anticipated ongoing interaction versus channel effects on ... · 476 human communication research /...

15
472 HUMAN COMMUNICAmON RESEARCH / June 1994 Rice, R* E#, &Z Borgman, C. L. (1983). The use of computer monitored data in information science and communication research. Journal of the American Societyfor Information Science; 4,247-256. Rice, R. E, & Love, G. (1987). Electronic emotion: Socioemotional content in a computer- mediated communication network. Communication Research, I4,85-108, Rogers, E MO (1986). Communication technology; The new media in society. New York Free Press. Salancik, G e R a , 8 ~ Pfeffer, J. (1978). A social information processing approach to job attitudes and task design. Administrative Science Quarterly, 23,224253. Saunders, C (1990). The strategic contingencies theory of power: Multiple perspectives. louml of Management Studies, 27,1-18. Schmitt, J. & Fulk, J. (1991). Organizational colleagues, media richness, and electronic mail. Communication Research, 18,487-523. Scott, W. R. (1987). The adolescence of institutional theory. Administrative Science Quarterly, 32,493-511. Scott Morton, M. S. (1991). The corporation of the 1990s: bzforma tion technology and organizational transformation. New York: Oxford University Press. Siegel, J. (1989). Managers’ communication preferences for and actual use of new telecom- munications technology. Paper presented to the Academy of Management. Siegel, J., Dubrovsky, V., Kiesler, S.fl&McGuire, T. W. (1986). Group processes in computer- mediated communication. Organizational Z3ehavior and Human Decision Processes, 37, 157-187. Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social context cues: Electronic mail in organizational communication. Mamgmmt Science, 32,1492-1512. Sproull, L., Kiesler, S., & Zubrow, D. (1984). Encountering an alien culture. JoumZ of Social Issues, 40,31-48. Stein, L. I. (1967). The doctor-nurse game. Archives of Genera2 Psychiatry, 16,6%704. Telhs-Nayak, M., & Tellis-Nayak, V. (1984). Games that professionals play: The social psychology of physician-nurse interaction. So&Z Science and Medicine, Z&l0631069. Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated communication. Communi- cation Research, 29,52-90. Walther, J. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1992). Relational communication in computer-mediated interaction. Human Communication Research, l9,50-88. Welsh, L. (1982). Using electronic mail as a teaching tool. Communications of the ACM, 23, 131-152. Winters, D. (1985). Perceptions of occupational status of army nurses and physicians regard- ing themselves and each other. Military Medicine, 250,297-299. Zuboff, S. (1988). In the age of the smart machine. New York Basic. i A n t O n I n V e r C h a E f o n R e C o m i n C o I n t JOSEPH B. WALTHER Northwestern University Previous research on the interpersonal tone of computer-mediated communication shows d@krent effects using longitudinal computer-mediated groups than arefound in research using one-shot groups, even before the developmental aspects associated with time can accrue. One factor distinguishing these approaches is the anticipation offiture interaction experienced by longitudinal groups. This research reports an experiment assessing the relative effects of anticipatedfuture interaction and d#erent communication media (computer-mediated versus face-to-face communication) on the communication of relational intimacy and composure. Asynchronous and synchronous. computer confmencing and face-to-face groups were exam- ined. Results show that the assignment of long-&n versus short-term partnerships has a larger impact on anticipated future interaction reported by computer-medtited, rather than face-to-face, partners. Evidence also shows that anticipation is a more potent predictor of several relational communication dimensions than is communication condition. Implications for theoy and practice are identtfied. The way you meet people in cyberspace puts a different spin on affiliation: in traditional kinds of communities, we are accustomed to meeting people, then getting to know them; in virtual communities, you can get to know people and then choose to meet them. (Rheingold, 1993, pp. 26-27) I n t h e l a s t t w o d e c a r e s o n c o c t i o n ( C M C h a s m o u n i t w o u s e e e x C M h b a w i d e t o o l a n d i n s o m s e t m a b e t m e o m g e r i a c h o i ( M a r i n p r e C M C i n i n c c a t i o r e s e a s a g r e d e a o f r e s o n t h n a o f C f o n s o c i c o g n i n t e a n d p s y p r t a t h e w a y p e o p e x c h m e s v i s t h m e a n i c s o n t o t r a d f a c e a n d g r o c o ( & M a r k 1 9 8 7 S . K i e s S i e & M c G 1 9 A l t a Joseph WaZther (Ph.D., University of Arizona, 1990) is an assistant professor of Communication Studies at Northwestern University Completion of this research was supported by a faculty development grant from the School of Speech at Northwestern University. Aprevious version of this article was presented at the annual meeting of the Speech Communication Association, November, 1993. H u m a n C o m m u n R e s e a V o l . 2 0 N o . 4 , @ 1 9 9 4 I n t e r n C o m m u A s s o J u n e 1 9 9 4 7 3 473

Upload: others

Post on 28-Oct-2019

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

472 HUMAN COMMUNICAmON RESEARCH / June 1994

Rice, R* E#, &Z Borgman, C. L. (1983). The use of computer monitored data in information science and communication research. Journal of the American Societyfor Information Science; 4,247-256.

Rice, R. E, & Love, G. (1987). Electronic emotion: Socioemotional content in a computer- mediated communication network. Communication Research, I4,85-108,

Rogers, E MO (1986). Communication technology; The new media in society. New York Free Press. Salancik, G e R a , 8 ~ Pfeffer, J. (1978). A social information processing approach to job attitudes

and task design. Administrative Science Quarterly, 23,224253. Saunders, C (1990). The strategic contingencies theory of power: Multiple perspectives.

louml of Management Studies, 27,1-18. Schmitt, J. & Fulk, J. (1991). Organizational colleagues, media richness, and electronic mail.

Communication Research, 18,487-523. Scott, W. R. (1987). The adolescence of institutional theory. Administrative Science Quarterly,

32,493-511. Scott Morton, M. S. (1991). The corporation of the 1990s: bzforma tion technology and organizational

transformation. New York: Oxford University Press. Siegel, J. (1989). Managers’ communication preferences for and actual use of new telecom-

munications technology. Paper presented to the Academy of Management. Siegel, J., Dubrovsky, V., Kiesler, S.fl & McGuire, T. W. (1986). Group processes in computer-

mediated communication. Organizational Z3ehavior and Human Decision Processes, 37, 157-187.

Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social context cues: Electronic mail in organizational communication. Mamgmmt Science, 32,1492-1512.

Sproull, L., Kiesler, S., & Zubrow, D. (1984). Encountering an alien culture. JoumZ of Social Issues, 40,31-48.

Stein, L. I. (1967). The doctor-nurse game. Archives of Genera2 Psychiatry, 16,6%704. Telhs-Nayak, M., & Tellis-Nayak, V. (1984). Games that professionals play: The social

psychology of physician-nurse interaction. So&Z Science and Medicine, Z&l0631069. Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated communication. Communi-

cation Research, 29,52-90. Walther, J. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1992). Relational communication in computer-mediated

interaction. Human Communication Research, l9,50-88. Welsh, L. (1982). Using electronic mail as a teaching tool. Communications of the ACM, 23,

131-152. Winters, D. (1985). Perceptions of occupational status of army nurses and physicians regard-

ing themselves and each other. Military Medicine, 250,297-299. Zuboff, S. (1988). In the age of the smart machine. New York Basic.

i

A n t i c i p a t e d O n g o i n g I n t e r a c t i o n V e r s u s C h a n n e l E f f e c t s o n R e l a t i o n a l C o m m u n i c a t i o n i n C o m p u t e r - M e d i a t e d I n t e r a c t i o n

JOSEPH B. WALTHER Northwestern University

Previous research on the interpersonal tone of computer-mediated communication shows d@krent effects using longitudinal computer-mediated groups than arefound in research using one-shot groups, even before the developmental aspects associated with time can accrue. One factor distinguishing these approaches is the anticipation offiture interaction experienced by longitudinal groups. This research reports an experiment assessing the relative effects of anticipatedfuture interaction and d#erent communication media (computer-mediated versus face-to-face communication) on the communication of relational intimacy and composure. Asynchronous and synchronous. computer confmencing and face-to-face groups were exam- ined. Results show that the assignment of long-&n versus short-term partnerships has a larger impact on anticipated future interaction reported by computer-medtited, rather than face-to-face, partners. Evidence also shows that anticipation is a more potent predictor of several relational communication dimensions than is communication condition. Implications for theo y and practice are identtfied.

The way you meet people in cyberspace puts a different spin on affiliation: in traditional kinds of communities, we are accustomed to meeting people, then getting to know them; in virtual communities, you can get to know people and then choose to meet them. (Rheingold, 1993, pp. 26-27)

I n t h e l a s t t w o d e c a d e s , r e s e a r c h o n c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a - t i o n ( C M C ) h a s m o u n t e d , i t w o u l d s e e m , e x p o n e n t i a l l y C M C h a s b e c o m e a w i d e s p r e a d t o o l , a n d i n s o m e s e t t i n g s , m a y b e “ t h e m e d i u m o f m a n a - g e r i a l c h o i c e ” ( M a r k u s , i n p r e s s ) . C M C i n t e r e s t s i n t e r p e r s o n a l c o m m u n i - c a t i o n r e s e a r c h e r s , a s a g r e a t d e a l o f r e s e a r c h o n t h e n a t u r e o f C M C f o c u s e s o n s o c i a l c o g n i t i v e , i n t e r p e r s o n a l , a n d p s y c h o l o g i c a l p r o c e s s e s t h a t a f f e c t t h e w a y p e o p l e e x c h a n g e m e s s a g e s v i s - a - v i s t h e m e d i u m , a n d i n c o m p a r i - s o n t o t r a d i t i o n a l f a c e - t o - f a c e a n d g r o u p c o m m u n i c a t i o n ( C u l n a n & M a r k u s , 1 9 8 7 ; S . K i e s l e r , S i e g e l , & M c G u i r e , 1 9 8 4 ) . A l t h o u g h t h e r e a r e

Joseph WaZther (Ph.D., University of Arizona, 1990) is an assistant professor of Communication Studies at Northwestern University Completion of this research was supported by a faculty development grant from the School of Speech at Northwestern University. Aprevious version of this article was presented at the annual meeting of the Speech Communication Association, November, 1993.

H u m a n C o m m u n i c a t i o n R e s e a r c h , V o l . 2 0 N o . 4 , @ 1 9 9 4 I n t e r n a t i o n a l C o m m u n i c a t i o n A s s o c i a t i o n

J u n e 1 9 9 4 4 7 3 - 5 0 1

473

Page 2: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

474 HUMAN COMIMUNICATION RESBIRCH / June 1994

exceptions (e-g-, Yates & Orlikowski, 1?92), most theoretical accounts of CMC take into prominence cognitive and interpersonal antecedents and/or consequences of CMC effects. Although some argue that new media require new constructs and theories, others contend that there is more to be learned about new media using at least somewhat older general notions about interpersonal interaction.

