what makes a destination beautiful_dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

Upload: ghirasim-erika-lolita

Post on 02-Jun-2018

222 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    1/12

    What makes a destination beautiful? Dimensions of tourist aestheticjudgment

    Ksenia Kirillova, Xiaoxiao Fu, Xinran Lehto*, Liping Cai

    Purdue University, School of Hospitality and Tourism Management, United States

    h i g h l i g h t s

    The beauty of tourist destinations is uniquely judged and admired.

    Experiential rather than classic dimensions of aesthetic judgment are salient in tourism aesthetics.

    Dimensions are equally prominent in judgment of nature-based and urban destination.

    Destination planners should employ existing aesthetic inventory in strategic planning.

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:

    Received 10 May 2013

    Accepted 18 December 2013

    Keywords:

    Aesthetic judgment

    Tourism experience

    Destination management

    Experience-based productsDestination planning

    Tourism aesthetics

    a b s t r a c t

    Drawing on the literature in environmental psychology, the current study attempted to reveal di-

    mensions of tourist aesthetic judgment in the context of both nature-based and urban tourist destina-

    tions. Two-stage analysis of semi-structured interview data from a theoretical sample of 57 individuals

    yielded 21 aesthetic dimensions that were categorized into nine themes: Scale, Time, Condition, Sound,

    Balance, Diversity, Novelty, Shape, and Uniqueness. The identied themes were further conceptualized

    into a two-dimensional plane along Concrete-Abstract and Subjective-Objective continuums. This

    research posits that tourism allows a unique appreciator-object dyad where individuals are fully

    immersed in a destination in pursuit of a non-routine and oftentimes novel experience. The beauty of

    tourism destination is uniquely judged, admired, and appreciated, and the assessment of the beauty goes

    beyond the visual aspects and engages all senses. The ndings make a theoretical contribution to the

    existing aesthetics literature and bear practical implications for destination planning, branding, and

    management.

    2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    Tourism experience is a critical concept in tourism marketing

    and management literature; therefore, researchers have paid

    increasing attention to this area, exerting efforts in both conceptual

    deliberations and empirical validations (e.g. Cohen, 1979a; Li, 2000;

    Mannell & Iso-Ahola, 1987; Mkono, Markwell, & Wilson, 2013;Otto& Ritchie,1996; Quan & Wang, 2004; Sternberg,1997; Uriely,2005).

    Tourism scholars, however, haveyet to fully investigate the touriste

    environment exchange (Lehto, 2013). The environmental qualities

    of a destination can impact a tourist experience profoundly ( Todd,

    2009). Tourists interaction with a destinations overall environ-

    ment and their internalization of what they see and sense could

    play a key role in their overall trip satisfaction. When people plan to

    travel for pleasure, they seek destinations that, in their opinion,

    maximize the possibility to receive a pleasurable experience ( Lue,

    Crompton, & Fesenmaier, 1993). One source of such pleasure is

    the aesthetic qualities of the destination. In tourism management

    literature, it has been acknowledged that aesthetic characteristics

    affect tourists experience and satisfaction, contributing to their

    loyalty towards a destination (Lee, Jeon, & Kim, 2011) and thusintention to return (Baloglu, Pekcan, Chen, & Santos, 2004). Desti-

    nationsaesthetic qualities, such as scenery, have been an integral

    element of many satisfaction and perceived image scales used in

    tourism research (e.g. Alegre & Garau, 2010; OLeary & Deegan,

    2003).

    Despite the fact that numerous studies have recognized the

    importance of aesthetic qualities of a destination, these qualities

    have so far been largely reduced to a single dimensional variable

    such as the place is beautifulin destination attribute satisfaction

    assessment. Although the notion of product aesthetics has been

    explored in consumer behavior literature in conjunction with

    * Corresponding author. Tel.: 1 765 494 2085.

    E-mail addresses: [email protected] (K. Kirillova), [email protected] (X. Fu),

    [email protected](X. Lehto),[email protected](L. Cai).

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Tourism Management

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m/ l o c a t e / t o u r m a n

    0261-5177/$e see front matter 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2013.12.006

    Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/02615177http://www.elsevier.com/locate/tourmanhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2013.12.006http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2013.12.006http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2013.12.006http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2013.12.006http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2013.12.006http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2013.12.006http://www.elsevier.com/locate/tourmanhttp://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/02615177http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.tourman.2013.12.006&domain=pdfmailto:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    2/12

    product choice (e.g. Creusen & Schoormans, 2005), design (e.g.

    Bloch, 1995; Riemann, Zaichkowsky, Neuhaus, Bender, & Weber,

    2010), and overall product evaluation (Yamamoto & Lambert,

    1994), the aesthetic component as judged by consumers has yet

    to be a focus in tourism research. Tourism aesthetics implies multi-

    sensory lived experience which may entail inter-relations not

    only between a tourist and the surroundings but also among po-

    tential dimensions of the interactive experience (Ittelson, 1978). As

    such, this lived experienceoffers opportunities for phenomeno-

    logical exploration. Additionally, tourists may use home environ-

    ment as a reference point in assessing whether the destination is

    beautiful. For example, interpretation of a destinations aesthetics

    may be derived from the similarity or contrast between ones home

    environment and the vacation environment. Due to the potentially

    diverse aesthetic judgments among tourists, it will be of theoretical

    signicance to zoom in on the area of tourism aesthetics for its

    multi-faceted dimensionality.

    The search for the answer to what do we nd beautiful? is

    one of the most long-hauled quests in philosophy. Dening beauty

    as which gives pleasure when seen, Plato, Aristotle, and St.

    Thomas Aquinas asserted that the beauty resides within an object

    and is not subjected to observers biased evaluations (Beardsley,

    1975). Hume (1757/2013) and Kant (1790/1987), on the otherhand, posited that beauty is in nature subjective. No response to

    an object is superior because one judges beauty based on personal

    values such as religious beliefs, cultural background, political

    views, and other normative values. Thus, aesthetic pleasure is a

    natural human response, and as such, its extent could be distinct

    across different individuals (Ginsborg, 2013). Modern philosophers

    conceptualized aesthetic judgment as the object-related cognitive

    part of aesthetic processing and thus it can be assessed in social

    science while aesthetic emotion can only be measured by neuro-

    psychological means (Leder, Belke, Oebrest, & Augustin, 2004, p.

    503). Moreover, empirical research in art appreciation found that

    people tend to perceive symmetric and round objects as beautiful

    (Jacobsen, Schubotz, Hfel, & Cramon, 2006; Silvia & Barona,

    2009).Unlike the art experience where the appreciator is an outside

    observer, an individual is immersed in the object of appreciation

    in environmental aesthetics (Berleant, 2005). Aesthetic qualities of

    places have been previously explored conceptually in environ-

    mental aesthetics (e.g.Berleant, 2005; Carlson & Lintott, 2008) and

    empirically in environmental psychology (e.g. Kaplan, Kaplan, &

    Brown, 1989) and urban design (e.g. Daniel, 2001), where both

    urban and natural landscapes were employed as contexts. Tourism

    aesthetics, however, could possess its own traits and characteris-

    tics in that tourism experience involves the full immersion of an

    individual into an environment that may be distinct from his/her

    everyday living surroundings (Volo, 2009). The experience may

    trigger human senses to become more responsive to outside

    stimuli and allows more complex humane

    environment in-teractions and exchanges. Thus, how and why tourists perceive a

    destination beautiful could potentially be related or unrelated to,

    similar to or distinct from the criteria researchers utilize to assess

    routine (home) environments (Maitland & Smith, 2009). Never-

    theless, until now, these areas have been sorely neglected in

    tourism marketing and management literature. As a pioneering

    attempt, the current study initiates an inquiry into tourist

    aesthetic judgment. Specically, the study aims to examine how

    aesthetic gratication is provided in both urban and nature-based

    tourist destinations. Given the scarcity of existing empirical

    studies in this area and a need for in-depth understanding of

    tourist aesthetic judgment, the present study employed a quali-

    tative assessment through personal interviews to uncover both

    theoretical and practical insights.

