a study of the relationship between defects of

78
Central Washington University Central Washington University ScholarWorks@CWU ScholarWorks@CWU All Master's Theses Master's Theses 1957 A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of articulation in A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of articulation in Speech and Emotional Stability of children in the Primary Grades Speech and Emotional Stability of children in the Primary Grades Frances P. Oechsner Central Washington University Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.cwu.edu/etd Part of the Educational Methods Commons, and the Social and Philosophical Foundations of Education Commons Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Oechsner, Frances P., "A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of articulation in Speech and Emotional Stability of children in the Primary Grades" (1957). All Master's Theses. 176. https://digitalcommons.cwu.edu/etd/176 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Master's Theses at ScholarWorks@CWU. It has been accepted for inclusion in All Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@CWU. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Upload: others

Post on 14-Jan-2022

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

Central Washington University Central Washington University

ScholarWorks@CWU ScholarWorks@CWU

All Master's Theses Master's Theses

1957

A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of articulation in A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of articulation in

Speech and Emotional Stability of children in the Primary Grades Speech and Emotional Stability of children in the Primary Grades

Frances P. Oechsner Central Washington University

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.cwu.edu/etd

Part of the Educational Methods Commons, and the Social and Philosophical Foundations of

Education Commons

Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Oechsner, Frances P., "A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of articulation in Speech and Emotional Stability of children in the Primary Grades" (1957). All Master's Theses. 176. https://digitalcommons.cwu.edu/etd/176

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Master's Theses at ScholarWorks@CWU. It has been accepted for inclusion in All Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@CWU. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Page 2: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

A STUDY OF THE RELATIONSHIP

BETWEEN DEFECTS OF ARTICULATION IN SPEECH AND

EMOTIONAL STABILITY OF CHILDREN IN THE PRIMARY GRADES

A Thesis

Presented to

the Graduate Faculty

Central Washington College of Education

In Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements for the Degree

Master of Education

by

Frances P. Oechsner

August 1957

Page 3: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

LD 5771.3

q:»CIA.L -:.o\..u.c;noN ~

88457

Page 4: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

APPROVED FOR THE GRADUATE FACULTY _________________________________ Milo L. Smith, COMMITTEE CHAIRMAN _________________________________ Keith Rinehart _________________________________ Eldon E. Jacobsen

Page 5: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

To the following people the writer wishes to express

appreciation: Mr. Milo L. Smith, Assistant Professor of

Speech, for valuable assistance and criticism, and under

whose direction this study was completed; Dr. Keith

Rinehart, Assistant Professor of English, for helpful

suggestions and advice; and especially for the contri­

butions of Dr. E. E. Jacobsen, Associate Professor of

Psychology.

This thesis is dedicated to F. and J., who, on the

basis of random sampling, are highly improbable.

Page 6: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

TABLE OF CONTENTS

CHAPTER

I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . II. CRITICAL REVIEW OF PREVIOUS INVESTIGATIONS •

Emotional Instability as a Causal Factor

in Articulatory Defects. . . . . . . Reviews of Studies on the Relationship

Between Articulatory Defects and

. .

PAGE

1

3

4

Emotional Instability. • • • • • • • 9

III.

Summary and Discussion • • • • . . . . . . STANDARDS FOR EVALUATION • • •

The Standardized Personality Test •••

The Speech Attitude Scale. • • ••

The Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . The speech defective group • . . . . . . The controls . . . . . . .

Administration of Tests. . . . . . . . Limitations •• . . . . . . . . .

IV. EVALUATIONS AND RESULTS. . . . . . .

16

19

19

22

25

25

28

36

38

40

The California Test of Personality • • 41

. . . . . . Personal adjustment.

Social adjustment ••• . . . . . . . Total adjustment • . . . . . . . . . . .

The Speech Attitude Scale •• • • • 0 •

44

44

45

46

Page 7: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

CHAPTER

V. SUMMARY . . . . . . . . . . . • • . . . . . BIBLIOGRAPHY ••• . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX A: Speech Attitude Scale •• . . . . . . . . APPENDIX B: Sample Data Used in Computing a

v

PAGE

55

58

63

Stability Coefficient for the Speech

Attitude Scale • • • . • . • • • • • • 65

APPENDIX C: Matching Criteria for Parallel-Group

Technique. • • • • • • • • . • • . . . APPENDIX D: Sample Data for Computing a t-Test of

Significance Between Matched Pairs of

Children with Articulation Disorders

and Speech Normal Children on the

Speech Attitude Scale ••••••• • •

APPENDIX E: Comparative Raw Scores of Children with

Articulation Disorders and Speech

Normal Children for California Test

of Personality--Primary Series (AA)--

All Sections • • . . . . . . . APPENDIX F: Comparative Scores of Children with

Articulation Disorders and Speech

Normal Children for the Speech

. .

Attitude Scale •••.•..• . . . .

66

68

69

70

Page 8: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

LIST OF TABLES

TABLE PAGE

1. A Comparison of the Grade Levels of the Speech

Defective Group and the Control Group . . . 31

2. A Comparison of the Speech Defective Group and

the Control Group According to Sex and Grade 32

3. A Comparison of Available Intelligence 'qi,uotient

Scores on the Speech Defective Group and

Control Group . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4. A Comparison of Classroom Teachers 1 and

~riter's Estimates of the Severity of the

33

Speech Defect • • . • • • • . . • . 34

5. A Comparison of Mean Scores on California Test

of Personality (Primary Series) for Children

with Articulation Disorders and Speech

Normal Children • • • . • • • •

6. A Comparison of Mean .Scores on Speech Attitude

Scale for Children with Articulation Disorders

43

and Speech Normal Children. • . • • • • 48

7. A Comparison of the Two Groups on the Number

of "Poor Speech Attitudesn Answers on the

Speech Attitude Scale • . . • • • . • • • • 50

Page 9: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

This study is concerned with the relationship

between defects of articulation in speech and emotional

instability in elementary school children.

The question of the extent to which these two

factors are associated is of considerable interest to

those working in the field of speech rehabilitation for

two reasons: First, many writers have mentioned emotional

instability as a frequent cause of articulatory defects,

yet little has been reported in the way of objective data

to substantiate such assertions. Secondly, if emotional

instability is commonly found in speech defective elemen­

tary school children, it may well be that such emotional

instability is the result of, if not the cause of, the

speech defect. In either case, a clearer understanding

of the emotional status of children with articulatory

defects is desirable and it is hoped this study may make

some contribution to this end.

The general procedure followed throughout the study

involved a comparison of children having articulatory

defects with speech normal children for personal and

social adjustment. The parallel-group technique was

Page 10: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

2

used, having the speech defect as a variable, with personal

and social adjustment as the factors to be measured. In

addition, an attempt was made to evaluate the attitudes

toward speech in both the experimental and control groups.

The specific questions to which answers were sought

may be stated as follows:

1. Is there a marked difference in emotional stability in a group of children having articulatory defects as compared with a control group of the same general intelligence, sex, and other ancillary factors?

2. Is there a marked difference in attitude toward speech in a group of speech defective children compared with a control group?

Page 11: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

CHAPTER II

CRITICAL REVIEW OF PREVIOUS INVESTIGATIONS

Before proceeding to a discussion of the related

historical data and the literature involved, it will be

necessary to define articulatory defects and to explore

the incidence of such defects. Articulatory disorders

consist primarily of abnormal substitution, distortion,

insertion, or omission of speech sounds. Vivian I. Roe,

in studying the effect of maturation upon defective arti-

culation in the elementary grades, found sound substitu-

tions to be the most frequently observed error among

articulatory speech defectives. 1 Articulation cases

present a wide variety of symptoms and may range in

severity from an intermittent lisp to a multitude of

defective consonants. Where there is no demonstrable

structural or constitutional deficiency, the defect is

said to be functional. James F. Bender and Victor M.

Kleinfield found that nearly 90 per cent of the speech

handicaps encountered in the educational system were of

1vi vi an I. Roe, r'The Effect of Maturation Upon Defective Articulation in the Elementary Grades, 11

(unpublished Master's thesis, The University of Indiana, Bloomington, 1940), p. 46.

Page 12: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

the bad habit or functional type. 2

Speech therapists who have worked with children in

elementary schools agree that articulatory defects are

the most prevalent type of speech defect. The rehabili-

tation program requires considerable effort, and also

absorbs a large share of the funds being appropriated

for special education. This opinion is substantiated by

the report of the White House Conference on Child Health

and Protection of 1930, which estimated that articulatory

defects comprise approximately 70 per cent of all speech

defects of elementary school children.3

A review of the literature pertinent to this study

involves (a) a discussion of the opinions of various

writers as to the role of emotional instability as a

causal factor in articulatory defects and (b) a review

of other studies concerned with the relationship between

articulatory defects and emotional instability.

