impairment of the binocular coordination of saccades in strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic...

10
Pergamon PH: S0042-6989(97)00064-3 Vision Res., Vol.37, No. 19, pp. 2757-2766, 1997 © 1997Elsevier Science Ltd.All fights reserved Printedin Great Britain 0042-6989/97 $17.00+ 0.00 Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus ZOI KAPOULA,*:~ MARIA PIA BUCCI,* THOMAS EGGERT,* LOIC GARRAUD'~ Received 12 February 1996; in revised form 20 December 1996 To examine the link between binocular vision and binocular coordination of saccades we studied subjects with convergent strabismus since childhood with mild or no amblyopia: three subjects had small squint (<10 prism D) and preserved peripheral binocular visual function with gross stereopsis; four subjects had larger squint (18-35 prism D) and no detectable stereopsis. A standard paradigm was used to elicit horizontal saccades; binocular recordings were made with the IRIS device. For subjects with small strabismus, saccades were disconjugate (unequal between the two eyes) typically by I deg. Subjects with larger strabismus exhibited even larger and more variable disconjugacy (typically 1.8 deg). Post-saccadic eye drift was consistently divergent in subjects with small strabismus and tended to reduce the convergent squint angle. In contrast, in subjects with large strabismus drift was convergent. The impairment of the binocular control of saccades is attributed to the deficiency of disconjugate oculomotor adaptive capabilities necessary to compensate for the natural asymmetries or changes in the two oculomotor plants; such deficiency would be more severe in subjects with large strabismus who have neither central nor peripheral binocular vision. © 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd Saccades Disconjugacy Post-saccadic drift Binocular vision INTRODUCTION Studies of saccades and particularly of their binocular coordination, in strabismics are rather scarce. The binocular coordination of saccades is essential for achieving binocular vision after every change in fixation. Reciprocally, binocular vision is necessary to ensure binocular oculomotor coordination via oculomotor adap- tive mechanisms. Several recent studies in normal subjects have shown remarkably fast (Van der Steen & Bruno, 1995; Kapoula et al., 1995), and also slower (Lemij & Collewijn, 1991; Oohira et al., 1991; Schor et al., 1990; Oohira & Zee, 1992) adaptive changes in the binocular coordination of saccades when the visual input from one eye is altered; such changes aim to overcome the induced visual disparity and thus to re-establish single binocular vision. Since binocular vision is deficient in strabismics then the question arises whether their saccades are still yoked and whether disconjugate adaptive capabilities are preserved; such capabilities are needed to compensate for natural asymmetries and developmental changes of the oculomotor plants. Evi- dence for deficient binocular control of saccades comes *Laboratoire de Physiologiede la Perception et de l'Action, UMR 9950, CNRS-Coll~ge de France, 75005 Paris, France. tH6pital de Douarnenez,Bretagne29100, France. ~To whom all correspondenceshouldbe addressed [Tel 331-44 27 16 29; Fax 331-44 27 13 82; Email [email protected]]. from the study of De Faber et al. (1994). These authors examined adult subjects, most of them with large divergent strabismus (20-40 prism D) and with severe unilateral amblyopia. They reported large, direction- specific, deficits: saccades in the direction where the preferred, non-amblyopic eye abducted were larger in that eye by as much as 25% (divergent disconjugacy). The goal of the present paper was to study the degree of binocular coordination of saccades in strabismic subjects without severe amblyopia whose main sensory deficiency is in binocular vision. To test the role of binocular vision we recruited two groups of subjects: subjects with small convergent strabismus (< 10 prism D) who had no foveal binocular vision but maintained peripheral binocular vision; and subjects with large convergent strabismus (18-35 prism D) who had neither central nor peripheral binocular vision. We found an impairment of the binocular coordination of saccades which was more severe in large strabismus with the most reduced binocular visual capabilities than in small strabismus. METHODS Subjects General characteristics. Seven subjects, two female and five male, were studied. Their ages ranged from 20 to 39 yr. Table 1 describes the main clinical characteristics of subjects. All subjects had a history of early-onset non- 2757

Upload: others

Post on 27-Jun-2020

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

Pergamon PH: S0042-6989(97)00064-3

Vision Res., Vol. 37, No. 19, pp. 2757-2766, 1997 © 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All fights reserved

Printed in Great Britain 0042-6989/97 $17.00 + 0.00

Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus ZOI KAPOULA,*:~ MARIA PIA BUCCI,* THOMAS EGGERT,* LOIC GARRAUD'~

Received 12 February 1996; in revised form 20 December 1996

To examine the link between binocular vision and binocular coordination of saccades we studied subjects with convergent strabismus since childhood with mild or no amblyopia: three subjects had small squint (<10 prism D) and preserved peripheral binocular visual function with gross stereopsis; four subjects had larger squint (18-35 prism D) and no detectable stereopsis. A standard paradigm was used to elicit horizontal saccades; binocular recordings were made with the IRIS device. For subjects with small strabismus, saccades were disconjugate (unequal between the two eyes) typically by I deg. Subjects with larger strabismus exhibited even larger and more variable disconjugacy (typically 1.8 deg). Post-saccadic eye drift was consistently divergent in subjects with small strabismus and tended to reduce the convergent squint angle. In contrast, in subjects with large strabismus drift was convergent. The impairment of the binocular control of saccades is attributed to the deficiency of disconjugate oculomotor adaptive capabilities necessary to compensate for the natural asymmetries or changes in the two oculomotor plants; such deficiency would be more severe in subjects with large strabismus who have neither central nor peripheral binocular vision. © 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd

Saccades Disconjugacy Post-saccadic drift Binocular vision

INTRODUCTION

Studies of saccades and particularly of their binocular coordination, in strabismics are rather scarce. The binocular coordination of saccades is essential for achieving binocular vision after every change in fixation. Reciprocally, binocular vision is necessary to ensure binocular oculomotor coordination via oculomotor adap- tive mechanisms. Several recent studies in normal subjects have shown remarkably fast (Van der Steen & Bruno, 1995; Kapoula et al., 1995), and also slower (Lemij & Collewijn, 1991; Oohira et al., 1991; Schor et al., 1990; Oohira & Zee, 1992) adaptive changes in the binocular coordination of saccades when the visual input from one eye is altered; such changes aim to overcome the induced visual disparity and thus to re-establish single binocular vision. Since binocular vision is deficient in strabismics then the question arises whether their saccades are still yoked and whether disconjugate adaptive capabilities are preserved; such capabilities are needed to compensate for natural asymmetries and developmental changes of the oculomotor plants. Evi- dence for deficient binocular control of saccades comes

*Laboratoire de Physiologie de la Perception et de l'Action, UMR 9950, CNRS-Coll~ge de France, 75005 Paris, France.

tH6pital de Douarnenez, Bretagne 29100, France. ~To whom all correspondence should be addressed [Tel 331-44 27 16

29; Fax 331-44 27 13 82; Email [email protected]].

from the study of De Faber et al. (1994). These authors examined adult subjects, most of them with large divergent strabismus (20-40 prism D) and with severe unilateral amblyopia. They reported large, direction- specific, deficits: saccades in the direction where the preferred, non-amblyopic eye abducted were larger in that eye by as much as 25% (divergent disconjugacy).

The goal of the present paper was to study the degree of binocular coordination of saccades in strabismic subjects without severe amblyopia whose main sensory deficiency is in binocular vision. To test the role of binocular vision we recruited two groups of subjects: subjects with small convergent strabismus (< 10 prism D) who had no foveal binocular vision but maintained peripheral binocular vision; and subjects with large convergent strabismus (18-35 prism D) who had neither central nor peripheral binocular vision. We found an impairment of the binocular coordination of saccades which was more severe in large strabismus with the most reduced binocular visual capabilities than in small strabismus.

METHODS

Subjects

General characteristics. Seven subjects, two female and five male, were studied. Their ages ranged from 20 to 39 yr. Table 1 describes the main clinical characteristics of subjects. All subjects had a history of early-onset non-

2757

Page 2: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

2758 Z. KAPOULA et al.

TABLE 1. Clinical characteristics of subjects

Subject

Angle of Surgical and/or Age Corrected visual strabismus botulinum

Sex (yr) acuity (prism D) Preferred eye treatment Time from last

treatment Stereo acuity

MB

DB

MG

(A) Subjects with small strabismus M 20 RE: 20/20 4-ET - - Surgery

LE: 20/40 RE, LE M 38 RE: 20/25 4-ET - - Toxin

LE: 20/25 RE M 36 RE: 20/25 7-ET - - Surgery LE

LE: 20/20 Toxin RE

(B) Subjects with large strabismus FH F 24 RE: 20/25 22-ET LE Surgery LE

LE: 20/20 5 hyper PB M 28 RE: 20/40 25-ET LE Surgery LE

LE: 20/20 Toxin RE BM F 39 RE: 20/20 28-ET LE Surgery

LE: 20/20 5 hyper RE, LE TG 1 M 29 RE: 20/40 30-ET LE Surgery RE

LE: 20/20 TG2 M 30 RE: 20/40 14-ET LE Toxin RE

LE: 20/20

11 yr

3 yr

13 yr 4 yr

4 yr

4 yr 6 yr

10 yr

10 yr

10 months

3 0 0 "

240"

3000"

m

E

m

m

ET, esotropia; hyper vertical deviation. LE, left eye; RE, right eye. No subject had severe amblyopia; binocular vision was assessed with stereoacuity tests.

paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation.

Prior strabismus treatment. All subjects had under- gone strabismus eye surgery and/or injection of botuli- num toxin in their extraocular muscles. These treatments, however, were done long ago; the latest one was the botulinum on subject TG2 and this was 10 months before our oculomotor testing. It is known that the paralysis of the eye muscle produced by the toxin does not last more than 3 months (Scott, 1981). Consequently, no subject had eye muscle paresis at the time of our oculomotor testing. Subject TG was examined twice: the first time his squint was 30 prism D; the second time, a year later, his squint was reduced to 14 prism D due to botulinum treatment.

Visual acuity. All subjects had good visual acuity in both eyes. During the experiment subjects wore their habitual corrective lenses. Note that subject DB was recruited because his angle was small and he had good binocular vision. About a month before our oculomotor testing, however, this subject presented neuro-ophthal- mological symptoms, such as fluctuating deficits in the visual field and fluctuating reduced visual acuities. The neuro-ophthalmological examination done the day of our experiment showed no visual field defect and his visual acuity was good in both eyes (20/25, see Table 1A). His squint, when measured at the synoptophore was 4 prism D. It was, however, larger and particularly variable, when measured with eye movement recording.

