heslin_2005_

25
Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113–136 (2005) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/job.270 Conceptualizing and evaluating career success PETER A. HESLIN* Cox School of Business, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas, U.S.A. Summary Within the vast literature on the antecedents of career success, the success criterion has gen- erally been operationalized in a rather deficient manner. Several avenues for improving the conceptualization and measurement of both objective and subjective career success are iden- tified. Paramount among these is the need for greater sensitivity to the criteria that study par- ticipants, in different contexts, use to construe and judge their career success. This paper illustrates that contextual and individual factors are likely to be associated with the relative salience of objective and subjective criteria of career success. Drawing on social comparison theory, propositions are also offered about when self- and other-referent success criteria are likely to be most salient. A broader research agenda addresses career success referent choice, organizational interventions, and potential cultural differences. This article maps out how future research can be more sensitive to how people actually do conceptualize and evaluate their own career success. Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Introduction Career success has long been a construct of considerable interest to career scholars (e.g., Parsons, 1909; Hughes, 1958) and practitioners (e.g., Robbins, 2003; Ziglar, 1997), not to mention the multi- tude of individuals engaged in a career (Hall, 1976, 2002). The career literature is replete with theories (e.g., Krumboltz, 1994), models (e.g., Holland, 1997), and accounts of career intervention programs (e.g., Chartrand & Rose, 1996) aimed at predicting and ultimately facilitating career success. It is also an important outcome in many areas of career scholarship, such as those pertaining to career explora- tion (Blustein, 1997) and decision making (Hartung & Blustein, 2002). In addition, a multitude of stu- dies have investigated how variables such as gender (e.g., Lyness & Thompson, 2000), personality (e.g., Seibert & Kraimer, 2001), education (e.g., Judge, Cable, Boudreau, & Bretz, 1995), mentoring relationships (e.g., Peluchette & Jeanquart, 2000), and career tactics (Judge & Bretz, 1994) are empiri- cally related to subsequent ‘career success.’ Received 29 July 2003 Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Revised 11 December 2003 Accepted 26 April 2004 * Correspondence to: Peter A. Heslin, Cox School of Business, Southern Methodist University, Box 750333, Dallas, TX 75275- 0333, U.S.A. E-mail: [email protected]

Upload: nguyen-hoang-minh-quoc

Post on 25-Sep-2015

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

Heslin_2005_

TRANSCRIPT

  • Journal of Organizational Behavior

    J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

    Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/job.270

    Conceptualizing and evaluatingcareer success

    PETER A. HESLIN*

    Cox School of Business, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas, U.S.A.

    Summary Within the vast literature on the antecedents of career success, the success criterion has gen-erally been operationalized in a rather deficient manner. Several avenues for improving theconceptualization and measurement of both objective and subjective career success are iden-tified. Paramount among these is the need for greater sensitivity to the criteria that study par-ticipants, in different contexts, use to construe and judge their career success. This paperillustrates that contextual and individual factors are likely to be associated with the relativesalience of objective and subjective criteria of career success. Drawing on social comparisontheory, propositions are also offered about when self- and other-referent success criteria arelikely to be most salient. A broader research agenda addresses career success referent choice,organizational interventions, and potential cultural differences. This article maps out howfuture research can be more sensitive to how people actually do conceptualize and evaluatetheir own career success. Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

    Introduction

    Career success has long been a construct of considerable interest to career scholars (e.g., Parsons,

    1909; Hughes, 1958) and practitioners (e.g., Robbins, 2003; Ziglar, 1997), not to mention the multi-

    tude of individuals engaged in a career (Hall, 1976, 2002). The career literature is replete with theories

    (e.g., Krumboltz, 1994), models (e.g., Holland, 1997), and accounts of career intervention programs

    (e.g., Chartrand & Rose, 1996) aimed at predicting and ultimately facilitating career success. It is also

    an important outcome in many areas of career scholarship, such as those pertaining to career explora-

    tion (Blustein, 1997) and decision making (Hartung & Blustein, 2002). In addition, a multitude of stu-

    dies have investigated how variables such as gender (e.g., Lyness & Thompson, 2000), personality

    (e.g., Seibert & Kraimer, 2001), education (e.g., Judge, Cable, Boudreau, & Bretz, 1995), mentoring

    relationships (e.g., Peluchette & Jeanquart, 2000), and career tactics (Judge & Bretz, 1994) are empiri-

    cally related to subsequent career success.

    Received 29 July 2003Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Revised 11 December 2003

    Accepted 26 April 2004

    * Correspondence to: Peter A. Heslin, Cox School of Business, Southern Methodist University, Box 750333, Dallas, TX 75275-0333, U.S.A. E-mail: [email protected]

  • By contrast, curiously little scholarly attention has been devoted to analyzing the nature of career

    success (Greenhaus, 2003; Heslin, 2003a; Sturges, 1999). One framework for categorizing how

    career success has been operationalized is Everett Hughes (1937, 1958) theoretical distinction

    between the objective and the subjective career. Specifically, Hughes defined the objective career

    as directly observable, measurable, and verifiable by an impartial third party, while the subjective

    career is only experienced directly by the person engaged in her or his career. Thus, objective career

    success is defined by verifiable attainments, such as pay, promotions, and occupational status, which

    have long been considered the hallmarks of career success across a wide range of societies

    (Nicholson, 2000). Indeed, Arthur and Rousseau (1996) found that more than 75 per cent of the

    career-related articles published in major interdisciplinary journals between 1980 and 1994 focused

    on objective perspectives.

    Subjective career success is defined by an individuals reactions to his or her unfolding career

    experiences (Hughes, 1937, 1958). Recognition of the importance of subjective success dates back

    at least to Thorndikes (1934) operationalization of career success as job satisfaction, as well as the

    objective criteria of earnings and job status. Although objective criteria have dominated much of the

    subsequent career success literature, subjective criteria have increasingly been adopted within career

    success research over the last decade (Greenhaus, 2003; Hall, 2002). However, the validity of these

    subjective career success measurements has ultimately been bounded by the extent to which they cap-

    ture the phenomenological meaning of career success to those surveyed.

    This paper begins by briefly reviewing the most common approaches to assessing both objective and

    subjective career success. Rather than attempting an exhaustive survey of the career success literature,

    we provide exemplars that direct attention to some strengths and limitations in the way that objective

    and subjective success have been operationalized, together with some suggestions for improved

    measurement.

    Four implicit assumptions that are prevalent in much of the career success literature are identified.

    The first is that objective outcomes (e.g., pay and promotions) are adequate proxies for success, pre-

    sumably even beyond the managerial and professional contexts in which most career success

    research has been conducted (Greenhaus, 2003; Sullivan, 1999). The second related presumption

    is that job and career satisfaction (i.e., Greenhaus, Parasuraman, & Wormley, 1990) adequately cap-

    ture the breadth of dimensions upon which people react to their careers. Third is the inherent assump-

    tion that people are similar in their concern about the success they attain in the objective, compared

    to subjective domain. Finally, the career success literature largely presumes that people concep-

    tualize and evaluate their career success only relative to self-referent criteria, such as their career

    aspirations.

    This paper strives to encourage career scholars to transcend each of these four assumptions. This is

    done by first highlighting the importance of discovering objective metrics that are meaningful within

    particular career contexts beyond the managerial and professional spheres. Second, we show that peo-

    ple conceptualize and evaluate their career success in realms (e.g., work-life balance, contribution,

    fulfillment) that go beyond how subjective career success has typically been conceptualized and mea-

    sured. Third, in an attempt to address the paucity of theory about how different people conceptualize

    their career success, we identify some potential correlates of when objective versus subjective criteria

    of success are likely to be most salient. Fourth, drawing on social comparison theory (Festinger, 1954),

    we demonstrate that people use other-referent as well as self-referent criteria to evaluate their career

    success. Other-referent success explains unique variance in overall subjective career success (Heslin,

    2003a). Thus, we use relevant theory and research to suggest when self- and other-referent success

    criteria are each most likely to be salient. Empirical tests of these propositions may ultimately lead

    to improvements in the way career success is typically conceptualized and measured in the extant lit-

    erature, as reviewed next.

    114 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • Operationalizations of Objective and Subjective Success

    Objective criteria

    Salary (Thorndike, 1934), salary growth (Hilton & Dill, 1962) and promotions (Thorndike, 1963) are

    the most widely used and readily accessible indicators of career success (Hall, 1976, 2002). These

    objective measures can have the substantial benefits of being readily available from existing records,

    standardized (at least within firms), and efficient to collect. They are free from self-serving and

    common-method variance, if collected by means other than self-report. They are valued by many peo-

    ple, as anecdotally reflected by Zig Ziglars (1997) quip that: Folks who say they dont care about

    money will probably lie about other things too! Finally, both Hall and Chandler (this issue), as well

    as Nicholson and de Waal Andrews (this issue), discuss how objective attainments can, under certain

    circumstances, lead to subjective career success.

