kindergarten mathematics misalignment mathematics ... also noted the importance of mathematics...

32
KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT 1 The Misalignment of Kindergarten Mathematics Content Mimi Engel Vanderbilt University Amy Claessens University of Chicago Tyler Watts George Farkas University of California at Irvine March 13, 2015 Acknowledgments: The data used in this paper come from The National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) and are available through a restricted-use license agreement. The authors are grateful to the Spencer Foundation and to the National Institute of Child Health and Human Developmentsupported Irvine Network on Interventions in Development (HD065704 P01) for research support. The authors thank Pamela Davis-Kean, Greg Duncan, and Robert Siegler for helpful feedback. All errors are our own.

Upload: truongnguyet

Post on 17-Jun-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

1

The Misalignment of Kindergarten Mathematics Content

Mimi Engel

Vanderbilt University

Amy Claessens

University of Chicago

Tyler Watts

George Farkas

University of California at Irvine

March 13, 2015

Acknowledgments: The data used in this paper come from The National Center

for Education Statistics (NCES) and are available through a restricted-use license

agreement. The authors are grateful to the Spencer Foundation and to the National

Institute of Child Health and Human Development– supported Irvine Network on

Interventions in Development (HD065704 P01) for research support. The authors

thank Pamela Davis-Kean, Greg Duncan, and Robert Siegler for helpful feedback.

All errors are our own.

Page 2: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

2

Abstract

Analyzing data from two nationally representative kindergarten cohorts, we

examine the mathematics content in kindergarten classrooms. We replicate prior

research and find that kindergarten teachers emphasize basic content that students

already know. Although teachers reported increased coverage of advanced content

between the 1998-99 and 2010-11 school years, they continued to place more

emphasis on basic content. We find that time on advanced content is positively

associated with student learning, while time on basic content has a negative

association with learning. We also explore the extent to which content covered in

kindergarten aligns with the Common Core State Standards in Mathematics. We

argue that increased exposure to more advanced mathematics content could

benefit the vast majority of kindergartners.

Page 3: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

3

Despite evidence of and advocacy regarding the importance of

mathematics education for young children, mathematics is under-emphasized in

kindergarten, particularly when compared with reading (Engel, Claessens, &

Finch, 2013). Teachers of young children report being uncomfortable and unsure

teaching mathematics (Ginsberg et al., 2008). This discomfort is concerning,

particularly when considered in light of findings that suggest that young children

are able to understand and learn a wide range of mathematics in early childhood

(Clements & Sarama, 2011).

Children’s skills in mathematics at kindergarten entry, as well as their

learning in mathematics during kindergarten, are more important predictors than

reading skills or socioemotional behaviors of their subsequent success in both

reading and mathematics (Claessens & Engel, 2013; Claessens, Duncan, & Engel,

2009; Watts, Duncan, Siegler, & Davis-Kean, 2014). Key advocacy groups have

also noted the importance of mathematics instruction for young children. The

National Council for Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM), the National Association

for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC), and the National Mathematics

Advisory Panel (NMAP) have all argued that children under the age of six are

ready to learn, and will benefit greatly from exposure to, a range of mathematics

content (NAEYC & NCTM, 2002; NCTM, 2007, NMAP, 2008).

Research using data from the late 1990s found that the majority of the

mathematics content to which most kindergarteners were exposed covered topics

Page 4: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

4

they had already mastered (Engel et al., 2013). Although most kindergartners

entered school proficient in basic counting and knowledge of basic shapes,

kindergarten teachers reported emphasizing this content far more than topics that

students had yet to master, such as addition and subtraction. Further, exposure to

advanced content was associated with larger cross-kindergarten test score gains in

mathematics, whereas exposure to basic content was associated with smaller gains

for almost all students (Claessens, Engel, & Curran, 2014; Engel et al., 2013).

Despite this evidence, there is little agreement about what mathematics

kindergarteners should be taught.

There have been shifts in both public discourse and public policy toward

an increased academicization of kindergarten (Russell, 2011). Yet, research

suggests that this increased focus on academic content has been primarily

centered on reading instruction. Bassok and Rorem (2013) found that

kindergarten teachers reported dramatic increases in time spent on reading

between 1998 and 2006. Further, they reported a rise in teachers’ expectations

with regard to children’s school readiness skills across content areas during this

period. These changes to kindergarten have been controversial, however, with

some advocates arguing that an increased academic focus interferes with the

important role of play and socialization in kindergarten (Miller & Almon, 2009).

Recent policy changes have continued to highlight academics in

kindergarten. In 2010, the Common Core State Standards for Mathematics

Page 5: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

5

(CCSSM) were introduced. They have been fully or partially adopted by 43 states.

