bennett 2005 factors associated with student plagiarism in a post 1992 university

Upload: jorge-lima

Post on 06-Jul-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    1/27

    Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found athttp://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=caeh20

    Download by: [b-on: Biblioteca do conhecimento online UAC] Date: 12 April 2016, At: 08:00

    Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education

    ISSN: 0260-2938 (Print) 1469-297X (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/caeh20

    Factors associated with student plagiarism in apost‐1992 university

    Roger Bennett

    To cite this article: Roger Bennett (2005) Factors associated with student plagiarism in a

    post‐1992 university, Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 30:2, 137-162, DOI:10.1080/0260293042000264244

    To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0260293042000264244

    Published online: 14 Sep 2010.

    Submit your article to this journal

    Article views: 876

    View related articles

    Citing articles: 38 View citing articles

    http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/0260293042000264244#tabModulehttp://www.tandfonline.com/doi/citedby/10.1080/0260293042000264244#tabModulehttp://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/0260293042000264244http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/0260293042000264244http://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCode=caeh20&page=instructionshttp://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalCode=caeh20&page=instructionshttp://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0260293042000264244http://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.1080/0260293042000264244http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/caeh20http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=caeh20

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    2/27

     Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education

    Vol. 30, No. 2, April 2005, pp. 137–162

    ISSN 0260-2938 (print)/ISSN 1469-297X (online)/05/020137–26

    © 2005 Taylor & Francis Ltd

    DOI: 10.1080/0260293042000264244

    Factors associated with student

    plagiarism in a post-1992 university

    Roger Bennett*London Metropolitan University, UK TaylorandFrancisLtdCAEH300203.sgm10.1080/0260293042000264244Assessment&EvaluationinHigherEducation0260-2938(p rint)/1469-297X (online)OriginalArtic le2005Taylor&FrancisLtd302000000April [email protected] 

    A model intended to explain the incidence of plagiarism among undergraduates in the Business

    Studies department of a post-1992 university was constructed and tested on a sample of 249

    students completing Business Studies units at a post-1992 university in Greater London. It was

    hypothesised that the occurrence of plagiarism could be predicted via three attitudinal consider-

    ations (individual perceptions of the ethicality of the practice, fear of penalties if caught and fear of 

    failing a degree); two personal traits (goal orientation and academic integration), and three situa-

    tional factors (financial, current grades achieved, and how strictly lecturing staff enforced anti-

    plagiarism rules). Proposals concerning possible inter-relations and interactions among these

    variables were also formulated and assessed.

    Introduction

    Most university teachers discourage students from engaging in plagiarism on the

    grounds that the practice is fraudulent and deceptive, involves the theft of intellectual

    property, and ‘conceals and misrepresents the originality of the true author’ (Clough,

    2003, p. 2). Allegedly, moreover, plagiarism undermines the moral fibre of the perpe-

    trator (people who admitted to cheating while at university have been found to be

    more likely to commit dishonest acts in employment—Sims, 1995), and inhibits the

    student’s intellectual development (Yeung et al., 2002). The habitual plagiarist does

    not acquire the academic skills of analysis and evaluation, and will not learn how to

    synthesise ideas or engage in rational argument. Instead the person simply replicates

    the words of others without adding anything that is new.

     Nature and incidence of plagiarism in higher education

    Hannabuss (2001) defined plagiarism as ‘the unauthorised use or close imitation of 

    the ideas and language/expression of someone else’ and then the representation of this

    *Department of Business and Service Sector Management, London Metropolitan University, 84

    Moorgate, London EC2M 6SQ, UK. Email: [email protected].

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    3/27

    138 R. Bennett 

    work as the plagiarist’s own (p. 313). In the context of university education, however,

    plagiarism does not have a single meaning and can range from the citation of a few

    sentences without attribution through to the copying out of an entire manuscript.

    Myers (1998) noted how plagiarism in the academic world ‘exists not in law as copy-

    right does, but as institutional  rules and regulations’ (p. 2). Hence, conventions relat-ing to what does and does not constitute plagiarism are formulated and interpreted

    differently across institutions. Nevertheless, common themes emerge in most exposi-

    tions of the construct, usually involving the notions of intent, deliberate deception and

    failure to acknowledge sources (see Larkham & Manns, 2002, p. 340 for details).

    As plagiarism covers a sizeable continuum ranging from ‘sloppy paraphrasing to

    verbatim transcription with no crediting of the source’ (Larkham & Manns, 2002, p.

    340), a number of authors have sought to distinguish between less and more serious

    forms of the practice. ‘Minor plagiarism’ has been stated to comprise activities such

    as cutting and pasting relatively small amounts of material from web pages without

    acknowledgement (Davis, 2000), the reproduction of a sentence or two without

    quotation marks and without a citation (Standler, 2000), paraphrasing without refer-

    ences, and inventing fictitious references (Bjorklund & Wenestam (1999). Major

    plagiarism, according to Standler (2000) occurs when ‘a significant fraction of the

    entire work was written by someone else’ (p. 2). Standler (2000) noted however that

    there was no legal  distinction between major and minor forms of plagiarism. This was

    unfortunate, Standler (2000) continued, because differences in the penalties imposed

    for various levels of plagiarism could be very large, ranging from a mild rebuke to

    permanent exclusion from an institution. Another explanation for researchers’ inter-

    est in the distinction between major and minor plagiarism has been the observationof a substantially greater willingness among students to perpetrate minor as opposed

    to major plagiarism (see Kuehn et al., 1990; Franklyn-Stokes & Newstead, 1995;

    McCabe & Trevino, 1996; Newstead et al., 1996; Bjorklund & Wenestam, 1999;

    Davis, 2000). Possibly, students’ internal rationales for committing serious acts of 

    academic dishonesty differ from those resulting in minor offences (Kuehn et al.,

    1990). Bjorklund and Wenestam (1999) suggested that minor plagiarism tended to

    be far more ‘opportunistic in nature than deliberately planned’ (p. 4).

    Irrespective of how plagiarism is defined, evidence appears to be emerging that its

    incidence has risen sharply within several western countries. In Australia, forinstance, an investigation completed at Monash and Swinburne universities found

    that nearly 80% of all the undergraduates and half the postgraduates surveyed were

    willing to confess to having plagiarised in one way or another during their time at

    college (Maslen, 2003). The most frequent forms of plagiarism were (1) the use of 

    unattributed material, and (2) the sharing of work with other students for assign-

    ments that were supposed to be completed individually (see also McCabe & Trevino,

    1996). Large increases in the prevalence of plagiarism have been documented in

    Scandinavia (Bjorklund & Wenestam, 1999), the United Kingdom (Larkham &

    Manns, 2002; Furedi, 2003) and the USA (Allen et al., 1998; McKenzie, 1998).

    Hammond’s (2002) survey of published estimates of rates of general academic cheat-ing (including plagiarism) in British universities between 1941 and 2001 revealed a

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    4/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 139

    systematic upward trend. In the 1940s around 20–25% of all students were routinely

    reported to have admitted to cheating in some way. By the 1990s the figure was typi-

    cally 60–65%. Trends of a similar magnitude specifically concerning plagiarism have

    been observed in other western nations (see Bjorklund & Wenestam, 1999, p. 4),

    though usually in relation to ‘non-serious’ offences (Franklyn-Stokes & Newstead,1995). Although the high figures currently cited for plagiarism may be due in part to

    a greater willingness of students to confess (rather than to an intensification of the

    practice per se —Bjorklund & Wenestam, 1999), the outcomes to a steadily expanding

    body of research literature suggest strongly that a substantial rise in university student

    plagiarism has in fact taken place. Some of this literature is explored in the next

    section.

    Why students plagiarise

    A major factor underlying the long-term upward trend in the incidence of plagiarism,

    according to Larkham and Manns (2002), Maslen (2003) and others (see Bjorklund

    & Wenestam, 1999), has been the introduction across the western academic world of 

    modularisation (and the increased sizes of teaching groups that it necessitates) in

    conjunction with heavy reliance on written assignments during the academic year

    (rather than end of unit examinations) as the primary means of student assessment.