    Organizational changes over the last two decades have, however, reduced the relevance of some

    traditional objective indicants of career success. For instance, trends such as organizational delayering,

    downsizing, and outsourcing have lessened the scope (Evans, Gunz, & Jalland, 1997) and relative

    desirability (Hall, 2002; Reitman & Schneer, 2003) of hierarchical progression through promotion.

    This applies even to MBA graduates; those who have earned the degree widely promoted as the cre-

    dential for access to a successful managerial career, characterized by upward mobility on a corporate

    ladder (Brousseau, Driver, Eneroth, & Larsson, 1996). For instance, a recent survey of the managerial

    careers of 116 MBA graduates over a 13-year period revealed that two-thirds of them had not followed

    this prototypical managerial career path (Reitman & Schneer, 2003). This study also found that those

    who had pursued a more protean managerial career had not paid a price in terms of their income, career

    satisfaction, or job security. In general, an increasing number of former professionals, managers, and

    even senior corporate officers are choosing to become contractors and consultants (Cappelli, 1999;

    Inkson, Heising, & Rousseau, 2001): generic titles that lack the clear occupational status and rank that

    pertained to their previous organizational positions.

    Objective criteria of career success are also liable to being both contaminated, in that they are affected

    by factors that are beyond an individuals control, as well as deficient (Campbell, Dunnette, Lawler, &

    Weick, 1970), in that they do not capture relevant facets of the focal construct. Sources of contamination

    in the objective success of global managers, for instance, are illustrated by Hollenbeck and McCalls

    (2003) observation that countries differ substantially in their power structures, taxation systems, eco-

    nomic and social stratification, markers of status, and norms of saving. Even within societies, the objec-

    tive success experienced by those such as nurses, plumbers and taxi drivers will similarly be strongly

    affected by factors such as occupational pay norms, labor market conditions, and competition: factors

    which change independently of the actions of an individual engaged in one of these occupations. More-

    over, as Thorndike (1963) observed, objective criteria of career success have limited meaning in the

    many jobs where pay and promotions are institutionalized, such as in the civil service and the military.

    The deficiency of traditional objective criteria, such as pay and promotions, stems from the fact that

    these are not the only objective outcomes that people seek from their careers. For instance, both school

    teachers (Parsons, 2002) and academic mentors (e.g., McGrath, 2003) often frame their career success

    in terms of hard data on the learning and other attainments of their students and proteges. Similarly, bus

    and taxi drivers conceivably base their career success on their years of driving without an accident,

    industrial designers on e-mails of peer recognition for their creativity, and doctors on the proportion

    of emergency patients lives they save. Even when continual attainment of such objective outcomes

    does not lead to an increase in pay, promotions, occupational status, or rank, their value as objective

    indicants of career success is not necessarily diminished.

    CONCEPTUALIZING AND EVALUATING CAREER SUCCESS 115

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • Many people also desire less tangible, subjective outcomes such as work-life balance (Finegold &

    Mohrman, 2001), as well as a sense of meaning (Wrzesniewski, 2002), purpose (Cochran, 1990), trans-

    cendence (Dobrow, 2003), and contribution (Hall & Chandler, this issue) from their work. Friedman

    and Greenhaus (2000) had more than 800 business professionals indicate the relative importance of 15

    potential indicators of their career success. A factor analysis revealed five dimensions of the meaning

    of career success: status, time for self, challenge, security, and social. With the exception of status,

    these results reveal a considerable emphasis on subjective career success criteria that go beyond the

    objective outcomes of prestige, power, money, and advancement.

    Receiving high pay and promotions also do not necessarily make people feel proud or successful

    (Hall, 2002; Korman, Wittig-Berman, & Lang, 1981; Schein, 1978). In fact, they can cause work and

    personal alienation (Burke, 1999), as well as depressive reactions. Bandura (1997) described how

    newly appointed managers who do not delegate adequately can soon become overwhelmed and

    depressed, potentially leading to both subjective and objective career failure. Such evidence that sub-

    jective success is not necessarily a function of objective attainments highlights the importance of learn-

    ing more about the nature of subjective career success, as well as the causal relationship between

    different objective and subjective career outcomes.1

    Indeed, the potential deficiency of objective success criteria has long been recognized. For example,

    40 years ago Hilton and Dill (1962, p. 163) observed that the shortcomings of salary as a measure of

    mans progress are well known. Thus, it is puzzling that salary and promotions continue to be applied

    as the sole criteria of career success in many studies (e.g., Chenevert & Tremblay, 2002; Hurley &

    Sonnenfeld, 1998; Judiesch & Lyness, 1999; Lyness & Thompson, 2000). The potential deficiencies

    in objective success measurement may be reduced by future research that conceptualizes and assesses

    objective success in a manner that is guided by the career concerns and status hierarchies that char-

    acterize particular career contexts. Another more commonly adopted method of dealing with the defi-

    ciency of objective criteria is to measure subjective career success, in conjunction with objective

    attainments.

    Subjective criteria

    Schein (1978) argues that it is important to determine if people considered to have hierarchical and

    financial success are also satisfied with their career. Unlike objective success criteria, subjective mea-

    sures may detect important career outcomes that are not readily assessable from personnel records or

    by expert raters (Gattiker & Larwood, 1988).

    Subjective career success is most commonly operationalized as either job or career satisfaction.

    Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, and Barrick (1999) argue that as individuals who are dissatisfied with many

    aspects of their jobs are unlikely to consider their careers to be successful, job satisfaction is the most

    salient aspect of subjective career success. Many other studies (e.g., Boudreau, Boswell, & Judge,

    2001; Judge & Bretz, 1994; Judge et al., 1995; Murrell, Frieze, & Olson, 1996; Tsui & Gutek,

    1984) have also used job satisfaction as a proxy for subjective career success.

    One limitation of doing so is that a person who thinks they have a highly successful career does not

    necessarily consider it to be less successful if they begin a job that they find dissatisfying. Second, a

    person could be highly satisfied with their current job, though dissatisfied with the career attainments

    which preceded it. Third, a gratifying job with limited prospects for future career opportunities could

    invoke minimal feelings of career success. Fourth, a person may conversely hate what they are doing

    1Discussion of this latter issue is beyond the scope of the present paper, though is taken up from several different perspectives byArthur et al. (this issue), Hall and Chandler (this issue), and Nicholson and de Waal Andrews (this issue).

    116 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • but be happy with the state of their career because of the prospects it brings (Heslin, 2003a). For

    instance, Wilensky (1960) described how graduate students defer gratification by working long hours

    for little direct compensation, in the hope of being well rewarded following graduation. Finally, high

    job satisfaction does not necessarily lead to subjective career success when it exacts a high toll in terms

    of health, family relationships, or other salient personal values.

    Subjective career success thus includes reactions to actual and anticipated career-related attainments

    across (a) a broader time frame than ones immediate job satisfaction (Greenhaus, Callanan, &

    Godshalk, 2000), as well as (b) a wider range of outcomes, such as a sense of identity (Law, Meijers,

    & Wijers, 2002), purpose (Cochran, 1990), and work-life balance (Finegold & Mohrman, 2001).

    Although job satisfaction may contribute to subjective career success, they are conceptually distinct

    constructs that are not necessarily related. It follows that future research should avoid adopting job

    satisfaction as a sole proxy for subjective career success.

    Career satisfaction is most often assessed using the widely adopted (e.g., Boudreau et al., 2001;

    Judge et al., 1995; Seibert & Kraimer, 2001) career satisfaction scale developed by Greenhaus et al.

    (1990). Although such standardized measures generally have acceptable levels of internal consistency,

    such characteristics are not necessarily sufficient to validly assess each respondents subjective career

    success. For example, standardized scales with items measuring satisfaction with hierarchical success

    (e.g., Peluchette & Jeanquart, 2000) or advancement (e.g., Greenhaus et al., 1990) are likely to be of

    limited relevance to the increasing number of people who work on a contract basis (Inkson et al.,

    2001), run their own small business (Tullar, 2001), value other features of their career (e.g., service

    and companionship) much more highly (Aronsson, Bejerot, & Haerenstam, 1999), or are contentedly

    career plateaued (Slocum, Cron, Hansen, & Rawlings, 1985). Assessing superfluous, irrelevant con-

    structs is problematic owing to the inflation of the error within the resulting measurements of subjec-

    tive career success. Thus, more needs to be understood about how to balance the imperatives to assess

    only what really matters to the person being surveyed, while also making efficient and comparable

    measurements.

    Improving subjective career success measurement

    Three avenues for improving the conceptualization and measurement of subjective career success are

    (a) drawing upon research into what employees want, (b) paying greater attention to how people in

    different career contexts conceptualize their career success, and (c) adopting more qualitative methods.

