can’t learn, won’t learn? an ... - university of salford

119
CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN EXTENSION OF DWECK AND LEGGETT’S MOTIVATIONAL FRAMEWORK A dissertation submitted to The University of Manchester for the degree of Master of Science in the Faculty of Humanities 2012 JOHN HUDSON MANCHESTER BUSINESS SCHOOL

Upload: others

Post on 26-Apr-2022

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN?

AN EXTENSION OF DWECK AND LEGGETT’S MOTIVATIONAL FRAMEWORK

A dissertation submitted to The University of Manchester for the degree of Master of Science in the Faculty of Humanities

2012

JOHN HUDSON

MANCHESTER BUSINESS SCHOOL

Page 2: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

1

Table of Contents List of tables........................................................................................................ 3

List of figures ...................................................................................................... 4

Abstract ............................................................................................................... 5

Declaration .......................................................................................................... 6

Intellectual property statement ............................................................................. 7

Dedication ........................................................................................................... 8

Acknowledgements ............................................................................................. 9

1.0 Introduction ................................................................................................. 10

2. 0 Literature review ........................................................................................ 14

2.1 Mindset ................................................................................................... 14

2.2 Goal orientation ....................................................................................... 19

2.2.1 Mindset and goal orientations ........................................................... 21

2.3 Locus of control ....................................................................................... 24

2.3.1 Locus of control, mindset and goal orientations: mediation? ............. 26

2.4 Self-efficacy ............................................................................................ 28

2.4.1 Locus of control and self-efficacy ..................................................... 29

2.4.2 Mindset, goal orientations and self-efficacy ...................................... 30

2.5 Overview and hypothesised model ........................................................... 32

2.6 Study aims ............................................................................................... 35

3. 0 Method ....................................................................................................... 37

3.1 Study design ............................................................................................ 37

3.2 Sample .................................................................................................... 37

3.3 Data collection instruments ...................................................................... 39

3.3.1 Mindset ............................................................................................. 39

3.3.2 Locus of control ................................................................................ 40

3.3.3 Goal orientation ................................................................................ 41

3.3.4 Public speaking self-efficacy ............................................................. 42

3.3.5 Public speaking measures.................................................................. 42

3.4 Procedure ................................................................................................ 44

Page 3: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

2

4.0 Results......................................................................................................... 46

4.1 Preliminary analyses ................................................................................ 46

4.2 Confirmatory factor analysis .................................................................... 48

4.2.1 Item Parcels ...................................................................................... 49

4.3 Structural equation models (SEM) ........................................................... 55

4.3.1 Missing data...................................................................................... 56

4.3.2 Structural models .............................................................................. 57

4.3.3 Additional analysis ........................................................................... 65

5 Discussion ...................................................................................................... 67

5.1 Summary of analysis and results .............................................................. 67

5.2 Discussion of results ................................................................................ 69

5.2.1 Mindset and locus of control ............................................................. 69

5.2.2 Relationships from LoC to goal orientations and PSSE ..................... 73

5.2.3 Goal orientations and dependent variables......................................... 75

5.2.4 Public speaking self-efficacy and dependent variables....................... 78

5.3 Limitations .............................................................................................. 79

5.4 Future research ........................................................................................ 81

5.5 Implications ............................................................................................. 84

5.6 Conclusion .............................................................................................. 86

6. References .................................................................................................... 88

7.0 Appendices ............................................................................................ 108

7.1 Appendix A: Questionnaire ................................................................ 108

7.2 Appendix B: Missing data analysis and demographic differences between groups with missing and complete data .................................................... 112

7.3 Appendix C: Confirmatory factor analyses on individual scales ......... 114

Word count: 15, 729

Page 4: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

3

List of tables

Table 3.1 Participant demographic details Page 38

Table 4.1 CFA fit statistics of scales using individual items Page 51

Table 4.2 Means, standard deviations and correlations between latent and observed variables

Page 54

Table 4.3 Assessments of construct convergent and discriminant validity: AVE, MSV and ASV

Page 55

Table 4.4 Fit statistics for structural models Page 62

Table 7.1 Comparison of demographics between two groups with complete and incomplete data for dependent variables

Page 113

Table 7.2 SPSS output showing mean differences of independent variables between two groups with complete and incomplete data

Page 114

Page 5: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

4

List of figures

Figure 2.1: Hypothesised model and direction of relationships Page 35

Figure 4.1. Measurement model Page 53

Figure 4.2:

SEM model 1: LoC mediating the effect sof mindset on goal orientations

Page 59

Figure 4.3 SEM model 2: covaried mindset and LoC model

Page 61

Figure 4.4: Final model Page 64

Figure 4.5: Test of significance of indirect effects of mindset on learning and avoidance goal orientations.

Page 66

Page 6: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

5

Abstract

Does it matter whether we believe our abilities are fixed or malleable? This study

aimed to test and extend Dweck and Leggett’s (1988) motivational framework,

which proposes that this belief is directly related to the type of goals people

pursue. However, empirical support has not been as robust as its theoretical basis

would imply, and it was contended that locus of control would mediate this

relationship and be predictive of goal orientations and self-efficacy, shown to

influence the self-regulatory strategies people use. This study answered calls for

greater integration of a range of social cognitive constructs by investigating their

influence in relation to public speaking and level of practice at a network of

public speaking clubs. Based on a sample of 161 people, confirmatory factor

analysis and structural equation modelling were used to assess the hypothesised

model. Results supported the mediating role of locus of control, as well as the

impact of goal orientations on long-term membership at the clubs. The latter also

had a positive relationship with public speaking self-efficacy. In common with

previous research, self-efficacy was also significantly related to the amount of

practice people engaged in, but contrary to expectations, goal orientations were

not. A number of methodological issues were identified and discussed alongside

the potential implications of this research. This was the first study to test the

addition of a mediating construct in Dweck and Leggett’s popular framework and

these results may illuminate equivocal findings of previous research looking at

mindset and goal orientation.

Page 7: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

6

Declaration

I declare that no portion of the work referred to in the dissertation has been

submitted in support of an application for another degree or qualification of this

or any other university of other institute of learning.

Page 8: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

7

Intellectual property statement

i. The author of this dissertation (including any appendices and/or schedules to this dissertation) owns certain copyright or related rights in it (the “Copyright”) and he has given the University of Manchester certain rights to use such Copyright, including for administrative purposes.

ii. Copies of this dissertation, either in full or in extracts and whether in hard or electronic copy, may be made only in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988 (as amended) and regulations issued under it or, where appropriate, in accordance with licensing agreements which University has from time to time. This page must form part of any such copies made.

iii. The ownership of certain Copyright, patents, designs, trademarks and other intellectual property (the “Intellectual Property”) and any reproductions of copyright works in the dissertation, for example graphs and tables (“Reproductions”) which may be described in this dissertation, may not be owned by the author and may be owned by third parties. Such Intellectual Property and Reproductions cannot and must not be made available for use without the prior written permission of the owner(s) of the relevant Intellectual Property and/or Reproductions.

iv. Further information on the conditions under which disclosure, publication and commercialisation of this dissertation, the Copyright and any Intellectual Property and/or Reproductions described in it may take place is available in the University IP Policy (see http://documents.manchester.ac.uk/display.aspx?DocID=487), in any relevant Dissertation restriction declarations deposited in the University Library, The University Library’s regulations (see http://documents.manchester.ac.uk/library/aboutus/regulations) and in The University’s Guidance for the Presentation of Dissertations.

Page 9: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

8

Dedication

To my absolutely wonderful mum and family. Thank you for all the love and

support you give me. This is for you.

Page 10: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

9

Acknowledgements

From a cast of thousands… Thanks to Paul Irwing, my supervisor, for his

patience when facing a barrage of confusing statistical questions and his

willingness to put up with my regular digressions into the world of sport.

To the lecturers and staff at MBS: Thanks for letting me in in the first place, and

for looking after me while I was here. All of you have helped make the last year

time well spent.

Thanks also to my classmates, who really are a great bunch of people and have

kept each other’s spirits up throughout.

Finally, huge thanks to my good friend Tom who has had to listen to my

digressions into the world of factor loadings and beta coefficients, when he would

rather have been talking about the Tour de France. Or the Olympics. Or

anything,

Thank you, one and all.

Page 11: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

10

1.0 Introduction

“Whether you think you can or cannot, you are right”

Henry Ford.

Ford’s maxim is a perennial favourite of motivational speakers and is not without

empirical support. It echoes one of the central tenets of Bandura’s (1977) self-

efficacy theory, while implicit in the statement is the belief that you have some

control, which relates to Rotter’s (1966) concept of locus of control. Both have a

strong evidence base and are among the most researched constructs in

psychological literature (Judge, Erez, Bono & Thoresen, 2002), playing a role in

persistence, performance and resilience (Bandura, 1997; Levenson, 1981; Stevens

& Gist, 1997). Meanwhile, a growing body of research has indicated there may

be a key variable underpinning these and influencing whether we truly believe we

can. Or cannot.

Mindset is a term coined by Carol Dweck (2006), and her theory asserts that

individuals hold one of two divergent perspectives regarding the fixedness or

malleability of personal characteristics (Dweck, 2000). People with an entity

mindset perceive personal abilities and characteristics as fixed traits, while

individuals with an incremental perspective believe these can be developed by

Page 12: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

11

dint of their own efforts (Dweck, 2006). This theory has its roots in educational

settings and although the majority of research was therefore conducted in

academic contexts (e.g. Aronson, Fried & Good, 2002; Howell & Buro, 2009;

King, 2012; Mueller & Dweck, 1998), there is a growing evidence base for its

applications in other domains (e.g. Beer, 2002; Burnette, 2010; Heslin, Latham

and VandeWalle, 2005). Dweck (2000) asserts that these opposing views

influence how learning situations are cognised (e.g. Mueller & Dweck, 1998) and

these notions of ability initiate self-regulatory mechanisms in either self-

enhancing or self-impeding ways (Bandura, 1997; Wood & Bandura, 1989).

Consequently, the contrasting mindsets have been linked with both learning

involvement (e.g. Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin & Wan, 1999), procrastination

(Howell & Buro, 2009) and performance outcomes (Blackwell, Trzensiewski and

Dweck, 2007). This research has also tended to support the adaptive benefits of

holding the incremental rather than entity perspective of one’s own abilities.

However, although studies in academic contexts have predominated, mindset-

related research has clear relevance to other domains such as the workplace,

where learning and development can be so crucial to the success of both

individuals and organisations (Buckley & Caple, 2004). Overall, there appears to

be solid support for the impact of mindset and, encouragingly, research has begun

to show that mindset itself may be malleable and therefore amenable to

interventions (e.g. Blackwell et al, 2007; Heslin, VandeWalle & Latham, 2006).

Page 13: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

12

Mindset also forms part of Dweck and Leggett’s (1988) conceptual framework

where it is proposed as antecedent to goal orientation – i.e. whether someone

focuses on task mastery goals or goals demonstrating competence or the

avoidance of appearing incompetent. Dweck and Leggett propose that mindset

directly influences an individual’s goal orientation, consequently impacting task

strategy and persistence. This framework has proved to be influential across the

literature (e.g. Bandura, 1997; Skinner, 1996), although research has tended to

demonstrate relatively modest, but significant, relationships (e.g. Payne, Young &

Beaubien, 2007) despite the strong theoretical underpinning.

As mindset involves cognitions about controllability of skill acquisition or

personal change, there are clear theoretical links with locus of control, which in

turn has been is associated with self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997). However,

although research has investigated some of these relationships in the literature

(e.g. Bandura & Wood, 1989; Dupeyrat & Mariné, 2005; Payne et al, 2007), there

have been calls for greater integration of promising concepts such as mindset into

existing social cognitive theory (Funder, 2001). Presently no research has

investigated these constructs together and this study aims to extend Dweck and

Leggett’s (1988) framework and the relationships between mindset and goal

orientation by incorporating locus of control and self-efficacy.

These constructs tend to most strongly manifest their effects concerning

challenging rather than ‘routine’ tasks (Bandura, 1997), so this study examines

Page 14: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

13

their relationships relative to a common source of anxiety, public speaking. This

research will test a hypothesised model (figure 2.1) integrating these in relation to

the level of involvement/engagement from members of a network of public

speaking clubs that provide opportunities to practice and improve these skills.

This will add to the literature by integrating these constructs into a single model,

extending Dweck and Leggett’s framework. It should also provide a greater

understanding of the mechanisms by which adaptive characteristics such as

internal locus of control and higher self-efficacy are developed, and thus may

inform training and development practice.

Page 15: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

14

2. 0 Literature review

Dweck and Leggett (1988) propose that mindset directly influences whether

people prioritise learning or the demonstration of competence, and this influences

peoples’ response to challenges. Dweck and Leggett’s motivational framework

forms the basis for a hypothesised model (figure 2.1) but this study aims to go

beyond their conception: it proposes that locus of control will mediate the

relationship between mindset and goal orientation and directly influence self-

efficacy, which in turn will be positively associated with the amount of public

speaking practice participants engaged in. The literature review will follow the

broad structure provided by the hypothesised model and forms the overarching

framework. It begins with a discussion of mindset, followed by goal orientation

and their association, before considering locus of control and its role as mediator.

The final construct in the model – self-efficacy – is then discussed and the review

concludes with an overview of some existing models which have incorporated

some of these variables. Finally, this section ends with a discussion of the

dependent variables and aims of the study.

2.1 Mindset

Dweck (2000) draws parallels between mindset and Kelly’s (1955) personal

construct theory, asserting that a person’s beliefs about the fixedness or

malleability of personal characteristics and abilities provide a filter through which

Page 16: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

15

relevant information is processed. These beliefs about oneself – implicit self-

theories - form a framework which guide how we interpret and respond to the

outside world.

In a recent publication, Dweck (2006) uses the term mindset to describe the

holding of either the fixed or malleable view but it should be noted that it has

been variously termed implicit-self theory, implicit theory of ability and implicit

theory of personality in Dweck (2000) and across the literature. However, for

clarity, the term mindset is employed here throughout. Furthermore, the terms

entity and incremental – the two contrasting mindsets - are fairly consistent across

the literature and will be used here, in addition to fixed and malleable

respectively, where appropriate.

Dweck conceptualises mindset as a relatively stable, dispositional individual

difference construct (Dweck, 2000; Dweck, Chiu & Hong, 1995). It is also

proposed as being domain-specific, so that beliefs in the fixedness of intelligence

would not necessarily translate to a fixed conception of social skills. However,

this is not borne out by Spinath, Spinath, Riemann & Angleitner (2003) or

Kornilova, Kornilov & Chumakova, 2009) who report correlations between

domain-specific scales between .48 and .53, indicating that mindset in one

domain was actually reasonably predictive of mindset in another. Nonetheless,

the majority of studies have used Dweck’s implicit theory of intelligence (i.e.

intelligence-mindset) measure (e.g. Haimovitz, Wormington & Corpus, 2011;

Page 17: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

16

Mueller & Dweck, 1998; Robins & Pals, 2002; Roedel & Schraw, 1995) which is

unsurprising given the predominantly academic focus of research, although a

wider range of measures have often been employed in other domains (e.g. Beer,

2002; Burnette, 2010). However, Dweck (2000) asserts that where a task cuts

across domains it is appropriate to use the domain-general measure employed

here, as per Heslin, VandeWalle & Latham (2006). Furthermore, as this study is

investigating mindset in relation to other general dispositional measures (locus of

control and goal orientation), and is hypothesising that it is antecedent to them, a

more specific focus would not make theoretical sense.

Research has identified and supported a number of implications of mindset: from

an individual perspective, it has commonly been associated with task persistence

(Mueller & Dweck, 1998) and self-regulatory strategies (e.g. Mangels,

Butterfield, Lamb, Good and Dweck, 2006), both of which have consequences for

the development of one’s abilities. Interestingly, these appear to operate

independent of cognitive ability or confidence in ability (Dweck et al, 1995). It is

also perhaps noteworthy that mindset appears to be statistically independent of

constructs such as self-esteem (Dweck et al, 1995) and neuroticism (Spinath et al,

2003).

Incremental theorists, with their belief in the malleability of their characteristics,

are theorised to have higher control convictions (Ziegler & Stoeger, 2010). They

therefore tend to demonstrate greater task persistence relative to entity theorists

Page 18: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

17

(e.g. Nussbaum & Dweck, 2008) as well as willingness to spend time practicing

skills (Cury, Da Fonseca, Zahn & Elliot, 2008). It is via such actions that

incremental theorists exceed their entity counterparts in learning and performance

(e.g. Blackwell et al, 2007). In common with other social cognitive constructs,

these effects are theorised to exert their greatest influence in the face of

challenges and setbacks (Dweck, 2000). Mueller and Dweck’s (1998) studies

support this view, showing significant differences between the two mindsets in

response to task failures. However, as the present study is looking at dispositional

mindset it should be acknowledged that Mueller and Dweck’s research used an

intervention to induce an incremental or entity view. Nonetheless, while it is

unclear whether results would have differed if participants were grouped based on

dispositional mindset, this still highlights its impact as well as suggesting the

malleability of mindset itself.

