30156175

Upload: albertocho

Post on 07-Jul-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    1/19

    Division of Comparative Physiology and Biochemistry Society for Integrative and

    Comparative Biology

     

    Proximate and Evolutionary Constraints on Energy Relations of Reptiles

    Author(s): Justin D. Congdon

    Source: Physiological Zoology , Vol. 62, No. 2 (Mar. - Apr., 1989), pp. 356-373Published by: The University of Chicago Press. Sponsored by the Division of Comparative

    Physiology and Biochemistry, Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology

    Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/30156175

    Accessed: 09-04-2016 04:08 UTC

     

    REFERENCES 

    Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:

    http://www.jstor.org/stable/30156175?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents

    You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

     

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at

    http://about.jstor.org/terms

     

    JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted

    digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about

    JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    Division of Comparative Physiology and Biochemistry, Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology, The University of Chicago Press are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,preserve and extend access to Physiological Zoology 

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    2/19

     3 5 6

     Proximate and Evolutionary Constraints on Energy

     Relations of Reptiles

     Justin D. Congdon

     Savannah River Ecology Laboratory, Drawer E, Aiken, South Carolina 29802

     Accepted 7/15/88

      Abstract

     Proximate constraints on the energetics of desert lizards and turtlesfall into four

     major categories: (1) absolute resource availability; (2) harvest rate limitations;

     (3)process rate limitations; and (4) limitation on harvest orprocessing resources

     imposed by risk ofpredation. Patterns ofbasking behavior and allocation of re-

     sources to reproduction and growth ofturtles at high latitudes suggest that ambient

     air and marsh temperatures combine to regulate the timing and rates of energy

     processing and allocation. Because reptile eggs contain all of the chemical energy

     available to developing embryos, interactions between the level ofparental invest-

     ment in each offspring and egg size must, by definition, occur. Thatportion of the

     egg material that is invested in the egg butfunctions asparental care of the hatch-

     ling after it leaves the eggfunctionsprimarily to affect hatchling quality rather than

     hatchling size. Therefore, with regard to many reptiles, optimal egg-size models

     would be more aptly considered to be optimal offspring-quality models.

      Introduction

     A number of evolutionary theories are based on assumed interactions among

     resource availability, behavior, morphology, and age-specific demograph-

     ics. These interactions presumably determine the association of an organ-

     ism's life-history traits and energy budgets. However, the argument that sim-

     ilar life-history phenotypes can result from very different selective forces

     (Wilbur, Tinkle, and Collins 1974) seems well founded and can certainly

     be extended to energy-allocation phenotypes as well. Interactions among

     trophic level, morphology, demographic environment, and phylogeny of an

     organism combine to make the mere presentation of total energy budgets,

     in the absence of an ecological and evolutionary context, of limited value.

     At present, total energy budgets of reptiles represent first-order approxima-

     tions of average energy allocations for all individuals or for individuals of

     each sex within a population (Avery 1975; Turner, Medica, and Kowalewsky

     Physiological Zoology 62(2):356-373. 1989. @ 1989 by The University of Chicago.

     All rights reserved. 0031-935X/89/6202-8831$02.00

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    3/19

     Constraints on Energetics of Reptiles 357

     1976; Congdon, Dunham, and Tinkle 1982; Nagy 1982). As such, the resolu-

     tion needed to distinguish among various sequential decision-making pro-

     cesses that lead to energy budget phenotypes is, at best, only partially at-

     tained.

     The purpose of this article is to review some concepts related to construc-

     tion and interpretation of ecological energy budgets that attempt to measure

     an individual's allocation of net assimilated energy to the competing com-

     partments of maintenance, growth, reproduction, and storage (Wiegert

     1968; Congdon et al. 1982; Nagy 1982; Anderson and Karasov 1988). To ac-

     complish this goal, I shall (1) discuss some broad categories of resource

     constraint on energy acquisition and allocation of desert lizards and temper-

     ate-zone turtles; (2) examine some data on ecological and life-history fac-

     tors that constrain energy allocations of desert lizards in general and turtles

     on the University of Michigan's E. S. George Reserve (ESGR) in Livingston

     County, Michigan; and (3) review life-history theories concerning predic-

     tions about energy-allocation decisions of reptiles.

     I. Major Categories of Constraints on Resource Availability

     Interpretations of ecological energy budgets and most, if not all, life-history

     theories are based on the assumption that the energy available to an individ-

     ual is finite. The maximum resources that can be harvested by individuals

     may be set by absolute quantity of resources available or by constraints im-

     posed during foraging, handling, or processing of resources. Temporal con-

     straints may take the form of biophysical limitations on energy acquisition

     and allocation that may be imposed daily or seasonally or by competing

     demands for time spent in social activities. Levels of resource acquisition

     for a given level of resources and associated biophysical and time constraints

     might then be further limited by risks associated with resource acquisition.

     Resource Availability

     A comparison of the types of energy constraints that may exist among rep-

     tiles indicates that correct identification of the type and cause of the limita-

     tion is important (table 1). The response of the animal to different types

     of energy-acquisition constraints may be the same in different habitats or

     different in similar habitats.

     Low primary productivity of desert habitats is primarily caused by low wa-

     ter availability (Noy-Meir 1973, 1974; Webb et al. 1978; Cunningham et al.

     1979; Hadley and Szarek 1981) that indirectly limits secondary production

     (Dunham 1981). Of secondary influence are the timing, frequency, and in-

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    4/19

     358 Justin D. Congdon

     TABLE 1

     Possible causes of limitation on energy acquisition and

     processing of reptiles

     Lmtations Location

     Absolute resource availability Deserts

     Harvest rate Deserts high latitude or altitude

     Biophysical

     Risk of harvest activities

     Social activities

     Process rate High latitude or altitude

      Food type

     Biophysical

     Water availability

     Risk of thermoregulating

     tensity of rainfall, soil type, and nitrogen availability (Lightfoot and Whitford

     1987) and their relative effects on annual and perennial plants (Evanari et

     al. 1976; Cunningham et al. 1979; Szarek 1979; Hadley and Szarek 1981).