Although much previous research reported depersonalizing effects of the medium, recent work has argued that developmental and social information exchange factors predict more positive interpersonal rela- tions. Walther ‘s (1992a) social information processing perspective holds that the form and the rate of interpersonal information exchanged via CMC differs from face-to-face (FtF) interaction, but that over time rela- tional effects via different media should be similar. Efforts to verify this position have met with mixed success. Although predictions were offered that longitudinal CMC would differ from FtF only in their initial interac- tions, findings have not demonstrated significant differences between media even in those early exchanges (Walther & Burgoon, 1992). This puzzling contrast emerges: One-shot CMC groups are less personal than FtF groups, but longitudinal groups’ first meetings are not. At present, no clear understanding has emerged that explains this contrast.

The purpose of this research is to re-examine propositions of the social information processing perspective on CMC, and to report an experiment involving communicators’ anticipation of future interaction. This latter variable has had an interesting, although inconsistent, history in interper- sonal communication research. Its potential effects speak directly to some previously untested assumptions of social information processing theory and, as will be shown, accounts for a great deal of the mystery concerning when CMC is likely to feel one way or another.

LITEMTURE REVIEW

CMC

The theoretical research on CMC falls into two general types. The first examines the effects of the CMC media on communication. The other seeks to explain media selection (e.g., Ciborra & Olson, 1988; Daft, Lengel, & Trevinof 1987; Fulk, Steinfield, Schmitz, & Power, 1987; Komsky 1991; Korzenny 1978; Kraut, Egido, & Galegher, 1988; Markus, 1987; Rice, Chang, & Torobin, 1992; Rice, Grant, Schmitz, & Torobin, 1990; Russ, Daft, & Lengel, 1990; Schmitz & Fulk, 1991; Steinfield, 1992; Trevino, Lengel, Boden- Steiner, Gerloff, & Muir, 1990; Trevino, Lengel, & Daft, 1987), although there are some exceptions and some hybrids. It is to this first concern that the present research is addressed.

Walther / ANTICIPATION VS. CHANNEL 475

Early laboratory research exploring CMC in group interaction reported empirical evidence that the content of CMC messages was more task- oriented than that of FtF groups (Hiltz, 1975; Hiltz, Johnson, & Agle, 1978; Hiltz, Johnson, & Turoff, 1986; Rice & Love, 1987; Siegel, Dubrovsky S. Kiesler, & McGuire, 1986). Invoking Social Presence Theory (Short, Williams, & Christie, 1976), these effects have been attributed to the lack of nonverbal cues in CMC. The theory holds that as communication channels filter out these cues, there is less salience of the co-presence of other people.

Using a similar focus on the number and types of cues filtered out, Sproull, S. Kiesler, and others have found that the absence of nonverbal “social context cues” causes CMC users to become depersonalized and interpersonally hostile. Such communication is marked by ticreased ex- cited and uninhibited messages such as “flaming” (insults, swearing, and hostile, intense language), greater seti-absorption versus other-orientation, as well as messages reflecting status equalization (Dubrovsb, S. Kiesler, & Sethna, 1991; S. Kiesler et al., 1984; McGuire, S. Kiesler, & Siegel, 1987; Siegel et al., 1986; Sproull & S. Kiesler, 1986).’

Such findings and theories have been widely reported and accepted (Rafaeli, 1988). For instance, Hiemstra’s (1982) review of early experi- ments on group conferencing led him to judge that “the research so far points to a general conclusion: As bandwidth narrows from face-to-face interaction to computer terminal interaction, the communication is likely to be experienced as less friendly emotional, and personal, and more serious, businesslike, depersonalized, and task oriented” (p. 883). Five years later, Rice and Love (1987) similarly summarized the literature as suggesting that “CMC, because of its lack of audio or video cues, will be perceived as impersonal and lacking in normative reinforcement, so there will be less socioemotional (SE) content exchanged” (p. 88). Even still, these approaches are propagated (SprouIl & Kiesler, 199lb, 1992) and widely repeated (see Lea, O’Shea, Fung, & Spears, 1992).

Similar conclusions about the impersonal nature of CMC have come from research involving self-reported projective assessments of media utility, in which participants rate the appropriateness or effectiveness of several communication media (FtF, telephone, CMC, etc.) for several types of communication purposes ranging across different levels of as- sumed equivocality or need for personalization (see Rice, 1984,1993; Rice & Shook, 1988). The results of these studies, as a whole, are mixed. Although some show more personalized assessments as a result of time and expe- rience with the media (Foulger, 1990), others show effects that are robust to time (Rice, 1993). Elsewhere, there are anecdotal reports that “interper- sonally involving interactions such as conflict resolution and negotiation consistently occur over electronic mail” (Fulk, Schrnitz, & Steinfield, 1990, p. 120; see also Markus, in press). In the final analysis, although “expec-

Page 3: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994

tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to be positively related to use” in terms of media selection (Steinfield, 1986, p. 781; Rice, 1998), the frequency of socially-oriented uses of CMC in organiza- tional field settings (see Walther, Anderson, & Park, in press) questions the correspondence between the projective and the actual use of CMC.

At the same time, a growing body of research has shown contradictory patterns. Field investigations have shown more supportive or more var- ied relations among communicators linked by CMC (e.g., Feldman, 1987; Ord, 1989; Weedman, 1991). Several case studies have described col- leagueships forming from serendipitous information exchanges (e.g., Johansen, DeGrasse, & Wilson, 1978). Some cases have quantified these warmer relational patterns (e.g., Beals, 1991). Field experiments have demonstrated that CMC task orientation is affected by exogenous vari- ables such as task complexity, task interdependence, environmental un- certainty, and the need for communication across locations (Steinfield, 1986) and organizational culture (e.g., Hiemstra, 1982). These efforts suggest that media bandwidth is an insufficient predictor of CMC effects on the nature of social interaction.

In light of these and other findings, some authors have claimed that technological determinist positions such as social presence and the lack of social context cues are a dead horse no longer to be beaten (Fulk, Schmitz, & Schwarz, 1992; Steinfield, 1992). Unfortunately this rejection is accompanied by references to new theories and evidence about media choice rather than media eficfs. Much of this media selection research argues that attributions about media utility are socially constructed through interaction in social networks (e.g., Fulk et al., 1990; Schmitz & Fulk, 1991; see Rice & Aydin, 1991). Such critiques do not address actual channel effects on communication behavior past the point of selection. More critically they dismiss previous media-effects theories without deal- ing squarely with the empirical between-condition communication differ- ences reported in early experimental research.

A recent theoretical alternative has focused on CMC/FtF differences as a function of the way participants process interpersonal information within the constraints of channels and time. Noting the difference be- tween lab and field studies, time and information-processing require- ments have been taken into consideration with which to discriminate CMC and FtF communication. According to the theory, rather than elimi- nating social information or “blinding” participants to it, CMC’s limited bandwidth might simply retard normal impression development and relational communication. The cause of this retardation is that CMC forces both task-related and social information into a single verbal/linguistic channel, and it takes more “real time” to exchange the same number of messages in CMC as it does in FtF interaction. This approach has been called a “social information processing perspective” (Walther, 1992a)

Walther / ANTICIPATION VS. CHANNEL 477

Research investigating this perspective has produced mixed results. A metaanalysis of previous empirical findings determined that time was a critical moderator of relational tone (Walther et al., in press). This work contrasted research with limited time periods against those lab and field studies that allowed unlimited interaction. Socioemotional expression was greater where interaction was longer. In another study Walther (19%) found that interpersonal impressions did develop over extended time interaction in CMC, and that they developed more slowly than in FtF interaction. These results support the social information processing approach.

Walther and Burgoon (1992) examined the relational communication dimensions of longitudinal FtF groups, and asynchronous CMC groups measured at three time points in five and a half weeks. Based on the social information processing perspective, they argued that differences between CMC and FtF should be confined to initial interactions and dissipate over time. Predictions specified initial differences between computer confer- encing and FtF groups consistent with previous cues&.ltered-out research, with convergence on relational communication dimensions between the two conditions over subsequent conversations. Participants’ reports con- firmed the convergence trends for some relational dimensions (immediacy/ affection, similarity/depth, composure/relaxation, dominance, at- tempted influence, receptivity/trust; see Burgoon & Hale, 1984,1987). On other dimensions, no effects between the conditions appeared at all (i.e., formality). On yet another dimension -task versus social orientation- CMC was more relationally positive than FtF from the very inception of interaction, and over time. However, with the exception of this last dimen- sion, in no cases were FtF scores at time one significantly different than CMC scores-contrary to hypotheses and in contrast to previous research.

In a similar analysis using outside coders’ observations, Walther (in press) found that asynchronous CMC was rated higher on all dimensions of relational intimacy (immediacy/affection, similarity/depth, receptivity/ trust), and on social (vs. task) orientation, from time-one and throughout. Although these results refute a strict bandwidth/depersonalization ap- proach, the lack of time-one channel differences in Walther and Burgoon (1992) and the superiority for CMC on some dimensions in Walther (in press) were not anticipated and are not accounted for by social informa- tion processing theory as originally articulated.

The question arising from these anomalous findings adds a different twist to the previous puzzles in the area. It was formerly simple enough to ask, why is CMC different in longitudinal versus restricted groups? The hypothetical answer was that groups develop over time and that one-shot experiments failed to capture these dynamics-the suspicion essentially that was tested by Walther and colleagues. The question now has become, why do CMC groups appear no less intimate and social than FtF groups

Page 4: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

478 FRJMAN COMMUNICA~ON RESEARCH / June 1994

even irz the initial meeting$ of longitudinal interaction, relative to one-shot CMC studies? The contention that CMC groups develop over time does not account for this phenomenon, because whatever effects may be gleaned over the course of time would not explain the similarity of longitudinal CMC groups to FtF groups in their initial encounters. These first meetings should resemble those of one-shot groups, as temporal dynamics would have had little chance to accrue. Some other effect may be operative, which may yield a differential impact in CMC conditions than it does in traditional FtF groups.

Anticipated Future Interaction

One suspicion has been that there may be some effect of the anticipation of future interaction on the uncertainty-reduction and affiliation processes employed by communicators in these longitudinal experiments (Walther, 199Zb). Although the zero-history group members in these studies,knew nothing about their partners as they undertook their meetings, partners hew they would be communicating with and interdependent upon their partners over time. At first glance, it might seem that the anticipation of future interaction should be constant across communication conditions, within either respective time frame: In both longitudinal and one-shot experiments, participants knew in advance whether their association was long-term or short-term. Were this the case, the anticipation factor should not account for the differences between the interpersonal behavior of longitudinal (similar) and short-term (dissimilar) CMC, relative to FtF. FtF communication in one-shot meetings might be much the same as one-shot CMC groups, because FtF partners had no specific indication of continued interaction.