The CCSSM were designed to guide teachers’ mathematics instruction by

emphasizing both processes and proficiencies (Common Core Standards

Initiative, 2010) with the goal of deepening students’ knowledge of mathematics

(Schmidt & Burroughs, 2013). For kindergarten teachers, the CCSSM contains

detailed standards outlining the mathematics content that should be taught during

the kindergarten year. Although the CCSSM will likely influence kindergarten

mathematics, the nature and implications of this change for learning in

mathematics remain unclear.

The Current Study

We replicate and extend prior research on kindergarten mathematics

content. Recent evidence indicates that replication – crucial for building

knowledge in the social sciences – is extremely rare in educational research

(Makel & Plucker, 2014). Engel and colleagues (2013) found kindergarten

mathematics content to be poorly aligned with student knowledge and skills in

mathematics at school entry. Although evidence suggests that kindergarten has

become more academic (Bassok & Rorem, 2013; Russell, 2011), we know little

about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten has

changed.

We leverage newly released data to conduct cross-cohort comparisons

exploring changes in kindergarten mathematics content coverage over time. We

Page 6: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

6

use data from two nationally representative cohorts of kindergarteners and their

teachers to replicate and extend prior research (Engel, et al., 2013). We explore

the association between teacher-reported content coverage and student learning,

addressing the following questions:

1) Has the mathematics content that kindergarten teachers report

teaching changed between 1998-99 and 2010-11?

2) Has the relationship between mathematics content exposure in

kindergarten and student learning changed across this period?

We also explore the extent to which mathematics content coverage in

kindergarten aligns with the CCSSM for kindergarten.1 We ask the following

questions with regard to this topic:

3) How does the mathematics instructional content that

kindergarten teachers reported covering align with the CCSSM

for kindergarten?

4) What is the relationship between measures of CCSSM content

coverage in kindergarten and student learning?

Methods

Data

We use data from two longitudinal, nationally representative studies of

kindergarteners conducted by the National Center for Educational Statistics

(NCES); the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 1998-99

Page 7: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

7

(ECLS-K) and the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of

2010-11 (ECLS-K:2011). As shown in Table 1, the analysis samples for the

ECLS-K (n=17,810) and the ECLS-K:2011 (n=15,090) differ in a number of

ways. In 1998-99, 61 percent of kindergarteners attended a full day program,

whereas 87 percent did so in 2010-11. Further, one quarter of kindergarteners

identified as Hispanic in 2010-11, a 32 percent increase from 19 percent in 1998-

1999. Both data sets include over 3000 kindergarten teachers who had taught

kindergarten for an average of nine years. All sample sizes we report are rounded

to the nearest ten, in compliance with NCES restricted data use agreements.

[Insert Table 1 here]

For the ECLS-K:2011, restricted base-year data can be requested from the

National Center for Educational Statistics. These data allow for cross-cohort

comparisons across the ECLS-K and the ECLS-K:2011 in terms of children’s

classroom experiences. Direct child cognitive assessment scores that have been

released for the newer cohort are, at this time, not directly comparable to scores

from the original ECLS-K. Information about the ECLS-K comes from its user’s

manual (Tourangeau, Nord, Le, Sorongon, & Najarian, 2009), and information

about the sample and measures for the ECLS-K:2011 is drawn from its base-year

user’s manual (Tourangeau et al., forthcoming).

The ECLS-K and the ECLS-K:2011 are large, longitudinal, nationally

representative studies of children who were in kindergarten in either 1998-99 or

Page 8: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

8

2010-11, respectively. Both data sets contain extensive information about sample

children and their families, classrooms, teachers, and schools. For the analyses

that follow, we draw our independent variables of interest from the teacher

surveys administered in the spring of 1999 and 2011, respectively. Our dependent

variables are student achievement test scores in mathematics from the spring of

kindergarten. Controls are drawn from parent, student, teacher, and school-level

data. To address the issue of missing data, we use multiple imputation, imputing

independent and dependent variables for both data sets. Please see online

appendix for details on our multiple imputation procedures.

Measures

Student Achievement. In both cohorts, student achievement in

mathematics and reading was assessed at kindergarten entry and again in the

spring of kindergarten. The mathematics assessments were designed to measure

conceptual and procedural knowledge and problem solving ability. The

assessments were developed using Item Response Theory (IRT) to allow

researchers to use the measures to examine growth in student achievement

longitudinally (Tourangeau et al., 2009). We control for fall test scores and spring

mathematics test scores are our dependent variable of interest. We use the IRT

scale scores provided in each dataset, which we standardize to a mean of zero and

a standard deviation of one within each cohort.