Ocular dominance and binocular vision capabilities. Ocular dominance was tested with the 4 prism D base out prism test: the prism was placed alternately either over the left or over the right eye. The eye for which the prism caused more frequently a refixation movement was considered as dominant. All patients with small strabis-

mus could alternate the fixating eye rapidly and no clear eye preference could be established. Under binocular vision these subjects exhibited a small suppression scotoma of the image in one eye. The presence of the scotoma was determined with the use of several clinical tests (Bagolini striated glasses test, 4 prism D base out prism test, synoptophore). Despite the absence of bifoveal binocular vision, stereoacuity was present for all these subjects although below normal values. Subject DB had a stereoacuity of 240 sec of arc on the TNO random dot and 60 sec of arc on the Titmus test. Subject MB had 300 sec of arc on the Titmus test; subject MG had only gross stereopsis (approx 3000 sec of arc, Titmus fly test and Worth four dot test). In summary, the three subjects with small squint preserved perifoveal and/or peripheral visual binocular function and gross stereopsis. For subject MB, fusional amplitudes were actually measured at the synoptophore using images with large disparities (>5 deg); these values were 10 and 6 prism D for convergence and divergence, respectively.

For subjects with large strabismus (Table 1B), the left eye was dominant. None of the four subjects presented any evidence for stereoacuity; all tests TNO, Titmus, Worth 4 dot were negative manifesting complete deficiency of binocular vision.

Control subjects. Three healthy emmetropic subjects (ZK, MP, MM) participated in this study. They had no history of strabismus and their binocular vision was normal (stereoacuity values at the TNO test were 60 sec of arc or better). All subjects had right eye dominance (subjects were asked to fixate a distant target through a small central hole of about 6 mm in diameter; the eye used to fixate the target was considered to be the dominant eye). This study was approved by French ethics committee CCPPRB No. 15.

Page 3: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

BINOCULAR SACCADE COORDINATION IN STRABISMUS 2759

Eye movement recording

The stimulation and data collection were directed by REX, a software developed for real time experiments and run on a PC (Dell 486P/33). Horizontal saccades from both eyes were recorded simultaneously with a photo- electric device mounted on spectacles (IRIS, SKALAR). The system had an optimal resolution of 2 min of arc and its range for lateral excursions was up to 30 deg; its linearity was within 3% for excursions up to 25 deg. A preliminary calibration was done for each eye individu- ally when it alone viewed LED targets: subjects were asked to fixate back and forth between a central and two peripheral LED light dots located at 25 deg on either side. Eye position signals were low-pass filtered with a cut- off frequency of 200 Hz and digitized with a 12-bit analogue-to digital-converter. Each channel was sampled at 500 Hz.

Oculomotor testing In a dark room, subjects were seated 123 cm in front of

an egocentric arc of light-emitting diodes (LEDs) positioned horizontally at eye level. The head of the subject was stabilized with a bite bar (with an individually fitted dental impression of the subject's upper teeth). A standard saccade paradigm was used to elicit saccades: the subject fixated a central small spot of red light created by the LED (5.6 min of arc). After a variable interval the central spot disappeared and a target appeared in the periphery, at a randomly chosen position to the left or to the right at 5, 10, 15 or 20 deg. The target was created by illuminating an array of LEDs and was either a normal E or a backward E 0.4 by 0.2 deg. The subject was instructed to fixate the target as accurately as possible and to discriminate whether it was a normal E. Target duration was approx 1 sec, after which the central LED dot was again illuminated. For all subjects with strabismus, this task was done first under both monocular viewing conditions (left eye viewing, right eye viewing) and then under binocular viewing. The change from one monocular viewing condition to the other or to the binocular viewing condition was done by opening or closing a black tissue curtain in front of each eye; the tissues were mounted on the head support. This technique was used to avoid possible head movements that could occur while putting on an eye occluder. Calibration factors were extracted from the monocular viewing conditions.

Data analysis A linear function was used to fit the calibration data.

Saccade onset was determined at the point where eye velocity reached 5% of the peak velocity; saccade offset was taken as the time when eye velocity dropped below 10 deg/sec. Post-saccadic eye drift was determined for a period of 160 msec after saccade offset. This value was chosen to be close to that required to reach the steady- state position in lesioned animals that develop ocular drift (e.g. Optican & Robinson, 1980). Ocular drift beyond 160 msec after the saccade includes also visually driven

Right 4

Normal

- 4

Left _1;~i "

0 2 0 0 400 6 0 0

Small Strabismus ,~, p O) ~$ s f O~

~ -4

0 200 400 600

Large Strabismus

4~ RE J

' LE -4 i

- 1 2 i I i i i )

0 200 400 600

Time (mseo)

FIGURE 1. Typical binocular recordings of rightward saccades from a normal subject (MM), from a subject with small strabismus (MG) and from a subject with large strabismus (FIt). The solid line is the position trace of the left eye, the dotted line is that of the right eye. Markers on the left eye position trace (middle panel) are placed by computer algorithms and indicate the onset of the saccade (i), the offset of its rapid, pulse component (p), and post-saccadic drift for the following 160 msec (s). The lower trace is the disconjugacy of the saccade (the difference between the left and the right eye). Divergent disconjugacy is negative, convergent disconjugacy is positive. Saccade traces from

the two eyes are offset.

components and our main interest here is on the earlier drift components related to the matching of the saccade pulse-slide-step signals.

