wolk a eb memory sys 2010

Upload: silvia-alvarado

Post on 02-Jun-2018

231 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    1/14

    MEMORY SYSTEMSDavid A. Wolk, Andrew E. Budson

    ABSTRACT

    Converging evidence from patient and neuroimaging studies suggests that memory isa collection of abilities that use different neuroanatomic systems. Neurologic injurymay impair one or more of these memory systems. Episodic memory allows us tomentally travel back in time and relive an episode of our life. Episodic memorydepends on the hippocampus, other medial temporal lobe structures, the limbicsystem, and the frontal lobes, as well as several other brain regions. Semantic memoryprovides our general knowledge about the world and is unconnected to any specificepisode of our life. Although semantic memory likely involves much of the neocortex,the inferolateral temporal lobes (particularly the left) are most important. Proceduralmemory enables us to learn cognitive and behavioral skills and algorithms thatoperate at an automatic, unconscious level. Damage to the basal ganglia, cerebellum,and supplementary motor area often impair procedural memory.

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4):1528.

    INTRODUCTION

    The ability to remember ones personalpast is a fundamental feature that definesthe human conscious experience. Dis-orders of memory can have devastating

    consequences for patients and families.Although numerous medical conditionsmay precipitate memory loss, the agingof our population, with its accordantincreased prevalence of Alzheimer dis-ease (AD) and other neurodegenerativeconditions, has accentuated the needfor the clinician to have a general un-derstanding of normal memory func-tioning and the differential diagnosisassociated with its disruption.

    Critical to any discussion of memoryis the concept of a memory system,which can be loosely defined as a brainsystem that supports the maintenanceof information that impacts behaviorafter passage of time from initial ac-quisition.1Although one of the earliest

    assertions that human memory is sub-served by several different memory sys-

    tems was by the 18th century Frenchphilosopher Maine de Biran, the charac-terization of the famous patient Henry

    Molaison (HM; 19262008) pro-vided the experimental support to pop-ularize this conception. HM underwentbilateral medial temporal lobe (MTL)

    resections for intractable epilepsy in1953. Unfortunately, the procedure re-sulted in the unintended consequenceof profound amnesia in which he wasunable to acquire new memories of ex-periences or events following the sur-gery. The description of this outcome

    firmly established the MTL system asessential to episodic memory and wasdisseminated to the scientific commu-nity in the highly influential 1957 manu-script by Milner and Scoville entitledLoss of Recent Memory after BilateralHippocampal Lesions.2

    15

    Relationship Disclosure: Dr Wolk has received personal compensation for consulting activities with GEHealthcare, Inc.; and Avacat Consulting, LLC. Dr Budson has received personal compensation for speakingengagements with Eisai Inc., Forest Laboratories, Inc., Johnson & Johnson Services, Inc., and Pfizer Inc.Dr Budsons compensation and/or research work has been funded entirely or in part by a grant from agovernmental organization to his university.Unlabeled Use of Products/Investigational Use Disclosure:Drs Wolk and Budson have nothing to disclose.

    Copyright # 2010, American Academy of Neurology. All rights reserved.

    KEY POINT

    A When medial

    temporal lobe

    structures are

    damaged andepisodic

    memory is

    impaired,

    learning can

    still occur

    through other

    memory

    systems such as

    procedural

    memory.

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    2/14

    It was the sparing of other aspectsof HMs learning and memory, how-

    ever, that provided the foundation for

    the notion of separable memory sys-tems. For example, his ability to learnnew motor skills, demonstrate the ef-

    fects of perceptual priming, and re-

    trieve remote pieces of semantic mem-ory suggested that these processes

    were not entirely dependent on MTLfunction. Additional dissociations re-

    vealed in HM and other patients provideevidence that there are separable mem-

    ory systems.Although there is not complete agree-

    ment on the best way to categorize thesesystems, almost all accounts involve sep-aration into declarative and nondeclar-ative forms of memory. Declarativemem-ories can be put into words and generally

    involve explicit accessor consciousaware-ness of information, whereas nondeclar-ative memories cannot be verbalized and

    are instead manifested by changes inbehavior. In the current review, we willdiscuss two forms of declarative memoryepisodic and semanticand one formof nondeclarative memoryprocedural.

    Working memory, another form of de-clarative memory, is covered elsewherein this issue, although we have includedit in selected tables and figures for com-parison (Table 1-1). As outlined below,one of the major values of consideringmemory in this manner is that these

    systems rely on a dissociable neuroanat-omy, which has variable sensitivity todifferent disease processes and, thus,has localizing and diagnostic implica-tions in the context of impairment.