Page 9: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

9

For the original ECLS-K, NCES used the hierarchical structure of the

assessments to create proficiency probability scores measuring student mastery of

various levels in mathematics and reading. Individual scores on these “proficiency

levels” are included as variables in the data files for the original ECLS-K. The

most basic proficiency level in mathematics measured student knowledge of

“Basic Counting and Shapes”, and the most advanced proficiency level (for

kindergarteners) measured “Basic Addition and Subtraction.” Proficiency

probability scores and proficiency levels have not been released for the ECLS-

K:2011. Thus, it is not possible to compare proficiency levels across the two

cohorts.

Both the mathematics and reading assessments created for the ECLS-K

were developed from national and state standards, and they were also designed to

align with the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). Although

the NAEP is not administered until fourth grade, the principles and content within

the NAEP were extrapolated to the kindergarten level. Further, mathematics

assessment designers also consulted the National Council for Teachers of

Mathematics (NCTM), and empirical work has shown that the CCSSM are

moderately aligned to the NCTM’s standards for mathematical practice (Porter et

al., 2011).

Content Measures

Page 10: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

10

Alignment measures. Replicating the measures used by Engel et al.,

2013, we create alignment measures in both cohorts using teacher reports.

Teacher surveys in both cohorts included items asking teachers about their

instruction with regard to mathematics content and activities. Teachers responded

on a six point Likert scale ranging from “never” to “every day”. We converted

these scales to a measure of number of days per month ranging from zero to 20.

We aligned these items to the proficiency levels created by NCES staff using

results from the fall of kindergarten mathematics test for the ECLS-K. We include

only items that matched the mathematics content described in the proficiency

levels. This procedure generated four categories of classroom-mathematics

content that were, again, aligned with the four proficiency levels in mathematics

in which kindergarteners in the original ECLS-K were scored. These correspond

to the measures used in Engel et al., 2013.

We calculate these as the average number of days per month of the items

contained within each category: Basic Counting and Shapes; Patterns and

Measurement; Place Value and Currency; and Addition and Subtraction. Alpha

values for the eight measures (four for each data set) range from .58 - .88.

Appendix Table 1 shows the individual items (with their means and standard

deviations) used to create each of the four scales. The online appendix provides

more detail on measure creation. Correlations between the four measures within

the two respective data sets are provided in Appendix Table 2.

Page 11: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

11

CCSSM Content Measures. We also analyzed the teacher-reported

mathematics content items to align them with the CCSSM kindergarten domains.

We developed measures from the 1998-1999 and 2010-2011 cohorts to explore

the extent to which teacher-reported mathematics content coverage in

kindergarten aligns with the CCSSM domains and how content aligned with the

CCSSM domains relates to student learning in mathematics during kindergarten.

Appendix Table 3 presents the teacher-reported items that we coded into

these domains. For our CCSSM Content Measures aligned with the CCSSM

kindergarten domains, alpha values range from .49 - .88. The number of items per

measure ranges from five for our measure of Counting and Cardinality to two

items each for our measures of Operations and Algebraic Thinking, Numbers and

Operations in Base Ten, and Geometry. Our measure of the CCSSM kindergarten

domain of Measurement and Data includes three items.

In order to facilitate comparisons between the Alignment Measures and

our CCSSM Content Measures, in Appendix Table 3 we also detail when an item

is categorized under a particular CCSSM domain as well as an Alignment

Measure. For example, “Count out loud” is categorized under the CCSSM

Content Measure aligned with the domain of Counting and Cardinality and is also

included in the Alignment Measure of Basic Counting and Shapes.

Coincidentally, each of our five measures representing the CCSSM Content Areas

includes two items from one of the four Alignment Measures. We note that our

Page 12: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

12

CCSSM Content Measures of Counting and Cardinality and Geometry each

include two (mutually exclusive) items from the Alignment Measure of Basic

Counting and Shapes.

Control Variables. We control for fall of kindergarten mathematics and

reading test scores. Additional control variables included in all of our models are

student gender, race/ethnicity, age at kindergarten entry, preschool attendance,

mother's education, family income, whether the student lived with a biological

parent, number of siblings in the household, number of books in the home, and

whether English was the primarily language spoken in the home. We also control

for teacher-reported overall time on mathematics and whether the classroom was

full day kindergarten. Teacher characteristics we control for include teacher

gender, ethnicity, age, whether the teacher had obtained a master's degree, years

of teaching experience, whether the teacher had taken a pedagogical development

class in mathematics, reading, and/or early education, respectively, and

certification level. Finally, we included administrator-reported measures of school

region, urbanicity, and the percentage of the student body eligible for free or

reduced-price lunch. Appendix Table 4 reports descriptive statistics for control

variables.