Although Dweck’s construct is proposed to play a causal role in performance

outcomes (Dweck et al, 1995), it should be expected to stem from the contrasting

self-regulatory strategies employed by the two divergent mindsets. Mindset

should firstly influence their interpretation of events; in line with the theorised

links between personal construct theory and mindset, Molden and Dweck (2006)

assert that a person’s entity or incremental view of their abilities will influence

their perception of failures and setbacks. A fixed conception of personal

characteristics would theoretically see failure as reflecting their ability and thus

Page 19: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

18

direct attention towards managing the appearance of failure (Dweck, 2000); for

example, minimising the importance of the task and making self-serving

attributions (e.g. Kennett & Keefer, 2006) or reducing effort (Mueller & Dweck,

1998). Conversely, an incremental mindset is likely to view failure as providing

diagnostic information about their present level, as well as actions required to

improve (Molden & Dweck, 2006). This suggests mindset may influence the

allocation of attentional resources, a view underlined by Mangels et al (2006) and

cognitive neuroscience-based research. Their study looked at how people attend

– and respond - to feedback, which is a key element in learning (e.g. Ericsson,

Krampe & Tesch-Romer, 1993). Mangels and colleagues found an entity mindset

was related to lower levels of neural activity in regions of the brain associated

with feedback processing, when receiving corrective task-related feedback, than

incremental theorists. The implication being that people with an incremental

mindset paid more attention to feedback and were also more likely to utilise it.

While this certainly underpins its potential impact on learning strategies and

effort, there has been criticism for the high proportion of academic and

laboratory-based studies. Its generalisability and weaker results outside the

‘laboratory’ have also been raised (e.g. Bråten and Strømsø, 2004). However,

numerous studies have demonstrated its applicability, linking an incremental

mindset to adaptive self-regulatory strategies such as feedback seeking behaviour,

in the workplace (Heslin and Latham, 2004) and linking an entity mindset with

avoidant coping strategies in weight management (Burnette, 2010). Mindset has

Page 20: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

19

also been associated with managers’ willingness to invest time in coaching and

supporting subordinates (Heslin et al, 2006; Van Vianen, Dalhoeven & De Pater,

2011). Furthermore, Beer’s (2002) suite of studies clearly demonstrate the impact

of mindset in relation to shyness, finding that incremental theorists (i.e. those with

an incremental mindset) with equivalent levels of shyness to a group of entity

theorists, were less likely to use avoidant strategies in a challenging social

situation. This is one of the few studies looking at these types of interaction and

appears particularly relevant to the present study, as fears over public speaking

are common and are related to widely-held social-evaluative concerns (Essau,

Conradt & Petermann, 1999). These avoidance and related strategies mentioned

in Beer’s research are indicative of particular goal orientations – dispositional

individual difference constructs which have been theorised as emerging from the

two opposing mindsets (Dweck & Leggett, 1988).

2.2 Goal orientation

Although a range of models and measures have derived from goal orientation

research (e.g. Button, Mathieu & Zajac, 1996; Elliot & McGregor, 2001;

VandeWalle, 1997) these can broadly be categorised as either learning or

performance goal orientations. Learning goal orientations are concerned with

improving ability, whereas performance orientations focus on task performance

and comparison with others (Fisher & Ford, 1998; Nicholls, 1984). Dweck and

Leggett’s (1988) model links entity beliefs with a tendency towards performance

Page 21: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

20

goal orientations while those with an incremental belief are likely have learning

goal orientations (Blackwell et al, 2007; Stipek & Gralinski, 1996).

Unsurprisingly, given the ‘benefits’ of an incremental mindset, the literature has

largely highlighted the advantages of learning over performance goal orientations

(e.g. Cannon-Bowers, Rhodenizer, Salas & Bowers, 1998; Fisher & Ford, 1998;

Utman, 1997).

The various researchers looking at goal orientation have employed an array of

labels to refer to similar concepts; so learning goal orientation also appears as

mastery, approach, and task orientations, while, performance goals have also

been branded as ego, avoidance and achievement orientations, among others.

However, for clarity, this study will use the terms learning (LGO) and

performance goal orientations (PGO) to refer to these conceptually similar but

variously termed constructs.

Like mindset, LGO has been positively associated with adaptive strategies (e.g.

Ames & Archer, 1988; VandeWalle & Cummings, 1997), which given the

proposed links between them should be expected, and it is via these strategies that

LGO is similarly associated with performance outcomes (e.g. Midgeley,

Middleton & Kaplan, 2001). Meanwhile, PGO is theorised to result in shallower,

less sophisticated learning strategies (Seifert, 1995), such as the reduced

attentional resources allocated to feedback in Mangels et al (2006). Furthermore,

PGO’s define success in relation to others, thus appearing less controllable than

self-referent goals and have been theorised to represent a threat to ego (Nicholls,

Page 22: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

21

1984). Dweck and Leggett propose that for individuals with low confidence in

ability, this leads to a tendency to engage in maladaptive strategies such as task

withdrawal due to lack of belief in the worth of effort (Utman, 1997).

Conversely, Stevens and Gist’s (1997) research suggests that LGO is effective in

learning situations precisely because it mitigates these judgments and subsequent

avoidance behaviours.

2.2.1 Mindset and goal orientations

The model which links these two constructs (Dweck & Leggett, 1988) has proven

popular and there has been a fair degree of support (e.g. Ames & Archer, 1988;

Miller, Behrens & Greene, 1993; Martocchio, 1994). Robins and Pals (2002)

indicate that entity or incremental perspectives played important roles in

performance and self-esteem respectively, but did so via the learning orientations

that emerged from these (Molden & Dweck, 2006). Meanwhile, Blackwell et al’s

(2007) longitudinal study indicated that students with an entity mindset were

more likely to ascribe failures to lack of ability, than incremental theorists. With

a conception of abilities as fixed and stable entities, individuals are more likely

view a situation as a fait accompli, leading to task helplessness and responses

such as withdrawal in the face of setbacks (Duda, 2001). This is characteristic of

an avoidance goal orientation which has been proposed as a key dimension of

PGO (VandeWalle, 1997). However, despite the theoretical support for the

relationship between mindset and goal orientations - alongside the deleterious

Page 23: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

22

impact of PGO and advantages of LGO - the evidence is not unequivocal and the

hypothesised model does not include a direct path between them.

A key aspect of this research relates to the relationship between these two

constructs so it is important to acknowledge and justify the exclusion of such a

path. On the basis of the clear conceptual links between mindset and goal

orientation theory (Hafsteinsson, Donovan & Breland, 2007) a consistently strong

relationship might be expected between them. Yet, although studies have

generally supported the direction of the relationships (e.g. VandeWalle, 1997),

they have often been relatively weak (e.g. Bråten & Strømsø, 2004; Kennett &

Keefer, 2006; Payne et al, 2007). There are three potential explanations here;

firstly, that mindset simply has little explanatory value in relation to goal

orientations – while this is possible, the theoretical links discussed previously

appear compelling and suggest otherwise. Alternatively, there may be issues with

its measurement. Dupeyrat and Mariné (2005) - in common with numerous

others (e.g. Bråten and Strømsø, 2005) - treat mindset as a two-factor construct

(i.e. entity and incremental), with mixed results. However, mindset was

originally conceptualised as a unidimensional construct with high scores

representing a tendency towards an incremental perspective and low scores

representing the opposite. Subsequent validation research lends clear support to

this view (e.g. Hong et al, 1999). Although Dweck (2000) asserts that differing

mindsets may be held in relation to different domains, there is no real logic to the

view that one can hold both an entity and incremental mindset regarding the same

Page 24: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

23

task concurrently. Therefore, treatment of mindset as a two-factor construct

appears conceptually confusing and may be clouding real relationships.

Skinner (1996) asserts that inconsistent results and conclusions are likely when

potentially different constructs are described by the same terms, therefore similar

issues may also stem from the sheer number of ways that goal orientation has

been conceptualised and measured (Grant & Dweck, 2003). Yet despite this

growing number of measures (e.g. Button et al, 1996; Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996;

VandeWalle, 1997), Carr, DeShon and Dobbins note that around 75% of the 27

studies they reviewed did not use established measures, making comparison and

assimilation of research problematic, while reliabilities are also frequently poor

(e.g. Dupeyrat & Mariné, 2005). However, goal orientation has been an

increasingly researched construct (e.g. Hafsteinsson et al, 2007; Payne et al, 2007;

VandeWalle, 1997) with sound theoretical underpinnings.

The rationale for the relationships between these constructs is sound, but not fully

borne out by research so this may be related to the varying conceptualisations and

measurement issues described. However, while this may be a factor, these

significant but weak relationships are relatively consistent across studies and an

alternative explanation is that previous theory and research has omitted a key

variable from their models. After all, mindset is a belief that it is possible to

change one’s ability/characteristic and although it has clear control-related aspects

Page 25: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

24

it does not necessarily indicate beliefs about one’s capacity to do so. And this

may be where Locus of Control can bridge the gap between mindset and goal

orientations.

2.3 Locus of control

The central tenet of mindset, regarding the belief an individual can or cannot

change their characteristics with effort and strategy, infers a sense of control; it

should therefore be expected to have a significant relationship with locus of

control (LoC). Rotter (1966) originally differentiated between people with an

internal and external LoC; ‘internals’ believe they are in control of their own fate

and are therefore more likely to take necessary steps to control it, while

‘externals’ tend to believe they are at the mercy of the environment and that

external factors are responsible (Ng, Sorensen & Eby, 2006). Although there

have been numerous subsequent models with differing factor structures (e.g.

Lefcourt, 1991; Levenson, 1981), it is the broad internal/external dichotomy that

is the focus here.

Perceived control has implications for how people respond to stressful situations

(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and it has been shown to reduce the effects of

potentially threatening situations into nonthreatening (Sanderson, Rapee and

Barlow, 1989). Considering the fear that is so commonly held in relation to

public speaking (Stein, Walker, & Forde, 1996) this is highly pertinent to the

present study. Externality has been linked with stressful situational appraisals

Page 26: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

25

which can lead to the helplessness and maladaptive responses characteristic of

avoidance goal orientations (Noe, 1988). Meanwhile internal control beliefs are

associated with LGO’s (Phillips & Gully, 1997) which do not involve the same

deleterious normative evaluations that activities such as public speaking may

invoke (e.g. DiBartolo, Frost, Dixon & Almodovar, 2001).

People who believe that outcomes are determined by their own actions are also

more proactive than those with external perspectives (Bandura, 1997). On this

basis, it might also be anticipated that an internal LoC would be associated with

performance outcomes (e.g. Judge & Bono, 2001). Nonetheless, direct

relationships between LoC and behavioural outcomes have been weaker than this

view would suggest (e.g. Biddle, 1999; Smith, 1989; Tett, Jackson & Rothstein,

1991) and LoC actually exerts its effects via self-regulatory constructs such as

goal orientation, as well as self-efficacy (e.g. Ng et al, 2006; Phillips & Gully,

1997). In support of this, Skinner (1996) proposes a tripartite model of control

whereby our general beliefs about the changeability of a situation (i.e. mindset)

are mediated by beliefs about our level of influence upon it (i.e. LoC) and this in

turn impacts our self-regulatory strategies (i.e. goal orientations). Although

Skinner’s framework is necessarily general and not intended as a testable model,

it provides a useful way of organising these constructs and indicating their broad

relationships.

Page 27: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

26

2.3.1 Locus of control, mindset and goal orientations: mediation?

However, in relation to the present study, there is scant research which has

considered LoC and mindset together. Dweck and Leggett (1988), along with

Seligman (1975), suggests that when faced with ‘failure’, a fixed conception of

one’s ability is associated with a lack of control – and related attributions -

resulting in ‘helpless’ rather than ‘mastery’ responses. This is characterised by

the belief in the futility of further effort and avoidance strategies (Elliot & Dweck,

1988), which have parallels with avoidance goal orientations (AGO). So,

controllability is considered by Dweck and Leggett but does not appear as a

discrete construct in their model.

However, Garofano and Salas (2005) appear to suggest that LoC and mindset may

be treated interchangeably, which is understandable given the control-related

parallels between them (Skinner, 1996). Nonetheless, despite the dearth of

research including both constructs, Dweck and colleagues’ (1995) validation

study does indicate only a small, but significant, correlation (r = .15; p < .01)

between the internality subscale of Levenson’s (1981) LoC scale and a domain-

specific measure of mindset. This is echoed by Burnette (2010) with Wallston

and Wallston’s (1978) Health LoC scale (r = .14; p <.05). It is plausible they

would be related, but contrary to Garofano and Salas, it is contended here that

they are not the same and that a belief that one’s abilities can be changed is not

enough. It must then be followed by the conviction that one has the capacity to

exercise that change, as per Skinner (1996) and Bandura (1977), which is where

Page 28: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

27

LoC and the self-efficacy come in. So it is asserted that an incremental mindset

should serve as a necessary - but not sufficient - precondition for these beliefs. So

this would indicate mindset as being antecedent to LoC.

The second element of the proposed mediating relationship concerns the

relationship between LoC and goal orientations. Control beliefs, such as LoC are

clearly influential in the formation of goal orientations; passive or avoidant

coping strategies, associated with PGO’s, are prompted by a general perception

that one is powerless to control outcomes (Seligman, 1975). Meanwhile a belief

that a person has control over what happens to them produces more proactive and

adaptive responses (Luthar, 1991), and has similarities with the previously

discussed constructs. According to this, an internal or external LoC would be

linked to LGO or AGO respectively. So, although LoC has often demonstrated

weaker than expected relationships with distal outcomes (Biddle, 1999), it may

simply be that LoC is mediated by more proximal constructs such as goal

orientations and self-efficacy. However, Bandura (1997) asserts that people who

see outcomes as being personally determined – analogous to an internal LoC – yet

lacking requisite expertise will have low self-efficacy and view tasks with “a

sense of futility”. Equally, Averill (1973) asserts that the mere conviction that

control is available is sufficient to encourage action. Yet these views are

somewhat simplistic and fail to acknowledge the effect of goal orientations, with

LGO shown to ameliorate the deleterious impact of low self-efficacy, while AGO

Page 29: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

28

may mitigate the benefits of ability. For example, Stevens & Gist (1997) show

that low self-efficacy is only problematic when accompanied by a performance

goal orientation. Furthermore, there would be little point in setting challenging

goals if an individual believes that results are due to factors beyond their control.

So the hypothesised model proposes that LoC will mediate the effects of mindset

on goal orientations, while also having a direct effect on self-efficacy.

2.4 Self-efficacy

Self-efficacy has been defined as the ‘beliefs in one’s capabilities to organise and

execute the courses of action required to produce given attainments (Bandura,

1997, p.3). The key role of self-efficacy in performance has been widely asserted

(e.g. Bandura, 1986, 1997; Bandura & Locke, 2003; Bouffard-Bouchard, Parent

& Larivee, 1991; Zimmerman, Bandura & Martinez-Pons, 2003) with low self-

efficacy shown to have a detrimental impact on functioning and task performance

(Bouffard et al, 2005). Furthermore, high self-efficacy has been shown as one of

the strongest predictors of goals and behaviour (Plotnikoff, Lippke, Cournega,

Birkett and Sigal, 2008), being associated with proactivity in seeking

opportunities to practice recently learned skills (Hill, Smith & Mann, 1987). It

has also been linked to a willingness to apply nascent skills outside of the training

environment and the likelihood of trying out difficult tasks (Quinones, Ford, Sego

& Smith, 1996). It is therefore expected that self-efficacy should be related to

levels of practice in the present study

Page 30: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

29

Although self-efficacy appears to be viewed in a way that simply suggests that

self-efficacy per se is beneficial, Bandura (2012) differentiates between ‘easy’

and ‘resilient’ self-efficacy. The former gained via easy successes and the latter

by learning how to manage failure so it is informative rather than disheartening.

It is therefore contended that an incremental mindset, internal LoC and learning

goal orientation, with their links to adaptive strategies and personal agency, would

provide fertile ground for the development of this ‘resilient’ brand of self-

efficacy. With the opposite being true for an entity mindset, external LoC and

AGO.

2.4.1 Locus of control and self-efficacy

It is affirmed by a number of sources that self-efficacy can influence effort and

persistence (e.g. Bandura, 1986; Ormrod, 2006; Zimmerman, 2000) and

individuals with an internal LoC are likely to display higher self-efficacy because

of their belief in their ability to exercise control over tasks (Ng et al, 2006).

Nonetheless, Bandura (2006) affirms that self-efficacy - a judgment of capability

- should be differentiated from LoC, which is a belief about whether outcomes are

determined by our own, or external factors. LoC has been often associated with

self-efficacy (e.g. Ng et al, 2006; Skinner, 1996), while Phillips & Gully’s (1997)

research indicated a direct causal path to self-efficacy, though not to the extent

that Averill’s (1973) assertion would indicate. It is unsurprising that LoC and

Page 31: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

30

self-efficacy should be linked, given they can each be considered under the rubric

‘control’ (Thompson & Spacapan, 1991) and have been proposed as part of the

superordinate construct of Core Self-Evaluation (CSE; Judge et al, 2002).