     For example, in the Mojave Desert, which averages approximately 1,200 m

     elevation, precipitation seldom exceeds 12.5 cm per year and occurs primar-

     ily during the relatively cold winter (Shreve 1925). Annual net primary pro-

     duction averages 14-43 g dry mass/m2. In contrast, most of the Sonoran

     Desert is below 600 m elevation, and rainfall there averages under 12 cm

     and occurs almost equally during winter and summer (Shreve 1942). Annual

     net primary production averages 92-129 g dry mass/m2. Precipitation has

     been shown to be tightly linked to resource availability in desert habitats

     (Turner 1973, 1974; Dunham 1981).

     Desert reptiles, whether herbivorous, insectivorous, or carnivorous, cer-

     tainly face resource limitation that fluctuates seasonally and sporadically.

     The intensity and frequency of these fluctuations can be expected to have

     major effects on the timing of energy storage and allocation. Sparse rainfall

     limits the amount of plant production and also may result in much of the

     available plant material's being too dry for herbivores to eat. For example,

     the chuckwalla (Sauromalus obesus; Nagy and Shoemaker 1975) and the

     desert tortoise (Scaptochelys agassizii; Nagy and Medica 1986), both herbiv-

     orous reptiles, may have energy available in the form of dried plant material,

     but this cannot be processed while there are no alternative sources of water.

     A similar condition has also been shown in jackrabbits in the Mojave Desert

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    5/19

     Constraints on Energetics of Reptiles 359

     (Nagy, Shoemaker, and Costa 1976). Unlike herbivores, both insectivorous

     and carnivorous reptiles obtain preformed water in food, so reduced rainfall

     acts primarily to reduce prey abundance rather than quality. Two studies

     have documented reduced growth rates in insectivorous lizards that were

     associated with low food resources (Dunham 1978; Ballinger and Cong-

     don 1980).

     Limitations on Harvest Rate

     Linked to absolute resource limitation in the desert are a set of constraints

     on the rate of harvesting resources (table 1). These constraints can be as

     obvious as the restrictions placed on activity over a relatively long period by

     seasonally high or low temperatures. In some habitats and situations, similar

     constraints can also occur on a daily basis (Karasov and Anderson 1984;

     Grant and Dunham 1987; Anderson and Karasov 1988). Perhaps the best

     study of this type of daily constraint has been made on an insectivorous liz-

     ard (Sceloporus merriami) in the Big Bend National Monument in west

     Texas (Grant and Dunham 1987). During summer, the thermal environment

     restricted daily activity to approximately a 2-h period in the morning and a

     brief period in late afternoon. Despite the apparent severity of time con-

     straints on activity, the lizard has an average preferred body temperature

     lower than that of any other North American iguanid lizard studied (Grant

     and Dunham 1987). The restricted daily activity period, coupled with the

     low productivity of desert habitat, would seem to place the lizard in a very

     poor environment for food acquisition. However, it would be interesting

     to know whether the same thermal constraints that affect lizard behavior

     concentrate the activity of many insect prey items into the same microhabitat

     and window of time.

     Competing demands on restricted activity times can also have a dramatic

     effect on the short-term energy balance of animals. For example, some male

     lizards spend much of their time defending boundaries of territories, dis-

     playing from prominent sites (Ruby 1981; Ruby and Dunham 1984), or ac-

     tively searching for females (Anderson 1986; Anderson and Karasov 1988).

     During the breeding season, male Sceloporusjarrovi are active most of the

     day every day, lose body mass rapidly, and are in a negative energy balance.

     In contrast, gravid females are active only for short periods about every third

     day and yet are able to maintain a constant body mass and energy balance

     (Congdon 1977). Large males of the wide-foraging lizard Cnemidophorus

     tigris spend much time patrolling their home ranges during the reproduc-

     tive season and maintain a constant energy balance, whereas females have a

     positive energy balance during vitellogenesis (Anderson and Karasov 1988).

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    6/19

     360 Justin D. Congdon

     Process Rate Limitation

     An organism's total energy intake may be limited not only by its ability to

     harvest food but also by its ability to process food already harvested. In con-

     trast to desert lizards that are probably not under severe process constraints,

     aquatic turtles occupy relatively productive habitats, with primary productiv-

     ity averaging 2,500 g/m 2/yr (as cited in Ricklefs 1979); however, low water

     temperatures may constrain the ability of temperate-zone turtles to harvest

     and process food. Evidence for this type of constraint was found in a compar-

     ison of growth and body size from two populations of slider turtles (Tra-

     chemys scripta) occupying a thermally enhanced and a natural habitat in

     South Carolina Gibbons 1970). Turtles from the warmer habitats had

     higher growth rates and attained larger body sizes than did those from the

     natural environment. Because of the positive relationship between tempera-

     ture and rate of digestion and gut clearance (Avery 1987), it may be pre-

     dicted that, among turtle populations, the severity of constraint will increase

     with the amount of plant material in the diet and with either latitude or

     altitude. The potential effect of harvest and processing constraints on the

     population dynamics of turtles may have been overlooked. Harvesting and

     processing constraints may be the major factors that determine (1) the reli-

     ance on stored lipids for reproduction (Congdon and Tinkle 1982) and (2)

     the timing of energy allocation to, and transfers among, energy budget com-

     partments of many turtles. In addition, these dual constraints may also limit

     the total amount of resources consumed by turtles each year. A suggestion

     that processing constraints may be influential can be seen in the following

     comparison of turtle populations from South Carolina and Michigan. Annual

     biomass production of the entire six species constituting a turtle community

     in South Carolina averaged 9.7 kg/ha (3.9 X 10-2% of average primary pro-

     ductivity), and a community consisting of three turtle species in Michigan

     averaged 7.3 kg/ha (2.9 X 10-2% of average primary productivity; Congdon

     and Gibbons 1989).