    Furedi (2003) in particular attributed the rise in rates of plagiarism that (supposedly)

    have occurred in the UK to ‘the prevailing instrumentalist orientation’ that has come

    to dominate approaches to teaching in British universities (p. 16). Manifestations of 

    the instrumentalist approach included the failure of lecturers to expect students toread textbooks, teachers circulating verbatim lecture notes, and ‘bullet point’ presen-

    tations of material in lectures.

    At the level of the individual student, three categories of variables are often

    advanced to explain why certain individuals commit non-trivial plagiarism (assuming

    that the student fully understands the meaning of the term in the first instance). The

    three categories concern students’ personal circumstances, personal traits, and

    whether the means and opportunity to plagiarise are readily to hand.

     Means and opportunity

    It has been alleged that the widespread availability of access to (1) the Internet, and

    (2) online academic journals have contributed much to the rising incidence of plagia-

    rism, as they have made it possible for students to find and save large amounts of infor-

    mation from diverse sources with little reading, effort or originality (McKenzie,

    1998). Also there now exist numerous Internet sites (some of which are free of charge)

    that provide complete essay or term papers. Increasingly, the services of paid for sites

    are customised and thus difficult to detect using conventional anti-plagiarism web-

    crawling software (see Phillips & Horton, 2000). Maslen (2003) reported research

    which concluded that each year as many as 500,000 essays submitted by Australianstudents contained text improperly copied from the Internet. More specifically, a

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    5/27

    140 R. Bennett 

    study of 1751 assignments from students of 17 subjects in six Australian universities

    revealed that 400 web sources had been used without acknowledgement, including

    five known ‘cheat sites’. One in eight students had borrowed at least 25% of their

    essays from the Internet. It is relevant to note however that while the Internet

    undoubtedly facilitates plagiarism, it does not, in the words of Furedi (2003), ‘possessthe moral power to incite otherwise honest students to cheat’ (p. 16).

    Lack of enforcement by college lecturers of rules and procedures designed to

    prevent plagiarism could encourage students to indulge in the practice (Davis et al.,

    1992; Council of Writing Program Administrators (CWPA), 2003) and apathy

    among university faculty in this regard might be widespread (Davis et al., 1992;

    Phillips & Horton, 2000). Bjorklund and Wenestam (1999) cited evidence suggesting

    that around 20% of tutors ignore cases of obvious plagiarism, often because of the

    stress and discomfort likely to result from dealing with the matter. Overall detection

    rates, according to Bjorklund and Wenestam (1999), are typically less than 1.5%.

    Establishing proof of a particular case can be difficult and time-consuming, the

    lecturer involved may fear that he or she will not be supported by senior management

    (Hammond, 2002), and even when miscreants are caught the penalties might be

    trivial (Davis et al., 1992). Moreover, significant resource implications ensue from the

    decision to pursue plagiarists diligently, especially during an end-of-semester ‘hand-

    ing-in period’.

    Personal traits

    Internal beliefs that academic cheating is immoral and dishonest are known todiscourage plagiarism (Franklyn-Stokes & Newstead, 1995). Sutton and Huba

    (1995) found that highly religious students were less likely to behave dishonestly

    than others. However the strongest motive for cheating by students (according to

    Bjorklund and Wenestam’s [1999] review of the literature on the subject) is the

    desire to obtain high grades, which itself may depend on other considerations. Thus,

    for example, the dishonest pursuit of good marks could be due to a person’s obses-

    sive innate need to prove his or her worth to him or herself and/or to the world at

    large (Davis et al., 1992; Lipson & MacGraven, 1993; Newstead et al., 1996;

    Anderman et al., 1998), or to a pathological fear of failure (Baird, 1980; CWPA,2003). Haines et al. (1986) found that students with lower than average grade point

    averages were more likely to plagiarise than their better performing classmates. Indi-

    viduals who receive extensive financial support from their families and/or who are

    under great parental pressure to succeed might be especially fearful of failure (and

    hence be more inclined to plagiarise) (Haines et al., 1986). On the other hand,

    according to Phillips and Horton (2000), parental support for college expenses

    might have the capacity to ‘reduce the student’s personal involvement and responsi-

    bility for the educational process’ (p. 151), leading to less rather than more concern

    for sound academic performance.

    Houle (1961) noted how certain students attend university in consequence of theirhaving a heavy ‘goal orientation’, manifest in the desire to achieve specific utilitarian

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    6/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 141

    outcomes such as enhanced job prospects and career development (as opposed to

    wanting to study for the sheer enjoyment of learning). Also, ever-growing rates of 

    participation in higher education have generated a widening earnings gap between

    those with and without qualifications (Archer, 2002). Possibly, the incentive to take

    ‘short cuts’ in order to succeed is greater among people with a pronounced goalorientation. In this vein, Davis et al. (1995) reported some (albeit limited) evidence

    indicating that individuals whose behavioural and emotional styles were character-

    ised by the aggressive struggle to achieve more in less time (often in competition with

    other students) had a higher propensity to plagiarise than the rest of their contempo-

    raries.

    An intense fear of the punishment that a person could receive for plagiarism might

    exert a negative influence on the inclination to engage in the practice (Haines et al.,

    1986). However, Braumoeller and Gaines (2001) found that even the most dire

    explicit warnings of punishment failed to discourage plagiarism among a sample of 

    180 political science students. On the other hand, informing students that their work

    would be run through plagiarism detection software was a highly effective deterrent.

    (A commercial plagiarism detection web crawler can trawl through around three

    billion web pages and two or three thousand journals to check the similarity of a

    student’s materials with web page sources—see Maslen, 2003). The severity of the

    disruption to a student’s life caused by his or her losing a grade, by having to repeat

    a unit or year, or ultimately by being expelled from an institution will be greater

    perhaps for an individual who has already made heavy personal investments in

    attending a degree programme. Such investments could include the time and effort

    previously committed, as well as financial sacrifices.

    Individual circumstances

    Students who need to take paid employment to help finance their time at university

    have less time for study (Larkham & Manns, 2002), and high academic workloads

    may need to be compressed into their available study periods (Baird, 1980; Lipson &

    MacGovern, 1993; Newstead et al., 1996). Sanders (2000) cited the results of a

    MORI survey of 1068 students in 20 UK universities which found that the proportion

    of students employed part-time during term-time had increased from 30% in 2001 to43% in 2002 (the proportion was 25% higher than average among students from

    lower social classes). Arguably the time pressures created by the need for students to

    take paid employment, in conjunction with the current emphasis on written within-

    course assignments as the primary means of assessment, are likely to cause growing

    numbers of students to resort to plagiarism. Furedi (2003) rejected this argument,

    however, on the grounds that there was no evidence whatsoever that financially disad-

    vantaged working class students cheated more frequently than people from comfort-

    able middle class backgrounds.

    Males have been found to be more ready to admit to academic dishonesty than

    females (Haines et al., 1986; Davis & Ludvigson, 1995; Ameen et al., 1996; Bjorklund& Wenestam, 1999). Also, business studies students are allegedly more likely than

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    7/27

    142 R. Bennett 

    others to cheat (Phillips & Horton, 2000). Research completed in a number of coun-

    tries has revealed heavily instrumental attitudes towards learning and teaching among

    marketing, business and management students (see Bennett & Kottasz, 2001, p. 473

    for details of relevant studies). Manifestations of such attitudes have been said to

    include the desire for the rapid accumulation of knowledge, for ‘quick-fix’ teachingand learning methods, wanting to ‘learn how to do’, and preferring bullet point

    approaches to the transmission of information. Orientations of this kind might be

    compatible with the tendency to engage in plagiarism.

     Academic integration

    Caruana et al.  (2000) found a connection between an individual’s lack of integra-

    tion in academic life and his or her propensity to behave dishonestly. Calabrese

    and Cochran (1990) similarly concluded that students who felt ‘alienated’ from

    their courses or institutions were more inclined to cheat than students who were

    better ‘integrated’ (especially in the social sense) with their universities. The

    absence of social integration (evidenced by substandard academic performance

    and poor relationships with fellow students and lecturers—see Saenz et al., 1999)

    was associated with a lack of concern both for the rules of the institution and for

    its objectives. A non-integrated individual did not feel any sense of cohesion with

    the institution or a significant attachment to it (Caruana et al., 2000). Boredom

    with a course and dissatisfaction with the institution frequently arose in these

    circumstances (Davies, 2000). Consequently, the poorly integrated student might

    come to view his or her course (and the conventions of academic documentationassociated with it) as personally unimportant (Haines et al., 1986; CWPA, 2003).