However, Judge and colleagues employ a general measure of self-efficacy, which

is considered questionable given its domain specificity (Ajzjen & Fishbeing,

2005), while Ajzen (2002) also asserts the substantial empirical evidence

regarding the distinctiveness of LoC and self-efficacy.

2.4.2 Mindset, goal orientations and self-efficacy

Furthermore, based on research linking learning (and avoidance) goal orientations

positively (and negatively) with self-efficacy (Hsieh, Sullivan & Guerra, 2007)

and similarly between mindset and self-efficacy (e.g. Martocchio, 1994), a case

could be made for the inclusion of such paths in the hypothesised model (figure

2.1) as do Phillips and Gully (1997). They assert that goal orientations are

predictive of self-efficacy because they influence how failures and setbacks are

interpreted and acted upon, although their model indicated relatively modest

relationships between self-efficacy and LGO (β = .13; p <. 05) as well as PGO (β

= -.14; p < .01). However, it is the response the orientation generates rather than

the orientation itself that should influence self-efficacy, which in the case of the

present study would be the amount of cumulative practice (i.e. length of

membership) – and it is expected that this would mediate the effect of goal

orientations on self-efficacy. It is noted that numerous previous studies have

Page 32: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

31

indicated direct effects of goal orientations on self-efficacy (e.g. Hastings &

West, 2011; Kozlowski, Gully, Brown, Salas, Smith and Nason, 2001; Leroy,

Bressoux, Sarrazin & Trouilloud, 2007; Phillips & Gully, 1997).

However, Bandura’s own causal process model emphasises the complex two-way

nature of self-efficacy and outcomes (Bandura, 1997; Bandura & Wood, 1989;

Wood & Bandura, 1989) whereby past performance influences efficacy beliefs

and self-regulatory strategies. These in turn influence task performance, thus

modifying efficacy beliefs and subsequent strategies. This feedback loop is

repeated each time, because, although high self-efficacy has been asserted as a

crucial causal factor learning and practice (e.g. Zimmerman, 2000), it is also an

outcome of it. This view supports the hypothesis that longer-term membership at

a public speaking club (and therefore more practice) would be positively

associated with self-efficacy, while self-efficacy would be positively related to the

amount of recent practice undertaken.

Models based on cross-sectional methodology omitting indicators of self-

regulatory strategies do not appear to account for any of the mechanisms by

which self-efficacy is actually developed. For example, enactive mastery

experiences - the strongest source of self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997) - are likely to

occur because of self-regulatory strategies such as willingness to practice and

persistence; such strategies and responses, according to goal orientation theory are

Page 33: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

32

less likely when one has ability and competence-related concerns (Bandura, 1997)

that are characteristic of avoidance goal orientations.

Although self-efficacy is more complex and nuanced than just described (see

Bandura, 1997), this should highlight that previous cross-sectional research

treating goal orientations as having a direct effect appears flawed on that basis.

The same rationale could be theorised regarding mindset and it too has been

treated similarly in a number of studies (e.g. Martocchio, 1994). So while

mindset and goal orientations should provide good foundations for the

development of self-efficacy – and therefore should be correlated - their influence

should not be direct. These should influence self-efficacy via the selection of

self-regulatory strategies, which lead to more effective learning via mechanisms

such as ‘production’ and ‘reproduction’ of the desired skills (i.e. practice) and

response to feedback (Bandura, 1977). In common with many previous studies,

the present research is also cross-sectional and it is for this very reason that the

hypothesised model does not include a direct path to self-efficacy from either goal

orientation or mindset.

2.5 Overview and hypothesised model

Dweck and Leggett’s (1988) model, proposing the direct links between the two

divergent mindsets and goal orientations, has proved a useful and popular one for

researchers, but it is argued here that it should be extended to incorporate LoC in

a mediating role. Payne and colleagues’ (2007) meta-analysis includes self-

Page 34: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

33

efficacy as well as [intelligence-related] mindset within their meta-analysis of

goal orientations. Their outline broadly follows the temporal order of this model,

with mindset and dispositional goal orientation being antecedent to task-specific

self-efficacy. Nonetheless, while Payne et al include a wide range of personality

and individual difference constructs in their analysis, they do not include LoC and

end with a call for future research to incorporate it. Similarly, Phillips and Gully

(1997) include LoC, goal orientations and self-efficacy, with only mindset absent

from their model; however they consider LoC and goal orientations as having

independent direct effects on self-efficacy which has already been queried.

Nonetheless, these and other models provide useful frameworks, but none

consider all of the constructs investigated here.

The research outlined throughout argues that mindset, locus of control and goal

orientations provide a platform for the development of self-regulatory strategies,

self-efficacy and subsequent behavioural responses and performance outcomes.

Therefore, the aim of this study is to investigate whether the hypothesised model

is predictive of practice levels and continued commitment to the learning of

public speaking skills. This is considered a particularly relevant context given the

effects of these constructs are most salient in the face of challenges, as public

speaking has been commonly identified as a fearful situation (Dwyer & Davidson,

2012). Furthermore, participation in the public speaking clubs studied here also

incorporates regular performance feedback, the response to which is strongly

influenced by factors such as goal orientations and self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997;

Page 35: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

34

Brett & VandeWalle, 1999). Therefore, the constructs included here should have

useful explanatory value in this context and be related to the amount of practice

people engage in, as well as their long-term involvement at their public speaking

club.

The hypothesised relationships between the variables in the model are shown in

figure 2.1, with hypothesised positive and negative relationships being indicated

by (+) and (-) symbols. In summary, the hypothesised model proposes that an

incremental mindset would support an internal locus of control, which in turn

would lead to the pursuit of learning – as opposed to avoidance – goals. It is

further theorised that learning goal orientations promote development of self-

efficacy via the self-regulatory strategies they facilitate (Bandura, 1997). Goal

orientations, as well as self-efficacy, should therefore be predictive of level of

public speaking practice, while goal orientations would also be expected to be

related to length of membership at their public speaking club, which could be

considered as being related to persistence. Additionally, it is also contended that

length of membership would naturally be associated with higher public speaking

self-efficacy due to the greater amount of practice that longer-term membership is

likely to provide.

Page 36: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

35

Figure 2.1: Hypothesised model and direction of relationships

Note: LoC = locus of control; AGO = avoidance goal orientation; LGO = learning goal orientation; Memb. = length of public speaking club membership; PSSE = public speaking self-efficacy; Practice = amount of public speaking practice

2.6 Study aims

In the introduction, it was asserted that research had investigated relationships

between mindset and some of these established constructs, but not as an

integrated whole. One of the main aims of the study is to address this and

respond to calls for greater integration of mindset with social cognitive theory.

An additional key aim is to further integrate Dweck and Leggett’s motivational

framework within this and test an extension of this by including a proposed

mediator; this has not previously been proposed or tested previously. Based on

the research and theory presented here, this could play a valuable role in

Mindset

LGO

AGO

LoC PSSE

Memb.

Practice+

_

+

+ +

+

_

_

+

+

Page 37: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

36

explaining previously modest results and inform future research using their

framework. Therefore, in addition to the hypothesised model, an alternative non-

mediated model where both LoC and mindset have direct effects on goal

orientations is also tested to allow comparison. Finally, as the majority of

research has been conducted in relation to educational establishments or

workplaces, this will also add to the knowledge regarding how these constructs

operate together, in relation to a voluntary undertaking, where individuals have

more discretion to engage in their chosen behaviours.

Page 38: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

37

3. 0 Method

3.1 Study design

The study was conducted using a cross-sectional survey design and the target

population were members of an organisation that oversees a network of public

speaking clubs. The independent variables were scores on measures of mindset,

LoC, AGO, LGO and Public-speaking self-efficacy (PSSE), while the two

dependent variables were the amount of public speaking practice (practice) and

length of membership (membership); these measures are discussed in section 3.3.

3.2 Sample

Demographic details are included in table 3.1 and the final sample comprised 161

participants (96 females and 65 males) between the ages of 18 and 80 (M = 37.9).

The majority were in full-time employment (56.5%), with 13% being self-

employed and 13% students. It was also noted that a large proportion of the

sample were university educated (75.1%), with the majority of these educated to

postgraduate level.

Page 39: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

38

Table 3.1: Participant demographic details

Demographic variable Category

Frequency (total n = 161)

% of sample

Gender Female 96 59.6% Male 65 40.4%

Age

Under 20 years old 2 1.2% 20 – 29 years 42 26.1% 30 – 39 years 66 41.0% 40 – 49 years 23 14.3% 50 – 59 years 13 8.1% 60 – 69 years 12 7.5% 70 – 79 years 2 1.2% 80+ years 1 0.6%

M = 37.89; SD = 12.86; range = 62 (18 – 80)

Employment status

Full time 91 56.5%

Part time 13 8.1%

Self-employed 21 13.0%

Student 21 13.0%

Unemployed 5 3.1%

Retired 6 3.7%

Missing data 4 2.5%

Occupational level

Not currently employed 32 19.9%

Admin 12 7.5%

Service 13 8.1%

Semi-skilled 2 1.2%

Skilled 12 7.5%

Junior mgmt. 27 16.8%

Senior mgmt. 57 35.4%

Missing data 6 3.7%

Educational Level

No qualifications 0 0%

Secondary school to age 16 7 4.3%

Secondary school to age 18 4 2.5%

Non-university higher education 25 15.5%

Undergraduate university education 49 30.4%

Postgraduate university education 72 44.7%

Missing data 4 2.5%

Page 40: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

39

3.3 Data collection instruments

Data collection was conducted via a questionnaire (Appendix A), which in

addition to the demographic information described previously, comprised the

measures described below. Unless stated otherwise, all scales were measured

using 7-point Likert scales, ranging from one (strongly disagree) to seven

(strongly agree). Reliabilities of scales in the present study were all satisfactory,

ranging from .69 to .93 and are presented in table 4.2 in the results section.

3.3.1 Mindset

Mindset was assessed using an eight-item questionnaire developed by Dweck

(2000), which includes four items where agreement indicates an incremental

mindset and four items indicating an entity mindset. Incremental items include

“You can always substantially change the kind of person you are”, while entity

items include “You can do things differently, but the important parts of who you

are can’t really be changed”. Previous research has demonstrated reliabilities in

the range of .78 (Sue-Chan and Wood, 2009) and .92 (Heslin et al, 2006). As

previously mentioned, this scale is selected in preference to domain-specific

measures of mindset (e.g. intelligence) as recommended by Dweck (2000)

because the skills used by members were thought to cut across domains, involving

speech-preparation, organisational skills as well as public-speaking itself.

Page 41: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

40

The unidimensionality of mindset is asserted by Dweck and demonstrated by

Hong et al (1999) and Levy, Stroessner and Dweck (1998), finding that

agreement with entity items represents disagreement with incremental items and

vice versa. However, Dweck and colleagues (e.g. Dweck et al, 1995) have

previously used artificial dichotomisation to reduce continuous level data into

dichotomous entity and incremental variables. This practice has been criticised

(e.g. Cohen, 1983, MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher & Rucker, 2002) for reducing

statistical power and therefore entity mindset items (1, 2, 4 & 6) were reverse-

scored so that on all items, higher scores reflected an incremental belief and lower

scores indicated entity beliefs, as per Heslin et al (2006).

3.3.2 Locus of control

The eight-item ‘internality’ subscale of Levenson’s (1981) measure was used to

assess Locus of Control. This gauges the degree to which people perceive

themselves as being in control of their own lives, versus the belief that external

factors are responsible (Oreg, 2003). This scale includes items such as “When I

make plans, I am almost certain to make them work” and “When I get what I

want, it’s usually because I worked hard for it”. No items are negatively worded

and on all items, higher scores indicate stronger internality perceptions. Levenson

(1974) reports relatively modest reliability (α = .64), but it is selected in

preference to other scales (e.g. Rotter, 1966) as it is superior to scales such as

Rotter (1966) in terms of social desirability effects (Lefcourt, 1991), while from a

practical perspective, with fewer items, it required less of participants time.

Page 42: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

41

3.3.3 Goal orientation

This was assessed using the learning and avoidance goal orientation subscales of

VandeWalle’s (1997) goal orientation scale; five items related to LGO (e.g. “I

enjoy challenging and difficult tasks where I’ll learn new skills”) and four items

related to the avoidance orientation (AGO) (e.g. “I would avoid taking on a new

task if there was a chance I would appear incompetent to others”. Reliabilities

for the learning and avoid subscales have been shown at .89 and .88 respectively

(VandeWalle, 1997). For all items, higher scores represented a stronger

inclination towards the relevant factor. It should be noted that the VandeWalle’s

(1997) scale includes an additional dimension ‘prove goal orientation’ – which is

concerned with the demonstration of competence, rather than avoidance of

appearing incompetent characterised by AGO. However, this orientation has

been shown to have consistently weak or inconsistent relationships (Hafsteinsson

et al, 2007; VandeWalle, Brown, Cron & Slocum, 1999). Therefore, as this

model included a number of relatively untested relationships, it was considered

appropriate to select only the two conceptually clearer orientations, AGO

(avoidance goal orientation) and LGO (Brophy, 2004).

Page 43: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

42

3.3.4 Public speaking self-efficacy

This was assessed using Schuuhrman, Alley, Marshall and Johnstone’s (2008)

Public Speaking Self-Efficacy scale; this comprises ten items measuring different

aspects involved in public speaking. Respondents are asked to indicate their level

of confidence on a seven-point scale (1 = “not at all confident”; 7 = “completely

confident”) in relation to statements such as “When public speaking I am [level of

confidence] that I can…connect well to an audience that is not familiar with the

topic”. Schuurman and colleagues report a reliability of .90 & .89 based on two

samples (n = 773; n = 279) and all items are scored so higher scores indicate

greater levels of public speaking self-efficacy.

3.3.5 Public speaking measures

The dependent variables are based on membership of a network of public

speaking clubs. Clubs hold meetings every fortnight and provide a number of

public speaking opportunities; meetings include designated slots for members to

deliver prepared speeches on topics of their choosing, as well as a segment set

aside for impromptu speaking. In addition, all speakers are provided with

feedback and meetings include a number of other roles which involve speaking in

front of the audience, providing further opportunities to develop skills, such as

giving feedback or presiding over meetings. The two dependent variables are

described below.

Page 44: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

43

Public speaking practice (practice)

The amount of public speaking practice undertaken by members was assessed by

asking respondents to indicate the number of times they had taken on speaking

roles at their club in the last six months. All meetings follow a standard format

which includes a set number of these roles which members can volunteer for, in

order to practice their public speaking. There are eight possible roles (e.g.

prepared speaker, speech evaluator, general evaluator) and participants indicated

how many of each of these roles they had undertaken in the previous six months.

These were summed for each participant to form a composite total ‘practice’

score.

It is acknowledged that this measure does rely on people’s recall and is why a six

month period was considered – to provide a long enough period to allow for short

spells of attendance/inactivity yet also short enough that people could recall their

involvement. Accurate, objective measures would have been preferred, but

official records were not available for confidentiality reasons. However, it was

considered at least an objective, rather than perceived, measure of practice.

Length of membership (membership)

Length of membership was intended to reflect a longer-term persistence or

commitment to improving public speaking skills. Participants were asked to

provide the number of years and months they had been a member of the public

speaking network and a composite ‘membership’ (in months) variable was

Page 45: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

44

computed by multiplying the number of years by 12 then adding the number of

months.

3.4 Procedure

Data collection was predominantly via an online questionnaire and after

contacting the area governor responsible for clubs in the north of England and

Scotland, these were to be disseminated to all members in this area via internal

club e-mail channels. As it later transpired this was not possible, the

questionnaire was instead circulated using snowball sampling via the researchers

own networks within the clubs; a paper and pencil version of the questionnaire

was also disseminated at clubs in the Greater Manchester area by the researcher.

Furthermore, the link to the online questionnaire was also posted on club- and

public-speaking -related web-based social networks such as Facebook, LinkedIn

and Twitter to promote it to a wider range of available participants.

When participants followed the online link to the questionnaire (appendix A) they

were presented with information about the study and its voluntary nature to

ensure informed consent. No personally identifiable information was requested,

so anonymity was assured. The questionnaire comprised the measures described

previously in this section. Although the questionnaire was targeted at club

members, as it was also disseminated online via various social networks it was

possible that non-members could also complete the questionnaire. To reduce the

possibility of non-members answering questions specific to membership

Page 46: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

45

involvement (i.e. practice and period of membership), the questionnaire included

a question asking if the respondent was a member of the public speaking clubs,

and a ‘no’ response lead to this set of questions being skipped*. Aggregated data

was then uploaded into SPSS for preparation and initial analysis, details of which

are provided in the results section.

*Is should be noted, that a technical issue with the online data collection

instrument meant that a number of participating club members indicated that they

had been unable to input information in the section regarding club membership

and participation (i.e. the dependent variables). It is not thought this affected a

large number of participants, but it is unclear and is discussed further in section

4.3.1.