Analysis Plan

We investigate the association between measures of teacher-reported

mathematics content emphasis and student achievement in mathematics. We

Page 13: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

13

estimate separate OLS regression models for each cohort in which we predict

student mathematics achievement as a product of mathematics instructional

content, and student, teacher, family and school characteristics. For the

Alignment Measures our model is specified as follows:

𝑀𝑎𝑡ℎ𝐴𝑐ℎ𝑖𝑠𝑘 = 𝑎1 + 𝛽1𝐶𝑜𝑛𝑡𝑒𝑛𝑡𝑖𝑘 + 𝛽2𝐴𝑐ℎ𝑖𝑓𝑘 + 𝛽3𝐶ℎ𝑖𝑙𝑑𝑖 + 𝛽4𝐹𝑎𝑚𝑖𝑙𝑦𝑖𝑓𝑘

+ 𝛽5𝑇𝑒𝑎𝑐ℎ𝑒𝑟𝑖𝑘 + 𝛽6𝑆𝑐ℎ𝑜𝑜𝑙𝑖𝑘 + 𝑒𝑖

where 𝑀𝑎𝑡ℎ𝐴𝑐ℎ𝑖𝑠𝑘 represents the mathematics achievement of child i measured

at the spring of kindergarten (sk). 𝐶𝑜𝑛𝑡𝑒𝑛𝑡𝑖𝑘 represents the days per month spent

on the set of each of the respective Alignment Content Measures (Basic Counting

and Shapes, Patterns and Measurement, Place Value and Currency, and Addition

and Subtraction) for child i during kindergarten (k). 𝐴𝑐ℎ𝑖𝑓𝑘 represents the

mathematics and reading achievement for child i measured in the fall of

kindergarten (fk). 𝐶ℎ𝑖𝑙𝑑𝑖 is a vector of background characteristics (e.g.,

race/ethnicity, gender). 𝐹𝑎𝑚𝑖𝑙𝑦𝑖𝑓𝑘 represents a vector of family background

characteristics (e.g., parental income). 𝑇𝑒𝑎𝑐ℎ𝑒𝑟𝑖𝑘 is a vector of teacher

characteristics (e.g., certification level, race/ethnicity) measured during

kindergarten. 𝑆𝑐ℎ𝑜𝑜𝑙𝑖𝑘 represents a vector of administrator-reported school

characteristics (e.g., proportion of students eligible for free or reduced-price

lunch) measured during the kindergarten year, and 𝑒𝑖 represents the error term.

Using the sample weights, we adjust all standard errors for

nonindependence due to sample-designed clustering in classrooms and schools.

Page 14: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

14

We also tested models in which standard errors were adjusted at only the

classroom level. The resulting standard errors were nearly identical. We

standardize all measures of instructional content within cohort. Thus, coefficients

estimated for our various measures of instructional content (𝛽1) estimate the effect

of a standard deviation increase in reported coverage of specific mathematics

content on end-of-kindergarten mathematics achievement. Please see online

appendix for detailed information regarding the multiple imputation procedures

and sample weights that we use.

We estimate these models separately for the 1998 and 2010 cohorts,

respectively, and compare coefficients calculated for the Alignment Measures for

each cohort. We then run the same models replacing the Alignment Measures with

our CCSSM Content Area measures (𝐶𝑜𝑛𝑡𝑒𝑛𝑡𝑖𝑘) for each cohort.

It is important to note that our data are observational, which precludes us

from estimating the causal impact of our measures of instructional content in

mathematics on end-of-kindergarten mathematics achievement. However, the

inclusion of an extensive set of control variables should substantially reduce bias

due to possible omitted variables.

Results

Table 2 shows the percentage of students who had mastered particular

content areas at kindergarten entry in 1998. These mastery rates were calculated

by NCES staff for the original ECLS-K, but are not available for the ECLS-

Page 15: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

15

K:2011. The vast majority of children – 92 percent – had mastered mathematics

content related to Basic Counting and Shapes at the start of kindergarten. In

contrast, almost no students – 3 percent – had mastered Basic Addition and

Subtraction.

[Insert Table 2 here]

Table 2 also presents Cronbach’s alpha values and descriptive statistics for

the four content Alignment Measures and the five CCSSM Content Area Measures

by cohort. Average days per month reported on two of the four Alignment

Measures – those that measure more advanced mathematics content – increased,

with magnitudes that are both statistically significant and substantively large.

Reported coverage of Place Value and Currency went from approximately 8.5

days per month in 1999 to over 10.5 days per month, on average (p < .001), in

2011. Similarly, reported time on Basic Addition and Subtraction went from 7.5

days per month to nearly 9.5 days per month (p < .001). In contrast, average time

on Basic Counting and Shapes increased by only .4 days per month, and time on

Patterns and Measurement also increased slightly (by .029 days per month).

Comparing time spent on items aligned with the CCSSM content in 1998-

1999 with time spent on the same content in 2010-2011, as was the case with the

misalignment measures (with which these measures overlap considerably),

teachers reported spending more days per month on most of the CCSSM Content

measures in 2010-2011 than in 1998-1999.

