improving reading strategies through peer response - … yang.pdf · improving reading strategies...

22
Improving reading strategies through peer response 3 Secondary English Education, 9(1) Improving reading strategies through peer response Jungwan Yang (Seoul National Univ. of Science and Technology) Yang, Jungwan. (2016). Improving reading strategies through peer response. Secondary English Education, 9(1), 3-24. This study is aimed at investigating how peer responses of Korean High school students work to improve studentsmetacognitive reading strategies. Depending on a quantitative research method and analysis, the finding of this investigation indicates that the peer response group employs more metacognitive reading strategies than the non- peer response group. In other words, this result points out that use of metacognitive reading strategies can be increased through peer response activities, which proves peer response plays an important role in enriching reading strategies. Considering the main result of this study, it is certain that Korean high school students need to be allowed opportunities to explore various reading strategies in English reading practice. Finally, this study discusses the finding from more educationally theoretical perspectives of interaction, Vytgotskys social ZPD concept, and integrative teaching methodology. I. INTRODUCTION Recently many language researchers have focused on reading strategies. They have paid attention to the importance of reading strategies which make language learners aware of the purpose and meaning of reading, which finally helps them to become good readers by enhancing overall language ability (Oxford, 2004). It is certain that metacognitive, cognitive, and socio-affective strategies are important in language learning. However this study focuses on metacognitive reading strategies because students without metacogtnitive approaches are learners without direction or opportunity to monitor their

Upload: vominh

Post on 06-Feb-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Improving reading strategies through peer response 3

Secondary English Education, 9(1)

Improving reading strategies through peer response

Jungwan Yang

(Seoul National Univ. of Science and Technology)

Yang, Jungwan. (2016). Improving reading strategies through peer response. Secondary English Education, 9(1), 3-24.

This study is aimed at investigating how peer responses of Korean High school students work to improve students’ metacognitive reading strategies. Depending on a quantitative research method and analysis, the finding of this investigation indicates that the peer response group employs more metacognitive reading strategies than the non- peer response group. In other words, this result points out that use of metacognitive reading strategies can be increased through peer response activities, which proves peer response plays an important role in enriching reading strategies. Considering the main result of this study, it is certain that Korean high school students need to be allowed opportunities to explore various reading strategies in English reading practice. Finally, this study discusses the finding from more educationally theoretical perspectives of interaction, Vytgotsky’s social ZPD concept, and integrative teaching methodology.

I. INTRODUCTION

Recently many language researchers have focused on reading strategies.

They have paid attention to the importance of reading strategies which make

language learners aware of the purpose and meaning of reading, which finally

helps them to become good readers by enhancing overall language ability

(Oxford, 2004). It is certain that metacognitive, cognitive, and socio-affective

strategies are important in language learning. However this study focuses on

metacognitive reading strategies because students without metacogtnitive

approaches are learners without direction or opportunity to monitor their

4 Jungwan Yang

progress, accomplishments, and future directions (O’Malley, Chamot, &

Kupper, (1989). A metacognitive approach makes students aware of the

purpose of reading to activate, monitor, regulate, and make sense out of text,

which finally helps students to become skilled readers (Sheorey & Mokhtari,

2001). It also leads language learners to be independent in their learning,

being aware of their learning process.

The importance of metacognitive strategies has been positively studied in

terms of constructively responsive readers using them (Anderson, 2003;

Pressley & Afflerbach, 1995; Yeo, 2006). This theory is grounded on that

idea that learners can benefit from interaction to enhance their learning

achievements (Mooney, 2000). In short, successful language learners must

have meaningful opportunities to interact with other interactants such as a

teacher, peers and texts. In this context, peer collaboration has also been

discussed from the view of social constructionism in which knowledge is

negotiated and best acquired through interaction (Kurt & Atay, 2007). That

is, peer collaboration is regarded to provide learners with an opportunity to

read or write their drafts and to discuss them with a peer audience while

their products or activities are taking shape. Classroom discussion as a peer

response also can be a positive aspect in supporting all phases of the

language learning process (Reid, 1994).

This study aims to explore how these two important fields in language

learning, namely metacognitive reading strategies and peer collaboration, are

interconnected to enhance learners’metacognitive reading strategy. This study

is in the same vain with the fact that reading strategies are conscious and

intended and controllable actions; therefore, readers’ comprehension abilities

can be advanced by extending reading strategies (Philip, 2005). In short, this

study is designed to investigate how Korean high school students perform

metacognitive reading strategies through employing peer response. The

following questions were framed for the research:

1) How do Korean high school students perform metacognitive strategies

while reading English texts?

2) How are peer response activities related to learners’ reading meta-

cognitive strategies?

Improving reading strategies through peer response 5

II. LITERATURE REVIEW

1. Metacognitive Reading Strategy

There are various definitions of metacognitve strategies, but O’Malley, et.

al (1989) highlights the importance of metacognition in contrast to cognitive

strategies; cognitive strategies are more directly related to individual learning

tasks and contain direct manipulation or transformation of the learning

materials, while metacognitive strategies are more involved with thinking

about the learning process, planning for studying, monitoring of

comprehension, and self-evaluation of learning after the language activity is

completed. Carrell (1998) also summarized metacognitive strategies in

reading: (a) clarifying the purposes of reading-understanding both the

explicit and implicit task demands; (b) identifying the important aspects of a

message; (c) focusing attention on the major content; (d) monitoring ongoing

activities to comprehend; (e) engaging in self-questioning to determine

whether goals are being achieved, and (f) taking corrective action when

comprehension fails.

