disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - brain

18
Brain (1996), 119, 1933-1950 Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients with cerebellar dysfunction Maurizio Versino, 2 Orest Hurko 1 and David S. Zee 1 1 Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Hospital, Baltimore, USA and the 2 Department of Neurology, University of Pavia, Italy Correspondence to: David S. Zee, MD, Department of Neurology, Pathology 2-210, Johns Hopkins Hospital, Baltimore, MD 21287-6921 USA Summary Recent research has implicated the cerebellum in conjugate ocular motor control, including steady gaze-holding and accuracy of pursuit and saccades. 'Whether the cerebellum also has a role in the control of the alignment of the eyes during fixation and of the yoking of the eyes during movement is less certain. We have studied binocular (disconjugate) ocular motor control in nine patients with cerebellar dysfunction and compared the results with those of normal subjects. Eye alignment during fixation and the yoking of the eyes during and immediately after saccades were quantified by recording the movements of both eyes using scleral search coils. Patients had disturbances of ocular alignment. All had an esophoria during monocular viewing and many an esotropia during binocular viewing, implying an increase in convergence tone. Most had a vertical misalignment that varied with horizontal eye position ('alternating skew deviation'). Patients showed conjugate dysmetria (saccade under- or overshoot and postsaccade drift) and disconjugate dysmetria (the eyes were poorly yoked during and immediately after saccades). Both the conjugate and disconjugate abnormalities were incomitant, i.e. they varied with orbital eye position. Correlations amongst the various abnormalities suggested that one part of the cerebellum, perhaps the dorsal vermis and the underlying posteriorfastigial nucleus, controls the conjugate size of saccades and that another part of the cerebellum, perhaps the flocculus/paraflocculus, controls the yoking of the eyes during saccades and both the dis- conjugate and conjugate components of postsaccade drift. Keywords: saccades; eye movements; cerebellum; disconjugate dysmetria; strabismus Abbreviations: LED = light-emitting diode; VOR = vestibulo-ocular reflex Introduction The past few decades of clinical and basic ocular motor research have produced a relatively simple scheme relating specific parts of the cerebellum to the control of eye move- ments in normal subjects (Keller, 1989; Lewis and Zee, 1993). The flocculus and paraflocculus help to stabilize images on the retina during tracking of moving targets (smooth pursuit and cancellation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) during combined eye-head tracking) and during attempted steady fixation. The nodulus modulates the low- frequency component of the VOR (as reflected in the gain and phase of the VOR at low frequencies of sinusoidal stimulation and in the time constant of the VOR in response to an impulse of acceleration). The dorsal vermis and the posterior fastigial nucleus help to control saccade amplitude and also pursuit tracking. This framework has led to hypo- theses about the pathophysiology and topical localization of many of the eye movement disorders shown by patients with © Oxford University Press 1996 cerebellar lesions (Zee et al., 1976; Waespe, 1992; Lewis and Zee, 1993; Gaymard et al., 1994; Kanayama et al., 1994; Moschner et al., 1994; Straube and Buttner, 1994; BUttner and Straube, 1995; Vahedi et al., 1995; Waespe and Muller- Meisser, 1996). Dysmetria of saccades is a particularly prominent cerebellar eye sign. To interpret the various patterns of saccade dysmetria it has been helpful to use the simplifying concept that saccades are generated by a pulse-step neuronal signal (Leigh and Zee, 1991). The pulse, a velocity command, provides the high-frequency phasic discharge that moves the eyes rapidly from one position to another. The step, a position command, provides the steady tonic discharge after the saccade that holds the eye still in its new position. The step signal is created from the pulse signal by the ocular motor gaze-holding network, or neural integrator. For saccades to be accurate, the pulse must be of the correct size. For the Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Upload: others

Post on 12-Feb-2022

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

Brain (1996), 119, 1933-1950

Disorders of binocular control of eye movementsin patients with cerebellar dysfunctionMaurizio Versino,2 Orest Hurko1 and David S. Zee1

1 Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins Hospital,Baltimore, USA and the 2Department of Neurology,University of Pavia, Italy

Correspondence to: David S. Zee, MD, Department ofNeurology, Pathology 2-210, Johns Hopkins Hospital,Baltimore, MD 21287-6921 USA

SummaryRecent research has implicated the cerebellum in conjugateocular motor control, including steady gaze-holding andaccuracy of pursuit and saccades. 'Whether the cerebellumalso has a role in the control of the alignment of the eyesduring fixation and of the yoking of the eyes during movementis less certain. We have studied binocular (disconjugate)ocular motor control in nine patients with cerebellardysfunction and compared the results with those of normalsubjects. Eye alignment during fixation and the yoking of theeyes during and immediately after saccades were quantifiedby recording the movements of both eyes using scleral searchcoils. Patients had disturbances of ocular alignment. Allhad an esophoria during monocular viewing and many anesotropia during binocular viewing, implying an increase in

convergence tone. Most had a vertical misalignment thatvaried with horizontal eye position ('alternating skewdeviation'). Patients showed conjugate dysmetria (saccadeunder- or overshoot and postsaccade drift) and disconjugatedysmetria (the eyes were poorly yoked during and immediatelyafter saccades). Both the conjugate and disconjugateabnormalities were incomitant, i.e. they varied with orbitaleye position. Correlations amongst the various abnormalitiessuggested that one part of the cerebellum, perhaps the dorsalvermis and the underlying posteriorfastigial nucleus, controlsthe conjugate size of saccades and that another part ofthe cerebellum, perhaps the flocculus/paraflocculus, controlsthe yoking of the eyes during saccades and both the dis-conjugate and conjugate components of postsaccade drift.

Keywords: saccades; eye movements; cerebellum; disconjugate dysmetria; strabismus

Abbreviations: LED = light-emitting diode; VOR = vestibulo-ocular reflex

IntroductionThe past few decades of clinical and basic ocular motorresearch have produced a relatively simple scheme relatingspecific parts of the cerebellum to the control of eye move-ments in normal subjects (Keller, 1989; Lewis and Zee,1993). The flocculus and paraflocculus help to stabilizeimages on the retina during tracking of moving targets(smooth pursuit and cancellation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex(VOR) during combined eye-head tracking) and duringattempted steady fixation. The nodulus modulates the low-frequency component of the VOR (as reflected in the gainand phase of the VOR at low frequencies of sinusoidalstimulation and in the time constant of the VOR in responseto an impulse of acceleration). The dorsal vermis and theposterior fastigial nucleus help to control saccade amplitudeand also pursuit tracking. This framework has led to hypo-theses about the pathophysiology and topical localization ofmany of the eye movement disorders shown by patients with

© Oxford University Press 1996

cerebellar lesions (Zee et al., 1976; Waespe, 1992; Lewisand Zee, 1993; Gaymard et al., 1994; Kanayama et al., 1994;Moschner et al., 1994; Straube and Buttner, 1994; BUttnerand Straube, 1995; Vahedi et al., 1995; Waespe and Muller-Meisser, 1996).

Dysmetria of saccades is a particularly prominent cerebellareye sign. To interpret the various patterns of saccade dysmetriait has been helpful to use the simplifying concept thatsaccades are generated by a pulse-step neuronal signal (Leighand Zee, 1991). The pulse, a velocity command, providesthe high-frequency phasic discharge that moves the eyesrapidly from one position to another. The step, a positioncommand, provides the steady tonic discharge after thesaccade that holds the eye still in its new position. The stepsignal is created from the pulse signal by the ocular motorgaze-holding network, or neural integrator. For saccades tobe accurate, the pulse must be of the correct size. For the

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 2: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

1934 M. Versino et al.

eyes to be held still after the saccade, the step signal mustbe correctly matched to that of the pulse, and be sustainedfor the duration of the fixation.

Using this scheme, two components of saccade dysmetriahave been identified: pulse (amplitude) dysmetria (causingsaccade overshoot or undershoot) and pulse-step mismatchdysmetria (causing a brief period of postsaccade drift ofseveral hundred milliseconds duration). Pulse dysmetria hasbeen attributed to lesions of the dorsal cerebellar vermis/posterior fastigial nucleus (Optican and Robinson, 1980; Viliset al., 1983; BUttner and Straube, 1995; Takagi et al.,1996; Waespe and Muller-Meisser, 1996); pulse-step matchdysmetria has been attributed to flocculus/paraflocculuslesions (Zee et al., 1981; Optican et al., 1986). When thestep signal is not sustained, the eyes will drift back tothe primary position in the same way as the slow phase ofgaze evoked nystagmus.