Page 47: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

46

4.0 Results

Data from the total sample of 161 were initially screened to assess missing values

and data normality. The subsequent analyses followed Anderson and Gerbin’s

(1988) two-step process, whereby confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) is used to

develop a measurement model incorporating mindset, locus of control, AGO,

LGO and PSSE, allowing factor loadings and overall fit of the model to the data

to be tested. In the second stage, structural equation modelling (SEM) enables the

testing of the hypothesised relationships between the constructs. This section

begins by discussing the initial screening of data, before describing the CFA and

SEM analyses.

4.1 Preliminary analyses

Firstly, data were prepared using SPSS 16; the four negatively-phrased mindset

items were reverse scored and composite scores computed for ‘length of

membership’ and ‘total practice’ as described in the previous section.

Prior to analysis, two cases were identified as missing a single value (items LoC1

& LoC7); this comprised only .03% of the available data and these were replaced

via single imputation using the expectation-maximisation algorithm. A second

more relevant missing data issue concerns a high proportion of missing data on

the dependent variables and the strategy for dealing with this is discussed in the

SEM section (4.3.1).

Page 48: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

47

Although boxplots highlighted a number of outliers, only two extreme outliers (z

> 3.29) were identified; one on the ‘practice’ variable (z = 4.64) and one on

‘membership’ (z = 3.50). While these did not indicate data entry errors, it was not

clear whether the ‘practice’ outlier in particular was spurious; based on the

researcher’s knowledge of the clubs this was considered to represent an extremely

high level; it also appeared particularly problematic to the degree of kurtosis.

Although the removal or transformation of outliers has been questioned (e.g.

Cortina & Gully, 1999) it has also been commonly recommended to maximise

data normality (e.g. Tabachnick & Fiddel, 2007; Wilcox, 1997), which was

considered important given the assumptions made by SEM, but more so in light

of the missing data and the impact kurtosis in particular may have when dealing

with it (Enders, 2001b). Therefore, to reduce - but not ignore - these cases, they

were transformed by taking the next highest variable score plus one unit, as

specified by Tabachnick and Fiddel (2007), which reduced kurtosis from 5.37 to

1.41. However, additional analyses using the untransformed data were conducted

and are reported alongside the statistics for the final tested models (table 4.4).

Subsequently, skew for all variables ranged from 1.81 to -1.33, while kurtosis

ranged from 2.70 to -1.13, and were within acceptable limits for SEM (Lei &

Lomax, 2005).

Page 49: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

48

4.2 Confirmatory factor analysis

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted initially to assess the

individual scales used, before testing of the final measurement model and

hypothesised structural models. Measurement and structural models (SEM) were

tested using AMOS Graphics (version 16.0.1), using Maximum Likelihood

estimation (ML). ML assumes multivariate normality and continuous level data,

which was not strictly met by the 7-point response scales used here (Bollen,

1989). However, ML has been supported even when strict multivariate

assumptions are not met (Iacobucci, 2009; 2010; Olsson, Foss, Troye & Howell,

2000). Moreover, item parcelling was employed to provide a closer

approximation of continuous level data (e.g. Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1996) and

improve indicator-to-factor ratios, which, at higher levels have been linked with

increased sampling errors and biased estimates of fit (e.g. Nasser & Wisenbaker,

2003).

To assess the adequacy of the models tested, it is widely recommended that

multiple statistics are employed (Hu & Bentler, 1990; Kaplan, 2000). In line with

this, and simulations assessing performance of fit indices with missing data (e.g.

Davey, Savla & Luo, 2005), the following are selected here: Chi-squared (χ2),

Root-Mean-Square-Error-of-Approximation (RMSEA); Comparative-Fit Index

(CFI) and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI). Schreiber, Amaury, Stage, Barlow and

King (2006) affirm that the CFI, TLI and RMSEA are suitable for the level of

Page 50: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

49

data used here while the standardised-root-mean square residual (SRMR) has

been recommended with ML (Hu and Bentler (1998).

To evaluate the fit between model and data, RMSEA statistics of ≤ .08 are

considered reasonable, with figures above .10 representing a poor fit (Hu &

Bentler, 1999). However, as the RMSEA has a tendency to over-reject models

with smaller samples (Byrne, 2010; Hu & Bentler, 1999), confidence intervals are

also reported here to indicate the precision of estimates. Furthermore, when the

χ2/df ratio is below two, this is generally suggestive of good fit (Ullman, 2006).

Hu and Bentler also suggest cut-off levels of ≤.08 for the SRMR with values of ≥

.95 for the CFI and TLI considered good (Hu & Bentler, 1999). However, it

should be noted that AMOS cannot produce the SRMR statistic when there is

missing data, as is the case with the dependent variables (discussed in section

4.3.1), so this is reported for the measurement model, but not available for the

structural portion of the analysis. Finally, for the testing of the structural models,

the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) is used to assess comparative model fit.

The AIC has been identified as an effective measure for comparing non-nested

models (Kline, 2005), with the lowest AIC representing the more accurate model

(Kuha, 2004).

4.2.1 Item Parcels

Confirmatory factor analysis was first conducted on individual scales using all

items to assess item loadings and inform the development of parcels. Fit statistics

Page 51: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

50

are presented in table 4.1 and show that none met all of the cut-off criteria, while

details of items and factor loadings are included in appendix C. Additionally,

three items on LoC (LoC1, 2 & 4) had poor loadings (.11, .30 and .34

respectively) and were removed prior to parcelling as they were not considered to

adequately represent the factor they were set to load onto.

It should also be noted that the mindset scale indicated particularly poor fit. It has

been clearly asserted as unidimensional (Hong et al, 1999), yet the poor fit and

modification indices showing correlated errors predominantly between entity-

worded mindset items (1, 2, 4 & 6) suggested a two-factor model. The fit for the

two-factor solution, with the four entity and incrementally worded items set to

load on separate factors showed substantially better fit (table 4.1). However, a

two-factor solution is theoretically unsatisfactory as already discussed and the

factors were highly correlated (r = .80). Therefore, in view of the generally good

factor loadings for the single factor CFA, items are parcelled on the basis of these

loadings and additional modifications only considered if this appeared

problematic in the measurement model.

Page 52: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

51

Table 4.1. Fit statistics for each scale using individual items

Scale df χ2 CFI TLI SRMR RMSEA 90% CI PCLOSE

Lower Upper Mindset 20 107.348 .886 .840 .061 .165 .135 .197 .000 LoC 20 39.672 .873 .822 .068 .078 .042 .114 .094 PSSE 35 181.916 .883 .850 .059 .162 .139 .186 .000 LGO 5 13.836 .978 .956 .035 .105 .041 .173 .072 AGO 2 10.863 .972 .917 .045 .166 .079 .269 .017

Two-factor Mindset 19 34.753 .979 .970 .032 .072 .031 .109 .159

Parcels were created using the method specified by Little, Cunningham, Shahar &

Widaman, (2002), where the highest loading items are allocated to separate

parcels, and then the lowest loading items are allocated in reverse order to the

corresponding parcels. Marsh and Hau (1999) assert that each latent variable

should retain at least three indicators and for PSSE and mindset scales, this

resulted in three parcels comprising between two and four items. Where scales

only had four and five items (i.e. LoC, AGO & LGO), following Marsh and

Hau’s (1999) minimum criteria, it was necessary for one (LGO, LoC) or two

items (AGO) to remain unparcelled. In this case, it was the strongest loading that

was kept unparcelled. Full details of items and parcels are shown in appendix C.

Further CFA to assess individual scales with parcels was not possible as with only

three indicators, these were just-identified and measures of adequacy could not be

calculated. Therefore CFA was used to assess the adequacy of the full

measurement model and the selected indices suggested a good fit between model

Page 53: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

52

and the data (²= 125.10, df = 80, p = .001; CFI = .971; TLI = .962; RMSEA =

.059; SRMR = .053). Inspection of modification indices (MI) suggested two

particular improvements, but there did not appear to be any reasonable theoretical

link; an MI of 11.89 was associated with AGO and a residual error on one PSSE

indicator (SE2) and an MI of 10.65 between residual errors on indicators of AGO

(AGO3) and Mindset (Mind2). Therefore, as per Jöreskog (1993), in the absence

of strong justification for respecifying the model no further changes were made

and this measurement model formed the basis for the subsequent analyses of

structural models. The full measurement model is displayed in figure 4.1 and also

shows the factor loadings for indicators which ranged from .62 (LoC1) to .94

(Avoid2). On the basis of the good fit shown by the measurement model, the

mindset latent variable was retained without further changes.

Page 54: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

53

Figure 4.1: Measurement model (²= 125.10, df = 80, p = .001; CFI = .971; TLI = .962; RMSEA = .059; SRMR = .053)

Mind

.82Mind1 e13.91

.76Mind2 e14

.87

.85Mind3 e15

.92

LoC

.38LoC1 e10.62 .48LoC2 e11

.70.56

LoC3 e12

.75

.37

LGO

.75LGO1 e7.86 .60LGO2 e8

.77.85

LGO3 e9

.92

.43

.27

AGO

.52AGO1 e4.72

.89AGO2 e5

.94

.72AGO3 e6

.85

-.39

-.22

-.17

PSSE

.86SE1 e1

.93 .76SE2 e2.87

.83SE3 e3

.91

-.29

.50

.45

.27

Page 55: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

54

Table 4.2: Means, standard deviations and correlations between latent and observed variables Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Mindset 35.36 9.19 (.91) 2. LoC 25.15 3.77 .42 (.69)1 3. AGO 13.60 5.03 -.12 -.30 (.86) 4. LGO 28.04 4.47 .22 .53 -.16 (.86) 5. PSSE 51.22 10.65 .21 .52 -.25 .36 (.93) 6. Membership (months) 47.99 53.45 .08 .20 -.26 .28 .56 - 7. Practice (roles) 17.27 14.13 .10 .25 -.20 .24 .23 .38 - Note. alpha coefficients in parentheses 1alpha coefficient based on five items retained from initial CFA

Means, standard deviations, scale reliabilities and correlations between latent and

observed variables are shown in table 4.2. In light of the apparent similarities

between LoC and mindset, discussed previously, it is notable that the moderate

correlations (r =.42) support the assertion that they do represent distinct

constructs. Scale reliabilities were all adequate, ranging from .69 (LoC) to .93

(PSSE).

Convergent and discriminant validity were assessed using calculations from Hair,

Black, Babin and Anderson (2007). Convergent validity was evaluated by

assessing the average variance extracted (AVE) for each of the latent constructs

(Hair et al, 2007); although the majority of these comfortably exceeded the

recommended threshold of AVE > .5 (Hair et al, 2007), the LoC scale fell just

short of this (AVE = .48).

Page 56: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

55

Table 4.3: Assessments of construct convergent and discriminant validity: AVE, MSV and ASV

Latent variable

Average Variance Extracted (AVE)

Maximum Shared Squared Variance (MSV)

Average Squared Shared Variance (ASV)

Mindset 0.81 0.14 0.08 LoC 0.48 0.20 0.15 AGO 0.71 0.15 0.08 LGO 0.73 0.25 0.16 PSSE 0.82 0.25 0.15

Meanwhile, all latent constructs indicated discriminant validity with AVE for all

scales being greater than both MSV (maximum shared squared variance) and

ASV (average squared shared variance) (table 4.3).

4.3 Structural equation models (SEM)

The approach taken to model testing was in line with Jöreskog’s (1993)

alternative models methodology, whereby models specified a priori are tested

(figures 4.2 & 4.3) and the best fitting model selected. This also follows advice

from MacCallum, Roznowski and Necowitz (1992) and Ullman (2007) who

caution against excessive modification of models to achieve best fit by ‘fitting

small idiosyncratic characteristics of the sample’. This was considered

particularly relevant in view of the present sample size and missing data issues.

Therefore, aside from the issues arising from preliminary data screening and item

retention decisions detailed previously, only the two proposed models were tested

and no further modification undertaken.

Page 57: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

56

4.3.1 Missing data

At this stage the observed dependent variables (‘practice’ and ‘membership’)

were added to test the hypothesised structural models. However, a key issue

should be highlighted; from the total sample of 161 which provided data on the

five constructs used in measurement models, only n = 81 provided responses on

the practice variable. There were two further cases missing data for membership

meaning there was data available from n = 79 for this variable. This appeared to

stem from either the technical issue described previously (section 3.4) or

questionnaires being completed online by non-members who therefore were not

required to complete the membership-specific section. It is unclear exactly how

many are due to the former and how many are simply due to non-membership –

although it believed the latter is responsible for the greater proportion. However,

in preference to listwise deletion of cases with incomplete data, which has been

shown to be unsatisfactory in these circumstances and sample size (Enders &

Bandalos, 2001), the decision was taken to employ Full Information Maximum

Likelihood estimation (FIML), a method which has been shown to provide

reasonable results even under conditions of similar ‘missingness’.

However, before discussing this, a missing-values-analysis in SPSS, indicated

significant differences in mean scores on four of the five independent variables

between the group providing full data and the group which did not. Full details

Page 58: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

57

are provided in table 7.2 (appendix B) and indicate that only the difference in

LGO were non-significant. These differences themselves may have been

interesting findings if missingness was purely due to membership versus non-

membership, but as this was not entirely the case it is mentioned only to highlight

the issue. Demographic comparisons of the two groups are displayed in table 7.1

(appendix B) and indicate both groups were broadly similar though the ‘missing

data’ population contained a greater proportion of students (23.8%) than the

‘complete data’ population (2.5%).

FIML is available within AMOS (Arbuckle, 1996; Arbuckle, 2007; Byrne, 2010;

Carter, 2006; Graham, 2009) and draws on information from all the available

variables in the dataset to estimate parameters (Enders, 2001a). It has been shown

as an effective solution for dealing with missing data, including when there are

distributional violations (Yuan, Wallentin & Bentler, in press). Furthermore,

simulations have shown FIML performs well across a range of conditions and has

produced satisfactory results even in small samples, or those featuring over 50%

missing data (e.g. Enders, 2010; Enders & Bandalos, 2001; Newman, 2003,

Wothke, 2000)

4.3.2 Structural models

SEM was used to test the hypothesised models including the observed dependent

variables ‘practice’ and ‘membership’. The two models tested here were broadly

Page 59: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

58

similar, with only the relationships between mindset with LoC and the two goal

orientations varying. The first model tested is the hypothesised one, which

proposes that LoC would mediate the relationship between mindset and goal

orientations (figure 4.2). However, an alternative model broadly following

Dweck and Leggett’s (1988) framework - with the addition of LoC - was also

tested. Both models hypothesised positive paths from LGO to, ‘practice’ and

‘membership’, with the same paths from AGO hypothesised as negative. Both

‘membership’ and LoC were hypothesised to have positive paths to PSSE while

PSSE was also predicted to be positively related to ‘practice’.

Page 60: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

59

Model 1: Locus of control as a mediator of mindset

The hypothesised model considered LoC as a mediator of mindset. This model

proposes that the belief that one’s abilities are malleable (fixed) contributes to an

internal (external) locus of control, and this influences the tendency to select

avoidance or learning goals. Across the selected indices, this model indicated

reasonable fit (²=180.346, df = 111, p<.001; CFI = .956; TLI = .939; RMSEA =

.062; AIC = 298.346) and table 4.4 indicates that even at the upper bounds of the

RMSEA 90% confidence interval, this model would still suggest an adequate fit.

Figure 4.2: SEM model 1: LoC mediating effect sof mindset on goal orientations (²=180.346, df = 111, p <.001; CFI = .956; TLI = .939; RMSEA = .062)

Mindset

.82Mind1 e1.91

.76Mind2 e2

.87

.86Mind3 e3

.93

.17

LoC

.34LoC1 e4.58

.50LoC2 e5

.71

.49LoC3 e6

.70

.09

AGO

.51AGO1 e7.71

.92AGO2 e8

.96

.70AGO3 e9

.84

.28

LGO.86

LGO3e10

.93

.59LGO2e11

.77

.74LGO1e12 .86

.48

PSSE

.83SE3e13

.77SE2e14.88

.85SE1e15

.92

.13

Membership

.17

Total practice

.42

.42

.31

.91

.11

-.22

.48

-.11

.53 -.30

e16

e17

e18

e19

e20

e21

.25

Page 61: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

60

However, in view of the popularity of Dweck and Leggett’s framework, it was

considered important to test an alternative model as a comparison. This more

closely follows their conception of the relationships between mindset and goal

orientations.

Model 2: Covaried mindset/LoC

Although the first model indicated good fit, this model was tested to rule out an

alternative theoretical explanation: that mindset has a direct effect on goal

orientations. This model proposes that an incremental mindset has direct effects

on AGO and LGO, while LoC will also have similar direct effects (figure 4.3).

All other relationships remained the same as model 1. The fit indices suggested

an acceptable fit, based on the specified criteria (²= 179.360, df = 109, p <.001;

CFI = .955; TLI = .937; RMSEA = .064; AIC = 301.360). However, in this

model, although path coefficients between mindset and both AGO (β =-.06; p =

.53) and LGO (β =.08; p. = .38) were in the expected direction, neither were

significant. Meanwhile, LoC’s relationships with both LGO (β = .48; p < .001),

PSSE (β = .41; p < .001) and AGO (β = -.26; p < .05) were significant, as in the

previous model, suggesting that LoC fully mediates the effect of mindset on goal

orientations. The model also indicated a comparatively poorer fit than the first

model (AIC = 301.360).