Furthermore, other studies have continued to show the role of

metacognitive strategies in learning: language learners’reading comprehension

and adjusting their reading rates (Oxford, 1990), and positive academic

learning experience and motivation (Paris & Winograd, 1990). More recently,

researchers have pointed out more specific results of metacognitive reading

strategies. Metacognitive reading strategies can enhance learners’

achievements by becoming aware of their own ideas and experiences and by

activating effective problem-solving strategies (Israel, Bauserman, &

Kinnucan-Welsch, 2005); metacognitive reading strategies can help

language learners to better comprehend texts. In other words, awareness and

monitoring of readers’ comprehension processes are critically important

aspects of skilled reading (Yang, 2014). Like this, many researchers point

out the significance of metacognitve knowledge and they claim that reading

strategy instruction should be taught through a metacognitive approach

(Walczyk, 2000). Whatever researchers’ interests are, they assert together

that language learners should activate their learning by becoming aware of

6 Jungwan Yang

their own thinking as they read, write, and by getting informed of effective

metacognitive reading strategies.

2. Peer Response

The interaction and the negotiation in the peer collaboration activity was

highlighted by Vygotsky’s theory of the Zone of Proximal Development

(ZPD). ZPD is one of the theories that support peer collaboration, which

places a greater emphasis on collaborative learning (Ferris & Hedgcock,

2005). Vygotsky’s ZPD (1978) states that social interaction is necessary for

the internalization of thought and language, and therefore collaborative

language learning could contribute to the negotiation and collaboration in the

internalization process, which finally leads to improved language abilities

(DiPardo & Freedman, 1987). The improved language abilities are grounded

on ZPD as the distance between the most difficult task someone can do

alone and the most difficult task someone can do with help (Mason, Harris, &

Graham, 2011).

In terms of reading and writing, ZPD explains why and how the students’

writing skill can be developed through peer feedback. While having peer

feedback, students can read their peers’ writings, as well as writing their

own papers. In reading their peers’ papers, they become aware of the

position of audience and this awareness makes them to be critical readers

(Rollinson, 2005). Besides, responses given by the student-reader can

support the student-writer to make meaningful development due to their

critical awareness of writing and reading (Clark, 2003). In short, peer

responses can help students’ development as the writer and the reader at

the same time (Kurt & Atay, 2007). In this study, the term ‘peer response’

is preferred by the researcher to investigate how students’ reading strategies

are enriched through interacting each other about their writing products.

Another rationale of peer response is based on the paradigm shift in

writing; the change from emphasis on product to emphasis on process

(Dyson & Freedman, 2002). This view of writing in progress admits an

active feature of writing which values what writers do as they write, not just

what writers produce (Dyson & Freedman, 2002). It is in line with the social

Improving reading strategies through peer response 7

aspects of writing and learning to write. Other researchers stated that the

process of peer response leads to development in the writer’s sense of

audience, voice, and power in the language of L2 children. The collaboration

of peer response enables students to develop critical thinking more

effectively than students working individually on the same task. Peer

feedback can increase students’awareness of their mistakes between the

higher and lower level of interlocutor (Prater & Bermudez, 1993). In Clark’s

study (2003), writing is even better when it is to be read by their peers

than when they are written to be read by teacher.

Parallel to this theoretical framework, many researchers have investigated

what occurs during peer collaboration sessions. Peer response groups

“provide the writer with an immediate account of the dynamics of

reader-text interaction”(Gere & Stevens, 1985, p. 104). According to Lee

(2009), peer feedback activities tend to generate more comments on the

content, organization, and vocabulary. This result indicates that peer feedback

is not limited to corrections on peer’s writing, but it is more about

interaction, suggestion, and negotiation to generate meaningful opinions

toward another student’s writing. Given this point, peer feedback could allow

students to make negotiation of their strength and weakness (Hyland, 2004)

through exchanging their ideas, comments, corrections, and suggestions,

which provides opportunities for the students to be better in writing and also

reading (Jiao, 2007; Kamimura, 2006).

Based on the above theoretical frame, this study is to investigate how the

peer response works in improving learners’metacognitive reading strategies

(Jiao, 2007; Kamimura, 2006).

III. METHODOLOGY

1. Sites

This study was conducted in a special program in which the researcher

was involved in teaching high school students for the winter vacation under a

special project supported from the administration for local autonomy. The

8 Jungwan Yang

purpose of this program was to increase students’awareness of how to study

Math and English. The English program was held across 10 days that

consisted mainly of reading and writing, and each day was composed of three

50 minute sessions. The participants in this study were 60 high school

students in the 10th grade from various schools. The 60 students were

divided into two classes with each class having 30 students whose English

competencies varied. When grouping classes, after the placement test, the

main focus was to distribute advanced, medium, low level students equally in

the same class.