    2. Literature review

    2.1. Aesthetics in management literature

    Aesthetics has received increasing yet still limited attention in

    the business management literature with scholarly interest

    centered on aesthetic products and experiential consumption

    (Charters, 2006). Generally, aesthetic products are believed to have

    four essential qualities: 1) the products aesthetic considerations

    must be the primary purpose; 2) the product is constructed to

    stimulate aesthetic consumption; 3) it is capable of providing

    intrinsic value; and 4) it strives in highly segmented markets

    (Charters, 2006). However, as most consumer goods possess the

    above-mentioned qualities to various degrees, aesthetic products

    could be conceptualized as a continuum consisting of products

    ranging from those of minimal aesthetic dimension to those

    entirely aesthetic (Bloch, Brunel, & Arnold, 2003). Assessment of

    aesthetic qualities is an important aspect of consumptive experi-

    ences (e.g.Baker, Grewal, & Parasuraman, 1994), and the concept of

    aesthetic product is distinct in the discussion of experiential con-

    sumption (Charters, 2006). For instance, Estheticswas proposed as

    one fundamental dimension in experience by Pine & Gilmore

    (1999) in their notion of experience economy, along with Enter-tainment, Education, and Escapism. Aesthetics has been also dis-

    cussed in hedonic consumption as having the capacity to generate

    strong emotional involvement (Hirschman & Holbrook, 1982). In

    his review of the existing literature on the topic, Charters (2006)

    asserts that there is a link between the experiential consump-

    tion of a product displaying beauty and the judgments,comprised of appreciation, quality evaluation, and taste (pp. 243e

    244). Such observations implythat aestheticpropertiesof a product

    are instrumental not only in stimulating consumption but also in

    evaluation of the entire consumptive experience.

    Aesthetic consumption and consumers ability to judge

    aesthetic qualities of a product are related to the idea of product

    design. In saturated markets, an aesthetically appealing product is a

    way of gaining buyersattention, communicating information, andproviding aesthetic pleasure to both sellers and users (Bloch, 1995).

    Moreover, aesthetic responses elicited by exposure to sensorial

    properties of a product rather than its functional characteristics

    tend to have a long-lasting effect on consumers as the product

    becomes part of users sensory environments (Jones, 1991).

    Holbrook and Zirlin (1985) argued, however, that the aesthetic

    component of a product is best realized during the functional use of

    the product, suggesting that only purely aesthetic products such as

    classical music or a painting could stand on their own during the

    consumptive experience.

    Such propositions have been well embraced in service man-

    agement where the signicance of aesthetic judgment has been

    reected in the theorizations and applications of servicescape or

    atmospherics (Bitner, 1992; Lovelock & Wirtz, 2004). It has beenempirically noted across a variety of service settings that cus-

    tomers experience is inevitably inuenced by the surrounding

    aesthetic cues. It was found, for instance, that facility aesthetics

    affects perceived servicescape quality and thus satisfaction, re-

    patronage intentions, and desire to stay at basketball and football

    stadiums and casinos (Wakeeld & Blodgett, 1996). Aesthetically

    pleasing dining environment attenuates perceived food and service

    quality and directly inuence behavioral intentions (Ha & Jang,

    2012). Finally, aesthetic judgment, which may or may not lead to

    the occurrence of aesthetic pleasure, seems to play an important

    role in the assessment of overall experience (e.g.Liu & Jang, 2009).

    Despite the recognition of aesthetics in business research in

    general, there has been a paucity of attention on tourism aesthetics

    in particular. One such effort is noted in the bed and breakfast

    K. Kirillova et al. / Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293 283

  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    3/12

  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    4/12

    approach is expected to result in unique phenomenological expe-

    riences for the observers and such experiences would largely attest

    to their aesthetic judgment and preference. Despite the call for

    phenomenological exploration of aesthetic experience in the city

    settings, this line of research has not received continued attention.

    2.3. Summary

    Table 1presents a summary of the reviewed research streams,

    highlighting insights that informed the current study. Philosophy,

    particularly the branch of aesthetics, established the subjectivity of

    aesthetic judgment while emphasizing the unique nature of envi-

    ronment appreciation. Environmental psychology and urban

    design research attest to the cognitive processes behind the nature

    and urban landscape appreciation. Additionally, literature in gen-

    eral management indicates that, in the era of experience economy,

    product aesthetics possesses critical implications for strategic

    marketing and management. In the case of service encounters,

    aesthetic judgment inuences perceived quality and satisfaction,

    which in turn affects behavioral intentions. Although destination

    management research has yet to zoom into the aesthetic compo-

    nent of tourism experience, for long it has considered beautiful

    scenery to be an attribute of a destination. As such, aesthetics couldbe inuential in destination choice, image formation, and overall

    satisfaction. Taken as a whole, the insights derived from these

    research streams contribute to conceptual understanding of the

    role of aesthetics in experiencing as well as managing a tourism

    destination.

    3. Objectives

    As the literature review demonstrated, aesthetic judgment in a

    destination setting deserves closer scrutiny in its own rights

    because a destination is different from transaction-based business

    settings. A destination experience necessitates touristsbodily and

    mental immersion, engagement and appreciation of non-routine

    environments and thus entails a much more holistic and globaljudgment. Such judgment may not be in conformity with the

    aesthetic appreciation in visual arts or generic product/service

    design. On the other hand, aesthetic attributes of vacation desti-

    nations are an integral part of destination image formation and

    overall satisfaction with tourism experience, leading to destination

    choice and revisit intentions. Understanding this aspect of tourism

    experience could provide invaluable insights into the process of

    aesthetic appraisal and allow practitioners to manage destinations

    more strategically. Despite the theoretical and practical importance

    of aesthetics in tourism, tourist aesthetic judgment has rarely been

    conceptually elaborated nor has it been empirically investigated;

    the questions of how and why tourists may judge a destination as

    beautiful remain under-explored.

    Tourism experience can be a unique aesthetic experience

    because an individual is physically relocated and immersed into a

    setting that could be drastically different from his or her everyday

    environment. The current study aimed at understanding how and

    why tourists nd certain aspects of vacation destinations to be

    beautiful. Specically, it attempted to reveal the dimensions of

    tourist aesthetic judgment of both nature-based and urban desti-

    nations. It should be noted that the goal was to uncover the general

    dimensions of aesthetic judgment of destinations rather than to

    compare and contrast the dimensions between the nature-based

    and urban settings.

    4. Method

    In-depth personal interview was used for data collection. This

    research adopted a qualitative approach with the goal of obtaining

    a rich and in-depth understanding of tourist aesthetic judgment.

    Previous research has been inconclusive about whether individual

    socio-demographic characteristics predict aesthetic preference. For

    example,Van den Berg et al. (1998)reported no signicant effect of

    personal factors on general aesthetic judgment, while other re-

    searchers found that age, social class (Howley, 2011), and gender

    (Strumse, 1996; Yabiku, Casagrande, & Farley-Metzger, 2008) may

    inuence landscape preferences. Since aesthetic judgment entails

    the joint outcome of a multitude of factors(Liu, 2003, p. 1277), atheoretical sampling technique was employed in order to include

    Table 1

    A summary of research streams.

    Research stream Representative works Contributing ideas

    Philosophy Hume (1757/2013)

    Kant (1790/1987)

    Leder, Belke, Oebrest, &

    Augustin (2004)

    Berleant (2005)

    Silvia and Barona (2009)

    C Subjective nature of aesthetic judgment

    C Idea of disinterestedness

    C Unlike aesthetic emotion, aesthetic judgment can be assessed in social science

    C Round and symmetric objects are judged as more beautiful

    C Environmental aesthetics implies complete immersion into an object of appreciation

    Environmental

    psychology

    Appleton (1975)

    Kaplan (1987)

    Van den Berg et al. (1998)

    C Aesthetic judgment is a cognitive process

    C Four domain of predictors of preferences for natural environments: Complexity,

    Mystery, Coherence, Legibility

    C Prospect-refuge theory

    C Environmental preferences vary according to user groups

    Urban design Williams (1954)

    Ittelson (1978)

    Weber et al. (2008)

    C City as a multisensory experience

    C Urban experience implies a psychological processing of information

    C Vegetation, Stylistic uniformity, Scale and Symmetry as predictors of preferences

    for urban landscapes

    General management Holbrook and Zirlin (1985)

    Baker et al. (1994)

    Pine and Gilmore (1998)

    Charters (2006)

    C Aesthetic products range from those of minimal aesthetic qualities to entirely aesthetic

    C Aesthetic quality is an important aspect of consumptive experiences

    C Aesthetics is one of the dimensions ofexperience economy

    C Product design as manifestation of aesthetics

    Service management Bitner (1992)

    Lovelock and Wirtz (2004)

    Liu and Jang (2009)

    C Aesthetics affects perceived service quality, satisfaction, and thus behavioral intentions

    C Aesthetics inuences perception of overall service experience

    Tourism destination

    management

    Um and Crompton (1990)

    Echtner and Ritchie (1991)

    Oh et al. (2007)

    C Aesthetics as a destination attribute

    C Beautiful scenery as a factor in the destination choice process

    C Aesthetics contributes to overall satisfaction with tourism experience

    C Aesthetics could lend to image formation

    K. Kirillova et al. / Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293 285

  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    5/12

    perspectives of individuals from a variety of socio-demographic

    background (Strauss & Corbin, 1990).

