victoria’s dirty secret: how sociocultural norms influence adolescent girls and women

15
http://psp.sagepub.com Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin DOI: 10.1177/0146167207310457 2008; 34; 288 Pers Soc Psychol Bull Erin J. Strahan, Adèle Lafrance, Anne E. Wilson, Nicole Ethier, Steven J. Spencer and Mark P. Zanna Victoria's Dirty Secret: How Sociocultural Norms Influence Adolescent Girls and Women http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/34/2/288 The online version of this article can be found at: Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: Society for Personality and Social Psychology can be found at: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Additional services and information for http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://psp.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/34/2/288 Citations at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Upload: zovem-se-hristina

Post on 08-Sep-2015

8 views

Category:

Documents


4 download

DESCRIPTION

Erin J. StrahanAdèle LafranceAnne E. WilsonNicole EthierWilfrid Laurier UniversitySteven J. SpencerMark P. ZannaUniversity of Waterloo

TRANSCRIPT

  • http://psp.sagepub.comPersonality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    DOI: 10.1177/0146167207310457 2008; 34; 288 Pers Soc Psychol Bull

    Erin J. Strahan, Adle Lafrance, Anne E. Wilson, Nicole Ethier, Steven J. Spencer and Mark P. Zanna Victoria's Dirty Secret: How Sociocultural Norms Influence Adolescent Girls and Women

    http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/34/2/288 The online version of this article can be found at:

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    On behalf of:

    Society for Personality and Social Psychology

    can be found at:Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Additional services and information for

    http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts:

    http://psp.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/34/2/288 Citations

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 288

    Victorias Dirty Secret: HowSociocultural Norms InfluenceAdolescent Girls and Women

    Erin J. StrahanAdle LafranceAnne E. Wilson Nicole EthierWilfrid Laurier University

    Steven J. SpencerMark P. ZannaUniversity of Waterloo

    for ideal appearance that are pervasive in society and par-ticularly directed at women. These norms convey the mes-sage that women are valued for their bodies andappearance, that standards for attractiveness are veryhigh, and that it is reasonable to measure oneself against,and strive for, such standards (Rodin et al., 1985).

    Role of Media Messages in Womens Body Image

    The media have been harshly criticized for creating andperpetuating a cultural standard for unrealistic thinness andbeauty that is difficult, if not impossible, for most women toattain. For example, the average American woman is 5 ft 4in. and 140 lb, whereas the average model is 5 ft 11 in. and117 lb (National Eating Disorders Association, 2002).Researchers also find that fashion models are thinner than98% of American women (Smolak, 1996). Correlational

    Authors Note: Adle Lafrance is now at the Ontario Institute for Studiesin Education of the University of Toronto. Nicole Ethier is now at theUniversity of Waterloo. The research in this article was supported bygrants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council to thethird, fifth, and sixth authors. Thanks to the McCulloch Group for theirassistance with the statistics in Study 2 and to Jennifer Crocker and PamelaSadler for their helpful comments on the research. Correspondence con-cerning this article should be addressed to Erin J. Strahan, Wilfrid LaurierUniversity, Brantford Campus, 73 George Street, Brantford, Ontario,Canada N3T 2Y3; e-mail: [email protected].

    PSPB, Vol. 34 No. 2, February 2008 288-301DOI: 10.1177/0146167207310457 2008 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

    The present studies tested whether the salience of socio-cultural norms for ideal appearance leads women tobase their self-worth more strongly on appearance,which in turn leads them to feel more concerned withothers perceptions and less satisfied with their bodies.Study 1 tested this model by manipulating the salienceof the sociocultural norm among female universitystudents. The model was supported. In Study 2 an inter-vention challenging the legitimacy of the socioculturalnorm was delivered to female and male adolescents.Compared to controls, females who received this inter-vention were less accepting of the sociocultural normsfor appearance, based their self-worth less strongly onappearance, and in turn were less concerned with oth-ers perceptions and were more satisfied with their bod-ies. The implications for women are discussed.

    Keywords: sociocultural norms for appearance; contingencies ofself-worth; body image; media images; intervention

    In Western society, women feel tremendous pressure tobe thin. Girls often start dieting as young as 10 years oldand this preoccupation with weight and body shape is evi-dent across the life span (Pliner, Chaiken, & Flett, 1990).In fact, womens dissatisfaction with their bodies is sowidespread that some theorists refer to it as a normativediscontent (Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985).Theorists and researchers have suggested that womensbody dissatisfaction is influenced by sociocultural norms

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • studies have shown that exposure to the media is negativelyrelated to body satisfaction (Hofschire & Greenberg, 2002)and positively related to internalization of a slender idealbody shape (Stice, Schupak-Neuberg, Shaw, & Stein, 1994).