     II. Factors Influencing Energy Availability and Allocation

     Basking Behavior and Thermal Constraints on Processing Rates

     A number of studies have attempted to determine the functional significance

     of basking by turtles (table 2; and see review by Moll and Legler [1971]).

     Basking out of the water or in mats of vegetation is the primary way that most

     aquatic turtles elevate their body temperature above that of the surrounding

     water (Boyer 1965; Auth 1975; Standora 1982; Crawford, Spotila, and Stan-

     dora 1983).

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    7/19

     Constraints on Energetics of Reptiles 361

     TABLE 2

     Suggestedfunctions for basking behavior in turtles other than

     raising body temperature

     unctonRference

     Control of external environment:

     Elimination of ectoparasites Cagle 1950

     Eliminate epizoic algae from

     carapace Neill and Alen 954

     Drying of integument to reduce

     bacterial and fungal

     infections Boyer 965

     Metabolic processes:

     Synthesis of vitamin D

     associated with calcium

     metabolismPritchard and Greenhood 968

     Enhanced digestive efficiency .. Kenyon 1925

     Enhanced digestive rates Gatten 1974; Hammond,

     Spotila, and Standora 1988;

     Kepenis and McManus 1974;

     Parmenter 1981

     Reproduction:

     Increased fat mobilization Whittow and Balazs 1982

     Increased rate of follicle

     development Congdon and Tinkle 1982;

     Whittow and Balazs 1982

     It has been stated that the time spent in thermoregulation (1) cannot be

     used as efficiently for other behaviors such as foraging for food, (2) may

     expose the animal to predators, and (3) would increase metabolic demands

     (Huey and Slatkin 1976). The time spent in aerial basking certainly restricts

     a turtle's ability to engage in other activities; however, basking in a vegeta-

     tion mat may also provide opportunities to feed. Two studies have used the

     concept of operative environmental temperatures (Te) to study basking be-

     havior of turtles (Crawford et al. 1983; Schwarzkopf and Brooks 1985). A

     general definition of Te is the temperature of an inanimate object of zero

     heat capacity with the same size, shape and radiative properties of the animal

     exposed to the same microclimate (Bakken and Gates 1975). A prediction

     from cost-benefit analysis of basking behavior is that turtles will bask when

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    8/19

     362 Justin D. Congdon

     T,'s are highest so that basking time can be minimized (Crawford et al.

     1983). This prediction makes the implicit assumption that time free from

     basking is the factor being optimized or maximized. However, if turtles are

     primarily process limited (table 1), then time spent at temperatures that

     promote rather than optimize enhanced digestive efficiencies and rapid gut

     clearance should be optimized (table 2). Thus, heating rapidly and return-

     ing to the water would only be the expected behavior of turtles with little

     or no food in their guts (i.e., no resources need processing, so the turtle may

     be expected to bask only long enough to raise its body temperature high

     enough to permit efficient foraging). When water temperatures are below

     that for efficient absorption of food material and rapid gut clearance, turtles

     with full guts will attempt to maintain their body temperature above that

     of the surrounding water regardless of whether they can reach preferred

     temperatures. Thus, turtles with full guts will not wait for optimum basking

     conditions but will take advantage of any opportunity to maintain body tem-

     perature as close to the preferred range as possible.

     The selected temperature of slider turtles (Trachemys scripta) varies

     from approximately 240 C for individuals with empty guts to 29 *C when their

     guts contain food (Gatten 1974). A similar temperature range was attained

     by basking painted turtles in Pennsylvania (22.5*-29.0 *C; Ernst 1972) and

     in Minnesota (26.3O-30.20C; Brattstrom 1965). After spring emergence,

     painted turtles become active at about 8 C but do not begin feeding until

     water temperatures reach approximately 150 C (Cagle 1950; Sexton 1959;

     Ernst 1972). In some years, monthly means of maximum water temperatures

     at approximately 1 m depth in East Marsh (ESGR) do not exceed 150 C until

     May. Mean minimum temperatures in some parts of East Marsh do not ex-

     ceed 15 C during the summer (fig. 1). Mean daily water temperatures at the

     bottom of East Marsh in 1987 increased from 170C in mid-May to 20'C at

     the end of June. The minimum and maximum water temperatures at approx-

     imately 0.5 m, recorded at Crane Pond (ESGR) between June 1 and August

     31, were 130C and 29 C in 1954 and 18'C and 31 0C in 1957 (Sexton 1959).

     These temperatures indicate that turtles can harvest food only during the

     months of May through September. And, during the summer months at high

     latitudes, they are restricted to the warmer portions of the aquatic habitat in

     order to maintain their body temperatures at levels higher than the mini-

     mum necessary to harvest and process food.

     Dietary Constraints on Growth

     Comparing the rate of allocation of energy to growth by juvenile turtles of all

     three species on the ESGR can indicate how each species views its resource

     environment. The three species attain maximum adult body sizes of 0.6 kg

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    9/19

     Constraints on Energetics of Reptiles 363

     EAST MARSH (MI)

     MEAN WATER TEMPERATURE

     M XMN

     30

     Cn 201

     w

     67891

    MONTHS (1981-1982)

     Fig. 1. Average minimum and maximum temperatures approximately 1 m

     deep in East Marsh taken every third day over 2 yr.