    One source of insight for the first avenue is Cangemi and Guttschalks (1986) discovery that, based on

    a survey of 35 000 employees, what employees most wanted (i.e., full appreciation for work done, feel-

    ing in on things, and sympathetic understanding of personal problems) differed substantially from

    what supervisors discerned that their employees most desired (i.e., money). Another example of

    research on what people want is Finegold and Mohrmans (2001) finding that among 4500 knowledge

    workers and managers from eight countries, work-life balance was rated as the most important out of

    the many facets of a career. The failure of most career success studies to include even a single item

    assessing work-life balance highlights how considering research on what people want could poten-

    tially alter the extant evidence about the antecedents of subjective career success.

    The second, contextual avenue is foreshadowed by a recent program of theorizing, research and

    symposia presentations, regarding how career success is conceptualized by different populations.

    These include professionals transitioning to working part-time (Lee & Dohring, 2003), people with

    a global career (Hollenbeck & McCall, 2003), young classical musicians (Dobrow, 2003), people

    working in not-for-profit organizations (Steinbereithner, 2003), and CEOs (Heslin, 2003b). An exam-

    ple of how career context can be associated with subtle differences in the criteria people use to evaluate

    CONCEPTUALIZING AND EVALUATING CAREER SUCCESS 117

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • their career success is illustrated by a survey of 1481 industrial/organizational (I/O) psychologists by

    Brooks, Grauer, Thornbury, and Highhouse (2003). This study found that compared to I/O psychology

    academics (i.e., those working in universities), I/O psychological practitioners reported valuing

    affiliation, structure, and money significantly more, though autonomy and science to a lesser extent

    than their academic counterparts. This systematic within-profession variation in relative concern with

    the objective criteria of money highlights the importance of sensitivity to sub-population differences in

    the criteria adopted for judging career success.

    Third, Arthur et al. (this issue) observed a paucity of qualitative career success research within the

    leading management and psychological journals over the last decade. Thus, there appears to be plenty

    of scope for more systematic, qualitative studies of how people making their careers in different con-

    texts conceptualize their career success. This approach to improving the measurement of career suc-

    cess is exemplified by Juntunen et al.s (2001) in-depth interviews regarding the meaning of career

    success to 18 Northern Plains American Indians. Evidence of the communitarian construal of career

    success among this population, in which success is largely measured in terms of contribution to the

    well-being of others, is exemplified by the following two statements by study participants:

    To be successful, I believe, is going back to where you were raised to help the people that are there.

    (p. 278)

    I make flutes for [the young kids] so they can learn. I did 250 last year for the kids. . . . Out of allthem kids, maybe one will be a flute player and carry on a tradition. If I can get this tradition going

    with some of the young people . . . then Im happy its a success. (p. 279)

    Such findings illustrate how qualitative research may reveal hitherto largely neglected facets (e.g.,

    societal contribution) of subjective career success (see also Hall & Chandler, this issue).

    In summary, several options for improving the measurement of objective and subjective success

    have been suggested. It has been shown that some people construe their career success in subjective

    ways that transcend the objective criteria that have dominated the extant literature (Greenhaus, 2003;

    Hall, 2002). Indeed, the wide range of success criteria that different people use highlights an oppor-

    tunity to explore how theory and research may suggest when particular types of success criteria are

    likely to be most salient. The potential utility of doing so is underscored by the observation that the

    widespread uncritical and atheoretical use of narrow measures of career success has probably con-

    strained the scope and validity of career success research (Arthur et al., this issue). One potential

    option for addressing this theoretical gap in the literature is to investigate when salient others expecta-

    tions and outcomes are likely to influence how people conceptualize their career success.

    Self- and Other-Referent Career Success

    Whether career attainments lead people to experience career success is likely to depend upon the stan-

    dards against which they are evaluated. Career outcomes may be evaluated relative to personal stan-

    dards (i.e., self-referent criteria), or the attainments and expectations of others (i.e., other-referent

    criteria).

    Self-referent success criteria generally reflect an individuals career-related standards and aspira-

    tions. By contrast, other-referent criteria involve comparisons with others, such as whether one is paid

    more or less than the industry average or a colleague who performs a similar role in the same or another

    organization (Goodman, 1974). In those commonly encountered cases in which there is a range of

    118 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • potential referents, career success judgments may vary substantially depending upon which are chosen

    as the basis for comparison (Bandura, 1986; Heath, Larrick, & Wen, 1999; Wood, 1989).

    Self-referent success criteria

    Gattiker and Larwood (1988) argue that subjective career success criteria reflect personal standards

    and preferences, such as whether an individual most prefers to have solitude or social stimulation.

    Self-referent criteria may also pertain to objective criteria, such as a career goal to earn a salary of

    at least $100k per annum by the age of 30. Regardless of what other people achieve, a person who

    is highly committed to such an aspiration is liable to experience career success if it is realized, while

    potentially feeling that their career is not successful if this goal is not attained. Instances when people

    value self-satisfaction from acting in accordance with their personal standards even more highly than

    material rewards (Bandura, 1997; Hall, 2002) further highlight the importance of considering self-

    referent subjective success.

    Self-report outcome measures in the career success literature have been almost exclusively self-

    referential, with two identified notable exceptions. One is Lawrences (1984) measure of the extent

    to which respondents feel that their careers are on schedule, ahead of schedule, or behind schedule,

    relative to their peers. The other exception is two items in Turban and Doughertys (1994) 4-item mea-

    sure that assesses perceived career success relative to the other-referent criteria of your co-workers?,

    and the feelings of significant others? Aside from these studies, does the relative absence of other-

    referent criteria in the career success literature mean that people rarely evaluate their career with refer-

    ence to the expectations and attainments of other people?

    Other-referent success criteria

    If you compare yourself with others you may become vain or bitter, for always there will be greater

    and lesser persons than yourself. (Desiderata)

    The motivational and affective perils of comparing oneself to others have been well documented

    (e.g., see Dweck, 1999). Theory and research suggest, however, that people nonetheless often evaluate

    their career success relative to the outcomes achieved by other people. Social comparison theory (Fes-

    tinger, 1954) states that (a) individuals are motivated to evaluate the outcomes they attain, and (b)

    when objective information concerning the adequacy of their actions and outcomes is not available,

    they will attempt to obtain such information by comparing their actions and outcomes to those of other

    similar people. Several hundred studies have validated and extended these basic premises of social

    comparison theory (Suls & Wheeler, 2000). Within organizational settings, Mumford (1983) argues

    that reinforcement contingencies, such as promotions or raises, provide a powerful incentive for indi-

    viduals to regularly engage in social comparisons with their peers, especially when vague criteria are

    used for allocating them. Similarly, the substantial evidence that people often act in the manner pre-

    dicted by Adams (1965) equity theory (e.g., Griffeth & Gaertner, 2001; Janssen, 2001) further shows

    that individuals use the outcomes of other people as referent points when evaluating their work-related

    outcomes.

    Schein (1990) provides two anecdotes of how social comparisons operate in evaluating career suc-

    cess. In the first, an entrepreneur who made two million dollars felt like a failure because his friends all

    owned 300-million-dollar companies. In the second example, another person who leveled off in middle

    management felt very successful because he went so far beyond the level his father attained. While

    there is a literature on the use of other referents when evaluating pay satisfaction (e.g., Goodman,

    1974; Toh & DeNisi, 2003), it has generally neither ventured beyond satisfaction with the fiscal aspect

    of career success, nor substantially influenced the literature in which career success is the criterion

    CONCEPTUALIZING AND EVALUATING CAREER SUCCESS 119

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • measure. Thus, greater scholarly attention appears warranted to the potentially social comparative nat-

    ure of how people construe and assess their overall career success.

    Heslin (2003a) explored the use of other-referent criteria by 71 part-time MBA students at a

    Canadian university. Participants were asked to evaluate their career success and to be specific about

    how they know the extent to which their career is successful. Independent ratings of their written

    responses revealed that 68 per cent of participants used other-referent criteria, such as: I am paid rela-

    tively well for my peer group, Others younger than me have done better career-wise, and My super-

    visors dont appreciate my above-average performance! A majority of participants used some other-

    referent criteria, and 39 per cent of all the criteria generated were other-referent. This study also exam-

    ined the relationship between career success relative to both self-referent and other-referent criteria.

    Self-referent success was measured using a slightly modified version of Greenhaus et al.s (1990)

    widely used career satisfaction scale (e.g., How satisfied are you with the income you have attained,

    relative to your career aspirations?; How satisfied are you with the autonomy you have attained, rela-

    tive to your career aspirations?). Other-referent success was measured with items that were identical,

    except each item surveyed perceived success relative to your peers rather than relative to your career

    aspirations. The results were that ratings of other-referent success accounted for an additional 12 per

    cent of variance in ratings of overall subjective career success, when career success relative to self-

    referent criteria was held constant. Thus, other-referent success appears to be a potentially important,

    albeit largely neglected facet of subjective career success.

