coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas ......2013/08/23  · prefrontal cortex...

6
Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas correlate with decision making Verónica Nácher a , Anders Ledberg b , Gustavo Deco b,c , and Ranulfo Romo a,d,1 a Instituto de Fisiología Celular-Neurociencias, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, 04510 Mexico D.F., Mexico; b Center of Brain and Cognition, Department of Information and Communication Technologies, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, 08018 Barcelona, Spain; c Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats, Passeig Lluís Companys, 2308010 Barcelona, Spain; and d El Colegio Nacional, 06020 Mexico D.F., Mexico Contributed by Ranulfo Romo, August 5, 2013 (sent for review July 4, 2013) Coherent oscillations in the theta-to-gamma frequency range have been proposed as a mechanism that coordinates neural activity in large-scale cortical networks in sensory, motor, and cognitive tasks. Whether this mechanism also involves coherent oscillations at delta frequencies (14 Hz) is not known. Rather, delta oscilla- tions have been associated with slow-wave sleep. Here, we show coherent oscillations in the delta frequency band between parietal and frontal cortices during the decision-making component of a so- matosensory discrimination task. Importantly, the magnitude of this delta-band coherence is modulated by the different decision alternatives. Furthermore, during control conditions not requiring decision making, delta-band coherences are typically much reduced. Our work indicates an important role for synchronous activity in the delta frequency band when large-scale, distant cortical net- works coordinate their neural activity during decision making. synchrony | low-frequency rhythm | brain circuits S tudies of the neural correlates of decision making in behaving monkeys have mainly been based on the analysis of ring rate patterns of neurons in individual cortical circuits, recorded one by one in succession, while trained monkeys perform sensory, motor, and cognitive tasks (13). These studies showed that the neuronal activities distributed across parietal and frontal lobe cortices correlate with processes that lead to decision making (47). However, how these spatially distant, cortical circuits co- ordinate their activities into a unied functional network during decision making remains poorly understood. It has been proposed that coherent oscillations of neuronal activities constitute a putative dynamical mechanism for medi- ating the interaction between different subsets of brain areas (811). Simultaneous recording from multiple intracortical areas in monkeys showed that the coherent higher frequency (beta and gamma bands) oscillations are linked to a broad variety of cog- nitive functions (1218). Cortical oscillations at lower frequen- cies (theta and alpha bands) have also been discussed in terms of long-range integrative processes (19, 20). In fact, recent evi- dence showed theta-band coupling between visual area V4 and prefrontal cortex during short-term memory (21) and among the rat prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental area, and hippocampus during working memory (22). It remains, however, probing whether coherent delta-band oscillations play a functional role in the in- teraction between cortical circuits. Delta-band oscillations are typically associated with slow-wave sleep (SWS; ref. 23), but an important question is whether delta-band oscillations during SWS and waking states represent the same underlying phenomenon (24). Recent ndings associated delta-band oscillations in indi- vidual cortical areas with attention (25). In monkey primary visual cortex (26) and human motor cortex (27), delta-band oscillations entrain to the rhythm of external sensory events in an attention- dependent manner. Here, we examined whether coherent oscillations coordinate the activity of ve simultaneously recorded cortical areas in the monkey performing a somatosensory discrimination task (7). We specically focused on whether coherent delta-band oscillations play a signicant functional role in linking cortical circuits during decision making. Results We analyzed coherence for frequencies between 1 and 45 Hz from local eld potentials (LFPs) simultaneously recorded from ve cortical areas in a trained monkey performing a somatosen- sory discrimination task (7) (Materials and Methods). In this task (Fig. 1A), the monkey discriminates the difference in frequency between two mechanical vibrations delivered sequentially to one ngertip. Crucially, the monkey must hold the rst stimulus frequency (f1) in working memory, must compare the second stimulus frequency (f2) to the memory trace of f1 to form a de- cision of whether f2 > f1 or f2 < f1, and must postpone the decision report until a sensory cue triggers the motor report. The monkey was trained to perform the task up to its psychophysical threshold (Fig. 1 B and C). During each recording session, up to seven microelectrodes were individually inserted in each of the ve cortical areas for simultaneous recordings of the LFPs and the activity of single neurons during the task. The selected cortical areas were the primary somatosensory cortex (S1), secondary somatosensory cortex (S2), medial premotor cortex [MPC; the MPC areas of both hemispheres were not simultaneously recorded (Materials and Methods)], dorsal premotor cortex (DPC), and primary motor cortex (M1) (Fig. 1D). We based this selection on the pre- viously described responses of single neurons from these cortical areas associated with different components of the somatosensory discrimination task (7). Examples of the raw simultaneously re- corded LFPs during the discrimination task are shown in Fig. 1E and Fig. S1. Cortical Coherence Dynamics During the Discrimination Task. Next, to investigate low oscillatory (18 Hz) synchrony between LFPs from pairs of simultaneously recorded cortical areas during the task, we estimated the coherence between LFPs in short sliding windows (Materials and Method). Coherence is a measure of phase and amplitude consistency between two signals as a function of Signicance Delta oscillations have been largely associated with slow-wave sleep and anesthesia, when no conscious functions take place during these states. However, our study demonstrates that coherent delta-band oscillations reect the linkage between distant parietal and frontal cortical circuits during decision making. Thus, these ndings open an avenue for investigating whether the activity between distant cortical circuits oscillates in the delta frequency range during other cognitive functions. Author contributions: R.R. designed research; V.N. and R.R. performed research; V.N. and A.L. analyzed data; and V.N., A.L., G.D., and R.R. wrote the paper. The authors declare no conict of interest. 1 To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]. This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. 1073/pnas.1314681110/-/DCSupplemental. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1314681110 PNAS Early Edition | 1 of 6 NEUROSCIENCE Downloaded by guest on July 26, 2021