     (Chrysemyspicta), 1.7 kg (Emydoidea blandingi), and 13 kg (Chelydra ser-

     pentina). Juveniles of all three species occupy aquatic habitats on the ESGR;

     however, each species may occupy different microhabitats. Two of the spe-

     cies, C. picta and E. blandingi, are omnivores; C. serpentina is primarily a

     carnivore. The increase in body mass of juveniles of the omnivorous species

     followed similar trajectories (50 g yr-1), whereas C. serpentina increased in

     body mass approximately three times as rapidly (145 g yr-'; fig. 2). Growth

     rates of these juveniles suggest either that C. serpentina harvests more food

     or that the animal matter in its diet is of higher quality. The proportion of

     protein in the diet of T scripta dramatically influences growth rates of juve-

     niles. When fed food with 10% protein, they did not gain in body mass, and

     their plastrons curled. Juveniles fed a diet with either 25% or 40% protein

     grew at rates similar to those in the field (Avery 1987). Dietary protein con-

     tent is probably a major factor in diet selection and dietary shift from omni-

     vory toward herbivory that is associated with size or age of some emydid

     turtles (Clark and Gibbons 1969; Moll and Legler 1971; Parmenter 1981;

     Hart 1983).

     Allocation of Energy to Reproduction in Turtles

     In order to understand reproduction in turtles, it is necessary to know the

     total amount of energy allocated to each clutch of eggs, the amount allocated

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    10/19

     364 Justin D. Congdon

     EMYDOIDEA CHRYSEMYS CHELYDRA

     1800

     1600

     1400

    1200

     < 1000

     800

      6 0 0

    400 /

    200

     1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14

     A GE

     Fig. 2. Growth of three species of turtles on the E. S. George Reserve. Data

     are from hatching to sexual maturity.

     to each egg, the time over which the energy was harvested and allocated,

     and the relative contributions of stored versus directly harvested energy to

     each clutch.

     Follicle sizes of C. picta are smallest just after the nesting season, with

     substantial follicle enlargement taking place from late August through Octo-

     ber (Ernst 1971a, 1971 b; Congdon and Tinkle 1982). On average, the set of

     largest follicles found in females in October represented 50% of the energy

     of a complete clutch of eggs that would be laid during May and June of the

     following year. Energy allocated to follicle enlargement during summer and

     early fall was presumably obtained directly from harvested resources, be-

     cause stored lipids in females also increased during this period. The addi-

     tional 50% of the energy to complete follicle enlargement to ovulatory

     size was allocated between spring emergence in late March and mid-May

     when nesting began (Tinkle, Congdon, and Rosen 1981; Congdon and

     Tinkle 1982).

     The energy allocated to follicles during spring presumably came entirely

     from stored body lipids, because the decrease in lipid levels of females dur-

     ing the period was almost equivalent to the increase in lipids in follicles

     (Congdon and Tinkle 1982). In addition, examination of growth in C. picta

     from Michigan and Pennsylvania indicated that very little or no growth, and

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    11/19

     Constraints on Energetics of Reptiles 365

     presumably little or no feeding activity, was taking place before June (Sex-

     ton 1959; Ernst 1971c). At high latitudes, the temporal patterns of energy

     acquisition and allocation to growth and reproduction, coupled with the

     need to lay eggs early enough to allow adequate time for embryonic devel-

     opment, may be more limiting in some years than is absolute availability of

     energy. A similar pattern of follicle development during late summer has

     been observed in Sternotherus odoratus in Alabama, where follicles were

     fully developed (ovulatory size) between August and December, and ovula-

     tion of the first clutch occurred during late April of the following year (Mc-

     Pherson and Marion 1982).

     The minimum interval between the first and second clutches of C. picta

     in Michigan and T. scripta in South Carolina is approximately 12 d (J. W.

     Gibbons, unpublished data). This short interval indicates that (1) ovulation

     of a subsequent clutch of eggs can occur shortly after a clutch has been

     placed in a nest; (2) the follicles for the second clutch develop at the same

     time as those for the first clutch and, if necessary, complete vitellogenesis

     during the time the eggs for the first clutch are in the oviducts; and (3) the

     time between depositions of subsequent clutches is too short to allow a

     major portion of the energy for the second clutch to come from resources

     harvested during the interval.

     III. Energy Allocation and Life-History Theories

     In preparation for reproduction, a female must make three major determi-

     nations : (1) the total amount of energy available for present reproduction,

     (2) the quantity of energy to be allocated to each offspring, and (3) the

     number of individuals that can be produced by the present level of invest-

     ment in each offspring. The three determinations fall roughly into the con-

     ceptual categories of reproductive effort, parental investment, and optimal

     egg size.

     Reproductive Allocation: Theories and Morphological Constraint

     Some life-history theories deal with how an organism should apportion its

     finite resources among the competing compartments of maintenance,

     growth, storage, and reproduction. Central to these theories is the concept

     of reproductive effort, that portion of an animal's resource budget that is

     allocated to reproduction (Fisher 1930; Hirshfield and Tinkle 1975). How-

     ever, a number of proximate constraints may affect both the total amount of

     energy allocated to reproduction and the amount allocated to each individ-

     ual offspring of turtles.

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    12/19

     366 Justin D. Congdon

     All of the energy allocated to each individual turtle hatchling is contained

     within the egg. However, it has been argued that the eggs of turtles and

     other reptiles should be viewed as two distinct components in which energy

     is allocated by the female for (1) embryogenesis and (2) extended parental

     care in the form of yolk reserves that remain in the hatchling after it leaves

     the egg (Kraemer and Bennett 1981; Congdon and Tinkle 1982; Congdon,

     Tinkle, and Rosen 1983a; Congdon, Gibbons, and Greene 1983b; Troyer

     1983; Wilhoft 1986; Congdon and Gibbons 1987). Considering turtle eggs

     as a two-component system points out how the concepts of parental invest-

     ment and optimal egg size are tightly coupled in reptiles when compared

     to mammals or birds that provide extended parental care such as guarding

     and feeding of young.

     Trivers (1972) defined parental investment as any investment by the par-

     ent in an individual offspring that increases the offspring's chance of surviv-

     ing (and hence reproductive success) at the cost of the parent's ability to

     invest in other offspring. Under Trivers's definition, both categories of in-

     vestment, that used for embryogenesis and that used for fueling the devel-

     oped hatchling, fall under the overall category of PI. This definition is ade-

     quate only where it is assumed that all of the PI is used to make a larger

     offspring (salamanders [Nussbaum 1987]; insects [Parker and Begon 1986])

     rather than to provide it with posthatching reserves. By lumping together

     both categories of investment in an egg, Trivers's definition obscures impor-

     tant ecological and evolutionary questions about distinct processes that pro-

     ceed in different ways and at different times toward the common goal of

     making a successful offspring.

