (positive/

    negative) of the presented target word. The response latency

    is then measured. The associated evaluation of the prime in

    the participants memory is assumed to facilitate or inhibit

    participants response.

    The adjective evaluation technique is a sensitive measure

    of implicit attitudes and it has been shown to provide anaccurate test of the automaticity of evaluation (Fazio et al

    .,

    1995). Therefore, in the present study, we employ this tech-

    nique to measure implicit ethnic prejudice, and following

    Fazio et al

    . (1995) and Ekehammar, Akrami and Araya

    (2003), we use photographs to automatically activate ethnic

    stereotypes.

    Assessing explicit ethnic attitudes

    As mentioned above, explicit attitudes are measured by

    self-report instruments. One such instrument for measuring

    racial/ethnic beliefs is McConahay et al

    .s (1981) Modern

    Racism Scale, which is probably the most frequently used

    instrument for assessing racial/ethnic beliefs, mainly toward

    African-Americans. Since its introduction, though, the scale

    has received much criticism, for example for its reactivity

    (e.g., Fazio et al

    ., 1995). However, there have been attempts

    to develop valid measures of racial/ethnic beliefs, not only

    toward a specific minority group (African-Americans), but

    toward minority groups in general (e.g., immigrants). One

    such scale is the Modern Racial Prejudice Scale (Akrami

    et al

    ., 2000), developed for a European context, which we

    use in the present study.

    Motivation to control prejudiced reactions

    Due to the nature of prejudiced beliefs, scales that measure

    these beliefs can be influenced by different factors, for example,

    peoples motivation to control prejudiced reactions (e.g.,

    Dunton & Fazio, 1997; Plant & Devine, 1998). Plant andDevine (1998), for example, found that the expressed pre-

    judiced beliefs varied with peoples motivation to control their

    prejudice and whether these beliefs were expressed in private

    or public. Further, Dunton and Fazio (1997) found that the

    expression of racial/ethnic prejudice on self-report measures

    was moderated by the extent to which participants were

    motivated to inhibit their prejudiced responses. More speci-

    fically, Dunton and Fazio (1997; Fazio et al

    ., 1995) found

    that individuals with higher motivation to control their

    prejudice scored lower on the explicit prejudice measure,

    although their implicit attitude scores were high. However,

    for the low-motivated group the implicit and explicit attitude

    measures were congruent. Emphasizing the importanceof the impact of motivational factors on the measurement

    of explicit prejudice, we suggest that these factors should be

    further investigated.

    Aim and hypotheses

    The primary aim of the present research was to examine

    whether implicit and explicit attitudes are associated or not.

    To this end, we employ the adjective evaluation task as an

    implicit attitude measure, and the Modern Racial Prejudice

    Scale (Akrami et al

    ., 2000) as an explicit attitude measure.

    Moreover, we analyze the impact of motivation to control

    ones prejudiced reactions on the explicit attitude measure.We hypothesized that a negative correlation between moti-

    vation to control ones prejudice and scores on the Modern

    Racial Prejudice Scale would indicate the reactivity of the

    scale.

    More important, we examined what happens if the scores

    on Modern Racial Prejudice Scale are corrected for motiva-

    tional biases. We argue that an association between explicit

    and implicit ethnic attitude measures might be revealed if

    the scores on the Modern Racial Prejudice Scale are cor-

    rected for the motivation to control ones prejudice. Specific-

    ally, we hypothesized that, if the scores on Modern Racial

    Prejudice Scale are negatively correlated with motivation to

    control ones prejudice, no association between explicit and

    implicit racial/ethnic attitudes will be found. However, if the

    explicit attitude scores are corrected for the motivation to

    control prejudiced reactions, an association between explicit

    and implicit racial/ethnic attitudes will be found.

    METHODS

    Participants

    The participants were 42 Swedish students (two of which had

    non-Swedish origin), 19 non-psychology university students and

  • 7/23/2019 lskjgks

    3/6

    Scand J Psychol 46 (2005)

    Implicit and explicit prejudice

    363

    2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

    23 students at the local authority-administered adult education, a

    highly heterogeneous group with various backgrounds who can be

    expected to vary on ethnic prejudice, among other things. There were

    20 women and 22 men between 20 and 43 years (

    M

    = 26.8 years) of

    age. The university students took part in the experiment for course

    credit whereas the other participants received cinema vouchers

    (equivalent to &

    7).