Most of the findings relating the cerebellum to ocularmotor control have dealt with conjugate eye movements;little is known about the role of the cerebellum in disconjugateocular motor control, either of the static alignment of theeyes during fixation, or of the dynamic yoking of the eyesduring movement. There has been some evidence, however,implicating the cerebellum in several aspects of binocularmotor control, even as far back as the classical studies ofGordon Holmes who reported that patients with cerebellardamage may show a skew deviation, i.e. a vertical misalign-ment of the eyes that can not be attributed to a peripheraloculomotor or trochlear nerve palsy (Holmes, 1922). Analternating skew deviation (a vertical misalignment of theeyes that changes with horizontal eye position) has also beenreported in some patients with various lesions in the posteriorfossa (Goldstein and Cogan, 1961; Keane, 1985; Mosteret al., 1988; Hamed et al., 1993; Zee, 1996). Patients withcerebellar abnormalities have also been reported to have anesotropia (convergent squint) (Williams and Hoyt, 1989;Hoyt and Good, 1995) which is sometimes attributed to adivergence paralysis (Akman et al., 1995; Lewis et al., 1996)or to a divergence-beating nystagmus (convergent slow phaseswith divergent quick phases) (Yee et al., 1979). These typesof abnormalities hint at an excess of convergence tone withcerebellar lesions.

Experimentally, too, there is evidence that cerebellar lesionscan lead to abnormalities of disconjugate eye movementcontrol. Westheimer and Blair, in the pioneering work inwhich they described the ocular motor syndrome of thecerebellectomized monkey (1973), noted poor convergencein acutely lesioned animals. Burde et al. (1975) reportedalternating skew deviations in monkeys with cerebellarlesions. Vilis et al. (1983) found that cooling of the midlinedeep cerebellar nuclei in monkeys produced dysmetria ofsaccades that had both a disconjugate and a conjugatecomponent.

Because of the scant information about the role of thecerebellum in the binocular coordination of eye movements,we studied a group of patients with cerebellar lesions and,

by recording the movements of both eyes, systematicallyinvestigated both the static alignment of the eyes duringfixation and the yoking of the eyes during and immediatelyafter saccades. We show here that patients with cerebellarlesions have a considerable disturbance of binocular ocularmotor control, and suggest that the cerebellum plays animportant role in the binocular coordination of eyemovements.

MethodsTarget stimuliAn array of nine red light-emitting diodes (LEDs), each~1 mm in diameter, were positioned on a vertical flatsurface, 124 cm in front of the subject and arranged in a3X3 matrix. The middle row was at the eye level of thesubject, and the other two rows were 15° above and 15°below. In each row, the central LED was positioned alongthe midsagittal plane of the subject and the two lateralLEDs were left and right 15° with respect to the centre LED.

Eye movement recordingThe movements of both eyes were measured with scleralannuli using the magnetic field technique. The horizontaland vertical eye position signals were filtered (0-90 Hzbandpass), sampled at 500 Hz with 12 bit precision andthen stored to disk for later off-line analysis, by a digitalcomputer. System noise limited the angular resolution toabout 0.05°. The subject's head was stabilized with a chinrest. Calibration of each eye was performed duringmonocular viewing.

Experimental paradigmsIn the first paradigm, 15° vertical or horizontal saccadeswere elicited by illuminating one LED for 1 s and thenan adjacent LED for 1 s. Seven to ten trials of eachsaccade type were obtained. This paradigm was performedduring monocular and binocular viewing. Both staticalignment data and metrics of saccades were obtained fromthis paradigm. Independent calibrations were obtained forsaccades along each row and each column of three LEDsusing the monocular viewing data.

In the second paradigm, the centre, 0° LED wasilluminated for 1 s. Then, when it was turned off, either the15° left or the 15° right LED was flashed for 500 ms. After 2 sof darkness the eccentric LED was reilluminated for 500 msbefore the 0° LED was turned on again. This sequence wasrepeated 10 times for each direction. The subjects were askedto make a saccade to the flashed LED and then to keep thateye position even in the dark. Gaze-evoked nystagmus wasmeasured with this paradigm.

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 3: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

Binocular eye movements in cerebellar disease 1935

Table 1 Clinical characteristics of patients

Caseno.

123

4

56

7

8

9

Age(years)

453438

38

1755

68

50

47

Diagnosis

Amold-Chiari type 1Amold-Chiari type ISpinocerebellar

degenerationProgressive cerebellar

degenerationArnold-Chiari type IProgressive cerebellar

degeneration

Progressive cerebellardegeneration

Spinocerebellardegeneration

Spinocerebellardegeneration

Non-cerebellar signs

Brisk reflexes,downgoing toes

Brisk reflexes,upgoing toes

Brisk reflexes,upgoing toes

Non-ocular cerebellarmotor signs

Gait ataxiaStance and gait ataxiaGait ataxia and

appendicular dysmeytriaGait ataxia and

appendicular dysmeytriaNoneStance and gait ataxia,

appendicular dysmeytriaand dysarthric speech

Gait ataxia,appendicular dysmeytria,intentional tremor anddysarthric speech

Stance and gait ataxia,and appendicular ataxia

Gait ataxia

C7T or MRI scan

Tonsilar herniationCerebellar tonsils at C2Normal

Cerebellar atrophy,more marked in the vermis

Cerebellar tonsils at C2Cerebellar atrophy,

more marked in the vermis

Normal

Normal

Normal

Data analysisIn the first paradigm (saccades and fixation), each individualtrial was marked by using a computer algorithm to identifythe beginning of a saccade, 'I', when eye velocity reached257s, and the end of a saccade, 'P', when eye velocitydropped below 257s. Measures of static alignment werebased upon the position of the T mark, the position of theeyes just before the saccade to the next target location. Thedifference between T and 'P' determined the pulse portionof the saccade. The peak velocity of the saccade wasalso identified ('V'). Two other marks ('D' and 'S') werepositioned 80 and 160 ms, respectively, after the end of thepulse (at 'P'). The difference between the positions of 'D'and 'P' determined the immediate postsaccade drift. Theinvestigator verified the correct location of the marks and, ifnecessary, repositioned them. In addition, trials in which thesubject did not look steadily at the target or made a saccadein the wrong direction were eliminated. For the analysis ofalignment all trials were used including both monocular andbinocular viewing data. Saccade metrics were based uponthe binocular viewing data, using only those trials that beganor ended at the centre LED (0,0).

In the second paradigm (gaze-evoked nystagmus), allresponses were marked twice. The first set of marks werethe same as described as above and were used to computepostsaccade drift (comparing positions 'D' and 'P'). Withthe second marking, the investigator repositioned the 'D' andthe 'S', so that they were separated by 200 ms and placedlater, in the dark period when the eyes were drifting inthe slow phase of gaze-evoked nystagmus. The velocity ofthe slow-phase drift (which appeared roughly linear) wastaken as the average eye velocity in the 'D' to 'S' interval.A time constant was inferred from the ratio of the amplitudeof eccentric gaze and the velocity of the slow phases of gaze-evoked nystagmus.

Patients and normal subjectsTable 1 shows the main features of our patients. For the ninenormal subjects (six males and three females) the mean agewas 34.4 years (range 19-53 years). For the patients themean age was 43.5 years (range 17-68 years). No subjectswere taking medications known to influence ocular motility.Clinical diagnoses in the patients are presumptive except forthe patients with the Arnold-Chiari malformations and Patient6 who is a member of a family with known pure cerebellarcortical degeneration (Zee et al., 1976; Zee, 1982a). Patients8 and 9 are brother and sister. All recordings were madewithout subjects wearing any corrective lenses. Consent wasobtained from all subjects and patients according to theDeclaration of Helsinki.

ResultsDisorders of static ocular alignmentHorizontal misalignmentAll patients showed a disorder of either horizontal or verticalalignment or both. Figure 1 shows the phoria (misalignmentwith one eye viewing) and the tropia (misalignment withbinocular viewing) for horizontal fixation in the straightahead position of gaze. For the phoria all patients had anesodeviation and in five out of nine patients the value wasgreater than that of any of the normal subjects. For the tropia,six out of nine patients had a deviation that was greater thanthat of any of the normal subjects. In these six patients, fivehad an esotropia that ranged from 1.8 to 10.2°. Figure 1 alsoshows that in most patients the tropia was considerably lessthan the phoria, implying a relatively preserved ability to usedisparity cues to drive motor fusion.

Figure 2A shows the mean horizontal tropia values for thenine different positions in which alignment was measured.