     Parental investment in reptiles should be separated into energy invested

     in making a complete embryo (PIE), and energy invested by the female for

     parental care (PIC)-that is, energy in excess of that needed to produce a

     complete hatchling. This excess may be in the form of a yolk sac or of hatch-

     ling fat bodies that are both used by the hatchling after it leaves the egg. The

     separation of energy allocated to each egg is based on the assumption that

     some of the energy allocated to an egg by the female is done expressly to

     fuel the hatchling. Since data on turtles and other reptiles indicate that the

     lipids or energy left in the hatchling yolk sac when it leaves the egg or is

     born usually exceeds 50% of the original lipids in the egg, this investment

     is far from trivial (Kraemer and Bennett 1981; Congdon and Tinkle 1982;

     Congdon et al. 1983a, 1983b; Troyer 1983; Stewart and Castillo 1984; Wil-

     hoft 1986; Congdon and Gibbons 1987). This assumption could be shown

     to be incorrect by demonstrating that hatchlings incubated under optimal

     conditions hatch with no residual yolk sac or formed fat bodies. In this case

     all of the material in the egg would be in the form of hatchling tissues or

     waste products. Since all of the PIC must be added to the contents of the

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    13/19

     Constraints on Energetics of Reptiles 367

     egg, it must, by definition, increase the size of the egg, but it appears to

     influence hatchling quality in ways not directly related to hatchling size.

     Interactions between determinants of size or quality of offspring and off-

     spring numbers have been considered for some time (Darwin 1859). More

     recent considerations have centered on the idea that organisms should in-

     vest in offspring at the level that maximizes the fitness of the parents (Lack

     1948, 1954, 1968; Svardson 1949; Williams 1966; Nussbaum 1985, 1987).

     There are currently two major categories of evolutionary theories (i.e., opti-

     mality or canalization theories and developmental plasticity theories) that

     attempt to explain the range of variation in egg size either within or among

     females. Morphological constraint on egg size has also been offered to ex-

     plain some of the variation in egg size of turtles (Congdon and Gib-

     bons 1987).

     Optimal egg-size (OES) models (Williams 1966; Smith and Fretwell 1974;

     Brockelman 1975; Parker and Begon 1986) attempt to model the relation-

     ships and interactions between egg size and number. The models make the

     following assumptions: (1) parents have a limited amount of resources or

     energy available for a given reproductive bout; (2) a minimum amount of

     energy is required to produce a viable offspring; and (3) the fitness of off-

     spring is not linearly related to the amount of parental investment, that is,

     there is a level at which a given investment in offspring results in large gains

     followed by a level of investment where minimal or no increase in the fitness

     of offspring occurs (also see Pianka 1974; Schaffer and Gadgil 1975). If as-

     sumption (1) is true, it follows that, as the amount of energy invested in

     individual offspring goes up, the number of individuals produced must be

     reduced. However, if some factor other than absolute energy availability lim-

     its allocation to reproduction (e.g., time constraints or morphological con-

     straints such as volume of a turtle's body cavity, size of the pelvic opening,

     or parental investment that does not primarily influence the size of the

     hatchling), then results inconsistent with predictions from OES models can

     be obtained.

     A major prediction from OES theory is that, within a population, the

     amount of variation in reproductive output among females should result pri-

     marily from variation in the number of offspring produced and secondarily

     from variation in egg size. One problem is that the actual level of variation

     in reproductive output owing to variation in egg size that is acceptable under

     OES models has not been defined in either relative or absolute terms. A

     second problem is that the relationships among egg size, hatchling size,

     and hatchling quality have not been established. The above discussion of

     parental investment in egg material points out that egg size may be related

     to hatchling quality through care rather than exclusively in hatchling size.

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    14/19

     368 Justin D. Congdon

     Therefore, OES models might be better termed optimal offspring-quality

     models.

     In contrast to OES theorists, other investigators have proposed that natural

     selection should favor developmental plasticity that results in a range of

     reproductive characteristics under conditions in which environmental vari-

     ability is unpredictable (Robertson 1971; Capinera 1979; Kaplan 1980; Coo-

     per and Kaplan 1982; Caswell 1983; Kaplan and Cooper 1984). Within these

     models, variation in egg size should occur within a single reproductive bout

     or among reproductive bouts within a single year (Kaplan and Cooper

     1984). It might be expected that there are life-history traits and environmen-

     tal conditions that could result in either strategy, and the existence support-

     ing data for one theory should not be considered as refuting the other.

     A study of three species of emydid turtles (Chrysemyspicta, Deirochelys

     reticularia, and Trachemys scripta) found that pelvic constraint on egg size

     among the species appeared to be related to body size (Congdon and Gib-

     bons 1987). In two smaller-bodied species (C. picta and D. reticularia),

     egg size increased from the smallest to the largest gravid females, and the

     slopes of the lines relating egg size and pelvic opening width to body size

     were essentially equal. In contrast, eggs of female T scripta only slightly

     increased with body size compared to the other two species, and the slope

     of the line relating pelvic width to body size was five times steeper than was

     the line for egg width. The constraint on egg size due to pelvic opening size

     in C. picta and D. reticularia apparently resulted in a situation that is not in

     accord with that predicted by OES; i.e., substantially more of the variation

     in reproductive output was due to variation in egg size than was found in T

     scripta, where pelvic constraint did not exist. Thus, the additional factor of

     morphological constraint apparently limits the way reproductive invest-

     ments can be partitioned in some turtles, and the degree of limitation is

     greater in smaller-bodied species.