A. Emotional Instability as a Causal Factor in

Articulatory Defects

4

2James F. Bender and Victor M. Kleinfield, Principles and Practices of Speech Correction (New York: Pitman Publishing Company, 1938), p. 233.

3White House Conference on Child Health and Pro­tection, Special Education, Report of Committee on Special Classes (New York: D. Appleton-Century Company, 1932), pp. 107-109.

Page 13: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

5

The commonly mentioned causes of articulatory defects,

according to Charles Van Riper, a representative authority

in the field of speech correction, may be discussed in.

terms of organic abnormalities, motor incoordinations,

developmental retardation, perceptual deficiencies, and

emotional conflicts. 4

As illustrated by the quotations that follow, the

term "emotional conflicts 11 is identified in literature by

several terms: emotional instability, personality

inadequacy, social maladjustment, or "chance conditioning";

but for the purposes of this paper the popular term,

"emotional instability, 11 will be used.

The general acceptance of emotional instability as

a possible major syndrome in articulatory defects is

evident in the following excerpts from recognized author-

ities in the field of speech rehabilitation.

Ollie L. Backus, using the term 11 chance conditioning,"

discusses emotional instability as a cause of articulatory

disorders:

It may well be a 'catch-all' for cases whose cause we do not know or do not take the trouble to find. However, there are at the present time, at least, many so-called minor articulatory defects which can be explained only by the term 'chance conditioning.' Why,

4Charles Van Riper, Speech Correction, Principles and Methods (New York: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1947), p:-127.

Page 14: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

for instance, should a child whose speech is otherwise normal and whose anatomical, psychological, and neurological mechanism is apparently sound, have a lateral lisp on ~ and ( ? It seems necessary to predicate the factor of 'CKance cond~tioning as one the causes of articulatory defects.

Van Riper states that

6

Emotional conflicts may • • • serve as predisposing, precipitating, and maintaining causes of speech dis­orders. The literature is thronged with case studies showing the influence of personality and behavior problems in producing speech disorders •••• Some of our most difficult articulation cases are those in which the child has failed to acquire6 adult pronunciation because of emotional conflicts.

Mildred F. Berry and Jon Eisenson, using the term

Eersonality, emphasize the same general concept:

The role which the development of personality plays in speech is well known. Speech is so intimately connected with our personalities that any major devia­tion from the norm in pers~nal adjustment is certain to be reflected in speech.

Bender and Kleinfield recognize the influence of

emotional instability on speech, for they consistently

emphasize that speech correction should include a consider-

ation of such matters as mental hygiene and personality

50llie L. Backus, Speech Classroom Teacher (New York: pany, 1953), pp. 136-137.

Education, A Guide for the Longmans, Green, and Com:-

6v R . . t 31 d 13 -an iper, 912.· ci_., pp. an j.

7Mildred F. Berry and Jon Eisenson, The Defective in Speech (New York: .B'. S. CroJ_ ts and Company, 1955), p. 75.

Page 15: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

7

development. "Speech re-education includes self-analytical

treatment to find immediately underlying mental causes of

personality maladjustment and speech failure. 118 The authors,

furthermore, state that five to seven per cent of students

in the public schools are neurotic and that this percentage

is often exceeded in a group of speech handicapped

children.

A neurotic person is one who lacks emotional stability, is too easily aroused, whose behavior is controlled with difficulty; for example, compensation tendencies, exaggerated egotism, introversion, ambi­version, extroversion, worry, anxiety, vexati§n, negativism, and mental conflicts are evident.

Robert West, Lou Kennedy and Anna Carr feel there

are many speech disorders, the explanation for which lies

in the realm of abnormal psychology, psychopathology, or

psychiatry. These disorders, which may be vocal, articu-

latory, or linguistic, are almost always rooted in child-

hood experiences and attitudes. Anything that causes

the child to feel insecure in his social environment might

establish emotional habits that could easily persist into

adulthood. Some of the most common conditions are physical

inferiority, peculiarities of appearance, peculiarities

of dress and apparel, peculiar habits, mannerisms and

8Bender and Kleinfield, 212.• cit., p. 88.

9Ibid., p. 212.

Page 16: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

8

afflictions, inferior social standing, unusual home disci-

pline, and inferiority feelings. They believe the unique

nature of speech defects, both in special type and parti­

cular incidence, demands the study of the individua1. 10

Therefore, the speech defective may need as much attention

as the speech defect.

Backus believes speech is a gauge -- it is a test

of the psychic adjustment of the individual to the condi-

tions under which he must live. She stresses the fact

that

••• speech disorders and social maladjustment may have a common cause • . • social maladjustments may cause defective speech. Certain patterns of speech result rather habitually from social maladjustment. Yet, no causal relationship may exist between speech defects and social maladjstment. It is quite possible the speech defect and the personality problem seem to be present in an individ~~l and yet have no causal relationship whatsoever.

To the above few excerpts could be added many more,

for it is generally conceived that a child learns speech

as a part of the whole process of organizing his behavior

and learning to adjust to his environment. It is obvious,

lORobert West, Lou Kennedy, and .Anna Carr, Rehabilita­tion of Speech, ~ Textbook of Diagnostic and Corrective Procedures (New York: Harper and Brothers; Revised Edition, 1947), pp. 38-52.

11Backus, Q£· cit., pp. 115-119.

Page 17: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

9

then, that a study of the various speech defects and

disorders in children will perforce carry one into the

field of personality study and behavior problems.

Logical and reasonable as these opinions of author-

ities seem to be in suggesting emotional instability as a

causal factor in articulatory defects, the next step

becomes one of determining whether this causal factor is

supported by any experimental evidence. To further

establish a background, the discussion will continue to

research which has been completed, to determine whether

the child with articulatory disorders differs significantly

in emotional stability from the normal child.

B. Reviews of Studies on the Relationship Between

Articulatory Defects and Emotional Instability

Although a number of studies have been made on

certain aspects of the relationship between articulatory

defects and emotional instability, Berry and Eisenson in

1955 suggested the need for further research:

In order to properly determine the influence of speech defects on the personality of an individual, we should deal with persons whose defects are purely and wholly functional in origin, defects which as far as we can discern have no organic basis and no organic correlates.

Unfortunately, except in the case of stutterers, there is little experimental evidence of any sort that touches on the possible influence of speech

Page 18: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

10

defects on personality and the experimental evidence which is available has not taken cognizance of the possible concomitants we have mentioned. For the most part, mature persons, usually students at college level, have been the subjects of experimentation. Such subjects constitute a highly selected group and should not be considered represe~~ative of the speech defective population as a whole.

Among the earlier studies mentioned by Berry and

Eisenson of the personality traits of speech defectives

were those of Sara Stinchfield, 1 3 A. M. Templin, 14 w. E.

Moore, 1 5 and Jon Eisenson. 16

Stinchfield, in 1930, administered the Thurstone

Personality Schedule to forty-six speech defective fresh-

men at Mount Holyoke College. Sixty per cent of the

questions considered most significant by Thurstone as being

indicative of maladjustment appeared in the positive list

of high frequency answers.

In 1946, to substantiate the earlier findings,

12Berry and Eisenson, .£:£• cit., p. 65.

13sara M. Stinchfield, Speech Disorders (New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company, 1953), pp. 207-221.

14M. A. Templin, 11 A Study of Aggressiveness in Normal and Defective Speaking College Students," Journal of Speech Disorders, March, 1948, pp. 43-49.

l5w. E. Moore, "Personality Traits and Voice Quality Deficiencies," Journal of Speech Disorders, March, 1949, pp. 33-36.

16Berry and Eisenson, .£:£· cit., pp. 65-69.

Page 19: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

11

Stinchfield arranged a Trait Inventory with forty-six

desirable and undesirable traits to give to three groups

of students at Mount Holyoke College. One group of

students needed speech correction work, a second group

of students were classed as having superior speech, and

a large group of students with average speech ability.

The group needing correction checked more negative traits

than the other two on questions regarding disposition,

tact, courtesy, control of behavior, and undue sensitivity.

The speech correction group indicated by their scores they

considered themselves below the average and superior

group in such traits as evenness of disposition, courtesy,

quietness, good memory, behavior control, and degree of

sensitivity. In addition, the speech correction group

considered themselves more aggressive than the other

students. 17

M. A. Templin used the revised Moore-Gilliland test

to measure the single trait of aggressiveness with seventy-

one students enrolled in the Speech Clinic at Purdue

University and forty-nine normal subjects. 18 Her results,

while not statistically significant because of the small

17stinchfield,

18Templin, .212.·

.212.· cit., pp. 65-69.

cit., pp. 43-49.