    In extreme cases, dissatisfaction could lead to a desire to ‘punish’ the institution

    for its perceived inadequacies. A plagiarist might even want to punish more able

    fellow students simply because the latter are capable of superior academic perfor-

    mance.

    Academic integration implies that the student ‘fits in’ with university life and the

    overall academic environment. Hence the individual feels that he or she is the

    undergraduate ‘type’ of person (Kreger & Wrenn, 1990; Michie et al., 2001). Such a

    student is more likely than others to possess realistic perceptions of the amount of work needed to complete a degree, and to be reasonably self-confident of his or her

    ability to cope with work overload, examinations, financial pressures, and problems

    created by lack of time for friends and family (see Michie et al., 2001 for details of 

    the academic literature supporting these propositions). Kreger and Wrenn (1990)

    found that academic integration was significantly lower among people who had

    experienced a gap between school and higher education. Michie et al. (2001) simi-

    larly concluded that failure to integrate academically was more prevalent among

    older (i.e., aged 22 or more) people especially those who had entered higher educa-

    tion (HE) in consequence of ‘undesirable conditions’ (e.g., unemployment, divorce,

    or wanting to ‘get out of the house’ to have a break from caring for young children)(p. 456).

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    8/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 143

    Other possible influences

    Research completed by Davis and Ludvigson (1995) and by Anderman et al. (1998)

    found that plagiarists began their careers early in their educational lives (and long

    before their entry to university), so that the habit of plagiarism was well embedded

    prior to their becoming undergraduates. Thus, a student’s experiences of plagiarism

    (and its disciplinary consequences) at school and/or in Further Education (FE) might

    affect subsequent behaviour at university (see Bjorklund & Wenestam, 1999). Also

    certain schools or FE colleges inculcate in their students effective study skills (e.g.,

    regularly reviewing lecture notes, underlining passages in textbooks, preparing several

    drafts of essays, using other students for peer support—see Stoynoff, 1996) that will

    in the longer term result in better grades and hence less incentive to plagiarise. When

    a person arrives at university, the example set by fellow students (and perhaps the

    feeling that ‘everybody else does it, so why shouldn’t I?’) could encourage a person to

    plagiarise (Newstead et al., 1996).In Australia, foreign students have been found to plagiarise to a greater extent than

    home students (Maslen, 2003). Often, foreign students will be writing in an alien

    language and may have been schooled in an intellectual tradition that does not frown

    on copying (Myers, 1998; see also Larkham & Manns, 2002). Kuehn et al. (1990)

    found that student attitudes towards academic cheating in general differed signifi-

    cantly with respect to nationality.

    The present study

    Prior research in the plagiarism field has tended to focus on estimating the extents of 

    the practice in various institutions, and an explaining at the theoretical level why

    plagiarism happens. Empirical work on the causes of plagiarism has been limited, and

    has usually considered just one or two variables at a time. The investigation reported

    in the present paper adds to contemporary knowledge about the antecedents of 

    plagiarism through the development of a structured equation model of plagiaristic

    behaviour, followed by its testing on a sample of 249 students in a post-1992 univer-

    sity located in Greater London. (In 1992 the British government converted all the

    nation’s polytechnics into universities. Polytechnics were higher education institu-

    tions that offered a wide range of vocational and academic courses and accepted

    students from social backgrounds not traditionally associated with entry to university.

    Polytechnic students typically possessed matriculation qualifications somewhat lower

    than those demanded by the pre-1992 university sector.)

    The hypothesised model 

    A model suggested by the above mentioned literature is outlined in Figure 1, which

    explains plagiarism in terms of three attitudinal variables (perceptions regarding the

    propriety of the practice, fear of punishment, and fear of failing a degree), twopersonal traits (goal orientation and academic integration), and three situational

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    9/27

    144 R. Bennett 

    variables (financial circumstances, academic performance, and whether university

    staff strictly enforce anti-plagiarism regulations). An individual’s attitudes towards

    plagiarism are posited to depend on past experiences at school or FE college, the

    example set by other students, whether the student is enjoying his or her degree

    programme, and whether the person is religious and/or ‘ethical’. The possession of effective study skills is assumed to enhance a student’s academic performance and to

    reduce the fear of failure. It is hypothesised that students who feel they are under

    heavy parental pressure to succeed at university are more fearful of failing their

    degrees than others. Additionally it is suggested that people who have already

    invested a great deal in becoming a student will be deterred from engaging in plagia-

    rism by the prospect of losing a grade, a semester, a year, or even their entire degree

    if they are caught cheating. Figure 1 does not show the possible influences of state

    variables such as gender, age, or whether the person was an overseas student, as these

    were examined separately.Figure1. Thehypothesisedmodel

    Two moderating variables are proposed: (1) attitudes towards plagiarism, and (2)

    the intensity with which anti-plagiarism rules are enforced. A moderating variable is

    one that affects the strength of the relationship between two other variables, hence

    producing an ‘interaction’ effect1. Accordingly, an independent variable that is

    moderated by another variable will exert a high or low impact on the dependent vari-

    able according to the value of the third (moderating) variable. Hence it is posited in

    Figure 1 that, for instance, a student who experiences major financial difficulties will

    be substantially more likely than otherwise would be the case to hand in plagiarised

    work if it is also true that there is very little enforcement of university rules on plagia-

    rism. Likewise, students who possess casual attitudes towards plagiarism are assumedto be more inclined to plagiarise if their academic grades are poor, if they have little

    fear of the penalties for being caught plagiarising, and so on, as indicated in Figure 1.

     Measurement of variables

    Academic integration was measured by six items derived from Bennett’s (2003)

    application of the construct to the explanation of student drop-out rates in a post-

    1992 university. Examples of the items (see Bennett, 2003 for full details) are ‘I feel

    I am a “natural” university student’; ‘I often feel lost and bewildered at university’(negatively scored) and ‘I have found it very easy to make friends at university’.

    Thereafter the questions involved relations with lecturers, and the person’s level of 

    emotional attachment to his or her course2. The borrowed items were selected and

    adapted via  the application of the framework recommended by Engelland et al.

    (2001). Thus, candidate items were examined to ensure that they fell well within the

    scope of the domain of the relevant construct, that they expressed the theoretical

    construct in an effective manner, were worded at an appropriate level of abstraction

    and were compatible with the vocabulary of the target respondents, and were likely

    to generate outcomes similar to those of the original studies from which the modified

    items were taken. Two senior academics in the researcher’s home university indepen-dently assessed the adapted items in terms of these criteria. They also inspected the

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    10/27

    Experiences of the sacred in childhood  145

       F   i  g  u  r  e   1 .

       T   h  e   h  y  p  o  t   h  e  s   i  s  e   d  m  o   d  e   l

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    11/27

    146 R. Bennett 

    items that had not been taken from pre-existing scales vis-à-vis their clarity, relevance

    to the particular issue being investigated and to the population of interest, and their

    compatibility with the academic literature in the area (Churchill, 1979).

    Four items were used to assess goal orientation, based on Boshier’s (1991) ‘educa-

    tion participation scale’. Examples of the items are ‘I am only interested in learningthings that will help me in my future career’ and ‘I would do this degree for its intrin-

    sic interest, even if it would not lead to a job and eventual financial benefits’ (nega-

    tively scored). Unless specified otherwise (see below), all the questionnaire items

    were quantified on five point scales: 5 = strongly agree, 1 = strongly disagree.

    Responses to the six items for academic integration were factor analysed, a single-

    factor solution emerging (λ = 4.24, α = 0.87). This indicated that all the items were

    measuring the same construct, so the six items were combined into a single academic

    integration scale for use in subsequent analysis. The procedure was repeated for the

    four goal orientation items, which again generated a uni-dimensional outcome (λ =

    2.98, α = 0.84). Accordingly the four items were consolidated into a single measure.