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 4: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

1936 M. Versino et al.

Horizontal Deviation In Primary Position

Normals Patients

o

-2

-4

-6

-a

-10

-12

-14

-16

-18

i i riMean(Normals)

Q

a Tropia• Phoria

Eso

Fig. 1 Horizontal misalignment in straight-ahead gaze. Mean data (+SD) for normal subjects, andindividual data for nine normal subjects and nine patients are shown. Note that many patients had aconsiderable esophona and in many cases an esotropia, though the latter was usually smaller. Exo =exodeviation; Eso = esodeviation.

For normal subjects, as expected, the values of the tropiawere quite small (<S1.0°). For the patients the horizontaltropia values were larger, especially in the diagonal, mosteccentric positions of gaze (upper left, upper right, lowerleft, lower right). The horizontal misalignment tended to beincomitant; it varied with orbital position. Furthermore, thechange in ocular alignment relative to orbital position wasnonlinear, i.e. it did not change by a constant amount for agiven change in eye position. Using the largest differencebetween two individual horizontal tropia values from any ofthe nine positions of fixation, patients showed a significantdegree of incomitancy (nearly three times that of normalsubjects; median 2.9° in patients versus 1.1° in normalsubjects, P = 0.04, Mann-Whitney).

We also looked for changes in the degree of horizontalmisalignment with vertical eye position. This type ofincomitancy is often reflected in so-called 'A' patterns(greater esodeviation on up gaze or greater exodeviation ondown gaze) and ' V patterns (greater esodeviation on downgaze or greater exodeviation on up gaze). Comparing thevalues for the horizontal phoria (average of right eye andleft eye viewing data) in up and down gaze for targets onthe midline, none of the normal subjects and only three ofthe patients had a difference in horizontal phoria of > 1.1 °.Two patients had small 'A' patterns (Patient 2, 2.3° difference

and Patient 4, 4.4° difference) and one patient (no. 3) had asmall 'V pattern (2.8° difference).

Vertical misalignmentPatients also showed a vertical misalignment of the eyes. Aswas the case for the horizontal deviations, the degree ofvertical misalignment was smallest in the straight ahead andin the straight down positions of gaze (Fig. 2B). The verticalmisalignment tended to be incomitant, and the change inocular alignment relative to orbital position was nonlinear.Looking at the largest difference between two individualtropia values at any of the nine positions of fixation, themedian value was 2.6° in patients versus 0.5° in normalsubjects (P = 0.003, Mann-Whitney).

There was also a distinctive pattern of change of verticalalignment on lateral gaze (Fig. 3). Patients showed an'alternating skew' such that there was a considerabledifference in the vertical deviation depending upon right orleft gaze (median for patients, 1.22°; for normal subjects,0.11°). In six out of nine patients the right (abducting) eyewas higher on right gaze, and in six out of nine the left(abducting) eye was higher on left gaze. In 10 out of 12 ofthese measures the hyperdeviation increased in down gaze.In five out of nine patients, but in no normal subjects, the

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 5: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

Binocular eye movements in cerebellar disease 1937

Horizontal TropU

(A)

LC CC RC U CU RU LD CO

OrbtUI Position

VsttlcsJ Tropta(B)

1

LC CC RC LU CU RU LO CD RO

OrMtal Position

Fig. 2 Horizontal (A) and vertical (B) misalignment as a functionof the position of gaze. Mean values (+SD) for the absolutevalues of the tropia at each position are shown, both for thenormal subjects (light columns) and patients (dark columns).Position codes are given by the horizontal and vertical coordinates(using L = 15° left; R = 15° right; U = 15° up; D = 15° down;C = centre). Horizontal and vertical eccentricities are all 15°. Forexample, LC = 15° left and 0° vertical; CU = 0° horizontal and15° up, etc. Note that for the patients the tropias tended to begreater in the diagonal, most eccentric positions of gaze.

deviation actually changed direction with lateral gaze, i.e. aright hyperdeviation became a left hyperdeviation, or viceversa. In four of these five patients the abducting eye wasalways higher.

Disorders of dynamic ocular alignmentConjugate saccade dysmetria: abnormalities ofthe saccade pulseWe measured the conjugate component of the saccade pulse(average of right eye and left eye pulses), to look forconjugate pulse dysmetria. Typical raw records for saccadesmade by normal subjects and patients are shown in Figs 4

It

Fig. 3 Direction of vertical misalignment as a function ofhorizontal eye position ('alternating skew') in the nine individualpatients. Vertical tropias at 0° elevation and at left 15° and right15° eccentricity are shown. Note the considerable difference invertical tropia as a function of lateral gaze with the abducting eyeusually being higher.

(horizontal) and 5 (vertical). Data for individual subjects areshown in Figs 6 and 7. The group means and standarddeviations for the patients and the normal subjects aresummarized in Table 2. The following are the main findings.Most patients showed a conjugate pulse dysmetria that couldbe present for either horizontal or vertical saccades or forboth (Table 2, Dysmetria). Both hypo- and hypermetria wereobserved; hypermetria was typically greatest for downward,centripetal saccades. For both horizontal and verticalsaccades, six out of nine patients had dysmetria that was>2 SD from the mean value in the normal subjects; meanabsolute values of the dysmetria (either undershoot orovershoot) in degrees plus 2 SD were 2.3° horizontally and2.4° vertically. Using this criterion, only two patients (nos 2and 3) had no pulse dysmetria for either horizontal or verticalsaccades. The differences between the normal subjects andpatients were significant (P < 0.01 for horizontal and verticalsaccades, Mann-Whitney). Overall, in a given patient, therewas no consistent relationship between the degree ofhorizontal and vertical saccade dysmetria.

The pulse dysmetria was usually greater in one directionthan in the other. (Table 2, Directional asymmetry). Thepulse dysmetria also commonly varied with orbital position.Comparing patients and normal subjects, the mean of thedifferences between the amplitude of saccades for centri-petal and centrifugal saccades was statistically significantfor horizontal, but not for vertical saccades (Table 2,Incomitancy). The pattern of inconstancy for horizontalsaccades was variable; either centripetal or centrifugalsaccades could be larger. Although there was no statisticallysignificant difference in incomitancy for vertical saccadesbetween patients and normal subjects, when the differencewas large, it involved centripetal saccades being larger (e.g.downward saccades in Patient 8). This pattern was alsopresent, to a lesser extent, in normal subjects.

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 6: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

1938 M. Versino et al.

(A)

R

5deg

0.5 deg

— (C)

RE-LE '

5 deg

RE minus LE

RE-LE"

RE minus LE

200 ms

0.5 deg

200 ms

(B) (D)

5 deg RE-LE"

5 deg

0.5 deg

200 ms 200 ms

RE minus LE

Fig. 4 Horizontal saccades. Right and left eye (RE and LE) horizontal position traces from normalSubject 9 (A and B) and from Patient 3 (C and D). Rightward (A and C) and leftward (B and D)centrifugal saccades and the corresponding disconjugate component (RE minus LE) are shown. Verticaldotted line indicates the position of the end of the pulse portion of the saccade. In the normal subject(A and B) there is a transient divergence during the initial portion of the saccade followed byconvergence during the final portion of and immediately after the saccade. For the patient the pattern isquite different with a small initial divergence followed by a much larger convergence. For the patient(C and D) also note that following the leftward saccade (D) both eyes drift onward (conjugatepostsaccade drift component) but that there is also a disconjugate component as reflected in the REminus LE trace. For the rightward saccade (C) the disconjugate component is primarily convergent; forthe leftward saccade (D) the disconjugate component is primarily divergent. The patient had very littletropia in the primary position, but with eccentric gaze the ocular misalignment increased.

Conjugate saccade dysmetria: abnormalities ofthe saccade pulse-step match

In patients the conjugate component of postsaccade driftwas increased (average of right eye and left eye postsaccadedrift at 80 ms, illustrated in Figs 4 and 5, and quantified in

Fig. 7 and in Table 2). For horizontal saccades, six out ofnine patients and for vertical saccades, seven out of ninepatients had conjugate postsaccade drift that was >2 SDfrom the mean value in the normal subjects (i.e. >0.3°horizontally and >0.4° vertically). Using this criterion onlyone patient (no. 6) had no increase in conjugate postsaccade

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 7: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

Binocular eye movements in cerebellar disease 1939

RE-LE '

RE minus LE1 deg

200 ms

RE minus LE

200 ms

(D)

(B)

5 deg

1 deg

5 deg

D

RE-LE"

1 deg RE minus LE

RE minus LE

200 ms 200 ms

Fig. 5 Vertical saccades. Right and left eye (RE and LE) vertical position traces from normal Subject 9(A and B) and from Patient 3 (C and D). Upward (A and C) and downward (B and D) centrifugalsaccades and their corresponding disconjugate traces (RE minus LE) are shown. Vertical dotted lineindicates the position of the end of the pulse portion of the saccade. In the normal subject (A and B)there is little change in the vertical alignment of the eyes during, and immediately after, the saccade.For the patient there is a large conjugate component of postsaccade drift following the upward saccade(C). Note also the intrasaccade disconjugacy and the different patterns of postsaccade disconjugacy forup and down saccades.

drift for either horizontal or vertical saccades. The differencesbetween the normal subjects and the patients were significant(P < 0.05 for horizontal saccades and P < 0.01 for verticalsaccades, Mann-Whitney).