    Objective other-referent success criteria are exemplified by salary surveys, mean time to promotion

    data, and descriptions of successful career behaviors (e.g., Inkson et al., 2001; Zabusky & Barley,

    1996). Such criteria provide referent points for assessing whether ones career is on schedule relative

    to ones age-peers, which might be a facet of other-referent career success. For instance, based on the

    self-reports of 245 managers (aged 2266 years) from a large corporation, Lawrence (1984) found that

    perceptions of being behind, on, or ahead of schedule were related to career satisfaction, even when

    their perceptions of being ahead or behind time were incorrect.

    Other-referent career success criteria include, but extend beyond, comparing oneself to the

    attainments of others. They can also encompass our internalization of other peoples expectations. For

    instance, Gattiker and Larwood (1988) note that: what we think that our families think can determine

    how satisfied we are with our careers (p. 572, italics in original). Instances in which people choose to

    follow a long-standing family tradition of working in a particular profession (e.g., law, medicine, or

    plumbing), from among the wide range of options available to them, may reflect a combination of social

    learning and adopting the expectations of salient others as a criterion of career success (Krumboltz,

    1994).

    In summary, there is substantial theoretical and empirical reason to believe that people evaluate their

    objective and subjective career outcomes relative to other-referent, as well as self-referent criteria. In

    addition, there is preliminary evidence that evaluating other-referent career success provides unique

    information about subjective career success that has probably not been captured by most measures of

    this construct within the extant literature. These observations provide a basis for beginning to theorize

    about when different types of subjective career success criteria will be most salient.

    Types of Subjective Career Success Criteria

    Our argument that people evaluate their career success using self-referent and other-referent criteria,

    drawn from both the objective and subjective domains, is illustrated in Table 1. This table makes

    120 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • explicit that subjective career success encompasses reactions to both the objective (e.g., pay) and sub-

    jective (e.g., fulfillment) facets of ones career. It also highlights how, at any given point in the unfold-

    ing of a career, self- and other-referent criteria can be rather different. Instances when a persons

    satisfaction with her bonus changes dramatically upon hearing about the bonus received by her peers,

    as well as when an individual feels diminished upon discovering his parents attitude toward his career,

    exemplify the potential difference between self- and other-referent criteria of success.

    Table 1 also illustrates some potential correlates of when each of these four types of success criteria

    is likely to be most salient. The variables suggested are not intended to be exhaustive, but rather merely

    indicative of when different types of criteria may figure most prominently in how people evaluate their

    career success. Future research may identify and explore many others.

    As the standards people use to evaluate their experiences and attainments reflect the reciprocal influ-

    ence of both contextual and individual factors (Bandura, 1986, 1997), we first theorize about the poten-

    tial role of two contextual factors: whether the career is being pursued in a winner-take-all market

    (Frank & Cook, 1995), as well as the organizational culture (Kerr & Slocum, 1987) in which a person

    is working. Two individual factors we discuss are work orientation (Wrzesniewski, McCauley, Rozin,

    & Schwartz, 1997) and goal orientation (Dweck & Elliott, 1983). Finally, we suggest that whether a

    person has a linear or non-linear career type (Brousseau et al., 1996)a variable that embodies the

    inherent interaction between individual and contextual exigenciesmay influence when the different

    types of criteria outlined in Table 1 are most salient.

    Winner-take-all markets

    Frank and Cook (1995) argue that more and more individuals are being drawn to the pursuit of a lim-

    ited number of superstar positions in winner-take-all markets. These markets are characterized by

    huge rewards for high performance, relative to the performance exhibited by other people. For

    instance, Olympic gold medals are awarded for outperforming other contenders, rather than based

    on absolute achievements. The other defining characteristic of winner-take-all markets is that prizes

    tend to be allocated to a few top performers, with small differences in talent or effort resulting in mas-

    sive differences in income and other rewards. For instance, gold medal winners (e.g., Mary Lou Retton

    Table 1. Four types of subjective success criteria

    Objective domain Subjective domain

    Self-referent domain 1. Objective/self-referent 2. Subjective/self-referentExamples: Examples: My financial and promotion aspirations My goals for work-life balance

    and fulfilmentPotential correlate: Potential correlates: A market culture A calling orientation

    A non-linear careerOther-referent domain 3. Objective/other-referent 4. Subjective/other-referent

    Examples: Examples: My colleagues pay and my social My stimulation and fun, relative

    standing to my peersPotential correlates: Potential correlate: A winner-take-all market A clan culture A job or career orientation

    CONCEPTUALIZING AND EVALUATING CAREER SUCCESS 121

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • and Carl Lewis) often receive millions of dollars in product endorsements, while silver and bronze

    medalists commonly receive few, if any endorsements, and their almost identical athletic performances

    are soon forgotten.

    Information technologies enabling millions of people to listen and watch only star artists and ath-

    letes have, for many years, made the performing arts and professional sports winner-take-all markets.

    However, intense global competition is substantially increasing the stakes of winning, relative to being

    in second place, within many industries (e.g., software development, movie making, fashion, book

    publishing, academe, consulting, law). Organizational fortunes in these markets are widely thought

    to depend heavily on the performance of those occupying a few pivotal positions, such as the CEO,

    chief counsel, or movie director. The rewards offered to the incumbents of such roles tend to be enor-

    mous and far in excess of the rewards received by those who just miss out on such roles. Besides

    increasing the already substantial disparity between the rich and the poor, Frank and Cook (1995)

    argue that the slim prospect of becoming a wealthy star in one of these emerging winner-take-all mar-

    kets is decreasing the number and talent of those interested in making a career in other less glamorous,

    though socially important sectors, such as engineering, manufacturing, civil service, child care, and

    teaching.

    Despite these societal costs, Frank and Cook identify a range of psychological and social processes

    likely to attract and retain participants in winner-take-all markets. Aspirants consistently and notor-

    iously overestimate their chances of winning contests for highly prized occupational roles, such as

    being a supermodel, a CEO, a pro-basketball player, or a Wall Street banker (Arnett, 2000; Weinstein,

    1980). Cognitive limitations generally prevent people from learning about, remembering, and thus

    aspiring to emulate any more than the few leading stars in any field of endeavor. Finally, people

    can feel entrapped in winner-take-all markets. Failure to continually invest heavily in things they

    believe will improve their chances of success (e.g., expensive clothes, luxury cars, cosmetic surgery,

    steroid consumption, coaching) can seem tantamount to forgoing all they have already invested in

    striving to become successful.

    Social inducements to participate in winner-take-all markets include the conspicuously reported

    massive financial rewards received by winners in their respective fields. Moreover, popular publica-

    tions and television programs such as People magazine, Entertainment Weekly, and Lifestyles of the

    Rich and Famous appear devoted to celebrating and propagating materialism, conspicuous consump-

    tion, and exclusivity as criteria of success. Through social learning processes, such as role modeling

    and symbolic encoding of desired career outcomes, people tend to personally adopt the values and

    standards of those with whom they identify (Bandura, 1986, 1997). Thus, consumption of such star-

    oriented media is liable to increase the salience of objective and other-referent criteria of career suc-

    cess (e.g., ones income and status relative to ones referent group).

    Even stronger inducements to value objective and other-referent success are likely to come from

    personally participating in a winner-take-all market, via the mutually reinforcing mechanisms of

    the attractionselectionattrition (ASA) cycle (Schneider, 1987; Schneider, Smith, & Paul, 2001),

    and socialization (Trice & Beyer, 1993). Schneiders ASA model states that organizational members

    are likely to be relatively homogeneous, insofar as they are the ones who were attracted to, chosen by,

    and choose to remain with an organization, given its characteristic strategy, structure, culture, and

    reward system. There is evidence that people choose organizations that fit their need structure. For

    instance, Turban and Keon (1993) found that people with a high need for achievement choose to work

    in organizations with individual incentive systems, rather than rewards based on seniority.

    Following selection, organizations adopt socialization tactics to imbue newcomers with the values

    and perspectives of the organization. Finally, newcomers who are not effectively socialized tend to be

    dissatisfied and are unlikely to remain with an organization or within an industry (OReilly, Chatman,

    & Caldwell, 1991; Trice & Beyer, 1993). Thus, conspicuous rewards allocated for excelling ones

    122 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • peers, which are the hallmark of winner-take-all markets, are likely to be particularly salient to those

    working within such markets.

    Proposition 1: People working in winner-take-all markets use more objective and other-referent

    criteria of career success than those who work in fields where rewards are more evenly distributed,

    based on absolute rather than relative performance (see Table 1, quadrant 3).