    The students were asked how many hours per week they spent in employment;

    whether their family paid all, some, or none of their tuition fees; and whether their

    need to work part-time ‘seriously disrupted’ their academic studies. It turned out

    that 73% of the students in the sample were from families with incomes below the

    threshold at which they would have to pay any fees whatsoever, and a further 12%

    paid only partial fees. Thus the sample was relatively homogeneous in that most of 

    its members would have financial problems. Seventy-two per cent of the sample

    worked in paid employment for more than ten hours a week (25% for more than 15

    and 29% for more than 20 hours a week), so again there was relatively little varia-tion vis-à-vis  this criterion. There were many disparities, however, in students’

    assessments of the extents to which having to work part-time disrupted their studies,

    and these assessments correlated meaningfully and significantly with many other

    dimensions of the analysis. Hence this item was used as the ‘financial situation’

    independent variable.

    Three items evaluated the intensity with which college rules on plagiarism were

    enforced by lecturers, i.e., whether (1) it was ‘easy to get away with’ plagiarism on the

    student’s degree programme, (2) staff rarely bothered to check whether students’

    efforts contained plagiarised materials, and (3) staff did not really care whether workhad been plagiarised. Responses to these items were highly intercorrelated (R>0.63),

    and thus were composited into a single scale. Student attitudes concerning whether

    plagiarism was or was not a serious misdemeanour were examined via  four items

    implied by the literature review completed by Bjorklund and Wenestam (1999);

    namely whether the student believed that plagiarism was (1) fundamentally immoral,

    dishonest and shameful, (2) no more serious than driving a little above the speed limit

    in a car, (3) a natural and creative way of solving academic problems, and (4) a ‘really

    major’ offence deserving stiff penalties. A factor analysis of the four items revealed

    that it was acceptable to combine them into a single measure (λ = 3.1, α = 0.92).

    Fear of failure was assessed through two highly correlated (R = 0.81), items,namely, (1) ‘the thought of my not obtaining my degree is just too awful for me to

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    12/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 147

    contemplate’, and (2) ‘failing my degree would be an absolute catastrophe for me’.

    The two items were combined into a single scale. A student’s academic performance

    was measured by a single item which asked the person whether, on average, his or her

    grades were higher, lower or about the same as those of other students on the individ-

    ual’s programme. This was of course a self-reported measure, but research previouslycompleted in the same university had recorded a high correlation (R = 0.72) between

    responses to the same item and the actual  performances of a sample of 140 students

    (Bennett, 2003). It was not feasible to check definitive grades in the present study

    because of the need to preserve the student’s anonymity. The intensity of a person’s

    fear of the penalties for being caught plagiarising was evaluated through two items:

    (1) ‘I am terrified of what would happen to me if I were caught plagiarising’, and (2)

    ‘the penalties here for plagiarism are so severe that it is simply not worth taking the

    risk’. As the responses to the two items were significantly correlated (R = 0.81), they

    were combined into a single scale.

    ‘Religiosity’ was evaluated by asking the student whether he or she was ‘a very

    religious person’ (five point scale). The student’s overall ethical stance was

    measured using four items from various business ethics measures reported by

    Bruner and Hensel (1994), i.e., ‘stretching the truth can never be justified no matter

    what the circumstances’, ‘honesty and moral integrity are key principles in my life’;

    ‘it is absolutely essential that students always follow all university rules and regula-

    tions’; and ‘in order to succeed it is sometimes acceptable to behave unethically and

    to take unfair advantage’. A factor analysis of the four items generated a single factor

    solution (λ  = 2.79, α  = 0.81) so the items were consolidated to form a single

    measure. Peer influences vis-à-vis  plagiarism were assessed by two stand aloneitems: ‘everybody else on my course plagiarises to some extent or other, so why

    shouldn’t I?’ and ‘I know a lot of students who plagiarise’. A person’s response to

    the first of these items does not imply any particular pattern of response to the

    second, so the items were not consolidated. The students were also asked to tick off 

    a category to indicate their opinion of the percentage of all their fellow students that

    engaged in plagiarism (less than 5%; 6%–10%, etc., through to ‘more than 75%’).

    The respondent’s satisfaction with his or her degree programme was measured by

    three (highly intercorrelated, R>0.77) items adapted from Oliver’s (1993) ‘satisfac-

    tion with a course’ instrument, i.e., ‘doing this course has been an excellent experi-ence for me’, ‘the teaching and the educational facilities here are excellent’, and ‘this

    is one of the best courses I could possibly have taken’. The three items were

    composited into a single scale.

    A student’s prior experience of plagiarism at school or FE college was evaluated

    by two (not significantly correlated) items that asked (five point scales) whether

    plagiarism was ‘commonplace’ at the person’s previous institution, and whether

    teachers at that institution ‘did not really care’ whether students’ work was plagia-

    rised. The two items were entered separately in the subsequent analysis. Parental

    pressure was assessed by two stand alone items (which were not significantly corre-

    lated): ‘my parents are putting heavy pressure on me to succeed at university’, and‘my parents would be devastated if I were not to complete my degree’. Four items

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    13/27

    148 R. Bennett 

    were employed to measure the effectiveness of a student’s study skills, based on the

    suggestions of Stoynoff (1996); namely whether the person (1) took detailed notes

    of lectures, (2) read, reread and reviewed course learning materials very regularly,

    (3) prepared several drafts of essays before handing them in, and (4) believed he or

    she possessed ‘excellent study habits and skills’. All four items loaded significantlyonto the same factor in a factor analysis (λ  = 3.01, α  = 0.91) and thus were

    composited into a single scale. The level of an individual’s past investment in his or

    her university role was assessed through three items suggested by Meyer et al.

    (1993), i.e., ‘I have invested so much in being a university student that it would be

    a disaster for me if I were to leave now’; ‘I have had to make huge personal sacri-

    fices in order to become a university student’; and ‘my family has invested a great

    deal of money in sending me to university and supporting me during my time at

    university’. The first two of these items were significantly correlated (R = 0.72)

    with each other, but not with the third item, which was used as a separate entity in

    subsequent analysis.

    The dependent variable

    Students were asked to indicate on a five point scale (very often, often, occasionally,

    just once or twice, never) how frequently they had engaged in various activities that

    might be construed as plagiarism. Specifically (following Caruana et al., 2000) the

    student was presented with the following statement and options:

    I have copied and inserted into my own work, without acknowledgement of where thematerial came from, the following taken from published sources, from the Internet or from

    the work of other students:

    (a) a couple of sentences;

    (b) several sentences;

    (c) a paragraph;

    (d) a number of paragraphs;

    (e) an entire piece of work.

    Then the person ticked off how frequently each of the above had occurred. Further

    questions asked whether the person had (1) collaborated with other students onassignments that should have been completed individually, (very often, often, occa-

    sionally, etc.), and (2) made up references.

    The procedure adopted required the self-reporting of misbehaviour, so that a priori 

    the respondents might be expected to understate the extents of their dishonesty.

    There is evidence to suggest, however, that self-reports of acts of academic dishonesty

    are reasonably accurate (Allen et al., 1998), and under-reporting does not appear to

    have presented a major problem in previous investigations (for details see Franklyn-

    Stokes & Newstead, 1995; Bjorklund & Wenestam, 1999). Indeed, Caruana et al.

    (2000) reported ‘fairly high’ mean scores in response to the ‘serious plagiarism’

    section of their questionnaire (p. 28), indicating a substantial willingness to confessto dishonest behaviour.

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    14/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 149

    The sample

    A questionnaire containing the above-mentioned items plus some general queries

    concerning the individual’s age, gender, programme of study, etc. (though not

    including anything that might identify the student) was drafted consequent to a

    review of relevant literature and pre-tested via  (1) discussions with three experi-

    enced researchers in the educational field, and (2) a trial administration to a class of 

    41 second year students. (The class was arbitrarily selected as the first of the after-

    noon in the first lecture theatre on the first floor of the university campus in which

    the researcher’s office was located.) This pre-test facilitated the refinement of the

    draft questionnaire and the clarification of ambiguities. The final version of the

    questionnaire was then distributed to 327 second, third and fourth year students

    completing business units at a post-1992 university in Greater London. Second year

    students upwards were selected on the grounds that they would have substantial

    experience of relevant matters within the university. Documents were completedduring class and were collected by students who then shuffled them out of sight of 

    the lecturer in order to ensure total anonymity. All classes taking place over a two-

    week period in the business department in one of the university’s main campuses

    and whose class teacher agreed to participate in the exercise were covered, repre-

    senting 82% of all students undertaking business units in the building. Two

    hundred and forty-nine usable responses were obtained, of which 159 were from

    business majors.