There is good reason to suspect, however, that the anticipation of future interaction may actually be greater, and more of a constant in any FtF interaction, regardless of time frame, although lower and more variable in CMC. Previous research on anticipated future interaction in interper- sonal communication reveals some anomalies that suggest this to be the case. In several such efforts, the anticipated future interaction variable has been difficult to manipulate. For example, Kellermann (1986) indoctri- nated research participants by telling some participants they would defi- nitely interact with an assigned partner two different times, told others they might interact again, and told others they would meet their partners only once. On a manipulation check, those who were told they would meet more than once indicated a high anticipation of future interaction. Interestingly those who were told they would not meet again also showed a moderate anticipation of future interaction. Kellermann concluded, “indi- viduals often don’t expect to meet others, though they ‘hedge’ their bets and allow for some possibility of another interaction” (p. 57). Douglas (1987)

Walther / ANTICIPATION VS. CHANNEL 479

reported a similar problem; although continuously measured scores of actual anticipation did differ as a result of an anticipation manipulation, the mean score for the no-anticipation participants was surprisingly higher than expected. This lead Douglas to conclude that “no anticipated future interaction was not, in fact, operationalized” (p. 242). Both authors, in retrospect, felt that this phenomenon seemed especially plausible among student participants from the same college, who were not unlikely to recognize one another at campus or community locales.

Although the question of anticipated future interaction has not been considered in previous CMC research, it seems very likely that it may have been operative among FtF-control group-partners in early CMC ex- periments. Some anticipation of future interaction may be an inherent aspect of FtF conversation. FtF participants-especially student partici- pants-may “infer that they may recognize and talk to each other in the future” (Douglas, 1987, p. 242). Such a phenomenon seems far less likely however, in one-shot, zero-history CMC-only groups such as those em- ployed in early CMC experiments. The anticipation phenomenon is more likely to occur among people who see each others’ faces and hear their voices (as in experimental FtF groups) than among partners who never see or hear each other, as in zero-history CMC groups. Certainly they grasp the issue, how could they possibly know, down the road, whether they “bumped into each other,” or not? The upshot of this observation is that all FtF groups-one-shot or longitudinal-might experience some anticipation of future interaction, but that this effect is moderated in CMC and may only pertain where members are cognizant of a deliberate longitudinal relationship.

If this is indeed the case, then participants in CMC-only groups in their initial encounters should be affected much more by the anticipation of future interaction inherent in a longitudinal association than would be FtF partners. In other words, the knowledge of a long-term versus short-term encounter should affect CMC partners’ anticipation, but not FtF mem- bers’. This diagnosis is offered as a hypothesis.

Hl: CMC partners’ anticipation of future interaction is more greatly influenced by a long-term versus short-term assignment than is the anticipation among FtF partners.

If such a pattern as the above exists, and if it is shown to relate to the interpersonal behavior of CMC groups, then this factor may account for the previously observed differences between the relational behavior of one-shot CMC groups and longitudinal groups’ initial interactions. Of even greater consequence, if it can also be shown that the anticipation factor operates differently in one-shot CMC and FtF groups, then it must

Page 5: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

480 HUMAN COMMUNICA~ON RESEARCH / June 1994

be considered that the depersonalizing effects of CMC observed in past research may not be a function of the medium at all, but rather a function of the anticipation of future interaction that the medium moderates. A rationale for such an effect follows.

The importance of the anticipation of future interaction on interper- sonal behavior has received great attention in theoretical and empirical reformulations of Uncertainty Reduction Theory (Berger & Calabrese, 1975). According to the original theory there is a positive relationship between uncertainty and information-seeking, and an inverse relation- ship between uncertainty and liking/affiliative expression/intimacy. Although these relationships suggest static states at the point of pre- interaction (see Kellermann & Reynolds, 1990), they are conceived to predict the process of initial exchanges; uncertainty causes information- seeking, which reciprocally decreases uncertainty leading to increased liking and intimacy Later reformulations (e.g., Berger, 1979; Berger & Bradac, 1982) have posited a role of anticipated future interaction, such that anticipation increases the motivation to reduce uncertainw It can be reasoned, by extension, that greater anticipation of future interaction may lead to greater information seeking and intimacy/liking.2

At the same time, direct effects on interpersonal behavior or interper- sonal evaluations have been shown to result from anticipated future interaction. Studies have shown that anticipation of future interaction increases the amount of biographical and demographic information ex- changed among actors (Calabrese, 1975). Persons anticipating future in- teraction with a target person, when acquiring some information about the target, feel more similarity to that target person than do persons without such anticipation (Berger & Douglas, 1981). Anticipation causes actors to present themselves more positively and with greater friendliness (see for review Kellermann & Reynolds, 1990). Anticipated future inter- action interacts with gender and gender of one’s partner on dyadic self-disclosure (Cline & Musolf, 1985). In negotiation settings, anticipation causes more cooperative behavior (see Heide & Miner, 1992). All these “symptoms” of anticipated future interaction seem very much like what FtF groups and longitudinal CMC groups have been found to do, relative to one-shot CMC groups. Indeed, in one of the few one-shot CMC experi- ments that did not find more positive interaction evaluations in FtF than in CMC, the participants in each condition “were led to expect that they would be interacting further with the other subjects in the experiment” (Matheson & Zanna, 1990, p. 4).

In summary, previous theories cannot account for the anomalous dif- ferences between one-shot CMC groups’ and longitudinal CMC groups’

initial interactions with respect both to each other or between CMC and FtF controls, on intimacy (immediacy/affection, similarity/depth, receptivity/

Walther / ANTICIF’ATlON VS. CHANNEL 481

trust) and social orientation. The anticipation of future interaction is posited to affect CMC and FtF in the direction of greater intimacy and social vs. task orientation. The effect of anticipated interaction should account for differences in relational behavior more than should differ- ences between communication conditions.

Additionally the dimension of composure/relaxation will be exam- ined. The composure/relaxation dimension reflects the degree to which communicators express relaxation and calm, or tension, discomfort, and nervous arousal (Burgoon & Hale, 1987). Although no predicted or anoma- lous differences on composure have emerged in previous relational CMC research, the dimension holds a special interest nevertheless. S. Kiesler, Zubrow, Moses, and Geller (1985) predicted that communicators’ physi- ological arousal (pulse and palmar sweat) would be higher in CMC than FtF across time. They found, however, a main effect for time such that arousal declined significantly across three measurement points in both conditions. That particular study marks a break from the one-shot ap- proach to CMC/FtF; all participants in both conditions expected at least some ongoing interaction. It remains to be seen whether communication of arousal (non-composure) will change as a function of variation in anticipation; uncertainty reduction theory suggests that it should.

H2: Anticipated future interaction is a better predictor than is communication condition of (a) immediacy/affection, (b) similarity/depth, (c) receptivity/ trust, (d) composure/relaxation, and (e) social orientation.

Finally a research question was posed regarding the potential variation between two major classes of computer conferencing, synchronous (in which participants communicate simultaneously) and asynchronous (in which participants read and write messages at independent times). There are conflicting opinions regarding the effect of synchrony in CMC. Some researchers assert that the determining characteristic of CMC effects is the number and types of cues filtered out, and that asynchronous e-mail and synchronous conferencing are similar environments with common char- acteristics (S. Kiesler et al., 1985). If this is so, both forms of computer conferencing should be similarly different than FtF interaction. Others, such as Rafaeli (1988), have argued that the degree of “interactivity” a system features affects user’s adaptation and the communication they transmit (see Nass & Mason, 1990; Rice, 1986). From this perspective, asynchronous CMC should differ from both synchronous CMC and FE

What might be the interpersonal effects of one or another level of synchrony? This is difficult to know, as few if any studies have examined both. Despite the potential negative reactions to the absence of nonverbal cues, or in response thereto, the potential for positive effects in synchro-

Page 6: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

482 HUMAN COMMUNICA~ON RESEARCH / June 194

nous CMC are suggested by several observations. According to Rice (1986, p. 230), synchronous CMC allows immediate responses, and “personal cues often appear in new forms, while direct questions are almost always answered.” Ferrera, Brunner, and Whittemore’s (1991) linguistic analysis found that synchronous CMC users employ frequent you and 1 references, omission of articles and unstressed subject pronouns, the shortening of words through abbreviations and symbols, and cobula omissions, all of which may connote more familiar interaction.

As for asynchronous CMC, there are mixed opinions on its potential interpersonal effects. McGrath (1990) speculates that the response lags in combination with the loss of nonverbal cues may increase uncertainty and disrupt groups’ “entrainment” -their ability to synchronize their activi- ties, manage their temporal commitments, and “regulat[e] the flow of task and interpersonal interaction” (McGrath, 1991, p. 162). Walther (in press) counter-argues that asynchronous CMC alleviates entrainment problems of normal group activities. When partners need not coordinate in real time “temporal commitments become discretionary, and task versus interper- sonal interaction becomes, in a sense, de-regulated; both task and social exchange may exist without one constraining the time available for the other.” Asynchronous communication may facilitate more positive rela- tional communication, according to Walther and Burgoon (1992, p. 79), as it may foster “selective self presentation and relational behavior(s) . . . which are more stereotypically desirable.”

There seems no clear guideline as to which form of CMC is most appropriate for theoretical research about the medium. Most of the early CMC research experiments employed synchronous systems, although most field studies and organizational uses of CMC rely on asynchronous interaction (see Archer, 1990; Hiltz, Turoff, & Johnson, 1989). Some main- tam that FtF/synchronous comparisons are more appropriate. Alterna- tively Walther and Burgoon (1992) suggested that comparing FtF to asynchronous CMC maximizes experimental variance. One study which compared forced turn-taking in CMC to simultaneous CMC message generation found no effect on decision quality or task orientation of messages. The authors (Siegel et al., 1986) concluded that CMC effects are robust to these variations, but acknowledged that “greater generalizabil- ity would result from a study which compared simultaneous computer- mediated communication with [asynchronous] computerized communi- cation” (p. 177). Given this suggestion, and methodological implications of the answer (discussed below), the following is offered:

RQl: What differences exist between Asynchronous CMC, Synchronous CMC, and FtF communication on (a) immediacy/affection, (b) similarity/depth, (c) receptivity/trust, (d) composure/relaxation, and (e) social orientation?

Walther / ANTICPA~ON VS. CHANNEL 483

METHOD

In overview, participants were recruited and assigned to CMC or FtF groups of three. Half of the group members in each condition were led to expect ongoing interaction for three tasks over six weeks; the other half were led to expect different partners for each task. Meetings took place in each condition after which questionnaires were administered. i

Participants

In order to form zero-history groups, participants were recruited from several courses in three different departments in two colleges at a mid- *western university The project announcement indicated that the volun- tary participation required several weeks’ duration, and offered students course credit commensurate with that effort in accordance with the guide- lines of the classes from which they came. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three communication conditions. Three-person groups were formed on the basis that participants came from different courses- of-origin. Groups were randomly assigned to anticipation versus no- anticipation of ongoing interaction manipulations. In order to satisfy requirements for another ongoing research project, the number of FtF assignments was larger than the two CMC conditions (although the regression procedures reported below are robust to unequal sample sizes). The final demographics after participants were dropped for incomplete responses and other technical problems were as follows: N = 114, with 48 in FtF, 27 in asynchronous CMC, and 39 in synchronous CMC. Ages ranged from 18 to 38 with a mean of 22. Forty-four percent of participants were male and 56% were female. The median class was the junior level. Analysis of variance detected no differences between communication conditions or the anticipation manipulation groups in terms of demo- graphic compositions.