Page 16: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

16

Figure 1 highlights the poor fit between the content teachers cover and

what students know at kindergarten entry. We contrast the number of reported

days per month on each content measure with the percentage of students who had

mastered the most basic and the most advanced content areas at kindergarten

entry in 1998. In both cohorts, teachers reported the greatest emphasis –

approximately 13 days per month – on Basic Counting & Shapes; content that

over 90 percent of students had already mastered. They reported less emphasis on

content, such as Addition & Subtraction, that few students had mastered.

[Insert Figure 1 here]

We estimated separate ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions for both

cohorts in which we model student mathematics achievement in the spring of

kindergarten as a function of our measures of mathematics content, controlling for

relevant student, teacher, family and school characteristics including student

mathematics achievement at kindergarten entry. We adjust all standard errors for

nonindependence due to sample-designed clustering in classrooms and schools.

We also standardize all measures of instructional content within cohort. Thus,

coefficients estimated for our measures of instructional content estimate the effect

of a standard deviation increase in reported coverage of specific mathematics

content on end-of-kindergarten mathematics achievement.

Table 3 shows results from regressions using the Alignment Measures we

replicate from (Engel, et al., 2013) described above as well as our CCSSM

Page 17: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

17

Content Area Measures. Each pair of columns (e.g., columns 1 and 2) shows

results from regressions that are identically specified, with coefficients in odd-

numbered columns generated from the original ECLS-K and even numbered

columns showing results for the ECLS-K:2011.

[Insert Table 3 here]

Columns 1 and 2 present results that include all four of the Alignment

Measures in a single model. Results displayed in Table 3 for the original ECLS-

K are consistent with results reported by (Engel, et al., 2013). Moreover, the

coefficients estimated using the more recent ECLS-K:2011 are similar to

estimates for the original ECLS-K cohort, indicating that the association between

instructional content and student achievement in kindergarten did not change over

this time period.

Coefficients are small in magnitude, ranging from an absolute value of

.015 (p < .05) for Patterns and Measurement for the ELCS-K to .046 (p < .001)

for Addition and Subtraction for the ECLS-K:2011. Results indicate that, on

average, students benefit from additional exposure to content that they had not

mastered at kindergarten entry including Place Value and Currency and Addition

and Subtraction. Further, we find that in both 1998-99 and 2010-11, students

gained less when teachers reported spending more days per month on Basic

Counting and Shapes or Patterns and Measurement; content that the majority of

students had already mastered at the start of kindergarten.

Page 18: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

18

Columns 3 and 4 present results from models using the CCSSM Content

Area Measures to predict end of kindergarten mathematics achievement. We find

the largest positive effects for time spent on the CCSSM Content Areas that are

similar to our Alignment Measures that cover more advanced content (content that

most students had not yet mastered at kindergarten entry): Operations and

Algebraic Thinking (ECLS-K: β = .036, p < .001; ECLS-K:2011: β = .044, p <

.001) and Numbers in Base Ten (ECLS-K: β = .020, p < .001; ECLS-K:2011: β =

.028, p < .001). The measure we created to align with the CCSSM domain of

Measurement and Data is statistically insignificant, with coefficient estimates

near zero for both cohorts.

In addition to the results described above, we conducted a series of

alternative model specifications and robustness checks. We used listwise deletion

as well as missing data indicators as alternative means for dealing with missing

data. We also tested specifications using more extensive, but not directly

comparable, sets of control variables for each cohort. Finally, we explored

whether results differed for subgroups by student race/ethnicity, socioeconomic

status, and school entry mathematics achievement. Results were similar across all

model specifications and for all subgroups. Please see online appendix for

additional details.

Page 19: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

19

Discussion

Our investigation of the mathematics content covered in kindergarten

classrooms in 2010-11 showed a pattern of results that was largely similar to what

Engel and colleagues (2013) found using data from the 1998-99 school year. We

show that kindergarten teachers continue to report spending more days per month

teaching content that students already know – particularly time on Basic Counting

and Shapes. Teachers reported covering this content an average of 13 days per

month in both 1998-1999 and 2010-2011. Moreover, coverage of this basic

content was negatively associated with student achievement gains for both cohorts

of kindergarteners. Thus, the misalignment between content coverage and student

knowledge has continued, with no evidence of a reduction in coverage of basic

content.

Our efforts at replication are important in light of recent calls across fields

and disciplines for an increased focus on replication in social science research

(e.g., Lieberman, 2010; Nosek, Spies, & Motyl, 2012). Although replication is

crucial to the advancement of knowledge in social science, publication of explicit

replication studies is exceedingly rare in fields including economics,

developmental psychology, and education (Duncan, Engel, Claessens, & Dowsett,

2014; Makel & Plucker, 2014). Our study explicitly replicates prior research on

kindergarten mathematics teaching and learning (Engel et al., 2013), finding a

Page 20: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

20

consistent pattern of results and extending that work to explore results with newly

released data and additional measures of mathematics content.