    Organizational culture

    Even within industries and markets, differences in organizational culture may be associated with var-

    iation in how people conceptualize their career success. This potential dynamic can be illustrated by

    consideration of the prototypical market and clan organizational cultures studied by Kerr and Slocum

    (1987).

    Within a market culture, the relationship between the individual and the organization is contractual.

    Mutual obligations are explicitly specified: in return for some level of quantifiable performance, indi-

    viduals are given a commensurable and agreed level of financial reward. The other opportunities avail-

    able to both individuals and organizations determine whether and under what terms periodic

    employment contracts are renewed. Symbols of status and rank are not emphasized. Rather, a perfor-

    mance-based reward system pays large bonuses for meeting or exceeding quantified performance tar-

    gets. Little consideration is given to the qualitative aspects of performance, such as the long-term

    consequences of the manner in which short-term results are achieved. Kerr and Slocum observed that

    rather than promoting a feeling of membership in a social system, the market culture encourages a

    strong sense of independence and individuality in which everyone pursues his or her own interests

    (p. 103).

    Clan cultures, by contrast, are characterized by a more fraternal and committed relationship

    between the individual and the organization. Socialization and internalized values emphasize mutual

    interests and the tacit understanding that required contributions to the organization may exceed con-

    tractual agreements. In exchange for loyalty, senior managers in clan cultures show a greater concern

    for individuals employment security and career development than is typical of market cultures. For

    instance, promotion from within is much more common in clan cultures than in market cultures. Con-

    sideration of qualitative facets of performance, such as how results are achieved, introduces a degree of

    vagueness into the appraisal system. Compared to market cultures, financial bonuses are a small part of

    total compensation, while rituals and patterns of interaction that signify and cultivate a persons sense

    of belonging and status play an important role in clan cultures. For instance, the extensive collegial

    network within clan cultures tends to generate a sense of interdependence between organizational

    members, leading them to identify strongly with their peers, as well as their organization (Kerr &

    Slocum, 1987).

    Through the mutually reinforcing mechanisms of the ASA cycle and socialization, the quantitative

    and financially oriented nature of market cultures means that they are likely to be inhabited by people

    who primarily use objective criteria, especially pay, to evaluate their career success. By contrast, a

    relatively greater concern with subjective criteria of career success could be expected in clan organi-

    zational cultures. This is because within such cultures rich patterns of socialization and rituals cultivate

    the importance of non-pecuniary career outcomes such as the sense of fraternity, meaning, and belong-

    ing to an organization with a proud tradition and/or an important mission.

    Organizational culture may also be associated with differences in the salience attached to self-refer-

    ent versus other-referent criteria of career success. Social comparison theory (Festinger, 1954) empha-

    sizes that when clear performance metrics are not available to help individuals evaluate their

    accomplishments, they tend to evaluate themselves relative to the achievements of other people. A

    CONCEPTUALIZING AND EVALUATING CAREER SUCCESS 123

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • corollary is that social comparisons are fostered by the availability of accessible, salient bases for peer

    comparison (Festinger, 1954; Klein, 1997). Finally, in self-relevant domains, such as regarding ones

    career status, close relationships with potential referents tend to foster social comparisons (Pemberton

    & Sedikides, 2001; Tesser, 1980; Tesser, Millar, & Moore, 1988). Thus, the relatively vague perfor-

    mance criteria, the clear symbols of relative rank and status, and the closeness between organizational

    members within clan cultures is expected to foster a greater use of other-referent criteria of career suc-

    cess than typically occurs within market cultures. By contrast, the explicit linkage of rewards to quan-

    tifiable performance and the independence of organizational members within market cultures are both

    expected to increase the emphasis individuals give to self-referent criteria when evaluating their career

    success.

    Proposition 2: In clan cultures, subjective and other-referent criteria are relatively salient, compared

    to the objective and self-referent success criteria that are more salient in market cultures (see

    Table 1, quadrants 1 and 4).

    Work orientation

    People appear to differ in their prime reason for working and what they most seek from the work

    (Schein, 1990). For instance, research by both sociologists (e.g., Bellah et al., 1985) and psychologists

    (e.g., Schwartz, 1986, 1994; Wrzesniewski et al., 1997) has shown that most people have one of three

    distinct orientations to their work: seeing it primarily as either a job, a career, or a calling. The funda-

    mental distinctions between these three types of work orientation are as follows.

    People with a job orientation focus mainly on the financial rewards they receive for working, rather

    than pleasure or fulfillment. Work is viewed as a means for acquiring the resources needed to enjoy

    time away from the job, rather than as an end in itself. As the main goal of those with a job orientation

    is to make an income, they do not seek many other rewards from their work (Wrzesniewski et al.,

    1997).

    People with a career orientation exhibit a deeper personal investment in their work. They mark their

    achievements not only through monetary gain, but also through upward advancement within the occu-

    pational structure where they work. Wrzesniewski (2002) observed that the overarching goal of those

    who view their work as a career is to maximize their income, social status, power and prestige within

    their occupation (p. 232).

    Finally, people with a calling orientation strive to experience fulfillment as a result of performing

    their work. Work is seen as an inherent part of life and as an end in itself, rather than merely as a means

    to income or advancement. Those with a calling often feel that their work helps to make the world a

    better place. For instance, hospital cleaners who view their work as the meaningful task of facilitating

    the comfort and health of all who enter their hospital would probably exemplify a calling orientation

    (Dutton, Debebe, & Wrzesniewski, 2000).

    The usefulness of this tripartite work orientation concept has been established in several ways.

    Wrzesniewski et al. (1997) demonstrated that employees in a wide range of occupations, from clerical

    to professional, were unambiguous in construing their work primarily in terms of one of these three

    work orientations. In addition, empirical evidence of the predicted relationship with other work-related

    variables supports the construct validity of work orientation. Wrzesniewski et al. (1997) found that

    having a calling was associated with the highest life and job satisfaction, and with missing the fewest

    days of work. Work orientation has also been associated with how people engage in their work

    (Wrzesniewski & Dutton, 2001), the goals people pursue in job search, as well as the nature and occu-

    pational level of their new job (Wrzesniewski, 2002).

    124 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • In summary, the objective outcomes of pay and advancement are respectively the prime concern of

    those with either a job or a career orientation. By contrast, people with a calling are much more con-

    cerned with subjective outcomes that matter most to them, such as the sense of meaning and fulfillment

    they derive from their work. Rather than adopting standards of success that are guided by an organiza-

    tion in which they have a job or career, those with a calling are more likely to have self-set standards

    for evaluating their career. The multiple methods of assessing work orientation used by Wrzesniewski

    et al. (1997) may be useful in empirical tests of:

    Proposition 3: For those with a calling, subjective and self-referent criteria are more salient than

    objective and other-referent criteria of career success, compared to people with either a job or a

    career work orientation (see Table 1, quadrants 2 and 3).

    Goal orientation

    People differ in the types of goals they pursue and use to evaluate themselves. For instance, Dweck and

    Elliott (1983) distinguished between performance goals, where the purpose is to demonstrate, validate,

    and avoid negative judgments about ones competence, and learning goals, where the aim is to acquire

    new knowledge or skills. Considerable research (e.g., Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Erdley et al., 1997;

    Robins & Pals, 2002; VandeWalle, 1997) has established that a learning goal orientation is generally

    based on the incremental implicit theory that ones abilities are malleable and can thus be developed

    with persistent effort. On the other hand, underlying a performance goal orientation is typically the

    entity implicit theory that ones abilities are largely fixed, inherent attributes that are difficult to

    develop.

    Dweck and Leggett (1988) reasoned that the goal orientation resulting from these two types of

    implicit theories would affect the criteria people adopt to evaluate their success. Dweck and

    Bempechat (1983) found that entity theorist children adopted performance goals and reported that they

    felt smart when their schoolwork was error free (e.g., When I dont do mistakes) and surpassed that

    of their peers (e.g., When I turn in my papers first). By contrast, children with an incremental theory

    reported feeling smart when they worked on and mastered their personal learning goals (e.g., When

    Im reading a hard book; When I dont know how to do it and its pretty hard and I figure it out with-

    out anyone telling me). The learning orientation of those with incremental beliefs made them rela-

    tively unconcerned about their performance relative to their peers.

    Dweck (1999) theorized that in contrast to the personal learning goal orientation of incremental the-

    orists, the self-validation concerns of entity theorists would lead them to focus more on performance

    goals regarding how their presumably fixed abilities and resulting achievements compare to those of

    other people. Consistent with this theory, Butler (2000) reported that when self-appraising their aca-

    demic ability, compared to incremental theorists, children who were entity theorists paid more atten-

    tion to their performance relative to their peers. Similarly Heslin (2003a) found that part-time MBA

    students who were incremental theorists used relatively more self-referent criteria, while entity theor-

    ists used comparably more other-referent criteria to evaluate their career success. However, a more

    complete picture emerges from research regarding how the relationship between goal orientation

    and social comparisons depends upon ones level of attainment.