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    16

    TABLE 1-1 Comparison of Clinically Relevant Memory Systems

    MemorySystem Examples Awareness

    Length ofStorage

    Major AnatomicStructures

    Episodicmemory

    Remembering a short story,what you had for dinnerlast night, and what youdid on your last birthday

    ExplicitDeclarative

    Minutesto years

    Medial temporal lobe,anterior thalamic nucleus,mamillary body, fornix,prefrontal cortex

    Semanticmemory

    Knowing who was thefirst US president, the colorof a lion, and how a forkand comb are different

    ExplicitDeclarative

    Minutes toyears

    Inferior lateral temporallobes

    Proceduralmemory

    Driving a standardtransmission car andlearning the sequence of

    numbers on a touch-tonephone without trying

    ImplicitNondeclarative

    Minutes toyears

    Basal ganglia, cerebellum,supplementary motorarea

    Workingmemory

    Phonologic: keeping a phonenumber in your headbefore dialing

    ExplicitDeclarative

    Seconds tominutes;informationactivelyrehearsed ormanipulated

    Phonologic: prefrontalcortex, Broca area,Wernike area

    Spatial: Mentally following aroute, or rotating an object inyour mind

    Spatial: prefrontal cortex,visual association areas

    Reprinted from Budson AE, Price BH. Memory dysfunction. N Engl J Med 2005;352(7):629699. Copyright # 2005, with permission fromMassachusetts Medical Society. All rights reserved.

    KEY POINT

    A Memory

    impairment may

    be seen even if

    the medialtemporal lobes

    are spared.

    Frontal lobes,

    inferolateral

    temporal lobes,

    basal ganglia,

    and cerebellum

    may all cause

    certain kinds of

    memory

    impairment.

    "MEMORY SYSTEMS

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    3/14

    EPISODIC MEMORY

    Definitions

    The patient in Case 1-1 had a rela-

    tively selective impairment of episodicmemory.

    Episodic memory is the memory

    system that allows us to remember past

    experiences and episodes in our life, or

    as Endel Tulving put it, the kind of mem-

    ory that allows us to mentally travel

    in time.3 Two features differentiate epi-

    sodic memory from other memory sys-

    tems. (1) Episodic memory involves a

    form of consciousness in which theselfis

    centralautonoetic or self-knowing con-sciousness according to Tulving. In other

    words, it involves remembering ones

    own experience of an event. (2) Only

    episodic memory is tightly linked to a

    sense of time. Indeed, time forms part

    of the context by which these events arerepresented, allowing for differentiationof events in the recent past from moreremote events.

    Episodic memories may be fraction-ated in a number of different ways,

    which often have implications for thenature of an impairment of memoryand the underlying neural substrateinvolved. One such division is the dif-ference between item and associativememory. Whereas item memories are

    for individual items without context, as-sociative memories involve the linkingof multiple aspects of an event. For

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    17

    Case 1-1A 75-year-old man had decline in his memory over about 1 year. Per his wife, this was manifestedby his repeating questions and forgetting their daily plans. She noted little change in his abilityto perform instrumental activities of daily living, such as driving or handling the finances, buthe did have greater difficulty withremembering details of books or showsthat they had watched together. He

    admitted that his memory was poorerand felt a sense of foreboding about thefuture. On examination, he showed verypoor verbal and visual memory, andlimited knowledge of current eventsdespite avidly watching the news.Although he recalled 6/10 words on thethird immediate recall trial of a verbalmemory task, his delayed recall was 0/10,and he only recognized 4/10 items andmade one false alarm. His retention of astory based on initial encoding was very

    poor. Nonetheless, he performed inthe normal range on almost all testsof language, executive functioning,attention, and visuospatial ability. Hewas given a diagnosis of amnestic mildcognitive impairment. Note thediminutive hippocampi on his MRI(Figure 1-1).

    Comment. This patient has an impairment of episodic memory. A relatively isolatedimpairment of episodic memory is a common feature of early AD given the early neuropathologyin the MTLs with this condition. This patient has a high likelihood of progressing from amnesticmild cognitive impairment to clinical AD.

    FIGURE 1-1 MRI coronal T1-weighted image. Notethe relatively disproportionate atrophyof the bilateral hippocampi consistentwith the temporolimbic memory impairmentof this patient.

    A Episodic memory

    is the type of

    memory we

    usually mean

    when we talk

    about memory.

    It is memory for

    an episode of

    ones life.

    KEY POINTS

    A Episodic memory

    impairments

    are common

    and oftendisrupt the lives

    of patients and

    their families.

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    4/14

    example, one may remember havingseen a friend earlier in the day and alsothe color of the friends shirt and the

    location of the meeting. A commontest of associative memory is to havesubjects study word pairs. At testing,the subject is shown one word in thepair and is asked to recall the second,associated word. A related concept issource memory, which is the ability toremember the specific context from

    which a memory came. A commonmemory error is related to this notion,sometimes referred to as reality mon-itoring. An example is when you are

    unable to remember whether youactually turned off the stove or justthought about turning it off. Sourcememory is frequently tested in thelaboratory by having subjects study twolists. At testing, they need to decide notonly whether a particular item wasstudied, but remember from which listit came.

    A related formulation to the item

    versus associative or source memory

    distinctions is the difference between

    familiarity and recollection,4 a differ-ence that may reflect dissociable under-

    lying medial temporal and neocortical

    structures. Familiarity is conceptualized

    as an acontextual sense of prior en-

    counter. An example of an experience of

    familiarity is when people see someone

    that they are sure they have previously

    met but cannot recall how it is that they

    know that person (That person is so

    familiar to me! Where do I know him

    from?). In contrast, recollection is themore detailed retrieval of information

    (Oh, thats Bob. I met him at my sis-

    ters birthday party last week). Al-

    though sometimes recollection occurs

    spontaneously, at other times addi-

    tional conscious, effortful searching of

    ones memory stores is needed.A final important distinction is be-

    tween retrograde and anterograde am-nesia. Relative to the time of the braininjury, anterograde amnesia is the in-

    ability to form new memories, whereasretrograde amnesia is the loss of pre-

    viously acquired memories.