In the reading class, students were required to comprehend the meaning of

the main story, and to write about the issues dealt with in the main readings

as a warm-up or post-reading activity. In sum, students read the textbook,

Reading Explorer 1 published by National Geographic Learning in 2015, and

they explored 4 topics such as Amazing Animals, The Power of Music, Into

Space, and Stories and Storytellers. For this investigation, the researcher as

a teacher prepared a form and a rubric to help students doing peer feedback

(Appendix 1). The feedback form was designed to promote interaction with

each other discussing questions in regards to the clarity of the idea, the

completeness of the elements, and the schematic structures of the writing.

2. Process

Class A as a control group was asked to write, without peer collaboration,

their responses to issues discussed in the main reading. These students

subsequently received feedback from a teacher. In the last 50 minutes class,

students revised their writings following the teacher feedback. Class B as an

experimental group was required to write, with peer collaboration, their

responses to issues discussed in the main reading, and then was to exchange

their feedbacks to their peers’ writings and discussed to clarify their ideas.

In three 50 minutes class, 50 minutes were allotted to peer responses, and

after the peer activity, the teacher collected their writings and gave feedback

of each product.

On the first session, the teacher explained to both classes the scheme of

academic writing including content, organization, word choice, grammar, and

Improving reading strategies through peer response 9

authentic ownership. Both classes also received a rubric for measuring the

overall quality of the paper. This holistic scoring rubric was used to increase

students’awareness of writing by being involved with the concept of audience,

purpose, content, organization, and language use. Students were then asked

to apply this guideline to their own writings. However, only the experimental

group-class B was introduced to the concept of peer collaboration and peer

response activity. Students of Class B were in groups of 4 to 5 members for

peer response activity. To help students for this activity, the teacher gave

them some questions to prompt interaction with each other as they reviewed

their drafts.

These questions are based on meaning-based feedback to ask how writers

explore ideas to choose and then decide which ones to incorporate into their

organizing ideas. They read their group members’ drafts and made their

responses. Feedback given by peers could be spoken or written feedback.

This research focuses on the written feedback given by peers to improve

their writing, especially narrative writing. Written peer feedback was given in

form of marks, written comments, written correction, and there was a form

provided for students to give more suggestions. Students then revised their

drafts based on the discussions after the peer response in class. Students of

Class A, however, revised individually. A narrative text, as well as other text

types, should be written in a process oriented scheme where peer feedback

activities can take place. Generally, peer feedback might be useful to enhance

students’ awareness on the grammatical mistakes and mechanical mistakes.

For the example, a narrative that tells a story in the past necessitates

mostly past tense (Feez & Joyce, 2002). Students not aware of this aspect

might use inappropriate tenses in their narrative, and peer feedback can

stimulate their awareness on this kind of mistake. Mechanically, the correct

use of punctuation in the (narrative) writing is also important to shape the

meaning; here peer feedback may generate correction when the mistakes are

observed by the students. Specifically, peer feedback is useful to develop the

idea, content, clarity, mechanics, and the organization of the students’

narrative writing (Clark, 2003).

10 Jungwan Yang

3. Data Collection and Analysis

This research employed a self-report measure, the Meta-cognitive

Awareness of Reading Strategies Inventory (MARSI). MARSI was developed

by Sheorey and Mokhtari (2001) to assess students’ metacognitive

awareness and their perceived use of reading strategies while reading

academic or school-related materials. As a quantitative questionnaire, it

provides a valid and reliable instrument aimed at measuring readers’

meta-cognitive awareness

(Sheorey & Mokhtari, 2001). This questionnaire was influenced from

Rosenblatt’s (1978) reader response theory which emphasized the interaction

between readers and the text. This self-report instrument asks how readers

consider and perceive their reading strategies. In other words, this

questionnaire reveals how readers plan, monitor, evaluate and use information

when they comprehend their reading contexts. It is said that students

reflections could follow the readers’ thinking processes as descriptions of

metacognition (Sheorey & Mokhtari, 2001).

MARSI was originally performed using three factors (Global Reading

Strategies, Problem-Solving Strategies, and Support Reading Strategies) and

its reliability for the total 30 entries of the questionnaire was .89 (Sheorey

& Mokhtari, 2001). However, in this study, MARSI was modified to 24

entries for high school students. In this revised MARSI, the first factor

(Global Reading Strategies) had 10 items representing a set of reading

strategies oriented toward a global analysis of text. The second factor

(Problem-Solving Strategies) includes 6 items that were around strategies

for solving problems when readers encountered difficulty in reading. As the

third factor, Support Reading Strategies of 8 items involved practical

strategies such as reference materials, taking notes, that were used as

functional or support strategies.

On the first day of class, students in both classes were introduced to a

strategic learning approach which was employed through reading texts and

they were required to mark on each entry, reflecting their attitudes toward

metacognitive reading strategies. After completing the program, students

were required again to rate the same questionnaire-how often they used the

Improving reading strategies through peer response 11

reading strategy described in that statement using a 5-point Likert-type

scale ranging from 1 (I never do this) to 5 (I always do this). The SPSS 16

program was used to analyze the data.