    Butler (1992) argued that for those with a performance goal orientation:

    . . . relative outcome is perceived as the major determinant of success and satisfaction . . . however,because satisfying (performance-oriented) goals requires one to demonstrate high, rather than low

    ability, interest in normative comparisons should be stronger among more competent than less com-

    petent individuals. (p. 935)

    CONCEPTUALIZING AND EVALUATING CAREER SUCCESS 125

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • Consistent with this hypothesis, Butler (1992) found that compared to those with a learning goal

    orientation, sixth-grade children with a performance goal orientation paid more attention to normative

    information about their performance on a divergent thinking task, particularly if they had performed

    well. In addition, the performance-oriented students attributed success on this task to ego-involved

    causes (relative ability, desire to outperform others, and desire to avoid doing worse than others) sig-

    nificantly more than did those with a learning goal orientation. Butler concluded that performance

    goal-oriented subjects who realized that they had relatively poor ability may have avoided establish-

    ing how poor it was, whereas high scorers may have received hedonic satisfaction from confirming the

    degree to which they were superior to others (p. 941). In a study of college students, Butler (1993)

    similarly observed that high performers with a performance goal orientation strived to outperform

    others and sought more normative information than either low performers with a performance goal

    orientation or those with a learning goal orientationa finding that was again replicated by Butler

    (1999).

    In summary, Dweck (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Dweck, 1999) theorized that people with a perfor-

    mance goal orientation would be more interested in normative comparisons than those with a learning

    goal orientation. Butlers (1992, 1993, 1999) evidence in this regard suggests that career research may

    fruitfully explore the following proposition:

    Proposition 4: Performance goal orientation is positively associated with use of other-referent cri-

    teria of career success, especially by those who perceive their career as successful.2

    Well-validated goal orientation scales (e.g., VandeWalle, 1997; VandeWalle, Brown, Cron, &

    Slocum, 1999) are available to facilitate empirical tests of this proposition.

    Career type

    Work careers come in many different forms. Organizations play a large role in determining the shape

    careers can take, but many authors have pointed out that careers increasingly transcend organizations

    (e.g., Arthur & Rousseau, 1996; Hall, 2002). One resulting key distinction in the literature is between

    linear and non-linear careers.

    A linear career is focused on progressive steps upward in an organizational hierarchy to positions of

    greater authority (Brousseau et al., 1996); Sturges (1999) labels this career type as climber. Brousseau

    et al. (1996) observed that People who see the ideal career in linear terms often find it difficult to

    imagine any other definition of success (p. 56). The zero-sum, competitive nature of linear careers

    can be expected to lead people to continually assess their career attainments relative to those of other

    people, such as their organizational cohort; that is, to use other-referent success criteria.

    Non-linear careers encompass many other types, such as boundaryless (DeFillippi & Arthur, 1994;

    Arthur & Rousseau, 1996), cosmopolitan (Gouldner, 1957), expert (Brousseau et al., 1996; Sturges,

    1999), protean (Hall, 1976; Hall & Moss, 1998), transitory (Brousseau et al., 1996), or self-realizer

    (Sturges, 1999) careers. These variously involve a lifelong commitment to developing a high level of

    skill in a particular field or specialty, periodic shifts between related occupational areas, specialties or

    disciplines, or regular changes between often seemingly unrelated careers. A commonality is often a

    deeply held commitment to discovering ones personal values, before shaping a career that satisfies

    these values (Brousseau et al., 1996; Sturges, 1999; Hall, 2002). Compared to traditional, organization-

    ally based linear careers, those engaged in non-linear careers are much more inclined to set their own

    2This proposition is not represented in Table 1, as it pertains only to self- and other-referent criteria.

    126 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • career agenda and determine the yardsticks by which its success is measured. As Hall (2002) observed,

    a non-linear, protean career contract is with the self and ones work (p. 34). Thus, non-linear careers

    can be expected to be evaluated against more self-referent criteria than linear careers.

    Linear career success is measured in terms of position in an organizational hierarchy, together with

    traditional symbols of success such as a large office, accountability for many staff, and demonstrable

    power to influence other people (Brousseau et al., 1996). However, it has been noted above that orga-

    nizational delayering, downsizing, and outsourcing have lessened the scope (Evans et al., 1997) and

    relative desirability (Hall, 2002; Reitman & Schneer, 2003) of objective success criteria such as hier-

    archical progression.

    Hall (2002) suggested that for those with a non-linear career, the path to the top has been replaced

    by what Herb Shepard (1984) called the path with a heart. Sheppard used this term to describe success

    in terms of ones unique vision and central values in life (p. 34). The resulting self-referent, subjective

    criteria are likely to include a broader range of elements than those that are used to evaluate a linear

    career. In contrast to the narrow concern with pay, power, and promotions that characterize linear

    careers, Brousseau et al. (1996) argued that non-linear careers tend to be driven by motives to experi-

    ence outcomes including personal growth, creativity, variety, and independence. Similarly, when some

    people re-evaluated their linear careers in light of the September 11 tragedy, Wrzesniewski (2002)

    described how their non-linear career transitions to roles such as teaching, nursing, and fire-fighting

    were guided by a broader range of altruistic, subjective success criteria than they had previously held.

    Thus, the following proposition is offered:

    Proposition 5: Subjective and self-referent criteria are particularly salient when individuals have a

    non-linear, rather than a linear career (see Table 1, quadrant 2).

    Discussion

    Career success research increasingly assesses both objective and subjective career outcomes, appar-

    ently presuming that people define their career success in largely the same way (i.e., current salary,

    promotions and job satisfaction). Consideration is rarely given to how individuals may differ in the

    types of criteria they most emphasize when evaluating their careers.

    This paper is intended to encourage and heuristically aid career theorists and researchers to focus on

    a broader range of subjective success criteria when striving to model and assess whatever career suc-

    cess means to the individuals and population(s) under consideration. The related propositions suggest

    the conditions under which each of the four types of criteria illustrated in Table 1 are most likely to be

    salient. Empirical testing of the propositions offered is required. The results will suggest the types of

    studies in the extant career success literature that are most likely to have questionable criterion-related

    and ecological validity, as a result of inattention to, for instance, participants other-referent career

    success. They may also ultimately assist scholars to model and assess as much as possible of what

    matters most to the focal population, while minimizing the noisy data that results from inadvertently

    giving equal weighting to ratings of criteria that the people being studied consider irrelevant or rela-

    tively unimportant.

    Even though most people who have careers are not white, male, well-educated managers or profes-

    sionals working in large, hierarchical organizations, the vast majority of careers research has been

    focused on this very narrow subset of all the people who are engaged in a career (Blustein, 2001;

    Sullivan, 1999). The five propositions offered in this paper are intended to be merely suggestive

    of the factors that could influence the conceptualization of career success by people from diverse

    CONCEPTUALIZING AND EVALUATING CAREER SUCCESS 127

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • educational, gender and socio-economic demographics, working in settings beyond large, profit-

    oriented organizations. More theory and research is needed on what career success means to people

    such as entrepreneurs, older workers, the self-employed, the physically and mentally challenged,

    migrant workers, expatriates, stigmatized workers (e.g., people living with HIV), teleworkers,

    the under-employed, single parents, volunteers etc. Many avenues for further enquiry can be readily

    imagined.

    Research agenda

    Based upon the premise that organizations attract individuals who hold the values they embody (Judge

    & Bretz, 1992; Schneider, 1987; Schneider et al., 2001), research could assess whether the objective

    criteria of pay, promotions, and occupational status are more salient within the definition of career

    success adopted by people who work for profit-based organizations, compared to those who work

    for not-for-profit organizations (Steinbereithner, 2003).

    Potentially relevant gender differences should also be considered. For instance, a meta-analysis by

    Konrad, Ritchie, Lieb, and Corrigall (2000) revealed that men most value objective outcomes such as

    money and advancement, while women tend to evaluate their career success in broader and more sub-

    jective ways, using criteria such as feelings of accomplishment, growth and development, challenge in

    their work, interpersonal relationships, and opportunities to help others at work. Research may use-

    fully investigate individual and contextual boundary conditions on these gender differences in career

    success criteria.

    Broader issues raised by the propositions offered in this paper pertain to the mechanisms and con-

    sequences of (a) who is chosen as a referent, (b) how organizations may strive to shape conceptions of

    success, and (c) the potential role of national cultural differences.

