examination of the correlates of the three dimensions of job burnout

12
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/14596463 A meta-analytic examination of the correlates of the three dimensions of job burnout. ARTICLE in JOURNAL OF APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY · MAY 1996 Impact Factor: 4.31 · DOI: 10.1037//0021-9010.81.2.123 · Source: PubMed CITATIONS 959 DOWNLOADS 509 VIEWS 238 2 AUTHORS, INCLUDING: Blake E Ashforth Arizona State University 87 PUBLICATIONS 11,984 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Available from: Blake E Ashforth Retrieved on: 22 July 2015

Upload: mark-pit

Post on 07-Sep-2015

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

.

TRANSCRIPT

  • Seediscussions,stats,andauthorprofilesforthispublicationat:http://www.researchgate.net/publication/14596463

    Ameta-analyticexaminationofthecorrelatesofthethreedimensionsofjobburnout.

    ARTICLEinJOURNALOFAPPLIEDPSYCHOLOGYMAY1996

    ImpactFactor:4.31DOI:10.1037//0021-9010.81.2.123Source:PubMed

    CITATIONS

    959

    DOWNLOADS

    509

    VIEWS

    238

    2AUTHORS,INCLUDING:

    BlakeEAshforth

    ArizonaStateUniversity

    87PUBLICATIONS11,984CITATIONS

    SEEPROFILE

    Availablefrom:BlakeEAshforth

    Retrievedon:22July2015

    http://www.researchgate.net/publication/14596463_A_meta-analytic_examination_of_the_correlates_of_the_three_dimensions_of_job_burnout?enrichId=rgreq-6ce4b9cf-5c7b-4050-9f43-7decbf76e405&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzE0NTk2NDYzO0FTOjEyOTczNjg5ODU1MTgxMUAxNDA3OTQzMDk1NjIx&el=1_x_2http://www.researchgate.net/publication/14596463_A_meta-analytic_examination_of_the_correlates_of_the_three_dimensions_of_job_burnout?enrichId=rgreq-6ce4b9cf-5c7b-4050-9f43-7decbf76e405&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzE0NTk2NDYzO0FTOjEyOTczNjg5ODU1MTgxMUAxNDA3OTQzMDk1NjIx&el=1_x_3http://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-6ce4b9cf-5c7b-4050-9f43-7decbf76e405&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzE0NTk2NDYzO0FTOjEyOTczNjg5ODU1MTgxMUAxNDA3OTQzMDk1NjIx&el=1_x_1http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Blake_Ashforth?enrichId=rgreq-6ce4b9cf-5c7b-4050-9f43-7decbf76e405&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzE0NTk2NDYzO0FTOjEyOTczNjg5ODU1MTgxMUAxNDA3OTQzMDk1NjIx&el=1_x_4http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Blake_Ashforth?enrichId=rgreq-6ce4b9cf-5c7b-4050-9f43-7decbf76e405&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzE0NTk2NDYzO0FTOjEyOTczNjg5ODU1MTgxMUAxNDA3OTQzMDk1NjIx&el=1_x_5http://www.researchgate.net/institution/Arizona_State_University?enrichId=rgreq-6ce4b9cf-5c7b-4050-9f43-7decbf76e405&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzE0NTk2NDYzO0FTOjEyOTczNjg5ODU1MTgxMUAxNDA3OTQzMDk1NjIx&el=1_x_6http://www.researchgate.net/profile/Blake_Ashforth?enrichId=rgreq-6ce4b9cf-5c7b-4050-9f43-7decbf76e405&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzE0NTk2NDYzO0FTOjEyOTczNjg5ODU1MTgxMUAxNDA3OTQzMDk1NjIx&el=1_x_7
  • Journal of Applied Psychology1996, Vol. S I . No. 2, 123-133

    Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.002I-90IO/96/S3.00

    A Meta-Analytic Examination of the Correlates of theThree Dimensions of Job Burnout

    Raymond T. LeeUniversity of Manitoba

    Blake E. AshforthConcordia University

    This meta-analysis examined how demand and resource correlates and behavioral andattitudinal correlates were related to each of the 3 dimensions of job burnout. Both thedemand and resource correlates were more strongly related to emotional exhaustion thanto either depersonalization or personal accomplishment. Consistent with the conserva-tion of resources theory of stress, emotional exhaustion was more strongly related to thedemand correlates than to the resource correlates, suggesting that workers might havebeen sensitive to the possibility of resource loss. The 3 burnout dimensions were differ-entially related to turnover intentions, organizational commitment, and control coping.Implications for research and the amelioration of burnout arc discussed.

    Over the past dozen years, the phenomenon of job

    burnout has been investigated in a variety of service oc-

    cupations and settings. The construct has been linked to

    job stress and is thought to represent a unique response

    to frequent and intense client-patient interactions

    (Cordes&Dougherty, 1993; Maslach, 1982).

    According to Maslach (1982), burnout consists of

    three dimensions: emotional exhaustion, depersonaliza-

    tion (felt distance from others), and diminished personal

    accomplishment. Recent efforts have been made toward

    understanding how various correlates are associated with

    each of the three dimensions (Burke & Richardsen,

    1993; Cordes& Dougherty, 1993;Shirom, 1989).

    The conservation of resources theory of stress

    (Hobfoll, 1989; Hobfoll & Freedy, 1993) provides a

    framework for understanding how such correlates are re-

    Raymond T. Lee, Department of Business Administration.University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada; BlakeE. Ashforth, Department of Management, Concordia Univer-sity, Montreal. Quebec, Canada.

    Portions of this study were presented at the 13th World Con-gress of Sociology: Sociology of Mental Health Working Group,Bielefeld, Germany, July 1994.

    This study was funded in part by a 1993-1994 University ofManitoba, Faculty of Management research support grant.