    Researchers have also investigated the causal role ofmedia images by experimentally manipulating exposure tothese images. Reviews of this literature have suggested thatthe relation between media exposure and body image is notentirely straightforward (Levine & Smolak, 1998). Severalstudies demonstrate that exposure to ideal images increasesbody dissatisfaction (Irving, 1990; Stice & Shaw, 1994).However, other studies have found little or no impact ofexposure to idealized media images on body satisfaction(Champion & Furnham, 1999). Still other researchers havefound that the effect of exposure to ideal images is moderatedby individual differences or social factors (D. Henderson-King, E. Henderson-King, & Hoffman, 2001; Mills, Polivy,Herman, & Tiggeman, 2002) and can in fact have a posi-tive impact on body satisfaction under certain conditions.Although these findings paint a fuzzy picture of the role ofthe media in females dissatisfaction with their bodies, arecent meta-analysis suggests that the media most typicallyhave a negative impact on women (Groesz, Levine, &Murnen, 2002). Groesz et al. (2002) examined 43 effectsreported in 25 studies and found that women who wereexposed to idealized media images felt significantly moredissatisfied with their bodies than women who wereexposed to control images.

    Other researchers have examined mechanisms thathelp account for the relation between media and bodyimage, such as thin-ideal internalization (e.g., Heinberg& Thompson, 1995) and social comparison processes(e.g., Tiggemann & McGill, 2004). First, the extent towhich women internalize societys standards for thin-ness (thin-ideal internalization) is a risk factor for eatingdisorders, and women who internalize these standardshave lower body satisfaction after exposure to thinmedia images. Thin-ideal internalization has also beenshown to mediate the relation between media exposureand body satisfaction (Stice et al., 1994).

    Second, social comparison processes may help accountfor the impact of media images on womens self-views.Research demonstrates that women compare themselveswith the unrealistically high standards presented in themedia (Strahan, Wilson, Cressman, & Buote, 2006).Furthermore, the number of social comparisons womenmake after exposure to ideal images partially mediatesthe images effect on body dissatisfaction and negativemood (Tiggemann & McGill, 2004).

    Contingent Self-Worth

    In the current research we investigate an additionalmechanism: We suggest that sociocultural norms affect

    womens self-image by increasing the degree to whichthey base their self-worth on physical appearance.Theorists and researchers have long recognized that suc-cesses and failures in personally valued domains deter-mine self-worth far more than outcomes in moreinconsequential arenas (James, 1890/1950; Kernis, 2003).Indeed, Crocker and Wolfes (2001) Contingencies ofSelf-Worth Scale measures individual differences inpeoples tendency to base their self-worth on domainsincluding academic achievement, familys acceptance,moral virtue, and appearance. Individual differences inself-worth contingencies predict reactions to domain-specific outcomes. For example, students who werehighly contingent on academics experienced greaterself-esteem fluctuations following acceptances andrejections from graduate schools (Crocker, Sommers, &Luhtanen, 2002).

    Crocker (2002) theorizes that external contingenciesof self-worth put people at risk for negative outcomesbecause they lead people to look to others for valida-tion. Past research points to the role of external contin-gencies in body image. Sanchez and Crocker (2005)found that individuals who wanted to resemble theirgenders ideal appearance reported greater external con-tingencies of self-esteem, which led to lower global self-esteem and more disordered eating symptomatology. Inaddition, Patrick, Neighbors, and Knee (2004) foundthat women with self-esteem based on external factorssuch as achievement and appearance compared them-selves to models more readily than those low in contin-gent self-esteem, resulting in more negative affect.

    The Current Research

    Our research differs from this past work in two ways.First, we examine one specific external contingencyappearance-contingent self-worthrather than externalcontingencies in general (i.e., achievement, othersacceptance, appearance) as past work has done(Sanchez & Crocker, 2005) because we argue thatsociocultural norms for appearance should target thisspecific domain. Second, we treat contingent self-worthas a mediator variable that may be affected by situa-tional context rather than as a preexisting individualdifference. Although there is ample evidence of chronicindividual differences in contingencies of self-worth(Crocker & Wolfe, 2001), to our knowledge, this is thefirst research to suggest that situational factors mayinfluence the degree to which people base their self-worth on a particular domain. For example, the sameindividual might base his or her self-worth morestrongly on academics while immersed in a study groupand more strongly on appearance after reading a fash-ion magazine.