Page 62: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

61

Figure 4.3: SEM model 2: covaried mindset and LoC model (²= 179.360, df = 109, p <.001; CFI = .955; TLI = .937; RMSEA = .064)

Mindset

.82Mind1 e1.91

.76Mind2 e2

.87

.85Mind3 e3

.92LoC

.35LoC1e4

.59

.50LoC2e5

.71

.51LoC3e6 .71

.08

AGO

.51AGO1 e7.71

.92AGO2 e8

.96

.70AGO3 e9

.84

.26

LGO.87

LGO3e10

.93

.59LGO2e11

.77

.74LGO1e12 .86

.48

PSSE

.83SE3e13

.77SE2e14.88

.85SE1e15

.92

.13

Membership

.17

Total practice

.31

.91

.11

-.23

.48

-.11

e16

e17

e18

e19

e21

.25

-.26

.41

.48 -.06.08

.40

Page 63: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

62

Table 4.4: Fit statistics for alternative models

Model df ² CMIN/DF CFI TLI RMSEA 90% CI PCLOSE AIC

1. LoC mediator 111 180.346* 1.625 .956 .939 .062 .045 .079 .109 298.346

2. Covaried mindset/LoC 109 179.360* 1.646 .955 .937 .062 .046 .080 .093 301.360

3. LoC mediator untransformed1 111 177.842* 1.602 .958 .941 .061 .044 .078 .132 295.842

4. Covaried mindset/LoC untransformed2

109 176.895* 1.623 .957 .939 .062 .045 .079 .113 298.985

Note. CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root-mean-square error of approximation; AIC = Akaike information criterion. 1 same as model 1, includes untransformed outliers 2 same as model 2, includes untransformed outliers * p < .001

Final model

Of the two models, the hypothesised model indicated superior fit on all of the

selected fit indices (table 4.4), though both models could be classified as

reasonable-to-good fit based on these. Both models were also similar in the

amount of variance they explained on the dependent variables, with the only

difference in explanatory value being the additional 2% and 1% of variance on

LGO and AGO respectively accounted for by the LoC-as-mediator model (model

1). Nonetheless, despite the relative similarity in fit and variance explained, the

hypothesised model was still superior on both counts and is preferred to the

alternative.

Page 64: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

63

Further to the discussion of outliers (section 4.1), there were some differences in

the model statistics between the data with transformed or untransformed outliers;

the untransformed data actually produced better fit (see models 3 and 4 in table

4.4), though explained 1% less variance on ‘membership’ due to the reduced path

coefficient from LGO (β = .23, p < .01) than those with the transformations (β =

.25, p < .01). Nonetheless, with both datasets, each of the models supported the

hypothesis that LoC and mindset would be significantly related.

The selected model (figure 4.2, and summarised in figure 4.4) suggests the effect

of mindset on LGO and AGO is mediated by LoC and supports the hypothesised

positive relationship between incremental beliefs and stronger perceptions of

internality (β =.42; p < .001). Furthermore, the positive and negative

relationships between LoC with LGO (β = .53; p < .001) and AGO (β = -.30; p <

.01) respectively, were also as hypothesised and in view of the superior fit of this

model and the direct non-significant paths from mindset to AGO and LGO in the

alternative model, this may offer some insight into the equivocal results of

previous research which has looked at these relationships (e.g. Payne et al, 2007).

In common with previous research (e.g. Phillips & Gully, 1997) and as

hypothesised, the path between LoC and PSSE was positive and significant (β =

.42; p < .001), with an internal LoC being associated with higher levels of public

speaking self-efficacy. Interestingly however, while goal orientations were

significantly related to continued membership of their public speaking clubs and

supported both hypotheses (AGO β = -.22 p < .05; LGO β = .25; p < .05), the

Page 65: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

64

hypothesis that goal orientations would be significantly related to the amount of

public speaking practice in the previous six months was not (AGO β = -.11 p =

.33; LGO β = .11; p = .34). Meanwhile, the apparent cumulative benefits of

practice (inferred by length of membership) on self-efficacy was also indicated by

the positive relationship between length of membership and PSSE (β = .48; p <

.001). Furthermore, following the results of the membership to PSSE path and

Bandura’s (1997) theory regarding the unfolding and dynamic nature of self-

efficacy, PSSE was also positively related to the amount of practice undertaken (β

= .31; p < .001).

Figure 4.4: Final model including standardised path coefficients. Note. *** p < .001; ** p < .01; * p < .05; ns = non-significant

Overall, the model accounted for 17% of the variance in ‘practice’ and 13% of

variance in ‘membership’, providing some support for the hypothesised impact of

the dependent variables considered here. The model also accounted for 48% of

Mindset

LGO

AGO

LoC PSSE

Memb.

Practice.42***

-.30**

.53***

0.42***

.31*

.11 ns

-.11 ns-.22*

.25*

.48***

Page 66: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

65

the variance in PSSE with the largest path coefficient being associated with length

of membership. Meanwhile, of the variables that were set as predictors of

membership, 28% the variance on LGO and 9% on AGO were explained by the

model. Mindset, proposed as antecedent to LoC, accounted for 17% of the

variance in internality beliefs.

4.3.3 Additional analysis

Significance of LoC mediation of mindset

In view of the discussions around LoC as mediator and the support the model

received, it was appropriate to consider the magnitude and significance of the

indirect effects of mindset on goal orientations. AMOS does not – by default –

give such significance statistics but the use of bootstrapping within AMOS does

allow both significance and confidence intervals to be estimated (Arbuckle, 2007;

Cheung & Lau, 2008). With bootstrapping, data is ‘resampled’ a predetermined

number of times to build up an empirical estimation of the sampling distribution

of a statistic and this distribution provides a foundation for the construction of

confidence intervals around the indirect effect (Cheung & Lau, 2008, p.301).

However, this is not available in the presence of missing data, as is the case with

the dependent variables; therefore, only the portion of the model involving

mindset, LoC and goal orientation was tested. It is recognised that the strength of

relationships among these path coefficients would differ slightly from those in the

Page 67: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

66

full model, so although these results are a useful test of this mediation, in the

context of the full model, these should be considered with caution.

The bootstrapping process followed Cheung and Lau (2008) and employed their

recommended use of 95% confidence intervals and specification of 1000

bootstrap samples. This analysis, using the bias corrected statistic indicated that

mindset had significant indirect effects on each of the goal orientations (AGO

standardised indirect effect = -.12, [95% CI: -.02 ~ -.27]; LGO standardised

indirect effect = .20 [95% CI: .08 ~ .35]), which further supports the model, albeit

with the aforementioned caveat. Figure 4.5 displays the direct and indirect

relationships and significance levels.

Figure 4.5: test of significance of indirect effects of mindset on learning and avoidance goal orientations. Note. Dotted lines indicate indirect effect of mindset on goal orientations via LoC; standardised path coefficients shown. ** p < .01, *** p < .001)

Page 68: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

67

5 Discussion

5.1 Summary of analysis and results

This study aimed to test a model based upon Dweck and Leggett’s motivational

framework comprising mindset and goal orientation. It adds to the existing

literature by testing the theory-based addition of locus of control in order to shed

light on consistently weaker-than-expected relationships. The further inclusion of

self-efficacy means this model is the first to incorporate these constructs and

heeds Funder’s (2001) call for greater integration of mindset within social

cognitive theory. Furthermore, the model was tested in relation to a task which is

widely seen as anxiety provoking – public speaking – and is thus congruent with

the view that the effects of such social cognitive constructs are most salient when

facing challenging tasks or difficulties. As the study was conducted relative to a

voluntary activity, it also complements the majority of research in academic or

work settings where individuals may not have the same discretion to exercise (or

not) their preferred self-regulatory strategies. The discussion will begin with a

brief overview of findings, before discussing individual relationships in more

depth and in the order specified by the model. Specific issues are discussed

throughout, while limitations and recommendations for future research are

followed by the study implications and conclusion.

Page 69: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

68

The final structural model indicated that LoC mediated the relationship between

mindset and goal orientations; a finding which provides a plausible extension of

Dweck and Leggett’s framework. Although it was noted that an alternative

model, more closely approximating their model, explained the same amount of

variance in both of the dependent variables, the lack of significant direct paths

from mindset to goal orientations when LoC was present, in addition to the

theoretical rationale discussed in the literature review, supported the LoC-as-

mediator model. These results also indicate that in comparison with LoC, the

effects of mindset are not as influential in the development of goal orientations as

Dweck and Leggett (1988) and numerous researchers may suggest.

This model potentially offer a resolution to previous research into the direct

effects of mindset on LGO and the generally weak relationships reported

previously. However, contrary to hypotheses, the model indicated that neither

goal orientation was related to the amount of public speaking practice undertaken

by members at their clubs in the previous six months. Nonetheless, these did

significantly predict length of membership, which perhaps suggests that goal

orientations have a greater influence on longer-term perseverance than

short/medium-term strategy. The length of membership was significantly related

to level of public speaking self-efficacy which is unsurprising and entirely in line

with research showing the benefits of practice. The model explained 13% of the

variance in membership length, while further support for the role of self-efficacy

was also provided by its significant association with amount of practice. Overall,

Page 70: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

69

the model explained 17% of the variance in public speaking practice and provides

an interesting starting point for research incorporating these five constructs.

5.2 Discussion of results

5.2.1 Mindset and locus of control

One of the key issues the hypothesised model sought to address was the modest

relationships between mindset and goal orientations demonstrated by previous

research. A search of the literature indicated that no previous studies have

proposed and investigated the presence of LoC as a mediator. Both constructs

have control-related elements (Skinner, 1996) and correlation between mindset

and LoC were expected, though this was moderate (r = .42) and tests of

discriminant validity indicated they were clearly two different constructs, based

on the measures used here. The direct significant path between mindset and LoC

supports the first hypothesis and suggests that a perception of one’s abilities as

being amenable to change (i.e. mindset) promotes a view of having more control

over one’s own development (Dweck et al, 1995). So in response to the mixed

findings regarding smaller direct effects of mindset on goal orientations, it is

suggested that an incremental mindset alone is insufficient and needs to be

accompanied by beliefs about one’s ability to exert control over the

‘environment’, which has been strongly linked with adaptive strategies (Skinner,

1996). The significant relationships between LoC and each of the goal

orientations supports the hypothesis that LoC would mediate the effect of mindset

Page 71: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

70

on both AGO and LGO; LoC and goal orientation relationships are discussed in

section 5.2.2. Additional support for the mediated relationship was provided by

the supplementary analysis which suggested this mediated relationship was

significant. However, due to the requirement that the analysis could not include

the two variables associated with the missing data, this must be considered

tentative rather than conclusive. Overall, this suggests the model may be useful in

interpreting previous related findings, though this does not invalidate Dweck and

Leggett’s theory and may provide a valuable explanatory extension of it.

Mindset explained 17% of the variance in LoC, so although the relationship was

significant and is comparable with other social cognitive research looking at

related relationships (e.g. Phillips and Gully, 1997), it leaves a lot of variance

unaccounted for and suggests the need to consider additional variables in future

models. It is understandable that a single construct would not be solely or

majorly responsible for the variance of a construct such as LoC which is derived

from such a wide range of sources, including ability, experience and early

developmental factors (Chorpita & Barlow, 1998; Phillips & Gully, 1997;

Skinner, 1996). Therefore, while it was asserted previously that an incremental

mindset may be a necessary, but not sufficient, precondition for action to ensue, it

is also clear that mindset alone is not sufficient to explain internality of LoC.

Nonetheless, mindset is well supported by research affirming its impact in various

contexts (e.g. Beer, 2002; Burnette, 2010; Heslin et al, 2006; Levy et al, 1998;

Page 72: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

71

Mueller & Dweck, 1998) and there appears little doubt that it is a useful and

influential construct.

Taken at face value the results may be useful in developing our understanding of

mindset and its relationships, but there several matters which should first be

considered. Firstly, closer inspection of LoC and mindset items highlighted a

particular semantic issue. Whereas the LoC measure used the first person

singular for all items (i.e. ‘I’ or ‘my), the mindset scale uses the second person

singular or plural (i.e. ‘you’) which can refer to either the self or ‘people in

general’ (Oxford English Dictionary). This is noted by Bråten and Strømsø

(2005) in relation to the mindset measure but they conclude that it is acceptable

for their purposes. However in psychometric terms, Azjen and Fishbein (2005)

suggest this would lead some people to view items at a more global level. This

ambiguity is likely to lead mindset items being interpreted at a more general level

than LoC items, which might well have influenced the relationship between the

two constructs. For example, some people interpreting mindset questions at a

‘people in general’ level may believe that ‘[other] people’ can change their

abilities, yet not feel they themselves could; an example of such thought patterns

can be seen with low self-esteem (e.g. Judge, Erez & Bono, 1998; Marsh, 1996).

So some respondents may therefore indicate higher levels of mindset because of

its more global framing, while also indicating lower internality beliefs, which is

more specific. This would arguably reduce the strength of the relationship

between mindset and LoC and may indicate the ‘real’ relationship is actually

Page 73: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

72

larger in magnitude. Similarly, as the goal orientation items also use the first

person (i.e. ‘I’ & ‘me’), this may also apply to these relationships and both direct

and indirect effects of mindset on goal orientations may also be stronger than

found here. As Dweck’s measures are widely used, this may also be an additional

explanation for some of the previous research findings discussed previously.

Nonetheless, further issues with the mindset measure relate to some

psychometrically questionable items. For example, one item includes the

colloquialism ‘you can’t teach an old dog, new tricks’ (item 4), which may or

may not make sense to all participants and it is unclear how it might be

interpreted. The double-barrelled nature of two of the questions also detracts

from their clarity (Bowling, 1997).

The confirmatory factor analysis of the mindset scale also suggested issues with

the way this is measured; theoretically, the two-factor mindset model does not

make sense but it did indicate a better fit. However, the subsequent parcelling

strategy appeared to ameliorate these problems and indicated no issues in the

measurement model, but they cannot be completely ignored. It is contended that

these issues, in addition to those mentioned previously are particularly important

here, because this study is examining a relationship that has not been looked at

before so there is no real empirical precedent that can be used to validate such

conclusions. Nonetheless, the theoretical underpinnings underlying the model

Page 74: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

73

tested here suggest the relationship found is not spurious, but given the level of

research attention that mindset has received, a well-designed measure founded on

good psychometric principles should be a priority to improve inferences derived

from it.

5.2.2 Relationships from LoC to goal orientations and PSSE

The direct paths from LoC to both AGO and LGO were also supported, in line

with the mediating role of LoC; the significant positive path from LoC to PSSE

was also indicated by the model. These support the hypothesised relationships

from LoC. The latter is in line with the view that perceptions about capability to

carry out a particular action/skill are influenced by more general beliefs that

outcomes may be influenced by behaviours (Bandura, 1977). It also echoes

previous theory (e.g. Bandura, 1997; Skinner, 1996) and research (Bandura &

Wood, 1989; Phillips & Gully, 1997). However, the relationships between LoC

and goal orientations have not been as extensively studied and were based

predominantly on theory rather than research. Although Seifert and O’Keefe

(2001) do provide some evidence, they employed a series of three-item scales and

aside from reliabilities - which were reasonable - no information is provided

regarding their construction or validation. Nonetheless, the relationships

demonstrated by the final model here are supportive and with regards to the LoC

to AGO path, may also shed light on another frequently equivocal finding. As

previously stated, this study employed only the LGO and AGO dimensions of

VandeWalle’s (1997) measure, because the other, prove orientation, has so often

Page 75: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

74

shown non-significant relationships (VandeWalle et al, 1999), appearing to have

more complex interactions with performance (Payne et al, 2007) and is not always

maladaptive (e.g. Stevens & Gist, 1997). However, as both prove and AGO have

been conflated into an overall performance goal orientation (PGO) by other

measures (e.g. Button et al, 1996; Dweck, 2000), this is likely to mask their true

effects. For example, Phillips and Gully (1997) – using Button and colleagues’

goal orientation measure - found LoC and PGO to be unrelated. The findings

here, offer further support for goal orientation measures which clearly

differentiate between these PGO dimensions.

The model accounted for 28% of the variance of LGO, suggestive that an

incremental mindset and subsequent internal locus of control are indeed

influential in the development of a learning goal orientation. However, the model

also indicates that an entity mindset and low internality do not equate to

avoidance goal orientations to the same degree, with 9% of AGO variance

explained by the model. It may also be taken as support for VandeWalle’s (1997)

model of goal orientations in that AGO does not simply appear to be the exact

opposite of learning goal orientation. A further consideration is that this study

only employed the internality subscale of Levenson’s (1981) measure and the

inclusion of the two dimensions proposed to relate explicitly to an external LoC –

chance and powerful others – should be expected to improve the amount of AGO

variance predicted. It could also be argued that other factors such as

Page 76: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

75

conscientiousness or motivations, not included here, might supersede evaluative

concerns related to AGO (e.g. Bouffard, Boisvert, Vezeau, and Larouche, 1995)

Furthermore, the internality subscale used her only approached adequate

reliability (α = .69) after the removal of three items - a similar finding to Walkey

(1979) – in addition to low convergent validity. This has clear implications when

investigating the relationships between these constructs, particularly as they are so

central to the study. There are a wide range of other LoC measures (e.g.