    Stimulus materials

    The primes consisted of 132 color photographs of faces (head shots,

    6

    5 cm) taken from the Internet. Sixty-six of these represented faces

    (33 men and 33 women) that had foreign-looking features (i.e., dark

    hair, dark eyes, and sometimes dark skin) and 66 had a paler and

    blonder appearance and represented Swedish faces (33 men and 33

    women). The immigrant photographs were chosen based on a previ-

    ous study of which particular national categories people associate

    with the word immigrant in Sweden (Akrami, Ekehammar & Araya,

    2003). The photographs represented the most frequent national

    categories obtained in that study (e.g., Asian, African, Middle East,

    South American, and Mediterranean). All photographs were modified

    to have a white background and identical size.

    Apparatus

    The experiment was conducted using an AST Premmia GX P/133

    computer with a NEC MultiSync XP 21 monitor run with a resolu-

    tion of 1024

    768 and 75 Hz refresh rate. The computer monitor

    was controlled by the Authorware 3.1 program.

    Procedure

    On arrival, participants were greeted by the experimenter and were

    told that the main purpose of the experiments was to examine vari-

    ous memory and language phenomena. The experimental procedure

    was adopted from Fazio et al

    . (1995) and consisted of six phases,described below, where the fourth phase involved the actual priming

    task.

    The first phase

    was run to obtain baseline data and involved the

    presentation of words on the computer monitor. Participants were

    led to believe that this phase involved a language-testing task

    where the speed for assigning word-valence was measured. The par-

    ticipant had to press a key labeled negative

    orpositive

    as quickly as

    possible to indicate her/ his response to the valence of the word. The

    list of words comprised 24 adjectives; 12 negative (

    aggressive, trouble-

    some, stupid, deceitful, lazy, impolite, passive, careless, disturbed, gloomy,

    boring, disgusting

    ) and 12 positive (

    pleasant, decent, intelligent, loyal,

    reliable, tidy, smart, kind, sound, nice, handsome, honest

    ). Participants

    were instructed to respond as fast and accurately as possible. Each

    adjective was presented twice and remained on the center of the

    screen until the participant responded. The presentation order of theadjectives was randomized for each participant. A 2.5 s interval

    separated each trial. Response latency for each trial (from adjective

    onset to response) was recorded to the nearest millisecond.

    The second phase

    (face-memory-testing) ostensibly involved the

    ability to memorize faces for later recognition. The participants task

    was simply to attend to the faces presented on the computer screen.

    They were told that they would be given a recognition task involving

    these faces subsequently. The stimuli were 16 immigrant and Swedish

    facial photographs (four male and four female faces from each

    group). Each photograph was presented twice, and remained on the

    screen for 5 s. A 2.5 s interval separated each presentation. The

    presentation order of the photographs was randomized for each

    participant.

    The third phase

    involved a recognition test of the previously pre-

    sented faces. The photographs from the previous phase and 16 new

    filler photographs were presented on the computer monitor. Particip-

    ants were told to press the key labeled yes

    if the face had appeared,

    and no

    if the face had not appeared in the previous phase. Each face

    remained on the screen for 5 s and a 2.5-s interval separated each

    trial. The participants responses were recorded.

    The fourth phase

    involved the actual priming task. Participants were

    led to believe that this phase involved a combination of a language

    and a face-memory task. Twenty-four Swedish (12 men and

    12 women) and 24 immigrant (12 men and 12 women) facial photo-

    graphs (not used in the previous phases) served as primes. The 24

    adjectives from the first phase were used as target adjectives. On any

    given trial, a prime photograph was presented for 315 ms, followed

    by a 135-ms interval before the onset of the target adjective. A 2.5-s

    interval separated each trial. Four blocks of trials were presented.

    Each block consisted of 48 trials in which the primes appeared once,

    followed by one of the adjectives. Over the course of the four blocks,

    each prime was paired, randomly, with two positive and two negative

    adjectives. The instruction and procedures were identical, with one

    exception, to those of the first phase. Participants were told that it

    was important to attend to the presented faces because they would

    be asked to recognize these faces later. The participants responsesregarding the valence of the adjective, and the response latencies

    were recorded to the nearest millisecond.