Upload: others

Post on 28-Feb-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas ......2013/08/23  · prefrontal cortex during short-term memory (21) and among the rat prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental

Coherent delta-band oscillations between corticalareas correlate with decision makingVerónica Náchera, Anders Ledbergb, Gustavo Decob,c, and Ranulfo Romoa,d,1

aInstituto de Fisiología Celular-Neurociencias, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, 04510 Mexico D.F., Mexico; bCenter of Brain and Cognition,Department of Information and Communication Technologies, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, 08018 Barcelona, Spain; cInstitució Catalana de Recerca i EstudisAvançats, Passeig Lluís Companys, 2308010 Barcelona, Spain; and dEl Colegio Nacional, 06020 Mexico D.F., Mexico

Contributed by Ranulfo Romo, August 5, 2013 (sent for review July 4, 2013)

Coherent oscillations in the theta-to-gamma frequency range havebeen proposed as a mechanism that coordinates neural activityin large-scale cortical networks in sensory, motor, and cognitivetasks. Whether this mechanism also involves coherent oscillationsat delta frequencies (1–4 Hz) is not known. Rather, delta oscilla-tions have been associated with slow-wave sleep. Here, we showcoherent oscillations in the delta frequency band between parietaland frontal cortices during the decision-making component of a so-matosensory discrimination task. Importantly, the magnitude ofthis delta-band coherence is modulated by the different decisionalternatives. Furthermore, during control conditions not requiringdecision making, delta-band coherences are typically much reduced.Our work indicates an important role for synchronous activity inthe delta frequency band when large-scale, distant cortical net-works coordinate their neural activity during decision making.

synchrony | low-frequency rhythm | brain circuits

Studies of the neural correlates of decision making in behavingmonkeys have mainly been based on the analysis of firing rate

patterns of neurons in individual cortical circuits, recorded oneby one in succession, while trained monkeys perform sensory,motor, and cognitive tasks (1–3). These studies showed that theneuronal activities distributed across parietal and frontal lobecortices correlate with processes that lead to decision making (4–7). However, how these spatially distant, cortical circuits co-ordinate their activities into a unified functional network duringdecision making remains poorly understood.It has been proposed that coherent oscillations of neuronal

activities constitute a putative dynamical mechanism for medi-ating the interaction between different subsets of brain areas (8–11). Simultaneous recording from multiple intracortical areas inmonkeys showed that the coherent higher frequency (beta andgamma bands) oscillations are linked to a broad variety of cog-nitive functions (12–18). Cortical oscillations at lower frequen-cies (theta and alpha bands) have also been discussed in termsof long-range integrative processes (19, 20). In fact, recent evi-dence showed theta-band coupling between visual area V4 andprefrontal cortex during short-term memory (21) and among therat prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental area, and hippocampusduring working memory (22). It remains, however, probing whethercoherent delta-band oscillations play a functional role in the in-teraction between cortical circuits. Delta-band oscillations aretypically associated with slow-wave sleep (SWS; ref. 23), but animportant question is whether delta-band oscillations during SWSand waking states represent the same underlying phenomenon(24). Recent findings associated delta-band oscillations in indi-vidual cortical areas with attention (25). In monkey primary visualcortex (26) and human motor cortex (27), delta-band oscillationsentrain to the rhythm of external sensory events in an attention-dependent manner.Here, we examined whether coherent oscillations coordinate

the activity of five simultaneously recorded cortical areas in themonkey performing a somatosensory discrimination task (7). Wespecifically focused on whether coherent delta-band oscillations

play a significant functional role in linking cortical circuits duringdecision making.

ResultsWe analyzed coherence for frequencies between 1 and 45 Hzfrom local field potentials (LFPs) simultaneously recorded fromfive cortical areas in a trained monkey performing a somatosen-sory discrimination task (7) (Materials and Methods). In this task(Fig. 1A), the monkey discriminates the difference in frequencybetween two mechanical vibrations delivered sequentially to onefingertip. Crucially, the monkey must hold the first stimulusfrequency (f1) in working memory, must compare the secondstimulus frequency (f2) to the memory trace of f1 to form a de-cision of whether f2 > f1 or f2 < f1, and must postpone thedecision report until a sensory cue triggers the motor report. Themonkey was trained to perform the task up to its psychophysicalthreshold (Fig. 1 B and C).During each recording session, up to seven microelectrodes

were individually inserted in each of the five cortical areas forsimultaneous recordings of the LFPs and the activity of singleneurons during the task. The selected cortical areas were theprimary somatosensory cortex (S1), secondary somatosensorycortex (S2), medial premotor cortex [MPC; the MPC areas ofboth hemispheres were not simultaneously recorded (Materialsand Methods)], dorsal premotor cortex (DPC), and primary motorcortex (M1) (Fig. 1D). We based this selection on the pre-viously described responses of single neurons from these corticalareas associated with different components of the somatosensorydiscrimination task (7). Examples of the raw simultaneously re-corded LFPs during the discrimination task are shown in Fig. 1Eand Fig. S1.

Cortical Coherence Dynamics During the Discrimination Task.Next, toinvestigate low oscillatory (1–8 Hz) synchrony between LFPsfrom pairs of simultaneously recorded cortical areas during thetask, we estimated the coherence between LFPs in short slidingwindows (Materials and Method). Coherence is a measure of phaseand amplitude consistency between two signals as a function of

Significance

Delta oscillations have been largely associated with slow-wavesleep and anesthesia, when no conscious functions take placeduring these states. However, our study demonstrates thatcoherent delta-band oscillations reflect the linkage betweendistant parietal and frontal cortical circuits during decisionmaking. Thus, these findings open an avenue for investigatingwhether the activity between distant cortical circuits oscillatesin the delta frequency range during other cognitive functions.