For each individual saccade we measured in degrees the disconjugacy (left-right eye difference) of the amplitude of the saccade and of the post-saccadic eye drift. We use the term intrasaccadic disconjugacy to denote the difference of the amplitude of saccades. Positive values indicate convergent disconjugacy: sac- cade amplitude is larger in the adducting eye or it has more onward drift than the abducting eye (the right eye is abducting for rightward saccades, the left eye for leftward saccades). Statistics were performed using the Student's t-test.

Page 4: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

2760 Z. KAPOULA et al.

RIGHTWARD SACCADES

LEFTWARD SACCADES

A "O

8 o~ D O

8

i*' l 8 o.0

~,-6.5

STRABISMUS

NORMALS SMALL LARGE

j ÷

t i T.t TT , ;

ft

+ I t • T

+

, + ,

8 * o.o r ~ LT "T'

CI -6.B

ZK MP MM MB DB MG FH PB BM TG1 TG2

[]

FIGURE 2. Bars are individual means of saccade amplitude disconjugacy together with their standard deviation. Means are shown separately for rightward and leftward saccades and for each viewing condition: binocular viewing (BEV), left eye viewing (LEV), right eye viewing (REV). Each mean is based on 30--40 saccades. For subjects with strabismus, particularly large strabismus, disconjugacy is large and of variable sign (divergent, convergent). *Statistically significant at the level P < 0.05 (Student's t-test comparing the mean obtained for the binocular viewing condition with that obtained for monocular viewing). +Statistically significant at the level P < 0.05 (comparison between the mean for left eye viewing and the mean for the

right eye viewing condition).

BEV

L.EV

RESULTS

Saccade amplitude disconjugacy Figure 1 shows typical examples of rightward

saccades. Normal saccades show a small divergent intrasaccadic disconjugacy followed by small convergent post-saccadic drift. In contrast, the saccade from the subject with small strabismus shows larger and con- vergent intrasaccadic disconjugacy; the saccade is followed by a divergent drift. The saccade from the subject with large strabismus shows large divergent intrasaccadic disconjugacy followed by a slow conver- gent post-saccadic drift.

Figure 2 shows the individual average intrasaccadic disconjugacy separately for rightward and for leftward saccades. For all three normal subjects the mean disconjugacy was small (< 1 deg) and was consistently divergent regardless of the direction of the saccade. The intrasaccadic disconjugacy was almost always smaller (<0.6 deg) when the fight dominant eye was used to fixate the target.

For two of the subjects with small strabismus (MB and MG), the intrasaccadic disconjugacy was only slightly larger than that of normals; for subject DB, disconjugacy values were abnormally high. The main difference from

normals is that for all three subjects with small strabismus the disconjugacy was not consistently divergent; it was divergent or convergent at similar rates. Sixty-four percent of the saccades made by MB (all viewing conditions, leftward and rightward saccades together) had convergent disconjugacy; for subjects DB and MG these values were 52% and 62%, respectively.

Subjects with large strabismus showed larger values of intrasaccadic disconjugacy. Only rightward saccades under the binocular viewing condition (BEV) from subject TG2 exhibited small disconjugacy (0.24 deg), comparable with that found in normals. The other subjects, including TG2 in the other viewing conditions and saccade directions, showed large disconjugacy (range: 0.85-4.22 deg). Subject TG in both experiments showed consistently divergent disconjugacy, regardless of the saccade direction and of the viewing condition. In contrast, for the other three subjects the sign of disconjugacy depended on the direction of the saccades: for FH and BM, in all viewing conditions, disconjugacy was consistently divergent for rightward saccades (100% and 82%) but convergent for leftward saccades (100% and 86% of the saccades made by FH and BM, respectively). Subject PB showed consistently divergent disconjugacy except for rightward saccades in the right

Page 5: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

(a)

MB

9.

0

4.5 0

-4.5

-9.0

0

9.0

4.5 0

-4.5

..

..

..

.

9.0

10

20

MG

Sm

all

stra

bis

mu

s

J °

i

to

~

DB

9.

0

4.5 0

-9.0

I

' ,

' ,

,, 0

10

20

O

o r~

9.0

4.5 0

-4.5

-9.0

FH

....

.,..

..'°

°'°"

...

. 9

.0

.,I..9

;n~ "

~'''.

'; *

4.5 0

-4.5

, -9

.0

0 10

20

PB

Lar

ge

stra

bis

mu

s

9.0

4.5

" ~

~ ~

-4.5

° ,

~ -9

.0

0 1'

0

BM

9.

0

. oO

0o

°°,

. 4.

5 a

.?--

-.'-

'o""

....

........

~...

...-:

..o~'

';;'~

"'*;

" .

_~-~

'~r

* .

~."

n'.

'-

" o

" 0

~'*

** -4

.5

' :

-9.0

10

20

(b)

3.0

0.5

-2.0

-4.5

-7.0

PB

4.0

O%

o ,

; *_

.

1.5

• "

# *

o

~t

***

~ -3

.5

' ;0

'

2'0

-6.0

BM

tl

! t

1'0

2'~

Sac

cad

e am

pli

tud

e (d

eg)

TG

2

0 '

1'0

' '

~0

FIG

UR

E 3

. (a

) In

tras

acca

dic

disc

onju

gacy

vs

aver

age

sacc

ade

ampl

itud

e (L

+R

/2).