Anticipation

Participants were instructed that they would be working in groups of three, on three consecutive tasks over a period of six weeks, to study some questions regarding group interaction. In order to vary anticipated future interaction, participants were told to expect one of the following assign- ments: (1) that they would work with the same two partners on all three tasks throughout the project-the long-term condition-or (2) that they would be assigned new partners for each of the three tasks they worked on-the short-term condition. This manipulation strategy is similar to those used in previous studies (e.g., Cline & Musolf, 1985; Douglas, 1987;

Page 7: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

484 HUMAN COMIIMUNICA~ON RESEARCH / June 194

C. Kiesler# S Kiesler, & Pallak, 1967), with obvious variations given the group nature, time frame, and communication channel.

CMC

TWO CMC systems were used. The first was an asynchronous confer- encing system, Vaxnotes. Vaxnotes participants read and entered com- ments at times of their own choosing, without the temporal co-presence of their partners. Messages were imprinted with usemames-first initial plus last name, in this case-and the date and time of entry Messages were organized within topical areas, and users imprinted each mesage with a subject line; the topics, message numbers, and subject lines appeared at sign-on as a table of contents. The system also indicated which messages were new (yet unread), and users could advance between unread mes- sages with a single keystroke. CMC participants were trained in the use of the system at one of six training meetings (arranged so that members of groups came to separate sessions). They were also acquainted with the participation details for the project and told that they would receive final group assignments and instructions as to whether they would work with the same group or different groups on each of three projects.

Participants picked up final instructions posted on a (manual) bulletin board and began their conferences the next week. A manipulation check was included in the instruction material, along with an informed consent form, and some pretests (the results to be reported elsewhere). The manipulation check asked participants to confirm their subject number, usemame, group number, and long-term vs. short-term group assign- ment. It also listed the names of the other group members and asked whether any participants knew each other outside these groups, and none did. These questionnaires and informed consent forms were returned to a tip-off box below the bulletin board by the asynchronous CMC participants.

The other CMC system was synchronous, the “Phone” utility on the Vax minicomputer. Phone requires all participants to participate in real time. The screen is split into several horizontal areas, in this case four (one for each group member and one for a recording device). Each pane shows the usemame of the participant who writes in that frame. The Phone system displays each message to all participants as it is being composed, a character at a time. Participants all used the system in different rooms within the same building, but because they were instructed to report to different places, they did not encounter each other off-line. Participants were given instructions on the use of the system along with pretests, manipulation checks, and consent forms. When all questions had been answered satisfactorily, conferencing began.

The participants addressed one of three tasks used in previous confer- encing research (Walther & Burgoon, 1992). Each task presented an aca-

Walther / ANTICIPATION VS. CHANNEL 485

demic policy dilemma and asked the participants to review all relevant issues, discuss alternatives, and to make a consensual recommendation to university administration. They were instructed to indicate when they had completed the task, in a conference note. The tasks were assigned to groups through simple rotation. Asynchronous CMC groups read the task, along with their completion deadline, as the first message in each conference; synchronous groups read a photocopy As the deadline ap- proached, asynchronous participants were informed in a conference note that there was a questionnaire to pick up from the bulletin board, to be filled out immediately after the task was completed and returned to the drop-off box as soon as possible. Participants were instructed that their next task (or task plus group assignment) would be posted on the bulletin board.

FtF

FtF groups received an orientation via printed instructions and individual discussions in secluded rooms prior to their meeting. These instructions also informed them of their condition assignments. They received the same pretests, questionnaires, and forms as the CMC participants.

FtF discussions took place in interaction laboratory rooms resembling conference rooms. A conference table with table-top microphone, three upholstered chairs, paper name placards, and the same tasks-in printed form-met participants as they entered the room. A one-way mirror covered a portion of one wall. Participants were given no other guidance regarding how to proceed, except to notify an assistant in the next room when they deemed their task complete. Participants were separated into individual rooms and completed dependent measure questionnaires when they finished their task. They were told that they would be informed of the date for their next meeting (or the date plus their new group members) on the bulletin board.

When all of these “first” meetings were completed and all question- naires were returned, participants were notified that their participation requirements were actually complete. Participants received and signed a debriefing form to indicate their understanding of the true purpose of the experiment and the deceptive manipulation. They were further informed that they would receive full credit as if they had participated in three, rather than one, meetings; no complaints were registered.

Measures

A measure was devised to assess participants’ perceived anticipation of future interaction with other group members, as a continuous variable.

Page 8: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

486 HUM-AN COMMUNICA~ON RESEARCH / June 1994

Although the dichotomous assignment into long-term/ short-term teams should affect anticipation of future interaction, previous manipulation failures (see Douglas, 1987; Kellermann, 1986) and interest in specificity suggested that the wisest course was to tap participants’ actual, rather than assigned, anticipation. The measure included items based on pre- vious research on anticipation, including items designed to assess partici- pants’ expectation that they might recognize their partners during some later encounter at various locations (see Kellermann, 1986). The latter items were included because this recognition has been troublesome in previous explorations of anticipated interaction. Interviews with an offset participant group determined the most expected places for chance meet- ings to occur, in order to generate expected recognition items. In total, nine items administered in the posttest assessed the anticipation variable (see Appendix 1).

A factor analysis was conducted on the anticipation scales. A priori criteria for the acceptability of factors included eigenvalues of 1.5 or greater, no fewer than three items, acceptable alpha reliability and mean- ingfulness and interpretability of each factor. Initial principal components analysis suggested eigenvalues for three factors, but two factors contained only two items even after rotation. A two-factor model (which suggested eqec&ncy and recognition dimensions) met the above criteria except that reliability for the second factor was .48 with any more than two items. A unidimensional model produced lower-than-desirable alpha reliability of .78 with one item deleted, but seemed to describe the data in the most meaningful way, and was used in subsequent analyses.

Also included in the posttest were scales measuring relational commu- nication. As is well-accepted, messages may vary along many dimensions besides their content, and relational communication refers to those mes- sage aspects people employ to reflect and to manage their interpersonal affect, status, and their relationship definition (see Burgoon & Hale, 1984). Using a multidimensional approach to explore the interpersonal tone of CMC offers researchers more refined analyses than reliance on less com- plex measures (see Lea, 1991; Walther, 1992a). In order to untangle unex- plained trends on certain relational communication dimensions from previous research using these measures, several dimensions were selected for assessment in the present study These included dimensions related to intimacy: immediacy/afiction, incorporating affection, inclusion, and in- volvement; simihrity/‘epth, or the extent to which partners express homo- phily and the desire for a deeper relationship; and receptivity/trust, the expression of openness, rapport, and confidence. The other dimensions include c~~p~s~~e/~e~~~~~i~~, or the degree to which communicators ex- press calmness versus tension and arousal, and social- versus iask-orientation, reflecting the range of personal to work-related concern (Burgoon & Hale, 1987, which offers the original measurement items; the version used in

Walther / ANTICIPATION VS. CHANNEL 487

this research was modified for groups, as in Walther & Burgeon, 1992, and is available upon request). Alpha reliabilities and the number of items for these dimensions in the present administration, after removal of detract- ing items, were immediacy/affection, .93 (14 items); similarity/depth, .75 (9 items); receptivity/trust, .78 (7 items); composure/relaxation, .80 (7 items); and task-social orientation, .80 (4 items).

RESULTS

Anticipation by Condition

Hypothesis one predicted that CMC groups’ actual anticipation of future interaction is more influenced by assigned group longevity than is that of FtF groups. Analysis of variance was conducted using a 3 (condi- tions) by 2 (short-term/long-term) design3 Hl was supported. Actual anticipation score was affected by a condition by term-assignment inter- action, F(2,91) = 3.02, p = .054. Amain effect for communication condition also emerged, F(2,91) = 30.47, p c .OOl. The pattern of means indicates that Asynchronous CMC and Synchronous CMC members reflected the an- ticipation of future interaction in accordance to that expected by their length of continued association, but that FtF participants’ anticipation was unaffected by the longevity of their groups (see Table 1). Post hoc Newman-Keuls’ tests revealed that among the short-term group mem- bers-those with no solid basis to expect another encounter-FtF had significantly higher anticipation of future interaction than did either CMC condition.

These results show that communication media affect anticipated future interaction perceptions, and that anticipation is differentially amenable to the effect of a long-term versus short-term assignment depending on the medium of discussion. They establish a necessary first step in clarifying the differences between the relatively impersonal behavior of one-shot CMC groups in past research, compared to the more personal behavior of one-shot or long-term FtF groups and longitudinal CMC. This clarifica- tion depends, however, on the extent to which anticipated future interac- tion affects relational communication, relative to the effect of communi- cation condition, as proposed in the second hypothesis.

Prior to the analyses for hypothesis 2, RQl was addressed by compar- ing the means from communication conditions on each relational commu- nication variable. This test was also used to confirm whether either CMC condition differed from FE Had they not, further analysis comparing the conditions effect to the anticipation effect would be far less informative. This test also had the capability to reveal whether or not both CIVIC conditions differed from each other and from FtF levels. If they did not,

Page 9: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

488 HUMAN COMMUNICA~ON RESEARCH / June 1994

TABLE 1

Condition and Assigned Longevity Effects on Anticipation of Future Interaction

Longevity Assignment

Conznzunicfation condition Ongoing Z-dWt

Anticipation mean Asynchronous CMC 2.40a (.69) 2.16a (99)

Synchronous CMC 3.03b* (.59) 2.1ga* (.82)

Face-to-Face 4.02’ ‘( .75) 4.11b (74)

NOTE: Standard deviations in parentheses. Letter superscripts indicate significant c&mm differences. * indicates significant row difference.

then the amount of variance due to the difference between all three conditions might be attenuated by the similarity of two conditions, in subsequent tests, when compared to the variance due to anticipation.

Research question 1 was explored by means of a oneway analysis of variance procedure, and the results were analyzed using a liberal Least Significant Difference (LSD) test at alpha = .05. These tests showed that of the two CMC conditions, only Asynchronous CMC differed from FtF on immediacy/affection, similar$y/depth, receptivity/trust, and composure/relaxation. In each case, CMC was lower than FtF (see table 2). There were no differences for social vs. task-orientation. Asychro- nous CMC also differed from Synchronous CMC on similarity/depth, receptivity/trust, and composure/relaxation. For the next hypotheses, then, only Aspchronous CMC and FtF conditions were compared, in order to provide the most clear and meaningful hypothesis tests. This combination comprises the most conservative test of the anticipation effect, not only because there is clearly more difference between these two conditions than among the three, but also because, as it happened, they are the two with the least variance in anticipated future interaction as a result of the manipulation.