Page 20: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

12

differences reported and too few subjects, tend to show

that normal speakers were more aggressive than the speech

defectives; and of the speech defectives, that the

stutterers were more aggressive than the students with

articulatory defects.

W. E. Moore, in 1948, administered the Bernreuter

Personality Inventory to 119 students at Colorado State

College and Kent State University who had voice quality

deficiencies to find that students with breathy voices

were likely to be high in neurotic tendencies and intro­

version, while those with a whine rated as probably

emotionally unstable and lower in dominance. The students

with harsh metallic voices were inclined to be dominant

and more emotionally stable. 19

In 1940, Eisenson sought to find whether the traits

of college speech defectives, as measured by a standard­

ized personality inventory, differ from those of normal

college students and whether the personality traits of

the speech defectives attending a clinic differ from those

of the classroom speech defectives. Using again the

Bernreuter Personality ~nventory, he found (1) the clinic

group slightly more neurotic than the class speech defective

l9Moore, QI?.• cit., pp. 33-36.

Page 21: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

13

group and the normal group, (2) both defective groups

were less self-sufficient than the control group, (3) the

clinic group was more introverted than the class speech

defectives and both were more introverted than the normal

group, (4) the normal speakers were more dominant than

either the clinic or the class speech defectives, and (5)

the clinic group was less self-confident than the class

speech defectives and the latter group more self-conscious

(less self-confident) than the control group. There were,

however, no appreciable differences in sociability among

the groups considered. 20

From the above studies, the following results

indicate: (1) the personality traits of college speech

defectives are different, slightly and undesirably so, from

college students with normal speech, (2) the differences

in personality traits which appear between mild speech

defectives and normal speakers are more serious when clinic

students are compared with normal speakers. Thus, there

seems to be a positive relationship between the serious-

ness of the speech deficiency and the tendency for the

defective individual to possess undesirable personality

traits. There seems to be a tendency for speech defective

20Berry and Eisenson, .2.:2· cit., pp. 67-68.

Page 22: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

individuals of college age to present a personality

picture which includes traits considered to be socially

undesirable.

14

But, in seeking to ascertain whether the speech

defective child in the primary grades of elementary school

differs significantly from the normal child in personality

traits or emotional stability, one finds the research

limited.

Numerous discussions of the relationship between

speech disorders and personality defects in children are

to be found in periodicals and recent texts. The factors

of age, emotion, environment, thinking difficulties, anti­

social trends, economic status, parental coddling, and

parental anxiety have all been mentioned as related to

the retardation of speech. But the majority of these

opinions, as found in the periodicals and texts, lack the

support of reported empirical research and statistical

data.

~uintilla Anders, in 1945, made a study of the

personal and social adjustment of children with functional

articulatory defects. Using fifty-three children ranging

from six to twelve years of age, she obtained a speech

score, a mental age score, a teacher's rating, and a

personality score with the California Test of Personality.

Page 23: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

15

The subjects were found to be above average in adjustment, 21

the median of test scores being 75 and the mean 65.64.

Insofar as her study is concerned, speech correction for

functional articulatory defects cannot be justified on the

basis of preventing inevitable personality maladjustments.

Sister Mary Rose Powers used the same test to

compare a group of one hundred junior high school stutterers

matched according to sex, age, and intelligence with one

hundred junior high school non-stutterers. In self-

adjustment, no significant difference in the two groups

was found; in social adjustment a tendency toward a

significant difference was noted; and in total adjustment

no difference was indicated. She concluded that both

groups may be considered equally well adjusted. 22

Kenneth S. Woods, in 1946, sought to determine

whether articulatory defects of children were definitely

and significantly associated with maladjustment and

undesirable traits (determined by interviews and question­

naires) of the parents. In completing his study, he

21~uintilla M. Anders, "A Study of the Personal and Social Adjustment of Children with Functional Articulatory Defects" (unpublished PhM thesis, University of Wisconsin, Madison, 1945), pp. 18-54.

22sister Mary Rose Powers, "Personality Traits of Junior High School Stutterers as Measured by the California Test of Personality" (unpublished Master's thesis, the University of Illinois, Urbana, 1944), pp. 45-62.

Page 24: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

16

administered the California Test of Personality to a group

of speech defective children. The children's scores were

widely scattered for all three sections of the test and

his were not considered significant in showing maladjust-

ment among the children. Therefore, he concluded that

the speech defective children did not differ signifi-

cantly from test norms on the California Test of Personality. 23

C. Summary and Discussion

In final analysis of the preceding discussion, the

following conclusions may be formulated:

1. Articulatory defects are estimated to comprise

at least seventy per cent of the total cases

of defective speech.

2. Such defects may be considered to consist of

abnormal substitutions, distortions, inser-

tions, or omission of the speech sounds.

3. The causes of articulatory defects are discussed

in terms of organic abnormalities, motor

incoordinations, developmental retardation,

perceptual deficiencies, and emotional

instabilities.

23Kenneth S. Wood, "Parental Maladjustment and Functional Articulatory Defects in Children," Journal of Speech Disorders, VII, December, 1946, 4, pp. 255-275.

Page 25: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

17

4. The majority of authors consider emotional

instability a causal factor in articulatory

defects, especially functional articulatory

defects.

5. Of the several research studies that are reported

with college students, the speech correction

groups were found to possess socially undesir-

able personality traits. They tend to be more

neurotic and less emotionally stable than

other college students having normal speech.

6. Of the three reported studies with elementary

school children, there is little evidence of

the possible influence of speech defects on

personality. In the limited number of cases

sampled, the speech defective group indicated

no definite tendency toward maladjustment.

The above data indicate a diversity of opinion about

the emotional instability of people with articulatory

speech defects. In college students, speech defects

indicate emotional instability; in elementary school

pupils, such a relationship has not yet been shown. There-

fore, a need for further investigation is warranted.

Before the conclusion can be accepted that articulatory

defects in elementary school children are not associated

Library l""l:,n,t:: : 1

, \.-lfaxhingt;:m CoJJlllrf > icn

, :; ".;1;':;;, ·,';: ;:J.shi'V'h"?'I'\

Page 26: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

18

with emotional instability, more observation is necessary.

There is a need for statistical data that would:

1. Show the attitude of speech defective children

toward speech, and

2. Sample the personal and social adjustment of a

number of children with articulatory defects.

This analysis will attempt to further investigate

the problem of whether emotional instability is evident

in children with articulatory defects, and obtain evi­

dence concerning the hypothesis that the speech defect

itself contributed to theemotional instability. If the

hypothesis is supported, speech training would apparently

be an effective instrument for resolving any mild or

more serious maladjustment.

Page 27: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

CHAPTER III

STANDARDS FOR EVALUATION

This particular approach to an objective analysis

concerning the relationship of articulatory defects to

emotional instability involved (a) the selection of a

standardized personality inventory, (b) the construction

of a speech attitude scale, (c) the selection of a group

of children with articulatory defects and an equated

control group, (d) the administration of the tests to the

two groups, and (e) the recognition of the limitations

of the study.

A. The Standardized Personality Test

The first problem in this empirical study was the

selection of a standardized personality inventory. After

a survey of several inventories, the California Test of

Personality (CTP)--Primary Series, was chosen because of

the following distinctive features: (1) it is designed

to reveal the extent to which a group of pupils is adjusting

to the problems and conditions which confront them, (2) it

indicates how pupils feel about themselves (personal

adjustment) and how they function as social beings (social

adjustment), (3) it permits a comparison in terms of

inventory scores, the adjustment patterns and habits of

Page 28: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

20

a specific group (i.e., speech defective children) with a

large representative group, (4) it is based upon a study

of over 1,000 adjustment patterns and responses to specific

situations which confront children of these ages, and (5)

it is graded, so that it may be used on groups ranging

from grade one through college.

In critical analysis of the CTP, Percival Symonds

commented, "This instrument would appear to be one of the

most carefully prepared questionnaires of this type. 1124

For more conclusive evidence of reliability in

definite terms, the 1953 manual of the CTP reports the

reliability coefficients (apparently internal consistency

coefficients) to be: Total Adjustment .88, Personal

Adjustment .83, and Social Adjustment .so. 25

In statistical analysis of the CTP, Eldon E. Jacob­

sen reports the stability coefficients (with five to six

weeks' interval) to be: Total Adjustment, .69:.06 for the

+ first grade, and .77-.04 for the third grade; Personal

Adjustment, .52:.os for the first grade, and .73:.04 for

24Percival M. Symonds, (Professor of Education, Columbia University) in Oscar K. Buros, The Third Mental Measurements Yearbook (New Jersey: Mental Measurements Yearbook, 1941), p. 1214.