    Functional Neuroanatomy ofEpisodic Memory

    The MTLand particularly thehippocampusis traditionally thoughtto be the anatomic seat of episodicmemory, as exemplified by the severeamnesia of HM; however, a numberof other neural systems appear to beinvolved. The processes that supportepisodic memory occur from the timethe to-be-remembered event is encoun-

    tered (encoding) to the act of remem-bering (retrieval). In between are pro-cesses involved in the maintenance ofthese memories. If the memory is tolast for an extended period of time, anadditional process known asconsolida-tionoccurs. Given the disparate natureof these operations, it is perhaps notsurprising that episodic memory re-quires diverse neural systems for itsproper function and, thus, a variety ofbrain injuries can result in impairedmemory. Historically, it has been diffi-cult to gain traction on the nature ofneural activity associated with thesedifferent stages of memory. The adventof functional neuroimaging techniqueshas allowed for assessment of neuralactivity during memory encoding andretrieval, which has added greatly toour understanding of these processes(Figure 1-2). We will outline a num-ber of critical brain regions associated

    with episodic memory function.Medial temporal lobe. Much ofwhat we know about normal episodicmemory function comes from studyingpatients with amnesia resulting fromMTL lesions. The MTL is a complex struc-ture frequently divided into hippocam-pal and extrahippocampal regions.5 The

    hippocampalstructures include the den-tate gyrus, cornus ammonis subfields(CA 1, CA2, and CA3), and the postsub-iculum. Extrahippocampal structures

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    18

    KEY POINT

    A The hippocampus

    and other

    medial temporal

    lobe structuresare critical for

    normal episodic

    memory function.

    "MEMORY SYSTEMS

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    5/14

    include the entorhinal, perirhinal, andparahippocampal cortices.

    Whereas isolated hippocampal le-sions produce significant amnesia, inclu-sion of surrounding extrahippocampalMTL structures tends to produce evenmore profound memory loss.6 Some

    work has suggested that there may be adivision of labor within the MTLs withregard to the nature of their memorystores.68 Hippocampal lesions tend toproduce impairment on tasks of asso-ciative memory, source memory, and re-

    collection, with relative sparing of itemmemory and familiarity. However, theadditional involvement of extrahippo-campal MTL regions impairs item mem-ory as well. These and other findings sug-gest that the hippocampus is involvedin the binding of different elements ofa prior study episode. By contrast, theperirhinal cortex and parahippocampusappear critical for the storage of theseindividual elements.An influential modelis that the perirhinal cortex and para-

    hippocampus differentially encode ob-ject and spatial elements of an episode,respectively, which are then bound bythe hippocampus (Figure 1-3).8

    In addition to significant anterogradeamnesia, patients with MTL injury fre-quently experience retrograde amnesia.Often times, the retrograde amnesia isgreatest for events learned nearest thetime of MTL injury but is spared formore remote episodes. This somewhatparadoxical pattern of memory loss hasbeen labeled as Ribot law9 and may

    reflect the changing representation ofmemories over time. One popular ac-count to explain this phenomenon isthe standard consolidation model.10,11

    This model argues that when memoriesare initially formed, the MTLlikely, thehippocampusbinds neocortically rep-

    resented features of an event. Partialcues that reactivate elements of the epi-sode will also activate related featuresmediated by connections with the hip-pocampus. However, over time, these

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    19

    KEY POINT

    A An injury to

    episodic

    memory

    typically causes(1) anterograde

    amnesia: an

    inability to

    form new

    memories, and

    (2) retrograde

    amnesia: a loss

    of previous

    memories.

    There are,

    however,

    always (3) some

    preserved

    remote

    memories.

    FIGURE 1-2 Episodic memory. The medial temporal lobes, including the hippocampus andparahippocampus, form the core of the episodic memory system.

    Adapted from Budson AE, Price BH. Memory dysfunction. N Engl J Med 2005;352(7):692699.Copyright # 2005, with permission from Massachusetts Medical Society. All rights reserved.

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    6/14

    neocortical representations form theirown associations and the critical roleof the MTL in retrieval may become di-minished or absent, and, thus, an oldermemory may not be affected by MTLpathology.

    A number of conditions produce

    memory impairment through MTL in-jury. These conditions include AD, de-mentia with Lewy bodies, hippocampalsclerosis, posterior cerebral artery stroke,hypoxic-ischemic injury, and viral and

    limbic encephalitis. By far the mostcommon cause of memory loss inolder adults is AD, which is associated

    with extensive neuropathology in theMTL. In particular, early neurofibrillarytangle pathology is found in the ento-rhinal cortex layer II neurons, which

    form the perforant pathway (the maininput to the hippocampus). Such pa-thology appears to result in a cortico-

    hippocampal disconnection,

    12

    perhapspreventing the binding of different neo-cortical elements necessary for effec-tive encoding. Thus, it is not surprisingthat memory loss is an early feature ofthis condition (Case 1-1). As AD prog-resses, all regions of the MTL becomesignificantly involved, further devastat-ing the episodic memory system. Otherfactors that likely contribute to the mem-ory loss include reduced cholinergic in-put due to basal forebrain pathology and

    involvement of frontal-subcortical net-works. It has also recently become ap-parent that -amyloid (A), the proteinfragment that forms the hallmark amy-loid plaques of AD, in its soluble formmay inhibit long-term potentiation, acritical cellular mechanism for learningand memory.13