    The fifth phase

    involved a recognition test of the faces presented in

    the previous phase. Those photographs and 48 new filler photo-

    graphs were presented on the computer monitor. Participants were

    told to press the key labeled yes

    if the face had appeared, and no

    if

    the face had not appeared in the previous phase. Each photograph

    remained on the screen for 5 s. A 2.5-s interval separated each trial.

    The sixth and final phase

    involved attractiveness ratings of the

    facial photographs presented in the fourth phase. Participants were

    told that we were interested in assessing the extent to which the

    attractiveness of a face had an effect on their performance on previous

    tasks (in the fourth phase). They were instructed to press one of the ten

    keys labeled 1 (

    not attractive

    ) to 10 (

    very attractive

    ) to indicate their

    rating of the attractiveness of each face. Each photograph remainedon the screen until the participant had made her or his response.

    After completing this task, participants were, seemingly, thanked

    and dismissed. Before leaving the experiment room, however, parti-

    cipants were asked if they had a few minutes time to spare to help a

    research colleague from another university. They were told that a

    colleague was doing research in social psychology and was currently

    collecting material for his research. The experimenter informed them

    that we had promised to help him in collecting some of the data in

    our laboratory. Participants were then guided to another room and

    asked to complete a booklet containing the Modern Racial Prejudice

    Scale, and a Swedish version of the Motivation to Control Prejudiced

    Reactions Scale.

    The Modern Racial Prejudice Scale was constructed by Akrami

    et al

    . (2000) for measuring modern (covert, subtle, symbolic) racial/

    ethnic prejudice in a Scandinavian context following the item con-tents of McConahays (1986) Modern Racism Scale. Reliability and

    validity data for the scale are provided by Akrami et al.

    (2000). The

    scale contains 9 items (example: Discrimination against immigrants is

    no longer a problem in Sweden

    ). The scale had an internal consistency

    reliability (Cronbachs alpha) of 0.78 in the present sample. The

    Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions Scale was adapted to a

    Swedish context from Dunton and Fazio (1997) and consisted of

    7 items. The scale had a reliability of 0.70 in the present sample and

    includes such items as In todays society its important that one not be

    perceived as prejudiced in any manner

    and If I have a prejudice thought

    or feeling, I keep it to myself

    . The answers on the two scales were

    indicated on a 5-step scale ranging from Do not agree at all

    (1) to

    Agree fully

    (5). In order not to arouse participants suspicion about

  • 7/23/2019 lskjgks

    4/6

    364

    N. Akrami and B. Ekehammar

    Scand J Psychol 46 (2005)

    2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

    the main purpose of the study the scale items were embedded in

    items concerning general subjects (e.g., The social insurance system

    should be further developed

    .).

    Participants were given a pre-paid envelope addressed to our alleged

    colleague at another university. The experimenter informed parti-

    cipants to put it in a mailbox after completion and left the room.

    However, on their way to the mailbox the experimenter informed the

    participants that they could hand him the envelope and he could

    himself put it in the mailbox.

    None of the participants reported any suspicion about any part of

    the experiment, or noted any relation between the experiment and

    the booklet containing the Modern Racial Prejudice Scale and the

    Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions Scale. Participants were

    debriefed, thanked, and dismissed.

    Calculation of response latencies

    For each participant, baseline latency for each adjective was calcul-

    ated from the average of the two presentations of the adjective in the

    first phase. The response latencies for each target adjective (presented

    after the prime) were subtracted from the baseline. For every prime

    (face photograph), there were two negative and two positive targetadjectives. We calculated the average response latency for the positive

    and the negative adjectives. Further, for each participant, average

    positive and negative response latencies for Swedish and immigrant

    primes were then calculated.

    If a prime has a negative implicit association, its presentation

    should inhibit (slow) responses to positive adjectives and facilitate

    (speed up) the responses to the negative adjectives (Fazio et al.

    , 1986,

    1995). Thus, a high response latency (slower response) on negative

    adjectives would indicate a positive attitude. That is, the participant

    has a weak association between the specific prime and the negative

    adjective. A high response latency (slower response) on positive

    adjectives, on the other hand, indicates a negative attitude. That is,

    the participant has a weak association between the specific prime

    and the positive adjective.