    Other-referent selection

    Establishing that some people use others as a referent for evaluating their career success begs the ques-

    tion of who they use as a referent. Referents may be chosen from, for instance, the same or a different

    organization, business unit, department, profession, job level, industry, family, or nationality as the

    focal person. While it has long been established that people use multiple referents regarding their

    pay (Goodman, 1974), many questions remain about the selection and weighting of referents, espe-

    cially regarding the particularly subjective facets of career success (e.g. work-life balance, contribu-

    tion to society). Research is needed regarding how such choices are driven by an individuals salient

    identities, as mediated by factors such as information availability and referent relevance (Kulik &

    Ambrose, 1992).

    Use of theoretical impetus for such research is nicely exemplified by Toh and DeNisis (2003) appli-

    cation of social identity theory (Hogg & Terry, 2001) to the issue of when Host County Nationals

    (HCNs) are likely to compare their pay with that received by expatriates. Toh and DeNisi reasoned

    that the likelihood that HCNs will choose expatriates as referents is increased by, for instance, (a)

    expatriate pay practices that differentiate HCNs and expatriates, (b) high salience of the expatriate out-

    group, (c) proximity of HCNs to expatriates, as well as (d) integration of HCNs and expatriates within

    the host unit. In addition, expatriates high failure rates could reflect HCNs offering them mediocre

    support, as a result of the latter experiencing relative pay deprivation. This dynamic highlights the

    potentially consequential nature of theory-driven research contributing to knowledge about who is

    chosen as a basis for comparison when evaluating ones career success.

    In addition to the contextual factors addressed by Toh and DeNisi, research may fruitfully exam-

    ine how referent choice is influenced by an individuals immediate career objectives. For instance,

    128 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • social comparison research has established that children and students tend to make upward compar-

    isons (to those perceived as superior) when their goal is learning or development, while making

    more downward comparisons (to those perceived as having achieved less) when their immediate

    goal is self-enhancement (i.e., to feel successful; Wood, 1989). Do such processes occur in career

    domains? Are they consciously (or otherwise) harnessed and traded-off to achieve a desired balance

    between (a) feeling successful versus (b) becoming more successful, as a result adopting upward

    comparisons from which career development and inspiration may be derived, albeit at the cost of

    feeling not-so-successful in the immediate term? Do such self-regulatory processes differ more

    between people, or the career domains in which they are applied? Can they be usefully taught

    and consciously managed so that people can simultaneously optimize both their learning and their

    experience of career success?

    Organizational interventions

    Tight labor markets for knowledge workers and other scarce human resources have stimulated interest

    in creative ways to attract, motivate, and retain required talent (e.g., Kerr, 1999; Lawler & Finegold,

    2000). Among these options is offering ideological rewards (Blau, 1964), such as the opportunity to

    contribute to a worthwhile cause, e.g., to give unlimited opportunity to women (Mary Kay Cos-

    metics), to give ordinary folks the chance to buy the same things as rich people (Wal-Mart), Eleva-

    tion of the Japanese culture and national status (Sony), and To preserve and improve human life

    (Merck). Based on the premise that helping to advance cherished ideals is intrinsically rewarding

    (Blau, 1964, p. 239), espousal of a cause can represent a distinct inducement to elicit employee con-

    tributions and commitment (Collins & Porras, 1996; Thompson & Bunderson, 2003). Research is

    needed regarding how organizational proclamations about striving to benefit salient constituen-

    ciessuch as the environment, the poor, the unborn, families, oppressed women, future gen-

    erations, etc.potentially build and sustain devotion by nurturing members relevant subjective

    criteria of career success (e.g., having made a difference)?

    The prospect of stimulating the motivation, commitment, pride, loyalty and meaningfulness

    felt by organizational members (Collins & Porras, 1996)perhaps even in lieu of adequate tradi-

    tional inducements (e.g., pay)provides a powerful incentive for organizations to encourage the

    adoption of ideologically infused subjective criteria of career success. Thompson and Bunderson

    (2003, p. 574) define ideological currency as credible commitments to pursue a valued cause or

    principle (not limited to self-interest) that are implicitly exchanged at the nexus of the indivi-

    dualorganizational relationship. Thompson and Bunderson also outline mechanisms whereby

    the creation of ideological currency may backfire (e.g., result in moral outrage) when organizations

    are perceived to have violated their ideological commitments (e.g., to help the needy, protect the

    environment).

    How do such incidents affect the construal and experience of career success by organizational mem-

    bers? Under what circumstances do people respond by altering their personal career success criteria

    versus their organizational commitment/membership? Does the salience of personal values to those

    with a career orientation, rather than a job or work orientation (Wrzesniewski et al., 1997), make

    the former particularly likely to adopt ideologically based career success criteria and/or be particularly

    sensitive to perceived ideological breeches by their employer? Does the primacy of normative, perso-

    nal victories make ideological currency a less salient criterion of career success in winner-take-all mar-

    kets than in other contexts? Research addressing such questions will suggest when people are likely to

    adopt ideology-infused criteria of career success. It may also ultimately inform initiatives aimed at

    fostering ideologically based psychological contracts and organizational commitment, without incur-

    ring the strife associated with creating expectations that an organization is unwilling or unable to meet

    (Thompson & Bunderson, 2003).

    CONCEPTUALIZING AND EVALUATING CAREER SUCCESS 129

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • Cultural differences

    Although national cultural differences have a pervasive influence upon how people approach and eval-

    uate their work (Triandis, 1994; Hofstede, 2001), they have been largely neglected in the careers lit-

    erature (Brown, 2002; Spokane, Fouad, & Swanson, 2003). The potential for cultural differences to

    moderate the propositions offered in this paper is exemplified by Browns (2002, p. 53) call for

    research to test the proposition that:

    The primary bases for job satisfaction for people with an individualism social value in order of

    importance will be (1) the congruence between the values reinforced on the job and individuals

    work values; (2) conflicts that occur between the career role and other life roles; and (3) the

    approval of the work roles by others such as parents, spouses, and friends. Job satisfaction for peo-

    ple with a collective social value will be, in order of importance, (1) the extent to which the work

    role is approved by significant others such as parents, spouses, and friends; (2) conflicts between the

    career role and other life roles; and (3) the congruence between the values reinforced by the job and

    individuals work values.

    Do other-referent criteria of career success, such as evaluating ones attainments in terms of other

    peoples expectations and group achievements, figure more prominently in collectivist than individu-

    alist cultural contexts? Do people in being-oriented (Adler, 2002) or feminine (Hofstede, 2001) cul-

    turesthat give primacy to relationships, harmony, and balanceuse more subjective and less

    objective criteria of career success than those in more doing-oriented (Adler, 2002) or masculine

    (Hofstede, 2001) cultures? Empirical tests of these propositions may elucidate how cultural values

    place boundary conditions on the propositions summarized in Table 1, as well as on the cross-cultural

    generalizability of the primarily U.S.-centric career success literature.

    Implications for measurement

    Context matters! (Blustein, 1997; Higgins, 2001; Johns, 2001). It is anticipated that research stemming

    from the present propositions and related research agenda will yield broad heuristics, rather than firm

    prescriptions, about how best to measure career success within a particular context. Preliminary heur-

    istics include (a) use relevant theory and research to guide the exploration of what matters most to

    study participants, (b) ask the focal population how they know (or anticipate that they will judge)

    the extent to which their career is successful, (c) assess participants on, for instance, the 710 criteria

    they identify as most salient, and (d) ensure that each criterion assessed (e.g., promotions relative to

    peers) is valued and relevant to those surveyed. Research is needed to test, refine, and extend these

    measurement suggestions. For instance, it may be fruitful to explore the utility of weighting success

    criteria by their rated importance to the overall career success of the person being surveyed. While this

    approach should be fairly straightforward in the context of one-on-one career coaching, it would com-

    plicate the assessment, scoring, and comparability of responses when a group of individuals is sur-

    veyed simultaneously.

    Depending on the purpose of measuring career success, researchers and practitioners alike should

    carefully trade-off the issues of (a) fidelity to how each individual conceptualizes her or his career, (b)

    utilization of theory and findings regarding factors that influence the selection of different types of

    criteria, and (c) the efficiency and parsimony of the prevailing approach of assessing everyone relative

    to the same, standardized success criteria. This paper will have succeeded if it prompts reflection on

    the relative merits of these three considerations, when theorizing about and empirically assessing

    career success.

    130 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • Conclusion

    Career success potentially means much more to people engaged in a career than has typically been

    measured by empirical research on this topic. The resulting criterion deficiency has probably dimin-

    ished the validity with which many career theories, models, and intervention programs have been

    assessed. Similarly, the criterion-related validity of research on the antecedents of career success is

    likely to have been bounded by the extent to which study participants define their career success in

    terms other than the generally narrow, self-referent operationalization of this construct adopted in most

    studies. Thus, advances in the conceptualization and operationalization of career success may enhance

    theory building and research aimed at understanding, predicting, and facilitating the experience of

    career success.