    Strahan et al. / HOW SOCIOCULTURAL NORMS INFLUENCE WOMEN 289

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    boginja univerzumaHighlight

  • 290 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

    In sum, we propose that sociocultural norms forappearance suggest to women that their value is deter-mined by their appearance. Thus, exposure to mediaimages reflecting these norms should lead women tobase their self-worth more strongly on appearance,which in turn should reduce body satisfaction andincrease concern with others opinions (see Figure 1).

    The direct effects that we propose in our model areconsistent with past research. A meta-analysis of pastresearch has demonstrated that ideal images most typi-cally lead women to feel dissatisfied with their bodies (Groeszet al., 2002). In addition, Fredrickson and Robertss (1987)body objectification theory posits that females learn thatphysical appearance is valued by others and as a resultthey are socialized to take a third-person observer per-spective on their own bodies. This perspective leadswomen to feel self-conscious and to habitually monitorhow they appear to others. Fredrickson and Roberts arguethat media depictions of women as objects contribute towomens feelings of objectification. We suggest that idealimages in the media that highlight these socioculturalnorms may lead women to worry about whether they arethin or attractive enough in the eyes of others and, moregenerally, may lead women to feel concerned about howother people view them.

    More novel is our contention that salience of socio-cultural norms influences appearance-contingent self-worth, which in turn affects body satisfaction andconcern with others perceptions. Despite the Kelloggsadvertisement pronouncing, A womans value shouldnot be determined by the pound, the predominantmessage conveyed in popular media is that a womansworth can be determined by her weight and appearance.Women may conclude that if society values them fortheir appearance, then they, too, must determine theirworth by how they look. Although women tend to acceptsociocultural appearance norms as legitimate (Crocker,Cornwell, & Major, 1993), this ideal is difficult, if not

    impossible, for most women to attain. These normsshould lead women to base their self-worth on adomain in which they are virtually guaranteed to fallshort. As women become more invested in appearanceas a basis for self-worth, they should become moreevaluative of their appearance (leading to dissatisfactionin light of the high cultural standards). They may alsobecome painfully aware that others might judge themharshly on the same grounds.

    Past research has demonstrated that people withchronically high external contingencies of self-worth aremore vulnerable to media and gender ideals (Sanchez &Crocker, 2005). If we find that situational exposure tonorms can actually increase appearance contingencies,this process has important implications: It suggests thatunder the right circumstances any woman could becomevulnerable to the costs associated with basing self-worthon external contingencies.

    Overview of Studies

    In Study 1 we manipulated the salience of sociocul-tural norms for ideal appearance in the lab. We testedwhether exposure to images reflecting these norms hada direct effect on body satisfaction and concern withothers perceptions, and whether appearance-contingentself-worth accounted for these effects. Study 2 was afield study conducted at local public schools. Ratherthan manipulating salience of sociocultural norms, wecreated an intervention that challenged the legitimacy ofsociocultural norms for ideal appearance. We testedwhether adolescents who received this interventionbased their self-worth less strongly on appearance,which in turn led to greater body satisfaction and lessconcern about others opinions.

    STUDY 1

    First, we tested whether appearance contingencies ofself-worth would mediate the impact of ideal images onwomens body satisfaction and concern with others per-ceptions. We focused on young undergraduate womenwho are thought to be among those most susceptible tomedia impact (Groesz et al., 2002). Although weacknowledge that men may also be influenced by ideal-ized images in the media (Strahan et al., 2006), researchsuggests that norms still target women more pervasivelyand that women are more at risk for negative effects(Fallon & Rozin, 1985).

    In this first study, we manipulated the salience ofsociocultural norms for ideal appearance by exposingsome women to media images that strongly convey these

    Concern with Other Peoples

    Perceptions

    Body Satisfaction

    Appearance-ContingentSelf-Worth

    SocioculturalNorm for Ideal

    Appearance

    Figure 1 Effect of sociocultural norms on appearance-contingentself-worth, concern with others perceptions, and bodysatisfaction.

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • norms (commercials containing images of thin, attractivewomen) and others to neutral stimuli. Subsequently, wemeasured how strongly participants based their self-worth on appearance, how dissatisfied they were withtheir bodies, and how concerned they were with othersperceptions.

    Method

    ParticipantsParticipants were 68 female undergraduates in intro-

    ductory psychology courses who received one experi-mental credit for participation.