    To simplify the analyses, the positive and negative response laten-

    cies were combined to a single negativity index for each prime group

    (immigrant and Swedish). The index was calculated by (a) reversing

    the negative response latencies, and (b) taking the sum of negative

    and positive response latencies for every participant. Finally, we

    created a single measure of implicit negativity by subtracting the

    negative implicit attitude scores to Swedish primes from the negative

    implicit attitudes scores to immigrant primes. We will refer to this

    measure as Implicit Ethnic Prejudice, where higher scores indicate

    higher negativity toward immigrants.

    Response errors that involved responding positive when a negat-

    ive target word was presented, and negative when a positive target

    word was presented, were disregarded. There were 33 response errors

    to the negative and 21 to the positive adjective targets.

    To reduce the skewness associated with response latency data, the

    response latencies are usually subjected to a logarithmic transforma-

    tion (e.g., Blair & Banaji, 1996). In the present study, there were nodifferences between the results for logarithmically transformed and

    non-transformed data. Therefore, and for simplicity, we chose to

    present the non-transformed results.

    Correction of explicit measures

    To investigate the possibility that the scores on the Modern Racial

    Prejudice Scale may be influenced by the motivation to control ones

    prejudice, the Modern Racial Prejudice Scale scores were regressed

    on the Motivation to Control Prejudiced Reactions Scale scores (see

    Forsman, 1993; Jackson, 1967). The residuals for the Modern Racial

    Prejudice Scale scores were calculated for each participant.

    RESULTS

    Control analyses

    Outliers.

    Multivariate outliers, defined as those having stand-

    ardized residuals above z

    = 2.5 or below z

    =

    2.5, wereidentified (

    n

    = 2) and excluded from further analyses.

    Detection data.

    During the second and the fourth phase,

    participants were instructed to attend to the presented

    photographs for later recognition. Thus, the recognition data

    served as a measure of the participants attention, thereby

    testing whether participants had followed the instruction.

    The mean proportion of correctly recognized photographs

    was 0.98 in the third phase, and 0.87 in the fifth phase. This

    was statistically higher than chance level (0.50), t

    (39) =

    130.52,p

    < 0.001, and t

    (39) = 34.24,p

    < 0.001, respectively.

    Attractiveness ratings.

    To examine whether attractiveness ofthe presented faces had an impact on the response latencies,

    participants were instructed to rate the faces on the photo-

    graphs as to their attractiveness. Mean attractiveness for

    immigrant (

    M

    = 4.80, SD

    = 1.14) and Swedish (

    M

    = 4.56,

    SD

    = 1.17) facial photographs were computed. No signific-

    ant difference was found, t

    (39) = 1.76, p

    = 0.09.

    Main analyses

    Correlation analysis.

    A product-moment correlation was

    computed between the Modern Racial Prejudice scores and

    the scores on the Motivation to Control Prejudiced Re-actions Scale. The analysis revealed a significant correlation

    between the scales (

    r

    =

    0.51, p

    = 0.001).

    Implicit ethnic prejudice.

    The Implicit Ethnic Prejudice scores

    (

    M

    = 0.05, SD

    = 0.11) were analyzed by conducting a one-

    sample t

    -test. The analysis revealed that the mean was sig-

    nificantly different from zero, t

    (39) = 2.99,p

    < 0.005, which

    shows that the participants displayed more negative implicit

    attitudes when exposed to immigrant as compared to Swedish

    faces.

    Implicit and explicit ethnic prejudice.

    To make possible com-

    parisons with previous research (e.g., Devine, 1989; Lepore

    & Brown, 1997; Kawakami, Dion & Dovidio, 1998), we first

    examined whether participants with high and low explicit

    racial/ethnic attitudes (also) differ in their implicit ethnic

    attitudes. Based on the Modern Racial Prejudice scores (the

    explicit attitude measure) participants were classified by

    a median split as high and low on explicit prejudice. A one-

    tailed t

    -test revealed no significant difference in Implicit

    Ethnic Prejudice between the groups (

    M

    = 0.06, SD

    = 0.11, n

    =

    19 for high, and M

    = 0.05, SD

    = 0.11, n

    = 21 for low), t

    (38)

    = 0.27,p

    = 0.40. Thus, participants high and low in explicit

  • 7/23/2019 lskjgks

    5/6

    Scand J Psychol 46 (2005)

    Implicit and explicit prejudice

    365

    2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

    ethnic prejudice showed implicit ethnic prejudice to the same

    extent. However, an analysis based on the corrected

    Modern

    Racial Prejudice scores revealed that the high-prejudice

    group (

    M

    = 0.09, SD

    = 0.09, n

    = 20) showed significantly

    more implicit ethnic prejudice than the low-prejudice group(

    M

    = 0.01, SD

    = 0.11, n

    = 20), t

    (38) = 2.62, p

    = 0.01 (see

    Fig. 1).