We find that time on more advanced topics increased between the 1998-

1999 and 2010-2011 school years. This increase could be due to the

accountability policies ushered in with NCLB, as NCLB required states to set

academic standards in mathematics and reading for elementary school students. It

is unclear how NCLB might have affected kindergarten teachers, since

accountability testing is not mandated until the third grade year. Yet, empirical

work has shown that kindergarten teachers’ academic expectations rose during the

years following NCLB (Bassok & Rorem, 2013), and those expectations may

influence what content teachers cover.

The adoption of the CCSSM was met with optimism on the part of some

educational researchers, with a hope that the new standards would move teachers

toward emphasizing more rigorous content (e.g., Schmidt, 2004). However, our

findings suggest that merely aligning instruction with the topics or overarching

domains recommended by the CCSSM for kindergarten may not be enough to

help teachers shift toward systematically providing more instruction on advanced

content and less on basic content most students have mastered. Our analyses

suggest that the instructional domains prescribed by the CCSSM are split

relatively evenly across items measuring basic and advanced content (based on

what students know at kindergarten entry) in the ECLS-K, and the effect of time

Page 21: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

21

spent on these CCSSM domains on student achievement was predictably mixed.

This suggests that teachers could simultaneously provide instruction that is

aligned with the CCSSM content domains and continue to overemphasize the

basics, to the detriment of learning for the vast majority of their students.

Our CCSSM measures are limited in that we have few items per domain

and the ECLS-K teacher surveys were not designed to measure alignment with the

CCSSM. Another important limitation of our CCSSM measures is that they do

not capture the cognitive complexity with which teachers approached each topic.

The CCSSM has been touted as a more conceptually-oriented, cognitively

demanding approach to mathematics education than previous state standards

(Schmidt & Houang, 2012), and much of this added complexity comes from the

information that goes beyond the recommended topics. For example, under the

kindergarten Counting and Cardinality domain, the CCSSM recommends that

teachers help students “understand that the last number name said tells the number

of objects counted” (Common Core State Standards for Mathematics, 2010). It is

possible that this type of conceptual emphasis could support student achievement

gains beyond rote counting exercises. Unfortunately, our measures only indicate

whether teachers reported spending time on particular content. One of the

limitations of the data that we use is that we are unable to discern more

information regarding how that content was taught.

Page 22: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

22

Further, we are unable to draw causal inferences due to the observational

nature of the data we use. As such, we cannot rule out possible omitted-variables

that could be driving our estimates. Teachers who spend more time on advanced

content could be qualitatively different from teachers who emphasize basic topics

in a variety of unobserved ways. However, some experimental evidence suggests

that designing professional development sessions that prevent early-grade

teachers from spending too much time on content that students had already

mastered can produce higher student-achievement gains (Clements et al., 2013).

This evidence suggests that professional development may be a means for

supporting kindergarten teachers in shifting some of their mathematics

instructional time to more advanced, as opposed to very basic, content.

Conclusion

Our central findings are clear: teaching students mathematics content that

they have not already mastered is positively associated with mathematics

achievement gains during kindergarten. Previous research indicates that time on

more advanced content in kindergarten benefits students at all ability levels, both

those who attended preschool and those who did not (Claessens et al., 2014).

Based on the results of our study and related research, we argue that education

policy and practice would benefit from efforts to help kindergarten teachers shift

some of their time on mathematics instruction from basic content to increase time

Page 23: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

23

spent teaching mathematics that students have not already mastered at

kindergarten entry.

Page 24: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

24

Notes

1. We note that while we consider our questions related to the CCSSM to be

important and interesting, our ability to draw conclusions with regard to the

CCSSM for kindergarten are limited. In terms of instruments, both the

achievement tests and the teacher questionnaires used in the two ECLS

kindergarten studies were created prior to the development and implementation of

the CCSSM. Thus, these data are not designed explicitly to explore the

relationship between the CCSSM, content, and student learning. We consider

these questions to be an extension of our primary analyses.

Page 25: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

25

References

Barnett, W. S., & Yarosz, D. J. (2007). Who goes to preschool and why does it

matter?. Preschool Policy Matters, 7.

Bassok, D., & Latham, S. (2014). Kids Today: Changes in School Readiness in an

Early Childhood Era. Paper presented at the 2014 Annual Meeting of the

Association for Public Policy Analysis and Management.

Bassok, D.P, & Rorem, A. (2013). Is Kindergarten the New First Grade: The

Changing nature of kindergarten in the age of accountability. Paper presented

at the 2013 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child

Development.