Author contributions: R.R. designed research; V.N. and R.R. performed research; V.N. andA.L. analyzed data; and V.N., A.L., G.D., and R.R. wrote the paper.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.1To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected].

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1314681110/-/DCSupplemental.

www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1314681110 PNAS Early Edition | 1 of 6

NEU

ROSC

IENCE

Dow

nloa

ded

by g

uest

on

July

26,

202

1

Page 2: Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas ......2013/08/23  · prefrontal cortex during short-term memory (21) and among the rat prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental

frequency, and coherence values ranges between 0 (no consistentrelationship between the two signals) and 1 (a constant relation-ship). We note that a significant coherence between two corticalLFPs can result if the two areas receive similar external inputs or ifthe two areas are interconnected and communicate with eachother (or a combination of these possibilities). We obtained a totalof 14 parietal, frontal, and parietal-frontal interareal comparisons(Materials and Method). Time frequency maps of coherence forall pairs of areas revealed significant modulations in the low fre-quency range (1–8 Hz, Fig. 2; 1–45 Hz, Fig. S2) during the task.The low frequency range oscillations (delta, 1–4 Hz; theta, 4–8 Hz)between sensory areas in the parietal lobe (S1 and S2) showedstronger coherence modulations during the stimulation periodsand the delays immediately afterward (Fig. 2A). Motor areas inthe frontal lobe showed low frequency range coherence modu-lations throughout the task, but delta-band coherence was stron-gest during the delay period between the end of f2 and the cue thattriggers the motor response (pu; i.e., postponed decision period,Fig. 2B). This elevated delta-band coherence during the post-poned decision period was also observed between the corticalareas located in the parietal and frontal lobes (Fig. 2C). Thus, apattern of delta-band synchronization varying in magnitude andtime course for all pairs of cortical areas was observed during thepostponed decision period. The synchronous delta oscillations inthese cortical recorded areas are clearly present in the raw data(Fig. 1E and Fig. S1).

Delta Coherence Is Modulated by Decision Making. A fundamentalquestion is whether the observed coherent oscillations betweenspatially segregated cortical areas are directly associated with thedecision-making process of this task. To answer this question, weselected hit trials in which f2 is equal to 22 Hz. In this case, f2 (22

Hz) can be judged higher or lower depending on the variable f1frequency. This selection allows us to discard effects due to thedifferent f2 stimulus frequencies. Next, we computed coherencesseparately for trials corresponding to the two possible responses(f2 > f1 and f2 < f1) and subtracted the resulting coherencespectrograms (Fig. S3). To assess the statistical significance ofthese coherence differences, we performed a cluster-based non-parametric randomization test (Materials and Methods). Thisanalysis identified the pairs of cortical areas showing significantcoherence differences (Fig. 3A). For each time-frequency plot ofcoherence differences (Fig. 3B), we detected all connected time-frequency points above a threshold value and used the size ofthese clusters as the test statistic. The significant clusters are shownin red in Fig. 3C. Clusters spanned from f2 to reaction time period(∼500 ms after the pu) in delta and theta frequency bands. Foreach cortical area pair, we obtained the frequency values that bestcorresponded to the location of the significant clusters (the max-imum length of the horizontal cluster dimension shown as red linesin Fig. 3C). Coherence for the two groups of trials (f2 > f1 and f2 <f1) as a function of time (Fig. 3D) revealed significant coherencedifferences during the postponed decision period, when the fre-quency values are in delta frequency range (2–3 Hz, red values inFig. 3C). The consistency of these coherence differences acrossrecording sessions are shown in Fig. S4. We next investigatedwhether these coherence differences are restricted to the deltaband or whether they were also present in the theta frequencyrange (5–8 Hz). Fig. S5 shows that there were no significantcoherence differences at theta frequency during the postponeddecision period. However, significant coherence differences attheta frequency were found during the 500 ms immediatelyafter pu (i.e., reaction time period). However, the time at whichthe monkey releases its hand from the key after the pu cue is

13579

111315171921232527293133

f1 f2

pu

*

*

**

*

-1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

A

C

D.L. 4.9 D.L. 3.7

Pro

babi

lity

calle

d hi

gher

1

0.5

0

Frequency (Hz)14 18 22 26 30

S1

S2

MPC

M1DPC

D

f1 (Hz)

f2 (

Hz)

10 14 18 22 26 30 34

34

30

26

22

18

14

10

70

82

78

83

82

96

858773665750729090

708186

%

7875685963

B

ku pb

pd

kd pu

ku pbf1 f2

Discrimination taskDiscrimination task

Control testsControl tests

Passive stimulation

Visual instruction

0 0.5 3.5 4 7 Time (s)

E

Cha

nnel

num

ber

left

S1

left

S2

left

MP

Cle

ft D

PC

right

M1

Time (s)

Fig. 1. Somatosensory discrimination task and cor-tical recording sites. (A) Sequence of events duringthe discrimination task and control tests (f1, firststimulus; f2, second stimulus; kd, key down; ku, keyup; pb, push button; pd, probe down; pu, probe up;Materials and Methods). (B) Stimulus set duringrecordings. Each box indicates a (f1, f2) frequencystimulus pair. The numbers inside the box indicatesoverall percentage of correct trials for each (f1, f2)stimulus pair. (C) Psychophysical performance whenf1 was maintained fixed at 22 Hz and f2 was variable(red curve), and when f2 was fixed at 22 Hz and f1was variable (green curve). D.L., animal’s discrimi-nation threshold in hertz. (D) Top view of the mon-key brain and the cortical areas recorded (greenspots). (E) Raw local field potentials simultaneouslyrecorded during the discrimination task. We illus-trate one single trial in one example recording ses-sion. LFPs are aligned to the f1 stimulus onset. Foranalysis purposes, we selected only one channel ineach cortical area (red asterisks). Channel selectionvaried across sessions.