25Louis P. Thorpe, Willis W. Clark, and Ernest W. Tiegs, California Test of Personality--Primary Series: Manual (Los Angeles, California: California Test Bureau, 1953), p. 4.

Page 29: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

21

the third grade; Social Adjustment, .64=.06 for the first

grade, and .75=.04 for the third grade. 26

Although some of these considerations prompted the

selection of the inventory, there are certain limitations

which are recognized in the use of any personality inven-

tory. First, one might ask whether questions which are

asked and answered as a part of a school requirement can

be expected to reveal underlying trends which may be

apparently not felt to exist in the personality. Secondly,

by asking pupils questions about themselves, one is securing

evidence of only one kind of adjustment, namely, the pupil's

own attitude toward himself. Thus, the questionnaire is

more limited in its applications than its name, 11 Test of

Personality,u would indicate.

But these criticisms would apply with equal force to

all personality inventories of this general type, for such

instruments should not be used for the basis of a program

of individual diagnosis and treatment without knowing more

of the developmental history and family background of the

pupil. This statement would appear to be in accordance

with the viewpoint of the authors of the CTP:

26Eldon E. Jacobsen, 11 Assessment of Adjustment in Children and Adolescents: Reliabilities and Relationships Concerning Common Group Tests and Ratings and Their Rela­tionships to Judgments from Clinical Tests 11 (unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Washington, Seattle, 1955), p. 68.

Page 30: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

22

Personality is not something separate and apart from ability or achievement but includes them; it refers rather to the manner and effectiveness with which the whole individual meets his personal and social problems, and indirectly the manner in which he impresses his fellows ••• Individual reactions to items are obtained, not primarily for the usefulness of total or section scores, but to detect the areas and specific types of tendencies to think, feel, and actl

7which reveal

undesirable individual adjustment.

In research where group average differences in

specific traits or social adjustment are being investi-

gated, the value of such inventories becomes more definite

in indicating general tendencies toward emotional instability

or a difference in attitude of a group.

B. The Speech Attitude Scale

Accepting the hypothesis that a speech defect can

give rise to adverse emotional reactions to speech, these

reactions would be evident, as appearing on a speech

attitude scale. The construction of this scale presented

a problem in that a measuring instrument was necessary

which would sample a number of speaking situations, use a

language suitable for children in the primary grades, avoid

stereotyped answers, be similar in form to the selected

standardized personality inventory, and be brief enough

to permit administration in a reasonable time.

27Thorpe, Clark, and Tiegs, .2£· cit., p. 2.

Page 31: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

23

Suggestions for questions used in the Speech Attitude

Scale (SAS) were obtained from Franklin H. Knower's Speech

Attitude Scale28 and from William R. Tiffany's Speech

Attitude Scale for Stutterers. 29 However, the majority

of questions were formulated by the writer and sought to

sample as many of the child's speaking situations as

possible. Therefore, questions revealing his attitude

toward speech in the home, the school, and in the neighbor­

hood and community were felt to be pertinent. From a

preliminary group of sixty questions taken from the

previously mentioned sources, twenty-two questions were

chosen for the final scale, given in Appendix A.

The questions were worded so as to require a YES

or NO answer, as was the case with the personality inven-

tory. Every effort was made to phrase the questions

clearly, concisely, and in a language intelligible to the

elementary school child.

The greatest difficulty encountered was phrasing

questions which would elicit the child's reaction to

28Franklin H. Knower, ".A Study of Speech Attitudes and Adjustments," Speech Monographs, V (1953), pp. 130-203.

29william R. Tiffany, "An Experimental Study of the Growth of Speech and Stuttering Attitudes in Children," (unpublished Master's thesis, the University of Washington, Seattle, 1947), pp. 82-85.

Page 32: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

24

speech, rather than his attitude toward his parents or

the situations involved. Instead of asking, "Do other

people ever make fun of your speech?" or "Do you think

children should tell their parents about the things they

do?" the questions were worded, "Are you ever afraid that

other people make fun of your speech?" or Do you like to

tell your parents about the new things you do or see? 11

Thus, the SAS sought to determine the speech defective

child's reactions to speech in various situations since

it was felt those feelings could be the key to his intimate

personality status, as well as his possible improvement.

The scale was scored by counting the number of

undesirable responses and subtracting that number from

twenty-two, the total score.

Since the scale was designed by the writer to sample

a small segment of the population's attitude toward speech

situations, it was necessary to obtain an estimate of its

reliability. This was accomplished by administering the

scale twice to twenty-five youngsters in a Second-Third

grade room at the College Elementary School, with a week's

interval. For the first test, the mean was computed to

be 13.89, with a mean of 14.36 for the retest--a slight

rise in test scores. The reliability was established by

using the Product-Moment formula, and resulted in an r of

Page 33: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

25

.536, with a standard error of :.14. This would indicate

a moderate degree of reliability, significant beyond the

one per cent level of confidence. The restricted sample

of students in the College Elementary School, who showed

largely favorable speech attitudes, probably resulted in

a lower reliability coefficient than might be found with

a wider sample. The approach used for establishing the

reliability of the scale may be examined in Appendix B.

C. The Subjects

The Speech Defective Qroup

The children used in this study were selected from

grades one through three of the Auburn School District, a

medium class, urban area of King County, Washington. The

speech defective children had been screened from the Auburn

Schools by the classroom teachers and reported to the speech

therapist as articulatory cases.

Each teacher gave an opinion as to the severity of

the defect and an independent judgment was made by the

writer. The writer's judgment was made after listening

to a sample of the child's speech in a brief conversation

and making a phonetic inventory before the tests (this

situation is standard in part of the diagnosis for all

reported articulation cases, and was not devised to

Page 34: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

26

accommodate only this particular study). The articulation

defects were diagnosed as either mild, moderate, or severe.

To standardize the teachers' opinions, the following

categories were designed for their use:

1. A mild defect was considered one which would be

noticed by an untrained observer, but not considered

offensive. Slight articulation defects would not involve

more than two sounds.

2. A moderate defect refers to the type of speech

which can readily be recognized by a person as deviating

considerably from accepted speech. The sounds and omis­

sions would be serious enough to mark the speech as

unquestionably defective. Any number of sounds would be

affected.

3. A severe defect is one which definitely inter­

feres with communication. Such defects may have an organic

or functional basis, but preclude, to some degree, success­

ful social adjustment. Numerous sounds, so poorly pro­

nounced that recognition is almost impossible, are char­

acteristic of a severe speech defect.

Where the diagnosis made by the investigator and

the teachers differed, the estimate made by the teacher

was adopted on the assumption that the teacher was better

able to compare the subject's speech with that of other

Page 35: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

27

classroom children. However, arguments against this

procedure could easily be made, for the teacher, through

longer association with the child, might become accustomed

to the particular defect and thus give a biased judgment.

The therapist's judgment, being more objective, could

easily be more valid. Also, a diversity of opinion

between the speech therapist and the classroom teachers

as to the severity of the defect could be explained on the

basis of a difference in criteria used in judgment. Several

cases which were judged as moderate by the teachers were

estimated as mild defects by the writer. The difference

is logically explained by the fact that the testing and

interviewing were completed some months after the teachers'

judgments were made. Thus, the child with a moderate

defect may have benefitted by the therapy to a sufficient

extent as to be diagnosed as mild. This difference is

further substantiated by the fact that a few of the mild

cases were considered sufficiently rehabilitated to attend

speech classes only once a week at the time of testing.

The information compiled for each case included a

list of factors, as outlined in Appendix C. Since

intelligence scores were available only on the second

and third grade subjects, an estimate of the intelligence

of the first grade subjects was made by the classroom

Page 36: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

teacher of each subject. Information which regarded the

home situation of each subject was gained during the

writer's conference with the mothers of the subjects.

Also, at this time, an explanation of the purposes for

this particular study was made to the parent, and verbal

parental consent was given for the participation of each

subject in the program.

The Controls

28

The control group was selected by a parallel-group

technique (that is, both groups were as nearly equivalent

as possible, except for the one variable, the speech

deficiency). A mimeographed brief for grades one, two,

and three, describing each speech defective subject by

the factors listed in Appendix C was prepared and distri­

buted to each classroom teacher in the respective grades

in the Auburn district. Each teacher was then asked to

select a child, or children, from her class that most

nearly matched any of the subjects in the experimental

group on the basis of the characteristics outlined in

Appendix C.

Some difficulty was expressed by a few of the

teachers in the primary grades, who considered some

experimental cases unique ones an.dfelt that the equivalent

Page 37: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

29

was only to be found in a lower grade. In each case where

such difficulties occurred, a control match was found in

another classroom, or another school, if necessary, but

the criteria was unaltered.