    Theoretical sampling is the process of data collection whereby

    the researcher simultaneously collects, codes and analyzes data in

    order to decide what data to collect next (Coyne, 1997, p. 625). This

    sampling technique is not concerned with population representa-

    tion; rather, it is intended to contribute to developing codes and

    emergent theory (Becker, 1993). As the data collection and analysis

    progressed, individuals from under-sampled groups were further

    recruited to yield a nal sample reecting perspectives of various

    socio-demographic clusters. This process continued until theoret-

    ical saturation was reached (Sandelowski, 1995). As a result, the

    nal sample consisted of 57 individuals, of which 24 (42.1%) were

    males and 33 (57.9%) were females. The average age of respondents

    was 32 years old. The respondents educational background was

    distributed as follows: 37 have high school diplomas, two have an

    Associate degree, nine have a Bachelors degree, three have a

    Masters degree, and six have a Ph.D. degree. It should be noted that

    cultural background could inuence aesthetic judgment. For

    instance, Asian people tend to perceive objects and landscapes

    holistically while Americans are more analytical in information

    processing (Nisbett, 2003). To obtain consistent data for aesthetic

    judgment, this study focused on respondents of the U.S. descent.Semi-structured interviews were conducted by the rst and

    second authors from November 2012 to March 2013 in a Mid-

    western city of the U.S. Each interview lasted 20 min on average.

    First, interviewees general background information such as age

    and education was solicited. Second, as indicated in the literature

    review, although beauty of a destination cannot be considered a

    single proxy for satisfaction, it does contribute to overall satisfac-

    tion with tourism experience. Thus, interviewees were asked to

    recall one most beautiful nature-based destination and one most

    beautiful urban destination they had visited and rate their satis-

    faction with their experiences on a scale from 0 to 100%. Such a

    scale is associated with the standard grading system in the U.S.

    educational institutions and thus is deemed as closely related to

    respondents cognitive evaluation style. Third, the intervieweeswere probed to further explain in depth why they found the des-

    tinations beautiful. Finally, they were requested to recall and

    explain the ugliest (or least beautiful) aspects of the destination in

    order to cross-validate the information provided in the previous

    responses. Since membership checking was not logistically

    possible, the information received during the interviews was

    repeated back to the respondents to ensure its accuracy and elim-

    inate possibilities for multiple interpretations. All interviews were

    audio-recorded and then transcribed verbatim.

    Data analysis consisted of a two-stage process. At the rst stage,

    the data were subjected to thematic analysis in which the themes

    were identied within the explicit meanings of the data (Braun &

    Clarke, 2006). Thematic analysis resembles content analysis but

    pays greater attention to the qualitative aspects of the materialanalysed (Joffe & Yardley, 2004, p. 56). The six-step process for

    thematic data analysis advocated by Braun and Clarke (2006)was

    utilized in this research. First, the authors familiarized themselves

    with the data, jotted down initial ideas for coding, and discussed

    the possible patterns. In accordance with the nature of an inductive

    approach, no predened categories were imposed on the data.

    Second, the rst author analyzed half of the data while the second

    author scrutinized the rest of the data for initial coding. The third

    author acted as an auditor to verify both the process and the initial

    results of data coding (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). The authors collec-

    tively discussed and reached consensus on the proposed codes. It

    should be noted that in this particular study, the term code is

    replaced by dimension as the latter better serves as a unit of

    aesthetic judgment which seems to involve a spectrum anchored

    by opposite points, as demonstrated in the nature of aesthetic

    judgment (beautiful-ugly). Third, when all data were coded into

    dimensions, the researchers discussed the viability of certain di-

    mensions, re-coded when necessary, and examined potential

    themes. Fourth, the researchers collectively reviewed the initial

    themes and underlying dimensions for internal homogeneity and

    external heterogeneity both at the level of coded data extracts and

    the entire data set. Fifth, all authors nalized the themes. Sixth, the

    authors discussed the strategies for writing up the scholarly report,

    with attention paid to selection of quotes to illustrate the uncov-

    ered dimensions and themes. When disagreements among the

    researchers arose during the thematic analysis process, the re-

    searchers gathered and discussed the evidences pro and contra a

    certain dimension or theme until consensus was reached. For

    example, the dimension Colorful-Dull was originally allocated to the

    theme Diversity by one of the researchers. When the raw data for

    this dimension were further reviewed, it was agreed that this

    dimension was better suited to the theme Scale.

    The second stage of data analysis involved calculation of fre-

    quencies in order to assess the relative importance of the di-

    mensions and themes established during the rst stage (Creswell,

    2008). For example, if a respondent mentioned that a destination

    was beautiful because of its modern architectural style, a frequencyof one was recorded on the Modern-Historicdimension.

    5. Results and discussion

    5.1. Dimensions of aesthetic judgment

    Interviewees reported their travel experiences with 30 different

    nature-based destinations that ranged from such international

    destinations as Tibet and Maldives to such domestic vacation sites

    as Florida Keys and Upper Michigan counties. The majority of the

    interviewees (81.82%) visited nature-based destinations for the rst

    time and enjoyed them to a great extent (97.11%). Twenty-ve ur-

    ban destinations were discussed by the interviewees, varying from

    such international destinations as Beijing and Prague to domesticcities such as Chicago and New Orleans. In the case of urban des-

    tinations, for most respondents (72.73%), it was their rst visit, and

    they enjoyed those destinations to a substantial degree (72.7%).

    Data analysis revealed 21 dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment

    which were further organized into nine themes: Scale, Time, Con-

    dition, Sound, Balance, Diversity, Novelty, Shape, and Uniqueness.

    Scale, Time, and Condition are the three most frequently discussed

    themes.

    5.1.1. Scale

    First, tourists seem to form their aesthetic judgment of a

    destination based on the Scaleof the place. Specically, such di-

    mensions as ColorfuleDull, GrandeQuaint, Presence of peoplee

    Absence of people, Abundancee

    Scarcity, and Opennesse

    Narrownesspertain to characteristics related to the physical magnitude of a

    vacation destination.ColorfuleDullrefers to intensity of colors in a

    destination whileGrandeQuaintattests to physical proportions of a

    place.Presence of peopleeAbsence of peopleindicates the degree of

    crowdedness, Abundance-Scarcity connotes the amount of visual

    cues in the environment, andOpennesseNarrowness demonstrates

    the importance of spatial characteristics of a destination. Only one

    dimension (OpennesseNarrowness) has been previously explored in

    environmental psychology (Coeterier, 1996; Strumse,1994). Among

    the dimensions identied within the theme,ColorfuleDullseems to

    be the most commonly addressed. Tourists generally judge colorful

    and vibrant sceneries and features as beautiful, as illustrated by the

    account of a 20-year old male: When I say beautiful, I mean really

    vibrant color in the water and you can see it very clear, vibrant,

    K. Kirillova et al. / Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293286

  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    6/12

    vibrantly colored. At the same time, some travelers tend to

    consider dark-colored features as beautiful, as exemplied by The

    water was dark and calm, almost like glass but you couldn t see

    through (male, 62). Such diversity in judgments exists across

    nature-based and urban destinations.

    5.1.2. Time

    The second theme Timeconsists of such dimensions as Mod-

    erneHistoricand YoungeOld. While the former dimension refers to

    the physical attributes of a place, the latter relates to the age of

    other individuals observed at the destination. ModerneHistoric

    appears to be a more prominent dimension. Although the degree of

    being modern or historic seems to be shared as an important cri-

    terion for aesthetic judgment, whether or not one is preferred over

    the other appears to vary among interviewees. Some tourists

    appreciate the beauty of the destination because of its historic

    aspect, as testied by a 21-year old female who visited Chicago:

    Its kinda old architecture, old stone, and the area around it, too.

    There is just like.old historic feeling to it. So, and thats really

    pretty that the city could keep it like that for so long, too, and not

    like modernize everything.

    Other interviewees, however, perceive a destination as beautiful

    if it appears to be ultra-modern. For example, a 19-year old female

    noted, I like all the modern design in the city, especially those

    facilitated by technology. I dont like backwardness. Not surpris-

    ingly, this dimension was generally discussed as a criterion for the

    beauty of urban destinations.