2 of 6 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1314681110 Nácher et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by g

uest

on

July

26,

202

1

Page 3: Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas ......2013/08/23  · prefrontal cortex during short-term memory (21) and among the rat prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental

variable, and we cannot discard a possible movement effect inthe coherence differences found during this task period.To test whether delta-band coherence differences are explained

by differences in power in the same frequency band, we appliedthe same cluster-based analysis for power in each recorded area.The parietal areas (S1 and S2) displayed differences in this fre-quency range at 2 Hz, but none of the frontal lobe areas recordedfrom the right hemisphere (MPC and M1) showed significantpower differences for delta band as a function of the animal’sdecision (Fig. S6). However, significant beta-band (13–30 Hz)power differences were found for right MPC and right M1recorded areas during the postponed decision period, resultsthat have been reported (28). Significant beta-band clustersobserved during the f1 period for S1 and S2 areas are likely dueto stimulus entrainment.

Delta Coherence Is Context Dependent.To further test whether delta-band coherences are directly associated with decision making, wealso analyzed coherence in two control tasks: passive stimulationand visual instruction (Fig. 1A, Middle and Bottom; Materials andMethods). During passive stimulation, the same stimuli used duringthe discrimination task were delivered to the fingertip, but themonkey was not requested to perform the task. During visual in-struction, the trials proceeded exactly as in the discrimination task,

but the stimuli were not delivered to the skin and the movementswere guided by visual cues present from the start of the trial. Inthis last condition, it is not possible for the monkey to elaboratea decision based on the evaluation of somatosensory information,but the same movements as in discrimination task were executedfor reward. Compared with the discrimination task, both controlconditions had reduced delta-band coherence, in particular duringthe postponed decision period (Fig. 4). To test whether this re-duced coherence was caused by the fewer number of trials duringcontrol tasks (n = 30), we repeated the coherence analysis byselecting 30 random hit trials during the discrimination task (Fig.4B). Similar coherence results were found compared with thoseresults obtained during the discrimination task in which all hittrials were selected (Fig. 2). Fig. 4E shows the coherence differ-ences among the three tasks during the postponed decision period.Quantification of these differences during the postponed decisionperiod revealed that the magnitude of delta-band coherence inthe discrimination task is significantly higher compared with bothcontrol sets (Table S1). Moreover, we explored coherence differ-ences between f2 > f1 and f2 < f1 groups of trials by using thesame cluster-based randomization test for the pairs of corticalareas as we did in the discrimination task (Fig. 3). There were nosignificant coherence differences in the frequency range studied(1–8 Hz) during the whole time task, neither for the visual in-struction (Fig. S7 B and C), nor for the passive stimulation (Fig. S7D and E). Thus, under control conditions, delta-band coherencesdifferences did not depend on the animal’s decision report.

Spike-Field Synchrony. To investigate whether the synchronousdelta-band oscillations of the LFPs influence computations in thelocal neuronal circuits, we next made spike-triggered averages(STAs) of the LFPs (Materials and Methods). In many cases,STAs exhibited a strong oscillatory patterning in the frequencyrange of 1–3 Hz, indicating that temporal patterning of singleneuron discharges were correlated with the phase of delta-bandLFPs (Fig. S8). This result suggests that synchronized delta-bandactivity could coordinate the spiking activity of neurons in wide-spread cortical areas. Moreover, STAs were also modulated bythe different decision alternatives (Fig. S8D), showing that thesynchronization between spike times and LFPs carry decisionrelated information.

DiscussionOur findings demonstrate that the simultaneously recorded ac-tivity from parietal and frontal cortical areas is synchronized inthe delta-band frequency range during decision making. Thissynchronization is widespread, modulated by the different de-cision alternatives and context specific. Interestingly, these pat-terns of synchronization are prevalent for interactions betweenthe sensory areas (S1 and S2) located in the left parietal lobe(contralateral to the stimulated finger) and the motor areas(MPC and M1) located in the right frontal lobe (contralateral tothe responding hand/arm). Thus, this synchrony could facilitatethe communication between the left and right hemisphere duringdecision making. However, further analysis is required to showwhether this interaction is so.It is worth mentioning that previous studies in which simul-

taneous recordings from multiple cortical areas were made, thefunctional role of delta-band was not explored. One possibility isthat delta oscillations are hard to detect if the relevant taskperiods are of shorter duration (<1 s). The long postponed delayperiod (3 s) of our task allowed for the studying of delta-bandoscillations. Another possibility is that delta oscillations havebeen considered of little functional relevance in cognition be-cause they are consistently observed in SWS and during generalanesthesia (23, 29). There is no definitive evidence regarding theorigin of delta activity in the brain, but several studies place thesite of waking delta-band generation in the anterior medial

2

864

2

864

2

864

2

864

2

864

2

864

Fre

quen

cy (

Hz)

0 2 4 6 8

0 2 4 6 8

Time (s)

Time (s)

Coherence frontal

Coherence parietal

0.4 0.6 0.8

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8

f1 f2 pu

f1 f2 pu

S1S2

MPCDPC

MPC M1

DPC MPC

MPCM1

DPC M1

A

B

2

864

2

864

2

864

2

864

2

864

2

864

2

864

2

864

Time (s)Coherence parietal - frontal

0 2 4 6 8

0.2 0.4 0.6

f1 f2 pu

Fre

quen

cy (

Hz)

S1MPC

S1 MPC

S1DPC

S1 M1

S2MPC

S2 MPC

S2DPC

S2 M1

C

Fig. 2. Cortical coherence dynamics during the discrimination task (1–8 Hz).(A) Parietal pair areas. (B) Frontal pair areas. (C) Parieto-frontal pair areas.Time-frequency plots show averaged LFP-LFP coherence over all sessions (n =18) for simultaneously recorded cortical pairs. Coherence was calculated forhit trials by using an adaptive window that decreases in length with in-creased frequency in 100-ms consecutive window steps (note the boundaryeffects for lower frequencies). We show significant coherence results up to8 Hz (P < 0.05, nonparametric randomization test; Materials and Methods).White time frequency points outside boundary effects indicate that co-herence is not significant. Black vertical lines depict time task events: pre-sentation of the first stimulus (0-0.5 s), presentation of the second stimulus(3.5–4 s), and probe up event (7 s).