.

Hypotheses 2a through 2d were tested using multiple regression analysis, regressing each dependent variable on the effect of perceived anticipation, condition (effect coded), and their interaction. Although a stepwise, for- ward entry regression procedure would determine which predictors ac- counted for the greatest variance for each relational dimension, this approach was rejected because a theoretical question guided this effort, that is, does condition produce any difference when the effects of antici- pation are partialled out?

Following the “experimental design approach” to regression (Pedhazur, 1982), a full regression model was run with main effects first, plus the interaction effect vector, to see if the interactions accounted for significant incremental variance beyond the main effects. When the interaction term

Walther / ANTICIPATION VS. CHANNEL 489

TABLE 2

Mean Relational Communication Scores by Communication Condition

Asynchronous CMC Synchronous CMC FtF

Immediacy/Affection Similarity/Depth

3.55 (.4qa 3.78 (.51)atb 3.81 (.42)b

Receptivity/Tmst 3.24 ( .24)a 3.54 (34)b

Composure/Relaxation 3.65 ( .40)a 3.97 (.31)b

3.42 (.36)b

Task- vs. Social-Orientation 3.71 (.47)a 4.03 (.37)b

4.03 (.37)b

3.70 (.43)a 4.00 (*35)b

3.67 (.61)a 3.78 ( .49)a NOTE: Standard deviations in parentheses. Letter superscripts, where different, indicate significant pairwise difference, p < .05, two-tailed.

was not significant, it was dropped from consideration and analysis returned to the main effects; interaction effects will not be discussed below, except where significant. For the primary analysis, anticipation was entered into the regression equation first, and the conditions variable, second. This order allows the variance associated with anticipation to be partialled out before the conditions factor entered. Following Pedhazur (1982), each analysis was then repeated reversing the order of entry for main effect predictors, in order to see whether there was indeed any effect of the other variable. In a sense, this replicated the LSD tests using regression, with the added benefit of illustrating the mutual effects of the two predictors, to see if Conditions might compete with Anticipation’s effects.

Immediacy/Affection

H2a was supported. Anticipation was positively associated with immediacy/affection, and it was a better predictor of immediacy than was communication channel. Anticipation of future interaction was signifi- cant, adjusted R2 = .15, F( 1,73) = 14.43, p = .0003 b = .16. The subsequent entry of the Condition effect failed to acheive a significant change in variance accounted for.

When the order of entry was reversed, Conditions was significant as entered on the first step in the regression. Anticipation accounted for a significant increment in variance on the next step. With both factors in the model, the Conditions term became nonsignificant as revealed by the

’ t-tests of all 2~s in the model (see Table 3). .

Similarity/Depth

Hypothesis 2b was also supported. Anticipated future interaction had a positive association with similarity/depth, and Anticipation was the

Page 10: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

4 9 0 H U M A N C O M M U N I C A ~ O N R E S E A R C H / J u n e 1 9 9 4

best predictor, compared to the effects of communication condition. An- ticipation was statistically significant, adjusted R2 = .12, F(l,73) = 11.08, p = .OOl, b = .lO. The Conditions effect did not achieve significance on the next step. Reversing the order of entry, Conditions was significant on step one. Anticipation was significant on step two, and with its inclusion, Conditions became nonsignificant.

Receptivity/Tmst

Hypotheses 2c was confirmed as well. The Anticipation variable was significant, adjusted R2 = . 233, F(l,73) = 23.56, p < .OOOl. Anticipation was

positively related to receptivity/ trust, b - . - 185. The entry of the Conditions

variable was not significant in the primary analysis. Conditions w’as significant when it entered the model first, but with the significant entry of Anticipation on the second step, the Conditions term dropped out.

Composure

Similar results were obtained in confirmation of hypothesis 2d. The anticipation of future interaction variable proved a significant predictor, adjusted R2 = ,164, F(l,73) = 15.54, p = . 0002. Anticipation was positively

associated with composure, b = . 16. The Conditions term did not account

for a significant increment in variance beyond the effects of Anticipation. When the predictors were entered in the opposite order, a similar pattern was obtained with composure as with other outcomes. The Conditions variable was initially significant, but with the significant inclusion of Anticipation, the Conditions term became nonsignificant. H2d was also supported.

Social Orientation

Hypothesis 2e was not confirmed. Anticipation did not enter the model significantly The addition of the between-conditions vector did not yield significance, either. The overall regression equation, including the inter- action term, reflected only multiple R2 = .03, F(3, 71) = .249, p = 86. No effects obtained significance with the variables entered in reverse order.

Synchronous CMC/FtF Replication

A complete reanalysis was conducted using FtF and synchronous conditions. These results must be considered ancillary, because they risk alpha inflation from the second analysis of the FtF data, and some findings that might normally be considered at a more liberal alpha level (especially interaction terms) become especially tenuous. Additionally, the failure of

W a l t h e r / A N T I C P A T I O N V S . C H A N N E L 4 9 1

these conditions to differ using the LSD analysis warrants caution. A summary of the trends revealed in these analyses follows.

For immediacy/affection, the interaction of Anticipation and Condi- tion obtained significance. Separate regression lines were calculated for each condition. These revealed that participants exhibited somewhat opposing responses in immediacy/affection as a result of anticipated future interaction. Synchronous CMC’s intercept was higher than FtF’s; in CMC immediacy/affection declined slightly with greater Anticipation, whereas in FtF communication immediacy/affection increased with greater Anticipation.4 No main effects were significant.

On similarity/depth the Anticipation term was not significant when entered alone, but with the inclusion of the Conditions term, both factors were significant. These conditions are best described as parallel, with the intercept for synchronous CMC greater than FtF but with a common slope ( b = .104) for the Anticipation effect.

Opposing trends were apparent for the receptivity/trust dimension. The interaction term was significant. Asynchronous CMC had a higher intercept, but showed declining receptivity as anticipation increased. FtF communication showed more receptivity as anticipation went up. A similar effect was found in the significant interaction on social- vs. task- orientation, where Asynchronous CMC was more task oriented but be- came less so in the face of Anticipation. FtF showed little change as a result of Anticipation, with a b of .014 in the direction of greater task-orientation. No effects were obtained on composure/relaxation.

DISCUSSION

The present results offer very strong indications that the impersonal effects attributed to CMC in many early, one-shot experiments are not direct effects of the communication media. Rather, CMC acts as a modera- tor, by which participants may come to expect or not to expect ongoing interaction with their partners. This moderation takes place to a greater extent in CMC than it does in FtF communication. In FtF interaction, partners in one-shot encounters expect to recognize and interact with their partners in the future significantly moreso than do partners in Synchro- nous or Asynchronous CMC.

The actual anticipation of future interaction held by participants using asynchronous computer conferencing and FtF meetings significantly ac- counts for relational intimacy and composure, and communication me- dium has little effect once Anticipation is considered. In the cases of immediacy/affection, similarity/depth, receptivity/trust, and composure/ relaxation, communication condition did not account for a significant amount of the variance after the anticipation effect was removed. Only in

Page 11: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

492 I?.IUMAN COMMUNICAmON RESEARCH / June 1994

TABLE 3

Effects of Anticipated Future Interaction, Communication Channel, and Their Interactions on Relational Communication

Ou tame & Predictors R2 F R2 chunge F chunge p

Immediacy /Affection Asynchronous vs. FtF

l.AFI

2. Channel 3. Interaction

1. Channel 2. AFL

Synchronous vs. FtF l.AFI

2. Channel 3. Interaction

Sirnilarity /Depth Asynchronous vs. FtF

l.AFI 2. Channel 3. Interaction

1. Channel 2. AFI

Synchronous vs. FtF 1. AH 2. Channel 3. Interaction

Receptivity/Trust Asynchronous vs. FtF

1. AFT 2. Channel 3. Interaction

1. Channel 2. AFI

Synchronous vs. FtF 1. AFI

2. Channel 3. Interaction

Composure Asynchronous vs. FtF

l* AFL 2. Channel 3. Interaction

1. Channel .12 9.99

.17

.07

l Ol

.36 11.08

.06 4.80

.Ol 0.65

.24 23.56

.19

.Ol

.18 15.52

14.43

5.78

0.94

16.76

1.06

.OO

.Ol

.05 1.11

.09 7.96

.OO .15

.07 6.07

.oo .Ol

.03 2.27

.07 5.82

l 08 6.90

XI0 l 38

.Ol 1.31

.OO .27

.07 6.86

.OO

.05 .02

4.43

.Ol

.OO

.44

.05

.06 5.28

.OO

.82

.30

.02*

.Ol

.34

.70 -02

.oo

.91

.14

.03*

.02

.42**

.01*

.53

.OO

.26

.60

.OO*

.Ol

.31

.89

.04

.oo

.51

.82

.OO*

.02

Walther / ANTICPATION VS. CmNNEL 493

TABLE 3 Continued

Outcome & Predictors R2 F R2 chunge Fchunge p

Composure Synchronous vs. FtF

1. AFI .03 2.24 2. Channel

.14 .Ol .%

3. Interaction .50

.Ol &I4 .51

Task/Social-Orientation Asynchronous vs. FtF

1. AFT .oo 0.08 2. Channel

.77 .Ol .41 .52

3. Interaction .OO .26 .61 1. Channel .Ol 0.43 2. AFI

*51 .oo .06 .80

Synchronous vs. FtF l.AFI .oo 0.35 2. Channel

.56 .03 2.73 JO

3. Interaction .05 4.15 .OG NOTE: Three sets of results are presented for each outcome variable. For Asynchronous CMC/FtF regressions, the primary analysis entered (1) Anticipation of Future Interaction (AF) first, (2) Conditions second, and (3) the interaction term; secondary analyses entered (1) Conditions first and (2) AFI second. For Synchronous CMC/FtF regression, AFI was entered first. * denotes that the predictor became non-significant upon the inclusion of the term on the next step. ** denotes that the predictor achieved significance upon the inclusion of the next term on the next step.

the case of social-orientation did anticipation fail to predict communica- tion, but the condition effect similarly failed to reach significance. This is the same outcome variable that showed greater social orientation in asynchronous conferencing than in FtF communication in previous lon- gitudinal research (Walther & Burgoon, 1992); there may be some other factor operating on this construct than has been examined heretofore.

The present results must be reckoned against theoretical positions regarding CMC. Clearly the restricted bandwidth approaches are severely challenged by these results. The filtering out of cues position dictates a very strong medium effect over and above whatever antecedents and dynamics participants experience. Clearly this was not the case. The present research directly assessed whether the impersonal communica- tion of CMC versus FtF communication seen in one-shot experiments, and CMC’s positive character in longitudinal settings, could be explained through specification of an intervening variable. The anticipation of fu- ture interaction accounted for the specified effects. The social presence and lack of social context cues perspectives are insufficient in this regard.