The two groups may be compared in Tables 1, 2, 3,

and 4.

Table 1 indicates the distribution of number in

both groups. Twelve of the speech defective group are

from grade one, twelve are from grade two, and twelve

are from grade three, giving a total of thirty-six experi­

mental cases. The same number, with the same ratio of

students from each grade, were included in the control

group.

Table 2 gives a comparison of the two groups,

according to sex and grade. In grades one and three,

the number of males was dominant, with nine from the

first, and eight from the second. However, this ratio

was reversed in the second grade, and there were seven

girls, with only five boys.

Table 3 shows a comparison of the available intel­

ligence quotient scores for both groups. Of the speech

defective group, only thirty-six per cent of the scores

were available, and those showed a mean intelligence

quotient of 98.8. Only twenty-nine per cent of the scores

were available for the control group, with those showing

Page 38: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

30

a mean intelligence quotient of 101.6.

Table 4 shows a comparison of the classroom teachers'

and the writer's estimates as to the severity of the speech

defect for the experimental group. In grade one, the

majority of the cases were judged severe by the teachers,

but the writer judged only sixteen per cent of the grade­

one subjects to be severe articulation cases.

In grade two, the teachers estimated that half of

the cases were mild, and half were moderate articulation

cases, but the writer judged that seventy-five per cent

of the cases were mild articulation problems, and only

twenty-five per cent as being moderate in degree of

severity.

In grade three, the teachers' estimates were fairly

even distributed, but the writer rated sixty-six per

cent of the experimental cases to be mild articulation

problems, with only twenty-six per cent and eight per

cent in the moderate and severe categories, respectively.

Page 39: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

TABLE 1

A COMPARISON OF THE GRADE LEVELS OF THE SPEECH

DEFECTIVE GROUP AND THE CONTROL GROUP

Grade Defects Controls

I

II

III

Totals

12

12

12

36

12

12

12

36

31

Page 40: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

TABLE 2

A COMPARISON OF THE SPEECH DEFECTIVE GROUP AND

THE CONTROL GROUP ACCORDING TO SEX AND GRADE

Grade

I Defects Controls

II Defects Controls

III Defects Controls

Total Defects Total Controls

Male

9 9

(75%)

5 5

(42%)

8 8

(67%)

22 22

(61%)

Female

3 3

(25%)

7 7

(58%)

4 4

(33%)

14 14

(39%)

32

Page 41: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

TABLE 3

A COMPARISON OF AVAILABLE INTELLIGENCE QUOTIENT

SCORES ON THE SPEECH DEFECTIVE GROUP

AND CONTROL GROUP

Group Per Cent of Scores Available

Defects 36

Controls 29

33

Mean

I.Q.

98.8

101.6

Page 42: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

TABLE 4

A cor1PARISON OF CLASSROOM TEACHERS' AND wRITER' s

ESTIMATE OF THE SEVERITY OF THE SPEECH DEFECT

Group Classroom Teachers' writer's Estimate Estimate

Mild Moderate Severe Mild Moderate

Grade I Defects 1 4 7 5 5

(8%) (33%) (59%) (42) (42%)

Grade II Defects 6 6 0 9 3

(50%) (50%) (0%) (75%) (25%)

Grade III Defects 4 5 3 8 3

(33%) (42%) (25%) (66%) (26%)

·I'otals 11 15 10 22 11 (30%) (42%) (28%) (62%) (31%)

34

Severe

2 (16%)

0 (0%)

1 (8%)

3 (7%)

Page 43: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

From the preceding discussion and the Tables, the

following information may be summarized about the groups

tested:

1. An equal distribution of number was found in

each grade.

2. Sixty-one per cent of the cases tested were

boys.

3. The control group had a slightly higher mean

intelligence quotient than the speech defec­

tive group. However, the limited number of

test scores available makes any assumption

35

as to the differences in intelligence between

the two groups insignificant.

4. Forty-two per cent of the experimental group

were considered, by the teachers, to have

moderate speech defects, but the writer

ranked sixty-two per cent of the experimental

group as being mild speech defectives.

Using the matching criteria (as found in Appendix

C) the groups were equated as closely as possible. This

could eliminate the possibility that any differences in

self-ratings between the two groups might arise as factors

of non-equation.

Page 44: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

36

D. The Administration of the Tests

Since the subjects ranged in grade one through three,

it was possible to use one form (AA) of the CTP to obtain

self-estimates on personality for each subject.

The SAS was given first in every case. With the

first grade students who did not have sufficient reading

vocabulary to follow the printed questions, it was nec­

essary to read the questions aloud to each pupil and in

some cases where reading skills were not as well developed

as necessary, the responses of the pupils were recorded

by the investigator. To keep all the factors in the

administration of the test as constant as possible, the

writer read the test questions to all older groups, but

permitted them to encircle their own answers. The first

grade students were tested individually, or in groups of

two, with a short recess between sections one and two of

the personality inventory. They were seated with their

backs to each other at small tables and used markers to

follow the questions in their booklets. For the second

and third grades, where the pupils were able to follow

the questions when read aloud, the test was read clearly

and slowly to groups ranging from four to eight. In these

cases, each child encircled his own answers.

Rapport was established first, by explaining to each

Page 45: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

37

child or group of children that they had been selected

as one of seventy-two boys and girls to be called from

class work to help adults determine how children generally

feel about speech and other matters pertaining to speech

in the home and school. Secondly, rapport was established

by the writer's further explanations that grown-ups often

forget how they felt about speech when they were in grade

school, and it was hoped that the children felt their

honest opinions about speech would enable adults to do a

better job of helping those who had more difficulty with

speech than they did. Thirdly, the fact was stressed

that there were no correct answers to the questions, and

the reason they were asked was that adults agreed they did

not know the answer, and needed a 11 second grader's good

opinion. 11 And lastly, it was emphasized that their

thoughtful, honest answers to the questions on the tests

would be used to help other boys and girls to speak

effectively and clearly.

By putting two sample questions on the board as

examples (i.e., "Do you have a dog at home?" and "Did you

walk all the way to school this morning?") and encircling

a sample YES or NO, the examiner illustrated that there

were no right or wrong answers to the questions, because

some children would answer YES, and others NO. Thus,

Page 46: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

since there were no right or wrong answers, they were

merely to answer as honestly as possible as to how they

felt about the matter expressed in each question.

38

The questions were read so as not to betray by

inflection or expression the "right" answer. \-!here doubt

or hesitancy was evident, the examiner asked the child to

think how he felt or what he did about a situation most

of the time, and then encircle that answer.

The administration of the SAS first proved advanta­

geous because the questions in the test asked for opinions

about speech and speaking situations and confirmed the

statements made in the establishment of rapport. As the

method of answering was similar in the inventory that

followed (the CTP), further explanation was unnecessary;

the questions appeared to be answered in a serious,

thoughtful manner.

E. The Limitations

There are certain limitations in this particular

study that should be noted.

The limited number of both experimental and control

cases prevented as adequate a statistical sampling of

factors in the criteria for matching as might be desired.

The limited number of experimental cases did not

Page 47: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

yield a normal sampling as to severity of defect.

Because of the limited number of control cases,

there were some limitations of the factors involved in

the matching criteria.

A test of reliability for the SAS would have been

more meaningful, had it been administered to both groups

involved in this study.

Areas of exploration that could have been included

in the matching criteria:

A. Intelligence ratings of parents and siblings.

B. Personality ratings of parents and siblings.

39

This chapter has presented the standards for evalua­

tion, and has discussed the reliabilities for the two

test instruments that were used. The results of the

statistical analysis of this study are discussed in the

following chapter.

Page 48: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

CHAPTER IV

EVALUATION AND RESULTS

This particular problem involves an attempt to test

the hypothesis that emotional instability, as measured by

the California Test of Personality (CTP), is evident in

children with articulatory defects. Such a finding would

show that the speech defect contributes to the emotional

instability or that emotional instability is a relevant

antecedent of articulatory disorders.

To resolve the problem, certain methods of evalua­

tion were employed: (1) selection of a standardized

personality inventory, (2) the construction of a speech

attitude scale, (3) the selection of a group of children

with articulatory defects and an equated control group,

the articulation defect being the variable under control,

with personality as the factor to be measured (in this

case serving as the dependent variable), (4) the admin­

istration of the tests to the two groups, and (5) the

evaluation of the results.

A comparison of the experimental and control groups

used in this study was made by analyzing the scores on

(A) the CTP and (B) the Speech Attitude Scale (SAS). In

evaluating the data on the SAS, a correlation coefficient

was calculated to obtain an estimate of its reliability.