Nácher et al. PNAS Early Edition | 3 of 6

NEU

ROSC

IENCE

Dow

nloa

ded

by g

uest

on

July

26,

202

1

Page 4: Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas ......2013/08/23  · prefrontal cortex during short-term memory (21) and among the rat prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental

frontal cortex (30, 31). Moreover, recordings in waking animalsshow the existence of delta-band oscillations in the nucleusaccumbens (32) and dopamine neurons of the ventral tegmentalarea (33), structures that are part of the brain reward system.These observations, together with those reported in attentionaltasks (25–27), show that delta-band oscillations occurring in in-dividual brain areas play a role in cognitive functions. Our resultsshow that distant cortical circuits are linked in the delta-frequencyband during decision making. However, whether the activity be-tween distant cortical circuits oscillates in the delta frequencyrange during other cognitive functions is an open question.

Materials and MethodsThis studywas performedonone adultmalemonkey (Macacamulatta), weighing12 kg. All procedures followed the guidelines of the National Institutes of Healthand Society for Neuroscience. Protocols were approved by the Institutional An-imal Care and Use Committee of the Instituto de Fisiología Celular.

Discrimination Task. The paradigm used here has been described (34, 35). Themonkey sat on a primate chair with its head fixed in an isolated, sound-proof room. The right hand was restricted through a half-cast and kept ina palm-up position. The left hand operated an immovable key (elbow at∼90°), and two push buttons were in front of the animal, 25 cm away fromthe shoulder and at eye level. The centers of the switches were located 7 and10.5 cm to the left of the midsagittal plane. In all trials, the monkey firstplaced the left hand and later projected to one of the two switches. Stimuliwere delivered to the skin of the distal segment of one digit of the right,restrained hand, via a computer-controlled stimulator (2 mm round tip; BMESystems). The initial probe indentation was 500 μm. Vibrotactile stimuli weretrains of short mechanical pulses. Each of these pulses consisted of a single-cycle sinusoid lasting 20 ms. Stimulus amplitudes were adjusted to equalsubjective intensities; for example, 71 μm at 12 Hz and 51 μm at 34 Hz (adecrease of ∼1.4% per Hz). During discrimination trials (Fig. 1A), the me-chanical probe was lowered (probe down; pd), indenting the glabrous skinof one digit of the hand; the monkey placed its free hand on an immovablekey (key down; kd); after a variable prestimulus delay (0.5–3 s) the probe

oscillated vertically at the frequency of the first stimulus (f1); after a fixeddelay (3 s), a second mechanical vibration was delivered at the secondstimulus (f2) frequency; after another fixed delay (3 s) the probe is lifted offfrom the skin (probe up; pu); the monkey released the key (ku) and pressedeither a lateral or a medial push button (pb) to indicate whether f2 was ofhigher or lower frequency than f1, respectively. The monkey was rewardedwith a drop of liquid for correct discriminations. Performance was quantifiedthrough psychometric techniques (Fig. 1 B and C).

Control Tests. Visual instruction. Trials during the visual instruction begin ex-actly as described in the discrimination task, but when the probe touched theskin, one of the push buttons was illuminated. Themonkey had to respond byholding the immovable key until the light was turned off and the probe waslifted off from the skin, which triggers the hand/armmovement. The monkeywas rewarded for pressing the previously illuminated push button (100%correct responses). Note that in this condition, the vibratory stimuli are ab-sent. The yellow box in Fig. 1A represents the time in which one of the pushbuttons remained illuminated.Passive stimulation.During this condition, the monkey was trained to maintainits free arm motionless during the trial (Fig. 1A). Stimuli were delivered tothe fingertip and the animal remained alert by being rewarded with dropsof liquid at different times, but no motor response with the free handwas required.

Recording. Data acquisition, amplification, and filtering were described indetail (36). In brief, LFPs and the activity of single neurons were simulta-neously recorded with an array of seven independent, movable micro-electrodes (1–1.5 MΩ) inserted in each of five cortical areas. Electrodeswithin an area were spaced 305 or 500 μm apart (37). Spike sorting wasperformed manually on-line, and single neurons were selected if theyresponded to any of the different components of the discrimination task(7, 36). The cortical areas were the S1, S2, MPC, DPC, and M1 (Fig. 1D).Recordings in S1, S2, and DPC were made in the hemisphere contralateral tothe stimulated hand (left hemisphere), and in M1 contralateral to theresponding hand/arm (right hemisphere). Recordings in MPC were madeeither in the hemisphere contralateral or ipsilateral to the responding arm indifferent sessions (n = 11 and n = 7, respectively). MPC areas were not

Fre

quen

cy (

Hz)

Fre

quen

cy (

Hz)