    We are indebted to Monica Filz for assistance with the datacollection and Stephen Hladkyj for assistance with the meta-analysis.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressedto Raymond T. Lee, Department of Business Administration,Faculty of Management, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg,Manitoba, Canada R3T 2N2. Electronic mail may be sent viaBitnet to [email protected].

    lated to burnout. The theory suggests that burnout oc-

    curs when certain valued resources are lost, are inade-

    quate to meet demands, or do not yield the anticipated

    returns. The major demands of work include role ambi-

    guity, role conflict, stressful events, heavy workload, and

    pressure. The major resources include social support

    from various sources; job enhancement opportunities,

    such as control, participation in decision making, and au-

    tonomy; and reinforcement contingencies (Burke & Ri-

    chardsen, 1993; Cordes& Dougherty, 1993). The theory

    also states that certain behavioral and attitudinal out-

    comes arc likely to occur as a result of resource loss and

    burnout. The major outcomes include behavioral coping

    responses; turnover intentions; and erosion of organiza-

    tional commitment, job involvement, and job satisfac-

    tion (Burke & Richardsen, 1993; Kahili, 1988).

    Using the conservation of resources framework, recent

    studies have investigated how certain demand and re-

    source predictors are associated with each of the burnout

    dimensions (Lee & Ashforth, 1993a; Leiter, 1993;Leiter

    & Maslach, 1988). Leiter and Maslach suggested that

    work demands and resources are potential sources of

    stress in the stress-strain-coping-self-evaluation process

    (Cummings & Cooper, 1979; Lazarus & Folkman,

    1984). Emotional exhaustion as a form of strain is di-

    rectly affected by these correlates, whereas depersonaliza-

    tion as a form of defensive coping and personal accom-

    plishment as a form of self-evaluation are indirectly

    affected through emotional exhaustion.

    Leiter and Maslach's (1988) model has received par-

    tial support. Lee and Ashforth (1993a, 1993b) found

    several of the correlates to be associated with emotional

    exhaustion and depersonalization, but the correlates

    123

  • 124 LEE AND ASHFORTH

    were only weakly associated with personal accomplish-

    ment. Moreover, Golembiewski's (1989) model and Lee

    and Ashforth's (1993b) data have raised some doubts re-

    garding the sequential ordering among the burnout di-

    mensions as posited by Leiter and Maslach.

    In light of these findings, Leiter {1993) proposed a re-

    vised model in which the demand and resource correlates

    are posited to be differentially associated with the three

    dimensions. Specifically, the demands are posited to be

    more strongly related to emotional exhaustion, whereas

    the resources are posited to be more strongly related to

    either depersonalization or personal accomplishment. As

    Hobfoll and Freedy (1993) stated, demands trigger strain

    in the form of physical and emotional exhaustion,

    whereas resources help to overcome the need for defen-

    sive coping and enhance one's self-efficacy. Leiter (1991 b,

    1993) also posited that outcomes reflecting withdrawal

    tendencies (e.g., escape coping, low job involvement, and

    desire to quit) are more related to either emotional ex-

    haustion or depersonalization, whereas outcomes re-

    flecting positive self-efficacy (e.g., control coping and fa-

    vorable work attitudes) are more related to personal

    accomplishment.

    Leiter's (1993) revised model also posits how the burn-

    out dimensions themselves may be interrelated (cf. Go-

    lembiewski, 1989; Leiter, 1989). Specifically, it is now

    believed that both emotional exhaustion and personal ac-

    complishment develop in parallel with (rather than fol-

    lowing from) each other. Hence, both of these dimensions

    are posited to be "reactions to different aspects of the

    work environment that pose difficulties for human ser-

    vice workers" (Leiter, 1993, p. 246).

    Although the recent models are based in part on Hob-

    foil's (1989) conservation of resources theory, the empir-

    ical evidence has been piecemeal and limited in scope.

    Thus, our meta-analysis assessed the associations be-

    tween various correlates and each of the burnout dimen-

    sions. Only studies using the Maslach Burnout Inventory

    (MBI; Maslach & Jackson, 1981, 1986) were included

    because the scale is the most widely used operationaliza-

    tion of burnout. As a multidimensional measure, the

    MBI makes it possible to determine whether the symp-

    toms of burnout are empirically distinct from each other

    and whether they are differentially associated with other

    variables. Therefore, our study provides a summary of

    the bivariate associations reported in the literature and

    has implications for the diagnosis and the amelioration

    of burnout.

    Method

    Literature Search

    We undertook both computer and manual searches for pub-lished articles that used the MBI. Using the key phrase "Mas-

    lach Burnout Inventory," the computer search scanned the ab-stract databases of clinical psychology, education, management,nursing, psychology, social work, and sociology from 1982 to1994. The manual search scanned the reference lists of Burkeand Richardsen (1993), Cordes and Dougherty (1993), Kahili(1988), and Shirom (1989) for published sources not found inthe aforementioned abstracts.

    The search yielded 77 sources, of which 58 provided suffi-cient information to conduct the meta-analysis. Of the remain-ing 19 sources, 11 examined a correlate unique to that study,and 8 did not provide information on reliability estimates, in-tercorrelations among the variables, or both. A letter requestingthe missing information was sent to one of the authors of eachof these 8 articles. This request yielded 3 additional sources, fora total of 61.1 The remaining 5 and the 11 with unique corre-lates were excluded from the analysis.

    The 61 studies used in the meta-analysis covered 33 corre-lates based on 56 independent samples. The sample sizes rangedfrom 34 to 906 (M = 198.07, SD = 172.08). Approximately80% of the studies sampled human service providers (e.g.,teachers, nurses, counselors, police officers, and social workers),whereas most of the remaining studies sampled supervisors andmanagers of such service providers. Because the symptoms ofburnout appear to be fundamentally similar across hierarchicallevels (Lee & Ashforth, 1993a, 1993b; Maslach & Jackson,1986), we did not differentiate the results on these bases.

    Each study was coded on whether the MBI used the frequencyresponse format, the intensity response format, or both (seeMaslach & Jackson, 1981); the reliability estimates of all mea-sures; and the correlations among the burnout dimensions andcorrelates.

    Mela-Analytic Procedure

    We followed the procedure given by Hunter and Schmidt(1990). This procedure determines the extent to which corre-lation differences across studies can be attributed to certain sta-tistical artifacts. It is only after such artifacts are taken into ac-count that the corrected correlations between variables, as wellas the variance in correlations across studies, can be estimated.