    In addition, and using a more appropriate method, product-

    moment correlation analysis was conducted between the

    continuous implicit and explicit prejudice scores. The analyses

    revealed no significant relationship between the implicit and

    the uncorrected

    explicit scores (

    r

    = 0.20, p

    = 0.22) whereas

    the relationship was significant when using the corrected

    explicit scores (

    r

    = 0.38, p

    = 0.02). Thus, this pattern corre-

    sponds to the analyses above using a median split on the

    explicit scores.

    DISCUSSION

    With few exceptions (e.g., Dunton & Fazio, 1997; Fazio et al

    .,

    1995; Plant & Devine, 1998), previous research has not

    emphasized the importance of examining the role of peoples

    motivation to control prejudiced reactions when measuring

    ethnic attitudes. Responding to this state of affairs, and

    following Fazio et al

    . (1995; Dunton & Fazio, 1997), we

    investigated the role of motivation and its impact on the

    association between implicit and explicit ethnic attitude

    measures.

    The results of our study revealed evidence for participants

    implicit ethnic prejudice. Specifically, following immigrant

    primes, response latencies were slow to positive adjectives and

    fast to negative adjectives, as compared to Swedish primes.

    In addition, the correlation analysis showed a strong negative

    relationship between modern ethnic prejudice (our explicit

    measure) and motivation to control prejudiced reactions, which

    demonstrates the reactivity of the Modern Racial Prejudice

    Scale and its sensitivity to motivational factors.

    More important, the analyses revealed no significant cor-

    relation between implicit and explicit ethnic prejudice, thus,

    no association between explicit and implicit prejudice was

    found. However, when the modern ethnic prejudice scores

    were corrected for motivational biases, a significant correla-

    tion (i.e., association) was obtained. The same pattern of

    results was obtained when analyzing people high and low

    (median-split) on explicit prejudice. Thus, the results con-

    firm our hypothesis that motivation to control prejudiced

    reactions has a moderating role in the association betweenexplicit and implicit prejudice. However, it should be noted

    that the association between explicit and implicit prejudice

    even after the correction was not especially strong. In accord

    with Fazio et al

    . (1995) and Dunton and Fazio (1997), we

    can conclude that peoples motivation to control or conceal

    prejudiced reactions seems to be an important factor to take

    into account when studying the relationship or association

    between implicit and explicit prejudice. Finally, it must be

    added that other factors can also be of importance for the

    implicit-explicit prejudice relationship, for example, whether

    the outgroup is normatively protected against discrimination

    or not (Franco & Maass, 1999).

    Devine (1989), in her dissociation model, argued that aperson might hold a positive attitude toward a social group

    and at the same time automatically activate negative stereo-

    types on the mere presence of a stereotyped group member.

    On this basis, Devine predicted that implicit and explicit

    measures of ethnic prejudice might be dissociated and she

    obtained empirical verification of this prediction as well.

    Several studies, however, have shown that Devines assump-

    tion might not be valid and various explanations have

    been offered (e.g., Kawakami et al

    ., 1998; Lepore & Brown,

    1997; Locke, MacLeod & Walker, 1994; Moskowitz, Wasel,

    Gollwitzer & Schaal, 1999; Wittenbrink, Judd & Park, 1997).

    Although the present study seems to speak against Devines

    (1989) dissociation model as well, we argue that it providesconvergent evidence for automatic stereotype activation

    and confirms the assumption that stereotypes are activated

    automatically on the mere presence of a stereotyped group

    member.

    In conclusion, the present research has demonstrated the

    utility of the adjective evaluation technique as a method for

    measuring implicit prejudice and the sensitivity of self-report

    instruments to motivational biases when measuring explicit

    prejudice. Our findings lend additional support to the evidence

    presented by Fazio et al.

    (1995; Dunton & Fazio, 1997) for

    the impact of motivation on the implicit-explicit prejudice

    relation in a different social context, a European country, and

    in relation to a different target group, immigrants in general.