    Extended medial temporal mem-

    ory system. Several structures with sig-nificant connectivity to the MTL properare critical to episodic memory, as evi-denced by the amnesia associated withtheir injury. Many of these regions werepreviously described by Papez in thecircuit that bears his name.14 Lesions tothe mamillary bodies, bilateral fornices(an efferent pathway from the hippo-campus to the mamillary bodies), andthe anterior thalamic nucleus (whichreceives inputs via the mamillothalamictrack from the mamillary bodies) allproduce episodic memory impairment

    thatis difficult to distinguish fromhippo-campally based amnesia (Figure 1-2).6

    Additionally, the posterior cingulateand retrosplenial cortex also have denseconnections with the hippocampus andanterior thalamic nucleus, and amnesiaassociated with retrosplenial lesionshave been described.15 Although iso-

    lated lesions in these related structuresare uncommon, Korsakoff syndrome rep-resents a classic form of amnesia associ-ated with pathology in the anterior

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    20

    FIGURE 1-3 Functional organization of the medialtemporal lobe system. Neocortical inputregarding the object features (what)

    converges in the perirhinal cortex (PRC) and lateral entorhinalarea (LEA), whereas details about the location (where) ofobjects converge in the parahippocampal cortex (PHC) andmedial entorhinal area (MEA). These streams converge inthe hippocampus, which represents items in the context inwhich they were experienced. Reverse projections followthe same pathways back to the parahippocampal andneocortical regions. Back projections to the PHC-MEA maysupport recall or context, whereas back projections to thePRC-LEA may support recall of item associations.

    Adapted with permission from Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C.The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci2007;30:123152.

    "MEMORY SYSTEMS

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    7/14

    thalamic nucleus and the mamillarybodies.16

    Frontal lobes.While their deficit is

    generally more subtle than that seenwith medial temporal lesions, patientswith frontal lobe damage frequentlyexhibit episodic memory impairment.These patients tend to have relativelyselective impairment on memory tasksthat require the linking of different fea-tures of a prior episode. As such, pa-tients with frontal lobe damage tendto have difficulty on tests of associa-tive or source memory,17 memory fortemporal order of presentation,18 and

    recollection.4

    However, performance isoften normal on tests of item memoryor familiarity. Patients with frontal lobedamage also tend to have more dif-ficulty on memory tasks that requirespontaneously generated encoding orretrieval strategies, but show improve-ment in the context of environmentalsupport. For example, patients withfrontal lobe lesions tend to performmore poorly on a memory test if told tosimply study a list of words rather than

    when instructed to perform a specificencoding operation, such as to form amental image of each study item. Thisnotion also applies to different test for-mats, as an unconstrained free recall taskis much more difficult for patients withfrontal lobe lesions than cued recall orrecognition memory.19 On these lattertasks, these patients may show little orno deficit at all. Thus, as a general prin-ciple, prefrontal regions appear to be

    involved in different aspects of cognitivecontrol mechanisms that enhance mem-ory encoding and retrieval20 rather thansupporting the retention of information(likely a medial temporal function).

    In addition to failure to retrieve in-formation from prior events, patients

    with frontal lobe lesions are particularly

    susceptible to memory distortions andfalse memories. This is likely related totheir poor memory for contextual orassociative details of a prior episode.21

    These patients may conflate details ofvarious events and are susceptible toerrors of reality monitoring reflective of

    source memory confusions. In more ex-treme cases, patients may exhibit spon-taneousand sometimes elaborative con-fabulations. Potential contributors to thetendency to confabulate include com-bined MTL and frontal lobe dysfunction(such as is seen in AD, frontotemporaldegeneration, and Korsakoff amnesia),poor selection or focus of the to-be-retrieved memories, impaired monitor-ing and editing of retrieved information,and impaired source monitoring and re-

    trieval of temporal contextual details.22

    Differences in memory performancebetween conditions that affect the MTLand those that affect the frontal lobescan be conceptualized by analogy. Theepisodic memory system can be thoughtof as a filing system. The frontal lobesare analogous to the file clerk of theepisodic memory system, the MTL (andPapez circuit) to the recent memory filecabinet, and other cortical regions tothe remote memory file cabinet. Thus,if the frontal lobes are damaged, it isdifficultbut not impossibleto getinformation in and out of storage. Ad-ditionally, when the frontal lobes aredamaged, the information stored inmemory may be distorted due to im-proper filing that leads to an inaccuratesource, context, or sequence. If, on theother hand, the MTLs are impaired, itmay be impossible for recent informationto be stored. Older information that has

    been consolidated over months to yearsis likely stored in other cortical regionsand will therefore be available for retriev-al even when the MTL or Papez circuit isdamaged. See Table 1-2 for character-istics of memory impairment due tofrontal versus medial temporal injuries.