Page 49: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

41

A. The California Test of Personality

In comparing the two groups on the CTP, the scores

on personal adjustment, social adjustment, and total

adjustment were used. The personal adjustment score was

obtained from six sub-tests containing questions purporting

to ascertain the child's self-reliance, sense of personal

worth, sense of personal freedom, feeling of belonging,

withdrawing tendencies, and nervous symptoms. The social

adjustment score was determined from questions purporting

to assess the student's social standards, social skills,

anti-social tendencies, family relations, school relations,

and community relations. By adding the personal adjustment

score and the social adjustment score, the total adjustment

of each child was calculated.

To establish the statistical significance of the mean

difference between the two groups, t-tests were computed.

As shown in the following table, the t-test results

indicate a high level of significance for all areas.

From an examination of the data presented in Table 5,

a comparison of the two groups may be made to determine

the relative emotional stability of the speech defective

child and the child with no articulatory disorder.

Inspection of mean scores, and the mean difference of the

scores, and level of confidence shows that the two groups

Page 50: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

42

are significantly different in personal adjustment, social

adjustment, and total adjustment. In every section of

the test, the speech normal children showed a higher group

mean. For example, finding the significance to be beyond

the one per cent level of confidence would suggest, if

the experiment were replicated, that ninety-nine times

out of one hundred we would expect the mean of the speech

normal group to be higher than the mean for the speech

defective group.

Page 51: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

TABLE 5

A COMPAHISON OF MEAN SCORES ON CALIFORNIA TEST OF PERSONALITY--PRIMARY SERIES

FOR CHILDREN WITH ARTICULATION DISORDERS AND SPEECH-NORMAL CHILDREN

AREAS OF Group Mean Mean Std. error Significance MEASUREMENT N Raw Scores Diff. of mean D.F. t Level

difference

Personal Adjustment 6.64 1.05 35 6.32 Beyond 1%

Experimentals 36 27.61

Controls 36 34.25

Social Adjustment 3.42 1.35 35 2.35 Beyond 2%

Experimentals 36 35.19

Controls 36 38.61

Total Scores 10.05 1.93 35 5.21 Beyond 1%

Experimentals 36 62.81

Controls 36 72.86

~ \.N

Page 52: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

44

As a group, speech defective children are character-

istically significantly less emotionally stable, as

determined b~ the CTP.

1. Personal Adjustment

On the personal adjustment section of the CTP, the

speech normal group show a mean of 34.25, which is higher

than the speech defective group's average of 27.61. The

standard deviations of !7.28 for the speech defectives and

!6.63 for the controls indicate that the scores of both

groups on personal adjustment are similarly distributed.

The mean difference between the two groups is 6.64. The

obtained value of t, 6.32, exceeded the one per cent level

of significance.

2. Social Adjustment

The scores on social adjustment of the two groups

show the same trend as the total and personal adjustment

scores. The control group children in the primary grades,

with mean scores of 38.61 and a standard deviation of

!6.08, may have a few more social skills and fewer anti-

social tendencies. As such, the responses suggest that

the majority of them probably maintain better family,

school, and community relations than the children with

articulatory defects who have a mean score of 35.19 with

Page 53: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

45

+ a standard deviation of -6.93. Again, the two distri-

butions are similar. The mean difference between the two

groups is 3.42. With thirty-five degrees of freedom,

t resulted in a figure of 2.35, one that is beyond the two

per cent level of significance.

3. Total Adjustment

Total adjustment scores suggest that the speech

defective group with a mean of 62.81 SD !12.57 are not as

+ emotionally stable as the controls with 72.86 SD -8.37.

However, the standard deviation of the control group at

!s.37 indicates less variability within the group than

the speech defective group with!12.57. The speech defec-

tives' scores are less clustered around the mean--being

much more diversified than the control groups' scores. A

mean difference between the two groups was computed to

be 10.05 and the t-test resulted in a figure of 5.21.

This revealed that the significance of the obtained

differences was beyond the one per cent level of confidence.

On the basis of these results, it is possible to

reject the null hypothesis (that there is no difference

between the means of the two groups) as improbable, and

regard the obtained differences in the test results as

being truly representative of two different populations,

and not occurring as chance variation from the selection

Page 54: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

of samples. It can be assumed with considerable confi­

dence that children with speech disorders, as a group,

manifest less adequate self-perceptions of personality

than children without speech disorders.

46

Complete data, showing the distributions of the raw

scores, means, and standard deviations for both groups

on the CTP are shown in Appendix E.

B. The Speech Attitude Scale

In comparing the two groups on the SAS, each test

was scored by counting the number of undesirable responses,

from a speech therapist's standpoint, and subtracting that

number from twenty-two, the total score.

In order to estimate the reliability of the scale,

a correlation coefficient was calculated. This was

accomplished by administering the scale twice (one week

interval) to a Second-Third grade room at the College

Elementary School, then computing a Product-Moment Cor­

relation Coefficient between the two tests. The stability

coefficient was found to be .536, with a standard error

of ~.14, giving the scale a moderate degree of reliability,

significant beyond the one per cent level of confidence.

The individual scores used for the Product-Moment Cor­

relation of the scale may be examined in Appendix B.

Page 55: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

47

A t-test of significance was computed for the mean

difference between scores on the SAS. An example of the

calculation of t is included in Appendix D. As shown in

Table 6, a mean for the speech defectives on the SAS

proved to be 9.28 SD ±2.73, with the mean for the controls

being 12.36 SD ±2.60, giving a mean difference between

the two groups of 3.08, with a t of 5.22. This shows

the difference to be significant at the one per cent level

of confidence. This suggests that there is only one chance

in one hundred that the difference is due to chance

factors.

Inspection of the data shows that there is a definite

difference between the two groups in attitude toward speech,

as measured by the SAS. The control group tends to have

a less negative (more favorable) attitude toward situa­

tions involving speech. However, the scores for the

control group tend to be more diversified. Complete

data, showing the distributions of the raw scores, the

means, and the standard deviations for both groups on the

SAS are shown in Appendix F.

Page 56: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

AREA OF MEASUREMENT

TABLE 6

A COMPARISON OF MEAN SCORES ON SPEECH ATTITUDE SCALE

FOR CHILDREN WITH ARTICULATION DISORDERS AND SPEECH-NORMAL CHILDREN

N Group Mean Raw Scores

Mean Diff.

Std. error of

mean diff.

Speech Attitude Scale 3.08 • 59

Experimentals

Controls

36

36

9.28

12.36

D.F.

35

Significance t Level

5.22 Beyond 1%

+:­OJ

Page 57: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

.An item-by-item analysis of the SAS, as given in

Table 7, provides further comparison of the two groups

and throws further light on the speech defective child's

attitude toward speech. Scoring was accomplished by

designating items "incorrect" which show "poor speech

attitudes" insofar as speech therapists are concerned.

49

It should be noted that on seven questions, the

speech defective children show a more definite negative

attitude toward speech than the control group. On ques­

tion 1, eighty-three per cent of the speech defective

children indicate that their fathers do not let them

talk as much as they would like at home, but only sixty

per cent of the speech normal children indicated the same

answer. In question 6, forty-one per cent of the speech

defectives answered NO to the question, nno your )arents

think that you speak well'?" compared to sixteen per cent

of the controls. The answers on these two questions

tend to suggest a reaction on the part of the speech

defective group to parental attitudes regarding speech.

On the other questions--numbers 13, 14 and 15-­

the speech defective group indicated more negative atti­

tudes toward speech than the control group.