    First, we calculated the uncorrected weighted mean correla-tion (/) by using the zero-order correlations. Second, becausereliability estimates might have varied widely across studies, wecorrected each within-study zero-order correlation for mea-surement unreliability (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). Where reli-ability estimates were not reported, we substituted the weightedmean reliability (see Table 1) into the correction formula.Then, we calculated the individually corrected weighted meancorrelation (rc). Because so few studies reported scale ranges,means, and standard deviations, it was not possible to correctfor range restrictions.

    Third, we computed the variance of the individually cor-rected weighted mean correlation after removing the variances

    1 The correlations were provided upon request for the articlesby Constable and Russell (1986); Ross, Altmaier, and Russell(1989); and Russell, Altmaier, and Van Velzen (1987).

  • META-ANALYSIS OF JOB BURNOUT 125

    Table 1Weighted Mean Reliability Estimates oj All Variables

    Correlate

    Job stressorsRole ambiguityRole clarityRole conflictRole stressStressful eventsWorkloadWork pressurePhysical comfort

    SupportSocial supportSupervisor supportCoworker supportWork friendsCommunity bondFamily resourcesPeer cohesionTeam cohesion

    Job enhancement opportunitiesAutonomyInnovationParticipationSkill utilizationTask orientation"

    k

    66

    1034

    554

    6121324252

    114664

    N

    1,518929

    2,612703903

    1,238681433

    1,0103,3773,340

    153433244657156

    2,177433

    1,7631.489

    433

    xx

    .78

    .85

    .79

    .76

    .87

    .76

    .79

    .80

    .69

    .85

    .86

    .80

    .80

    .70

    .74

    .86

    .79

    .80

    .83

    .80

    .75

    Correlate

    Reinforcement contingenciesUnmet expectationsContingent rewardsNoncontingent rewardsContingent punishmentsNoncontingent punishments

    Frequency response formatEmotional exhaustionDepersonalizationPersonal accomplishment

    Intensity response formatEmotional exhaustionDepersonalizationPersonal accomplishment

    Behavioral and attitudinal outcomesControl copingPreventive copingTurnover intentionsOrganizational commitmentJob involvementJob satisfactionAttitudes toward workplace climate

    k

    22222

    474747

    999

    32768

    162

    N

    248496496496496

    9,6689,6689,668

    1,6371,6371,637

    425470

    1,2311,8661,0773,788

    227

    Rxx

    .59

    .96

    .69

    .84

    .80

    .86

    .76

    .77

    .82

    .78

    .79

    .83

    .64.78.87.72.79.80

    Note, k = number of samples providing reliability information; N = total number of individuals in the k samples; 7?Xx = weighted mean reliability

    estimate of each variable across the k samples." Extent to which the work environment emphasizes efficiency and good planning.

    due to sampling error and measurement unreliability. We alsocomputed the percentage of variance due to uncorrected arti-facts. Fourth, we computed both the credibility and confidenceintervals (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). The credibility intervalwas based on the standard deviation of the distribution of effectsizes after correcting for statistical artifacts and provided infor-mation on whether moderators were likely to be present.2 Theconfidence interval was based on the standard error of the meaneffect size and provided information on the accuracy of the in-dividually corrected weighted mean correlation estimate. Last,we conducted a test for the homogeneity of effect sizes acrosssamples by using Hunter and Schmidt's (1990) Q statistic. Be-cause of disagreements over the appropriate ratio of artifacts tosample variance to infer that the effect sizes are homogeneous(cf. Hunter & Schmidt, 1990), we used both the credibility in-terval and the Q statistic to detect moderators (Whitener,1990). Where the Q statistic could not be calculated, we usedonly the credibility interval.

    To determine if the effect sizes are homogeneous, Whitener(1990, p. 317) recommended that the credibility interval bechecked to see if it is "sufficiently large" or includes zero. How-ever, it is unclear what qualifies as a sufficiently large interval.Thus, the effect sizes were considered homogeneous if the cred-ibility interval did not include zero and the Q statistic was notsignificant. We computed the confidence interval for homoge-neous effect sizes by using the formula provided by Whitener(1990, p. 316). The effect sizes were considered heterogeneousif the credibility interval did include zero or if the Q statistic was

    significant (either case indicated that a moderator was probablyoperating). We computed the confidence interval for heteroge-neous effect sizes by using the formula provided by Whitener(1990, p. 317).

    Results

    Table 1 reports the number of studies, the total sample

    size, and the weighted mean reliability estimate for each

    variable. Tables 2-5 report the uncorrected weighted

    mean correlation, the individually corrected weighted

    mean correlation, the variance, the lower and upper

    range of the uncorrected weighted mean correlation

    based on the 95% confidence interval, and the results of

    the Q test for each correlate.

    Mela-Correlations Among the Burnout Dimensions

    Of the 61 studies included in the meta-analysis, 33

    used the frequency response format, 11 used the intensity

    2 A credibility interval that includes zero indicates the prob-ability of a disordinal moderator, where the relationship be-tween two variables is positive in certain samples and negativein others. However, a moderator is still possible even when theinterval does not include zero but is large enough to suggest thatthe effect sizes are heterogeneous (Whitener, 1990).

  • 126 LEE AND ASHFORTH

    response format, and the remaining 17 used both, asMaslach and Jackson (1981) did originally. Of the 17studies that used both formats, 9 provided intradimen-

    sional correlations between response formats. Across all

    three dimensions, the individually corrected weighted

    mean correlations were greater than .90, suggesting that

    the two formats are largely redundant and that only oneis necessary (Maslach & Jackson, 1986; Schaufeli, Enz-

    mann, & Girault, 1993).Table 2 shows the meta-correlations among the three

    burnout dimensions. For the frequency response format,

    emotional exhaustion was strongly related to depersonal-ization (rc = .64), whereas both dimensions were moder-

    ately negatively related to personal accomplishment (rcs= .33 for emotional exhaustion and .36 fordepersonalization). The individually corrected weighted

    mean correlations among the three dimensions for the

    intensity response format were similar in magnitudes.Given the high correlations between the two response for-

    mats and that only a few studies used the intensity re-sponse format, all subsequent meta-analyses examinedthe three dimensions using only the frequency response

    format.