DPC MPC

DPC M1

S1 MPC

S1DPC

S1 M1

S2 MPC

S2 M1

2468

2468

2468

2468

2468

2468

2468

2

2

2

3

2

2

2

0.40.6

0.4

0.6

Coh

eren

ce

0.40.6

0.2

0.40.6

0.60.40.2

0.60.40.2

0.30.50.7

Coh

eren

ce

f1 f2 pu f1 f2 pu f1 f2 pu

f1 f2 pu f1 f2 pu f1 f2 pu

0 2 4 6 8

0 2 4 6 8

-0.1 0 0.1 0.2

-0.1 0 0.1 0.2

Coherence differences frontal

Coherence differences parietal - frontal

0 2 4 6 8 0 2 4 6 8

0 2 4 6 80 2 4 6 8

Time (s)

Time (s)

Time (s)Time (s)

Time (s) Time (s)

f2 > f1f2 < f1

A B C D

Fig. 3. Significant coherence differences betweenf2 > f1 and f2 < f1 for groups of hit trials in whichthe f2 stimulus frequency is equal to 22 Hz. (A)Schematic view of the cortical pairs from which co-herence is calculated (green spots). (B) Time-frequencyplots of the coherence differences between bothgroups of trials (Fig. S3B). (C) Time-frequency plotsshowing the clusters where there are coherencedifferences between f2 > f1 minus f2 < f1 groups oftrials. Only significant clusters are plotted in redcolor (P < 0.05, two-sided test; Materials and Meth-ods). Horizontal red lines indicate the frequencyvalues where the significant cluster has the maxi-mum length. (D) Time course of coherence for eachgroup of trials at frequency values (horizontal redlines). Error bars correspond to SEM (±2) over re-cording sessions (n = 18). Black vertical lines depict timetask events: presentation of the first stimulus (0–0.5 s),presentation of the second stimulus (3.5–4 s), andprobe up event (7 s).

4 of 6 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1314681110 Nácher et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by g

uest

on

July

26,

202

1

Page 5: Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas ......2013/08/23  · prefrontal cortex during short-term memory (21) and among the rat prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental

recorded simultaneously, leading a total of 14 interareal pairs. LFPs weresimultaneously recorded by using a 250-Hz Butterworth digital low-passfilter of fourth order and were stored at 2 kHz for offline analysis. Theneuronal signal of each microelectrode was sampled at 30 KHz. During thediscrimination task, we collected data by using the stimulus set of Fig. 1B, 10trials for each stimulus frequency pair. During control sets, we collected fivetrials for the six stimulus frequency pairs with black boxes. All LFPs recordedfrom S1 and S2 had cutaneous receptive fields confined to the distal seg-ments of the glabrous skin of fingertips 2, 3, or 4. Recording sites changedfrom session to session.

Data Analysis. Data were analyzed offline by using custom-build Matlab code(MathWorks). For the work presented here, we selected 18 experimentalsessions in which the monkey had similar psychophysical thresholds (34).Before the LFP data were submitted to statistical analysis, the whole trialscontaminated with artifacts (e.g., because of movement or electronic in-terference) were removed based on visual inspection of the data for allchannels recorded simultaneously in each session. Similar raw LFPs signalsfrom the same areas were observed (Fig. 1E). The most probable explanationcould be the short distance between microelectrodes within the microelec-trode array (38). The simplest strategy for across area comparisons was toselect only one representative channel in each cortical area. For this purpose,two criteria used were as follows: (i) LFPs from S1 area should clearly showmodulation as a function of stimulus frequency during the stimulus pre-sentation, indicating that the microelectrode was placed in the center of thecutaneous receptive field and (ii) power spectra showing task-relatedmodulations. Only trials with correct responses during the discriminationtask were used for data analysis.

Data were filtered to have power between 1 and 45 Hz and down sampledto 200 Hz. We used a digital finite impulse response (FIR) filter as imple-mented in the Matlab functions fir1 and filtfilt to achieve this aim. Examplesof filtered LFPs are shown in Fig. S1. Spectral quantities were estimated foreach trial separately in moving windows by using bin sizes that depended onthe frequency, i.e., shorter windows for higher frequencies. At each pointin time (middle of a window), we estimated the best fitting complex ex-ponential for frequencies between 1 and 45 Hz in steps of 1 Hz by usingordinary least squares. Given that we used short bin windows, which cor-respond to an oversampling of the spectra, similar results would be obtainedby interpolating standard fast Fourier Transform-based estimates. The am-plitude at each frequency and time point was taken as the trial average ofthe modulus (absolute value) of the exponentials. To investigate the lineardependence between each pair of areas, separately for each frequency, we

computed the correlation (over trials) between the complex exponentials.Note that this correlation can be viewed as an estimator of coherence, andwe refer to this quantity as “coherence.” To make this procedure more ro-bust, we used Spearman’s rank correlation and the coherence estimate canhence be viewed as a semiparametric estimate of coherence. The resultswere similar when we used Pearson’s product-moment correlation co-efficient (39). Furthermore, the results remained similar when we used theMatlab function mscohere for coherence estimates. Significance tests werebased on a permutation procedure where the original (i.e., nonpermuted)results were compared with the tail of a distribution obtained by permutingthe trials differently for the two areas under investigation (40). Time-fre-quency maps of coherence values were thresholded by using the N × P highestvalue obtained from in the permutation distribution (where N is the totalnumber of permutations and P is the desired “P value”). Thus, we tested forcoherence against a null hypothesis of complete independence and correctedfor the multiple comparisons (one per frequency and time-bin) by using themaximum coherence value as the test statistic (40, 41).