The data in Fig. 7 show that there was no consistentrelationship between the absolute degree of pulse dysmetriaand of postsaccade drift. The direction of any conjugatepostsaccade drift was usually the same (onward) as the pulseportion of the saccade in both patients and normal subjects,both for horizontal saccades (72% of cases for patients and95% for normal subjects) and for vertical saccades (78% ofcases for patients and 84% for normal subjects), regardlessof whether the pulse was too big (overshoot) or too small(undershoot). In other words, at least for the circumstances

in which the conjugate pulse was too big, the ensuingpostsaccade drift did not appear to be corrective for theconjugate pulse dysmetria.

Disconjugate saccade dysmetria: abnormalyoking of saccadesWe measured the disconjugate component of saccades next(difference between right eye and left eye pulses) using boththe right eye pulse to left eye pulse ratio and the left/rightpulse difference corrected for the size (conjugate pulse) ofthe saccade. Raw records from an individual patient areshown in Figs 4 and 5 and data for individual subjects and

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 8: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

1940 M. Versino et al.

Conjugate Horizontal Pulse

• L15-0,• 0-R15DR15-0;

D0-L16

Patients 1-9

Conjugate Vertical Pulse

(B)

15

• D15-0• 0-UP15DUP15-0D0-D15

Nts Patients 1-9

Fig. 6 Conjugate saccade pulse dysmetria (A, horizontal; B, vertical). Absolute values of the pulsecomponent in movements to and from the primary position are shown. For horizontal saccades (A)there is considerable variability in the influence of movement direction and orbital position on thepattern of dysmetria among patients. For vertical saccades (B) downward overshoot is a more consistentfinding. Nls = normal subjects (mean+SD).

individual trial types are depicted in Fig. 8 and quantitativelysummarized in Fig. 9. The group means and standarddeviations for the patients and for the normal subjects aresummarized in Table 3. All patients showed some degree ofdisconjugate pulse dysmetria that could be present for eitherhorizontal or vertical saccades or for both {see Table 3,Dysmetria). For horizontal and vertical saccades (two

different sets of) five out of nine patients and one normalsubject had disconjugate pulse dysmetria that was >2 SDfrom the mean value in the normal subjects. There tended tobe a relative divergence during horizontal saccades for bothnormal subjects (80% of cases) and patients (73% of cases).

We found no consistent relationship between the degreesof horizontal and vertical disconjugate saccade dysmetria.

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 9: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

Binocular eye movements in cerebellar disease 1941

Conjugate Dysmetria (Horizontal)0.16

0.14

Nls Patients 1 - 9

Conjugate Dysmetria (Vertical)0.16

0.14

Nls Patients 1 - 9

Fig. 7 Conjugate saccade pulse dysmetria and conjugate postsaccade drift dysmetria (A, horizontal; B,vertical). Absolute values of pooled data from the four saccade types, made to and from the primaryposition; for pulse dysmetria, values represent the average of absolute differences from 15° (size oftarget step) and for postsaccade drift dysmetria actual values are shown. Nls = normal subjects(mean+SD).

There were also no statistically significant differences in thepulse dysmetria as a function of saccade direction (right orleft, up or down; see Table 3, Directional asymmetry).Comparing patients with the normal subjects, the mean ofthe differences between the amplitude of the pulsedisconjugacy for centripetal and for centrifugal saccades wasstatistically significant for horizontal, but not for verticalsaccades (Table 3, Incomitancy). Even so, for verticalsaccades the mean value was considerably larger for thepatient group. Finally, there was a consistent relationship

between the degree of disconjugate pulse dysmetria and thedegree of conjugate postsaccade drift for horizontal (linearregression, r = 0.84) but not for vertical saccades.

Disconjugate saccade dysmetria: disconjugatepostsaccade driftThe disconjugate component of postsaccade drift was larger inpatients (difference between right eye and left eye postsaccade

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 10: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

1942 M. Versino et al.

Table 2 Pattern of conjugate saccade

Conjugate parameters (%, mean±SD)

dysmetria

Horizontal

Controls Patients

Vertical

Controls Patients

Pulse dysmetriaDirectional pulse asymmetryPulse incomitancyPostsaccade drift

7.6±3.84.3 + 3.04.9±3.01.8±0.8

18.8+7.3**15.6+17.719.7±9.8**3.9+2.2*

8.4±3.78.8±5.3

10.7+6.12.0±0.9

20.0±11.8**32.3±23.6**15.5±13.05.6±2.2**

All values are given as percentages (i.e. 100 X each of the following ratios) and represent group means±SD. Dysmetna = average ofabsolute values (15-pulse/15) of averages for each saccade type in each patient. Directional asymmetry (right/left or up/down) =absolute value of the difference between average absolute value of right (or up) saccades and average absolute value of left (or downsaccades)/average absolute value of averages for each saccade type in each patient. Incomitancy (orbital-position dependency) = absolutevalue of the larger difference between centripetal (cp) and centrifugal (cf) saccades in the same direction (e.g. right versus left, up versusdown)/average value of the cp and cf saccades. Postsaccade drift = absolute value of the conjugate component of saccade drift/amplitude of conjugate pulse. **P ^ 0.01 and *P < 0.05 (Mann-Whitney), significant difference between normal and patient groups.

Table 3 Pattern of disconjugate saccade

Disconjugate parameters (%; mean±SD)

dysmetria

Horizontal

Controls Patients

Vertical

Controls Patients

Pulse dysmetriaDirectional pulse asymmetryPulse incomitancyPostsaccade drift

3.4±2.23.1+4.24.0±6.01.8+0.7

6.0±2.8*3.7±3.07.3±3.9*3.7+1.9*

1.5±0.80.8+1.01.6±0.80.5+0.2

3.9±3.3*2.1 ±0.33.0±2.31.3±0.9

All values are given as percentages (i.e. 100 X each of the following ratios) and represent group means±SD. Dysmetria = absolutevalue of pulse-pulse difference/conjugate saccade size for horizontal (or vertical) saccades; Directional asymmetry (right/left or up/down) = absolute value of the difference between average absolute values of the pulse-pulse difference (corrected for conjugate pulsesize) for right (or up) saccades and for left (or down saccades). Incomitancy (orbital-position dependency) = absolute value of the largerdifference between the pulse-pulse differences (corrected for conjugate pulse size) for centripetal and centrifugal saccades for right (orup) versus left (or down) saccades. Postsaccade drift = absolute value of the disconjugate component of postsaccade drift/amplitude ofconjugate pulse. *P ^ 0.05 (Mann—Whitney), significant difference between normal and patient groups.

drift at 80 ms; Figs 4, 5 and 9; Table 3, Disconjugate drift).For horizontal saccades, six out of nine patients and forvertical saccades, five out of nine patients had disconjugatepostsaccade drift that was >2 SD from the mean value inthe normal subjects. The differences between the normalsubjects and patients were significant for horizontal saccades(P < 0.05, Mann-Whitney) but not for vertical saccades.

The data in Fig. 9 also show that in patients there wasa consistent relationship between the absolute degree ofdisconjugate pulse dysmetria and of disconjugate post-saccade drift (linear regression of mean values for eachpatient; r = 0.82 for horizontal saccades and r = 0.73 forvertical saccades). Looking at the relative directions of thedisconjugate component of the saccade pulse and of thepostsaccade drift, for normal subjects the direction of hori-zontal postsaccade disconjugate drift (convergent versusdivergent) was opposite to the intrasaccade drift in 84%of cases. For patients, the drift was opposite in 73% ofcases. For vertical saccades, neither in normal subjectsnor in patients was there a consistent relationship betweenthe directions of the intrasaccade pulse difference and thesubsequent postsaccade disconjugate drift (being opposite50% of the time in normal subjects and 57% of the time in

patients). In other words, the disconjugate postsaccade driftdid not appear to be corrective for the difference betweenthe saccade pulses of each eye.