    Homogeneity of Effect Sizes

    Before examining the individually corrected weighted

    mean correlations between the correlates and the burn-out dimensions, we determined the number of homoge-

    neous effect sizes. For emotional exhaustion, the effect

    sizes were homogeneous for 13 of 32 correlates; for de-

    personalization, the effect sizes were homogeneous for 16

    of 33 correlates; and for personal accomplishment, theeffect sizes were horn ogeneous for 10 of 3 2 correlates. Be-

    cause of the small number of studies per variable, it was

    not possible to conduct any moderator analysis on corre-

    lates with heterogeneous effect sizes. Their individuallycorrected weighted mean correlations should thus be in-

    terpreted with caution, especially where both the credi-

    bility and confidence intervals include zero (Whitener,

    1990).

    Mela-Correlations Between the Correlates and the

    Burnout Dimensions

    For the demand correlates, Table 3 shows that the indi-

    vidually corrected weighted mean correlations betweenemotional exhaustion and role conflict, role stress, stress-

    ful events, workload, and work pressure were all greater

    than or equal to .50. Table 4 shows that the individuallycorrected weighted mean correlations between deperson-alization and role ambiguity, role conflict, role stress,

    stressful events, and workload were all greater than orequal to .34. Table 5 shows that the individually correctedweighted mean correlations between personal accom-

    plishment and the demand correlates were generally

    weak, with the highest being .22.For the resource correlates, Table 3 shows that the in-

    dividually corrected weighted mean correlations between

    emotional exhaustion and social support, supervisor sup-port, community bond, innovation, participation, unmet

    expectations, and noncontingent punishment were all

    greater than or equal to | .301. Table 4 shows that the in-dividually corrected weighted mean correlations between

    Table 2Mela-Correlations Among the Job Burnout Dimensions

    95% CI

    Burnout dimension-burnout dimension k N r rc VAR Lower Upper% due toartifacts Q statistic

    Frequency response format

    Emotional exhaustion-depersonalizationEmotional exhaustion-personal accomplishmentDepersonalization-personal accomplishment

    474747

    9,6688,9778,977

    .52-.27-.29

    .64-.33-.36

    .029

    .024

    .053

    .54-.46-.52

    .75-.21-.26

    88.7655.6988.00

    36.4010.4449.01*

    Intensity response format

    Emotional exhaustion-depersonalizationEmotional exhaustion-personal accomplishmentDepersonalization-personal accomplishment

    999

    1,6371,6371.637

    .53-.18-.27

    .65-.22-.34

    .050

    .082

    .085

    .53-.37-.54

    .77-.08-.15

    86.6786.6987.48

    69.53**67.70**71.92**

    Note. The boldface r^s indicate that the 95% credibility interval did not include zero. In cases where the corrected variances were less than zero, theQ statistic could not be calculated. When credibility intervals did not include zero and the Q statistic was not significant, the confidence interval (CI)for homogeneous effect sizes was constructed. Otherwise, the CI for heterogeneous effect sizes was constructed, k = number of studies; N = combinedsample size; r = uncorrccted weighted mean correlation; rc = weighted mean correlation after correcting for within-study measurement unreliability;VAR = variance of after correcting for sampling error and measurement unreliability; % due to artifacts = percentage of variance due to artifactsother than sampling error and measurement unreliability.V :.()]. **/>

  • META-ANALYSIS OF JOB BURNOUT 127

    Table 3Meta-Correlations With Emotional Exhaustion (Frequency)

    95% CI

    Correlate

    Job stressors (demands)Role ambiguityRole clarityRole conflictRole stressStressful eventsWorkloadWork pressurePhysical comfort

    Support (resources)Social supportSupervisor supportCoworker supportWork friendsCommunity bondFamily resourcesPeer cohesionTeam cohesion

    Job enhancement opportunities (resources)InnovationAutonomyParticipationSkill utilizationTask orientation'

    Reinforcement contingencies (resources)Unmet expectationsContingent rewardsNoncontingent rewardsContingent punishmentsNoncontingent punishments

    Behavioral and attitudinal outcomesControl copingTurnover intentionsOrganizational commitmentJob involvementJob satisfactionAttitudes toward workplace climate

    k

    66

    1134

    654

    613142

    425

    2

    4

    11

    674

    22

    222

    377

    8172

    N

    1,518929

    2,824703903

    1,450681433

    1,0103,5893,552

    153433244

    657156

    4332,1771,7631,701

    433

    248496496496496

    4251,2312,0781,0774,000

    227

    r

    .16

    -.30.42

    .50

    .45

    .52

    .41

    -.09

    -.26-.31-.18

    ,05-.38-.16-.08-.20

    -.24-.13-.26-.20-.20

    .38-.24

    .12

    .09

    .27

    -.26.37

    -.38-.03-.26-.11

    rc

    .21-.35

    .53

    .62

    .52

    .65

    .50-.11

    -32-.37-.22

    .05-.48-.17-.10-.22

    -.30-.15

    .31-.28-.26

    .53-.26

    .15

    .10

    .32

    -.30.44

    -.43-.04-.31

    -.13

    VAR

    .215

    .012

    .032

    .002

    .039

    .009

    .006

  • 128 LEE AND ASHFORTH

    Table 4

    Meta-Correlations With Depersonalization (Frequency)

    95% Cl

    Correlate

    Job stressors (demands)Role ambiguityRole clarityRole conflictRole stressStressful eventsWorkloadWork pressurePhysical comfort

    Support (resources)Social supportSupervisor supportCoworker supportWork friendsCommunity bondFamily resourcesPeer cohesionTeam cohesion

    Job enhancement opportunities (resources)InnovationAutonomyParticipationSkill utilizationTask orientation"

    Reinforcement contingencies (resources)Unmet expectationsContingent rewardsNoncontingent rewardsContingent punishmentsNoncontingent punishments

    Behavioral and attitudinal outcomesControl copingPreventive copingTurnover intentionsOrganizational commitmentJob involvementJob satisfactionAttitudes toward workplace climate

    k

    66

    1124

    554

    7121324

    25

    2

    4I I

    5

    64

    222

    22

    324

    76

    122

    A"