Eac

h sy

mbo

l in

dica

tes

the

disc

onju

gacy

of

a sa

ccad

e; c

ross

es i

ndic

ate

the

disc

onju

gacy

of

righ

twar

d sa

ccad

es,

squa

res

indi

cate

the

dis

conj

ugac

y o

f le

ftw

ard

sacc

ades

. D

ata

from

the

bin

ocul

ar v

iew

ing

cond

itio

n. L

ine

fit

to r

ight

war

d an

d le

ftw

ard

sacc

ades

. (b

) O

rbit

al

posi

tion

dep

ende

ncy

of t

he

disc

onju

gacy

of

sacc

ades

: ce

ntri

fuga

l (c

ross

es)

and

cent

ripe

tal

(squ

ares

) ri

ghtw

ard

sacc

ades

of

sim

ilar

am

plit

ude

reco

rded

und

er B

EV

hav

e ve

ry d

iffe

rent

dis

conj

ugac

y. L

ine

fit t

o ce

ntri

peta

l an

d ce

ntri

fuga

l sa

ccad

es.

0 ~n

0 0 Z

to

Page 6: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

2162 Z. KAPOULA et al.

STRABISMUS

LARGE +

i +i NORMALS SMALLS -+=l

RIGHTWARD

SACCADES

LEFIWARD SACCADES

MB DB MCI Ftl PB EM Ml M2 IJLEV

n REV

FIGURE 4. Bars are individual means and standard deviations of post-saccadic drift disconjugacy. Positive values indicate convergent drift, negative values divergent drift. Other notations as in Fig. 2. For normals and for subjects with large strabismus, post-saccadic eye drift in the first 160 msec is consistently convergent; in contrast, for subjects with small strabismus post-

saccadic eye drift is predominately divergent.

eye viewing condition. For three of the four subjects, viewing with the right, non-preferred eye produced a larger disconjugacy than viewing with both eyes (asterisk sign), or viewing with the left, preferred eye (cross sign); these differences reached statistical significance for subjects PB and BM and TG2, rightward saccades.

Figure 3(a) shows that for all subjects with large strabismus the amplitude of the disconjugacy increases with the amplitude of the saccade. Saccades beyond 10 deg of amplitude show large disconjugacy (4-5 deg), corresponding approximately to 20-30% of the ampli- tude of the saccade. In contrast, for subjects with small strabismus (MB, MG) the size of the disconjugacy is small regardless of the amplitude and of the direction of the saccade. Only subject DB showed an increase of the disconjugacy for saccades beyond 10 deg. Figure 3(b) shows positional specificity in two subjects with large strabismus. Centrifugal rightward saccades from subject PB had a divergent disconjugacy of up to 3 deg, while centripetal rightward saccades had larger divergent disconjugacy of up to 5 deg. Centripetal saccades larger than 10 deg from subject BM had a convergent

disconjugacy of up to 3.8 deg, while centrifugal saccades of similar size had a divergent disconjugacy of up to 5.2 deg.

In summary, subjects with strabismus presented larger saccade amplitude disconjugacy than the known, small, divergent disconjugacy of normals, which was confirmed in our study. In contrast to normals, saccade disconjugacy in strabismics could be either convergent or divergent. Its amplitude was, in general, larger in subjects with large strabismus than in subjects with small strabismus, and depended on the direction of the saccade, as well as on the orbital position of the eyes.

Post-saccadic drif in the$rst 160 msec after the saccade

Figure 4 shows the average amplitude of disconjugate post-saccadic eye drift for each individual subject and for each viewing condition. For the three normal subjects, post-saccadic eye drift was convergent for both rightward as well as leftward saccades. With one exception (ZK, leftward saccades under left eye viewing), the mean disconjugacy of post-saccadic eye drift was small (<0.3 deg). For subjects with small strabismus disconju-

FIGURE 5 (opposite). Saccade sequences from a normal and from several strabismic subjects; all data are from the binocular viewing condition. The solid line is the position trace of the left eye the dotted line is that of the right eye; the offset of the two traces indicates the squint angle. ‘Ihe thick trace is the average of the two eyes (L + R/2), the cyclopean eye, for the normal subject this trace is offset for clarity. The lower trace in each graph is the disconjugacy trace (the difference between the left and the right eye). In strabismics, post-saccadic eye motion is composed of several components in the same or opposite directions

(indicated by arrows). Recordings under monocular viewing conditions show similar drifts with multiple components.

Page 7: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

B I N O C U L A R S A C C A D E C O O R D I N A T I O N IN S T R A B I S M U S 2 7 6 3

17 O

O

O , . L

- 1 5

Convergent 2

Divergent - 2

Normal subject MM Left eye

Disconjugacy LE-RE ~ _ - _ . - .

I I ! i I i i i I i i ~

1 2 3 4 5 6

-14

-30

20

16

-14

-30

7

DB Subjects with small s t rabismus

2

- 1 4

0 1 2 3 4 5 6

- 3 0

18

11

M B

I I

. . . . . . . ° - 1 9 . e

1 2 3 4 5 6

-35

25

Subjects with large s t rabismus FH

,a r L ' L , , ,2- ? ,,f. . . . . . . . . . . . . . .

: ................ L__.~, , j :: I o 0

i . . . . . . . . . . . . o l

0 1

PB

i i i

2

,L 20

, - . - - - . . . . ~ . . . . . . , . ,

i t

' o - i ° o , . . . . .

i i i i i i i i i ! : ~>

1 2 3 4 5 6

MG

12

- 4

-20

. ° m . . . . . . . . . . . . •

" ° ' . ° ° 2 . . . . . . . . ~ °

- 6 . . . . . . " : "

0 1 2 3 4 5 6

- 2 4

25

18

BM

i

. o l

. . . . ° ' % . . . . . . . . . % .