The most striking difference between groups appears

in the answers to question 20, where sixty-five per cent

Page 58: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

TABLE 7

A COMPARISON OF THE TWO GROUPS ON THE NUMBER OF "POOR SPEECH ATTITUDE 11 ANSWERS ON THI£ SPEECH ATTITUDE SCALE

Per Cent of Defects Questions with Desirable Attitude Answer Answering in terms

of Poor Speech Attitude

1. Does your father let you talk as much as you like at home? (YES)

2. Does your mother let you talk as much as you like at home? (Yes)

3. Do your parents often correct you at home when you speak? (NO)

If YES, does it bother you to have them correct you? (NO)

4. Do your parents often correct your speech in front of others? (NO)

If YES, does it bother you to have them correct your speech in front of others? (NO)

5. Do you have to be careful how you speak for fear you will be corrected? (NO)

83 (NO)

69 (NO)

81 (YES)

35 (YES)

50 (YES)

45 (YES)

74 (YES)

Per Cent of-~Coiitrois Answering in terms of Poor Speech Attitude

60

50

70

20

35

30

65

(NO)

(NO)

(YES)

(YES)

(YES)

(YES)

(YES)

\Jl 0

Page 59: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

TABLE 7 (Continued)

Questions with Desirable Attitude Answer Per Cent of Defects Answering in terms of Poor Speech Attitude

6. Do your parents think that you speak well? (YES) 41

7. Are you ever afraid that other people will make fun of your speech? (NO) 20

8. Would you like to be allowed to ask your parents more questions about things? (NO) 60

9. Do you like to tell your parents about the new things you've done or seen? (YES) 10

10. Do you like to tell the things you have done or seen in your class? (YES) 20

11. Do you like to read aloud to the class? (YES) 30

12. Do you think other people in your class speak better than you do? (NO)

13. Do others like to listen to you when you tell about the things that happen to you? (Y.ES)

80

35

(NO)

(YES)

(YES)

(NO)

(NO)

(NO)

(YES)

(NO)

Per Cent of Controls Answering in terms of Poor Speech Attitude

16 (NO)

14 (YES)

59 (YES)

10 (NO)

16 (NO)

24 (NO)

65 (YES)

11 (NO)

\J1 I-'

Page 60: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

TABLE 7 (Continued)

Questions with Desirable Attitude Answer Per Cent of Defects Answering in terms of Poor Speech Attitude

14. Do you enjoy talking to the older children? (YES) 38

15. Is it hard for you to talk to a group of children who are not your good friends? (NO) 75

16. Do you worry about talking to grownups or strangers because of your speech? (NO) 30

17. Do you think that pretending or talking to make-believe playmates is more fun than talking to your friends? (NO) 16

18. Do you think that your parents like to have you talk when company is present? (YES) 84

19. Do you think we should make fun of the people who do not speak well on the radio, or in the movies, or TV? (NO) 6

20. Do you ever feel ashamed of yourself because of your speech? (NO) 65

(NO)

(YES)

(YES)

(YES)

(NO)

(YES)

(YES)

Per Cent of Controls Answering in terms of roor Speech Attitude

10 (NO)

52 (YES)

12 (YES)

11 (YES)

80 (NO)

0 (YES)

20 (YES)

\Jl I\)

Page 61: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

53

of the speech defective group indicated that they have

felt ashamed of themselves because of their speech, while

only twenty per cent of the control group felt the same

way. The answers of the speech defective groups suggest

an awareness of the speech defects.

The item-by-item analysis of the SAS substantiates

the earlier findings that the two groups do differ in

their reactions to speech, with the speech defective

group showing a consistently higher percentage of "poor

speech attitude" answers than the control group.

In analysis of the questions on the SAS, it seems

that the wording of these questions may have been such

that they called for an attitude toward parental discipline

rather than toward speech. That a great many fathers do

not let their children talk as much as they like in the

home, that many parents often correct the child's speech,

that some parents feel children ask too many questions,

and that most parents feel children are not to take too

active a part in conversation with company--all can

readily be considered a part of the home discipline. How

much such discipline directly or indirectly affects the

child's speech is beyond the scope of this paper.

The results as presented, have shown that the con­

trol group consistently scored a higher mean in all

Page 62: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

54

sections of the OTP, in personal adjustment, social adjust­

ment, and in total scores.

The results have also shown that the control group

show a higher mean than the experimental group on the SAS.

The results have further shown that children with

articulatory defects are not as well adjusted as children

without speech defects (as indicated by the OTP), both in

personal and social adjustments, and in total adjustments.

In addition, the results have shown that children

with articulation defects have a less positive attitude

toward speech than do the children without speech defects.

These differences were tested for significance and

suggest the improbability of the difference being due to

chance selection of the sample, but rather actual differ­

ences in emotional adjustment and attitude toward speech.

Page 63: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

CHAPTER V

SU111"1ARY

The purpose of this study was to determine whether

a group of children with articulatory defects differed

significantly in emotional stability from a control group

when measured by a standardized personality inventory

designed to reveal the personal and social adjustment and

a speech attitude scale devised to detect reactions to

speech situations. To accomplish this, an experimental

group of thirty-six speech defective children and a control

group of thirty-six speech normal children were selected

from the primary grades, twelve in each group from each

grade. The groups were matched according to all avail­

able relevant factors (listed in Appendix C). Scores

were obtained for each child in the two groups on (1)

personal adjustment, (2) social adjustment, and (3) total

adjustment, taken from the California Test of Personality

(CTP), and (4) the Speech Attitude Scale (SAS), constructed

by the researcher.

From the tabulation of data, the following conclu­

sions are justifiable and answer the questions posed in

the opening chapter:

1. Children with articulatory defects in the primary

Page 64: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

grades do differ significantly in personal

adjustment, social adjustment, and total

adjustment in personality from a matched

control group when measured by the CTP.

56

2. The children with articulatory defects tend to

have a less positive attitude toward speech

than a control group of speech normal children,

when measured on the SAS.

From the above conclusions of this study, the

following observations are made as highly probable:

1. Emotional instability, as measured by the CTP,

can be substantiated as a causal factor in

articulatory defects, or the possibility exists

that a speech defect may be a relevant ante­

cedent of emotional instability.

2. The speech defect can be a causal factor in

the difference of attitude toward speech, or

even more important, from a preventative

standpoint, speech attitude seems to be a

relevant antecedent of the defect.

Before such observations are accepted as fact,

however, further research is needed. First, it is felt

that a more effective speech attitude scale could be

designed through further experimentation. Such an

Page 65: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

57

instrument could make it possible to determine early, a

difference in attitude toward speech and thus make speech

correction more effective in absolving such difficulties.

Secondly, further research with children diagnosed as

having severe or moderate defects is needed to substan-

tiate the relationship between articulatory defects and

emotional stability. This small sampling contained only

three cases diagnosed as severe in the experimental group

of the present study. More severe cases in the experi-

mental group, rather than those diagnosed with less

severity would provide greater definitiveness. Also, it

would be desirable to have investigations on the diversity

of judgments by therapists and teachers as to the severity

of the defect. Thirdly, research which will test a

sufficiently large number of children in each grade,

especially the older elementary school children, is needed

to determine the possible growth of any difference in

attitudes toward both speech and emotional stability.

And finally, experimentation with two groups of children

with articulatory defects--one group having speech cor­

rection and the other not--would ascertain the part that

speech rehabilitation might take in alleviating the less

favorable attitudes toward speech and emotional stability.

Page 66: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

.x.HcIVHDOI'IS:I 9:

Page 67: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

BIBLIOGRAPHY

A. BOOKS

Backus, Ollie L. Speech in Education, ! Guide for the Classroom Teacher. New York: Longmans Green and Co., 1953.

Bender, James F., and Victor M. Kleinfield. Principles and Practices of Speech Correction. New York: Pitman Publishing Co., 1938.

Berry, Mildred F., and Jon Eisenson. The Defective in Speech. New York: F. S. Crofts and Co., 1955·

Stinchfield, Sara M. Speech Disorders: StudJ of Various Defects of Speech. Brace and Co., 1953.

! Psychological New York: Harcourt,

Symonds, Percival M., in 0. K. Buros. The Third Mental Measurements Yearbook, Mental Measur"8Ili'ents Yearbook, New Jersey, 1941.

west, Robert, Louis Kennedy, and Anna Carr. The Rehabili­tation of Speech, ~ Textbook of Diagnostic and Correc­tive Procedures. Rev. Ed. New York: Harper and Bros., 1947.

Van Riper, Charles. Speech Correction: Principles and Methods. New York: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1947.

white House Conference on Child Health and Protection. Special Education, Report of Committee on Special Classes. New York: D. Appleton-Century Co., 1932.

B. PERIODICALS

Knower, Franklin H. "A Study of Speech Attitudes and Adjustments," Sueech Monographs, V (1953), pp. 130-203.

Moore, W. E. "Personality Traits and Voice Quality Deficiencies," Journal of Speech Disorders, 4 (March, 1949), pp. 33-36.

Page 68: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

60

Templin, M. A. 11 A Study of Aggressiveness in Normal and Defective Speaking College Students, 11 Journal of Speech Disorders, March, 1948, pp. 43-49.

Wood, Kenneth Scott. "Parental Maladju,stment and Func­tional Articulatory Defects in Children, 11 Journal of S~eech Disorders, 7, No. 4 (December, 1946), pp. 255-2 5.

C. PERSONALITY INVENTORIES

Bernreuter, R. G. Personality Inventory. Stanford, Cali­fornia: Stanford University Press, 1931.

Thorpe, Louis P., Willis W. Clark, and Ernest W. Tiegs. California Tes! of Personality--Primari and ElementarJ Series. Los Angeles, California: California Test Bureau, 1953.

Thurstone, L. L., and T. G. Thurstone. Personality Schedule. Chicago, Illinois: University of Chicago Press, 1929.