    5.1.3. Condition

    Dimensions under the third theme Conditionmainly pertain

    to the state of physical features of vacation destinations. Cleane

    Dirtyattests to the perceived hygienic conditions at a destination

    while Well-maintained-Run-down emphasizes the importance of

    upkeep of its physical attributes. Such dimensions have been noted

    previously in the literature of urban aesthetics (Galindo & Hidalgo,2005; Nasar, 1994). CleaneDirty seems to be the most salient

    dimension in interviewees aesthetic judgments. It attests to a

    destinations ability to not only maintain a litter-free environment

    but also manage features that do not appear as clean such as

    homeless people, stray animals, and poor air quality. Negative

    comments regarding visible trash on the ground as being the least

    beautiful aspect of a tourist destination seem to be more prominent

    in cases of nature-based destinations. This is not surprising since

    people do not expect to see traces of human activity in natural

    settings and thus litter is perceived dirtierthan in cases of urban

    destinations. This dimension echoes the notion of Naturalness

    (Scott, 2002) in that tourists traveling to nature-based destinations

    expect to see a landscape untouched by human activity. It also re-

    lates to the dimension of Human touche

    No human touch in thetheme Balance as travelers tend to judge nature-based attractions

    as more beautiful if no evident human element is present.

    5.1.4. Sound

    In addition to visual stimuli, tourists also judge the aesthetics of

    destinations based on auditory perceptions. The fourth theme

    Sound contains such dimensions as LivelyePeaceful, Nature-

    madeeHuman-made, and LoudeQuiet.The LivelyePeaceful dimen-

    sion showsthat tourists form aestheticjudgments basedon thepace

    of sounds heard at a destination while Nature-madeeHuman-made

    andLoudeQuietilluminate the importance of source and volume of

    these auditory cues. These dimensions are unique to tourism aes-

    thetics, implying full immersion into the surrounding environment

    on the part of the tourists (Berleant, 2005). It is noteworthy that

    LivelyePeacefuland LoudeQuietattest to different modes of sound

    perception and are not mutually exclusive. For instance, a 55-year

    old female describes her experience at Niagara Falls as:

    The sheer force of the water is pretty awe inspiring. If you go out in

    one of theboats at the bottom youcan get a prettygood perspective

    of how powerful they are. The noise is unbelievable but not scary or

    anything, just impressive and kinda peaceful.

    5.1.5. Balance

    Tourists also judge the beauty of a destination along the di-

    mensionsHuman-toucheNo human touch, AuthenticeArticial, and

    CohesiveeOut of place, collectively forming the fth theme Bal-

    ance. Overall, this theme demonstrates the appropriateness of

    experiential cues to an environment. Specically, Human toucheNo

    human touch relatesto the suitability of visual cuesto thesetting.The

    presenceof human-inuenced elementscontributesto the beauty of

    urban destinations while viewed as ugly in the nature setting. As a

    40-year oldmale noted about his travel experience to the U.S. beach

    destination:youhave onebeautifulnatural scenery in onedirection

    and a parking lot in the other.The AuthenticeArticialdimension

    relates to the extent of perceived integrity of a destination to itsintrinsic properties while CohesiveeOut of place refers to overall

    cohesiveness, oruidow, of visualcuesat a destination.Thistheme

    has been previously explored in environmental psychology, as

    testied by equivalent terms such as Harmony (Galindo & Hidalgo,

    2005) and Compatibility (Kaplan et al., 1989).

    5.1.6. Diversity

    The dimensionDiverseeAlike, comprising the sixth theme Di-

    versity,indicates the variety of visual and other experiential cues

    during the tourism experience. Regardless of the destination type,

    DiverseeAlike is a dimension commonly discussed and most trav-

    elers consider diverse settings more beautiful. For example, the

    interview with a 51-year-old male discloses that:

    The reason I chose Hawaii over some other areas that I found

    particularly beautiful, mountains with snow, lakes, and.

    isbecause Hawaii has just about everything. Although I didnt

    experience snow in Hawaii, it does snow and youcan ski in Hawaii.

    It has mountains, it has lush tropical gardens and owers, it has

    beautiful ocean. Ocean has varying colors. For instance, if you go in

    summer, it looks green, almost brown water.

    This theme is related toKaplan et al.s (1989)Complexity; how-

    ever, in Kaplans matrix, Complexityalso accounts for ne and so-

    phisticated aspects of an environment in addition to the diverse

    aspect. It is worth mentioning that although the preference for di-

    versity was addressed for both urban and natural landscapes, trav-

    elers did emphasize the appreciation of a harmonious combination

    of the diverse features. The notion of balance closely resembles

    Kaplan et al.s (1989) Cohesivenessand, along with Complexity, are

    two of the dimensions in his matrix that appear to be prominent in

    tourist aesthetic judgment. A 20-year old male explained:

    Seattle is beautiful because of the combination of different things. I

    get to see everything I want to see at the same time. There is

    combination of motion and stillness but they blend really well. The

    borders between things were harmonious. They dont seemto try to

    disrupt the environment.

    5.1.7. Novelty

    Another remarkable dimension of tourist aesthetic judgment is

    NoveleTypical that comprises the seventh theme Novelty and

    exempli

    es the contrast between a familiar and a new

    K. Kirillova et al. / Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293 287

  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    7/12

    environment. As noted similarly in previous research, novelty and

    typicality are joint predictors for aesthetics judgment (Hekkert,

    Snelders, & van Wieringen, 2003). The sense of place identity can

    contribute to individuals aesthetic judgment, meaning that

    familiar places and landscapes tend to be perceived as more

    beautiful (McAndrew, 1998; Twigger-Ross & Uzzell, 1996). Thus,

    these researchers have advocated the notions of congruency and

    continuity in aesthetic judgment of a place. From the touristsperspective, however, it appears that novelty plays an important

    role in aesthetic judgment. Tourists consider novel, never-before-

    seen aspects of a destination as more beautiful than familiar

    landscape. This can be best illustrated in a 21-year old female s

    account of her Caribbean trip:

    I guess when I think that something is beautiful, I think of something

    thatis notmyeveryday life,and I feel that everydayis something that

    is very routine. In Indiana, the same stuff is everywhere,

    so.something new and peaceful. Thats, I guess, thatsmydenition

    of beauty, justrelaxing,peaceful. I dont likethecold,I dont likesnow,

    so anything thats like tropical and warm is like gorgeous, so

    yeah.down there it was not like my everyday life, it wasnt like.it

    wasnt like traveling to Florida or something.its still pretty,

    but.

    its stillprettykindof like everydayin a way..butdowntherewe

    had a car but wedidnt need itto go out toplaces,we kind of walked

    everywhere, and being right on a beach was just gorgeous.just

    being able to do stuff you dont get to do everyday-like we got to go

    snorkeling, paddle boating and.so, it was just absolutely gorgeous.

    5.1.8. Shape

    The eighth theme Shape is relatively less prominent as none of

    the classic dimensions of aesthetic judgment such as Coherence,

    Complexity, Openness, Roundness, and Symmetry seems to play a

    major role in tourist aesthetic judgment. This may again attest to

    the uniqueness of aesthetic judgment by tourists who are relocated

    and immersed in an atypical environment for an unusual experi-

    ence. The aesthetic experience at a tourist destination is not about

    visual observations only; instead, it engages all ve senses in theprocess of aesthetic judgment. Therefore, tourists are receptive to

    other perceptual cues of a destination, and such classic dimensions

    as RoundeAngular, SymmetriceAsymmetric, and Sophisticatede

    Simplisticbecome less central to the judgment.

    5.1.9. Uniqueness

    The last theme Uniqueness suggests that tourist aesthetic

    judgment appear to depend on whether the destination possesses

    uniquely identiable features, along the UniqueeOrdinarydimen-

    sion. Destinations with unique features tend to be perceived as

    beautiful and therefore possess a pull motivation force, as posited

    in the pushepull theory (Dann,1981). This nding is not surprising,

    given that novelty-seeking has been acknowledged as a funda-

    mental motive that prompts tourists (Krippendorf,1987). While thethemes Uniqueness and Novelty may appear related, they are dis-

    cussed quite differently by the interviewees. Uniqueness refers to a

    destinations feature that makes the place distinctively identiable.