Duttweiler, 1984; Lefcourt, 1991; Rotter, 1966; Spector, 1988), yet together these

have often been shown to demonstrate poor convergent validity (Goodman &

Waters, 1987) and emphasises that it cannot be assumed that a single measure is

truly representative of the construct. Therefore, comparisons using different

measures would be useful and appropriate. For example, of the four dimensions

on Lefcourt’s (1991) scale, two are expressly concerned with the degree to which

one holds internal attributions of ability or effort. At face value, both of these

appear to have strong parallels with mindset and be expected to have

demonstrated even stronger relationships.

5.2.3 Goal orientations and dependent variables

The effects of goal orientations on performance tend to occur via the differing

self-regulatory and learning strategies they promote (e.g. Payne et al, 2007); this

includes maladaptive avoidance behaviours such as dropping out, in the case of

AGO (e.g. Duda, 1989). The relationships between both AGO (β = -.22; p < .01)

Page 77: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

76

and LGO (β = .25; p < .01) to length of public speaking club membership in this

study are suggestive of a similar pattern and support both related hypotheses.

This suggests that individuals with avoidant goal orientations are simply less

likely to continue attending. Conversely, with LGO, this relationship is reversed

and higher levels linked to length of membership. As this study related to public

speaking, which is often feared precisely because of concerns about appearing

foolish to others (Leary, 1983), this is especially relevant. It is expected that

longer-term attendance and thus more practice opportunities would be linked with

improved self-efficacy- a finding supported by this study, but these results also

add to our understanding about why. This in itself is valuable to practitioners who

may be looking for ways to promote involvement with training activities or

development opportunities and emphasise the benefits of creating an environment

that encourages learning goal orientations (Utman, 1997).

Nonetheless, despite empirical support (e.g. Pintrich & de Groot, 1990;

VandeWalle et al, 1999), goal orientations did not significantly predict the

amount of public speaking practiced in the previous six-months and hypotheses

regarding these relationships were not supported. There are a number of

explanations for this though firstly, it should be noted that the relationships were

in the expected direction, but were not sufficiently strong (AGO: β = -.11, p =.33;

LGO: β = .11, p =.34). On the one hand, the sample size and a lack of statistical

power may be an issue, but regardless of significance, the relationships were

small and whether such an effect has practical use is also questionable. However,

Page 78: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

77

from a theoretical perspective, in light of social cognitive research indicating the

importance of motivational factors such as specific task motivation or perceived

meaning (e.g. Ommundsen, 2001; Seifert & O’Keefe, 2001), the inclusion of such

a variable could conceivably have mediated this relationship (e.g. VandeWalle et

al, 1999). After all, having a general predisposition to enjoy learning new skills is

not analogous to enjoying the learning of this particular new skill. Nor should it

mean that a person with a high LGO would continue practicing a skill that has

either little interest or relevance to them.

Meanwhile, in contrast to the majority of related studies in educational or

workplace settings (e.g. Phillips & Gully, 1997; Seifert & O’Keefe, 2001) where

participation is essentially mandatory, this study looked at involvement in an

entirely voluntary endeavour. Therefore, because AGO’s are more likely to drop

out (e.g. Duda, 1989) and there is nothing to ‘prevent’ them, it is probable that the

measure of speaking practice used here is subject to a restriction of range, as those

with AGO’s are less likely to continue. This, in tandem with the decreased

likelihood of them even seeking such a development opportunity in the first

instance (Porath, Spreitzer, Gibson & Garnett, 2012) would attenuate these

relationships further and supports the view they are less likely to be represented in

the final sample. Therefore, the sample and measure of public-speaking practice

used here is likely to represent a conservative test of the hypothesis that goal

orientations are related to willingness to practice. Consequently, in this instance,

Page 79: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

78

continued membership may be a better indicator of the impact of these constructs

than amount of practice.

5.2.4 Public speaking self-efficacy and dependent variables

The relationships between PSSE and the dependent variables were as

hypothesised and in line with a huge amount of existing research regarding the

impact of self-efficacy on behaviours and the development of self-efficacy (see

Bandura, 1997; Bandura & Wood, 1989; Hardy, Jones & Gould, 1996; Wood &

Bandura, 1989). Additionally, the model accounted for 48% of the variance in

PSSE, suggesting that the factors with causal paths going towards PSSE were

good predictors. The length of membership showed a strong relationship with

PSSE (β = .48, p < .001) and although the cross-sectional nature of the study

precludes causal assertions, there is substantial evidence supporting the view that

the amount of time people spend practicing is causally related to their levels of

self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997). Moreover, it could be inferred that this

relationship between self-efficacy and practice is based upon the length of

membership - and thus cumulative amount of practice - which has additive benefit

of increasing the likelihood of practicing further. Although this is extrapolating

somewhat, it does lend further support to the bi-directional model of

performance/self-efficacy development (Bandura, 1997).

Page 80: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

79

5.3 Limitations

Naturally, there are limitations and as previously mentioned, the cross-sectional

nature of this study limits assertions of causality. While this is hardly unique to

this study, it is worth looking at precisely why this would be important here.

The interactive nature of self-efficacy and outcomes described by Bandura (1997)

means the model proposed here provides only a snapshot of a complex and

dynamic process. Without baseline measures at the very least, it makes it difficult

to tease out the true nature of these relationships. However, while this is

undoubtedly true and common to numerous other studies (e.g. Kozlowski et al,

2001; Phillips & Gully, 1997), this study has at least attempted to model their

relationships as suggested by more robust longitudinal studies (e.g. Bandura &

Wood, 1989; Wood & Bandura, 1989).

Therefore subsequent research should look to employ a longitudinal design,

recognising the reciprocal relationships between self-regulatory mechanisms

(Bandura, 1997). It is also recommended that a multi-trial methodology be

adopted (e.g. Bandura & Wood, 1989; Wood & Bandura, 1989), whereby

measures are taken at set points throughout the study in an effort to capture their

dynamic and evolving nature.

The use of data from all participants to estimate the missing data on the dependent

variables (i.e. FIML) reflected issues in the data collection; both of which must be

Page 81: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

80

recognised as a limitation. Nonetheless, FIML - the method selected to deal with

this - has been shown to perform well in these circumstances, providing unbiased

estimates under missing-at-random (MAR) conditions (Wothke, 2000) and can

perform effectively even when data is not quite MAR (Muthén, Kaplan & Hollis,

1987). However, large proportions of missing data can reduce the power of a

model by inflating the standard error of estimates (Sternberg, 2006). Although

FIML is still superior in that regard to listwise deletion (Enders & Bandalos,

2001), in view of the proportion of missing data and inherent power limitations,

this concern does appear particularly relevant. Results concerning the dependent

variables should therefore be viewed cautiously. Nonetheless, assertions

regarding the mindset, LoC and goal orientation portion of the model are based on

the full sample.

This sample is somewhat smaller than ideally recommended for SEM (e.g. Hu &

Bentler, 1999) and the possibility these results are idiosyncratic to this small

population cannot be ruled out; particularly considering the high proportion of

university educated individuals and senior managers in the sample. This and the

data-related limitations all have implications regarding the generalisability of this

study. The missing-values-analysis also indicated significant differences

between the ‘full data’ and ‘incomplete data’ groups on four of the five

independent variables. Whilst this does not preclude valid parameter estimates

from FIML, it highlights a further reason to not overgeneralise these findings.

Page 82: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

81

These group differences may have been interesting in themselves, if it was clear

these two groups were exclusively differentiated by their membership (or not) of

the public speaking clubs. But due to the presence of an indeterminate minority

of members in the ‘missing data’ group due to technical issues with the online

questionnaire, this would have been meaningless.

In addition to restriction of range discussed previously, the accuracy of the

‘practice’ measure is also unclear. Although measures of club involvement were

intended to be objective, they did rely on people’s recall of both their period of

club membership and the number of roles they had taken in the previous six

months. As such, it is acknowledged these are likely to be imperfect, as opposed

to more accurate records maintained by the clubs themselves, which were

unavailable for obvious confidentiality reasons. However, although they may

approximate a reasonable indicator of willingness to practice, as this is a

voluntary activity, attendance was also likely to be influenced by peoples

circumstances and availability, over and above their desire to improve their skills.

The study did not account for such mitigating factors, such as schedule conflicts

or family responsibilities, that may have reduced their amount of practice.

5.4 Future research

The point was made in the literature review that some previous models have not

including key variables in their models or have failed to account for theoretically

Page 83: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

82

supported relationships (e.g. Phillips & Gully, 1997). Therefore, by the same

token the present study must also acknowledge the same. There are several

additional factors which could be theorised to improve the explanatory value of

this model, some of which have been mentioned previously.

The final model left a large proportion of the variance unaccounted for in both

‘length of membership’ and ‘amount of practice’. In a relatively complex model

such as this, it is not entirely surprising and in line with Bandura (2012), future

studies should look to include other variables shown to influence behavioural

outcomes. There was no objective measure of performance included in this

model, but in line with Bandura and Wood’s (1989) cyclical model of

performance and self-efficacy development, it would be expected that such a

measure would then itself influence continued membership and self-efficacy and

so on. However, factors such as goal level and other goal-setting theory variables

would be usefully incorporated into this model (Bandura & Cervone, 1983) and

would appear particularly relevant as evidence suggests these processes are more

strongly applicable to those with LGO (e.g. VandeWalle, Cron & Slocum, 2001).

This indicates that it is not as simple as either one orientation or the other leading

to either involvement or withdrawal.

Furthermore, it would also be helpful to obtain measures of public-speaking

anxiety, as well as motivation for improving their skills (i.e. task meaning), which

would provide more information on the perceived size and importance of the

Page 84: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

83

challenge people are undertaking (e.g. Seifert & O’Keefe, 2001). Such measures

would be more informative as a baseline measure in a longitudinal study than in

the cross-sectional design used here, so their initial effects can be discerned.

It may also be fruitful to extend the call for greater integration of mindset and

social cognitive constructs to include personality traits; for example, is an entity

mindset – with its apparent links to avoidance and maladaptive strategies (Mueller

& Dweck, 1998) - predicted by neuroticism? Spinath et al (2003) appear to

suggest not, but very little research has looked at this and their measure was

domain-specific, rather than the general measure used here. Neuroticism could

also be argued to have direct effects on AGO (e.g. Wiggins, 1996). Therefore

more research looking at the personality-related correlates of this more

fundamental indicator of mindset would improve our understanding of its

antecedents.

Additionally, this study focused on individual difference and dispositional

influences on practice levels, yet clearly situational factors are important (e.g.

Ames & Archer, 1988) and not addressed here. Dispositions such as goal

orientation do not operate in a vacuum and Tabernero and Wood (1999) suggest

they are influenced by the relative importance of situational goals, and which

behaviours and outcomes are actually being rewarded (Ames & Archer, 1988).

After all, one can have an incremental mindset or learning goal orientation, but

how robust is this in the face of an extremely results-oriented culture? Future

Page 85: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

84

research may examine this model in relation to differing psychological

organisational environments. For example, the work of Dollard and Bakker

(2010) on psychosocial safety climate – the degree of support to apply new skills

or take ‘risks’ – may complement research showing how organisational culture

and climate play an important over and above such individual differences (e.g.

Murphy & Dweck, 2010).

However, before testing more variables within the model, it is perhaps most

pressing to develop and assess robust measures of mindset, as research has tended

to be based on variations of Dweck’s (2000) measure. It has good reliability, but

that is no guarantee of quality. It has also demonstrated discriminant validity

against numerous other constructs (Dweck et al, 1995), but alongside some of the

issues described previously, a better measure would provide more confidence

regarding the construct - which appears to have genuine value – and the

inferences that may be drawn from any relationships found.

5.5 Implications

These results provide a promising initial support for this model and for the

argument that previous research has indeed missed a key variable when using

Dweck and Leggett’s framework. If these results can be replicated addressing

some of the issues discussed, it could change the way previous results are

interpreted. Aside from this theoretical implication, there may also be practical

Page 86: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

85

ones. Research has increasingly looked at mindset in organisational contexts and

there may also be some interesting related points.

These results suggest mindset plays a role in the development of LoC, which has

been associated with a range of adaptive behaviours, such as proactivity, and

action oriented – versus helplessness – responses (Ng et al, 2006; Seligman, 1975;

Skinner, 1996). Encouragingly, research has shown that an incremental mindset

can be induced or trained (e.g. Blackwell et al, 2007; Heslin et al, 2006), so this

may be usefully explored by employers. Furthermore, in relation to the points

made regarding organisational climate, evidence also suggests that the type of

praise and feedback people get can significantly influence their self-regulatory

strategies via similar pathways indicated by the final model (e.g. Mueller &

Dweck, 1998). So careful consideration of this may also be warranted, although

this is essentially an extension of what has been known in organisational terms for

years – i.e.that people will enact behaviours that are rewarded (Schneider, White

& Paul, 1998). Praise people for effort and work, rather than just ability or results

and it can also positively influence LGO, which has situational, as well as

dispositional, antecedents (Payne et al, 2007). However, while organisations may

fear that a decreased focus on performance would reduce it should take note of

VandeWalle et al (1999). They found LGO positively associated with sales

performance, unlike PGO; furthermore - in support much of what has been argued

here – these positive outcomes were completely mediated by strategies such as

planning and effort.

Page 87: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

86

5.6 Conclusion

One of the aims of this study was to test a model integrating a group of social

cognitive constructs, not previously tested together. It has confirmed established

relationships such as LoC and self-efficacy and also supports Bandura’s dynamic

conception of the self-efficacy with its relationships to length of membership and

amounts of recent public speaking practice. The model has also provided initial

evidence of new relationships, with the mediating role of LoC between mindset

and goal orientations. This supports the second objective, which was to

investigate this relationship and offer a plausible explanation for the previously

modest results using Dweck and Leggett’s model. Meanwhile, the study also

examined this in relation to a discretionary skill development activity and

provided partial support for the effects of these constructs on some indicators of

commitment to improve public speaking skills.

However, a number of methodological limitations and caveats were noted, and

would need to be addressed before firm conclusions are asserted; nonetheless,

some of these methodological issues may themselves add to the literature, as

issues such as the wording of Dweck’s mindset measure and its impact do not

appear to have been considered in this way previously.

Overall, this study and its results would appear to concur with Heslin’s (2010)

conclusion that mindset is not a ‘magic bullet’, but in tandem with the empirical

support for its value, they do indicate it has a role to play. Furthermore, this

Page 88: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

87

research also provides a first look at a relationship not previously considered in

the literature and offers a useful platform for further research, pending validation

of these findings.

Page 89: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

88

6. References

Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory

of planned behaviour. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32 (4), 665-

683.

Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes on behavior. In D.

Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The Handbook of

Attitudes. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

Ames, C. & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: students’

learning and motivation processes. Journal of Educational Psychology,

80, 260-267.

Anderson, J. C. & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in

practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological

Bulletin, 103, 411–423.

Arbuckle, J. L. (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete

data. In G. A. Marcoulides & R. E. Schumacker (Eds.), Advanced

structural equation modeling. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,

Inc.

Arbuckle, J. L. (2007). Amos 16 User’s Guide. Chicago: SPSS.

Aronson, J., Fried, C., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype

threat on African American college students by shaping theories of

intelligence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113-125.

Averill, J. R. (1973). Personal control over aversive stimuli and its relationship to

stress. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 286-303.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: towards a unifying theory of behavioural

change. Psychological Review, 84 (2), 191-215.

Page 90: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

89

Bandura A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social

Cognitive Theory. Harlow: Prentice-Hall.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H.

Freeman.

Bandura, A. (2006). Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In F. Pajares & T.

Urdan (Eds.). Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents. Greenwich: Information

Age Publishing.

Bandura, A. (2012). On the functional properties of perceived self-efficacy

revisited. Journal of Management, 38 (1), 9-44.

Bandura, A. & Cervone, D. (1983). Self-evaluative and self-efficacy mechanisms

governing the motivational effects of goal systems. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 45, 1017-1028.

Bandura, A. & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects

revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88 (1), 87-99.

Bandura, A. & Wood, R. (1989). Effect of perceived controllability and

performance standards on self-regulation of complex decision making.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56 (5), 805-814.

Beer, J. S. (2002). Implicit self-theories of shyness. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 83(4), 1009-1024.

Biddle, S. J. H. (1999). Motivation and perceptions of control: Tracing its

development and plotting its future in exercise and sport psychology.

Journal of sport and exercise psychology, 21, 1-23.

Blackwell, L. S., Trzniewski, K. H. & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of

intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A

longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78 (1), 246-

263.

Page 91: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

90

Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. London: Wiley.

Bouffard, T., Bouchard, M., Goulet, G., Denoncourt, I., & Couture, N. (2005).

Influence of achievement goals and self-efficacy on students' self-

regulation and performance. Journal of Psychology, 40 (6), 373-384.