    Sixty-eight per cent of the respondents classified themselves as non-white; 58%

    were male and just 13% were from overseas. Sixty per cent were aged 21 years or

    over; 60% had entered via BTEC, GNVQ or Access Course qualifications. (It was in

    fact quite difficult to measure entry qualifications as such, because a large number of 

    the students possessed a mix of qualifications, e.g., a GNVQ plus one low grade ‘A’

    level, although at least half were known to have used a BTEC certificate or GNVQ to

    gain entry.) More than 90% of the British (rather than overseas) respondents were

    living at home with their immediate family or with a partner. All the students in the

    sample should in principle have been fully aware of the meaning of plagiarism. Unit

    handbooks carry definitions of the term and cite examples. The topic is covered

    explicitly in the student’s first year as part of a lecture and in classes on how to refer-

    ence sources. Thereafter tutors are expected to explain clearly how the concept relatesto specific subject areas.

    Analysis of the data

    Descriptive results

    Less than a majority of the students (46%) agreed or strongly agreed with the state-

    ment that plagiarism was ‘fundamentally immoral and shameful’, yet a large number

    of the respondents engaged in the practice (see Table 1). Forty-six per cent admit-

    ted to having copied an entire paragraph into their own work without acknowledge-ment; 31% to having lifted several paragraphs from an unacknowledged source; and

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    15/27

    150 R. Bennett 

    25% to having handed in a complete piece of work that had been copied (16% of 

    the sample confessed to having done this more than ‘once or twice’). The figures

    listed in Table 1 correspond broadly to the findings of previous surveys completed

    in the field, i.e., that it is likely that up to 80% of students plagiarise to some extent

    (Hammond, 2002; Maslen, 2003), and that around a quarter copy full paragraphs

    (Maslen, 2003). Respondents’ opinions concerning the percentage of the total

    student body that engaged in plagiarism varied substantially. A third thought that

    less than eleven per cent of all students handed in plagiarised work; 25% believedthe true figure exceeded 35%. Most of the students (57%) disagreed or strongly

    disagreed with the proposition that lecturers ‘rarely bothered’ to check whether

    students’ work contained plagiarised materials; 72% did not believe that it was ‘easy

    to get away with plagiarism’; and 75% disagreed/strongly disagreed with a statement

    worded ‘the lecturing staff do not really care whether work has been plagiarised’.

    The latter outcome is in line with Bjorklund and Wenestam’s (1999) suggestion that

    around a fifth of all lecturers may ignore (or at least not take too seriously) plagia-

    rised submissions, but it fails to support the notion that there is widespread apathy

    towards the problem among teaching staff (cf. Davis et al., 1992; Phillips & Horton,2000).

    Table 2 lists some key statistics that offer an insight into certain key traits and opin-

    ions among the students in the sample. Fifteen per cent of the responses fell in the

    strongly agree/agree categories of the academic integration composite, indicating

    poor academic integration as the item was scored negatively. A bare majority of the

    sample was goal orientated. More than a third of the students believed that having to

    work part time in paid employment seriously disrupted their studies. Overall, the

    students in the sample did not have particularly ethical stances (Table 2 item [g]),

    though half felt that plagiarism was unethical (item [d]). Thirty per cent of the

    students’ responses fell in the bottom two categories of the composite measuring theeffectiveness of a person’s study skills (item [i]).

    Table 1. Frequency of plagiarism

    Percentages

    5 4 3 2 1 Mean Standard

    deviation

    Skewness

    (Z-value)A couple of sentences 12 14 26 28 20 2.69 1.29 1.13

    Several sentences 2 14 32 23 29 2.36 1.10 0.64

    A single paragraph 4 2 17 23 54 1.77 1.03 4.36

    A number of paragraphs 2 4 11 14 69 1.45 0.82 5.57

    An entire piece of work 2 9 5 9 75 1.26 0.69 8.28

    Has collaborated with other students on work

    supposed to be completed individually

    5 5 27 24 39 2.12 1.04 1.09

    Has made up references 5 8 20 20 47 2.40 1.02 3.97

    Key: 5 = very often, 4 = often, 3 = occasionally, 2 = just once or twice, 1 = never.

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    16/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 151

    Test of the model 

    It can be seen from Tables 1 and 2 that several of the variables in the hypothesised

    model depicted in Figure 1 were not normally distributed. Hence the model was esti-mated using the method of partial least squares (specifically the PLS Graph Package

    Version 3 (Chin, 2001)), which makes no pre-assumptions about the statistical distri-

    butions of variables3. Significant relationships were identified through an experimen-

    tal procedure whereby all the candidate independent variables were entered in

    regression equations, firstly in total, thereafter in various combinations. An indepen-

    dent variable was removed if it failed to attain significance at the 0.05 level in any

    configuration of independent variables. The data was also examined for significant

    differences with respect to gender, age, programme of study, ethnicity, and whether

    the student was from overseas. No meaningfully significant differences (p = 0.05 or

    below) were detected vis-à-vis  any of these considerations. This pre-analysis soon

    revealed that two model estimations were necessary: one for minor plagiarism and

    another for major plagiarism. Students’ responses regarding the frequency with which

    they plagiarised ‘a couple of sentences’ were highly correlated (R = 0.77) with their

    replies vis-à-vis plagiarising several sentences. Hence the two items were combined

    into a single scale to measure the incidence of minor plagiarism. This scale correlated

    significantly with the frequencies with which the students in the sample made up ficti-

    tious references (R = 0.63) and with the incidence of collaboration with other

    students on work that should have been completed individually (R = 0.68). Hence

    the newly constructed minor plagiarism scale represented a reasonable proxy for theseother two variables. Major plagiarism was measured as the combination of the

    Table 2. Key statistics

    Percentages

    SA A N D SD Mean Standard

    deviation

    Skewness

    (Z-value)(a) Academic integration composite (scored

    negatively)

    5 10 21 45 19 2.47 1.70 13.33

    (b) Goal orientation composite 12 40 15 25 8 3.42 1.04 0.56

    (c) ‘Having to work part-time seriously

    disrupts my studies’

    12 22 27 31 8 2.97 1.12 0.76

    (d) Composite concerning whether

    plagiarism is regarded as unethical

    15 35 32 9 9 3.60 1.02 3.18

    (e) Fearful of failing a degree 30 40 10 10 10 3.93 1.08 3.92

    (f) Fearful of the penalties resulting

    from being caught plagiarising

    27 28 30 7 8 3.67 1.11 1.84

    (g) Ethical stance composite 6 10 42 29 13 2.64 1.01 0.80

    (h) Satisfaction with course composite 15 30 29 18 8 3.20 1.18 1.05

    (i) Effectiveness of study skills composite 9 22 39 22 8 3.10 0.99 0.26

    Key: SA = strongly agree, A = agree, N = neither agree nor disagree, D = disagree, SD = strongly disagree

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    17/27

    152 R. Bennett 

    students’ responses to the two questionnaire items concerning the frequencies with

    which the person plagiarised ‘a number of paragraphs’ and ‘an entire piece of work’.

    There was a high correlation between these items (R = 0.81), meaning that individ-

    uals who often copied a number of paragraphs without acknowledgement were also

    extremely likely to copy an entire piece of work. (The pattern of the results from thehypothesis tests were the same when the models were re-estimated using the two

    items as stand alone variables rather than as a composite. This was also true of the

    two items in the minor plagiarism composite.) Significant pathways are shown in

    Figures 2 and 3; associated parameter estimates are listed in Table 3.