IV. FINDING

Investigating this study, the quantitative questionnaire test (MARSI) was

conducted twice before and after this English reading program. An

independent-samples t-test (Table 1) was conducted to compare class A

(control group) with class B (experimental group) in the pre-test. After

analyzing the T-test for equality of means for all question items between

Class A and Class B, the result shows that there was not a statistically

significant difference in both groups except the question number 3 (global

strategy- I think about what I know to help me understand what I read.) and

questions number 7(global strategy-I think about whether the content of the

text fits my reading.) The p-values of 22 questions out of total 24 are

greater than 0.05 (p<0.05), while question 2 and question 7 showed a

statistically significant difference: question 2 in Class A (M=3.10, SD=

1.155) and Class B (M =2.47, SD=1.074), condition; t(58)=2, p=0.032;

question 2 in Class A (M=2.50, SD=1.225) and Class B(M=1.80, SD=.761),

conditions; t(58)=2.658, p=0.011.

<Table 1> Independent Sample Test

Independent Sample Test

 

Levene’s Test for

Equality of Variances t-test for Equality of Means

F Sig. t df

Sig. (2-tailed)

Mean Differe

nceStd. Error Difference

95% Confidence

Interval of the Difference

Lower Upper

Q 3(pre)

Equal variances assumed

.119 .732 2.199 58 .032 .633 .288 .057 1.210

Equal variances not assumed

    2.199 57.697 .032 .633 .288 .057 1.210

12 Jungwan Yang

As the statistical results (Appendix 2 & 3) show, both groups increase the

means score of MARSI. This result shows that the experimental group used

relatively fewer metacognitive reading strategies than control group before

being involved in this research, but after this program, the experimental

group employed more metacognitive reading strategies than control group in

this investigation.

The following <Table 2> shows that the control group has 10 items

showing a significantly big difference in pre and post test.

<Table 2> Paired Sample Test of Control Group

Q 2. I take notes while reading to help me understand what I read.Q 6. I summarize what I read to reflect on important information in the text.Q 7. I think about whether the content of the text fits my reading purpose.Q 10. I skim the text first by noting characteristics like length and organization.

Q 7(pre)

Equal variances assumed

8.424

.005 2.659 58 .010 .700 .263 .173 1.227

Equal variances not assumed

    2.659 48.493 .011 .700 .263 .171 1.229

Paired Samples Testa

 Group =Control

Paired Differences

t dfSig.

(2-tailed)MeanStd.

Deviation

Std. Error Mean

95% Confidence Interval of the

Difference

Lower Upper

Q 2SUP1(pre) - SUP1(post)

-.533 .937 .171 -.883 -.183 -3.117 29 .004

Q 6SUP3(pre) - SUP3(post)

-1.233 .728 .133 -1.505 -.962 -9.280 29 .000

Q 7GLOB4(pre) - GLOB4(post)

-.367 .850 .155 -.684 -.049 -2.362 29 .025

Q 10GLOB5(pre) - GLOB5(post)

-.500 .900 .164 -.836 -.164 -3.042 29 .005

Q 11PROB2(pre) - PROB2(post)

-.300 .750 .137 -.580 -.020 -2.192 29 .037

Q 14GLOB6(pre) - GLOB6(post)

-.500 .861 .157 -.822 -.178 -3.181 29 .003

Q 18PROB5(pre) - PROB5(post)

-.633 .999 .182 -1.007 -.260 -3.471 29 .002

Q 19GLOB8(pre) - GLOB8(post)

-.467 .776 .142 -.756 -.177 -3.294 29 .003

Q 20SUP7(pre) - SUP7(post)

-.533 .681 .124 -.788 -.279 -4.287 29 .000

Q 21PROB6(pre) - PROB6(post)

-.833 .834 .152 -1.145 -.522 -5.473 29 .000

Improving reading strategies through peer response 13

Q 11. I try to get back on track when I lose concentration.Q 14, I decide what to read closely and what to ignore.Q 18 I stop from time to time and think about what I’’m reading.Q 19 I use context clues to help me better understand what I’’m reading.Q 20 I paraphrase (restate ideas in my own words) to better understand what I read Q 21 I try to picture or visualize information to help me remember what I read.

Explaining some questions in more detail, Question 2 in control group “I

take notes while reading to help me understand what I read,” showed a

significant difference: pre-test(M=1.57, SD=.728); for post-test (2.10,

SD=1.062); t(29)=-3.117, p=.004. Question 6“I summarize what I read to

reflect on important information in the text” also showed a significant

difference(pre-test: M=2.00, SD=.788; post-test: M=3.23, SD=.728;

t(29)=-9.280, p=.000); Question 7 “I think about whether the content of the

text fits my reading purpose”(pre-test; M=2.50, SD=1.225; post-test;

M=2.87, SD=1.137; t(29)=-2.362, p=0.025); Question 10 ‘I skim the text

first by noting characteristics like length and organization’(pre-test: M=3.00,

SD=1.145; post-test; M=3.50, SD=1.137; t(29)=-3.042, p=0.05); Question

11’ I try to get back on track when I lose concentration’(pre-test: M=3.47,

SD=1.074; post-test; M=3.77, SD=1.006; t(29)=-2.192, p=0.37).

However, the following <Table 3> indicates that the experimental group

has 16 items that significantly increased in the post- test.

<Table 3> Paired Sample Test of Experimental Group

Paired Samples Testa

 Group= Experimental

Paired Differences

t dfSig.