To detect significant differences in coherence between groups of trials(f2 > f1 and f2 < f1 decision responses), we applied a cluster-based non-parametric randomization test (41), but this time across groups randomlytaking the same number of trials for each condition. This step eliminated anypossible bias from different sample sizes. By clustering neighboring samples,that is time-frequency points that presented significant coherence differ-ences as a function of the animal’s decision (Fig. S3B), this test deals with themultiple comparisons problem (41). For each sample, a dependent-samplet value was computed. All samples were selected for which this t valueexceeded a priori threshold (P < 0.05, two-sided test), and these sampleswere subsequently clustered on the basis of temporal-frequency adjacency(red clusters in Fig. 3B). The size of the largest cluster was used as the teststatistic. This procedure was done within sessions (n = 18) and separately foreach pair of areas. By randomizing the data across the two conditions andrecalculating the test statistic a maximum of 10,000 times, we obtained a ref-erence distribution of maximum cluster size values to evaluate the statistic ofthe actual data. This procedure is standard in the human brain imaging litera-ture (e.g., ref. 42) and has been used before in electrophysiological studies (41).

For calculating the STA at each recording site, the LFP signals were band-pass filtered between 1 and 4 Hz. Neurons with stationary firing rates acrosstrials that modulated their firing rates as a function of the decision report(except for S1 neurons) (7), and recorded from the same electrode selectedfor the LFP-LFP coherence analysis, were chosen. The STA analysis was doneon data in an interval between 4.5–6.5 s after the onset of the first stimulus(the postponed decision period). This interval was divided into three

0.40.6

0.40.6

Coh

eren

ce4

8F

requ

ency

(H

z) 6

2

4

86

2

4

86

2

4

86

2

4

86

2

4

86

2

4

86

2

Fre

quen

cy (

Hz)

f1 f2 pu f1 f2 pu f1 f2 pu

DPC M1

DPC MPC

0 2 4 6 8 0 2 4 6 8 0 2 4 6 8

0 2 4 6 80 2 4 6 80 2 4 6 8

Time (s)

Time (s)

Coherence frontal

Coherence parietal - frontal

S1 MPC

S1DPC

S1 M1

S2 MPC

S2 M1

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8

0.2 0.4 0.6

4 65 7

4 65 7

Coh

eren

ce

0.40.2

0.6

0.40.2

0.6

0.40.2

0.6

0.40.2

0.6

0.40.2

0.6

Discrimination taskVisual instructionPassive stimulation

f1 f2 pu f1 f2 pu f1 f2 pu

Time (s)Time (s)

Time (s)Time (s)

Time (s)

Time (s)

f2 pu

f2 pu

A B C D E

Fig. 4. Coherence comparison between discrimina-tion task and control tests. (A) Schematic view of thecortical pairs from which coherence is calculated(green spots). (B–D) Time-frequency plots of aver-aged LFP-LFP coherence over all sessions (n = 18) forthe discrimination task in which 30 random hit trialswere selected, visual instruction, and passive stimu-lation, respectively. We show significant coherenceresults up to 8 Hz (P < 0.05, nonparametric ran-domization test; Materials and Methods). Whitetime frequency points outside boundary effects in-dicate that coherence is not significant. Black verticallines depict time task events: presentation of the firststimulus (0–0.5 s), presentation of the second stimulus(3.5–4 s) and probe up event (7 s). (E) Time course ofcoherence at 2 Hz for each task type during the delayperiod between f2 and pu. Error bars correspond toSEM (± 2) over recording sessions (n = 18).

Nácher et al. PNAS Early Edition | 5 of 6

NEU

ROSC

IENCE

Dow

nloa

ded

by g

uest

on

July

26,

202

1

Page 6: Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas ......2013/08/23  · prefrontal cortex during short-term memory (21) and among the rat prefrontal cortex, ventral tegmental

intervals of 1 s each. The first 500 ms after the end of the second stimulusand the last 500 ms before the probe up event were not included in theanalysis to prevent phase locking between spikes and LFPs due to stimulusand movement-related activity. STAs were calculated by averaging LFPsegments ± 1 s around every spike recorded in correct hit trials. To charac-terize the variability of the STA results, the same analysis was applied to“surrogate data” where the order between LFP trials and spike -trials wererandomly permuted. For each recorded cell, we made 100 such permutations(shown as gray curves in the background of Fig. S8 B and C). To explore STAmodulations as a function of the animal’s decision, the STA analysis wasdone separately for f2 > f1 and f2 < f1 groups of hit trials in which the f2stimulus frequency was equal to 22 Hz (Fig. S8D).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. We thank R. Desimone for helpful comments on anearlier version of the manuscript. R.R.’s research was partially supported byInternational Research Scholars Award 55005959 from the Howard HughesMedical Institute, Dirección de Personal Académico de la Universidad Nacio-nal Autónoma de México Grant IN203210 and Consejo Nacional de Ciencia yTecnología Grant CB-2009-01-130863. V.N. was supported by a Ministerio deEducación y Ciencia (MEC)-Fullbright Postdoctoral Fellowship from the Span-ish Ministry of Science and Technology and Dirección de Personal Académicode la Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. A.L. was supported by theRamón y Cajal program from the Spanish government. G.D. was supported bythe European Research Council Advanced Grant DYSTRUCTURE 295129 andEuropean Union Seventh Framework Programme FP7/2007-2013 under Grant269921 (BrainScaleS).

1. Schall JD (2001) Neural basis of deciding, choosing and acting. Nat Rev Neurosci 2(1):33–42.

2. Romo R, de Lafuente V (2013) Conversion of sensory signals into perceptual decisions.Prog Neurobiol 103:41–75.

3. Gold JI, Shadlen MN (2007) The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci30:535–574.

4. Kim JN, Shadlen MN (1999) Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex of the macaque. Nat Neurosci 2(2):176–185.

5. Platt ML, Glimcher PW (1999) Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex.Nature 400(6741):233–238.

6. Hoshi E, Tanji J (2004) Differential roles of neuronal activity in the supplementary andpresupplementary motor areas: From information retrieval to motor planning andexecution. J Neurophysiol 92(6):3482–3499.