J

o ° ~ o ~ e l ~ e 4 *

0 1 2 3 4 5 6

T i m e ( s e c )

FIGURE 5---caption opposite.

Page 8: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

2764 z. KAPOULA et al.

gacy of drift was, in general, larger in amplitude than for normals. Most important, with one exception (DB, right eye viewing), the mean post-saccadic eye drift was divergent. The percentages of the saccades followed by divergent drift were 80% and 83% for subjects MB, MG (all saccades in all viewing conditions), and 89% and 68% for subject DB, in the binocular viewing and in the right eye viewing condition, respectively.

For subjects with large strabismus, post-saccadic drift disconjugacy was consistently convergent for all viewing conditions and for both saccade directions. The overall percentages of saccades followed by convergent drift were 65%, 82%, 77%, 98%, and 98% for subjects FH, PB, BM, TG1, TG2, respectively. The amplitude of the disconjugate drift was small for subject FH, and for leftward saccades from subjects PB and BM. For all other cases drift amplitudes were substantially larger than those of normals. Thus, post-saccadic eye drift was more variable in large strabismus (range: 0.07-1.23 deg) than in normals (range: -0.07-0.65 deg), even though drift was consistently convergent for both populations.

The differences between the viewing conditions (Fig. 4, asterisk and cross sign), were rather idiosyncratic for subjects with small strabismus. For subjects with large strabismus, drift disconjugacy was larger when viewing with the right, non-preferred eye rather than with both eyes or with the left eye (PB, TG2 rightward saccades; BM leftward saccades).

Post-saccadic eye drift and fixational instabili~

Figure 5 shows sequences of five or more saccades from a normal subject and from subjects with small strabismus, as well as from subjects with large strabis- mus. The difference between the three groups of subjects can be observed in the disconjugacy trace. For the normal subject, one sees the small convergent drift of the eyes in the first 200 msec after the saccades well described in the literature; the eyes almost reach their steady state position afterwards and subsequent fixational stability is good. For subjects with small strabismus one can discern after the saccade the short time constant divergent drift described above; this is frequently followed by a second, con- vergent drift component [Fig. 5 (subjects DB, MB and MG upward arrows)], and even by a third divergent component (downward arrows, subject MG) that can- celled the second, convergent component. For all three subjects the first divergent drift component prevailed; this divergent drift brought the eyes to a more divergent alignment than that at the end of the rapid, pulsatile component of the saccade. For subjects with large strabismus the overall fixational pattern between sac- cades is predominantly convergent with a time course slower than that of the divergent drifts of subjects with small strabismus. A second, divergent component at approximately 300--500 msec before the next saccade could be distinguished for subject BM (indicated by upward arrows). In most cases, post-saccadic eye motion in subjects with large strabismus brought the eyes to a

more convergent position than that at the end of the saccades.

In summary, the early part of post-saccadic eye motion was corrective (with respect to the baseline squint angle) in subjects with small strabismus but not in subjects with large strabismus. Subsequent fixational stability was poor for all strabismic subjects, as multiple drift components were going on almost throughout the period between saccades.

DISCUSSION

Impairment of binocular yoking of saccades

It is well known that the binocular yoking of normal horizontal saccades is not perfect: at the end of the rapid part of the saccade a small divergent disconjugacy exists (<0.5 deg for saccades with amplitudes below 20 deg). This phenomenon has been reported by several authors (Kapoula et al., 1987; Collewijn et al., 1988; Zee et al., 1992; Maxwell & King, 1992). Kapoula et al. attributed divergence during the saccade to a disconnection of the tonic vergence innervation by the high-burst of the saccadic pulse activity. Subsequent studies proposed more peripheral explanations. Thus, Zee et al. considered differences in the mechanical properties of the lateral and medial lateral rectus as a possible cause and/or a delay in the arrival of premotor signals at the motoneurons of the medial rectus with respect to the time of arrival at the lateral rectus; the delay could be due to the abducens internuclear neurons between the saccade burst neurons and the motoneurons of the medial rectus. Maxwell and King confirmed the presence of divergent disconjugacy in normal monkeys and also attributed it to a delay between the activation of the medial and the lateral rectus. Nevertheless, for children 5-10 years old, Fioravanti et al. (1995) observed convergent intrasacca- dic disconjugacy followed by divergent disconjugacy and divergent post-saccadic drift. The authors attribute these phenomena to incomplete compensation of mechanical asymmetries of the two oculomotor plants. It is possible that the small, consistently divergent disconjugacy seen in adults could be what is left uncorrected by the adaptive disconjugate oculomotor mechanisms. A divergent rather than a convergent intrasaccadic remnant would be easier to correct with a preprogrammed convergent post- saccadic drift. The recordings of three normal subjects in our study are in agreement with prior reports on adults (see Fig. 5, Normal): the eyes diverge during the first portion of the saccade and converge subsequently during and after the saccade. This stereotyped pattern was not confirmed in our strabismic subjects. In Fig. 5, for subjects with small strabismus (DB and MG), one sees frequently an initial divergent intrasaccadie disconjugacy followed by convergent intrasaccadic disconjugacy; the latter, however, is larger in amplitude, leaving a convergent disconjugacy at the end of the saccade. Another frequent pattern for subject MB and DB, is convergent disconjugacy throughout the saccade. For subjects FH and BM the disconjugacy is throughout the