    For example, several respondents indicated that Chicago Gate,

    known as the Bean, did not convey much aesthetic value on its

    own but it was unique to Chicago and therefore beautiful as a

    landmark attraction. Novelty, on the other hand, attests to those

    properties of a destination that are original and never experienced

    before by a tourist. Therefore, Uniqueness is relatively objective

    whereas Novelty, as the opposite of familiarity, is more subjective

    and varies among tourists. For instance, if a person grew up in a

    tropic island, he or she may perceive snowy mountain slopes as

    beautiful, more so than tourists from mountainous areas. This point

    is well illustrated by a 51-year old males account:

    Trees.the trees that really impressed me.made the biggest

    impression on me were the trees we saw around some waterfalls.

    They had these, I call them the roots dropping from the branches,

    they were very tall trees but they had these.they looked like binds

    that would go all the way down to the ground. Just once again,

    different unique to me, to my experience, if you grew up next to

    them, they are not, but they are unique to me. Yeah, these trees

    made the biggest impression on me but there were many different

    trees.

    fruit trees, there are many, but these are particularly

    memorable.

    It is worth noting that the interviewee utilizes the word

    unique to refer to the more subjective uniqueness which is

    better classied as Novelty. As an innatepursuit of tourists, novelty-

    seeking has been an important theoretical construct in explaining

    travel motivation and destination choice behavior (Cohen, 1979b).

    The desire for Novelty bears practical implications for destination

    management. Since the perceived level of novelty is relatively

    subjective and varies by tourists, destination planners should take

    into consideration the effect of geographic or cultural distance

    when attempting to identify and segment markets. New tourism

    products or services should be developed and marketed, forinstance on a seasonal base, to enhance the appeal of the destina-

    tion by attracting novelty seekers.

    Table 2 depicts the dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment

    along with their frequencies. Additionally, Fig. 1 graphically pre-

    sents the above results.

    In marketing philosophies and practices, product has been

    portrayed as bundles of attributes(e.g.Engel, Blackwell & Kollat,

    1978). Also, the possible presence of interactions among the attri-

    butes may inuence consumers response to the outside stimulus

    (Holbrook & Moore, 1981). Translated into the tourism context

    where the productcontains both tangible and intangible aspects,

    the judgment and appreciation of a destination tends to be expe-

    riential and holistic in nature. This may render the previously

    established dimensions of aesthetic judgment imperfectly trans-

    ferrable. The current study showed that respondents often linked

    the beauty of a tourism destination to other domains of life such as

    religion, history, poverty, safety, lifestyle, and other experiential

    dimensions of destination experience such as friendliness of local

    people. For example, inviting atmosphere, historical past, conve-

    niently located hotels, and a variety of activities in which a tourist

    can participate were frequently mentioned as contributor to the

    beautyof a destination.While these aspects do not directly attest to

    aesthetic judgmentper se, they illustrate the multifaceted nature of

    tourism experience. Since tourism experience can be understood as

    a service experience that involves delicate interplay between the

    experience of and satisfaction with leisure and tourism activities(Otto & Ritchie, 1996, p. 167), a successfully managed tourism

    experience should engage not only the key aesthetic cues, but also

    other experiential aspects at the destination.

    5.2. A framework of tourist aesthetic judgment

    Although the identied themes appear to be distinct from each

    other, a closer examination suggests that they do share underlining

    commonalities. Specically, the themes could be organized into a

    two-dimensional plane along two continuums: ConcreteeAbstract

    and SubjectiveeObjective (see Fig. 2). One could interpret the

    themes along the ConcreteeAbstract dimension as from being

    easily manipulated on a physical level (Concrete) to requiring

    higher order manipulation strategies (Abstract). The Subjectivee

    Objective dimension runs from the dimensions that are subject to

    interpersonal variance (Subjective) to those relatively more

    K. Kirillova et al. / Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293288

  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    8/12

    universally agreed (Objective). The quadrants are designated todemonstrate the relative nature and directions of the major themes

    in tourist aesthetic judgment. An important distinction should be

    made. The Objective anchor of the continuum connotes the ease of

    arriving to a similar judgment by people of distinct backgrounds. It

    still attests to the subjectivity of aesthetic judgment as proposed by

    the cognitive approach, rather than an objective nature of the

    judgment. It should be noted that the framework and positions of

    the themes were proposed as a result of elaborate discussions

    among the researchers and therefore are not absolute. Since the

    conceptualization entails researcherssubjective assessment of the

    data, it requires further empirical validation.

    The judgment of the Uniqueness and Scale of the destinations

    has been established more from an objective and abstract

    perspective (Quadrant I). When respondents discussed their

    aesthetic judgments under the two themes, they tended to refer to

    the destination qualities that are easy to observe and difcult to be

    modied. The dimensions constituting these themesare notas easyto be manipulated at the physical level.UniqueeOrdinaryunder the

    theme of Uniqueness and ColorfuleDarkunder the theme of Scale

    are examples of such. For example, it would not be appropriate for

    New York City to change its aesthetic quality from grand to quaint.

    It is possible, however, for destinations to augment their unique-

    ness. For instance, local festivals, special events, landmark buildings

    and monument could enhance the sense of place and thus its

    uniqueness. On the other end, Balance, Novelty, and Diversity are

    more of subjective conception of the destination aesthetics and in

    the meanwhile abstract. Similar to Quadrant I, the themes in

    quadrant II were elaborated by the respondents from an abstract

    perspective, as exemplied in NoveleTypical and DiverseeAlike.

    However, due to the subjective nature, these themes are not as easy

    for manipulation on a physical level and require higher order

    manipulation techniques such as careful implementation of mar-

    keting segmentation. By utilizing geographical segmentation, for

    Fig. 1. Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.

    Table 2

    Frequency of themes and dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.

    Dimensions Frequency Dimensions Frequency

    Scaletotal: 411 Diversitytotal: 123

    ColorfuleDull(intensity of color) 107 DiverseeAlike(variety of visual and other cues) 123

    GrandeQuaint(physical proportion) 104 Noveltytotal: 110

    Presence of PeopleeAbsence of People(degree of crowdedness) 91 NoveleTypical(contrast of familiar and new environment) 110

    AbundanceeScarcity(amount of visual cues) 61 Shape total: 97

    Opennesse

    Narrowness(spatial characteristics) 48 Sophisticatede

    Simplistic(degree of complexity) 84Timetotal: 218 RoundeAngular(shape of visual cues) 7

    ModerneHistoric(perceived age of a destination) 214 SymmetriceAsymmetric(degree of symmetry of visual cues) 6

    YoungeOld(perceived age of people observed) 4 Uniquenesstotal: 65

    Conditiontotal: 191 UniqueeOrdinary(amount of uniquely identiable features) 65

    CleaneDirty(perceived hygienic condition) 129

    Well-kepteRun-down(upkeep of physical attributes) 62

    Soundtotal: 186

    LivelyePeaceful(pace of sound) 62

    Human-madeeNature-made(source of sound) 65

    LoudeQuiet(volume of sound) 59

    Balancetotal: 143

    Human toucheNo human touch(suitability of visual cues to setting) 59

    AuthenticeArticial (extent of perceived integrity) 50

    CohesiveeOut of place(ow of visual cues) 34

    K. Kirillova et al. / Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293 289

  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    9/12

    instance,it is possible to targettourists driven by novelty in orderto

    maximize the possibility for positive aesthetic judgment. Similarly,

    psychographic segmentation could reach individuals with the po-

    tential to have positive aesthetic judgment in Balance and Diversity.

    Younger tourists, for instance, could favor a greater degree of di-versity than more mature travelers.

    Quadrants III and IV, while differ on the SubjectiveeObjective

    continuum, are of similar nature when it comes to the level of

    abstraction. Quadrant III presents the theme (Time) that could be

    manipulated and altered to suite the expectations of potential

    tourists, although such manipulations still require knowledge of

    the subjective assessment of tourists. At the same time, both

    ModerneHistoricand YoungeOldwere spoken of in a concrete way

    with examples such as building features and site-specic factors at

    the destinations. Thus, if a destination wishes to appear more

    modern, cobblestone instead of asphalt could be utilized to nish

    the main streets. Sound, Shape and Condition in destinations have

    been discussed objectively, too, but from a relatively more concrete

    standpoint. Similar to quadrant I, the themes in quadrant IV attestto relatively objectiveness but are associated with richer diversity

    of aesthetic judgment among tourists and the abstract nature of

    these dimensions. For example, Human-madeeNature-madeunder

    the theme of Sound and SymmetriceAsymmetricunder the theme of

    Shape testify to the objectivity and concreteness in respondents aesthetic judgment. The themes in this quadrant are the easiest to

    manipulate since well-maintained destinations attributes of classic

    shapes and pleasant sounds are generally preferred.