Saccade velocitiesWe analysed saccade peak velocity data using horizontaland vertical centrifugal saccades starting from the primaryposition. For each subject and for each type of saccade weplotted the mean values of saccade amplitude and peakvelocity. In spite of some differences due to different valuesfor the mean amplitude of the saccade pulse in patients, thenormal subject and patient data did not cluster separately. Itwas always possible to compute a linear regression with noabsolute value residual greater than three standard deviationsfrom the regression. Moreover, the sign of the residuals wasnever in the direction that would indicate that the saccadesmade by the patients were slower. Finally, we never foundan outlier on the basis of Cook's coefficient (an index of theinfluence of an individual point on the regression statistic).Overall, there were no significant differences between normalsubjects and patients in peak saccade velocities.

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 11: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

Binocular eye movements in cerebellar disease 1943

Horizontal PP ratios

1.2

1.15

1.05

iDL15-0• 0-R15OR16-0D0-L16

0.85

0.85

1-9

Vertical PP ratios

Fig. 8 Disconjugate component of dysmetria for the saccade pulse (A, horizontal; B, vertical). Pulseratios (right eye pulse/left eye pulse) indicate the degree of disconjugacy during the saccade. Bothdirectional asymmetries and orbital position effects (incomitancy) were present to varying degrees indifferent patients. Nls = normal subjects (mean + SD).

Gaze-evoked nystagmusPatients 2, 4 and 6 had downbeat nystagmus at right andleft 15° eccentric fixation. Slow-phase velocities rangedbetween 2 and 137s. Most patients showed a horizontal gaze-evoked nystagmus. Six of nine patients had an inferred timeconstant of the horizontal neural integrator of <15 s (range1.1-14.2 s), while only one normal subject had a time

constant <15 s (7.4 s). The slow-phase drifts of gaze-evokednystagmus were disconjugate (Fig. 10). Figure 11 shows thedifference in slow-phase velocities between the two eyesfollowing saccades to right and left (15° eccentricity). Thedisconjugacy was also incomitant, with most subjects showinga considerable difference in the degree of disconjugacy whencomparing drift from right and from left gaze. Even when

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 12: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

1944 M. Versino et al.

Dlsconjugata dysnwtria (horizontal)(A)

177

lEpPdHfer• Dlsconjdrffl

H i *

(A)

R

5deg RE-LE~

RE minus LE

Patients 1 -9 200 ms

D<scon|ugata dy»m«trta (vartlcai)

(B)

0.1

0.M

I~L nd 1 .

• PPtBftV

|-|

1 1—L_

Nl* P«tl*nti 1 - •

Fig. 9 Comparison of the disconjugate components of the saccadepulse (PPdiffer) and postsaccade drift (Disconjdrift) (A,horizontal; B, vertical). Absolute values of pooled data from foursaccade types, made to and from the primary position. Values arenormalized to the size of the conjugate pulse. Nls = normalsubjects (mean + SD).

1 deg RE minus LE

corrected for the degree of conjugate drift (dividing dis-conjugate drift velocity by conjugate drift velocity), thedegree of disconjugacy of gaze evoked nystagmus wasincomitant. Finally, the degree of disconjugacy of the slowphases of gaze-evoked nystagmus and the degree of dis-conjugacy of the immediate horizontal postsaccade driftwere significantly correlated (linear regression, r = 0.57).

DiscussionOur patients showed oculomotor signs that are typical ofcerebellar disease including saccade under and overshoot,postsaccade drift and gaze-evoked and down-beat nystagmus.In spite of the heterogeneity of the underlying pathology inour patients none showed any ocular motor abnormalitiesthat could not be attributed to cerebellar dysfunction (Lewisand Zee, 1993; Moschner et al., 1994). Our patients showedno slowing of horizontal or vertical saccades; thus, involve-ment of the abducens or oculomotor nuclei or the portions ofthe paramedian pontine and mesencephalic reticular formationthat contain premotor saccade burst neurons can be excluded.

The main new finding of this study is that patients

200 ms

Fig. 10 Gaze-evoked nystagmus at 15° eccentricity. Right and lefteye (RE and LE) horizontal position traces and difference trace(RE minus LE) from normal Subject 7 (A) and Patient 2 (B).

with cerebellar disturbances show prominent disturbances ofbinocular eye movement control. The disorders of static eyealignment during fixation included horizontal esodeviations,suggesting a bias toward convergence, and vertical (skew)deviations, which changed with horizontal eye position suchthat the abducting eye was usually higher. Disorders ofdynamic alignment associated with saccades were alsoprominent, with both intrasaccade and postsaccade dis-conjugacy. Patients also showed disconjugacy of gaze-holdingso that on lateral gaze, the centripetal drift of gaze-evokednystagmus was different in the two eyes. We will discussand compare these abnormalities in turn, and then developideas about the role of the cerebellum in the control of eyemovements, and how cerebellar lesions might lead to dis-orders of the control of eye alignment and eye movementconjugacy.

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 13: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

Binocular eye movements in cerebellar disease 1945

Dtsconjugato Oaz*-ovok«d Nystagmus

IV L 1l l

PI

•i l l

1 1 1 4 • < 7 > •

PitknH

Fig. 11 Disconjugacy of the slow phases of gaze-evoked nystagmus in each of the nine patients.Sustained drift following saccades to 15° right (light columns) and left (dark columns) eccentricities isshown. The degree of disconjugacy was often different in the two directions.

Disturbances of saccade accuracy: conjugatecontrolConjugate saccade pulse dysmetria, either undershoot orovershoot, is a classical sign in patients with cerebellardisease (Goldstein and Cogan, 1961; Zee et al., 1976; Zee,19826; Botzel et al., 1993; BUttner and Straube, 1995; Takagiet al., 1996). Even though the brainstem is sometimesinvolved in cerebellar dysfunction in patients, autopsystudies have confirmed that abnormalities of eye movementsincluding dysmetria of saccades can be present when thecerebellum alone is affected (Zee, 1982a). The disturbancesof the conjugate control of saccades shown by our patientswere similar to those described previously in cerebellarpatients.

Evidence from experimental animals with lesions restrictedto the cerebellum also establishes an essential role for thecerebellum in controlling saccade amplitude (Ritchie, 1976;Optican and Robinson, 1980; Vilis and Hore, 1981; Sato andNoda, 1992; Goldberg et al., 1993; Robinson et al., 1993).Results of single unit recordings and electrical stimulationin monkeys (Ohtsuka and Noda, 1992; Fuchs et a/., 1993;Helmchen era/., 1994; Helmchen and BUttner, 1995; Ohtsukaand Noda, 1995) and of studies of eye movements (Vahediet al., 1995; Waespe and Miiller-Meisser, 1996) and trans-cranial magnetic stimulation in humans (Hashimoto andOhtsuka, 1995) all point to the dorsal cerebellar vermis(lobules VI and VII) and the posterior 'ocular motor' portionof the fastigial nucleus as the critical structures withinthe cerebellum that participate in the control of saccadeamplitude.

Our patients also showed brief drift of the eyes immediatelyfollowing saccades. This postsaccade drift is thought to reflecta mismatch between the neuronal pulse and step signals thatgenerate saccades. The cerebellar flocculus and paraflocculusare critical structures for the correct matching of the saccadepulse and step (Zee et al., 1981; Optican et al., 1986).

Postsaccade drift was usually onward independent of whetherthe pulse was too big or too small. The emergence of thisparticular pattern of pulse-step mismatch suggests that theinherent, 'default' pattern of saccade control is for the stepsignal to be larger than the pulse signal.

Disturbances of eye alignmentA prominent finding in our patients was static horizontalmisalignment. Patients frequently showed a bias towardsconvergence with esophorias being present in all patients,and esotropias in most. The absence of saccade slowingmakes it unlikely that the brainstem and peripheral nerve andmuscle are involved. These deviations were also incomitant,though the so-called 'A' and 'V patterns, in which horizontaldeviations vary in a characteristic way with vertical eyeposition, occurred in only a few patients and then were quitesmall. The degree of misalignment was greatest in diagonal,most eccentric positions of gaze, i.e. those least frequentlyheld during natural fixation. This last finding is compatiblewith the idea that a function of the cerebellum is to correctfor the nonlinear mechanical properties of orbital tissueswhich become more important when the eyes are moved tomore eccentric orbital positions.

Why cerebellar lesions should lead to a bias towardconvergence is not known. In rabbits the cerebellar flocculushas an inhibitory projection to the medial rectus but not tothe lateral rectus muscles (Ito et al., 1977) so that a lesionin the flocculus could lead to a convergence bias. Hence, ifthere is a similar inherent bias towards divergence in thenormal cerebellum of primates, cerebellar lesions could leadto excessive convergence. We did not measure the vergencecapabilities of our patients directly. But the finding that theirtropias were usually smaller than their phorias implies atleast a relatively preserved ability to use disparity cues todrive vergence and motor fusion.