(2-tailed)Mean

Std. Deviatio

n

Std. Error Mean

95% Confidence Interval of the

Difference

Lower Upper

Q 1GLOB1(pre) - GLOB1(post)

-.433 .935 .171 -.783 -.084 -2.538 29 .017

Q 2SUP1(pre) - SUP1(post)

-.733 1.172 .214 -1.171 -.296 -3.426 29 .002

Q 3GLOB2(pre) - GLOB2(post)

-.633 .890 .162 -.966 -.301 -3.898 29 .001

Q 6SUP3(pre) - SUP3(post)

-1.367 .999 .182 -1.740 -.993 -7.490 29 .000

Q 7GLOB4(pre) - GLOB4(post)

-.900 .803 .147 -1.200 -.600 -6.139 29 .000

Q 9SUP4(pre) - SUP4(post)

-2.067 1.048 .191 -2.458 -1.675 -10.798 29 .000

14 Jungwan Yang

Q 1. 1 I have a purpose in mind when I read. Q 2 I take notes while reading to help me understand what I read.Q 3, I think about what I know to help me understand what I read.Q 6, I summarize what I read to reflect on important information in the text.Q 7, I think about whether the content of the text fits my reading purpose.Q 9, I discuss what I read with others to check my understanding.Q 14, I decide what to read closely and what to ignore.Q 16, When the text becomes difficult, I pay closer attention to what I’’m reading.Q 17, I use tables, figures, and pictures in the text to increase my understanding.Q 18, I stop from time to time and think about what I’’m readingQ 19, I use context clues to help me better understand what I’’m reading.Q 20, I paraphrase (restate ideas in my own words) to better understand what I readQ 21, I try to picture or visualize information to help me remember what I read. Q 22, I use typographical aids like boldface and italics to identify key informationQ 23, I critically analyze and evaluate the information presented in the text.Q 24, I go back and forth in the text to find relationships among ideas in it.

Here, the above results are discussed in more detailed. First, Question 1,

3, 7, 16, 17, 22, 23, and 24 were ranked as the most used strategies in the

experimental group, but Question 10 and 11 as the most used strategies in

the control group were not listed in the experimental group as the most used

strategies. Question number 1‘I have a purpose in mind when I read’in

experimental group showed a statistically significant difference; (pre-test:

M=2.30, SD=1.022; post-test; M=2.73, SD=.868; t(29)=-2.538, p=0.17);

Question 3‘I think about what I know to help me understand what I read‘

(pre-test: M=2.93, SD=1.081; post-test; M=3.10, SD=.803; t(29)=-3.898,

Q 14GLOB6(pre) - GLOB6(post)

-.733 1.081 .197 -1.137 -.330 -3.717 29 .001

Q 16PROB4(pre) - PROB4(post)

-.833 .950 .173 -1.188 -.479 -4.805 29 .000

Q 17GLOB7(pre) - GLOB7(post)

-.433 .774 .141 -.722 -.144 -3.067 29 .005

Q 18PROB5(pre) - PROB5(post)

-1.067 1.015 .185 -1.446 -.688 -5.757 29 .000

Q 19GLOB8(pre) - GLOB8(post)

-.900 .845 .154 -1.215 -.585 -5.835 29 .000

Q 20SUP7(pre) - SUP7(post)

-1.233 .971 .177 -1.596 -.871 -6.954 29 .000

Q 21PROB6(pre) - PROB6(post)

-1.433 1.305 .238 -1.921 -.946 -6.017 29 .000

Q 22GLOB9(pre) - GLOB9(post)

-.767 1.223 .223 -1.223 -.310 -3.434 29 .002

Q 23GLOB10(pre) - GLOB10(post)

-1.967 .890 .162 -2.299 -1.634 -12.104 29 .000

Q 24SUP8(pre) - SUP8(post)

-.467 .776 .142 -.756 -.177 -3.294 29 .003

Improving reading strategies through peer response 15

p=0.01); Question 7 ‘I think about whether the content of the text fits my

reading purpose‘(pre-test: M=2.87, SD=1.137; post-test; M=2.70,

SD=.877; t(29)=-6.139, p=0.000); Question 16 ‘When the text becomes

difficult, I pay closer attention to what I’‘m reading‘(pre-test: M=3.27,

SD=.944; post-test; M=3.80, SD=.925; t(29)=-4.805, p=0.000); Question

17 ‘I use tables, figures, and pictures in the text to increase my

understanding,(pre-test: M=3.73, SD=.828; post-test; M=3.60, SD=1.003;

t(29)=-3.067, p=0.005); Question 22 ‘I use typographical aids like boldface

and italics to identify key information‘(pre-test: M=2.83, SD=.791;

post-test; M=3.00, SD=.947; t(29)=-3.434, p=0.002).

Next, as the most noticeable statistical result, the following two

strategies were used in the experimental group; Question 23 ‘I critically

analyze and evaluate the information presented in the text‘(pre-test:

M=1.80, SD=.407; post-test; M=3.50, SD=.861; t(29)=-12.104, p=0.000);

Question 24 ‘I go back and forth in the text to find relationships among

ideas in it‘(pre-test: M=3.20, SD=.805; post-test; M=3.50, SD=.731;

t(29)=-3.294, p=0.003.

V. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION

This research investigated how peer responses worked to improve

students’ metacognitive reading strategy. As a conclusion, the following

reading strategies were increased through the peer response process using

writing as a product; I have a purpose in mind when I read; I think about

what I know to help me understand what I read; When the text becomes

difficult, I pay closer attention to what I’’m reading; I use tables, figures, and

pictures in the text to increase my understanding; I critically analyze and

evaluate the information presented in the text; I go back and forth in the

text to find relationships among ideas in it.

The findings of this study revealed that the peer response group employed

more metacognitive reading strategies than the non- peer response group,

which indicated peer response played an important role in enriching reading

strategies. In other words, Korean high school students were allowed

16 Jungwan Yang

opportunities to explore and experiment with effective reading strategies in

English writing and reading practice, through which they came to improve

their metacognitive reading strategies. Here, this study discusses some

significant issues emerging from this result.

First, the fact that the peer response group used more metacognitve

reading strategies can be explained by students’ interactions. When they

interacted with each other to clarify their writings through asking and

answering, they discussed naturally the main topic, intention, and meaning of

the passage. At the same time, they tried to analyze critically and evaluate

their peers’ writings as the reader. This process could stimulate not only

students’awareness of their faulty or insufficient writing skills, but also their

reading strategies. This attitude naturally led to identifying, monitoring, and

engaging their reading and writing process, which are the main aspects of

metacognitive reading strategies (강동호, 2012). This self-awareness

provided a chance to reflect on their writings through reading their peers’

drafts, which could link to metacognitive reading strategies. It means that

peer feedback does not merely give chance to comment or correct peer’s

drafts, but it also provides opportunities that students need to apply

metacognitive reading strategies naturally through the process of peer

responses. This process seems to makes the students more critical on their

own reading. This proves what Rollinson (2005) stated that peer feedback

also trains students to be critical reader on their own writing.

Considering learning is a process, this peer response approach deals with

interaction and cooperation to negotiate, discuss, persuade and communicate

(Kim, 2014). In sum, peer collaboration can engage language learners in a

collaborative environment rather than a teacher- directed approach in class.

Instead of receiving prescriptive comments from teachers, peers would

interact with one another’s writing as readers, and challenge and negotiate

meaning during their cooperation (Prater & Bermudez, 1993). Accordingly,

this interaction improves language students’reading skills. This point is also

in line with Vygotsky’s ZPD concept. That is, students in this study

interacted with each other about the products their peers made. This

interaction can be explored in the perspective of sociocultural aspect. When

the students were asked to write with a sense that it would be read by an

Improving reading strategies through peer response 17

authentic audience of peers, they came to seek for more efficient strategies

to organize and develop their ideas clearly. Furthermore, they wanted to

make the reader satisfied by clarifying their ideas and giving their own voice

about the topic.

Second, this study can be evidence that strategies can be taught in class.

The interactions between writing and reading can be implemented in a real

classroom to enhance reading strategies. This research method can give the

learners the opportunity to develop their metacogtnitive reading skills when

they need to write and read. A teacher should encourage students’ interest,

enjoyment. and motivation to study (Graham, Gillespie & McKeown, 2013).

The reading strategies will help students place the developmental stages of

metacognition into perspective with appropriate strategies. Language learners

need to learn when to use various metacognitive processes which make them

aware of their learning processes and monitor their cognitive processes. In

this study, peer response activities were used to explore students’

metacognitive reading strategies. Through interacting their responses,

students can reflect on their thinking process, which leads to metacognitive

reading strategies consciously and unconsciously. This result is hoped to

support the fact that language learners come to have more responsibility for

their studies by generating, applying and monitoring effective strategies while

interacting each other, which is the argument of this investigation.

REFERENCES

강동호. (2012). 영어 읽기, 쓰기 교수모형: 한국 중등학교 사례연구. 한국 중

등영어교육학회 5(2), 97-119.

Anderson, N. (2003). Scrolling, clicking and reading English: Online

reading strategies in a second/foreign language. The Reading

Matrix.

Carrell, P. L. (1998). Can reading strategies be successfully taught? The

Language Teacher, 22, 7-14.

Clark, E. (2003). First Language Acquisition. Cambridge University Press.

DiPardo, A., & Freedman, S. W. (1987). Historical overview: Groups

18 Jungwan Yang

in the writing classroom(Technical Report No. 4). Berkeley,

CA: Center for the Study of Writing. Dyson, A. H., & Freedman, S. W. (2002). Writing, In J. Flood, Diane

Lapp (Eds.), Handbook of Research on Teaching the English

Language Arts. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

967-991

Ferris, D. R., & Hedgcock, J. S. (2005). Teaching ESL composition; Purpose,

process and practice (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Feez, S., & Joyce, H. (2002). Text-Based Syllabus Design. Sydney:

NCELTR, Macquarie University.

Gere, A. R., & Stevens, R. S. (1985). The language of writing groups: How

oral response shapes revision. In S. W. Freedman (Ed.), The

acquisition of written language (pp. 85-105). Norwood, NJ: Ablex

Publishing Corporation.

Graham, S., Gillespie, A., & McKeown, D. (2013). Writing: importance,

development, and instruction. Reading and Writing, 26(1), 1-15.

Hyland, K. (2004). Genre and second language writing. Ann Arbor:

University of Michigan Press.