What then of the rival social information processing perspective? This position, too, has predicted but failed to find differences between CMC

Page 12: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

494 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994

and FtF in initial meetings. It has not previously accounted for the antici- pation moderator. Yet this perspective offers some assumptions in its calculus that may be reconsidered. .

The social information processing perspective holds that the motiva- tion to affiliate is similar among persons communicating FtF or via CMC (Walther, 1992a). It also assumes that the affiliation motive acts as a drive, suggesting a “uniform propulsion” among all interactants to pursue positive relational states through communication, with the medium of that communication subsequently expediting or retarding relational de- velopment. Elsewhere, the assumptions by Berger (1979) and work of those exploring anticipated future interaction show that this “uniform propulsion” assumption may be too simple. The affiliation motive may be. a variable, affected by anticipation. A weakness apparent in the social information processing perspective is that it has not allowed for differ- ences in the affiliation drive; they have implicitly been assumed away as random error. The current work suggests that the effects of anticipated future interaction on CMC groups’ interpersonal behavior may indeed be more systematic and influential than previously expected. The social information processing perspective must be re-examined in this light.

The differences among synchronous and asynchronous CMC, relative to FtF communication, have been the source of conflicting opinions. The results of the research question offer intriguing points for continued speculation about these variations and their effects. Mean comparisons between communication conditions on relational communication most often showed the difference lying between asynchronous CMC against the two synchronous (computer conferencing and FtF) conditions. The “interactivity“ approach seemed to see some initial backing here: Media differed along the lines of participation simultaneity rather than as a result of a partial-cues (CMC) versus full-cues (FtF) distinction. Yet such a conclusion would be premature. The interaction trends among synchro- nous CMC and FtF that showed opposing reactions to Anticipation on immediacy, receptiviv, and task-orientation, suggest that the two syn- chronous conditions differ in other ways yet.

The final results of the research question-which CMC condition is more like FtF-poses a paradox. Synchronous CMC is most like FtF in that there is less mean variation in relational communication between these two conditions, yet some differences between them emerge in their inter- actions with Anticipation. Asynchronous CMC, on the other hand, is least like FtF relational communication, at a gross level, but in their develop- ment and response to temporal variations, the difference between these channels becomes insignificant. It is beyond the scope of the present article to suggest what other factors are at work in these variations. However, researchers are cautioned not to accept swiftly either synchro-

Walther / ANTICIFTA’ITON VS. CHANNEL 495

nous or asynchronous CMC as “the right medium” for testing theoretical claims about the media.

There is *always some concern about generalizing to the world from findings drawn from students. This may be especially so as it was explic- itly recognized that students are very likely to experience serendipitous future interaction. These concerns are somewhat mitigated, however, for two reasons. First, most previous CMC research has also used student participants; there would be less replication value if the anticipation variable was tested on a different subpopulation. Second, and more importantly CMC in the “real world” is most frequently used among those with ample opportunity for other forms of contact. Rockart and DeLong (1988) report that corporate e-mail users often employ group distribution lists for messages to nearby co-workers. Finholt and Sproull (1990) found an average of 19% of e-mail originated within a hundred yards of its destination, with another 13% from elsewhere in the same building in one organization (see Culnan & Markus, 1987; Schaefermeyer & Sewell, 1988; Sherblom, 1988). It seems that nonstudent CMC users may find themselves in circumstances to expect future interaction, too. It is also noted that the present design does not necessarily generalize to interac- tion among people who regularly use many media in their ongoing communication.

At the same time, there is some ecological reality to conditions in which partners expect longitudinal CMC-only associations. Numerous exam- ples of this may be found in the literature, reflecting academic, organiza- tional, and social groups (e.g., B&son & Eveland, 1990; Finholt & Sproull, 1990; Hilts, 1986; Kerr & Hiltz, 1982; Lea & Giordano, 1993).

Implications for the use of asynchronous CMC-the form most com- mon in field applications- are readily apparent. CMC appears to be more interpersonally positive when used by members who at least think that their association will have some longevity The use of CMC for “meet- ings,” or ad hoc, one-shot groups-rather than ongoing “teams” or “task forces” -runs some risk of greater impersonality Further research must explore the influences of media, temporal dynamics, and relational com- munication on the effectiveness and satisfaction of CMC groups under varying arrangements; as we know from the sometimes curvilinear rela- tionship of group cohesion to task activity (see Shaw, 1981), what is most interpersonally satisfying is not always most productive.

Page 13: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

496 HUMAN COMMUNICAmON RESEARCH / June 1994

APPENDIX 1.

Anticipation of future interaction (74nterval semantic differential) scales.

1

2.

3.

4.

5. 6.

7.

8. 9.

To what extent do you anticipate future interaction with your conversational partner in the near future? To what extent do you evecf to meet your conversational partner again whether you want to or not? What is the likelihood you could have a chance meeting with your conver- sational partner somewhere? How likely are you to recognize your conversational partner if you bumped into him/ her somewhere? To what extent do you want to meet your conversational partner again? If you met your conversational partner somewhere else, how likely would that person be to recognize you? What is the likelihood you might recognize your conversational partner at the student union? . l . at the grocery store? . . . in the library?

NOTES

1. Although two reviews and one research report by these theorists have reported positive as well as negative emotional expressions in CMC, and that physiological arousal did not differ in CMC and FtF interaction (S. Kiesler et al., 1985), their theoretical position-the “lack of social context cues” hypothesis -remains basically unchanged (see Sproull & Kiesler, 199la, 199lb).

2. Kellermann and Reynolds (1990) empirically confirmed a direct positive relationship between anticipated future interaction and information seeking. Their findings on the direct relationship between anticipation and liking were inconsistent.

3. As was pointed out by one of the reviewers, there may be some question whether a three-by-two analysis of variance design is appropriate for these data. A three-by-two assumes that the variables are completely crossed, yet one may argue that there is a qualitative difference between “continued CMC interaction” and “continued FtF interac- tion.” One alternative strategy would be to conduct a more fully-crossed, 9 cell design, where participants in each communication condition either anticipated one meeting, more than one meeting using the same medium, or more than one meeting using &Tu~Iz~ media. Such variations might demonstrate a confound in the present design. However, they would move the research out of its present focus and would not address the hypothesis more effectively. Further, given the directions specified in the hypothesis, a confound would only be present if both of the CMC conditions also failed to differ as a result of manipulation, which the synchronous CMC results show not to be the case.

In respect to these concerns, however, another analysis was performed that focused more directly on the effects within each condition, and then compared those effects between conditions. The first step was to compute effect size estimations for long-term vs. short-term assignment on the anticipation of future interaction reported by group members; t-tests were performed within each condition, and the t coefficients were converted to effect size estimates, r (see Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1991). The results were asynchronous CMC I = .144,

Walther / ANTICIPA~ON VS. CHANNEL 497

synchronous CMC r = .52, and FtF T = .066. The manipulation had a moderate to large effect within the synchronous CMC condition (by Cohen’s [1977] standard) with progressively smaller effects within asynchronous CMC and FtF. The next step compared these effects across conditions. Each r was converted to a Fisher z and contrast analyses were performed (Rosenthal, 1991). The effect of the manipulation on anticipated future interaction in syn- chronous CMC was greater than its effect in FtF, binomial 2 = 2.025, p = .022. The manipu- lation effect did not differ between asynchronous CMC and FtF conditions, Z = 34. A post-hoc, nonorthogonal test showed no difference in the effects between the two CMC conditions. These tests provide partial support for the hypothesis. The salience of a long-term versus short-term partnership affected the anticipation of future interaction in at least one form of CMC more than it did in FtF communication.

4. Immediacy/affection in synchronous CMC = (3.97 - .07 x Anticipation); in FtF com- munication, immediacy/affection = (3.52 + .19 x Anticipation). Similarity/depth in synchro- nous CMC = (3.24 + .lO x Anticipation); for FtF, similarity/depth = (3.00 + .10 x Anticipation). For synchronous CMC, receptivity/trust = (4.12 - ,052 x Anticipation), although FtF recep- tivity = (3.56 + .12 x Anticipation). On task-social orientation, synchronous CMC showed (4.35 - .27 x Anticipation); task-orientation in FtF was (3.73 + ,014 x Anticipation).

REFERENCES

Archer, N. P (1990) A comparison of computer conferences with face-to-face meetings for small group business decisions. Behaviour G Information Technology, 9,307-317.

Beals, D. E. (1991, April). Computer-mediated communication among beginning teachers. 7W.E. Journal, pp. 74-77.

Berger, C. R (1979). Beyond initial interaction: Uncertainly, understanding, and the devel- opment of interpersonal relationships. In H. Giles & R. St. Clair (Eds.), bnwge and social psychology (pp. 122-144). Oxford: Blackwell.

Berger, C. R., & Bradac, J. J. (1982). Language and social knowledge: Lhzcertainty in interpersonal relations. London: Arnold.

Berger, C. R, & Calabrese, R. J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and beyond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human Communication Research, I, 99-112.

Berger, C. R., & Douglas, W. (1981). Studies in interpersonal epistemology: III. Anticipated interaction, self-monitoring, and observational context selection. Communication Mono- graphs‘ 48,183196.

Bikson, T K., & Eveland, J. D. (1990). The interplay of work group structures and computer support. In J. Galagher, R. E. Kraut, & C. Egido (Eds.), Intellectual teamwork: Social and technologicalfoundations of cooperative work (pp. 245-290). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1984). The fundamental topoi of relational communication. Communication Monographs, 51,193-214.

Burgoon, J. K., & Hale, J. L. (1987). Validation and measurement of the fundamental themes of relational communication. Communication Monographs, 54,19-41.

Calabrese, R. J. (1975). The effects of privacy and probability of future interaction on initial interaction patterns. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University,

Ciborra, C. C., & Olson, M. H. (1988). Encountering electronic work groups: A transaction costs perspective. In Proceedings of Confmence on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW ‘88), pp. 94-101. New York ACM Press.

Cline, R J., & Musolf, K. E. (1985). Disclosure as social exchange: Anticipated length of rel,ationship, sex roles, and disclosure intimacy. Westem ]ournal of Speech Conmunication, 49,~56.

Page 14: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

498 H U M A N C O M M U N I C A R O N R E S E A R C H / J u n e 1 9 9 4

C o h e n , Jg ( l g m . S t a t i s t i c a l p o w e r a n a l y s i s f o r t h e b e h a v i o r a l s c i e n c e s ( r e v , e d . ) . N e w Y o r k A c a d e m i c P r e s s .

C u l n a n , M . J - , & t M a r k u s , M . L . ( 1 9 8 7 ) . I n f o r m a t i o n t e c h n o l o g i e s . I n E M . J a b l i n , L . L , P u t n a m , K . I I . R o b e r t s , & K L . W . P o r t e r ( E d s . ) , H a n d b o o k o f o r g a n i z a t i o n a l c o m m u n i c a t i o n : A n i n t e r - d i s c i p l i m y p e r s p e c t i v e ( p p . 4 2 0 4 4 3 ) . N e w b u r y P a r k , C A : S a g e .