    A number of conditions producememory impairment that is due, atleast in part, to frontal lobe dysfunc-tion. In addition to frontal strokesand mass lesions, other conditions

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    21

    KEY POINTS

    A Alzheimer

    disease is by far

    the most

    common causeof episodic

    memory

    impairment.

    A Memory

    distortions,

    false memories,

    and confabulation

    may occur with

    damage to

    frontal cortex.

    A Think of episodic

    memory as afiling system:

    The frontal

    lobes are the

    filing clerk, the

    medial temporal

    lobes are the

    recent memory

    file cabinet, and

    other cortical

    regions are the

    older memory

    file cabinet.

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    8/14

    associated with a frontally based epi-sodic memory impairment includefrontotemporal degeneration, vasculardementia (particularly when associat-ed with subcortical white matter dis-ease), dementia with Lewy bodies,multiple sclerosis, depression, and headtrauma. Distinguishing between memo-ry impairment due to medial temporalinjury and that associated with frontallobe dysfunction has potential diagnos-tic value. For example, while measuresof free recall and associative memory donot differentiate patients with AD fromthose with subcortical vascular demen-tia, measures of recognition memory

    (with patients with AD performingmore poorly) appear to have betterspecificity.23 Although generally moresubtle, healthy age-associated mem-ory loss tends to be qualitatively similarto memory loss due to frontal lobeinjury. This phenomenology is consis-tent with data supporting the relative-

    ly selective vulnerability of frontal lobefunction in aging as a result of cortical

    volume loss, anterior white matter dis-ruption, and dopaminergic depletion.24

    Other regions. Several other re-gions appear to be important substratesfor episodic memory function. Lesionsof the basal forebrain, often due to an-terior communicating artery aneurysm

    rupture, produce memory impairment.This region is the main source of cho-linergic input to the MTLs and neocor-tex. Blockade of acetylcholine with themuscarinic antagonist scopolamine pro-duces amnesia in healthy individuals.25

    The relative decline in acetylcholine as-sociated with basal forebrain pathologyin AD is the rationale for the use of cho-linesterase inhibitors in this condition.Given a general decline in cholinergic

    function with aging, it is not surprisingthat cholinergic blockers, such as sco-polamine, have greater effects on mem-ory and cognition in older than youngadults, which is why anticholinergicmedicines should be avoided in olderindividuals.

    Recent work, driven largely by the

    functional imaging literature, has sug-

    gested that the parietal lobes also par-

    ticipate in episodic memory retrieval.

    Studies have consistently revealed

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    22

    TABLE 1-2 Memory Deficits With Medial Temporal Lobe VersusFrontal Lobe Lesions

    Characteristic Medial TemporalLobe Lesion Frontal Lobe Lesion

    Immediate memory Normal to mildly impaired Impaired

    Free recall Impaired Impaired

    Recognition memoryor cued recall

    Impaired Often normal

    Source or associativememory (ie, recollection)

    Impaired Impaired

    Item memory(ie, familiarity)

    Impaired Normal

    Effects of environmentsupport

    Minimal enhancement ofmemory performance

    Significant enhancementof memory performance

    Tendency for falsememory or confabulation

    Variable High

    "MEMORY SYSTEMS

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    9/14

    midline and lateral parietal activations

    associated with successful retrieval of

    memories.26 While midline structures,

    including retrosplenial cortex, have beenimplicated in episodic memory, priorwork had not suggested a role for thelateral parietal cortex. A recent focuson episodic memory in patients withlateral parietal lesions has revealed, per-haps, subtle deficits in this population.

    While these patients appear to retrievecontextual details of prior episodes to asimilar extent as controls, they seem tohave greater difficulty doing so spon-

    taneously and their memories may be

    associated with less confidence or vivid-

    ness.27,28A number of potential hypoth-eses have been postulated as to thefunction of the parietal cortex in epi-

    sodic memory and are just now begin-ning to be tested experimentally.26

    SEMANTIC MEMORY

    Definitions

    The patient in Case 1-2 had a rela-tively selective impairment of seman-tic memory. Semantic memory definesour knowledge of the world, includinggeneral informationabout objects, peo-ple, historical events, and word mean-

    ing.3

    Examples of semantic knowledge

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    23

    Case 1-2A 73-year-old man had several years of cognitive decline. Most salient to him was difficultynaming and even recognizing a variety of items that used to be familiar to him. For example,his wife bought a bag of microwave popcorn that he examined at great length, eventually askinghis wife what it was used for. On the way to one of his clinic visits he saw a cement truck andcommented that he had never seen such an unusual truck before. He described marked difficultyin being able to name or even recognize close friendsthey did not look familiar to him. Despitethese issues, he had minimal functional decline and scored 24/30 on the Mini-Mental StateExamination. His wife described his

    day-to-day memory as essentiallyunchanged. He spoke fluently onexamination and had reasonablecomprehension of simple words. He hadmarked naming impairment on theBoston Naming Test (14/30 correct), andhe could name only three vegetables in1 minute. He performed average to aboveaverage on tests of executive function,attention, and visuospatial memory.An MRI scan revealed severe bilateralanterior and inferior-lateral temporallobe atrophy. This patient wasfelt to have the early stages of semanticvariant of primary progressive aphasia.Note the significant atrophy in theanterior, inferior, and lateral temporallobe on an MRI scan (Figure 1-4).