Israel, S., C. Block, Bauserman, K., & Kinnucan-Welsch, K. (2005).

Metacognition in literacy learning: theory, assessment,

instruction and professional development. Mahwah: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associate Inc.

Jiao, L. Y. (2007). Application of cooperative learning in teaching college

English writing. US-China Foreign Language (ISSN 1539-8080).

http://www.articlesbase.com/language -articles.

Kamimura, T. (2006). Effects of peer feedback on EFL student writers at

different levels of English proficiency: A Japanese context. TESL

Canada Journal, 23(2), 12-39.

Kim, J. S. (2014). Language learning potential of writing in an additional

language. Secondary English Education, 7(4). 19-43.

Kurt, G., & Atay, D. (2007). The effects of peer feedback on the writing

anxiety of prospective Turkish teachers of EFL. Journal of Theory

and Practice in Education, 3(1), 12-23.

Improving reading strategies through peer response 19

Lee, N. S. (2009). Written peer feedback by EFL students: Praise, criticism,

and suggestion. Komaba Journal of English Education, 1, 129-139.

Mason, L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (2011). Self-regulated

strategy development for students with writing difficulties.

Theory Into Practice, 50(1), 20-27.

Mooney, C. G. (2000). Theories of childhood: An introduction to

Dewey, Montessori, Erikson, Piaget, & Vygotsky. St. Paul, MN: Redleaf Press.O’ Malley, J. M., Chamot, A. U., & Kupper, L. (1989). Listening

comprehension strategies in second language acquisition.

Applied Linguistics, 10(4), 418-437.

Oxford, R. (1990). Language learning strategies: What every teacher

should know. Boston: Heinle & Heinle.

Oxford, R. (2004). Effect of the presence and difficulty of task on

strategy use: an exploratory study. International Review of Applied

Linguistics, 42, 1-47.

Paris, S., & P. Winograd. (1990). How metacognition can promote

academic learning and instruction. In B. F. Jones & L. Idol

(Eds.), Dimensions of thinking and cognitive instruction (pp.

15-51). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

Philip, B. (2005). Towards a social-motivational metaconitive strategy

instructional model. University Kebangsaan Malaysia, Bangi.

Prater, D. L., & Bermudez, A. B. (1993). Using peer response groups with limited English proficient writers. The Bilingual Research Journal, 17(1 & 2), 99-116.Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The

nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbarm.

Reid, J. (1994). Responding to ESL students’ texts: The myths of

appropriation. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 273-292.

Rollinson, P. (2005). Using peer feedback in the ESL writing class.

ELT Journal, 59(1), 23-30.

Rosenblatt, L. M. (1978). The Reader: the text: The poem. Carbondale,

IL: Southern Illinois University.

Sheorey, R., & K. Mokhtari, (2001). Differences in the metacognitive

20 Jungwan Yang

awareness of reading strategies among native and non-native

readers. System 29, 431-449.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher

psychological processes. Cambridge, M.A: Harvard University Press.

Walczyk, J. (2000). The interplay between automatic and control

processes in reading. Reading Research Quarterly 35(4).

554-566.

Yeo, K. (2006). Automatization in Second Language Learning. British

American Language and Literature, 81, 205-224.

Yang, J. W. (2014). A study of the relationship between critical reading

development and metacognitive reading strategies. Secondary English

Education, 7(4), 45-65.

Improving reading strategies through peer response 21

Appendix 1: Peer response sheet for a meaning based feedback (Hansen & Liu,

2005)

______________________________________________________________________

Name of writer:

Name of reader:

1) What did you like best about my paper? Why?

2) Was it clear from the topic sentence what the problem was?

If not, what needs to be changed to make it clear?

3) Were all solutions relevant? Were they well supported?

Where can I add more details and support?

4) Was the order of the solutions logical and effective?

How can I reorganize them to make it more effective?

5) What transition words or phrases were needed to be added (or omitted)

to help you follow the paper better?

6) I have difficulty with __________________.

Please help me find any errors with this.

Appendix 2 (Paired Sample Statistics of Control Group)

Paired Samples Statisticsa

Mean NStd.

DeviationStd. Error

Mean

Pair 1GLOB1(pre) 2.70 30 .988 .180GLOB1(post) 2.93 30 .907 .166

Pair 2SUP1(pre) 1.57 30 .728 .133SUP1(post) 2.10 30 1.062 .194

Pair 3GLOB2(pre) 3.10 30 1.155 .211GLOB2(post) 2.93 30 1.081 .197

Pair 4GLOB3(pre) 2.90 30 .960 .175GLOB3(post) 3.07 30 1.112 .203

Pair 5SUP2(pre) 2.77 30 1.223 .223SUP2(post) 2.90 30 1.062 .194

Pair 6SUP3(pre) 2.00 30 .788 .144SUP3(post) 3.23 30 .728 .133

Pair 7GLOB4(pre) 2.50 30 1.225 .224GLOB4(post) 2.87 30 1.137 .208

Pair 8PROB1(pre) 3.17 30 1.020 .186PROB1(post) 3.33 30 .959 .175

Pair 9SUP4(pre) 1.87 30 .776 .142SUP4(post) 1.80 30 .805 .147

Pair 10GLOB5(pre) 3.00 30 1.145 .209GLOB5(post) 3.50 30 1.137 .208

22 Jungwan Yang

Appendix 3 (Paired Sample Statistics of Experimental Group)