    Minor plagiarism was significantly associated with many but not all of the variables

    depicted in the initial hypothesised model. However, past experiences at school or in

    FE and peer group influences failed to exert an impact. The strongest effects on the

    incidence of minor plagiarism were a person’s attitude towards the practice (i.e.,

    whether it was seen as fundamentally improper), and the degree of an individual’s

    academic integration. Students whose part time employment interfered with their

    studies; who were obtaining low grades and/or were under heavy parental pressure to

    succeed were more likely to plagiarise at the minor level than others. High levels of 

    goal orientation were also a significant determinant of decisions to plagiarise. A

    person’s attitudes towards plagiarism were significantly influenced by his or her over-

    all moral position (though not by religiosity). As expected, ineffective study skills

    contributed to low grades and, it turned out, were directly connected with poor

    academic integration. Two significant interaction terms were identified, indicated as

    M1 and M2 in Figure 2. Firstly it emerged that students with a lax attitude towards

    plagiarism and  whose part time paid employment was interfering with their studieswere especially likely to plagiarise (M1). Secondly, the impact of low grades on the

    tendency to plagiarise was substantially higher among students with a lax attitude

    towards plagiarism issues (M2).Figure2. Determinantsofminorplagiarism

    Figure 3 shows that major plagiarism was explained by a smaller number of 

    variables than was the case for minor plagiarism, and three of these variables did

    not appear in Figure 2. Fear of failing a course encouraged major plagiarism; fear

    of the penalties that might be imposed if a person was caught discouraged

    students from engaging in the practice. As with minor plagiarism, lax attitudes

    and lack of academic integration exerted significant influences on serious plagiaris-tic activities, However, when the connections between (1) the extent of major

    plagiarism, and (2) the individual items of the academic integration composite

    were examined in detail an unanticipated outcome was discovered. Students who

    reported that they had excellent relationships with their lecturers confessed to

    engaging in major plagiarism to a substantially greater extent than others. Hence

    this item was taken out of the academic integration composite and thereafter

    regarded as a stand alone variable in its own right. As an independent variable,

    the item had a highly significant impact on the incidence of major plagiarism. It

    seems, therefore, that students who enjoyed friendly personal relationships with

    their lecturers also felt that serious plagiarism would either be overlooked, or notresult in serious penalties.

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    18/27

    Experiences of the sacred in childhood  153

       F   i  g  u  r  e   2 .

       D  e  t  e  r  m

       i  n  a  n  t  s  o   f  m   i  n  o  r  p   l  a  g   i  a  r   i  s  m

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    19/27

    154 H-G. Heimbrock

       F   i  g  u  r  e   3 .

       D  e  t  e  r  m

       i  n  a  n  t  s  o   f  m  a   j  o  r  p   l  a  g   i  a  r   i  s  m

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    20/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 155

    Figure3. Determinantsofmajorplagiarism

    Conclusion and discussion

    The students in the sample were drawn mainly from families at the bottom end of the

    UK distribution of incomes and belonged to ethnic groups that represent minorities

    within the UK population as a whole. Most of the respondents had entered university

    on the basis of the minimum academic qualifications permissible under the British

    matriculation system. It emerged that some of the factors that were significantly asso-

    ciated with the incidence of plagiarism among these students differed according to the

    level of the plagiarism involved, implying that it is not appropriate to apply a singleset of policies when attempting to reduce the occurrence of the practice. Clearly,

    Table 3. Standardised parameter estimates*

    Minor plagiarism

    Major

    plagiarism

    Dependent variable Explanatory variablesExtent of plagiarism Goal orientation 0.23

    (2.91)

    Academic integration   −0.33

    (6.55)

    −0.28

    (5.53)

    Financial situation   −0.21

    (3.48)

    Academic performance**   −0.22

    (3.02)

    Parental pressure 0.20

    (2.77)

    Attitude towards plagiarism   −0.46(8.76)

    −0.50(11.66)

    M1 2.90

    (6.13)

    M2 3.12

    (7.07)

    Relationships with staff 0.19

    (2.04)

    Fear of failure 0.39

    (4.61)

    Fear of penalties if caught   −0.22

    (3.99)

    Academic integration Effectiveness of personal

    study skills

    0.19

    (2.04)

    0.28

    (3.77)

    Attitudes towards plagiarism Ethical position 0.52

    (11.64)

    0.67

    (12.49)

    Academic performance Effectiveness of personal

    study skills

    0.21

    (2.98)

    *T-values in parentheses. These were obtained via the jack-knifing facility available on PLS Graph Version 3.

    **Excludes the item concerning relationships with members of the academic staff.

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    21/27

    156 R. Bennett 

    there is little point in tackling an outbreak of plagiarism via  the intensification of 

    measures that are not relevant to the level of plagiarism in question.

    Nevertheless, certain variables were connected with the likelihood that a student

    would plagiarise at both the major and minor levels, presumably because of the

    reasons cited in the prior literature in the field as previously outlined. Individualspossessing innately ‘ethical’ moral positions disapproved of plagiarism to a greater

    extent than others, and were substantially less likely to hand in plagiarised work (cf.

    Franklyn-Stokes & Newstead, 1995). Perhaps therefore there is a need for university

    staff to address forcefully the issue of academic integrity during introductory

    programmes and to explain clearly and sympathetically the objective need for honesty

    in academic life. Concomitantly, the second common factor underlying both major

    and minor plagiarism, i.e., lack of academic integration, must be confronted at the

    earliest possible opportunity (see Calabrese & Cochran, 1990; Caruana et al., 2000).

    A significant minority of the respondents reported that they felt ‘lost and bewildered’

    at university, implying that academic integration was a major issue so far as these indi-

    viduals were concerned. Academic integration is strongly associated with a person’s

    perception that it is somehow ‘natural’ and fundamentally ‘right and proper’ for the

    individual to be a university undergraduate. Such perceptions are less common

    perhaps among ‘non-traditional’ students. It follows that first year (preferably first

    semester) courses should aim to make ‘non-traditional’ students feel ‘at home’ in an

    academic environment and to believe that a university education is a natural experi-

    ence for people from all sorts of cultural and socio-economic backgrounds. Poor

    study skills comprised the third common factor applicable to both major and minor

    plagiarism (see Bjorklund & Wenestam, 1999). Ineffective study skills were associ-ated with substandard academic performance, lack of academic integration, and the

    tendency to plagiarise. Again, this suggests the desirability of concentrating resources

    on the development of study skills at the very start of a degree programme.

    The level of a student’s satisfaction with his or her degree course did not impact

    significantly on either major or minor plagiarism. Individuals who were highly satis-

    fied with the education they were receiving were just as likely to plagiarise as people

    who were dissatisfied. This finding might be explained, perhaps, by the fact that

    dissatisfied students can be just as able, hard-working, and ‘ethical’ as individuals

    who are highly satisfied with their course. Thus, academic ability and personal ethicalintegrity might have exerted influences that outweighed the potential effects of the

    satisfaction variable on the propensity to plagiarise. For instance, a dissatisfied

    student could have held deep moral principles that prevented the person from cheat-

    ing, even if he or she inwardly wished to ‘punish’ the university (through plagiarising)

    for having been the cause of the student’s dissatisfaction. Equally, a student’s age,

    extent of past investment in getting to and attending university, and prior experiences

    at school or FE college did not appear to be connected with the decision to plagiarise.

    As regards the non-significance of a student’s age in the present analysis, it should be

    noted that a substantial majority of the students in the sample (60%) were over 21

    years of age. It is likely therefore that the average age of the individuals in this partic-ular sample was considerably higher than the average ages of the students included in

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    22/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 157

    earlier investigations in the plagiarism field. (Unfortunately few of these other studies

    provided precise details of the age structures of their samples.) Hence it is possible

    that there was insufficient variation in the data to enable all potential associations

    between age and the two dependent variables to be uncovered. The same consider-

    ation might explain the statistical insignificance of the variable measuring the extentof a person’s past investment in getting into university. In financial and most other

    socio-economic terms the sample was substantially homogeneous, so that the

    students’ past experiences and levels of effort devoted to gaining matriculation qual-

    ifications were likely to have been very similar, again resulting in a paucity of mean-

    ingful variation in the data relating to this variable. Likewise, the great majority of the

    students in the sample would have attended the same kinds of schools in geographical

    areas with similar characteristics and thus would have had comparable experiences of 

    school or FE college prior to entering university.