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 14: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

1946 M. Versino et al.

Although little studied, there is some electrophysiologicalevidence that the cerebellum could participate in the controlof vergence and perhaps ocular alignment. The flocculuscontains neurons that discharge with the angle of vergence(Miles et al., 1980) and stimulation in the cerebellar flocculuscan lead to movements of one eye only (Balaban andWatanabe, 1984). The nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis,which projects to the cerebellum, and the posterior nucleusinterpositus (the globose and emboliform nuclei in humans)contain cells that discharge in relation to different aspects ofthe near response including vergence (Gamlin and Clarke,1995; Gamlin et al., 1996).

Vertical misalignment was also prominent in our patients.The deviations were incomitant and greatest in diagonal,most eccentric positions of gaze, as was the case for thehorizontal misalignment. Our patients often showed a charac-teristic pattern of alternating skew deviation on right and leftlateral gaz£ usually with the abducting eye higher. In mostcases the deviation on lateral gaze was greater on down gazegiving a pattern of 'inferior rectus skew' (Moster et al.,1988). This particular pattern of eye misalignment has beendescribed both in patients with brainstem disturbances andin patients with cerebellar disturbances (Keane, 1985; Zee,1996). It will be discussed further below.

In view of the abnormalities of eye alignment shown byour patients one might ask why diplopia is not reported morecommonly by patients with cerebellar disease. First, thetropias were less than the phorias so that in some cases thedeviations with binocular viewing were not large enough tocause diplopia. Furthermore, our measures of tropias weremade during fixation of a single small light emitting diodein an otherwise dark room. With a larger, more natural fieldof stimulation, disparity-driven vergence mechanisms wouldhave been more likely to overcome the phorias completely.In addition, the deviations were least in the most naturallyand frequently used positions of fixation (straight ahead andstraight down). Finally, oscillopsia from any accompanyingspontaneous nystagmus may have obscured the diplopia sincenystagmus also tends to be more prominent in eccentricpositions of gaze. Regardless of whether or not our patientsreported diplopia, they showed a consistent and prominentdisorder of ocular alignment which we attribute to theircerebellar disease.

Disturbances of saccade accuracy: disconjugatecontrolOur patients also showed a prominent abnormality of theyoking of the eyes during and immediately after saccades;the eyes did not travel together. All patients showed someabnormality of saccade pulse conjugacy, and just as forconjugate pulse dysmetria, the changes were often incomitant.Similarly, postsaccade drift was frequently disconjugate. Forindividual patients, the absolute degree of disconjugacyduring saccades correlated well with the absolute degree (but

not the direction) of disconjugate drift immediately after thesaccade and, at least for horizontal saccades, with the absolutedegree of conjugate postsaccade drift. Furthermore, the con-jugate component of saccade pulse dysmetria did not correlatewell with its disconjugate component, nor with the conjugateor disconjugate components of postsaccade drift.

A hypothesis which could explain these findings is thatone part of the cerebellum, perhaps the dorsal vermis,participates in the control of the conjugate size of saccades,and another part of the cerebellum, perhaps the flocculus/paraflocculus, participates in the control of the yoking ofthe eyes during saccades and in the control of both thedisconjugate and conjugate components of postsaccade drift.The finding that monkeys with dorsal vermal lesions showabnormalities in the control of conjugate pulse size (saccadesovershoot and undershoot) but no abnormal yoking of theeyes (Takagi et al., 1996) is compatible with this idea. It isnot clear where the deep cerebellar nuclei fit into this scheme,since large lesions in the deep cerebellar nuclei createdysmetria of saccades with both conjugate and disconjugatecomponents (Vilis et al., 1983). More lateral portions of thecerebellar hemispheres also contain neurons that dischargein relationship to saccades (Mano et al., 1991). Their role,if any, in the control of saccade properties is unknown.

Gaze-evoked nystagmusMost of our patients showed horizontal gaze-evokednystagmus, even at only 15° eccentricity. The drift of gaze-evoked nystagmus reflects the time constant of the neuralintegrator, the neural network that converts velocitycommands to position commands, to create the step signalfor steady gaze-holding. The rate of decay of the step, asreflected in its time constant, determines how well eccentricpositions of gaze are held. The lower the time constant,the 'leakier' the integrator, and the higher the velocity ofcentripetal drift.

The slow phases of gaze-evoked nystagmus weredisconjugate. This is not surprising since the horizontal ocularmisalignment in our patients was incomitant. Accordingly,when the eyes drift toward a new position in which eyealignment changes, the eyes drift by different amounts. Thedisconjugacy of the slow phases of gaze-evoked nystagmusitself was incomitant, in accord with the fact that static ocularmisalignment did not change linearly with orbital position.Finally, the degree of disconjugacy of the slow phases of gaze-evoked nystagmus correlated well with that of immediatepostsaccade drift. This finding suggests that the control ofthe conjugacy of the amplitude and of the decay of the stepoutput of the neural integrator have a common anatomicalsubstrate within the cerebellum. The cerebellar flocculus/paraflocculus plays an important role in regulating the ocularmotor integrator, and may also participate in itsdisconjugate control.

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 15: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

Binocular eye movements in cerebellar disease 1947

The cerebellum and ocular motor plasticitySince many eye movement abnormalities appear immedi-ately after a cerebellar insult [most convincingly shownexperimentally during reversible block with cooling or withchemicals (Vilis and Hore, 1981; Sato and Noda, 1992;Kurzan et al, 1993; Robinson et al., 1993)] there is aunequivocal role for the cerebellum in the immediate, on-line control of eye movements. But there also appears to bean essential role for the cerebellum in the long-term adaptivemechanisms that monitor motor performance and keep brain-stem circuits calibrated for optimal visuo-oculomotorbehavior (Lewis and Zee, 1993; du Lac et al., 1995).

In the absence of such a 'learning' mechanism, not onlywould the ability to recover from the acute effects of aneurological lesion be limited, but additional eye movementabnormalities might emerge slowly as various ocular motorreflexes subserved by pathways that need not course throughthe cerebellum directly, gradually become uncalibrated. Inother words, maintenance of ocular motor accuracy is adynamic process, relying on a robust learning mechanism tomonitor visuomotor performance continuously and to fine-tune ocular motor innervation in the face of the inevitabledemands for recalibration that are incurred during naturaldevelopment and ageing, and in response to disease andtrauma. The cerebellum may be essential to this process.

Conjugate dysmetria of saccades after cerebellarlesionsConsidering this learning hypothesis, dysmetria of saccadesmay reflect both the immediate effects of cerebellardysfunction and the loss of the normal learning mechanismsthat maintain ocular motor accuracy in the long-term. Forexample, the cerebellum might be needed for the adjustmentsin innervation that must compensate for the inherent orbitalmechanical factors that make muscle forces a nonlinearfunction of eye position. Centripetally directed saccades areoften larger than centrifugally directed saccades in normalsubjects and this incomitant pattern of dysmetria sometimesbecomes exaggerated in patients or experimental animalswith cerebellar lesions (e.g. Ritchie, 1976; Optican andRobinson, 1980; Vilis and Hore, 1981; Botzel et al, 1993).As another example, the orientations of the eye musclesalso depend upon the positions of the eyes in the orbit inthe orthogonal direction (Miller and Demer, 1992); e.g. theactions of the oblique muscles and vertical recti vary withhorizontal eye position. If not corrected for, this anisotropycould be another possible source of an incomitant dysmetria.

Adaptation of conjugate saccade properties has beentested both in patients and in monkeys with cerebellarlesions and found to be wanting. The cerebellar dorsal vermisand probably the underlying fastigial nuclei seem critical forcorrection of saccade amplitude dysmetria (Optican andRobinson, 1980; Waespe and Baumgartner, 1992; Goldberget al., 1993; Waespe, 1995; Takagi et al., 1996; Waespe and

MUller-Meisser, 1996) and the flocculus and paraflocculusfor the correction of postsaccade drift (Optican et al., 1986).