     The preceding discussions have attempted to identify some obvious, and

     some less than obvious, ways that energy acquisition, processing, and allo-

     cation by reptiles can be influenced by extrinsic and intrinsic factors. Iden-

     tification of the way these factors limit the total resources available to or-

     ganisms and determine the timing of energy allocation is necessary for

     interpreting energy budgets and exploring the ways that energetics interacts

     with life-history and population processes.

     Acknowledgments

     This paper is dedicated to Richard M. Wiltse, foreman of the University of

     Michigan's E. S. George Reserve maintenance crew, who died in May 1988.

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    15/19

     Constraints on Energetics of Reptiles 369

     He was a friend and an enthusiastic supporter of the long-term turtle re-

     search program on the George Reserve. Earlier drafts of this manuscript

     were improved by comments from Roger Anderson, William Cooper, Gary

     Hepp, Gary Meffe, J. Whitfield Gibbons, David Scott, and Laurie J. Vitt. Fund-

     ing for this study was provided by NSF grants DEB-79-06301 and BSR-84-

     00861 and contract DE-AC09-76SR00-819 between the University of Georgia

     and the United States Department of Energy.

     Literature Cited

     ANDERSON, R. A. 1986. Foraging behavior, energetics of reproduction and sexual se-

     lection in a widely foraging lizard, Cnemidophorus tigris. Ph.D. diss. University of

     California, Los Angeles.

     ANDERSON, R. A., and W. H. KARASOV. 1988. Energetics of the lizard Cnemidophorus

     tigris and life history consequences of food-acquisition mode. Ecol. Monogr. 58:

     79-110.

     AUTH, D. L. 1975. Behavioral ecology of basking in the yellow-bellied turtle, Chry-

     semys scripta scripta (Schoepff). Bull. Florida State Mus. Biol. Sci. 20:1-45.

     AVERY, H. W. 1987. Roles of diet protein and temperature in the nutritional energetics

     of juvenile slider turtles, Trachemys scripta. M.S. thesis. State University College

     at Buffalo, Buffalo, N.Y.

     AVERY, R. A. 1975. Clutch size and reproductive effort in the lizard Lacerta vivipara

     Jacquin. Oecologia (Berl.) 19:165-170.

     BAKKEN, G. S., and D. M. GATES. 1975. Heat transfer analysis of animals: some implica-

     tions for field ecology, physiology, and evolution. Pages 255-290 in D. M. GATES

     and R. B. SCHMERL, eds. Perspectives of biophysical ecology. Springer-Verlag, New

     York.

     BALLINGER, R. E., and J. D. CONGDON. 1980. Food resource limitation of body growth

     rates in Sceloporus scalaris (Sauria, Iguanidae). Copeia 1980:921-923.

     BOYER, D. R. 1965. Ecology of the basking habit of turtles. Ecology 46:99-118.

     BRATTSTROM, B. H. 1965. Body temperatures of reptiles. Am. Midl. Nat. 73:376-422.

     BROCKELMAN, W. Y. 1975. Competition, the fitness of offspring, and optimal clutch

     size. Am. Nat. 109:677-699.

     CAGLE, F. R. 1950. The life history of the slider turtle, Pseudemys scripta troostii

     (Holbrook). Ecol. Monogr. 20:31-54.

     CAPINERA, J. L. 1979. Qualitative variation in plants and insects: effect of propagule

     size on ecological plasticity. Am. Nat. 114:350-361.

     CASWELL, H. 1983. Phenotypic plasticity in life-history traits: demographic effects and

     evolutionary consequences. Am. Zool. 23:35-46.

     CLARK, D. B., and J. W. GIBBONS. 1969. Dietary shift in the turtle Pseudemys scripta

     (Schoepff) from youth to maturity. Copeia 1969:704-706.

     CONGDON, J. D. 1977. Energetics of the montane lizard Sceloporusjarrovi: a mea-

     surement of reproductive effort. Ph.D. diss. Arizona State University, Tempe.

     CONGDON, J. D., A. E. DUNHAM, and D. W. TINKLE. 1982. Energy budgets and life

     histories of reptiles. Pages 233-271 in C. GANS, ed. Biology of the Reptilia. Vol.

     13. Academic Press, New York.

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    16/19

     370 Justin D. Congdon

     CONGDON, J. D., and J. W. GIBBONs. 1987. Morphological constraint on egg size: a

     challenge to optimal egg size theory? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 84:4145-4147.

     . 1989. Biomass productivity of turtles in freshwater wetlands: a geographic

     comparison. Savannah River Ecology Laboratory Symposium. Freshwater Wetlands

     (in press).

     CONGDON, J. D., J. W. GIBBONS, and J. L. GREENE. 1983b. Parental investment in the

     chicken turtle (Deirochelys reticularia). Ecology 64:419-425.

     CONGDON, J. D., and D. W. TINKLE. 1982. Reproductive energetics of the painted

     turtle (Chrysemyspicta). Herpetologica 38:228-237.

     CONGDON, J. D., D. W. TINKLE, and P. C. ROSEN. 1983a. Egg components and utiliza-

     tion during development in aquatic turtles. Copeia 1983:264-268.

     COOPER, W. S., and R. H. KAPLAN. 1982. Adaptive coin-flipping : a decision-theoretic

     examination of natural selection for random individual variation. J. Theor. Biol.

     94:135-151.

     CRAWFORD, K. M., J. R. SPOTILA, and E. A. STANDORA. 1983. Operative environmental

     temperatures and basking behavior of the turtle Pseudemys scripta. Ecology 64:

     989-999.

     CUNNINGHAM, G. L., J. P. SYVERTSEN, J. F. REYNOLDS, and J. M. WILLSON. 1979. Some

     effects of soil-moisture availability on above-ground production and reproductive

     allocation in Larrea tridentata (DC) Cov. Oecologia (Berl.) 40:113-123.

     DARWIN, C. R. 1859. On the origin of species by means of natural selection; or, the

     preservation of favoured traces in the struggle for life. Murray, London.