    Another variable that failed to attain significance was the intensity with which

    lecturers enforced university rules on plagiarism. As previously stated, most students

    believed that their lecturers were quite strict on enforcement, although there was little

    evidence of this affecting students’ behaviour. One possible reason for the lack of 

    significance of the enforcement variable could be that because the students were

    taught in large groups in a generally impersonal manner, many of them might not

    have believed that their lecturers were key players in the disciplinary (rather than

    administrative) process. Lecturers ‘enforce’ rules in that they report alleged cases of 

    plagiarism, but the rules themselves, investigative procedures, and the penalties for

    engaging in plagiarism are determined by the institution. The documentation distrib-

    uted to students about plagiarism come from the university, not from individuallecturers. Therefore, institutional  considerations might have been at the forefront of 

    students’ minds when deciding whether and how seriously to plagiarise.

    Major plagiarism appeared to be driven more by fear of failure (cf. Baird, 1980;

    CWPA, 2003) mitigated by fear of punishment (confirming the findings of Haines et 

    al., 1986 but refuting the suggestions of Braumoeller & Gaines, 2001) than by the

    desire to succeed (see Bjorklund & Wenestam, 1999). This result was due presum-

    ably to the large difference in the probable consequences of major and minor plagia-

    rism; which can range from a reprimand and having to redo a piece of work in the

    latter case, to the loss of a full year or to expulsion following a serious offence. Astudent’s fear of failure might be confronted by explaining to the person at an early

    point in his or her academic career that doing badly in an assignment or examination

    can be a positive and useful experience that identifies areas of weakness to which the

    person needs to direct his or her attention. Whereas the degree of the strictness with

    which faculty enforced university rules on plagiarism did not generally affect respon-

    dents’ behaviour, the existence of severe penalties did seem to deter major plagiarism

    (cf. Larkham & Manns, 2002). An unexpected result concerning major plagiarism

    was that students who reported that they had excellent relationships with their lectur-

    ers were more likely to plagiarise than others. This contradicts the conclusion of 

    Saenz et al. (1999), who alleged that students with poor relationships with academicstaff were more inclined to plagiarise. Accordingly, it may be that lecturers need to be

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    23/27

    158 R. Bennett 

    extremely careful to ensure that their overt attempts at being close to and friendly

    towards students do not induce the latter to believe that acts of plagiarism will be

    ignored or automatically forgiven. Possibly staff who ‘court popularity’ with students

    are prone to attract the attentions of major, though seemingly not minor, plagiarists.

    It is not entirely clear why this should be the case. The results clearly indicatedhowever that a dishonest student’s expectation of being able to get away with a serious

    act of plagiarism increased as the person’s relationship with a lecturer deepened.

    Additional research is needed into this matter.

    Minor plagiarism was associated with a wider range of variables than major

    plagiarism, including poor academic performance, parental pressure, financial situ-

    ation (in terms of whether having to undertake paid employment interfered with

    the student’s academic work), and goal orientation. The strength of the link

    between low grades and minor plagiarism could be reduced, perhaps, by explaining

    to incoming students that the results of assessments offer valuable information vis-

    à-vis how the student is progressing and the areas in which more work is necessary.

    The fact that accurate grades (i.e., those that have not been distorted by plagia-

    rism) indicate a person’s progress in a particular subject relative to (1) other people

    in his or her year, and (2) the individual’s own expectations, must be clearly eluci-

    dated. Students need to be made to understand the fundamental purposes of in-

    course assessment at the outsets of their academic careers. Equally, lecturers must

    emphasise to students that the (reasonable) desire to attain utilitarian employment-

    related objectives must not compromise academic integrity. The fact that acts of 

    dishonesty committed in employment situations lead to dire consequences must be

    pointed out.In contrast to the situation pertaining to minor plagiarism, the incidence of more

    serious forms of the practice was not affected by goal orientation. Possibly, there-

    fore, students’ desires to improve their job and career prospects by obtaining a

    better degree might in some cases impel them to engage in minor acts of plagia-

    rism, where the probability of detection and the consequent penalties are small.

    However, students with high levels of goal orientation may be extremely averse to

    the risk of disrupting their career plans through being caught in major acts of 

    plagiarism that are likely to result in the loss of an entire grade, a semester, or in

    expulsion. Similarly, the non-significance of parental pressure in relation to majorplagiarism could be due to the fact that a student’s parents would almost certainly

    find out if the individual were suspended or expelled from university, yet would be

    unlikely to learn about a small penalty imposed for a minor offence. Hence the

    desire to please parents through obtaining higher grades via major plagiarism might

    be offset by fear of the consequences of discovery. Students whose academic grades

    were poor were inclined to indulge in minor but not in major plagiarism. It seems

    plausible to suppose that the academically weak are sometimes more prone to take

    ‘short cuts’ by plagiarising in order to improve their performances. Nevertheless,

    the willingness to take these dishonest shortcuts did not apply when the danger of 

    incurring severe penalties consequent to major acts of cheating was present. Finan-cial hardship was another variable associated only with minor plagiarism. It is

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    24/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 159

    appropriate to note in this connection that students with financial difficulties typi-

    cally spend more hours in paid employment than better-off individuals, and thus

    may experience greater temptations to take short-cuts in their academic work via

    minor plagiarism. At the same time, the prospect of losing a semester or year

    through being caught consequent to an act of major plagiarism may be consider-ably more daunting to an indigent student than to others.  A-priori , therefore, the

    absence of a positive and significant link between financial hardship and major

    plagiarism is not entirely surprising.

    Parental pressure on a student to succeed at university is an exogenous factor

    beyond institutional control. Further research is needed into the antecedents and

    consequences of this variable, particularly in situations where, as in the university

    in which the research was completed, the majority of undergraduate students are

    from ethnic minorities and usually comprise the first generation of their families to

    attend university (parental pressure to do well might be intense in these circum-

    stances). Financial pressures on students represent another exogenous situation

    that a university cannot regulate. Nevertheless, the significance of the link between

    financial hardship and minor plagiarism is disturbing, and confirms the findings of 

    numerous previous studies (see above) that have alleged the existence of a connec-

    tion between students having to spend many hours in paid employment and their

    tendency to plagiarise. Students from families that offer them enough financial

    support to enable them to avoid having to spend many hours in paid employment

    will have more time available to devote to their studies. In the present investiga-

    tion however there were no significant correlations between the number of hours

    worked and (1) academic performance, or (2) academic integration. Interestingly,there was a significant negative connection (R = −0.39) between hours worked

    and satisfaction with a course: dissatisfied students allocated more of their avail-

    able time to paid employment. The relationship between time spent in employ-

    ment and satisfaction with a course is a further area that is worthy of additional

    investigation.

    Notes

    1. If Y = bX where b = a + cZ, then Y = (a + cZ) X, i.e., Y = aX + c(ZX). Hence a significant

    regression coefficient on the new variable formed by multiplying Z and X indicates the presence

    of an interaction effect. As is conventional, the interaction variables were mean centred to

    reduce possible problems caused by multicollinearity.

    2. A copy of the full questionnaire is available from the author.

    3. PLS Graph 3 calculates the standard errors on parameter estimates using a bootstrapping

    procedure. Although most of the variables involved five-point categorical data, it is assumed

    (in line with most other research using attitudinal data—see Allison, 1999, p.10) that the

    difference in the strength of agreement between, for example, points one and two on a scale

    is comparable to the difference between points four and five. This is regarded as a reason-

    able approximation in the present circumstances. Allison (1999) described the effects of 

    employing ordinal scales in linear regression as ‘innocuous’ in the absence of a priori 

    grounds for believing that non-linear responses should apply within particular divisions of a

    scale.

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    25/27

    160 R. Bennett 

    Notes on contributor

    Roger Bennett is a Professor in the Department of Business and Service Sector

    Management at London Metropolitan University. His main research interests

    are in the fields of marketing and business management, particularly in relation

    to non-profit organisations. Additionally Roger has completed a number of 

    investigations in the area of educational research, focusing on issues concerning

    student retention and motivation. Roger is the author of many books and a large

    number of journal articles on various aspects of business studies.