    1,518929

    2,824286903

    1,033681433

    1,1763,1723,135

    153433244657156

    4332,1771,3461,663

    433

    496496496496496

    425470564

    2.078749

    2,102227

    /

    .25-.28

    .27

    .41

    .39

    .24

    .18

    .04

    -.16-.19-.17-.07-.36-.15-.14-.28

    -.22-.10-.14-.29- .23

    .13-.10

    .18

    .08

    .20

    -.24-.25

    .24-.33-.09-.33-.12

    re

    .34-.27

    .37

    .54

    .50

    .34

    .22

    .06

    -.21-.24-.22-.08-.46-.21-.20-.34

    -.28-.13-.17-.39-.29

    .19-.11

    .25

    .10

    .25

    -.28-.37

    .31-.42-.11-.44-.15

    VAR

    .030

    .004

    .056

  • META-ANALYSIS OF JOB BURNOUT 129

    Table 5Meta-Correlations With Personal Accomplishment (Frequency)

    95% CI

    Correlate

    Job stressors (demands)Role ambiguityRole clarity

    Role conflictRole stressStressful eventsWorkloadWork pressurePhysical comfort

    Support (resources)Social supportSupervisor supportCoworker support

    Work friendsCommunity bondFamily resourcesPeer cohesionTeam cohesion

    Job enhancement opportunities (resources)InnovationAutonomyParticipationSkill utilizationTask orientation8

    Reinforcement contingencies (resources)Unmet expectationsContingent rewardsNoncontingent rewardsContingent punishmentsNoncontingent punishments

    Behavioral and attitudinal outcomesControl copingTurnover intentionsOrganizational commitmentJob involvementJob satisfaction

    Attitudes toward workplace climate

    k

    66

    1034754

    7

    1315242

    52

    411664

    }

    2y

    22

    32

    76

    1 ]

    2

    N

    1,131929

    2,271703903

    1,616681433

    1,1763,5893,614

    153433244657

    156

    4332,1771,763

    1,663433

    496496496496496

    425286

    2,078749

    2,241227

    r

    .09-.03

    -.15.00.18

    -.07-.07

    -.06

    .16

    .11

    .10

    .37-.21-.09-.17

    .09

    -.21.06.23.13

    -.26

    -.13.13

    -.13.02

    -.13

    .42-.13-.02-.10

    .22

    .23

    rc

    .11-.03-.21

    .00

    .22-.09-.09-.07

    .20

    .14

    .13

    .49-.26

    -.12-.22

    .12

    -.26.07.30.24

    -.33

    .19

    .14-.17

    .02-.16

    .52-.16-.02-.11

    .29

    .28

    VAR

    .374

    .066

    .105

    .040

    .160

    .068

    .007

  • 130 LEE AND ASHFORTH

    affected by the stressors. For many service providers, thekey decision is the amount of one or more resources theyneed to invest to meet the demands and to protect them-selves from further resource depletion. Strain occurs whenthe workers feel they no longer have sufficient emotional

    resources to handle the interpersonal stressors (cf. Hob-foil, 1989; Maslach, 1982). Instead, many adopt the de-

    fensive strategy of withdrawal (rather than engagement)

    through depersonalization (Lee & Ashforth, 1993a).

    Thus, it is not surprising to find that depersonalization wasalso strongly associated with role stress (rc = .54) and

    stressful events (rc = .50).A surprising finding was the weak associations (i.e., rcs

    < .30) between personal accomplishment and most ofthe resource correlates. The two exceptions were work

    friends (rc = .49) and participation (rc = .30). To theextent that work acquaintances provide individuals witha sense of competence through support, self-efficacy and

    self-appraisal may be heightened (Jayaratne & Chess,

    1986). Similarly, to the extent that participation leads to

    greater felt mastery and control (i.e., empowerment) over

    the work environment, self-efficacy and self-appraisal

    may also be enhanced.Although gains in resources are viewed by most work-

    ers as less salient than the prevention of loss, gains are not

    inconsequential (Hobfoll & Freedy, 1993). Resourcesmay directly compensate for certain losses, but they mayalso indirectly help workers cope with work demands. Forexample, with weak social support, interpersonal stres-

    sors will likely lead to emotional strain, but with strong

    support, their relationship will likely be attenuated

    (Jayaratne & Chess, 1986; Russell, Altmaier, & Van Vel-zen, 1987). This proposition is consistent with interac-

    tionist accounts of organizational behavior (see Mitchell& James, 1989).

    Consistent with Leiter's (1993) model, our findings re-

    veal that the three dimensions were differentially associ-

    ated with several of the behavioral and atliludinal corre-lates. Emotional exhaustion and depersonalization were

    strongly associated with turnover intentions and organi-zational commitment but were weakly associated with

    control coping. As a caveat, it is unclear whether the weak

    associations reflect the underuse of such a coping behav-ior or its lack of effectiveness and subsequent abandon-

    ment. In any case, the lack of an effective coping response

    might have served to reinforce subsequent feelings of

    helplessness and futility (Lee & Ashforth, 1993a). In

    contrast, personal accomplishment was strongly related

    to control coping, suggesting that a problem-focused re-sponse and a positive self-appraisal may be mutually re-

    inforcing (Lazarus &Folkman, 1984;cf. Leiter, 1991b).

    Our findings suggest that outcomes that stem fromemotional exhaustion and reflect the desire to withdraw

    may be offset by outcomes that stem from personal ac-complishment and reflect the desire to seek control.Whichever response (either withdrawing or seeking

    control) predominates may thus depend on the relativestrengths of emotional exhaustion and personal accom-

    plishment as each develops independently over time (Lee

    & Ashforth, 1993a; Leiter, 1993).