     DUNHAM, A. E. 1978. Food availability as a proximate factor influencing individual

     growth rates in the iguanid lizard Sceloporus merriami. Ecology 59:770-778.

     1981. Populations in a fluctuating environment: the comparative population

     ecology of the iguanid lizards Sceloporus merriami and Urosaurus ornatus. Misc.

     Pub. Mus. Zool., Univ. Mich. 158:1-62.

     ERNST, C. H. 1971a. Population dynamics and activity cycles of C. picta in SE Penn.

     J. Herpetol. 5:151-160.

     1971b. Sexual cycles and maturity of the turtle Chrysemyspicta. Biol. Bull.

     140:191-200.

     1971 c. Growth of the painted turtle, Chrysemyspicta, in SE Penn.J. Herpetol.

     5:151-160.

     1972. Temperature-activity relationship in the painted turtle, Chrysemys

     picta. Copeia 1972:217-222.

     EVANARI, M., E. D. SHULTZ, O. L. LANGE, L. KAPPEN, and U. BUSCHBOM. 1976. Plant

     production in arid and semi-arid areas. Pages 439-451 in O. L. LANGE, L. KAPPEN,

     and E. D. SHULZE, eds. Ecological studies: analysis and synthesis. Vol. 19. Springer-

     Verlag, New York.

     FISHER, R. A. 1930. The genetical theory of natural selection. Clarendon, Oxford.

     GATTEN, R. E. 1974. Effect of nutritional status on the preferred body temperature of

     the turtles Pseudemys scripta and Terrapene ornata. Copeia 1974:912-917.

     GIBBONS, J. W. 1970. The influence of terrestrial activities on the population dynam-

     ics of aquatic turtles. Am. Midl. Nat. 83:404-415.

     GRANT, B. W., and A. E. DUNHAM. 1987. Thermally imposed time constraints on the

     activity of the desert lizard Sceloporus merriami. Ecology 69:167-176.

     HADLEY, N. F., and S. R. SZAREK. 1981. Productivity of desert ecosystems. Biol. Sci.

     31:747-753.

     HAMMOND, K. A., J. R. SPOTILA, and E. A. STANDORA. 1988. Basking behavior of the

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    17/19

     Constraints on Energetics of Reptiles 371

     turtle Pseudemys scripta: effects of digestive state, acclimation temperature, sex,

     and season. Physiol. Zool. 61:69-77.

     HART, D. R. 1983. Dietary and habitat shift with size of red-eared turtles (Pseudemys

     scripta) in a southern Louisiana population. Herpetologica 39:285-290.

     HIRSHFIELD, M. F., and D. W. TINKLE. 1975. Natural selection and the evolution of

     reproductive effort. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 72:2227-2231.

     HUEY, R. B., and M. SLATKIN. 1976. Costs and benefits of lizard thermoregulation. Q.

     Rev. Biol. 51:363-384.

     KAPLAN, R. H. 1980. The implication of ovum size variability for offspring fitness and

     clutch size in several populations of salamanders (Ambystoma). Evolution 34:51-

     6 4 .

     KAPLAN, R. H., and W. S. COOPER. 1984. The evolution of developmental plasticity

     in the reproductive characteristics: an application of the adaptive coin-flipping

    principle. Am. Nat. 123:393-410.

     KARASOV, W. H., and R. A. ANDERSON. 1984. Interhabitat differences in energy acquisi-

     tion and expenditure in a lizard. Ecology 65:235-247.

     KENYON, W. A. 1925. Digestive enzymes in poikilothermal vertebrates: an investiga-

     tion of enzymes in fishes with comparative studies of those of amphibians, reptiles

     and mammals. U.S. Natl. Mar. Fisheries Ser. Bull. 41:197-200.

     KEPENIS, V., and J. J. MCMANUS. 1974. Bioenergetics of young painted turtles, Chry-

     semyspicta. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 48:309-317.

     KRAEMER, J. E., and S. H. BENNETT. 1981. Utilization of post-hatching yolk in logger-

     head sea turtles, Caretta caretta. Copeia 1981:406-411.

     LACK, D. 1948. The significance of clutch size. Ibis 89:302-352; 90:25-45.

     --. 1954. The natural regulation of animal numbers. Clarendon, Oxford.

     --. 1968. Ecological adaptations for breeding in birds. Methem, London.

     LIGHTFOOT, D. C., and W. G. WHITFORD. 1987. Variation in insect densities on creo-

     sotebush: is nitrogen a factor? Ecology 68:547-557.

     MCPHERSON, R. J., and K. R. MARION. 1982. Seasonal changes of total lipids in the

     turtle Sternotherus odoratus. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 71A:93-98.

     MOLL, E. 0., and J. M. LEGLER. 1971. The life history of a neotropical pond slider

     turtle, Pseudemys scripta (Schoepff), in Panama. Bull. Los Angeles County Mus.

     Nat. Hist. Sci. 11:1-102.

     NAGY, K. A. 1982. Ecological energetics of a lizard. Pages 24-54 in R. HUEY, T.

     SCHOENER, and E. PIANKA, eds. Lizard ecology: studies of a model organism. Har-

     vard University Press, Cambridge.

     NAGY, K. A., and P. A. MEDICA. 1986. Physiological ecology of desert tortoises in

     southern Nevada. Herpetologica 42:73-92.

     NAGY, K. A., and V. H. SHOEMAKER. 1975. Energy and nitrogen budgets of the free-

     living desert lizard Sauromalus obesus. Physiol. Zool. 48:252-262.

     NAGY, K. A., V. H. SHOEMAKER, and W. R. COSTA. 1976. Water, electrolyte, and nitrogen

     budgets of jackrabbits (Lepus californicus) in the Mojave Desert. Physiol. Zool.

     49:351-363.

     NEILL, W. T., and E. R. ALLEN. 1954. Algae on turtles: some additional considerations.