Claessens, A., Duncan, G., & Engel, M. (2009). Kindergarten skills and fifth-

grade achievement: Evidence from the ECLS-K. Economics of Education

Review, 28(4), 415-427.

Claessens, A., & Engel, M. (2013). How important is where you start? Early

mathematics knowledge and later school success. Teachers College Record,

115, 1-29.

Claessens, A., Engel, M., & Curran, F.C. (2014). Academic Content, Student

Learning, and the Persistence of Preschool Effects. American Educational

Research Journal, 51(2), 403-434.

Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2011). Early childhood mathematics intervention.

Science, 333(6045), 968-970.

Page 26: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

26

Common Core State Standards Initiative (2010). Common core state standards

for mathematics. Retrieved from: http://www.corestandards.org/Math.

Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C.,

Klebanov, P., ... & Japel, C. (2007). School readiness and later achievement.

Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1428.

Duncan, G. J., Engel, M., Claessens, A., & Dowsett, C. J. (2014). Replication and

Robustness in Developmental Research. Developmental Psychology. 50(11),

2417

Engel, M., Claessens, A., & Finch, M. A. (2013). Teaching students what they

already know? The (Mis) Alignment between mathematics instructional

content and student knowledge in kindergarten. Educational Evaluation and

Policy Analysis, 35(2), 157-178.

Ginsburg, H., Lee, J., & Boyd, J. (2008). Mathematics education for young

children: What it is and how to promote it. Society for Research in Child

Development, Social Policy Report, 22, 3-22.

Lieberman, M. (2012, March 18). Results we can believe in: Shaping up

psychological science. Psychology Today. Retrieved from

http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/social-brain-social-

mind/201203/results-we-can-believe-in

Page 27: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

27

Makel, M. C., & Plucker, J. A. (2014). Facts Are More Important Than Novelty

Replication in the Education Sciences. Educational Researcher,

0013189X14545513.

Miller, E., & Almon, J. (2009). Alliance for Childhood. College Park, MD:

Alliance.

National Association for the Education of Young Children and National Council

of Teachers of Mathematics (NAEYC and NCTM) (2002). Position statement.

Early childhood mathematics: Promoting good beginnings. Retrieved

December 21, 2009 from

http://www.naeyc.org/positionstatements/mathematics

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM) (2007). Position

Statement. What is Important in Early Childhood Mathematics? Retrieved

December 21, 2009 from http://www.nctm.org/about/content.aspx?id=12590

National Mathematics Advisory Panel (2008). Foundations for success: The final

report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Washington DC: U.S.

Department of Education.

Nosek, B.A., Spies, J. R., & Motyl, M. (2012). Scientific Utopia: II. Restructuring

incentives and practices to promote truth over publishability. Perspectives on

Psychological Science, 7, 615-631.

Porter, A., McMaken, J. Hwang, J., Yang, R. (2011). Common core standards the

new US intended curriculum. Educational Researcher 40, 103-116.

Page 28: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

28

Russell, J. L. (2011). From Child’s Garden to Academic Press The Role of

Shifting Institutional Logics in Redefining Kindergarten Education.

American Educational Research Journal, 48(2), 236-267.

Schmidt, W. H. (2004). A vision for mathematics. Educational Leadership, 61(5),

6-11.

Schmidt, W. H., & Burroughs, N. A. (2013). How the Common Core Boosts

Quality and Equality. Educational Leadership, 70(4), 54-58.

Watts, T. W., Duncan, G. J., Siegler, R. S., & Davis-Kean, P. E. (2014). What’s

Past Is Prologue Relations Between Early Mathematics Knowledge and High

School Achievement. Educational Researcher, 0013189X14553660.

Page 29: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

29

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for children and teachers from the ECLS-K and the ECLS-K:2011.

ECLS-K:1998-99 ECLS-K:2010-11 Difference P-Value

Child Characteristics

Full Day Kindergarten 0.61 0.87 0.26 0.001

White 0.58 0.52 -0.06 0.001

African American 0.15 0.13 -0.02 0.010

Hispanic 0.19 0.25 0.06 0.001

Asian 0.03 0.04 0.01 0.001

Other 0.05 0.05 0 0.029

Female 0.49 0.49 0 0.828

Age (in months at fall assessment) 68.44 68.48 0.04 0.579

Observations 17810 15090

Teacher Characteristics

Female 0.98 0.98 0.00 0.826

White 0.86 0.83 -0.03 0.004

Hispanic 0.04 0.08 0.04 0.001

African American 0.07 0.06 -0.01 0.709

Years teaching kindergarten 8.24 8.74 0.50 0.033

Observations 3020 3060

Note. Means were generated with the sample weights designed for non-response for each type of survey used. See

supplementary materials for information regarding the weights used. All non-missing cases for each variable were

used to calculate each respective mean. Values in the "difference" column represent the change in average teacher

and student characteristics between1998-1999 and 2010-2011. P-values denote the level of statistical significance

for the average change for each characteristic. Statistical significance was calculated using dummy-variable

regressions in which the two cohorts were pooled into a single dataset. P-values lower than .001 were rounded to

.001.