D. UNPUBLISHED MATERIALS

Anders, Quintilla Morgan. 11 A Study of Personal and Social Adjustment of Children with Functional Articulatory Defects. 11 Unpublished Phll Thesis, The University of Wisconsin, Madison, 1945.

Jacobsen, Eldon E. 11 Assessment of Adjustment in Children and Adolescents: Reliabilities and Relationships Concerning Common Group Tests and Ratings and Their Relationships to Judgments from Clinical Tests." Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Washington, Seattle, 1955.

Powers, Sister ivrary Bose. "Personality Traits of Junior High School Stutterers as Measured by the California Test of Personality." Unpublished MA thesis, Univer­sity of Illinois, Urbana, 1944.

Page 69: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

Roe, Vivian Irene. "The Effects of Maturation Upon Defective Articulation in Elementary Grades." Unpub­lished MA thesis, University of Indiana, Bloomington, 1940.

61

Tiffany, William R. "An Experimental Study of the Growth of Speech and Stuttering Attitudes in Children." Unpublished MA thesis, University of Washington, Seattle, 1947.

Page 70: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

XIITNJJdcIV

Page 71: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

APPENDIX A

SPEECH ATTITUDE SCALE

Instructions to pupils: After each of the following questions mark a circle around the YES or NO. The answers are not right or wrong, but show how you feel about speech.

1. Does your father let you talk as much as you like at home?

2. Does your mother let you talk as much as you like at home?

3. Do your parents often correct you at home when you speak?

If YES, does it bother you to have them correct you?

4. Do your parents often correct your speech in front of others?

If YES, does it bother you to have them correct your speech in front of others?

5. Do you have to be careful about how you speak for fear you will be corrected?

6. Do your parents think that you speak well?

7. Are you ever afraid that other people make fun of your speech?

8. Would you like to be allowed to ask your parents more questions about things?

9. Do you like to tell your parents about the new things you have done or seen?

10. Do you like to tell the things you have done or seen to your class?

YES NO

YES NO

YES NO

YES NO

YES NO

NO

YE$ NO

YES NO

YES NO

us NO

YES NO

YES NO

Page 72: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

11. Do you like to read aloud to the class?

12. Do you think other people in your class speak better than you do?

13. Do others like to listen to you when you tell about the things that happen to you?

14. Do you enjoy talking to the older children?

15. Is it hard for you to ta~k to a group of children who are not your good friends?

16. Do you worry about talking to grownups or strangers because of your speech?

17. Do you think that pretending or talking to make-believe playmates is more fun than talking to your friends?

18. Do you think that your parents like to have you talk when company is present?

19. Do you think we should make fun of the people who do not speak well on the radio, or in the movies, or TV?

20. Do you ever feel ashamed of yourself because of your speech?

64

YES NO

YES NO

YES NO

YES NO

Y}i;S NO

YES NO

YES NO

YES NO

YES NO

YES NO

Page 73: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

65

APPENDIX B

SAMPLE DATA USED IN COMPUTING A STABILITY COEFFICIENT FOR THE SPEECH ATTITUDE SCALE

Test Retest x2 y2 x y XY

19 20 361 400 380 17 18 289 324 306 16 15 256 225 240 15 15 225 225 225 15 12 225 144 180 15 13 225 166 215 15 17 225 289 255 15 12 225 144 180 14 16 296 256 224 14 13 196 166 182 14 12 196 144 156 14 17 196 289 238 14 18 196 324 252 14 14 196 196 196 14 11 196 121 154 14 15 196 225 210 13 13 166 166 166 13 15 166 225 215 13 12 166 144 156 12 16 144 256 192 12 16 144 256 192 12 11 144 121 132 12 11 144 121 132 10 10 100 100 100 10 12 100 144 120

M 13.84 f1 14.36

1 week's interval

Page 74: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

APPENDIX C

MATCHING CRITERIA FOR PARALLEL-GROUP TECHNIQUE

I. School grade

II. Sex

III. Race

IV. Chronological age

v. Retention

VI. Records of

A. Achievement B. Abilities

VII. Physical Rating

A. Size B. General Physical Condition C. Handicaps D. Attitude toward

1. Health habits 2. Personal appearance

VIII. Frequency of absences for previous school year

IX. Status of child in home

A. Adoption/Foster home B. Number of siblings C. Ages of siblings D. Adopted/half/step sisters/brothers E. Ordinal position

X. Parental Background

A. Cultural B. Educational C. Age group D. Health status

Page 75: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

67

E. Occupation

1. Father 2. Mother

a. Full time b. Part time

F. Marital status

1. Both parents living 2. Previous divorce

XI. Home Status

A. Socio-economic group B. Stability C. Place of residence

1. City 2. Country

D. Type of residence

1. Single unit 2. Multiple unit 3. Project area

XII. Parental attitude toward

A. Child B. School

Page 76: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

68

APPENDIX D

SAMPLE DATA FOR COMPUTING A t-TEST OF SIGNIFICANCE BETWEEN MATCHED PAIRS OF CHILDREN WITH ARTICULATION DISORDERS

AND SPEECH NORMAL CHILDREN ON THE SPEECH ATTITUDE SCALE

Ex12er. Cont. D d d2 10 5 5 -1.92 3 10 9 1 2.08 4 11 9 2 1.08 1 10 8 2 1.08 1 11 10 1 2.08 4 14 12 2 1.08 1 13 1 12 -8.92 80 13 10 3 .08 0 11 12 -1 4.08 17 12 12 0 3.08 9

9 6 3 .08 0 11 14 -3 6.08 37 15 8 7 -3.92 15 13 9 4 - .92 0 15 10 5 -1.92 3 11 13 -2 5.08 26 17 10 7 -3.92 15 11 10 1 2.08 4 15 7 8 -4.92 24 15 13 2 1.08 1 17 10 7 -3.92 15 12 13 -1 4.08 17

8 6 2 1.08 1 15 7 8 -4.92 24 17 9 8 -4.92 24 11 12 -1 4.08 17

7 9 -2 5.08 26 11 9 2 1.08 1 13 11 2 1.08 1 14 13 1 2.08 4 14 5 9 -5.92 35

9 9 0 3.08 9 9 7 2 1.08 1

16 9 7 -3.92 15 12 9 3 .08 0 13 8 2 -1.92

442 234 111 438

od + om + t 5.22 -3.49 d -.59

Page 77: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

69

APPENDIX E COMPARATIVE RAW SCORES OF CHILDREN

WITH ARTICULATION DISORDERS AND SPEECH NORMAL CHILDREN FOR CALIFORNIA TEST OF PERSONALITY PRil"IARY SERIES (AA)--ALL SECTIONS

Personal Adjustment Social Adjustment Total Scores Ex:12er. Cont. Ex12er. Cont. Ex12er. Cont.

25 29 34 32 59 61 17 39 31 44 48 83 23 31 30 28 53 59 31 32 41 44 72 76 22 36 31 24 53 60 34 31 28 40 62 71 25 22 39 35 64 57 37 38 35 46 72 84 31 35 24 23 55 58 26 29 32 34 58 63 28 28 32 29 60 57 15 21 19 34 34 55 28 32 36 41 64 73 30 34 32 40 62 74 31 37 46 38 77 75 32 36 39 43 71 79 27 37 34 39 61 76 32 37 39 46 71 83 31 39 45 40 76 79 19 37 31 45 50 82 32 38 41 41 73 79 17 38 28 42 45 80 28 28 32 32 60 60 34 34 44 43 78 77 30 37 43 42 73 79 35 40 41 37 76 77 18 31 28 44 46 75 16 29 29 36 45 65 35 35 45 42 80 77 40 44 45 44 85 88 19 35 32 45 51 80 34 40 39 36 73 76 18 34 22 37 40 71 30 39 39 36 69 75 31 37 44 47 75 84 23 34 27 41 70 22

N 36 36 36 36 36 36 M ~7.61 ?4.25 45.19 ?8.61 +62.81 ~2.86 0 -6.62 -z.28 -6.93 -6.08 -12.57 -s.22

Page 78: A Study of the Relationship Between Defects of

70

APPENDIX F

COMPARATIVE SCORES OF CHILDREN WITH ARTICULATION DISORDERS AND SPEECH NORMAL CHILDREN FOR

THE SPEECH ATTITUDE SCALE

Ex12erimentals Controls 5 10 9 10 9 11 8 10

10 11 12 14

1 13 10 13 12 11 12 12

6 9 14 :bl

8 15 9 13

10 15 13 11 10 17 10 11

7 15 13 15 10 17 13 12

6 8 7 15 9 17

12 11 9 7 9 11

11 13 13 14

5 14 9 9 7 9 9 16 9 12 8 1

N 36 36 l'1 +9.28 i2.36 0 -2.73 -2.60