     Ecology 35:581-584.

     NoY-MEIR, I. 1973. Desert ecosystems: environment and producers. Annu. Rev. Ecol.

     Syst. 4:25-51.

     . 1974. Desert ecosystems: higher trophic levels. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 5:195-

     214.

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    18/19

     372 Justin D. Congdon

     NUSSBAUM, R. A. 1985. The evolution of parental care in salamanders. Misc. Publ.

     Mus. Zool. Univ. Mich. 169:1-50.

     . 1987. Parental care and egg size in salamanders: an examination of the safe

     harbor hypothesis. Res. Pop. Ecol. 29:27-44.

     PARKER, G. A., and M. BEGON. 1986. Optimal egg size and clutch size: effects on

     environment and maternal phenotype. Am. Nat. 128:573-592.

     PARMENTER, R. P. 1981. Digestive turnover rates in freshwater turtles: the influence

     of temperature and body size. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 70:235-238.

     PIANKA, E. R. 1974. Evolutionary ecology. Harper & Row, New York.

     PRITCHARD, P. C. H., and W. F. GREENHOOD. 1968. The sun and the turtle. Int. Turtle

     Tortoise Soc. J. 2:20-25.

     RICKLEFS, R. E. 1979. Ecology. 2d ed. Chiron, New York.

     ROBERTSON, R. 1971. Lepidoptera genetics. Pergamon, Oxford.

     RUBY, D. E. 1981. Phenotypic correlates of male reproductive success in the lizard

     Sceloporusjarrovi. Pages 96-107 in R. D. ALEXANDER and D. W. TINKLE, eds. Natu-

     ral selection and social behavior. Chiron, New York.

     RUBY, D. E., and A. E. DUNHAM. 1984. A population analysis of the ovoviviparous

     lizard Sceloporusjarrovi in the Pinaleno Mountains of southeastern Arizona. Her-

     petologica 40:425-436.

     SCHAFFER, W. M., and M. D. GADGIL. 1975. Selection for optimal life histories in

     plants. Pages 142-157 in M. L. CODY and J. M. DIAMOND, eds. Ecology and evolu-

     tion of communities. Belknap, Cambridge, Mass.

     SCHWARZKOPF, L., and R. J. BROOKS. 1985. Application of operative environmental

     temperatures to analysis of basking behavior in Chrysemys picta. Herpetologica

     41:206-212.

     SEXTON, O. J. 1959. Spatial and temporal movements of a population of the painted

     turtle, Chrysemyspicta marginata (Agassiz). Ecol. Monogr. 29:113-140.

     SHREVE, F. 1925. Ecological aspects of the deserts of California. Ecology 6:93-103.

     - . 1942. The vegetation of Arizona. In T. H. KEARNEY and R. H. PEEBLES, eds.

     Flowering plants and ferns of Arizona. U.S. Dept. Agric. Misc. Publ.

     SMITH, C. C., and S. D. FRETWELL. 1974. The optimal balance between size and num-

     ber of offspring. Am. Nat. 108:499-506.

     STANDORA, E. A. 1982. A telemetric study of the thermoregulatory behavior and cli-

     mate-space of free-ranging yellow-bellied turtles, Pseudemys scripta. Ph.D. diss.

     University of Georgia.

     STEWART, J. R., and R. E. CASTILLO. 1984. Nutritional provision of the yolk of two

     species of viviparous reptiles. Physiol. Zool. 57:377-383.

     SVARDSON, G. 1949. Natural selection and egg number in fish. Rep. Inst. Freshwater

     Res. Drottningholm 29:115-122.

     SZAREK, S. R. 1979. Primary production in four North American deserts: indices of

     efficiency. J. Arid Environ. 2:187-209.

     TINKLE, D. W.,J. D. CONGDON, and P. R. ROSEN. 1981. Nesting frequency and success:

     implications for the demography of painted turtles. Ecology 62:1426-1432.

     TRIVERS, R. L. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. Pages 136-179 in

     B. G. CAMBELL, ed. Sexual selection and descent of Man. Aldine, Chicago.

     TROYER, K. 1983. Posthatchling yolk energy in a lizard: utilization pattern and inter-

     clutch variation. Oecologica 58:340-344.

     TURNER, F. B. 1973. Rock Valley Validation Site Report. US/IBP Desert Biome Re-

     search Memorandum. 73-2. Utah State University, Logan.

    This content downloaded from 177.230.235.179 on Sat, 09 Apr 2016 04:08:26 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/18/2019 30156175

    19/19

     Constraints on Energetics of Reptiles 373

     1974. Rock Valley Validation Site Report. US/IBP Desert Biome Research

     Memorandum. 73-3. Utah State University, Logan.

     TURNER, F. B., P. A. MEDICA, and B. W. KOWALEWSKY. 1976. Energy utilization by a

     desert lizard (Uta stansburiana). US/IBP Desert Biome Monogr. no. 1. Utah State

     University Press, Logan.

     WEBB, W., S. SZAREK, W. LAUENROTH, R. KINERSON, and M. SMITH. 1978. Primary pro-

     ductivity and water use in native forests, grasslands, and desert ecosystems. Ecol-

     ogy 59:1239-1247.

     WHITTOW, G. C., and G. H. BALAZS. 1982. Basking behavior of the Hawaiian green

     turtle (Chelonia mydas). Pac. Sci. 36:129-139.

     WIEGERT, R. G. 1968. Thermodynamic consideration in animal nutrition. Am. Zool.

     8:71-81.

     WILBUR, H. M., D. W. TINKLE, and J. P. COLLINS. 1974. Environmental certainty, tro-

     phic level, and resource availability in life history evolution. Am. Nat. 108:805-

     817.

     WILHOFT, D. C. 1986. Eggs and hatchling components of the snapping turtle Chely-

     dra serpentina. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 84A:483-486.

     WILLIAMS, G. C. 1966. Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton University Press,

     Princeton, N.J. 307 pp.