D a f t , R L L e n g e l , R . H . , & T m v i n o , L . K . ( 1 9 8 7 ) . M e s s a g e e q u i v o c a l i t y , m e d i a s e l e c t i o n , a n d m a n a g e r p e r f o r m a n c e : I m p l i c a t i o n s f o r i n f o r m a t i o n s y s t e m s . M I S Q u a r t e r & , 2 2 , 3 5 5 3 6 8 .

D o u g l a s , W ( 1 9 8 7 ) . Q u e s t i o n - a s k i n g i n s a m e - a n d o p p o s i t e - s e x i n i t i a l i n t e r a c t i o n s : T h e e f f e c t s o f a n t i c i p a t e d f u t u r e i n t e r a c t i o n . H u m a n C o m m u n i c a t i o n R e s e a r c h , 2 4 , 2 3 0 - 2 4 6 .

D u b r o v s k y , V J . , K i e s l e r , S , & S e t h n a , B . N . ( 1 9 9 1 ) . T h e e q u a l i z a t i o n p h e n o m e n o n : S t a t u s e f f e c t s i n c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d a n d f a c e - t o - f a c e d e c i s i o n - m a k i n g g r o u p s . H u m a n C o m p u t e r I n t e r a c t i o n , 6 , 1 1 9 - 1 4 6 .

F e l d m a n , M . S . ( 1 9 8 7 ) . E l e c t r o n i c m a i l a n d w e a k t i e s i n o r g a n i z a t i o n s . O f i c e ; T e c h n o l o g y a n d P e o p l e , 3 , 8 3 - 1 0 1 .

F e r r a r a , K , B r u n n e r , H . , & W h i t t e m o r e , G . ( 1 9 9 1 ) . I n t e r a c t i v e w r i t t e n d i s c o u r s e a s a n e m e r g e n t r e g i s t e r . W t i t t m C o m m u n i c a t i o n , 8 , 8 - 3 4 .

F i n h o l t , T . , & S p r o u l l , L . ( 1 9 9 0 ) . E l e c t r o n i c g r o u p s a t w o r k . O r g a n i z a t i o n S c i e n c e , 2 , 4 1 - 6 4 . F o u l g e r , D . A . ( 1 9 9 0 ) . M e d i u m a s p r o c e s s : 7 h e s t r u c t u r e , u s e , a n d p r a c t i c e o f c o m p u t e r c o n f m e n c i n g

o n I B M ’ s I B M P C c o m p u t e r c o n f m e n c i n g f a c i l i t y . U n p u b l i s h e d d o c t o r a l d i s s e r t a t i o n , T e m p l e U n i v e r s i p

F u l k , J . , S c h m i t z , J . A . , & S c h w a r z , D . ( 1 9 9 2 ) . T h e d y n a m i c s o f c o n t e x t - b e h a v i o r i n t e r a c t i o n s i n c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n . I n M . L e a ( E d . ) , C o n t e x t s o f c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n ( p p . 7 - 2 9 ) . L o n d o n : H a r v e s t e r W h e a t s h e a f .

F u l k , J . , S c h m i t z , J . , & S t e i n f i e l d , C . W . ( 1 9 9 0 ) . A s o c i a l i n f l u e n c e m o d e l o f t e c h n o l o g y u s e . I n J . F u l k & C . S t e i n f i e l d ( E d s . ) , O r g a n i z a t i o n s a n d c o m m u n i c a t i o n t e c h n o l o g y ( p p . 1 1 7 - 1 4 0 ) . N e w b u r y P a r k , C A : S a g e .

F u l k , J . , S t e i n f i e l d , C . W . , S c h m i t z , J . , & P o w e r , J . G . ( 1 9 8 7 ) . A s o c i a l i n f o r m a t i o n p r o c e s s i n g m o d e l o f m e d i a u s e i n o r g a n i z a t i o n s . C o m m u n i c a t i o n R e s e a r c h , 2 4 , 5 2 9 - 5 5 2 .

H e i d e , J . B . , & M i n e r , A . S . ( 1 9 9 2 ) . T h e s h a d o w o f t h e f u t u r e : E f f e c t s o f a n t i c i p a t e d f u t u r e i n t e r a c t i o n a n d f r e q u e n c y o f c o n t a c t o n b u y e r - s e l l e r c o o p e r a t i o n . A c a d e m y o f M a n a g e m e n t J o u r n a l , 3 5 , 2 6 5 2 9 1 .

H i e m s t r a , G . ( 1 9 8 2 ) . T e l e c o n f e r e n c i n g , c o n c e r n f o r f a c e , a n d o r g a n i z a t i o n a l c u l t u r e . I n M . B u r g o o n ( E d . ) C o m m u n i c a t i o n Y e a r b o o k 6 ( p p . 8 7 4 - 9 0 4 ) . B e v e r l y H i l l s , C A S a g e .

H i l t z , S . R . ( 1 9 7 5 ) . C o m m u n i c a t i o n s a n d g r o u p d e c i s i o n - m a k i n g : E x p e r j m e n t a l e v i d e n c e o n t h e p o t e n t i a l i m p a c t o f c o m p u t e r c o n f m e n c i n g ( R e s e a r c h R e p . N o . 2 ) . N e w a r k , N J : N e w J e r s e y I n s t i t u t e o f T e c h n o l o g y , C o m p u t e r i z e d C o n f e r e n c i n g a n d C o m m u n i c a t i o n s C e n t e r .

H i l t z , S . R . ( 1 9 8 6 ) . T h e “ v i r t u a l c l a s s r o o m ” : U s i n g c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n f o r u n i v e r s i t y t e a c h i n g . J o u r n a l o f C o m m u n i c a t i o n , 3 6 , 9 5 - 1 0 4 .

H i l t z , S . R . , J o h n s o n , K , & A g l e , G . ( 1 9 7 8 ) . R e p l i c a t i n g B a l e s ’ p r o b l e m s o l v i n g e x p e r i m e n t s o n a c o m p u t e r i z e d c o n f e r e n c e : A p i l o t s t u d y ( R e s e a r c h R e p . N o . 8 ) . N e w a r k , N J : N e w J e r s e y I n s t i t u t e o f T e c h n o l o g , C o m p u t e r i z e d C o n f e r e n c i n g a n d C o m m u n i c a t i o n s C e n t e r .

H i l t z , S . R . , J o h n s o n , K . , & T m o f f , M . ( 1 9 8 6 ) . E x p e r i m e n t s i n g r o u p d e c i s i o n m a k i n g : C o m m u n i c a t i o n p r o c e s s a n d o u t c o m e i n f a c e - t o - f a c e v e r s u s c o m p u t e r i z e d c o n f e r e n c e s . H u m a n C o m m u n i c a t i o n R e s e a r c h , 1 3 , 2 2 5 - 2 5 2 .

H i & z , S R . , T u o f f , M . , & J o h n s o n , K . ( 1 9 8 9 ) . E x p e r i m e n t s i n g r o u p d e c i s i o n m a k i n g , 3 : D i s i n h i b i t i o n , d e i n d i v i d u a t i o n , a n d g r o u p p r o c e s s i n p e n n a m e a n d r e a l n a m e c o m p u t e r c o n f e r e n c e s . D e c i s i o n S u p p o r t S y s t e m s , 5 , 2 1 7 2 3 2 .

J o h a n s e n , R , D e G r a s s e , R , & W t i n , T . ( 1 9 7 8 ) . G r o u p c o m m u n i c a t i o n t h r o u g h c o m p u t e r s : V o l . 5 . E j t e c t s o n w o r k i n g p a t t e r n s . M e n l o P a r k , C A : I n s t i t u t e f o r t h e F u t u r e :

K e l l e r m a n n , K , ( 1 9 8 6 ) . A n t i c i p a t i o n o f f u t u r e i n t e r a c t i o n a n d i n f o r m a t i o n e x c h a n g e i n i n i t i a l i n t e r a c t i o n . H u m a n C o m m u n i c a t i o n R e s e a r c h , 2 3 , 4 1 - 7 5 .

W a l t h e r / A N T I C r A T I O N V S . C H A N N E L 4 9 9

K e l l e r m a n n , K . ; & R e y n o l d s , R . ( 1 9 9 0 ) . W h e n i g n o r a n c e i s b l i s s : T h e r o l e o f m o t i v a t i o n t o r e d u c e u n c e r t a i n t y i n u n c e r t a i n t y r e d u c t i o n t h e o r y . H u m a n C o m m u n i c a t i o n R e s e a r c h , 1 7 , 5 - 7 5 .

K e r r , E . B . , & H i l t z , S . R . ( 1 9 8 2 ) . C o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n s y s t e m s : S t a t u s a n d e v a l u - a t i o n . N e w Y o r k A c a d e m i c P r e s s .

K i e s l e r , C . A . , K i e s l e r , S . B . , & P a l l a k , M . S . ( 1 9 6 7 ) . T h e e f f e c t o f c o m m i t m e n t t o f u t u r e i n t e r a c t i o n o n r e a c t i o n s t o n o r m v i o l a t i o n s . j o u r n a l o f P e r s o n a l i t y , 3 5 , 5 $ 5 - 5 9 9 .

K i e s l e r , S . , S i e g e l , J . & M c G u i r e , T . W . ( 1 9 8 4 ) . S o c i a l p s y c h o l o g i c a l a s p e c t s o f c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n . A m e r i c a n P s y c h o & S t , 3 9 , 1 1 2 3 - 1 1 3 4 .

K i e s l e r , S . , Z u b r o w , D . , M o s e s , A . M . , & G e l l e r , V ( 1 9 8 5 ) . A f f e c t i n c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n : A n e x p e r i m e n t i n s y n c h r o n o u s t e r m i n a l - t o - t e r m i n a l d i s c u s s i o n . H u m a n C o m p u t e r I n t e r a c t i o n , I , 7 7 - 1 0 4 .

K o m s k y S . H . ( 1 9 9 1 ) . A p r o f i l e o f u s e r s o f e l e c t r o n i c m a i l i n a u n i v e r s i t y . M a n a g e m e n t C o m m u n i c a t i o n Q u a r t e r l y , 4 , 3 1 0 - 3 4 0 .

K o r z e n n y E ( 1 9 7 8 ) . A t h e o r y o f e l e c t r o n i c p r o p i n q u i t y : M e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n s i n o r g a n i - z a t i o n s . C o m m u n i c a t i o n R e s e a r c h , 5 , 3 - 2 4 .