Bouffard, T., Boisvert, J., Vezeau, C. & Larouche, C. (1995). The impact of goal

orientation on self- regulation and performance among college students.

British Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 317–329.

Bouffard-Bouchard, T., Parent, S. & Larivee, S. (1991). Influence of self-efficacy

on self-regulation and performance among junior and senior high-school

aged students. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 14, 153-

164.

Bowling, A. (1997). Research Methods in Health. Buckingham: Open University

Press.

Brett, J. F. &VandeWalle, D. (1999). Goal orientation and goal content as

predictors of performance in a training program. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 84, 863– 873.

Bråten, I. & Strømsø, H. I. (2004). Epistemological beliefs and implicit theories

of intelligence as predictors of achievement goals. Contemporary

Educational Psychology, 29, 371-388.

Bråten, I. & Strømsø, H. I. (2004). The relationship between epistemological

beliefs, implicit theories of intelligence, and self-regulated learning among

Norwegian postsecondary students. British Journal of Educational

Psychology, 75, 539-565.

Brophy, J. (2004). Motivating students to learn (2nd edition). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

Buckley, R. & Caple, J. (2004). The Theory and Practice of Training (5th edition).

London: Kogan Page.

Page 92: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

91

Burnette, J. L. (2010). Implicit theories of body weight: Entity beliefs can weigh

you down. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 410-422.

Button, S. B., Mathieu, J. E., & Zajac, D. M. (1996). Goal orientation in

organizational research: A conceptual and empirical foundation.

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67, 26-48.

Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS: Basic Concepts,

Applications, and Programming. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cannon-Bowers, J.A., Rhodenizer, L., Salas, E. & Bowers, C.A. (1998). A

framework for understanding prepractice conditions and their impact on

learning. Personnel Psychology, 51, 291-320.

Carr, J. & DeShon, R. P., & Dobbins, H. W. (2001). A process model of goal

orientation. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Industrial

and Organizational Psychology, San Diego.

Carter, R. L. (2006). Solutions for missing data in structural equation modeling.

Research and Practice in Assessment, 1 (1), 1-6.

Cheung, G. W. & Lau, R. S. (2008). Testing mediation and suppression effects of

latent variables: Bootstrapping with structural equation models.

Organizational Research Methods, 11 (2), 296-325.

Chorpita, B. F. & Barlow, D. H. (1998). The development of anxiety: The role f

control in early environment. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 3-21.

Cohen, J. (1983). The cost of dichotomization. Applied psychological

measurement, 7 (3), 249-253.

Cortina, J. M. & Gully, S. M. (1999). So the great dragon was cast out . . . who

deceives the whole world. Newsletter of the Research Methods Division of the

Academy of Management, 14 (1).

Page 93: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

92

Cury, F., Da Fonseca, D., Zahn, I. & Elliot, A. (2008). Implicit theories and IQ

test performance: A sequential mediational analysis. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 783-791.

Davey, A., Savla, J., & Luo, Z. (2005). Issues in evaluating model fit with

missing data. Structural Equation Modeling, 12, 578–597.

DiBartolo, P. M., Frost, R. O., Dixon, A., & Almodovar, S. (2001). Can cognitive

restructuring reduce the disruption associated with perfectionistic

concerns? Behavior Therapy, 32, 167-184

Dollard, M. F. & Bakker, A. B. (2010). Psychosocial safety climate as a precursor

to conducive work environments, psychological health problems, and

employee engagement. Journal of Occupational and Organizational

Psychology, 83, 579-599.

Duda, J. L. (1989). Relationship between task and ego orientation and the

perceived purpose of sport among high school athletes. Journal of Sport

and Exercise, 11 (3), 318-335.

Duda, J. L. (2001). Achievement goal research in sport: Pushing the boundaries

and clarifying some misunderstandings. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances

in motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 129-182). Champaign: Human

Kinetics.

Dupeyrat, C. & Mariné, C. (2005). Implicit theories of intelligence, goal

orientation, cognitive engagement, and achievement: A test of Dweck’s

model with returning to school adults. Contemporary Educational

Psychology, 30, 43-59.

Duttweiler, P. C. (1984). The Internal Control Index: A newly developed measure

of locus of control. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 44, 209-

221.

Page 94: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

93

Dweck, C. S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality and

development. Hove: Psychology Press.

Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York:

Ballantyne Books.

Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in

judgments and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological

Inquiry, 6(4), 267-285.

Dweck, C. S. & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation

and achievement. Journal of personality and Social Psychology, 54, 5-12.

Dwyer, K. K. & Davidson, M. M. (2012). Is public speaking really more feared

than death? Communication Research Reports, 29 (2), 99-107.

Elliot, E. S. & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and

achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 5-12.

Elliot, A. J. & Harackiewicz, J. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement

goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 70 (3), 968-980.

Elliot, A. J. & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2*2 achievement goal framework.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501-519.

Enders, C. K. (2001a). A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms available for

use with missing data. Structural Equation Modeling, 8 (1), 128-141.

Enders, C. K. (2001b). The impact of nonnormality on full information

maximum-likelihood estimation for structural equation models with

missing data. Psychological Methods, 20, 352-370.

Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied Missing Data Analysis. New York: Guilford

Publications.

Page 95: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

94

Enders, C. K. & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The Relative Performance of Full

Information Maximum Likelihood Estimation for Missing Data in

Structural Equation Models. Structural Equation Modeling: A

Multidisciplinary Journal, 8 (3), 430-457

Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate

practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review,

100(3), 363-406.

Essau, C. A., Conradt, J. & Petermann, F. (1999). Frequency and comorbidity of

social phobia and social fears in adolescents. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 37 (9), 831-843.

Fisher, S. L., & Ford, J. K. (1998). Differential effects of learner effort and goal

orientation on two learning outcomes. Personnel Psychology, 51, 397-420.

Funder, D. C. (2001). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 197-221.

Garofano, C. M., & Salas, E. (2005). What influences continuous employee

development decisions. Human Resource Management Review, 15(4),

281-304.

Goodman, S. H. & Waters, L. K. (1987). Convergent validity of five locus of

control scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 47, 743-747.

Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world.

Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 549–576.

Grant, H., & Dweck, C. S. (2003). Clarifying achievement goals and their impact.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 541–553.

Hafsteinsson, L. G., Donovan, J. J., & Breland, T. B. (2007). An item response

theory examination of two popular measures of goal orientation measures.

Educational and Psychological Measurement, 67 (4), 719-739.

Page 96: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

95

Haimovitz, K., Wormington, S. V. & Corpus, J. H. (2011). Dangerous mindsets:

How beliefs about intelligence predict motivational changes. Learning and

Individual Differences, 21, 747-752.

Hair, J., Black, W., Babin, B., and Anderson, R. (2008). Multivariate data

analysis (7th ed.): Harlow: Pearson Education International

Hardy, L. Jones, G. & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding Psychological

Preparation for Sport: Theory and Practice of Elite Performers. London:

John Wiley & Sons.

Hastings, E. C. & West, R. L. (2011). Goal orientation and self-efficacy in

relation to memory in adulthood. Neuropsychology and Cognition, 18 (4),

471-493.

Heslin, P. A. (2010). Mindsets and employee engagement: Theoretical linkages

and practical interventions. In S. Albrecht, The Handbook of employee

engagement: Perspectives, issues, research and practice (pp. 218-226).

Cheltenham: Edwin Elgar.

Heslin, P. A., & Latham, G. P. (2004). The effect of upward feedback on

managerial behaviour. Applied Psychology: An International Review,

53(1), 23-37.

Heslin, P. A., Latham, G. P., & VandeWalle, D. (2005). The effect of implicit

personality theory on performance appraisals. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 90(5), 842-856.

Heslin, P. A., VandeWalle, D., & Latham, G. P. (2006). Keen to help? Manager's

implicit person theories and their subsequent employee coaching.

Personnel Psychology, 59, 871-902.

Page 97: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

96

Hill, T., Smith, N. D., & Mann, M. F. (1987). Role of efficacy expectations in

predicting the decision to use advanced technologies: The case of

computers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72 (2), 307-313.

Hong, Y., Chiu, C, Dweck, C. S., Lin, D. M. S., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit

theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 588-599.

Howell, A. J. & Buro, K. (2009). Implicit beliefs, achievement goal, and

procrastination: A mediational analysis. Learning and Individual

Differences, 19 (2), 151-154.

Hsieh, P., Sullivan, J. R. & Guerra, N. S. (2007). A closer look at college

students: Self-efficacy and goal orientation. Journal of Advanced

Academics, 18 (3), 454-476.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling:

Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological

Methods, 3, 424–453.

Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance

structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives.

Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.

Iacobucci, D. (2009). Everything you always wanted to know about SEM

(structural equations modeling) but were afraid to ask. Journal of

Consumer Psychology. 19, 673–680.

Iacobucci D (2010). Structural equations modeling: Fit indices, sample size, and

advanced topics. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20, 90–98.

Jöreskog, K. G. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K. A. Bollen & J.

S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Page 98: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

97

Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8: User’s reference guide.

Chicago: Scientific Software.

Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluation traits self-

esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability

with job satisfaction and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 86, 80-92.

Judge, T. A., Erez, A. & Bono, J. E. (1998). The power of being positive: The

relation between positive self-concept and job performance. Human

Performance. 11 (2), 167-187.

Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E. & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). Are measures of self-

esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy

indicators of a common core construct? Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 83 (3), 693-710.

Kaplan, D. (2000). Structural equation modeling: Foundations and extensions.

Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York: Norton.

Kennett, D. J. & Keefer, K. (2006). Impact of learned resourcefulness and

theories of intelligence on academic achievement of university students:

An integrated approach. Educational Psychology: An International

Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology, 26 (3), 441-457.

King, R. B. (2012). How you think about your intelligence influences how

adjusted you are: Implicit theories and adjustment outcomes. Personality

and Individual Differences, 53, 705-709.

Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling (2nd

ed.). London: The Guilford Press.

Page 99: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

98

Kornilova, T. V., Kornilov, S. A. & Chumakova, M. A. (2009). Subjective

evaluations of intelligence and academic self-concept predict academic

achievement: Evidence from a selective student population. Learning and

Individual Differences, 19 (4), 596-608.

Kozlowski, S. J., Gully, S. M., Brown, K. G., Salas, E., Smith, E. M., & Nason, E.

R. (2001). Effects of training goals and goal orientation traits on

multidimensional training outcomes and performance adaptability.

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 85, 1-31.

Kuha, J. (2004). AIC and BIC: Comparisons of Assumptions and Performance.

Sociological Methods and Research. (33) 2, 188-229.

Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal and Coping. New York:

Springer.

Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 371-375.

Lefcourt, H. M. (1991). Locus of control. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver,& L. S.

Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological

attitudes. San Diego: Academic Press.

Lei, M. & Lomax, R. G. (2005). The effect of varying degrees of nonnormality in

structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 12 (1), 1-27.

Leroy, N., Bressoux, P., Sarrazin, P., Trouilloud, D. (2007). Impact of teachers’

implicit theories and perceived pressures on the establishment of an

autonomy supportive climate. European Journal of Psychology of

Education, 22 (4), 529-545.

Levenson, H. (1974). Distinctions within the concept of internal-external control.

Journal of Personality Assessment, 36, 377-383.

Page 100: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

99

Levenson, H. (1981). Differentiating among internality, powerful others, and

chance. In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.). Research with the locus of control

construct. New York: Academic Press.

Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J. & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Stereotype formation and

endorsement: The role of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 74 (6), 1421-1436.

Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G. & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To

parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the merits.

Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9 (2), 151-

173.

Luthar, S (1991). Vulnerability and resilience: A study of high risk adolescents.

Child Development. 62, 599-616.

MacCallum, R. C., Roznowski, M., Necowitz, L.B. (1992). Model modifications

in covariance structure analysis: the problem of capitalization on chance.

Psychological Bulletin, 111 (3), 490-504.

MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J. & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the

practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological

Methods, 7, 19-40.

Mangels, J. A., Butterfield, B., Lamb, J., Good, C. D., & Dweck, C. S. (2006).

Why do beliefs about intelligence influence learning success? A social-

cognitive-neuroscience model. Social, Cognitive and Affective

Neuroscience, 1, 75-86.

Marsh, H. W. (1996). Positive and negative global self-esteem: A substantively

meaningful distinction or artifacts? Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 70 (4), 810-819.

Page 101: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

100

Marsh, H. W. & Hau, K. T. (1999). Confirmatory factor analysis: Strategies for

small sample sizes. In R.H. Hoyle (ed.), Statistical Strategies for Small

Sample Research. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Martocchio, J. J. (1994). Effects of conceptions of ability, and academic

achievement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 819-825.

Midgeley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. J. (2001). Performance-approach

goals: Good for what, for whom, under what circumstances, and at what

cost? Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 77-86

Miller, R. B., Behrens, J. T., & Greene, B. A. (1993). Goals and perceived ability:

Impact on student valuing, self-regulation, and persistence. Contemporary

Educational Psychology, 18, 2–14.

Molden, D. C. & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in psychology: A lay

theories approach to self-regulation, social perception and social

development. American Psychologist, 61 (3), 192-203.

Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can undermine

children's motivation and performance. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 75 (1), 33-52.

Murphy, M. C. & Dweck, C. S. (2010). A culture of genius: How an

organization's lay theory shapes people's cognition, affect and behavior.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36 (3), 283-296.

Muthen, B., Kaplan, D. & Hollis, M. (1987). On structural equation modeling

with data that are not missing completely at random. Psychometrika, 51,

431- 462.

Nasser, F. & Wisenbaker, J. (2003). A Monte Carlo study investigating the impact

of item parceling on measures of fit in confirmatory factor analysis.

Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63, 729–757.

Page 102: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

101

Newman, D. A. (2003). Missing data techniques and low response rates. In C. E.

Lance & R. J. Vandenberg (Eds.), Statistical and Methodologial Myths

and Urban Legends. London: Routledge

Ng, T. W. H., Sorensen, K. L. & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of control at work: A

meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 1057-1087.

Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability,

subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological

Review, 91, 328–346.

Noe, R. A. (1988). Women and mentoring: A review and research agenda.

Academy of Management Review, 13, 65-78.

Nussbaum, A. D., & Dweck, C. S. (2008). Defensiveness vs. remediation: Self-

theories and modes of self-esteem maintenance. Personality and Social

Psychology Bulletin, 34, 127-134.

Olsson, U. H., Foss, T., Troye, S. V. & Howell, R. D. (2000). The performance of

ML, GLS, and WLS estimation in structural equation modeling under

conditions of misspecification and nonnormality. Structural Equation

Modeling, 7 (4), 557–595.

Ommundsen, Y. (2001). Self-handicapping strategies in physical education

classes; the influence of implicit theories or the nature of ability and

achievement goal orientations. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 2, 139-

156.

Oreg, S. (2003). Resistance to change: Developing an individual differences

measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88 (4), 587–604.

Ormrod, J. E. (2006). Educational Psychology: Developing Learners (5th edition).

Harlow: Pearson Education International.

Page 103: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

102

Payne, C. P., Youngcourt, S. S. & Beaubien, J. M. (2007). A meta-analytic

examination of the goal orientation nomological net. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 92 (1), 128-150.

Phillips. J. M. & Gully, S. M. (1997). Role of goal orientation, need for

achievement, and locus of control in the self-efficacy and goal-setting

process. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83 (5), 792-802.

Pintrich, P. R. & de Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning

components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 81, 33-40.

Plotnikoff, R. C., Lipke, S., Courneya, K. S., Birkett, N., & Sigal, R. J. (2008).

Physical activity and social cognitive theory: A test in a population sample

of adults with type 1 or type 2 diabetes. Applied Psychology: An

International Review, 57 (4), 628-643.

Porath, C., Spreitzer, G., Gibson, C. & Garnett, F. G. (2012). Thriving at work:

Towards its measurement, construct validation, and theoretical refinement.

Journal of Organizational Behavior, 33, 250-275.

Quinones, M. A., Ford, J. K., Sego, D. J. & Smith E. M. (1995). The effects of

individual and transfer environment characteristics on the opportunity to

perform trained tasks. Training Research Journal, 1, 29-48.

Robins, R. W. & Pals, J. L. (2002). Implicit self-theories in the academic domain:

Implications for goal orientations, attributions, affect, and self-esteem

change. Self and Identity, 1 (4), 313-336.

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control

of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80(1), 1-28.

Roedel, T. D. & Schraw, G. (1995). Beliefs about intelligence and academic

goals. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 464-468.

Page 104: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

103

Sanderson, W. C., Rapee, R. M.,& Barlow, D. H. (1989). The influence of an

illusion of control on panic attacks induced via inhalation of 5.5% carbon

dioxide-enriched air. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 157–162.

Schneider, B., White, S. S. & Paul, M. C. (1998). Linking service climate and

customer service perceptions of service quality: Test of a causal model.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 83 (2), 150-163.

Schreiber, J. B., Amaury, N., Stage, F. K., Barlow, E. A. & King, J. (2006).

Reporting structural equation modelling and confirmatory factor analysis

results: A review. Journal of Educational Research, 99 (6), 323-338.