Disconjugate dysmetria of saccades emergingwith cerebellar lesionsSince each pair of yoke muscles consists of two anatomicallydifferent muscles (e.g. the lateral rectus of one eye and themedial rectus of the other) any corrections in orbital-positiondependent nonlinearities would have to be tailored differentlyfor each eye. Hence, in the absence of a mechanism tocorrect for potential sources of saccade dysmetria, bothconjugate and disconjugate patterns of dysmetria might beexpected to emerge during saccades. If the cerebellum makesboth immediate, 'on-line' adjustments in saccade control andalso participates in more long-term ocular motor learning, itfollows that some variability in the pattern of dysmetriawould be expected from patient to patient based on theindividual subject's inherent imperfections and prior 'ocularmotor history'. These factors may contribute to the variabilityin dysmetria of saccades amongst patients even from thesame family (Zee et al., 1976).

Disturbances of eye alignment after cerebellarlesionsThe hypothesis that the emergence of dysmetria aftercerebellar lesions is due, at least in part, to loss of an adaptivemechanism might also apply to the maintenance of static eyealignment. When a normal subject wears a patch over oneeye for a few days, to eliminate disparity cues, disorders ofvertical alignment appear, often with the pattern of analternating hyperdeviation (Liesch and Simonsz, 1993;Neikter, 1994). Vertical alignment has been shown to be underadaptive control with visual, 'disparity' inputs providing apotent error signal for change (Ygge and Zee, 1995;McCandless et al., 1996). Hence, if the cerebellum is involvedin the maintenance of static eye alignment, and uses disparitycues as the error signal, patching of one eye could lead tothe appearance of 'cerebellar'-type oculomotor signs byvirtue of depriving the cerebellum of the necessary errorsignal to monitor oculomotor performance in the same wayas would a direct lesion in the cerebellum.

More direct tests of a role for the cerebellum in adaptationof eye alignment have given conflicting results. In humanbeings with cerebellar dysfunction, Milder and Reinecke(1983), but not Hain and Luebke (1990), found impairedshort-term (hours) phoria adaptation (adaptation to a laterallydisplacing prism placed in front of one eye for minutes tohours). The differences in the results between these twostudies may have depended upon whether or not the deepcerebellar nuclei, or extracerebellar structures were involved,as was possibly the case in the Milder and Reinecke study.Judge found a normal range of phoria adaptation in monkeysthat had had their cerebellar flocculi and paraflocculi removed,

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 16: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

1948 M. Versino et al.

but no prelesion control data were available and only theability to adjust to a comitant deviation was measured(Judge, 1987).

Disturbances of binocular ocular motor controland the VORThe abnormalities of disconjugate ocular motor control thatappear following cerebellar lesions may also relate to theneeds of the vestibular system. With the eyes being separatedlaterally in their orbits, geometrical considerations dictatethat when viewing near objects while the head is moving,the eyes must move disconjugately or even disjunctively(such as when moving forward or backward) for the VORto ensure optimal stabilization of gaze (Viirre et al., 1986;Gresty et al., 1987; Snyder and King, 1992; Seidman et al.,1995). Hence a need for a disconjugate capability, andpresumably a mechanism to keep it properly calibrated,already exists in the phylogenetically oldest types of eyemovements, the slow and quick phases of vestibularnystagmus. It has been shown in the monkey that somecells in the flocculus discharge in relation to the angle ofvergence (Miles et al., 1980). Whether these cells have somerole in adjusting the VOR for viewing distance, or participatein some other aspect of disconjugate ocular motor control isnot known.

The VOR and skew deviationVestibular stimuli may also elicit changes in static eyealignment. Consider that a skew deviation is a physiologicallynormal response to lateral head tilt in lateral-eyed animals.It may be that in pathological circumstances in frontal-eyedanimals, such a phylogenetically old reflex pattern of skewdeviation emerges and indeed this has been suggested as themechanism for the frequent occurrence in human patientswith brainstem lesions of the ocular tilt reaction, with itsskew deviation, cyclorotation of the eyes and head tilt (Brandtand Dieterich, 1995). Analogously, an alternating skewdeviation, as was frequently seen in our patients, can beinterpreted as a vestibular abnormality deriving from thenormal response of a lateral-eyed animal to fore and aft pitchwith the eyes in different lateral positions in the orbit (Zee,1996). Thus, one can envisage that cerebellar lesions lead todisorders of eye alignment on the basis of interferencewith the normal mechanisms that maintain conjugacy ordisconjugacy of the vestibular response, as called for by theparticular pattern of vestibular stimulation.

ConclusionOur results implicate the cerebellum in a number of aspectsof binocular (disconjugate) control of eye movements,including the maintenance of correct eye alignment duringfixation and the correct yoking of the eyes during and

immediately after saccades. Some of these abnormalities maybe related to the hypothetical role for the cerebellum inocular motor learning. While the exact loci within thecerebellum that control the yoking of the eyes are notknown, it may be that the cerebellar flocculus/paraflocculusis important for disconjugate ocular motor control.

AcknowledgementsWe acknowledge the support of NIH grant EY0I849 and aResearch to Prevent Blindness Manpower Award to D.S.Z.and the Premio Giuseppe Carlo Riquier to M.V. Dr MichaelRosenberg referred one of the patients.

ReferencesAkman S, Dayanir V, Sanac AS, Kansu TS. Acquired esotropiaas presenting sign of cranio-cervical junction anomalies. Neuro-ophthalmology 1995; 15: 311-4.

Balaban CD, Watanabe E. Functional representation of eyemovements in the flocculus of monkeys (Macaca fuscata). NeurosciLett 1984; 49: 199-205.

Botzel K, Rottach K, BUttner U. Normal and pathological saccadicdysmetria. Brain 1993; 116: 337-53.

Brandt T, Dieterich M. Central vestibular syndromes in roll, pitch,and yaw planes. Neuro-ophthalmology 1995; 15: 291-303.

Burde RM, Stroud MH, Roper-Hall G, Wirth FR O'Leary JL. Ocularmotor dysfunction in total and hemicerebellectomized monkeys. BrJ Ophthalmol 1975; 59: 560-5.

BUttner U, Straube A. The effect of cerebellar midline lesions oneye movements. Neuro-ophthalmol 1995; 15: 75-82.

du Lac S, Raymond JL, Sejnowski TJ, Lisberger SG. Learning andmemory in the vestibulo-ocular reflex. [Review]. Annu Rev Neurosci1995; 18: 409-41.

Fuchs AF, Robinson FR, Straube A. Role of the caudal fastigialnucleus in saccade generation: I. Neuronal discharge pattern. JNeurophysiol 1993; 70: 1723-40.

Gamlin PD, Clarke RJ. Single-unit activity in the primate nucleusreticularis tegmenti pontis related to vergence and ocularaccommodation. J Neurophysiol 1995; 73: 2115-9.

Gamlin PD, Yoon K, Zhang H. The role of cerebro-ponto-cerebellarpathways in the control of vergence eye movements. Eye 1996.In press.

Gaymard B, Rivaud S, Amarenco P, Pierrot-Deseilligny C. Influenceof visual information on cerebellar saccadic dysmetria [seecomments]. Ann Neurol 1994; 35: 108-12. Comment in: AnnNeurol 1995; 37: 413-4.

Goldberg ME, Musil SY, Fitzgibbon EJ, Smith M, Olson CR. Therole of the cerebellum in the control of saccadic eye movements.In: Mano N, Hamada I, DeLong MR, editors. Role of the cerebellumand basal ganglia in voluntary movement. Amsterdam: ElsevierScience, 1993: 203-11.

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 17: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

Binocular eye movements in cerebellar disease 1949

Goldstein JE, Cogan DG. Lateralizing value of ocular motordysmetria and skew deviation. Arch Ophthalmol 1961; 66: 517-8.

Gresty MA, Bronstein AM, Barratt H. Eye movement responses tocombined linear and angular head movement. Exp Brain Res 1987;65: 377-84.

Hain TC, Luebke AE. Phoria adaptation in patients with cerebellardysfunction. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1990; 31: 1394-7.

Hamed LM, Maria BL, Quisling RG, Mickle JP. Alternating skewon lateral gaze: neuroanatomic pathway and relationship to superioroblique overaction. Ophthalmology 1993; 100: 281-6.

Hashimoto M, Ohtsuka K. Transcranial magnetic stimulation overthe posterior cerebellum during visually guided saccades in man.Brain 1995; 118: 1185-93.

Helmchen C, BUttner U. Saccade-related Purkinje cell activity inthe oculomotor vermis during spontaneous eye movements in lightand darkness. Exp Brain Res 1995; 103: 198-208.

Helmchen C, Straube A, BUttner U. Saccade-related activity inthe fastigial oculomotor region of the macaque monkey duringspontaneous eye movements in light and darkness. Exp Brain Res1994; 98: 474-82.

Holmes G. Clinical symptoms of cerebellar disease and theirinterpretation. Lancet 1922; 2: 59-65.