Page 30: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

30

Table 2. Cronbach's alphas, means, and standard deviations for Mathematics Content Measures created from teacher

survey items.

ECLS-K:1998-1999 ECLS-K:2010-2011

Mean

difference P-Value

Alignment Measures

%

Mastered

1998

α Mean

(days/month) SD

α

Mean

(days/month) SD

Basic Counting & Shapes (4 items) 92 0.58 12.73 4.16

0.58 13.17 3.94 0.44 0.925

Patterns & Measurement (5 items) 55 0.78 7.55 4.47

0.78 7.84 4.43 0.29 0.071

Place Value & Currency (4 items) 21 0.60 8.47 5.18

0.59 10.76 5.30 2.29 0.001

Addition and Subtraction (2 items) 3 0.88 7.84 6.57

0.86 9.66 6.52 1.82 0.001

CCSSM Content Area Measures

Counting and Cardinality (5 items)

0.59 11.46 3.99

0.60 12.79 3.77 1.33 0.001

Operations and Algebraic Thinking (2 items) 0.88 7.84 6.57

0.86 9.66 6.52 1.82 0.001

Numbers and Operations in Base Ten (2 items) 0.59 9.71 6.96

0.56 12.60 6.76 2.89 0.001

Measurement and Data (3 items)

0.78 8.12 5.28

0.76 8.55 5.25 0.43 0.006

Geometry (2 items)

0.49 9.28 5.62

0.59 9.42 5.79 0.14 0.006

Note. Means were generated with the sample weights designed for non-response in each cohort for the spring kindergarten teacher survey. See

the appendix text for information regarding the weights used. Only observations in the analysis samples were considered (ECLS-K-1999:

teacher N= 3020; ECLS-K:2011: teacher N= 3060), and all non-missing cases on each variable were used to calculate each respective mean.

See the appendix for means for each item used to generate the content measures. Values in the "difference" column represent the change in the

amount of time teachers spent on each topic between 1998-1999 and 2010-2011. Positive values indicate that teachers reported spending more

time on a given topic in 2010-2011. P-values denote the level of statistical significance in each reported content measure average change,

which were estimated using dummy-variable regressions in which both cohorts were pooled as one dataset. P-values lower than .001 were

rounded to .001.

Page 31: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

31

Table 3. Coefficients and standard errors from ordinary least squares regressions

predicting spring of kindergarten mathematics achievement with Alignment and

Common Core Measures of mathematics content.

1998-99 2010-11 1998-99 2010-11

Independent Variables (1) (2) (3) (4)

Alignment Measures

Basic Counting and Shapes

-

0.031*** -0.023**

(0.006) (0.007)

Patterns and Measurement -0.015* -0.020**

(0.006) (0.007)

Place Value and Currency 0.035*** 0.042***

(0.006) (0.006)

Addition and Subtraction 0.037*** 0.046***

(0.006) (0.007)

CCSSM Content Area Measures

Counting and Cardinality

0.013* 0.019**

(0.006) (0.007)

Operations and Algebraic

Thinking

0.036*** 0.044***

(0.006) (0.007)

Numbers in Base Ten

0.020*** 0.028***

(0.005) (0.006)

Measurement and Data

-0.011 -0.005

(0.006) (0.007)

Geometry

-0.027*** -0.040***

(0.006) (0.007)

Covariates X X X X

Observations 17810 15090 17810 15090

Note. Coefficients and standard errors were generated from 10 MI datasets for each

cohort, and both datasets were weighted with the "svyset" commands in Stata 13.0

according to the "base year" weights designed for use with student, parent, and teacher-

level measures. Standard errors were adjusted to account for sample designed clustering.

Standard errors are in parentheses. All coefficients displayed were generated from fully-

controlled models. See Appendix Table 4 for a full list of covariates. * p < .05, ** p <

.01, *** p < .001

Page 32: KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT MATHEMATICS ... also noted the importance of mathematics instruction ... about whether and how the mathematics content taught in kindergarten

KINDERGARTEN MATHEMATICS MISALIGNMENT

32

Figure 1. Student mastery rates and teacher reported coverage of mathematics content in kindergarten.

12.73

7.84

13.17

9.66

0

4

8

12

16

20

Days/Month Basic Counting & Shapes Days/Month Addition & Subtraction

1998-1999 2010-2011

In 1998, 92% of students had mastered Basic Counting & Shapes at kindergarten entry, whereas only 3% had mastered basic Addition & Subtraction.

92%

3%

Day

s p

er M

on

th