7. Hernández A, et al. (2010) Decoding a perceptual decision process across cortex.Neuron 66(2):300–314.

8. Fries P (2005) A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication throughneuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci 9(10):474–480.

9. Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W (2001) Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony intop-down processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 2(10):704–716.

10. Buzsáki G, Draguhn A (2004) Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science304(5679):1926–1929.

11. Hipp JF, Engel AK, Siegel M (2011) Oscillatory synchronization in large-scale corticalnetworks predicts perception. Neuron 69(2):387–396.

12. Murthy VN, Fetz EE (1992) Coherent 25- to 35-Hz oscillations in the sensorimotorcortex of awake behaving monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89(12):5670–5674.

13. Bressler SL, Coppola R, Nakamura R (1993) Episodic multiregional cortical coherenceat multiple frequencies during visual task performance. Nature 366(6451):153–156.

14. Brovelli A, et al. (2004) Beta oscillations in a large-scale sensorimotor cortical network:Directional influences revealed by Granger causality. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101(26):9849–9854.

15. Buschman TJ, Miller EK (2007) Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in theprefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315(5820):1860–1862.

16. Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R (2009) High-frequency, long-rangecoupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science 324(5931):1207–1210.

17. Pesaran B, Nelson MJ, Andersen RA (2008) Free choice activates a decision circuitbetween frontal and parietal cortex. Nature 453(7193):406–409.

18. Salazar RF, Dotson NM, Bressler SL, Gray CM (2012) Content-specific fronto-parietalsynchronization during visual working memory. Science 338(6110):1097–1100.

19. von Stein A, Sarnthein J (2000) Different frequencies for different scales of corticalintegration: From local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. Int JPsychophysiol 38(3):301–313.

20. Palva S, Palva JM (2007) New vistas for alpha-frequency band oscillations. TrendsNeurosci 30(4):150–158.

21. Liebe S, Hoerzer GM, Logothetis NK, Rainer G (2012) Theta coupling between V4 andprefrontal cortex predicts visual short-term memory performance. Nat Neurosci 15(3):456–462, S1–S2.

22. Fujisawa S, Buzsáki G (2011) A 4 Hz oscillation adaptively synchronizes prefrontal,VTA, and hippocampal activities. Neuron 72(1):153–165.

23. Hobson JA, Pace-Schott EF (2002) The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: Neuronalsystems, consciousness and learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 3(9):679–693.

24. Knyazev GG (2012) EEG delta oscillations as a correlate of basic homeostatic andmotivational processes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36(1):677–695.

25. Fries P, Womelsdorf T, Oostenveld R, Desimone R (2008) The effects of visual stimu-lation and selective visual attention on rhythmic neuronal synchronization in ma-caque area V4. J Neurosci 28(18):4823–4835.

26. Lakatos P, Karmos G, Mehta AD, Ulbert I, Schroeder CE (2008) Entrainment of neu-ronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science 320(5872):110–113.

27. Saleh M, Reimer J, Penn R, Ojakangas CL, Hatsopoulos NG (2010) Fast and slow os-cillations in human primary motor cortex predict oncoming behaviorally relevantcues. Neuron 65(4):461–471.

28. Haegens S, et al. (2011) Beta oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network reflectsomatosensory decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108(26):10708–10713.

29. Schomer DL, Lopes da Silva FH (2011) Niedermeyer’s Electroencephalography: BasicPrinciples, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields (Lippincot Williams & Wilkins,Philadelphia).

30. Michel CM, Lehmann D, Henggeler B, Brandeis D (1992) Localization of the sources ofEEG delta, theta, alpha and beta frequency bands using the FFT dipole approxima-tion. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 82(1):38–44.

31. Alper KR, et al. (2006) Correlation of PET and qEEG in normal subjects. Psychiatry Res146(3):271–282.

32. Leung LS, Yim CY (1993) Rhythmic delta-frequency activities in the nucleus accumbensof anesthetized and freely moving rats. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 71(5-6):311–320.

33. Shi WX (2005) Slow oscillatory firing: A major firing pattern of dopamine neurons inthe ventral tegmental area. J Neurophysiol 94(5):3516–3522.

34. Hernández A, Salinas E, García R, Romo R (1997) Discrimination in the sense of flutter:New psychophysical measurements in monkeys. J Neurosci 17(16):6391–6400.

35. Lemus L, et al. (2007) Neural correlates of a postponed decision report. Proc Natl AcadSci USA 104(43):17174–17179.

36. Hernández A, et al. (2008) Procedure for recording the simultaneous activity of singleneurons distributed across cortical areas during sensory discrimination. Proc Natl AcadSci USA 105(43):16785–16790.

37. Eckhorn R, Thomas U (1993) A new method for the insertion of multiple microprobesinto neural and muscular tissue, including fiber electrodes, fine wires, needles andmicrosensors. J Neurosci Methods 49(3):175–179.

38. Kajikawa Y, Schroeder CE (2011) How local is the local field potential? Neuron 72(5):847–858.

39. Derrick TR, Bates BT, Dufek JS (1994) Evaluation of time-series data sets using thePearson product-moment correlation coefficient. Med Sci Sports Exerc 26(7):919–928.

40. Maris E, Oostenveld R (2007) Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data.J Neurosci Methods 164(1):177–190.

41. Maris E, Schoffelen JM, Fries P (2007) Nonparametric statistical testing of coherencedifferences. J Neurosci Methods 163(1):161–175.

42. Ledberg A (2000) Robust estimation of the probabilities of 3-D clusters in functionalbrain images: Application to PET data. Hum Brain Mapp 9(3):143–155.

6 of 6 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1314681110 Nácher et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by g

uest

on

July

26,

202

1