    References

    Allen, J., Fuller, D. & Luckett, M. (1998) Academic integrity: behaviours, rates and attitudes of 

    business students towards cheating, Journal of Marketing Education, 20(1), 41–53.

    Allison, P. (1999) Multiple regression: a primer  (London, Sage).Ameen, E., Guffey, D. & McMillan, J. (1996) Gender differences in determining the ethical sensi-

    tivity of future accounting professionals, Journal of Business Ethics, 15(5), 591–597.

    Anderman, E., Griesinger, T. & Westinger, G. (1998) Motivating and cheating during early

    adolescence, Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(1), 84–93.

    Archer, L. (2002, May 31) A question of motives, Times Higher Education Supplement, p. 14.

    Baird, J. (1980) Current trends in college cheating, Psychology in the Schools, 17(3), 515–522.

    Bennett, R. (2003) Determinants of undergraduate student drop-out rates in a university business

    studies department, Journal of Further & Higher Education, 27(2), 123–141.

    Bennett, R. & Kottasz, R. (2001) Marketing undergraduates’ attitudes towards query-based

    instructional machines as a possible learning medium, British Journal of Educational Technol-

    ogy, 32(4), 471–482.Bjorklund, M. & Wenestam, C. (1999) Academic cheating: frequency, methods and causes,

    Proceedings of the 1999 European Conference on Educational Research, Lahti, Finland. Available

    online at: www.leeds.ac.uk/educol/documents/00001364.htm

    Boshier, R. (1991) Psychometric properties of the alternative form of the education participation

    scale, Adult Education Quarterly, 41(2), 150–167.

    Braumoeller, B. & Gaines, B. (2001) Actions do speak louder than words: deterring plagiarism with the

    use of plagiarism detection software (Washington DC, American Political Science Association).

    Available online at: www.apsanet.org/PS/dec01/braumoeller.cfm

    Bruner, G. & Hensel, P. (1994)  Marketing scales handbook: a compilation of multi-item measures

    (Chicago, IL, American Marketing Association).

    Calabrese, R. & Cochran, J. (1990) The relationship of alienation to cheating among a sample of 

    American adolescents, Journal of Research & Development in Education, 23(2), 65–72.

    Caruana, A., Ramaseshan, B. & Ewing, M. (2000) The effect of anomie on academic dishon-

    esty among university students,  International Journal of Educational Management,  14(1),

    23–30.

    Chin, W. (2001) PLS graph user’s guide version 3 (Houston, TX, Soft Modeling Inc).

    Churchill, G. (1979) A paradigm for developing better measures of marketing constructs, Journal 

    of Marketing Research, 16(2), 64–73.

    Clough, P. (2003) Old and new challenges in automatic plagiarism detection  (Sheffield, Plagiarism

    Advisory Service, University of Sheffield).

    Council of Writing Program Administrators (CWPA) (2003) Defining and avoiding plagiarism: the

    WPA statement on best practice (New York, CWPA).

    Davies, P. (2000) Computerised peer assessment,  Education & Training International,  37(4),

    346–355.

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    26/27

     Factors associated with student plagiarism 161

    Davis, S. & Ludvigson, H. (1995) Additional data on academic dishonesty and a proposal for

    remediation, Teaching of Psychology, 22(2), 119–121.

    Davis, S., Grover, C., Becker, A. & McGregor, L. (1992) Academic dishonesty: prevalence, deter-

    minants, techniques and punishments, Teaching of Psychology, 19(1), 16–20.

    Davis, S., Pierce, M., Yandell, L. & Arnow, P. (1995) Cheating in college and the Type A person-

    ality, College Student Journal, 29(4), 493–497.Engelland, B., Alford, B. & Taylor, R. (2001) Cautions and precautions on the use of ‘borrowed’

    scales in marketing research, in: T. Sutor (Ed.)  Marketing advances in pedagogy, process & 

     philosophy, Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Society for Marketing Advances  (New

    Orleans, Society for Marketing Advances), 152–156.

    Franklyn-Stokes, A. & Newstead, S. (1995) Undergraduate cheating: who does what and why?

    Studies in Higher Education, 20(2), 159–172.

    Furedi, F. (2003, July 25) Shortcut to success, Times Higher Education Supplement, p. 16.

    Haines, V., Diekhoff, G., LaBeff, E. & Clark, R. (1986) College cheating: immaturity, lack of 

    commitment and the neutralising attitude, Research in Higher Education, 25(4), 342–354.

    Hammond, M. (2002) Cyber-plagiarism: are FE students getting away with words? (Leeds, University

    of Leeds). Available online at: www.leeds.ac.uk/educol/documents/00002055.htmHannabuss, S. (2001) Contested texts: issues of plagiarism, Library Management, 26(6), 311–318.

    Houle, C. (1961) The inquiring mind  (Madison, WI, University of Wisconsin Press).

    Kreger, L. & Wrenn, R. (1990) Perspectives and differences, New Directions for Student Services,

    51(1), 37–47.

    Kuehn, P., Stanwyck, D. & Holland, C. (1990) Attitudes toward cheating behaviours in the ESL 

    classroom, TESOL Quarterly, 24(2), 313–317.

    Larkham, P. & Manns, S. (2002) Plagiarism and its treatment in higher education,  Journal of 

     Further & Higher Education, 26(4), 339–349.

    Lipson, A. & McGraven, N. (1993) Undergraduate academic dishonesty at MIT: results from a study of 

    attitudes and behaviour of undergraduates, faculty and graduate teaching assistants (Boston, MA,

    Massachusetts Institute of Technology).Maslen, G. (2003, July 10) Dirty marks, The Bulletin, 3–4.

    McCabe, D. & Trevino, K. (1996) What we know about cheating in college,  Change,  28(1),

    28–33.

    McKenzie, J. (1998) The new plagiarism: seven antidotes to prevent highway robbery in an elec-

    tronic age, Educational Technology Journal, 7(8), 1–11.

    Meyer, J., Allen, N. & Smith, C. (1993) Commitment to organisations and occupations: exten-

    sion and test of a three-component conceptualisation,  Journal of Applied Psychology,  78(4),

    538–551.

    Michie, F., Glachan, M. & Bray, D. (2001) An evaluation of factors influencing the self-concept,

    self-esteem and academic stress for direct and re-entry students in higher education,  Educa-

    tional Psychology, 21(40), 455–472.Myers, S. (1998) Questioning author(ity): ESL/EFL, science and teaching about plagiarism,

    TESL-EJ, 3(2), 11–20.

    Newstead, S., Franklyn-Stokes, A. & Armstead, P. (1996) Individual differences in student cheat-

    ing, Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(2), 229–241.

    Oliver, R. (1993) Cognitive, affective and attribute bases of the satisfaction response,  Journal of 

    Consumer Research, 20(4), 418–430.

    Philips, M. & Horton, V. (2000) Cybercheating: has morality evaporated in business education?

    International Journal of Educational Management, 14(4), 150–155.

    Saenz, T., Marcouldes, G., Junn, E. & Young, R. (1999) The relationship between college experi-

    ence and academic performance among minority students, International Journal of Educational 

     Management, 13(4), 199–207.

    Sanders, C. (2002, February 1) Debt grows ever bigger and even more painful,  Times Higher 

    Education Supplement, pp. 6–7.

  • 8/18/2019 Bennett 2005 Factors Associated With Student Plagiarism in a Post 1992 University

    27/27

    162 R. Bennett 

    Sims, R. (1995) The severity of academic dishonesty: a comparison of faculty and student views,

    Psychology in Schools, 32(3), 233–238.

    Standler, R. (2000) Plagiarism in colleges in the USA. Available online at: www.rbs2.com/plag.htm

    Stoynoff, S. (1996) Self-regulated learning strategies of international students: a study of high and

    low achievers, College Student Journal, 30(2), 329–336.

    Sutton, E. & Huba, M. (1995) Undergraduate student perceptions of academic dishonesty as afunction of ethnicity and religious participation, NASPA Journal, 33(1), 19–34.

    Yeung, S., Wong, S. & Chan, B. (2002) Ethical beliefs of hospitality and tourism students

    toward their school life, International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management, 14(4),

    183–192.