    6. Conclusion

    The current study unearths a new understanding of what

    makes a destination beautiful by dissecting the dimensions of

    tourist aesthetic judgment. As a pioneering attempt, it makestheoretical contribution to the existing knowledge base of aes-

    thetics literature. Based on ndings from 57 in-depth interviews,

    this study identied and presented nine themes of aesthetic

    judgment of tourist destination. They are Scale, Time, Condition,

    Sound, Balance, Diversity, Novelty, Shape, and Uniqueness. A

    theoretical framework of tourist aesthetic judgment was proposed,

    with the identied themes plotted along two continuums based on

    their theoretical relevance: AbstracteConcrete and Subjectivee

    Objective. Included in the nine themes are a variety of dimensions

    that pertain to both nature-based and urban destinations and do

    not replicate the existing, classic aesthetic dimensions previously

    discovered in general aesthetics literature. This suggests that

    tourism allows a unique appreciator-object dyad where in-

    dividuals are immersed in a setting in pursuit of an unusual, non-

    routine experience. As such, tourist aesthetic judgment is distinct

    from classic aesthetic assessment in art works where the appreci-

    ator is a purely outside observer of the objects. The beauty of

    tourism destination is uniquely judged, admired, and appreciated.

    Tourist assessment of the beauty goes beyond the visual aspects,

    and engages all senses. This stance coincides well with Urrys

    (2002)argument which posited that tourist experience involves a

    variety of sensescapes, including soundscapes, smellscapes, taste-

    capes, and touch, in addition to the conventional conception of

    tourist gazeand sightseeing.

    Although it was not the goal of this study to compare and

    contrast aesthetic judgment of nature-based and urban destina-

    tions, this research indirectly explored the dimensions of aesthetic

    judgment in the context of both nature and urban tourist destina-

    tions. All the identied themes appear to be equally salient in both

    contexts. Nevertheless, while tourists employ the same dimensions

    in their aesthetic judgment, they seem to apply different criteria

    across different destination types. In the Presence of peoplee

    Absence of people dimension, for instance, interviewees tend to

    assign a greater aesthetic value to nature-based places with little

    human presence. To the contrary, urban destinations are perceived

    as less beautiful if they are not populous. In a nature-based setting,

    quiet and peaceful sound originated from a natural source (e.g.owing water, birds, animals, etc.) and pristine scenery without

    visible traces of human activity (a me and nature wilderness

    experience) is considered as more beautiful. In an urban environ-

    ment, lively human-made sounds, sophisticated layouts, and

    presence of human activity are perceived as beautiful because they

    addto the image of an urbanplace. This distinctionbetween natural

    and urban destination is presented here as an emergent nding

    since the nature of collected data did not provide a basis for sys-

    tematic comparison between the two.

    The ndings of this study also bear pragmatic implications for

    the planning, branding, and management of destination experi-

    ence. Marketing of sensory experiences, which include attractive

    visual and other stimuli, can contribute to an organization or a

    brands identity (Schmitt & Simonson, 2009). Although to variousdegrees, tourism destinations can be considered to be aesthetic

    products, and, as such, they facilitate experiential consumption.

    Similar to aesthetics in the service environment, tourism aesthetics

    is an important value-added component in tourism experience and

    may serve as a major satiser of tourist experiential needs. Tourism

    aesthetics may also exert inuence over long-term attitudinal and

    behavioral attributes of tourists, such as loyalty. The aesthetic

    judgment dimensions uncovered can form the basis of a potentially

    valuable toolkit for destination planning, marketing and manage-

    ment. For example, an aesthetic-laden strategic approach can

    accentuate the identity of the destination and create unique posi-

    tioning among competitors. In a market saturated with similar

    destination experience offerings, the strategic implications of

    tourism aesthetics need to be factored into marketersbranding

    and marketing considerations. This study also found that although

    tourists utilize classical and previously explored dimensions while

    executing aesthetic judgments, the dimensions that are uniquely

    pertinent to the tourism experience appear to be more inuential

    in their aesthetic gratication. Specically, the dimensions related

    to scale and sound, uniqueness, perceived authenticity, and time of

    a destination appear to be more important than the dimensions

    related to shape, cohesiveness, and complexity. At a more abstract

    level, it seems that more experiential themes such as Diversity,

    Novelty, Sound, Uniqueness, Scale, and Time are particularly salient

    in tourism aesthetics as compared to environmental aesthetics.

    This and other ndings provide direct implication for destination

    management practices. Although the statement of all perceptions

    start with the eye

    holds true in general aesthetic research (Schmitt

    Fig. 2. A framework of tourist aesthetic judgment.

    K. Kirillova et al. / Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293290

  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    10/12

    & Simonson, 2009, p. 85), a variety of multifaceted sensory expe-

    riences seem to be involved in tourism.

    Overall, the proposed nine-theme, two-dimension framework

    lays the groundwork for a better understanding of tourist aesthetic

    judgment in both nature-based and urban destinations, and

    accordingly facilitate the formulation of effective marketing stra-

    tegies, including product development, brand communication

    messages, and market segmentation. Since tourism experience in

    essence involves our aesthetic or sensual existence (Quan & Wang,

    2004) and such aesthetic experience is closely linked to tourists overall satisfaction as suggested by our ndings, destination plan-

    ners need to understand the specicities of consumer judgment of

    what is beautiful when designing and packaging tourism expe-

    rience. General marketing practices have focused on isolated at-

    tributes and benets, due to the lack of a broad consideration of

    brand positioning (Schmitt & Simonson, 2009). Guided by the two-

    dimensional framework, destination managers could cater to the

    full spectrum of tourists aesthetic enjoyment while crafting and

    implementing marketing strategies. While destinations are

    increasingly perceived as undifferentiated due to their typical of-

    ferings, aesthetics can become a key differentiating element in the

    competitive market. By satisfying tourist aesthetic and experiential

    needs, destinations can establish a powerful point of differentiationto produce desirable customer impressions.

    The framework also offers guidance for tourism destination

    planning when deciding how to maximize key visual and other

    perceptual cues that are favorable to aesthetic judgments, and

    minimize the distracting elements. Since aesthetic judgments are

    in essence subjective, tourism planners should carefully evaluate

    potential target markets in terms of how background charac-

    teristics could inuence judgments of destinations aesthetic

    qualities. This information needs to be reconciled with the des-

    tinations tourism attraction inventory, and the conclusion of

    whether the aesthetic qualities could result in positive aesthetic

    judgments should be made. If the match is not likely, destination

    planners need to consider if aesthetic qualities are to be changed

    or a different target market is to be selected. For instance, if themismatch occurs on the dimensions located in Quadrants III and

    IV (Time, Condition, Shape, and Sound), a planner may choose to

    modify certain aspects of the destination to cater to the prefer-

    ences of the target market. On the other hand, if the mismatch

    arises on the dimensions located in Quadrants I and II (Scale,

    Uniqueness, Balance, Novelty, and Diversity), it may not be

    feasible to change aesthetic qualities of the destination, given

    destinations nancial and time constraints. In such situations,

    selection of a different target market could be strategic. Hence,

    this framework can be useful for a destination to reconcile its

    current supply with potential demand so as to improve tourist

    experience quality while sustaining a competitive advantage of

    this destination.

    The value of the ndings extends beyond the tourism domain.The results contribute to the existing literature in environmental

    psychology by establishing how aesthetic judgments are made

    under new and less familiar environmental conditions. Given that

    this study identied several dimensions not previously found in the

    literature and noted that the beauty of home environment is closely

    associated with residentssatisfaction and quality of life (Widgery,

    1982), psychologists maynd it important to determine the roles of

    these dimensions in residents aesthetic judgments. Additionally,

    the current results could inform research in leisure studies as one

    does not have to travel far from home to have an aesthetic expe-

    rience. For example, providing diverse and unique recreational

    environments within the same geographical locale (urban or na-

    ture) could augment restorative properties of recreational activities

    and thus improve residentsquality of life.

    6.1. Limitations and future research

    Like other studies, this research is not free of limitations. First,

    the presented results are derived utilizing a theoretical sample.

    While every attempt was made to diversify the sample, it is still

    possible that certain dimensions did not emerge in this research

    but are still of great importance in tourist aesthetic judgment.

    Second, as suggested byHume (1757/2013), aesthetic judgment is

    notonly in essence subjective but also highly dependenton cultural

    background of an appreciator. With the focus on the U.S. re-

    spondents, the study did not consider cross-cultural issues of

    aesthetic judgment. Additionally, as an exploratory study, it neither

    accounted for personal and socio-demographic characteristics of

    the respondents nor investigated travel motivation and behavior.