Page 9: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

BINOCULAR SACCADE COORDINATION IN STRABISMUS 2765

saccade either convergent or divergent, and system- atically divergent for subject PB. Thus, in contrast to normals, there is no a single stereotyped pattern of intrasaccadic disconjugacy in strabismics. The second finding of our study is that saccade amplitude disconju- gacy is, in general, larger and more variable in subjects with large strabismus. Saccade disconjugacy in such subjects creates incomitancy of the squint angle, even for gaze positions within the range of + 20 deg. Recall that none of our subjects had a history of paralytic strabismus or abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Thus, the most plausible interpretation of their oculomotor deficits is weakness or absence of disconjugate adaptive mechanisms. As suggested by Fioravanti et al. (1995) such mechanisms are needed during development, to compensate for the asymmetries of the oculomotor plants and/or differences in the premotor oculomotor relays. We suggest that in strabismus this ability is limited or absent. Our finding of more severe impairment of the binocular coordination of saccades in large strabismus supports the hypothesis of a deficiency of the adaptive mechanisms that could be more severe in subjects with large strabismus because of their reduced binocular visual capabilities. Additional support for this comes from another study (Bucci et al., 1997, this issue, pp. 2767- 2777), which tested in the same subjects disconjugate oculomotor adaptation to aniseikonia (image size in- equality), and found failure of adaptation in the subjects with large strabismus.

Our finding of impaired binocular control of saccades is compatible with the observations of De Faber et al. (1994). A basic difference, however, is that for divergent strabismus with important unilateral amblyopia studied by Faber et al., saccade disconjugacy was systematically larger and divergent when the non-amblyopic eye abducted. In our subjects with convergent strabismus and without amblyopia, one could not establish a directional specificity common to all subjects. Maxwell et al. (1995) studied subjects with deep unilateral amblyopia who had mostly convergent strabismus. They also reported marked saccade amplitude disconjugacy but no directional specificity. Thus, directional specificity seems to be related to the divergent type of strabismus rather than to amblyopia. Maxwell et al. suggested that saccade disconjugacy is related to the deep amblyopia rather than strabismus. Our finding of similar disconju- gacy in convergent strabismus without important am- blyopia shows that strabismus alone is sufficient to deteriorate the binocular yoking of saccades. Strabismus disrupts normal binocular vision, thereby disabling or reducing disconjugate oculomotor adaptive capabilities.

Post-saccadic drift and f ixational stability

We found substantial disconjugate post-saccadic drift immediately after the saccade and subsequent fixational stability to be rather poor, as motion of each eye continued or reversed its sign one or more times over the total period between saccades. Post-saccadic drift cannot be due to muscular peripheral effects since none

of the subjects had paralytic strabismus. Neither it could be due to strabismus surgery done 3-13 years ago. Inchingolo et al. (1996) reported that very large drifts following strabismus surgery are almost eliminated within the next year. Neither could botulinum toxin, used to correct strabismus, be responsible for any of the drift abnormalities, since the effect of toxin does not last more than 3 months (see Scott, 1981). Our observation of multiple drift components in the same or in the opposite directions is consistent with the study of Inchingolo et al. (1996). Children with convergent strabismus showed large, convergent post-saccadic eye drift with multiple components, which was orbital-position dependent. The persistence of convergent drifts we reported in adults with large strabismus would reflect limits or lack of oculomotor adaptive mechanisms. Such mechanisms are needed to match the saccade pulse-slide-step signals appropriately in order to counteract viscoelastic proper- ties of individual eye muscles and the resistive torque of orbital tissues (see Inchingolo et al., 1996).

Alternatively, the difference in the early post-saccadic drift between subjects with small strabismus and subjects with large strabismus could be due to some additional active post-saccadic corrective mechanisms in the former subjects. As previously mentioned, in subjects with small strabismus post-saccadic drift in the first 160 msec after the saccade was consistently divergent and prevailed over the subsequent components. It is possible that these subjects learn to program a divergence movement whose execution is synchronized with the end of the saccade. Inchingolo et al. (1996) did not examine children with small convergent strabismus (< 10 prism D) and it is not known whether this difference in post-saccadic eye drift behavior is already present in childhood. Recall that, for subjects with strabismus smaller than 10 prism D, perifoveal and/or peripheral binocular vision was present, as well as gross stereopsis. The corrective, divergent, post-saccadic drift could help to maintain this state of binocular sensory coordination. Thus, corrective post- saccadic drift behavior could be another indication of the extent to which sensory and oculomotor binocular coordination is preserved in strabismic subjects.

Changes in disconjugacy due to which eye views

In normal subjects, Kapoula et al. (1987), reported a change in the amplitude of disconjugate post-saccadic drift depending upon which eye is viewing the target. The reasons for such dependencies are not clear. Such observations suggest either an ability to alternate instantaneously between different sets of adaptive oculomotor adjustments or an ability to modify very rapidly the binocular coordination of post-saccadic drift. In the present study we observed changes for all subjects (normals as well as strabismics of either type) and for both the intrasaccadic disconjugacy, as well as the post- saccadic drift disconjugacy. The dependencies were, however, rather idiosyncratic both for normal subjects and for subjects with small strabismus. In contrast, for most subjects with large strabismus, the disconjugacy

Page 10: Impairment of the Binocular Coordination of Saccades in Strabismus · 2017-02-22 · paralytic strabismus without important abnormalities of vergence and accommodation. Prior strabismus

2766 Z. KAPOULA et al.

(intrasaccadic as well as post-saccadic) was largest when the subject viewed with the non-preferred eye. This suggests that the default settings for the binocular oculomotor coordination in such subjects are based on visual input from the preferred eye and that a further degradation of the binocular control is introduced if the subject cannot use the usually preferred eye to fixate.