    Study Limitations

    One limitation is that the meta-correlations show only

    the strength of the relations among the burnout dimen-

    sions and by no means deal with the controversy regard-ing their sequential ordering (Golembiewski, 1989;

    Leiter, 1989). The arguments over the correct sequence

    are based on circumstantial evidence of how stronglyeach dimension is related to certain antecedent and out-

    come variables (Cordes & Dougherty, 1993; e.g., see Lee

    & Ashforth, 1993b).A second limitation is that the strong association found

    between emotional exhaustion and five of the demand

    stressors may be partly attributed to a measurement arti-

    fact. Three of Maslach and Jackson's (1981, 1986) emo-tional exhaustion scale items (i.e., "I feel emotionally

    drained from my work," "Working with people directly

    puts too much stress on me," and "Working with peopleall day is really a strain for me") are direct measures of

    individual stress reactions. These items correspondclosely to items that tap perceived stress found in fre-

    quently used scales of role overload, role conflict, role

    ambiguity, stressful events, and work pressure.

    A third limitation is that only certain demands and re-sources were examined here. The fact that emotional ex-

    haustion was strongly associated with several of the cor-

    relates does not necessarily imply the absence of othercorrelates that may be more strongly associated with the

    other two dimensions.

    Two sets of correlates not examined here are demo-graphic (e.g., sex, age, and education) and dispositional

    attributes (e.g., Type A personality, psychological hardi-ness, and locus of control). Although these attributes

    have been linked to burnout (Cordes & Dougherty, 1993;

    Mclntyre, 1984;Nowack, 1986), their potential as mod-erators requires further clarification and investigation.

    Implications for Future Research

    Although the demand and resource correlates of jobburnout have been the focus of much attention, their

    links to stress-coping and adjustment require further

    study. According to the primacy of loss hypothesis, indi-viduals are more sensitive to demands and thus overcom-pensate in the amount of resources expended to preventfurther loss (Hobfoll & Freedy, 1993). To the extent that

  • META-ANALYS1S OF JOB BURNOUT 131

    burnout develops because of this overcompensation, twoquestions are worth investigating: (a) To what extent can

    individuals deal with or even overcome burnout by reap-

    praising their demands as potential gains (challenges)

    rather than certain losses (obstacles; Lazarus & Folkman,

    1984; Tversky & Kahneman, 1981) and (b) if demandscan indeed be reframed as potential gains, how can re-

    sources be used to change the way stressors are appraised?For example, with the right kind of supervisory support,

    workers may come to perceive ambiguous role expecta-

    tions as opportunities to carry out their own initiatives

    (potential gains) rather than as restrictions on their ac-tions (certain losses).

    A broader question that needs to be addressed is the

    extent to which the burnout phenomenon is generaliz-able to occupations other than human services (Shirom,

    1989). Perhaps the most distinguishing aspect of burn-

    out is not so much its psychological and behavioral symp-

    toms but rather the demands and the resources perceivedto be the most salient in service settings (cf. Cordes &

    Dougherty, 1993). Service providers may work in situa-

    tions with demands and resources that are different from

    those experienced by, say, production workers in manu-

    facturing settings. If true, this suggests that stress re-

    searchers should identify and include demands and re-sources perceived by their study participants to be themost relevant for their work context (Maslach, 1982).

    Moreover, a comparison of the perceived importance of acommon core of demands and resources between service

    and nonservice workers may reveal how and why stressful

    situations are dealt with differently between occupations.

    Last, longitudinal designs may show that how workershandle burnout is guided by the type and the frequency

    of feedback on the usefulness of their initial coping be-

    haviors (Leiter, 1991 b). This longitudinal approach sug-

    gests several forms of reciprocal relationships involvingburnout and adjustment responses (cf. Cummings &

    Cooper, 1979). Multiple data points over time may thushelp determine the kinds of feedback required to amelio-rate or prevent burnout. Such longitudinal data collec-

    tion should lead to a better understanding of how burnout

    develops and contribute to the broader research on

    stress-coping.

    References

    References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in

    the meta-analysis.

    *Anderson, M. B. G., & Iwanicki, E. F. (1984). Teacher moti-

    vation and its relationship to burnout. Educational Admin-

    istration Quarterly, 20, 109-132.

    *Barrick, R. K. (1989). Burnout and job satisfaction of voca-

    tional supervisors. Journal of Agricultural Education, 30, 35-

    41.

    Bloom, K. D., Buhrke, R. A., & Scott, T. B. (1988). Burnout

    and job expectations of state agency rehabilitation counsel-

    lors in North Dakota. Journal of Applied Rehabilitation

    Counseling, 19, 32-36.

    *Brookings, J. B., Bolton, B., Brown, C. E., & McEvoy, A.

    (1985). Self-reported job burnout among female human ser-

    vice professionals. Journal of Occupational Behaviour, 6,

    143-150.

    Burke, R. J., & Richardscn, A. M. (1993). Psychological burn-

    out in organizations. Unpublished manuscript, York Univer-

    sity, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

    Clanton, L. D., Rude, S. S., & Taylor, C. (1992). Learned re-

    sourcefulness as a moderator of burnout in a sample of reha-

    bilitation providers. Rehabilitation Psychology, 37, 131 -140.

    *Constable, J. F., & Russell, D. W. (1986). The effect of social

    support and the work environment upon burnout among

    nurses. Journal of Human Stress, 11, 20-26.

    Cordes, C. L., & Dougherty, T. W. (1993). A review and inte-

    gration of research on job burnout. Academy of-Management

    Review, 18, 621-656.

    *Corrigan, P. W., Holmes. E. P., Luchins, D., Buican, B., Basil,

    A., & Parks, J. J. (1994). Staff burnout in a psychiatric hos-

    pital: A cross-lagged panel design. Journal of Organizational

    Behaviour, 15, 65-74.

    Cummings, T. G., & Cooper, C. L. (1979). A cybernetic frame-

    work for studying occupational stress. Human Relations, 32,

    395-418.

    *Davis-Sacks, M. L., Jayaratne, S., & Chess, W. A. (1985). A

    comparison of the effects of social support on the incidence

    of burnout. Social Work, 30, 40-44.

    *Dignam, J. T, Barrera, M., Jr., & West, S. G. (1986). Occupa-

    tional stress, social support, and burnout among correctional

    officers. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14,

    177-193.

    *Dolan, S. L., & Renaud, S. (1992). Individual, organizational

    and social determinants of managerial burnout: A multivari-

    ate approach. Journal oj'Social Behaviour and Personality. 7,

    95-110.