K r a u t , R . , E g i d o , C . , & G a l e g h e r , J . ( 1 9 8 8 ) . P a t t e r n s o f c o n t a c t a n d c o m m u n i c a t i o n i n s c i e n t i f i c r e s e a r c h c o l l a b o r a t i o n . I n P r o c e e d i n g s o f t h e C o n f m e n c e o n C o m p u t e r - S u p p o r t e d C o o p e r a t i v e W o r k ( C S C W ‘ 8 8 ) ( p p . 1 - 1 2 ) . N e w Y o r k : A C M P r e s s .

L e a , M , ( 1 9 9 1 ) . R a t i o n a l i s t a s s u m p t i o n s i n c r o s s - m e d i a c o m p a r i s o n s o f c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n . B e h a v i o u r & I n f o r m a t i o n T e c h n o l o g y , 2 0 , 1 5 3 - 1 7 2 .

L e a , M . , & G i o r d a n o , R . ( 1 9 9 3 , M a r c h ) . F r a m e w o r k s f o r m o d e l l i n g t h e r e a l w o r l d : O b j e c t , o r i e n t a t i o n , b a n d w i d t h a n d r e p r e s e n t a t i o n s o f t h e g r o u p . P a p e r p r e s e n t e d a t t h e I n t e m a - t i o n a l W o r k s h o p o n S o c i a l S c i e n c e , T e c h n i c a l S y s t e m s a n d C o o p e r a t i v e W o r k , P a r i s .

L e a , M . , O ’ S h e a , T . , F u n g , I ? , & S p e a r s , R . ( 1 9 9 2 ) . “ F l a m i n g ” i n c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u - n i c a t i o n : O b s e r v a t i o n s , e x p l a n a t i o n s , i m p l i c a t i o n . I n M . L e a ( E d . ) , C o n t e x t s o f c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n ( p p . 8 9 - 1 1 2 ) . L o n d o n : H a r v e s t e r - W h e a t s h e a f .

M a r k u s , M , L . ( 1 9 8 7 ) . T o w a r d a “ c r i t i c a l m a s s ” t h e o r y o f i n t e r a c t i v e m e d i a : U n i v e r s a l a c c e s s , i n t e r d e p e n d e n c e a n d d i f f u s i o n . C o m m u n i c a t i o n R e s e a r c h , 2 4 , 4 9 1 - 5 1 1 .

M a r k u s , M . L . ( i n p r e s s ) . E l e c t r o n i c m a i l a s t h e m e d i u m o f m a n a g e r i a l c h o i c e . O r g a n i z a t i o n S c i & n c e .

I

M a t h e s o n , K . , & Z a n n a , M . P . ( 1 9 9 0 ) . C o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n s : T h e f o c u s i s o n m e . S o & Z S c & z c e C o m p u t e r R e v i e w ( S p e c i a l I s s u e : C o m p u t i n g : S o c i a l a n d p o l i c y i s s u e s ) , 8,1-12.

M c G r a t h , J . E . ( 1 9 9 0 ) . T i m e m a t t e r s i n g r o u p s . I n J . G a l e g h e r , R . E . K r a u t , & C . E g i d o ( E d s . ) , I n t e l l e c t u a l t e a m w o r k : S o c i a l a n d t e c h n i c a l f o u n d a t i o n s o f c o o p e r a t i v e w o r k ( p p . 2 3 - 6 1 ) . H i l l s d a l e , N J : L a w r e n c e E r l b a u m .

M c G r a t h , J . E . ( 1 9 9 1 ) . T i m e , i n t e r a c t i o n , a n d p e r f o r m a n c e ( T I P ) : A t h e o r y o f g r o u p s . S m a l l G r o u p R e s e a r c h , 2 2 , 1 4 7 - 1 7 4 .

M c G u i r e , T . W . , K i e s l e r , S . , & S i e g e l , J . ( 1 9 8 7 ) . G r o u p a n d c o m p u t e r - m e d i a t e d d i s c u s s i o n e f f e c t s i n r i s k d e c i s i o n m a k i n g . J o u r n a l o f P e r s o n a l i & a n d S o c i a l P s y c h o l o g y , 5 2 , 9 1 7 , 9 3 0 .

N a s s , C . , & M a s o n , L . ( 1 9 9 0 ) . O n t h e s t u d y o f t e c h n o l o g y a n d t a s k : A v a r i a b l e - b a s e d a p p r o a c h . In J . F u l k & C . S t e i n f i e l d ( E d s . ) , O r g a n i z a t i o n s a n d c o m m u n i c a t i o n t e c h n o l o g y ( p p . 4 6 - 6 7 ) . N e w b u r y P a r k , C A S a g e .

O r d . J . G . ( 1 9 8 9 ) . W h o ’ s j o k i n g ? T h e i n f o r m a t i o n s y s t e m a t p l a y I n t e r a c t i n g w i t h C o m p u t e r s , 2,118-128.

P e d h a z u r , E . J . ( 1 9 8 2 ) . M u l t i p l e r e g r e s s i o n i n b e h a v i o r a l r e s e a r c h : E x p l a n a t i o n a n d p r e d i c t i o n ( 2 n d e d . ) . N e w Y o r k C B S C o l l e g e .

R a f a e l i , S . ( 1 9 8 8 ) . I n t e r a c t i v i t y : F r o m n e w m e d i a t o c o m m u n i c a t i o n . I n R I ? . H a w k i n s , J . M e W i e m a n n , & S P i n g r e e ( E d s . ) , A d v a n c i n g c o m m u n i c a t i o n s c i e n c e : M e r g i n g m a s s a n d i n t e r - p e r s o n a l p n x e s s e s ( p p . 1 1 0 - 1 3 4 ) . N e w b u r y P a r k , C A : S a g e .

Page 15: Anticipated Ongoing Interaction Versus Channel Effects on ... · 476 HUMAN COMMUNICATION RESEARCH / June 1994 tations about the effectiveness of using a channel have been shown to

500 HUMAN COMMUNICA~ON RESEARCH / June 1994

Rheingold, He (1993). 7’he virtual community: Homesteading on the electronic frontier, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley

Rice, R. E. (1984). Mediated group communication. In R. E. Rice & Associates (Eds.), T/ze new media: Communication, research, and technology (pp. 129-156). Beverly Hills, CA Sage.

Rice, R. E. (1986) Computer conferencing. In B. Dervin & M. J. Voigt (Eds.), Regress in communication sciences (Vol. 7, pp. 215-240). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Rice, R. E* (1993). Media appropriateness: Using social presence theory to compare tradi- tional and new organizational media. IWnan Communication Research, 29,45&484.

Rice, R. E., & Aydin, C. (1991). Attitudes toward new organizational technology: Network proximity as a mechanism for social information processing. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36,219-244.

Rice, R. E., Chang, S., & Torobin, J. (1992). Communicator style, media use, organizational level, and use and evaluation of electronic messaging. Mamgement Communication Quar- terly, 6,333.

Rice, R E., Grant, A. E,, Schmitt, J. & Torobin, J. (1990). Individual and network influences on the adoption and perceived outcomes of electronic messaging. So&Z Networks, 22, 27-55.

Rice, R. E., & Love, G. (1987). Electronic emotion: Socioemotional content in a computer- mediated network. Communication Research, 14,85-108.

Rice, R, & Shook, D. (1988). Access to, usage of, and outcomes from an electronic messaging system. ACM Transactions on Ofice Information Systems, 6,255276.

Rockart, J. F., & DeLong, D. W. (1988). Executive support systems: The emergence of top management computer use. Homewood, IL: Dow Jones-Irwin.

Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (rev ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R L, (1991). Essentials of behaviora research; Methods and data analysis (2nd ed.). New York McGrawHill.

Russ, G. S., Daft, R L., & Lengel, R. H. (1990). Media selection and managerial characteristics in organizational communications. Management Communication Quarterly, 4,151-175.

Schaefermeyer, M. J., & Sewell, E. H. (1988). Communicating by electronic mail. American Behavioral Scientist, 32,112-123.

Schmitz, J., & Fulk, J. (1991). Organizational colleagues, media richness, and electronic mail: A test of the social influence model. Communication Research, 28,487-523.

Shaw, M. E. (1981). Group dynamics: 7’he psychology of smaZZgroup behavior (3rcl ed.). New York McGraw-Hill.

Sherblom, J. (1988). Direction, function, and signature in electronic mail. T/ze JoumZ of Business Communication, 25,‘39-54.

Short, J., Wm, E., & Christie, B. (1976). 7%e so& psychology of telecommunications. London: Wfiey

Siegel, J., Dubrovsky, V., Kiesler, S., & McGuire, T. W. (1986). Group processes in computer- mediated communication. Organizational behavior and human decision pnxesses, 37,157-187.

Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social context cues: Electronic mail in organizational communication. Management Science, 32,1492-1512.

Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (199la, September). Computers, networks, and work. Scientz@ American, pp. 84-91.

Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (199lb). Connections: New ways of working in the networked organization. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.

Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1992, February 3) Don’t forget Dr, Frankenstein. Computerworld, p. 25. Steinfield, C. W. (1986). Computer-mediated communication in an organizational setting:

Explaining task-related and so&emotional uses. In M. L. McLaughlin (Ed.), Communi- cation yearbook 9, (pp. 777.804). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Walther / ANTICIPAmON VS. CHANNEL 501

Steinfield, C. (1992). Computer-mediated communications in organizational settings: Emerg- ing conceptual frameworks and directions for research. Management Communication Quarterly, 5,348-365.

Tmvino, L. IS., Lengel, R. H., Bodensteiner, W., Gerloff, E. A., & Muir, N. K. (1990). The richness imperative and cognitive style: The role of differences in media choice behavior. A&z?zqe- ment Communication Quarterly, 4,176197.

Trevino, L. K., Lengel, R H., & Daft, R. L. (1987). Media symbolism, media richness, and . media choice in organizations: A symbolic interaction& perspective. Communication

Research, 24,553574. Walther, J. B. (1992a). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A relational

perspective. Communication Research, 19,52-90. Walther, J. B. (1992b). A longitudinal experiment on relational tone in computer-mediated

and face to face interaction. In J. F. Nunamaker & R. H. Sprague (Eds.), Pnxeedings of the HawaiihW-national Confmence on System Sciences 1992. (Vol. 4, pp. 220-231). Los Alamitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society.

Walther, J. B. (1993). Impression development in computer-mediated interaction. We&m Journal of Communication, 57,381-398.

Walther, J, B. (in press). Relational aspects of computer-mediated communication: Experi- mental and longitudinal observations. Organization Science.

Walther, J. B., Anderson, J. F., & Park, D. (in press). Interpersonal effects in computer- mediated interaction: A meta-analysis of social and anti-social communication. Commu- nication Research.

Walther, J. B., & Burgeon, J. IS, (1992). Relational communication in computer-mediated interaction. Human Communication Research, 29,50-88.

Weedman, J. (1991). Task and non-task functions of a computer conference used in profes- sional education: A measure of flexibility h&rnational Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 34, 303-318.

Yates, J., & Orlikowski, W. J. (1992). Genres of organizational communication: An approach to studying communication and media. Academy of Management Review, 17,2!%326.