    Finally, the conceptual framework is developed based on explor-

    atory inquiry and therefore it requires further empirical validation,

    particularly, more rigorous examination of relative positioning of

    the themes along the continuums. Nonetheless, this framework of

    tourist aesthetic judgment represents an initial conceptualization

    of what constitutes aesthetic judgment of tourism destinations.

    Future research is invited to validate and extend the dimensions

    identied in this study, across different cultures and/or in different

    market segments. Specically, aesthetic judgment could be linkedto such outcome variables as satisfaction, revisit intentions, and

    destination choice. It would be also of interest to investigate how

    aesthetic judgment inuences travel motivation and behavior

    across various socio-demographic segments. Another avenue for

    further research is to establish conceptual and empirical relation-

    ships between aesthetic judgment and destination image. Future

    studies are encouraged to engage in systematic comparison of

    aesthetic judgments of nature-based and urban destinations to

    validate and expand the preliminary results emerged in this study.

    With availability of funding, researchers could utilize quasi-

    experimental design to establish causal relationship between

    destination attributes and aesthetic judgment in both types of

    destinations. Additionally, empirical validations are needed to see if

    certain dimensions are perceived with higher importance in tour-ists decision making and/or more readily applicable in actual

    marketing practices. Finally, developing a scale to measure

    perceived aesthetic qualities of destinations would be a logical next

    step in future research.

    References

    Albayrak, T., & Caber, M. (2013). The symmetric and asymmetric inuences ofdestination attributes on overall visitor satisfaction. Current Issue in Tourism,16(2), 149e166.

    Alegre, J., & Garau, J. (2010). Tourism satisfaction and dissatisfaction. Annals ofTourism Research, 37(1), 52e73.

    Appleton, J. (1975). The experience of landscapes. New York: John Wiley.Arriaza,M.,Canas-Ortega,J., Canas-Madueno,J.,& Ruiz-Aviles,P.(2004).Assessingthe

    visual quality of rural landscapes.Landscape and Urban Planning, 69(1), 115e125.

    Baker, J., Grewal, D., & Parasuraman, A. (1994). The inuence of store environmenton quality inferences and store image. Journal of the Academy of MarketingScience, 22(4), 328e339.

    Baloglu, S., & McCleary, K. (1999). A model of destination image formation. Annals ofTourism Research, 24(6), 868e897.

    Baloglu, S., Pekcan, A., & Santos, J. (2004). The relationship between destinationperformance, overall satisfaction, and behavioral intention for distinct seg-ments.Journal of Quality Assurance in Hospitality and Tourism, 4(3e4), 149e165.http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J162v04n03_10.

    Beardsley, M. (1975). Aesthetics from classical Greece to the present (Vol. 13). Uni-versity Alabama Press.

    Becker, P. (1993). Common pitfalls in published grounded theory research. Quali-tative Health Research, 3(2), 254e260.

    Berleant, A. (2005). Aesthetics and environment: variations on a theme . Burlington,VT: Ashgate Publishing Company.

    Bitner, M. (1992). Servicescapes: the impact of physical surroundings on customersand employees. Journal of Marketing, 56, 57e71.

    Bloch, P. (1995). Seeking the ideal form: product design and consumer choice.

    Journal of Marketing, 59 , 19e

    29.

    K. Kirillova et al. / Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293 291

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref2http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref2http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref2http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref2http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref2http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref3http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref3http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref3http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref3http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref4http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref4http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref4http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref4http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref4http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref4http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref6http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref6http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref6http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref6http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref6http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref6http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J162v04n03_10http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref8http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref8http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref8http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref8http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref9http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref9http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref9http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref9http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref9http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref10http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref10http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref10http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref10http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref10http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref11http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref11http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref11http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref11http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref11http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref11http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref12http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref12http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref12http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref12http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref12http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref12http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref12http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref12http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref11http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref11http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref11http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref10http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref10http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref9http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref9http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref9http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref8http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref8http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J162v04n03_10http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref6http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref6http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref6http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref5http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref4http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref4http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref4http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref3http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref2http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref2http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref2http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref1
  • 8/10/2019 What makes a destination beautiful_Dimensions of tourist aesthetic judgment.pdf

    11/12

    Bloch, P., Brunel, F., & Arnold, T. (2003). Individual differences in the centrality ofvisual product aesthetics: concept and measurement. Journal of ConsumerResearch, 29(4), 551e565.

    Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. QualitativeResearch in Psychology, 3(2), 77e101.

    Budd, M. (2002). The aesthetic appreciation of nature: Essays on the aesthetics ofnature. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.

    Cackowski, J., & Nasar, J. (2003). The restorative effects of roadside vegetation im-plications for automobile driver anger and frustration. Environment andBehavior, 35(6), 736e751.

    Carlson, A. (1979). Appreciation and the natural environment. Journal of Aestheticsand Art Criticism, 37, 267e275.

    Carlson, A., & Lintott, S. (2008). Nature, aesthetics, and environmentalism. New York:Columbia University Press.

    Carroll, N. (1995). On being moved by nature: between religion and natural history.In S. Kemal, & I. Gaskell (Eds.), Landscape, natural beauty, and the arts(pp. 244e266). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Charters, C. (2006). Aesthetic products and aesthetic consumption: a review. Con-sumption, Markets, and Culture, 9(3), 235e255.

    Chi, C., & Qu, H. (2008). Examining the structural relationships of destination image,tourist satisfaction and destination loyalty: an integrated approach. TourismManagement, 29(4), 624e636.

    Coeterier, J. (1996). Dominant attributes in the perception and evaluation of theDutch landscape. Landscape and Urban Planning, 34(1), 27e44.

    Cohen, E. (1979a). A phenomenology of tourist experiences. Sociology, 13(2),179e201.

    Cohen, E. (1979b). Rethinking the sociology of tourism. Annals of Tourism Research,6(1), 18e35.

    Coyne, I. (1997). Sampling in qualitative research. Purposeful and theoreticalsampling: merging or clear boundaries? Journal of Advanced Nursing, 26(3),623e630.

    Creswell, J. (2008). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methodapproaches (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Creusen, M., & Schoormans. (2005). The different roles of product appearance inconsumer choice. Journal of Product Innovation Management, 22, 63e81.

    Daniel, T. (2001). Whither scenic beauty? Visual landscape quality assessment inthe 21st century. Landscape and Urban Planning, 54(1e4), 267e281.

    Dann, G. (1981). Tourist motivation an appraisal. Annals of Tourism Research, 8(2),187e219.

    DeLucio, J., & Mgica, M. (1994). Landscape preferences and behaviour of visitors toSpanish national parks. Landscape and Urban Planning, 29(2), 145e160.

    Echtner, C., & Ritchie, J. (1991). The meaning and measurement of destination im-age. Journal of Tourism Studies, 2(2), 2e12.

    Engel, J., Roger, D., & Kollat, D. (1978). Consumer behavior(3rd ed.). Hinsdale, IL:Dryden Press.

    Fyhri, A., Jacobsen, J., & Tmmervik, H. (2009). Touristslandscape perceptions andpreferences in a Scandinavian coastal region. Landscape and Urban Planning,

    91(4), 202e

    211.Galindo, M., & Hidalgo, M. (2005). Aesthetic preferences and the attribution ofmeaning: environmental categorization processes in the evaluation of urbanscenes.International Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 19e27.

    Galindo, M., & Rodriguez, J. (2000). Environmental aesthetics and psychologicalwellbeing: relatinoships between preference judgments for urban landscapesand other relevant affective responses. Psychology in Spain, 4(1), 13e27.

    Ginsborg, H. (2013). Kants aesthetics and teleology. Retrieved from Stanford Ency-clopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/kant-aesthetics/.

    Godlovitch, S. (2004). Icebreakers: environmentalism and natural aesthetics. InA. Carlson, & A. Berleant (Eds.), The aesthetics of natural environments(pp.108e126). Ontario: Broadview Press.

    Ha, J., & Jang, S. (2012). The effects of dining atmospherics on behavioral intentionsthrough quality perception. Journal of Services Marketing, 26(3), 204e215.

    Hammitt, W. (1981). The familiarity-preference component of on-site recreationalexperiences.Leisure Sciences, 4(2), 177e193.