    However, to arrive at a firm conclusion and test the external

    validity of the findings, the present study should be replic-

    ated using non-student samples of participants. Finally, in

    future research the importance of also exploring other vari-

    ables that may moderate the relationship between implicit

    and explicit prejudice must be emphasized.

    This research was supported by Grant F0890/97 to Bo Ekehammar

    from the Swedish Council for Research in the Humanities and

    Social Sciences. The authors would like to thank Tadesse Araya for

    his insightful comments on an earlier version of this article.

    Fig. 1. Mean implicit prejudice for people with high and low scores

    on uncorrected and corrected (for motivational bias) explicit prejudice.

  • 7/23/2019 lskjgks

    6/6

    366

    N. Akrami and B. Ekehammar

    Scand J Psychol 46 (2005)

    2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

    REFERENCES

    Akrami, N., Ekehammar, B. & Araya, T. (2000). Classical and modern

    racial prejudice: A study of attitudes toward immigrants in

    Sweden. European Journal of Social Psychology

    , 30

    , 521532.

    Akrami, N., Ekehammar, B. & Araya, T. (2003). Category and stereo-

    type activation revisited: The intimate relation between category

    and stereotypes. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Blair, I. V. & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and controlled pro-

    cesses in stereotype priming. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology

    , 70

    , 1142 1163.

    Brauer, M., Wasel, W. & Niedenthal, P. (2000). Implicit and explicit

    components of prejudice. Review of General Psychology

    , 4

    , 79

    101.

    Crosby, F., Bromley, S. & Saxe, L. (1980). Recent unobtrusive studies

    of black and white discrimination and prejudice: A literature

    review. Psychological Bulletin

    , 35

    , 546563.

    Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic

    and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 56, 5 18.

    Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B. & Howard, A.

    (1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled

    processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 510540.

    Dunton, B. C. & Fazio, R. H. (1997). An individual difference

    measure of motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Personality

    and Social Psychology Bulletin,23, 316326.

    Ekehammar, B., Akrami, N. & Araya, T. (2003). Gender differences

    in implicit prejudice. Personality and Individual Differences, 34,

    15091523.

    Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C. & Williams, C. J.

    (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive

    measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013 1027.

    Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C. & Kardes, F. R.

    (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229238.

    Forsman, L. (1993). Giving extreme responses to items in self-esteemscales: Response set or personality trait? European Journal of

    Psychological Assessment, 9, 3340.

    Franco, F. M. & Maass, A. (1999). Intentional control over pre-

    judice: When the choice of the measure matters. European Journal

    of Social Psychology,29, 469477.

    Greenwald, A. G. & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition:

    Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review,

    102, 427.

    Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E. & Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998).

    Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The

    implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 74, 14641480.

    Jackson, D. N. (1967). Acquiescence response styles: Problems of

    identification and control. In I. A. Berg (Ed.), Response set in

    personality assessment(pp. 71114). Chicago, IL: Aldine.

    Katz, I. & Hass, R. G. (1988). Racial ambivalence and American

    value conflict: Correlational and priming studies of dual cognitive

    structures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55,

    893905.

    Kawakami, K., Dion, K. & Dovidio, J. F. (1998). Racial prejudice

    and stereotype activation. Personality and Social Psychology

    Bulletin,24, 407416.

    Lepore, L. & Brown, R. (1997). Category and stereotype activation:

    Is prejudice inevitable? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

    72, 275287.

    Locke, V., Macleod, C. & Walker, I. (1994). Automatic and controlled

    activation of stereotypes: Individual differences associated

    with prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 2946.

    McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and theModern Racism Scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.),

    Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 91126). New York:

    Academic Press.

    McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B. & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism

    declined in America? It depends upon who is asking and what

    is asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution,25, 563578.

    Moskowitz, G., Wasel, W., Gollwitzer, P. M. & Schaal, B. (1999).

    Preconscious control of stereotype activation through egalitarian

    goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 167185.

    Pettigrew, T. F. & Meertens, R. W. (1995). Subtle and blatant

    prejudice in western Europe. European Journal of Social Psy-

    chology,25, 5775.

    Plant, E. A & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motiva-

    tion to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and

    Social Psychology, 75, 811832.Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M. & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial

    prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with question-

    naire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,

    262274.

    Received 30 May 2003, accepted 25 May 2004