Pair 11PROB2(pre) 3.47 30 1.074 .196PROB2(post) 3.77 30 1.006 .184

Pair 12SUP5(pre) 2.70 30 1.119 .204SUP5(post) 3.00 30 .910 .166

Pair 13PROB3(pre) 2.90 30 1.029 .188PROB3(post) 3.17 30 1.053 .192

Pair 14GLOB6(pre) 2.30 30 1.149 .210GLOB6(post) 2.80 30 .961 .176

Pair 15SUP6(pre) 3.00 30 1.017 .186SUP6(post) 3.27 30 .907 .166

Pair 16PROB4(pre) 3.33 30 .959 .175PROB4(post) 3.27 30 .944 .172

Pair 17GLOB7(pre) 3.47 30 1.106 .202GLOB7(post) 3.73 30 .828 .151

Pair 18PROB5(pre) 2.70 30 1.119 .204PROB5(post) 3.33 30 .884 .161

Pair 19GLOB8(pre) 2.67 30 .922 .168GLOB8(post) 3.13 30 .819 .150

Pair 20SUP7(pre) 2.10 30 .885 .162SUP7(post) 2.63 30 .928 .169

Pair 21PROB6(pre) 2.00 30 .871 .159PROB6(post) 2.83 30 .874 .160

Pair 22GLOB9(pre) 2.57 30 1.040 .190GLOB9(post) 2.83 30 .791 .145

Pair 23GLOB10(pre) 1.70 30 .535 .098GLOB10(post) 1.80 30 .407 .074

Pair 24SUP8(pre) 3.23 30 .858 .157SUP8(post) 3.20 30 .805 .147

a. Group = Control

Paired Samples Statisticsa

Mean NStd.

DeviationStd. Error

Mean

Pair 1GLOB1(pre) 2.30 30 1.022 .187GLOB1(post) 2.73 30 .868 .159

Pair 2SUP1(pre) 1.60 30 .894 .163SUP1(post) 2.33 30 .922 .168

Pair 3GLOB2(pre) 2.47 30 1.074 .196GLOB2(post) 3.10 30 .803 .147

Pair 4GLOB3(pre) 3.07 30 1.048 .191GLOB3(post) 3.30 30 .915 .167

Pair 5SUP2(pre) 2.23 30 1.331 .243SUP2(post) 2.40 30 1.354 .247

Improving reading strategies through peer response 23

Pair 6SUP3(pre) 1.67 30 .758 .138SUP3(post) 3.03 30 .669 .122

Pair 7GLOB4(pre) 1.80 30 .761 .139GLOB4(post) 2.70 30 .877 .160

Pair 8PROB1(pre) 3.07 30 1.143 .209PROB1(post) 3.50 30 1.106 .202

Pair 9SUP4(pre) 1.70 30 .702 .128SUP4(post) 3.77 30 .728 .133

Pair 10GLOB5(pre) 2.87 30 1.224 .224GLOB5(post) 3.20 30 1.270 .232

Pair 11PROB2(pre) 3.30 30 1.291 .236PROB2(post) 3.60 30 1.133 .207

Pair 12SUP5(pre) 2.67 30 1.295 .237SUP5(post) 2.97 30 1.377 .251

Pair 13PROB3(pre) 2.90 30 1.125 .205PROB3(post) 3.17 30 1.177 .215

Pair 14GLOB6(pre) 2.40 30 1.192 .218GLOB6(post) 3.13 30 1.042 .190

Pair 15SUP6(pre) 3.00 30 1.414 .258SUP6(post) 2.70 30 1.236 .226

Pair 16PROB4(pre) 2.97 30 1.033 .189PROB4(post) 3.80 30 .925 .169

Pair 17GLOB7(pre) 3.17 30 1.147 .209GLOB7(post) 3.60 30 1.003 .183

Pair 18PROB5(pre) 2.53 30 1.042 .190PROB5(post) 3.60 30 .932 .170

Pair 19GLOB8(pre) 2.30 30 .988 .180GLOB8(post) 3.20 30 .805 .147

Pair 20SUP7(pre) 1.77 30 .858 .157SUP7(post) 3.00 30 .743 .136

Pair 21PROB6(pre) 1.80 30 1.064 .194PROB6(post) 3.23 30 .898 .164

Pair 22GLOB9(pre) 2.23 30 1.278 .233GLOB9(post) 3.00 30 .947 .173

Pair 23GLOB10(pre) 1.53 30 .629 .115GLOB10(post) 3.50 30 .861 .157

Pair 24SUP8(pre) 3.03 30 1.098 .200SUP8(post) 3.50 30 .731 .133

a. Group = Experimental

24 Jungwan Yang

Examples in: English

Applicable Languages: English

Applicable Levesl: Secondary

Key words: reading strategy, metacognition, peer response, interaction, negotiation

Yang, Jungwan

The Institute for Language Education and Research

Seoul National University of Science & Technology

138 Gongeung gil, Nowon-gu, Seoul 139-743, Korea

Tel: +82-2-970-7227

E-mail: [email protected].

Submitted: December 10, 2015

Revised: January 1, 2016

Accepted: January 20, 2016