    Hekkert, P., Snelders, D., & van Wieringen, P. (2003). Most advanced, yet accept-able: typicality and novelty as joint predictors of aesthetic preference in in-dustrial design. British Journal of Psychology, 94 , 111e124.

    Hepburn, R. (1966). Contemporary aesthetics and the neglect of natural beauty. InBritish analytical philosophy (pp. 285e310). London, UK: Routledge & KeganPaul Ltd.

    Hirschman, E., & Holbrook, M. (1982). Hedonic consumption: emerging concepts,methods and propositions. Journal of Marketing, 92e101.

    Holbrook, M., & Moore, W. (1981). Feature interactions in consumer judgments ofverbal versus pictorial presentations. Journal of Consumer Research, 8, 103e113.

    Holbrook, M., & Zirlin, R. (1985). Artistic creation, artworks, and aesthetic appre-ciation: some philosophical contributions to nonprot marketing. Advances inNonprot Marketing, 1, 1e54.

    Horayangkura, V. (1978). Semantic dimensional structures a methodologicalapproach.Environment and Behavior, 10(4), 555e584.

    Howley, P. (2011). Landscape aesthetics: assessing the general publicspreferencestowards rural landscapes. Ecological Economics, 72, 161e169.

    Hubbard, H., & Kimball, T. (1917). An introduction to the study of landscape design.New York: Macmillan.

    Hume, D. (1757/2013). Of the standard of taste . The Birmingham Free Press.Ittelson, W. (1978). Environmental perception and urban experience. Environment

    and Behavior, 10(2), 193e213.

    Jacobsen, T., Schubotz, R., Hfel, L., & Cramon, D. (2006). Brain correlates of aestheticjudgment of beauty.Neuroimage, 29(1), 276e285.

    Joffe, H., & Yardley, L. (2004). Content and thematic analysis. In D. Marks, &L. Yardley (Eds.),Research methods for clinical and health psychology(pp. 56e68).London, UK: Sage Publications.

    Jones, P. (1991).Taste today. New York, NY: Pergamon Press.Kant, I. (1790/1987). Critique of judgment. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett.Kaplan, S. (1982). Where cognition and affect meet: a theoretical analysis of pref-

    erence. In P. Bart, A. Chen, & G. Francescato (Eds.), Knowledge for design (pp.183e188). Washington, DC: Evironmental Design Research Association.

    Kaplan, R. (1985). The analysis of perception via preference: a strategy for studyinghow the environment is experienced. Landscape Planning, 12, 161e176.

    Kaplan, S. (1987). Aesthetics, affect, and cognition: environmental preference froman evolutionary perspective. Environment and Behavior, 19(1), 3e32.

    Kaplan, S., Kaplan, R., & Brown, T. (1989). Environmental preference: a comparisonof four domains of predictors. Environment and Behavior, 21(5), 509e530.

    Kozak, M. (2003). Measuring tourist satisfaction with multiple destination attri-butes. Tourism Analysis, 7(3/4), 229e240.

    Krippendorf, J. (1987). The holiday makers: Understanding the impact of leisure andtravel. London: William Heinemann.

    Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, D. (2004). A model of aestheticappreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology, 95,489e508.

    Lee, S., Jeon, S., & Kim, D. (2011). The impact of tour quality and tourist satisfactionon tourist loyalty: the case of Chinese tourists in Korea. Tourism Management,

    32(5), 1115e1124.Lehto, X. (2013). Assessing the perceived restorative qualities of vacation destina-

    tions. Journal of Travel Research, 3, 1e15.Li, Y. (2000). Geographical consciousness and tourism experience.Annals of Tourism

    Research, 27(4), 863e

    883.Lincoln, Y., & Guba, E. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Liu,Y. (2003).Engineeringaestheticsand aesthetic ergonomics:theoreticalfoundations

    and a dual-process research methodology. Ergonomics, 46(13e14), 1273e1292.Liu,Y., & Jang, S. (2009). The effects of dining atmospherics:an extendedMehrabiane

    Russell model.International Journal of Hospitality Management, 28(4), 494e503.Lovelock, C., & Wirtz, J. (2004). Services marketing: People, technology, strategy.

    Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Lue, C., Crompton, J., & Fesenmaier, D. (1993). Conceptualization of multi-

    destination pleasure trips. Annals of Tourism Research, 20(2), 289e301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0160-7383(93)90056-9 .

    Maitland, R., & Smith, A. (2009). Tourism and the aesthetics of the built environ-ment. In J. Tribe (Ed.),Philosophical issues in tourism (pp. 171e190). Bristol, UK:Channel View Publishing.

    Mannell, R., & Iso-Ahola, S. (1987). Psychological nature of leisure and tourismexperience.Annals of Tourism Research, 14(3), 314e331.

    McAndrew, F. (1998). The measurement of rootedness and the prediction ofattachment to home-towns in college students. Journal of Environmental Psy-

    chology, 18(4), 409e

    417.Mkono, M., Markwell, K., & Wilson, E. (2013). Applying Quan and Wang s structuralmodel of the tourist experience: a Zimbabwean ethnography of food tourism.Tourism Management Perspectives, 5, 68e74.

    Nasar, J. (1994). Urban design aesthetics: the evaluative qualities of building exte-riors.Environment and Behavior, 26(3), 377e401.

    Nasar, J. (1998). The evaluative image of the city. Thousand Oaks, CA: SagePublications.

    Nassauer, J. (1995). Culture and changing landscape structure. Landscape Ecology,10(4), 229e237.

    Nisbett, R. (2003). The geography of thought: how Asians and Westerners thinkdifferently and why?New York, NY: Free Press.

    Oh, H., Fiore, A. M., & Jeoung, M. (2007). Measuring experience economy concepts:tourism applications. Journal of Travel Research, 46(2), 119e132.

    Otto, J., & Ritchie, J. (1996). The service experience in tourism. Tourism Management,17(3), 165e174.

    OLeary, S., & Deegan, J. (2003). People, pace, place: qualitative and quantitativeimages of Ireland as a tourism destination in France. Journal of Vacation Mar-keting, 9(3), 213e226.

    Pine, B., & Gilmore, J. (1998). Welcome to the experience economy. Harvard BusinessReview, 76, 97e105.

    Quan, S., & Wang, N. (2004). Towards a structural model of the tourist experience: anillustrationfrom foodexperiences intourism.TourismManagement, 25(3),297e305.

    Riemann, M., Zaichkowsky, J., Neuhaus, C., Bender, T., & Weber, B. (2010). Aestheticpackage design: a behavioral, neural, and psychological investigation.Journal ofConsumer Psychology, 20(4), 431e441.

    Rogge, E., Nevens, F., & Gulinck, H. (2007). Perception of rural landscapes in Flan-ders: looking beyond aesthetics.Landscape and Urban Planning, 82, 159e174.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2007.02.006 .

    Sandelowski, M. (1995). Sample size in qualitative research. Research in Nursing &Health, 18(2), 179e183.

    Schmitt, B., & Simonson, A. (2009).Marketing aesthetics: The strategic managementof brands, identity and image. New York: Free Press.

    Scott, A. (2002). Assessing public perception of landscape: the LANDMAP experi-ence. Landscape Research, 27(3), 271e295.

    Silvia, P., & Barona, C. (2009). Do people prefer curved objects? Angularity, exper-tise, and aesthetic preference. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 27(1), 25e42.http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/EM.27.1.b.

    K. Kirillova et al. / Tourism Management 42 (2014) 282e293292

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref13http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref13http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref13http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref13http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref13http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref13http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref14http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref14http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref14http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref14http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref14http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref14http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref15http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref15http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref15http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref15http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref16http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref16http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref16http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref16http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref16http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref16http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref16http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref17http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref17http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref17http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref17http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref17http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref17http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref18http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref18http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref18http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref18http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref19http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref19http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref19http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref19http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref19http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref19http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref20http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref20http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref20http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref20http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref20http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref21http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref21http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref21http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref21http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref21http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref21http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref22http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref22http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref22http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref22http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref22http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref22http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref23http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref23http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref23http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref23http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref23http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref23http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref24http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref24http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref24http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref24http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref24http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref25http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref25http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref25http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref25http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref25http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref25http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref26http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref26http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref26http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref26http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref27http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0261-5177(13)00218-5/sref27http://refh