    Comment. This patient had arelatively selective deficit of semanticmemory, but essentially spared episodicmemory function. This case furtherillustrates the dissociation of these twomemory systems.

    FIGURE 1-4 MRI sagittal T1-weighted image. Note thesevere temporal lobe atrophy, whichincludes anterior, inferior, and lateralregions, relative to the rest of the brain.

    KEY POINT

    A Semantic memory

    is memory for

    knowledge of

    the world whenit is unconnected

    with a specific

    episode of

    ones life.

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    10/14

    aretigers have stripesandPhiladelphiais the largest city in Pennsylvania. Thisform of declarative memory can be dif-

    ferentiated from episodic memory be-cause its retrieval is not associated witha sense of self-experience or linked to aparticular spatial and temporal context.For example, remembering watchingPresident Barack Obamas inaugurationspeech on television with ones wifeis an example of an episodic memory

    while knowing that he is president is asemantic one. That episodic and seman-tic memory represent different mem-ory systems is supported by the disso-

    ciations in impairment associated withdifferent brain lesions. For example, thepatient HM, who had bilateral MTL re-sections, displayed profound amnesia

    with relative sparing of previously learnedsemantic information.

    Semantic memory impairment is mostfrequently manifested by naming defi-cits. This impaired naming is not miti-gated by the use of phonemic cues, andoften naming errors reflect semantically

    related word choices (eg, dogforlion).Different from a pure anomia, however,these patients will also display evidenceof nonverbal impairment, such as match-ingpictures ofitemsinto differentseman-tic categories, and difficulty in providingdefinitions or descriptions of items whenprovided with their names. Categoryfluency, in which patients are asked toname as many items as they can think

    of in a particular semantic category (eg,animals), is another bedside test that isoften impaired in those with semanticmemory dysfunction.

    Functional Neuroanatomy ofSemantic Memory

    While semantic memory is likely repres-ented in a distributed fashion through-out much of the neocortex, the infero-lateral temporal lobes (particularly theleft) are the brain regions whose injury

    is most associated with disruption ofsemantic knowledge. Indeed, seman-tic variant of primary progressive apha-sia, the archetypal disease producing arelative pure semantic knowledge im-pairment, is associated with relativelyfocal neurodegeneration in this region(Figure 1-5).

    Rare instances of category-specific

    semantic deficits have provided addi-tional insight into the neural organiza-

    tion of semantic memory. The litera-ture describes a number of patientswith relatively selective impairment ofknowledge of living things (eg, animalsand vegetables) but preserved knowl-

    edge of artifacts, such as tools.29 Theopposite dissociation has also beendescribed, strengthening the functionalsegregation of these forms of seman-tic memory. Work has suggested thatthese dissociated representations maybe a reflection of the nature by which

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    24

    FIGURE 1-5 Semantic, procedural, and working memory.The anterior and inferolateral temporallobes are important in the naming and

    categorization tasks by which semantic memory is typicallyassessed. However,in the broadest sense, semantic memory mayreside in multiple and diverse cortical areas that are related tovarious types of knowledge. The basal ganglia, cerebellum,and supplementary motor area are critical for proceduralmemory. The prefrontal cortex is active in virtually all workingmemory tasks; other cortical and subcortical brain regions willalso be active, depending on the type and complexity of theworking memory task.

    Adapted from Budson AE, Price BH. Memory dysfunction. N Engl J Med2005;352(7):692699. Copyright#2005,with permission from MassachusettsMedical Society. All rights reserved.

    KEY POINTS

    A Previously

    learned semantic

    information will

    be intact whena patient

    experiences an

    isolated loss of

    episodic memory.

    A The inferolateral

    temporal lobes

    (particularly the

    left) are critical

    for semantic

    memory.

    "MEMORY SYSTEMS

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    11/14

    one experiences these different cate-gories.30 For example, our knowledgeof animals usually is largely related to

    the visual modality, while our experi-ence with tools is more related to mo-tor representations.

    Semantic memory deficits are mostcommonly seen in patients with AD andare likely a reflection of the pathologyin inferolateral temporal neocortex. Ofcourse, AD is also associated with sig-nificant episodic memory impairment,

    which often dominates the initial clini-cal picture. Other conditions associated

    with semantic memory impairment in-

    clude semantic variant of primary pro-gressive aphasia, herpes encephalitis,trauma, and occasionally stroke.

    Overlap Between Semantic andEpisodic Memory

    Despite beingconsidered separatemem-orysystems,semanticand episodic mem-ory interact in important ways. Forexample, work from the cognitive psy-chology and functional imaging litera-ture supports the benefit of semanticencoding on subsequent episodic mem-ory of studied items.31,32 Further, se-mantic deficits themselves sometimescorrelate with performance on episodicmemory tasks, presumably by reduc-ing the effectiveness of the encodingprocess.33

    The episodic memory system is alsocritical for the formation of new se-mantic memories, as evidenced by theprofound impact of MTL lesions on such

    new semantic memory formation. Whilefactlike learning in patients with severebilateral medial temporal injury has beenreported, this appears largely relegatedto fragmented information likely sup-ported by perceptual learning ratherthan true pieces of new semantic infor-mation.34 Even relatively limited hippo-

    campal lesions can seriously degrade theacquisition of new semantic knowledge.