    *Eisenstat, R. A., & Felner, R. D. (1984). Toward a differenti-

    ated view of burnout: Personal satisfaction and organizational

    mediators of job satisfaction and stress. American Journal of

    Community Psychology, 12. 411-430.

    *Erera, I. (1992). Supervisors can burn-out too. Clinical Su-

    pervisor, 9, 131-148.

    *Fong, C. M. (1993). A longitudinal study of the relationships

    between overload, social support, and burnout among nursing

    educators. Journal of Nursing Education, 32, 24-29.

    *Friesen, D., & Sarros, J. C. (1989). Sources of burnout among

    educators. Journal of Organizational Behaviour, 10, 179-

    188.

    *Gaines, J., & Jermier, J. M. (1983). Emotional exhaustion in

    a high stress organization. Academy oj Management Journal,

    26, 567-586.

    Golembiewski, R. T. (1989). A note on Leiter's study: High-

    lighting two models of burnout. Group and Organization

    Studies, 14, 5-13.

    Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new at-

  • 132 LEE AND ASHFORTH

    tempt at conceptualizing stress. American Psychologist, 44,513-524.

    Hobfoll, S. E., & Freedy, J. (1993). Conservation of resources:A general stress theory applied to burnout. In W. B. Schaufeli,C. Maslach, & T. Marek (Eds.), Professional burnout: Recentdevelopments in theory and research (pp. 115-129). Wash-ington, DC: Taylor & Francis.

    *Huebner, E. S. (1992). Burnout among school psychologists:An exploratory investigation into its nature, extent, and cor-relates. School Psychology Quarterly. 7, 129-136.

    Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods ofmeta-analy-sis: Correcting error and bias in research findings. NewburyPark, CA: Sage.

    "Jackson, S. E., Schwab, R. L., & Schuler, R. S. (1986). Towardan understanding of the burnout phenomenon. Journal oj Ap-plied Psychology, 71, 630-640.

    "Jackson, S. E., Turner, J. A., & Brief, A. P. (1987). Correlatesof burnout among public service lawyers. Journal of Occupa-tional Behaviour, 8, 339-349.

    "Jayaratne, S., & Chess, W. A. (1984). The effects of emotionalsupport on perceived job stress and strain. Journal of AppliedBehavioral Science, 20, 141-153.

    "Jayaratne, S., & Chess, W. A. (1986). Job stress, job deficit,emotional support, and competence: Their relationship toburnout. Journal of Applied Social Sciences, 10, 135-151.

    Jorgenson, D. M. (1986). Burnout in nurse faculty: Relation-ships with management style, collegia! support, and work loadin collegiate programs. Journal of Professional Nursing, 2,252-260.

    "Jorgenson, D. M. (1992). Burnout in doctorally preparednurse family. Journal of Nursing Education, 31, 341-346.

    Kahili, S. (1988). Symptoms of professional burnout: A reviewof the empirical evidence. Canadian Psychology, 29, 284-297.

    *Koeske, G. F., & Koeske, R. D. (1989). Construct validity ofthe Maslach Burnout Inventory: A critical review and recon-ceptualization. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 25,131-132.

    "Kruger. L. J., Botman, H. I., & Goodenow, C. (1991) . An in-vestigation of social support and burnout among residentialcounsellors. Child and Youth Care Forum, 20, 335-352.

    Lazarus, R. L., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, andcoping. New \brk: Springer.

    *Lee, R. T, & Ashforth, B. E. (1990). On the meaning of Mas-lach's three dimensions of burnout. Journal of Applied Psy-chology, 75, 743-747.

    *Lee, R. T, & Ashforth, B. E. (1992). Work-unit structure andprocesses and job-related stressors as predictors of manage-rial burnout. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 1831-1847.

    "Lee, R. T, & Ashforth, B. E. (1993a). A further examinationof managerial burnout: Toward an integrated model. Journalof Organizational Behavior, 14, 3-20.

    "Lee, R. T., & Ashforth, B. E. (1993b). A longitudinal studyof burnout among supervisors and managers: Comparisonsbetween the Leiter and Maslach (1988) and Golembiewskiet al. (1986) models. Organizational Behaviour and HumanDecision Processes. 54, 369-398.

    "Leiter, M. P. (1988a). Burnout as a function of communica-tion patterns. Group and Organization Studies, 13, 111 -128.

    "Leiter, M. P. {1988b). Commitment as a function of stress re-actions among nurses: A model of psychological evaluationsof work settings. Canadian Journal of Community MenialHealth, 7, 115-132.

    Leiter, M. P. (1989). Conceptual implications of two models ofburnout: A response to Golembiewski. Group and Organiza-tion Studies, 14, 15-22.

    "Leiter, M. P. (1990). The impact of family resources, controlcoping, and skill utilization on the development of burnout:A longitudinal study. Human Relations, 11, 1067-1083.

    "Leiter, M. P. (1991a). Burnout as a crisis in professional rolestructures: Measurement and conceptual issues. Anxiety,Stress, and Coping, 5, 79-93.

    "Leiter, M. P. (199 Ib) . Coping patterns as predictors of burn-out: The function of control and escapist coping patterns.Journal of Organizational Behaviour, 12, 123-144.

    Leiter, M. P. (1993). Burnout as a developmental process: Con-sideration of models. In W. B. Schaufeli, C. Maslach, & T.Marek (Eds.), Professional burnout: Recent developments intheory and research (pp. 237-250). Washington, DC: Taylor

    & Francis."Leiter, M. P., & Maslach, C. (1988). The impact of interper-

    sonal environment on burnout and organizational commit-ment. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 9, 297-308.

    "Lewis, S. L., Becktell, P. J., Bonner, P. N., Campbell, M. A.,Cooper, C. L., & Hunt, W. C. (1992). Work stress, burnout,and sense of coherence among dialysis nurses. American Ne-

    phrology Nurse's Association Journal, 19, 545-553.Maslach, C. (1982). Burnout: The cost of caring. Englewood

    Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall."Maslach, C., & Florian, V. (1988). Burnout, job setting, and

    self-evaluation among rehabilitation counsellors. Rehabilita-tion Psychology, 33, 85-93.

    Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1981). The measurement of ex-perienced burnout. Journal of Occupational Behavior, 2, 99-113.

    Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1986). Maslach Burnout Inven-tory manual (2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psycholo-gists Press.

    Mclntyre, T. C. (1984). The relationship between locus of con-trol and teacher burnout. British Journal of Educational Psy-chology, 54, 235-238.

    "McMullen, M. B., & Krantz, M. (1988). Burnout in day careworkers: The effects of learned helplessness and self-esteem.Child & Youth Care Quarterly, 17, 275-280.

    "Miller, K. I., Ellis, B. H., Zook, E. G., & Lyles, J. S. (1990).An integrated model of communication, stress, and burnoutin the workplace. Communication Research, 17, 300-326.

    "Miller, K. L, Stiff, J. B., & Ellis, B. H. (1988). Communicationand empathy as precursors to burnout among human serviceworkers. Communication Monographs, 55, 250-265.

    Mitchell, T. R., & James, L. R. (1989). Theory developmentforum: Situational versus dispositional factors: Competingexplanations of behavior; Conclusions and future directions.Academy of Management Review, 14, 331-332,401-407.

    "Nagy, S. (1985). Burnout and selected variables as compo-

  • META-ANALYSIS OF JOB BURNOUT 133

    nents of occupational stress. Psychological Reports, 56, 195-

    200.

    Nowack, K.. M. (1986). Type A, hardiness, and psychological

    distress. Journal of Behavior Medicine, 9, 537-547.

    *Nowack, K. M., & Hanson, A. L. (1983). The relationship

    between stress, job performance, and burnout in college stu-

    dent resident assistants. Journal of College Student Personnel,23, 545-550.

    *Oehler, J. M., & Davidson, M. G. (1992). Job stress and burn-

    out in acute and nonacute pediatric nurses. American Journal

    of Critical Care, 7,81-90.

    *Oehler, J. M., Davidson, M. G., Starr, L. E., & Lee, D. A.

    (1991). Burnout, job stress, anxiety, and perceived social sup-

    port in neonatal nurses. Heart and Lung, 20, 500-505.

    *Oktay, J. S. (1992). Burnout in hospital social workers who

    work with AIDS patients. Social Work, 37, 432-438.

    Penn, M., Romano, J. L., & Foat. D. (1988). The relationship

    between job satisfaction and burnout: A study of human ser-

    vice professionals. Administration in Mental Health, 15, 157-165.

    *Randall, M., & Scott, W. A. (1988). Burnout, job satisfaction,

    and job performance. Australian Psychologist, 23, 335-347.

    *Richardsen, A. M., Burke, R. J., & Leiter, M. P. (1992). Oc-

    cupational demands, psychological burnout and anxiety

    among hospital personnel in Norway. Anxiety, Stress, and

    Coping, 5, 55-68.

    *Rimmerman, A. (1989). Burnout among beginning rehabili-

    tation workers in Israel and its relationship to social support,

    supervision, and job satisfaction. Rehabilitation Counseling

    Bulletin, 32, 243-247.

    *Ross, R. R., Altmaier, E., & Russell, D. W. (1989). Job stress,

    social support, and burnout among counseling center staff.

    Journal a/Counseling Psychology, 36, 464-470.

    *Russell, D. W., Altmaier, E., & Van Vclzen, D. (1987). Job-

    related stress, social support, and burnout among classroom

    teachers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 269-274.

    *Sarros, J. C, & Friesen, D. (1987). The etiology of admin-

    istrator burnout. Alberta Journal of Educational Research,

    33, 163-179.

    *Savicki, V., & Cooley, E. (1987). The relationship of work en-

    vironment and client contact to burnout in mental health

    professionals. Journal of Counseling and Development, 65,

    249-252.

    *Saxton, M. J., Phillips, J. S., & Blakeney, R. N. (1991). Ante-

    cedents and consequences of emotional exhaustion in the air-

    line reservations service sector. Human Relations, 44, 583-

    595.Schaufeli, W. B., Enzmann, D., &Girault, N. (1993). Measure-

    ment of burnout: A review. In W. B. Schaufeli, C. Maslach, &

    T. Marek (Eds.), Professional burnout: Recent developments

    in theory and research (pp. 199-215). Washington, DC: Tay-

    lor & Francis.

    *Schwab, R. L., Jackson, S. E., & Schuler, R. S. (1986). Educa-

    tor burnout: Sources and consequences. Educational Re-

    search Quarterly, 10, 14-29.

    Shirom, A. (1989). Burnout in work organizations. In C. L.

    Cooper & I. Robertson (Eds.), International review of indus-

    trial and organizational psychology (pp. 25-48). New \brk:

    Wiley.

    Starnaman, S. M., & Miller, K. I. (1992). A test of a causal

    model of communications and burnout in the teaching pro-

    fession. Communication Education, 41, 40-53.

    *Stevens, G. B., & O'Neill, P. (1983). Expectation and burnout

    in the developmental disabilities field. American Journal of

    Community Psychology, 11, 615-627.

    Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981, January 30). The framing

    of decision and the psychology of choice. Science, 24, 453-

    458.

    Vealey. R. S., Udry, E. M., Zimmerman, V., & Soliday, J.

    (1992). Intrapersonal and situational predictors of coaching

    burnout. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 14, 40-

    58.

    *Whitehead, J. T. (1987). Probation officer job burnout: A test

    of two theories. Journal of Criminal Justice, 15, 1-16.

    Whitener, E. M. (1990). Confusion of confidence intervals and

    credibility intervals in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psy-

    chology, 73,315-321.

    *Wilcoxon, S. A. (1989). Leadership behaviour and therapist

    burnout: A study of rural agency settings. Journal of Rural

    Community Psychology, 10, 3-14.

    Williams, C. A. (1989). Empathy and burnout in male and

    female helping professionals. Research in Nursing and

    Health, 12, 169-178.

    *Wolpin, J., Burke, R. J., & Greenglass, E. R. (1991). Is job

    satisfaction an antecedent or a consequence of psychological

    burnout? Human Relations, 44, 193-209.

    Received December 20, 1994

    Revision received August 3, 1995

    Accepted October 27, 1995