    Acknowledgements

    An earlier version of this paper was presented in P. A. Heslin and H. P. Gunz (Chairs), Unpacking and

    reconceptualizing career success, a symposium conducted at the annual meeting of the Academy of

    Management, Denver, Colorado, August 2002. The author thanks Michael Arthur, Martin Evans, Hugh

    Gunz, Tim Hall, Denise Rousseau, and John Slocum for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this

    manuscript.

    Author biography

    Peter A. Heslin is Assistant Professor of Management at the Cox School of Business, Southern

    Methodist University, Dallas, Texas, U.S.A. He received his PhD in Organizational Behavior and

    Human Resource Management from the Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto,

    Canada. His current research is focused on feedback, coaching, and career success, as well as the nat-

    ure and consequences of implicit theories about the malleability of personal attributes.

    References

    Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental socialpsychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267299). New York: Academic Press.

    Adler, N. (2002). International dimensions of organizational behavior. Cincinnati: South-Western.Arnett, J. J. (2000). High hopes in a grim world: emerging adults view of their futures and Generation X. Youthand Society, 31, 267286.

    Aronsson, G., Bejerot, E., & Haerenstam, A. (1999). Healthy work: ideal and reality among public and privateemployed academics in Sweden. Public Personnel Management, 28, 197215.

    Arthur, M. B., & Rousseau, D. M. (1996). Introduction: the boundaryless career as a new employment principle. InM. B. Arthur, & D. M. Rousseau (Eds.), The boundaryless career (pp. 320). New York: Oxford UniversityPress.

    CONCEPTUALIZING AND EVALUATING CAREER SUCCESS 131

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Bandura, A. (1997). Self efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.Bellah, R. N., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W. M., Swidler, A., & Tipton, S. M. (1985). Habits of the heart: Individualismand commitment in American life. New York: Harper & Row.

    Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley.Blustein, D. L. (1997). A context-rich perspective of career exploration across the life roles. Career DevelopmentQuarterly, 45, 260274.

    Blustein, D. L. (2001). Extending the reach of vocational psychology: toward an inclusive and integratedpsychology of working. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 59, 171182.

    Boudreau, J. W., Boswell, W. R., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Effects of personality on executive career success in theUnited States and Europe. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 5381.

    Brooks, M. E., Grauer, E., Thornbury, E. E., & Highhouse, S. (2003). Value differences between scientists andpractitioners: a survey of SIOP members. The Industrial-Organizational Psychologist, 40, 1723.

    Brousseau, K. R., Driver, M. J., Eneroth, K., & Larsson, R. (1996). Career pandemonium: realigning organizationsand individuals. Academy of Management Executive, 10, 5266.

    Brown, D. (2002). The role of work and cultural values in occupational choice, satisfaction, and success: atheoretical statement. Journal of Counseling and Development, 80, 4856.

    Burke, R. J. (1999). Career success and personal failure feelings among managers. Psychological Reports, 84,651653.

    Butler, R. (1992). What young people want to know when: effects of mastery and ability goals on interest indifferent kinds of social comparisons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 934943.

    Butler, R. (1993). Effects of task- and ego-achievement goals on information seeking during task engagement.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1831.

    Butler, R. (1999). Information seeking and achievement motivation in middle childhood and adolescence: the roleof conceptions of ability. Developmental Psychology, 35, 146163.

    Butler, R. (2000). Making judgments about ability: the role of implicit theories of ability in moderating inferencesfrom temporal and social comparison information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 965978.

    Campbell, J. P., Dunnette, M. D., Lawler, E. E., & Weick, K. E. (1970). Managerial behavior, performance andeffectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Cangemi, J. P., & Guttschalk, G. E. (1986). What employees really want from their jobs. Psychology: A Journal ofHuman Behavior, 23, 5761.

    Cappelli, P. (1999). The new deal at work: Managing the market-driven workforce. Boston, MA: Harvard BusinessSchool Press.

    Chartrand, J. M., & Rose, M. L. (1996). Career interventions for at-risk populations: incorporating social cognitiveinfluences. Career Development Quarterly, 44, 341354.

    Chenevert, D., & Tremblay, M. (2002). Managerial career success in Canadian organizations: is gender adeterminant? International Journal of Human Resource Management, 13, 920942.

    Cochran, L. R. (1990). The sense of vocation: A study of career and life development. Albany, NY: StateUniversity of New York Press.

    Collins, J. C., & Porras, J. I. (1996). Building your companys vision, Harvard Business Review, SeptemberOctober, 6577.

    DeFillippi, R. J., & Arthur, M. B. (1994). The boundaryless career: a competency-based perspective. Journal ofOrganizational Behavior, 15, 307324.

    Dobrow, S. (2003). Following their hearts? Subjective career orientations and career-related outcomes in youngclassical musicians. In P. A. Heslin (Chair), What more is there to career success? Symposium conducted at theannual meeting of the Academy of Management, Seattle, WA, August 2003.

    Dutton, J. E., Debebe, G., & Wrzesniewski, A. (2000). A social valuing perspective on relationship sensemaking.Working paper, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

    Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia, PA:Psychology Press.

    Dweck, C. S., & Bempechat, J. (1983). Childrens theories of intelligence. In S. Paris, G. Olsen, & H. Stevenson(Eds.), Learning and motivation in the classroom (pp. 239256). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Dweck, C. S., & Elliott, E. S. (1983). Achievement motivation. In P. H. Mussen (Gen. Ed.) & E. M. Hetherington(Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Social and personality development (Vol. IV, pp. 643691).New York: Wiley.

    Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. PsychologicalReview, 95, 256272.

    132 P. A. HESLIN

    Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 113136 (2005)

  • Erdley, C. A., Loomis, C. C., Cain, K. M., Dumas-Hines, F., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Relations among childrenssocial goals, implicit personality theories, and responses to social failure. Developmental Psychology, 33, 263272.

    Evans, M. G., Gunz, H. P., & Jalland, R. M. (1997). Implications of organizational downsizing for managerialcareers. Canadian Journal of Administrative Sciences, 14, 359371.

    Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117140.Finegold, D., & Mohrman, S. A. (2001). What do employees really want? The perception vs. the reality. Paper

    presented at the annual meeting of the World Economic Forum, Davos, Switzerland.Frank, R. H., & Cook, P. J. (1995). The winner-take-all society: How more and more Americans compete for fewerand bigger prizes, encouraging economic waste, income inequality, and an impoverished cultural life. NewYork: Free Press.

    Friedman, S. D., & Greenhaus, J. H. (2000). Allies or enemies? How choices about work and family affect thequality of mens and womens lives. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Gattiker, U. E., & Larwood, L. (1988). Predictors for managers career mobility, success, and satisfaction. HumanRelations, 41, 569591.

    Goodman, P. S. (1974). An examination of referents used in the evaluation of pay. Organizational Behavior andHuman Performance, 12, 170195.

    Gouldner, A. W. (1957). Cosmopolitans and locals: toward an analysis of latent social roles. AdministrativeScience Quarterly, 2, 281306.

    Greenhaus, J. H. (2003). Career dynamics. In W. C. Borman, D. R. Ilgen, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Comprehensivehandbook of psychology. Industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 519540). New York:Wiley.

    Greenhaus, J. H., Callanan, G. A., & Godshalk, V. M. (2000). Career management (3rd ed.). Fort Worth, TX:Dryden Press.

    Greenhaus, J. H., Parasuraman, S., & Wormley, W. (1990). Effects of race on organizational experiences, jobperformance evaluations, and career outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 33, 6486.

    Griffeth, R. W., & Gaertner, S. (2001). A role for equity theory in the turnover process: an empirical test. Journalof Applied Social Psychology, 31, 10171037.

    Hall, D. T. (1976). Careers in organizations. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Hall, D. T. (2002). Careers in and out of organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Hall, D. T., & Moss, J. E. (1998). The new protean career contract: helping organizations and employees adapt.Organizational Dynamics, 26, 2237.

    Hartung, P. J., & Blustein, D. L. (2002). Reason, intuition, and social justice: elaborating on Parsons careerdecision-making model. Journal of Counseling and Development, 80, 4148.

    Heath, C., Larrick, R. P., & Wen, G. (1999). Goals as reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 79109.Heslin, P. A. (2003a). Self- and other-referent criteria of career success. Journal of Career Assessment, 11, 262

    286.Heslin, P. A. (2003b). CEO career success as perceived value-added. In P. A. Heslin (Chair), What more is there tocareer success? Symposium conducted at the annual meeting of the Academy of Management, Seattle, WA,August 2003.

    Higgins, M. C. (2001). Changing careers: the effects of social context. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22,