Hoyt CS, Good WV. Acute onset comcomitant esotropia: when isit a sign of serious neurological disease? [Review]. Br J Ophthalmol1995; 79: 498-501.

Ito M, Nisimaru N, Yamamoto M. Specific patterns of neuronalconnexions involved in the control of the rabbit's vestibulo-ocularreflexes by the cerebellar flocculus. J Physiol (Lond) 1977; 265:833-54.

Judge SJ. Optically-induced changes in tonic vergence and AC/Aratio in normal monkeys and monkeys with lesions of the flocculusand the ventral paraflocculus. Exp Brain Res 1987; 66: 1-9.

Kanayama R, Bronstein AM, Shallo-Hoffmann J, Rudge P, HusainM. Visually and memory guided saccades in a case of cerebellarsaccadic dysmetria. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994; 57:1081-4.

Keane JR. Alternating skew deviation: 47 patients. Neurology 1985;35: 725-8.

Keller EL. The cerebellum. In: Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME, editors.The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements. Amsterdam: Elsevier,1989: 391-411.

Kurzan R, Straube A, BUttner U. The effect of muscimol micro-injections into the fastigial nucleus on the optokinetic response andthe vestibulo-ocular reflex in the alert monkey. Exp Brain Res 1993;94: 252-60.

Leigh RJ, Zee DS. The neurology of eye movements. 2nd ed.Philadelphia: F.A. Davis, 1991.

Lewis RF, Zee DS. Ocular motor disorders associated with cerebellarlesions: pathophysiology and topical localization. Rev Neurol (Paris)1993; 149: 665-77.

Lewis AR, Kline LB, Sharpe JA. Acquired esotropia due to Arnold-Chiari I malformation. J Neuro-ophthalmol 1996; 16: 49-54.

Liesch A, Simonsz HJ. Up- and downshoot in adduction aftermonocular patching in normal volunteers. Strabismus 1993; 1:25-36.

Mano N, Ito Y, Shibutani H. Saccade-related Purkinje cells in thecerebellar hemispheres of the monkey. Exp Brain Res 1991; 84:465-70.

McCandless JW, Schor CM, Maxwell JS. A cross-coupling modelof vertical vergence adaptation. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 1996; 43:24-34.

Milder DG, Reinecke RD. Phoria adaptation to prisms. Arch Neurol1983; 40: 339-42.

Miles FA, Fuller JH, Braitman DJ, Dow BM. Long-term adaptivechanges in primate vestibuloocular reflex. III. Electrophysiologicalobservations in flocculus of normal monkeys. J Neurophysiol 1980;43: 1437-76.

Miller JM, Demer JL. BiomechanicaJ analysis of strabismus. BinoculVis Eye Muscle Surg Q 1992; 7: 233-48.

Moschner C, Perlman S, Baloh RW. Comparison of oculomotorfindings in the progressive ataxia syndromes. Brain 1994; 117:15-25.

Moster ML, Schatz NJ, Savino PJ, Benes S, Bosley TM, SergottRC. Alternating skew on lateral gaze (bilateral abductinghypertropia). Ann Neurol 1988; 23: 190-2.

Neikter B. Effects of diagnostic occlusion on ocular alignment innormal subjects. Strabismus 1994; 2: 67-77.

Ohtsuka K, Noda H. Burst discharges of fastigial neurons in macaquemonkeys are driven by vision- and memory-guided saccades butnot by spontaneous saccades. Neurosci Res 1992; 15: 224—8.

Ohtsuka K, Noda H. Discharge properties of Purkinje cells in theoculomotor vermis during visually guided saccades in the macaquemonkey. J Neurophysiol 1995; 74: 1828-40.

Optican LM, Robinson DA. Cerebellar-dependent adaptive controlof primate saccadic system. J Neurophysiol 1980; 44: 1058-76.

Optican LM, Zee DS, Miles FA. Floccular lesions abolish adaptivecontrol of post-saccadic ocular drift in primates. Exp Brain Res1986; 64: 596-8.

Ritchie L. Effects of cerebellar lesions on saccadic eye movements.J Neurophysiol 1976; 39: 1246-56.

Robinson FR, Straube A, Fuchs AF. Role of the caudal fastigialnucleus in saccade generation. II. Effects of muscimol inactivation.J Neurophysiol 1993; 70: 1741-58.

Sato H, Noda H. Saccadic dysmetria induced by transient functionaldecortication of the cerebellar vermis [corrected] [published erratumappears in Exp Brain Res 1992; 89: 690]. Exp Brain Res 1992; 88:455-8.

Seidman SH, Telford L, Paige GD. Vertical, horizontal and torsionaleye movement responses to head roll in the squirrel monkey. ExpBrain Res 1995; 104: 218-26.

Snyder LH, King WM. Effect of viewing distance and location ofthe axis of head rotation on the monkey's vestibuloocular reflex. I.Eye movement responses. J Neurophysiol 1992; 67: 861-74.

Straube A, BUttner U. Pathophysiology of saccadic contrapulsion

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022

Page 18: Disorders of binocular control of eye movements in patients - Brain

1950 M. Versino et al.

in unilateral rostral cerebellar lesions. Neuroophthalmology 1994;14: 3-7.

Takagi M, Tamargo RJ, Zee DS. Dorsal cerebellar vermis lesionsimpair adaptive gain control of saccadic and pursuit eye movements[abstract]. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1996; 37 (3 Suppl): S472.

Vahedi K, Rivaud S, Amarenco P, Pierrot-Deseilligny C. Horizontaleye movement disorders after posterior vermis infarctions. J NeurolNeurosurg Psychiatry 1995; 58: 91^1.

Viirre E, Tweed D, Milner K, Vilis T. A reexamination of the gainof the vestibuloocular reflex. J Neurophysiol 1986; 56: 439-50.

Vilis T, Hore J. Characteristics of saccadic dysmetria in monkeysduring reversible lesions of medial cerebellar nuclei. J Neurophysiol1981; 46: 828-38.

Vilis T, Snow R, Hore J. Cerebellar saccadic dysmetria is not equalin the two eyes. Exp Brain Res 1983; 51: 343-50.

Waespe W. Deficits of smooth-pursuit eye movements in twopatients with a lesion in the (para-) floccular or dorsolateral pontineregion. Neuro-ophthalmology 1992; 12: 91-6.

Waespe W. Saccadic gain adaptivity in the two eyes in Wallenberg'slateral medullary syndrome. Neuro-ophthalmology 1995; 15: 193—201.

Waespe W, Baumgartner R. Enduring dysmetria and impaired gainadaptivity of saccadic eye movements in Wallenberg's lateralmedullary syndrome. Brain 1992; 115: 1123-46.

Waespe W, MUller-Meisser E. Directional reversal of saccadicdysmetria and gain adaptivity in a patient with a superior cerebellarartery infarction. Neuro-ophthalmology 1996; 16: 65-74.

Westheimer G, Blair SM. Oculomotor defects in cerebellectomizedmonkeys. Invest Ophthalmol 1973; 12: 618-21.

Williams AS, Hoyt CS. Acute comitant esotropia in children withbrain tumors. Arch Ophthalmol 1989; 107: 376-8.

Yee RD, Baloh RW, Honrubia V, Lau CGY, Jenkins HA. Slow build-up of optokinetic nystagmus associated with downbeat nystagmus.Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1979; 18: 622-9.

Ygge Y, Zee DS. Control of vertical eye alignment in three-dimensional space. Vision Res 1995; 35: 3169-81.

Zee DS. Cerebellar control of eye movements. In: Honrubia V,Brazier MAB, editors. Nystagmus and vertigo: clinical approachesto the patient with dizziness. New York: Academic Press, 1982a:241-9.

Zee DS. Ocular motor abnormalities related to lesions in thevestibulocerebellum in primate. In: Lennerstand DS, Keller EL,editors. Functional basis of ocular motility disorders. Oxford:Pergamanon Press, 1982b: 423-30.

Zee DS. Considerations on the mechanisms of alternating skewdeviation in patients with cerebellar lesions. J Vestib Res 1996.In press.

Zee DS, Yee RD, Cogan DG, Robinson DA, Engel WK. Ocularmotor abnormalities in hereditary cerebellar ataxia. Brain 1976; 99:207-34.

Zee DS, Yamazaki A, Butler PH, GUcer G. Effects of ablationof flocculus and paraflocculus of eye movements in primate. JNeurophysiol 1981; 46: 878-99.

Received April 16, 1996. Revised June 11, 1996.Accepted June 24, 1996

Dow

nloaded from https://academ

ic.oup.com/brain/article/119/6/1933/466557 by guest on 24 January 2022