In summary, this study shows, for the first time, impairment of the binocular coordination of saccades in subjects with non-paralytic convergent strabismus with no or only mild unilateral amblyopia. It suggests that binocular vision is important in maintaining binocular oculomotor coordination. The impairment of oculomotor coordination is more severe in subjects with large convergent strabismus. It causes substantial incomitancy and could compromise any attempt to use vision from the two eyes together (e.g. gross peripheral binocular vision). Post-saccadic eye drift is predominately divergent in small convergent strabismus and could aim to reduce the squint angle. In contrast, in subjects with large con- vergent strabismus, drift is predominately convergent, that is, in the direction of the static offset of the eyes. The results provide a reference to neuro-ophthalmologists on saccade control in strabismus and contribute to our understanding of the link between binocular vision and binocular oculomotor coordination.

The impairment of the yoking of the saccades and of the post-saccadic eye stability in large strabismus could be due to the diminished disconjugate oculomotor adaptive capabilities. In subjects with small strabismus the corrective nature of the post-saccadic eye drift and the smaller disconjugacy of saccades, could be ensured by residual adaptive mechanisms based on peripheral binocular vision. The companion report (Bucci et al.,

1997) tests this hypothesis for six of the seven subjects. The results show failure of adaptation only in the subjects with large strabismus who have no binocular vision at all, and support the explanation presented here for the inherent impairment of the binocular coordination of saccades.

REFERENCES

Bucci, M. P., Kapoula, Z., Eggert, T. & Garraud, L. (1997). Deficiency of adaptive control of the binocular coordination of saccades in strabismus. Vision Research, 37, 2767-2777.

Collewijn, H., Erkelens, C. J. & Steinman, R. M. (1988). Binocular

coordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. Journal of Physiology, 404, 157-182.

De Faber, J.-Tj. H. N., van Rijn, L. J. & Collewijn, H. (1994) Dynamics of saccades in strabismus and amblyopia. In Kauffmann, H. (Ed.), Transactions of the 21 th European Strabismological Association (pp. 303-307). Buren, The Netherlands: Aeolus Press.

Fioravanti, F., Inchingolo, P., Pensiero, S. & Spanio, M. (1995). Saccadic eye movement conjugation in children. Vision Research, 35, 3217-3228.

Inchingolo, P., Accardo, A., Da Pozzo, S., Pensiero, S. & Perissutti, P. (1996). Cyclopean and disconjugate adaptive recovery from post- saccadic eye drift in strabismic children before and after surgery. Vision Research, 36, 2897-2913.

Kapoula, Z., Eggert, T. & Bucci, M. P. (1995). Immediate saccade amplitude disconjugacy induced by unequal images. Vision Research, 35, 3505-35 18.

Kapoula, Z., Hain, T. C., Zee, D. S. & Robinson, D. A. (1987). Adaptive changes in post-saccadic drift induced by patching one eye. Vision Research, 27, 1299-1307.

Lemij, H. & Collewijn, H. (1991). Long-term nonconjugate adaptation of human saccades to anisometropic spectacles. Vision Research, 31, 1939-1954.

Maxwell, J. S. & King, W. M. (1992). Dynamics and efficacy of saccade-facilitated vergence eye movements in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 684, 1248-1260.

Maxwell, G. F., Lemij, H. G. & Collewijn, H. (1995). Conjugacy of saccades in deep amblyopia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 36, 2514-2522.

Oohira, A. & Zee, D. S. (1992). Disconjugate ocular motor adaptation in rhesus monkey. Vision Research, 32, 489-497.

Oohira, A., Zee, D. S. & Guyton, D. L. (1991). Disconjugate adaptation to long-standing, large amplitude, spectacle-corrected anisometropia. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 32, 1693-1703.

Optican, L. M. & Robinson, D. A. (1980). Cerebellar-dependent adaptive control of the primate saccadic system. Journal of Neurophysiology, 54, 904958.

Schor, C. M., Gleason, J. & Homer, D. (1990). Selective nonconjugate binocular adaptation of vertical saccades and pursuit. Vision Research, 30, 1827-1844.

Scott, A. B. (1981). Botulinum toxin injection of eye muscles to correct strabismus. The American Ophthalmological Society, 79, 735-770.

Van der Steen, J. & Bruno, P. (1995). Unequal amplitude saccades produced by aniseikonic patterns: effects of viewing distance. Vision Research, 35, 3459-347 1.

Zee, D. S., Fitzgibbon, E. J. & Optican, L. M. (1992). Saccade vergence interactions in humans. Journal of Neurophysiology, 68, 1624-1641.

Acknowledgements-The authors thank the orthoptist, Madame Jacqueline Franceschini, for her extensive help and Dr Findlay for comments on the manuscript. This research was supported by the French INSERMXRAMIF and a European Contract SCI*-(X91-0747 (TSTS). Maria Pia Bucci was supported by a fellowship from the University of Genes, Italy.