Schuurman, M. K., Alley, M., Marshall, M. D. & Johnstone, C. L. (2008). The

Effect of a Targeted Speech Communication Course on the Public

Speaking Self-Efficacy of Engineering Undergraduates. Proceedings of

the Annual Conference of American Society for Engineering Education,

June 2008, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania.

Seifert, T. L, & O’Keefe, B. A. (2001). The relationship of work avoidance and

learning goals to perceived competence, externality and meaning. British

Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 81-92.

Seligman, M. (1975). Helplessness: On Depression, Development and Death. San

Francisco: Freeman.

Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology. 71 (3), 549-570.

Smith, R. E. (1989). Effects of coping skills training on generalized self-efficacy

and locus of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,

228–233.

Spector, P. E. (1988). Development of the Work Locus of Control scale, Journal

of Occupational Psychology, 61, 335-340.

Page 105: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

104

Spinath, B., Spinath, F. M., Riemann, R. & Angleitner, A. (2003). Implicit

theories about personality and intelligence and their relationship to actual

personality and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 35,

939-951.

Stein, M. B., Walker, J, F., & Forde, D. R. (1996). Public speaking fears in a

community sample: Prevalence, impact on functioning, and diagnostic

classification. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53,169-174.

Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The rainbow project: Enhancing the SAT through

assessments of analytical, practical, and creative skills. Intelligence, 34,

321-350.

Stevens, C. K. & Gist, M. E. (1997). Effects of self-efficacy training and goal

orientation training on negotiation skill maintenance: What are the

mechanisms? Personnel Psychology, 50 (4), 955-978.

Stipek, D. & Gralinski, J. H. (1996). Children’s beliefs about intelligence and

school performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88 (3), 397-407.

Sue-Chan, C. & Wood, R. E. (2009). The moderating effect of trainee implicit

beliefs on the relationship between cognitive modelling orientation and

training outcomes. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 58 (2),

304-335.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics (5 ed.).

London: Pearson Education.

Tabernero, C. & Wood, R. E. (1999). Implicit theories versus the social construal

of ability in self-regulation and performance on a complex task.

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 104-127.

Page 106: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

105

Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N. & Rothstein, M. (1991). Personality measures as

predictors of job performance: A meta-analytic review. Personnel

Psychology, 44, 703-742.

Thompson, S. C. & Spacapan, S. (1991). Perceptions of control in vulnerable

populations. Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 293-304.

Ullman, J. B. (2006). Structural equation modeling: Reviewing the basics and

moving forward. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87 (1), 35-50.

Ullman, J. B. (2007). Structural equation modeling. In B. G. Tabachnick & L. S.

Fidell (Eds.), Using Multivariate Statistics (5th ed.). London: Pearson

Education Inc.

Utman, C. H. (1997). Performance effects of motivational state: A meta-analysis.

Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 170–182.

Van Vianen, A. E., Dalhoeven, B. A., & De Pater, I. E. (2011). Aging and

training and development willingness: Employee and supervisor mindsets.

Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32, 226-247.

VandeWalle, D. (1997). Development and validation of a work domain goal

orientation instrument. Educational and Psychological Measurement,

57(6), 995-1015.

VandeWalle, D. & Cummings, L. I. (1997). A test of the influence of goal

orientation in the feedback seeking process. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 82, 390-400.

VandeWalle, D., Brown, S. P., Cron, W. L. & Slocum, J. W. (1999). The

influence of goal orientation and self-regulation tactics on sales

performance: A longitudinal field test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84

(2), 249-250.

Page 107: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

106

VandeWalle, D., Cron, W. L. & Slocum, J. W. (2001). The role of goal

orientation following performance feedback. Journal of Applied Social

Psychology, 86 (4), 629-640.

Walkey, F. H. (1979). Internal control, powerful others, and chance: A

confirmation of Levenson’s factor structure. Journal of Personality

Assessment,43, 532–535.

Wallston, K. A., & Wallston, B. S. (1978). Development of the Multidimensional

Health Locus of Control (MHLC) scales. Health Education Monographs,

6, 160-170.

Wiggins, J. S. (1996). The Five-Factor Model of Personality: Theoretical

Perspectives. New York: Guildford.

Wilcox, R. R. (1997). How many discoveries have been lost by ignoring modern

statistical methods? American Psychologist, 53, 300-314.

Wothke, W. (2000). Longitudinal and multi-group modeling with missing data. In

T. D. Little, K. U. Schnabel, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Modeling longitudinal

and multiple group data: Practical issues, applied approaches and

specific examples. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Wood, R. & Bandura, A. (1989). Impact of conceptions of ability on self-

regulatory mechanisms and complex decision making. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 56 (3), 407-415.

Yuan, K. H., Wallentin, F. & Bentler, P. M. (in press). ML versus MI for missing

data with violation of distribution conditions. Sociological Methods &

Research.

Ziegler, A., & Stoeger, H. (2010). Research on a modified framework of implicit

personality theories. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 318-326.

Page 108: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

107

Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining Self-Regulation: a social cognitive

perspective, in M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, & M. Zeodmer, (eds.).

Handbook of Self-Regulation. San Diego: Academic Press.

Zimmerman, B. J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Self-motivation for

academic attainment: The role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal

setting. American Educational Research Journal, 29, 663-676.

Page 109: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

108

7.0 Appendices

7.1 Appendix A: Questionnaire

Life is filled with challenges, but what is it about each of us that affects the way we approach them? This research study focuses on how beliefs about our own skills and abilities can influence the way we engage with one such challenge: public speaking.

My name is John Hudson and I am carrying out this research project as part of a Masters degree at the University of Manchester. If you would like to help with this research, you can complete this short questionnaire which should take around 5-10 minutes.

You must be 18 years or over to participate. Participation is voluntary and we do not ask for your name, contact information or any personally identifiable information, so confidentiality and anonymity are assured. Thank you for your help John

Section 1: Background details

1. Gender: female male

2. Age: __________ years

3. Please tick your highest qualification or its equivalent:

Secondary school to age 16

Non-university higher education

Postgraduate university education

Secondary school to age 18

Undergraduate university education

No formal qualifications

4. Which of the following best describes your current employment status? Employed, full time Self employed Unemployed/not currently

working

Employed, part-time Student Retired

a. If you are currently in employment, which of the following best describes your current occupation?

Administrative

Junior professional/ managerial

Semi-skilled

Service Senior professional/ managerial

Skilled

Page 110: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

109

Public speaking and you

Are you a member of a Toastmasters public speaking club?

Yes � No � (if no, please go to section 2)

How long have you been a member of Toastmasters? _______years_______months

Please indicate the number of times you taken the following roles in the last SIX MONTHS

Prepared speech from the Toastmasters manual

Speech evaluator

Timekeeper

Grammarian

Table topics speaker

Table topics master

General evaluator

Toastmaster

Page 111: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

110

Section 2:

The following questions require you to record the extent to which you agree or disagree with statements. Please give your honest opinion, by circling the number that applies to you, where:

1 = totally disagree through to 7 = totally agree

If you change your mind about an answer, please put a cross over your original answer and circle a new number.

1 Whether or not I get to be a leader depends mostly on my ability. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

2 Whether or not I get into a car accident depends mostly on how good a driver I am. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

3 When I make plans, I am almost certain to make them work. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

4 How many friends I have depends on how nice a person I am 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

5 I can pretty much determine what will happen in my life. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

6 I am usually able to protect my personal interests 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

7 When I get what I want, it’s usually because I worked hard for it. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8 My life is determined by my own actions. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1 = totally disagree through to 7 = totally agree

1 The kind of person you are is something very basic about you and it can’t be changed very much.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

2 You can do things differently, but the important part of who you are can’t really be changed

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

3 You can significantly change your behaviour and characteristics 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

4 As much as I hate to admit it, you can’t teach an old dog new tricks. You can’t really change your deepest attributes

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

5 You can always substantially change the kind of person you are. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

6 You are a certain type of person and there is not much that can be done to really change that.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

7 No matter what kind of person you are, you can always change it a lot.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8 You can change even your most basic qualities. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Page 112: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

1 = totally disagree through to 7 = totally agree

Public speaking

For the final section, please select your level of confidence for each of the statements that complete the sentence below: -

“When speaking in front of an audience, I am [insert level of confidence] that I can…”

1 = Not at all confident 7 = Completely confident

1 ...make an effective presentation. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 2 ....develop a presentation with good content. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 3 ...keep the audience engaged. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 4 ...connect well to an audience that is familiar with the topic I

present. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

5 ...connect well to an audience that is not familiar with the topic I present.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

6 ...deliver a well-organized presentation. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 7 ...design effective visual aids. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 ...deliver effectively (eye contact, use of voice, movements, etc.). 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 9 ...give an effective summary of the information I presented. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 10 ...answer questions from the audience effectively. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Thank you Thank you for supporting this research project. Your participation is much

appreciated

1 I am willing to select a challenging work assignment that I can learn a lot from.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

2 I often look for opportunities to develop new skills and knowledge.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

3 I enjoy challenging and difficult tasks at work where I’ll learn new skills.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

4 For me, development of my ability is important enough to take risks.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

5 I prefer to work in situations that require a high level of ability and talent.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

6 I would avoid taking on a new task if there was a chance that I would appear rather incompetent to others.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

7 Avoiding a show of low ability is more important to me than learning a new skill.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8 I’m concerned about taking on a task at work if my performance would reveal that I had low ability.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

9 I prefer to avoid situations at work where I might perform poorly.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Page 113: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

112

7.2 Appendix B: Missing value analysis and demographic details between groups with full and missing datasets

Table 7.1: comparison of demographic variables between cases with missing/non-missing data for dependent variables

Complete data n = 81

Missing data n = 80

% of complete subsample

% of missing subsample

Gender Female 44 52 54.3% 65.0% Male 37 28 45.7% 35.0%

Age

Under 20 years old 0 2 0.0% 2.5% 20 – 29 years 15 27 18.5% 33.8% 30 – 39 years 36 30 44.4% 37.5% 40 – 49 years 13 10 16.0% 12.5% 50 – 59 years 8 5 9.9% 6.3% 60 – 69 years 7 5 8.6% 6.3% 70 – 79 years 1 1 1.2% 1.3% 80+ years 1 0 1.2% 0.0%

Mean age 39.63 36.12 - -

Employment

Full time 57 34 70.3% 42.5% Part time 3 10 3.7% 12.5% Self-employed 9 12 11.1% 15.0% Student 2 19 2.5% 23.8% Unemployed 5 0 6.2% 0.0% Retired 3 3 3.7% 3.8%

Missing data 2 2 2.5% 2.5%

Occupational level

Admin 7 5 8.6% 6.25% Service 6 7 7.4% 8.8% Skilled 5 7 6.2% 8.8% Semi-skilled 2 0 3.7% 0.0% Junior mgmt. 21 6 25.9% 7.5% Senior mgmt. 26 31 32.1% 38.8%

Missing data 4 2 4.9% 2.5%

Educational Level

No qualifications 0 0 0.0% 0.0% Secondary to 16 4 3 4.9% 3.8% Secondary to 18 1 3 1.2% 3.8% Non-uni higher ed. 11 14 13.6% 17.5% Undergrad. uni ed. 29 20 35.8% 25.0% Postgrad 34 38 42.0% 47.5%

Missing data 2 2 2.5% 2.5%d

Page 114: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

113

Table 7.2: SPSS missing value analysis showing differences in mean scores on independent variables between groups providing complete and incomplete data

Separate Variance t Testsa

Memb

period

Spk

Practice

SE_

mean LoC_mean Mind_mean LGO_mean AGO_mean

Memb

period

t . -.4 4.0 2.5 2.6 1.1 -2.4

df . 1.0 152.3 158.5 159.0 158.4 157.3

P(2-tail) . .749 .000 .012 .011 .252 .019

# Present 79 79 79 79 79 79 79

# Missing 0 2 82 82 82 82 82

Mean(Present) 47.99 17.03 54.4684 25.9045 37.2278 28.4557 12.6582

Mean(Missing) . 27.00 48.0854 24.4268 33.5610 27.6463 14.5122

Spk

practice

t . . 3.9 2.5 2.6 1.2 -2.6

df . . 148.3 157.7 158.7 159.0 158.6

P(2-tail) . . .000 .015 .011 .225 .010

# Present 79 81 81 81 81 81 81

# Missing 0 0 80 80 80 80 80

Mean(Present) 47.99 17.27 54.3580 25.8698 37.1852 28.4691 12.5926

Mean(Missing) . . 48.0375 24.4250 33.5125 27.6125 14.6250

For each quantitative variable, pairs of groups are formed by indicator variables (present, missing).

a. Indicator variables with less than 5% missing are not displayed.

Page 115: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

114

7.3 Appendix C: Confirmatory factor analyses on individual scales and parcels

Avoidance goal orientation CFA

AGO items 1 I would avoid taking on a new task if there was a chance that I would appear rather

incompetent to others. 2 Avoiding a show of low ability is more important to me than learning a new skill.

3 I’m concerned about taking on a task at work if my performance would reveal that I had low ability.

4 I prefer to avoid situations at work where I might perform poorly.

AGO parcels

AGO

.42

avoid1 e23.65 .41

avoid2 e24.64

.89

avoid3 e25.94

.72

avoid4 e26

.85

AGO

.50

AGO 1, 2 e24.71

.93

AGO 3 e25.96

.70

AGO 4 e26

.84

Page 116: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

115

Learning goal orientation CFA

LGO items 1 I am willing to select a challenging work assignment that I can learn a lot from.

2 I often look for opportunities to develop new skills and knowledge.

3 I enjoy challenging and difficult tasks at work where I’ll learn new skills.

4 For me, development of my ability is important enough to take risks. 5 I prefer to work in situations that require a high level of ability and talent.

LGO parcels

LGO

.65

learn1 e1

.81.81

learn2 e2.90

.68

learn3 e3.83

.53

learn4 e4

.73

.22

learn5 e5

.47

LGO

.74

LGO 2 e1.86

.59

LGO 3, 5 e2.76

.86

LGO 1, 4 e3

.93

Page 117: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

116

Mindset CFA

Mindset parcels

Mindset

.66

Mind1r e1

.81

.64

Mind2r e2

.80

.44

Mind4r e3

.66 .63

Mind6r e4.79

.29

mind3 e5

.54

.59

mind5 e6

.77

.57

mind7 e7

.76

.66

mind8 e8

.81

F1

.76

Mindset 1r, 3, 6r e1

.87

.81

Mindset 8, 4r, 5 e2.90

.86

Mindset 2r, 7 e3

.93

Mindset items (* items 1, 2, 4 & 6 reverse scored) 1* The kind of person you are is something very basic about you and it can’t be changed very much. 2* You can do things differently, but the important part of who you are can’t really be changed 4* As much as I hate to admit it, you can’t teach an old dog new tricks. You can’t really change your

deepest attributes 6* You are a certain type of person and there is not much that can be done to really change that. 3 You can significantly change your behaviour and characteristics 5 You can always substantially change the kind of person you are. 7 No matter what kind of person you are, you can always change it a lot.

8 You can change even your most basic qualities.

Page 118: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

117

Locus of control CFA

Locus of control parcels

.01

LoC1 e1.09

Loc2 e2.25

Loc3 e3.11

Loc4 e4.47

Loc5 e5.41

Loc6 e6.25

LoC7 e7.24

Loc8 e8

LoC

.50

.34

.68

.64

.50

.48

.30

.11

LoC

.43

LoC_6 e1.66

.40

LoC_3_8 e2.64

.61

LoC_5_7 e3

.78

LoC items (* items 1, 2, & 4 not included in parcels) 1* Whether or not I get to be a leader depends mostly on my ability. 2* Whether or not I get into a car accident depends mostly on how good a driver I am. 3 When I make plans, I am almost certain to make them work. 4* How many friends I have depends on how nice a person I am 5 I can pretty much determine what will happen in my life. 6 I am usually able to protect my personal interests 7 When I get what I want, it’s usually because I worked hard for it. 8 My life is determined by my own actions.

Page 119: CAN’T LEARN, WON’T LEARN? AN ... - University of Salford

118

Self-efficacy CFA

PSSE scale 1 ...make an effective presentation. 2 ....develop a presentation with good content. 3 ...keep the audience engaged. 4 ...connect well to an audience that is familiar with the topic I present. 5 ...connect well to an audience that is not familiar with the topic I present. 6 ...deliver a well-organized presentation. 7 ...design effective visual aids. 8 ...deliver effectively (eye contact, use of voice, movements, etc.). 9 ...give an effective summary of the information I presented. 10 ...answer questions from the audience effectively. PSSE parcels

PSSE

.73

SE1 e1

.86

.59

SE2 e2

.77

.80

SE3 e3

.90

.75

SE4 e4.87 .66

SE5 e5.81

.24

SE7 e7

.49

.67

SE8 e8

.82

.62

SE9 e9

.78

.42

SE10 e10

.65

.56

SE6 e6.75

PSSE

.87

SE 3, 7, 8, 2 e1.93

.75

SE 4, 10, 5 e2.87

.82

SE 1, 6, 9 e3

.91