    Autobiographic memories are an-other area in which episodic and seman-

    tic memories overlap. These memoriesof our own personal experiences aregenerally conceived as a type of episodic

    memory. More remote autobiographicmemories, however, often become qual-itatively more akin to semantic knowl-edge and are likely supported, in part,by the semantic memory system. Onesimple test is that if, when recalling amemory, you can see yourself in it, thememory is likely semantic rather thanepisodic, since you would not ordinarilysee yourself in a memory.

    PROCEDURAL MEMORY

    Definitions

    Procedural memory is the nondeclar-ative memory system that refers to theability to learn cognitive and behavioralskills and algorithms that operate at anautomatic, unconscious level. Examplesinclude learning to ride a bicycle or playthe piano. Because procedural memoryis spared in patients who have severedeficits of the episodic memory system(such as those who have undergone

    surgical removal of the MTLs), it is clearthat the procedural memory system isseparate and distinct from the episodicmemory system.35

    Functional Neuroanatomy ofProcedural Memory

    Patients with damage to the basal gan-glia or cerebellum show impairment inlearning procedural skills.36 Functionalneuroimaging has found that these

    regionsand the supplementary motorareabecome active as a new proce-dural memory task is being learned37

    (Figure 1-5). Because the basal ganglia,cerebellum, and supplementary motorarea are relatively spared in early AD,these patients show normal acquisitionand maintenance of their procedural

    memory skills, despite their episodicand semantic memory deficits.38

    Patients in the early stages of Parkin-son disease show impaired procedural

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    25

    KEY POINTS

    A Because

    Alzheimer disease

    affects the

    inferolateraltemporal lobes,

    it is the most

    common cause of

    semantic memory

    impairment.

    Semantic variant

    of primary

    progressive

    aphasia,

    encephalitis,

    trauma, and

    stroke are other

    causes of semantic

    memory

    impairment.

    A Procedural

    memory is

    memory for

    cognitive and

    behavioral skills

    and algorithms

    that are typically

    operating

    unconsciously

    such as riding abicycle.

    A Critical brain

    regions for

    procedural

    memory are the

    basal ganglia,

    cerebellum, and

    supplementary

    motor area.

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    12/14

    memory while performing nearly nor-mally on episodic memory tests.38 Proce-dural memory is also disrupted by other

    causes of damage to the basal gangliaor cerebellum, including Huntingtondisease, olivopontocerebellar degenera-tion, tumors, strokes, and hemorrhages.Patients with major depression mayalso show impairment in proceduralmemory tasks, perhaps because depres-sion involves dysfunction of the basalganglia.39

    Disruption of procedural memoryshould be suspected when patientsshow evidence of either the loss of

    previously learned skills (comparedwith their baseline) or substantial dif-ficulties in learning new skills. For ex-ample, patients may lose the ability toperform automatic, skilled movements,such as writing, playing a musical in-strument, or swinging a tennis racket.

    Although these patients may be able torelearn the fundamentals of these skills,

    explicit thinking becomes required fortheir performance. As a result, patients

    with damage to the procedural memory

    system lose the automatic effortlessnessof simple motor tasks that healthy in-dividuals take for granted. Lastly, it is

    worth noting that patients whose epi-sodic memory has been devastated bya static disorder, such as encephalitis,have had successful rehabilitation byusing procedural memory (and othernondeclarative forms of memory) tolearn new skills.40

    CONCLUSIONSEvidence from patient studies and morerecent neuroimaging research suggestthat memory is composed of separate

    and distinct systems. An understandingof these different memory systems willaid the clinician in the diagnosis andtreatment of the memory disorders oftheir patients.

    REFERENCES1. Budson AE, Price BH. Memory dysfunction. N Engl J Med 2005;352(7):692699.

    2. Scoville WB, Milner B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions.J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1957;20(1):1121.

    3. Tulving E. Memory and consciousness. Can Psychol 1985;26:112.

    4. Yonelinas A. The nature of recollection and familiarity: a review of 30 years ofresearch. J Mem Lang 2002;46:441517.

    5. Squire LR, Stark CE, Clark RE. The medial temporal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci2004;27:279306.

    6. Aggleton JP, Brown MW. Interleaving brain systems for episodic and recognitionmemory. Trends Cogn Sci 2006;10(10):455463.

    7. Yonelinas AP, Kroll NE, Quamme JR, et al. Effects of extensive temporal lobedamage or mild hypoxia on recollection and familiarity. Nat Neurosci2002;5(11):12361241.

    8. Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C.The medial temporal lobe and recognitionmemory. Annu Rev Neurosci 2007;30:123152.

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    26

    KEY POINT

    A Parkinson disease

    is the most

    common

    disorderdisrupting

    procedural

    memory.

    Huntington

    disease, tumors,

    strokes, and

    hemorrhages

    may also disrupt

    procedural

    memory.

    "MEMORY SYSTEMS

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    13/14

    9. Ribot T. Diseases of memory: an essay in the positive psychology. In: Smith WH, ed. Theinternational scientific series. New York: Appleton, 1882.

    10. Squire LR, Alvarez P. Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a

    neurobiological perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1995;5(2):169177.

    11. Moscovitch M, Nadel L, Winocur G, et al. The cognitive neuroscience of remoteepisodic, semantic and spatial memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2006;16(2):179190.

    12. Allen G, Barnard H, McColl R, et al. Reduced hippocampal functional connectivityin Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 2007;64(10):14821487.

    13. Selkoe DJ. Soluble oligomers of the amyloid beta-protein impair synaptic plasticityand behavior. Behav Brain Res 2008;192(1):106113.

    14. Papez JW. A proposed mechanism of emotion; 1937. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci1995;7(1):103112.

    15. Valenstein E, Bowers D, Verfaellie M, et al. Retrosplenial amnesia. Brain1987;110(pt 6):16311646.

    16. Harding A, Halliday G, Caine D, Kril J. Degeneration of anterior thalamic nucleidifferentiates alcoholics with amnesia. Brain 2001;123(pt 1):141154.

    17. Duarte A, Ranganath C, Knight RT. Effects of unilateral prefrontal lesions onfamiliarity, recollection, and source memory. J Neurosci 2005;25(36):83338337.

    18. Kopelman MD, Stanhope N, Kingsley D. Temporal and spatial context memory inpatients with focal frontal, temporal lobe, and diencephalic lesions.Neuropsychologia 1997;35(12):15331545.

    19. Wheeler MA, Stuss DT, Tulving E. Frontal lobe damage produces episodic memoryimpairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1995;1(6):525536.

    20. Blumenfeld RS, Ranganath C. Prefrontal cortex and long-term memory encoding:an integrative review of findings from neuropsychology and neuroimaging.Neuroscientist 2007;13(3):280291.

    21. Budson AE, Sullivan AL, Mayer E, et al. Suppression of false recognition in Alzheimersdisease and in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Brain 2002;125(pt 12):116.

    22. Kopelman MD. Disorders of memory. Brain 2002;125(pt 10):21522190.

    23. Tierney MC, Black SE, Szalai JP, et al. Recognition memory and verbal fluencydifferentiate probable Alzheimer disease from subcortical ischemic vascular

    dementia. Arch Neurol 2001;58(10):16541659.

    24. Buckner RL. Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors thatcause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 2004;44(1):195208.

    25. Kopelman MD, Corn TH. Cholinergic blockade as a model for cholinergicdepletion. A comparison of the memory deficits with those of Alzheimer-type

    dementia and the alcoholic Korsakoff syndrome. Brain 1988;111(pt 5):10791110.

    26. Wagner AD, Shannon BJ, Kahn I, Buckner RL. Parietal lobe contributions toepisodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9(9):445453.

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    27

    Copyright @ American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

  • 8/11/2019 Wolk a Eb Memory Sys 2010

    14/14

    27. Berryhill ME, Phuong L, Picasso L, et al. Parietal lobe and episodic memory: bilateraldamage causes impaired free recall of autobiographical memory. J Neurosci2007;27(52):1441514423.

    28. Ally BA, Simons JS, McKeever JD, et al. Parietal contributions to recollection:electrophysiological evidence from aging and patients with parietal lesions.Neuropsychologia 2008;46(7):18001812.

    29. Wolk DA, Coslett HB, Glosser G. The role of sensory-motor information in objectrecognition: evidence from a patient with a category-specific impairment. Brain Lang2005;94(2):131146.

    30. Warrington EK, Shallice T. Category specific semantic impairments. Brain1984;107(pt 3):829854.

    31. Craik FIM, Lockhart RS. Levels of processing: a framework for memory research.J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 1972;11(6):671684.

    32. Otten LJ, Henson RN, Rugg MD. Depth of processing effects on neural correlates ofmemory encoding: relationship between findings from across- and within-taskcomparisons. Brain 2001;124(pt 2):399412.

    33. Goldblum MC, Gomez CM, Dalla Barba G, et al. The influence of semantic and

    perceptual encoding on recognition memory in Alzheimers disease.Neuropsychologia 1998;36(8):717729.

    34. Manns JR, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. Semantic memory and the human hippocampus.Neuron 2003;38(1):127133.

    35. Corkin S. Lasting consequences of bilateral medial temporal lobectomy: clinicalcourse and experimental findings in H. M. Sem Neurol 1984;4(2):249259.

    36. Exner C, Koschack J, Irle E. The differential role of premotor frontal cortex andbasal ganglia in motor sequence learning: evidence from focal basal ganglia lesions.Learn Mem 2002;9(6):376386.

    37. Daselaar SM, Rombouts SA, Veltman DJ, et al. Similar network activated by youngand old adults during the acquisition of a motor sequence. Neurobiol Aging2003;24(7):10131019.

    38. Heindel WC, Salmon DP, Shults CW, et al. Neuropsychological evidence formultiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimers, Huntingtons, andParkinsons disease patients. J Neurosci 1989;9(2):582587.

    39. Sabe L, Jason L, Juejati M, et al. Dissociation between declarative and procedural

    learning in dementia and depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1995;17(6):841848.

    40. Glisky EL, Schacter DL. Extending the limits of complex learning in organic amnesia:computer training in a vocational domain. Neuropsychologia 1989;27(1):107120.

    Continuum Lifelong Learning Neurol 2010;16(4)

    28

    "MEMORY SYSTEMS