    ProcedureParticipants were invited to take part in what was

    described as a long-term memory study. Participantswere told that they would be shown a series of com-mercials and they should try to remember as muchdetail as possible. This cover story was given to ensurethat participants would attend to the commercials whileobscuring the studys true purpose.

    Participants were randomly assigned to the experi-mental or control condition. In the control condition,participants viewed four neutral commercials containingno people. This is the most typical control condition usedin past body image research (Groesz et al., 2002).Commercials advertised a cell phone, a gas station, apharmacy, and an insurance company. In the experimen-tal condition, participants viewed the same four neutraladvertisements intermixed with two additional commer-cials conveying sociocultural norms. One commercialfeatured supermodels wearing Victorias Secret bras, andthe other featured a very thin, attractive woman promot-ing Dove soap.1 The number of commercials participantsviewed varied by condition because we thought it wasmore important to equalize ad content than ad length.Participants in the thin commercials condition watchedapproximately 45 additional seconds of commercials,which is unlikely to account for the specific differencesthat we find between conditions.

    After watching the commercials, participants weretold they needed to take a break to allow a delay beforelong-term memory could be tested. During this break,participants completed questionnaires ostensibly for otherresearchers in the department. These measures actuallyincluded our mediator and dependent variables. Theexperimenter left the room while the participant com-pleted these measures to increase feelings of anonymity.Before leaving, participants were asked their height and,with their permission, were weighed, allowing us to cal-culate body mass index (BMI). Participants were fullydebriefed and probed for suspicion.

    Measures Contingencies of self-worth. This 29-item scale was

    adapted from a longer version of Crocker and Wolfes(2001) Contingencies of Self-Worth Scale. Nine of theitems measured the extent to which participants basedself-worth on appearance (e.g., When I think I lookunattractive, my self-esteem suffers; = .86) on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree).The remaining items were filler items and measuredother contingency domains (academic competence, fam-ily support, others approval, virtue).

    Appearance self-esteem. We included Heathertonand Polivys (1991) 20-item State Self-Esteem Scale.Body satisfaction was measured using the 6 items thatmeasure appearance self-esteem (e.g., I am pleasedwith my appearance right now; = .82) on a 5-pointscale (1 = not at all, 5 = extremely). The other items tapperformance and social self-esteem.

    Public self-consciousness. We assessed participantsconcerns with others opinions using the seven-itempublic self-consciousness subscale from Fenigstein,Scheier, and Busss (1975) Self-Consciousness Scale (e.g.,Im concerned about the way I present myself; =.74). Items were measured on a 5-point scale (0 =extremely uncharacteristic, 4 = extremely characteristic).

    Results and Discussion

    Appearance as a Basis of Self-Worth We propose that exposure to images conveying the

    sociocultural norm for beauty will lead women to basetheir self-worth more strongly on appearance. We con-ducted a one-way ANOVA with type of commercial as theindependent variable and score on appearance-contingentself-esteem as the dependent variable. Participants exposedto the commercials reflecting appearance norms basedtheir self-esteem more strongly on appearance (M =5.11, SD = 0.87) than did participants exposed to neu-tral commercials (M = 4.63, SD = 0.92), F(1, 66) = 4.96,p = .03, 2 = .07, supporting our contention thatsalience of these norms may actually alter womens self-worth contingencies.2

    We expected our manipulation to uniquely affect theappearance-contingency domain of self-worth. A test ofthe manipulations influence on the other contingenciesof self-worth revealed no condition effects for the aggre-gate of all nonappearance contingencies (p = .15). Withregard to specific contingency domains, type of com-mercial did not influence academic competence, familysupport, or virtue (ps > .15). However, participants inthe thin commercials condition had marginally higher

    Strahan et al. / HOW SOCIOCULTURAL NORMS INFLUENCE WOMEN 291

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 292 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

    scores on the domain of others approval than did thosein the control, F(1, 66) = 3.27, p = .08.

    Body SatisfactionWe expected that participants exposed to sociocul-

    tural appearance norms would report lower body satis-faction than control participants. We conducted aone-way ANOVA with type of commercial as the inde-pendent variable and appearance self-esteem as thedependent variable. Participants who viewed the idealimages were less satisfied with their bodies (M = 3.07,SD = 0.77) than were participants who viewed the neu-tral commercials (M = 3.48, SD = 0.73), F(1, 66) = 5.00,p = .03, 2 = .07.

    We expected that commercials should primarily affectappearance self-esteem. Further analyses revealed thatthe commercials did not affect performance self-esteem(p = .59) but that participants in the thin commercialscondition had marginally lower social self-esteem, F(1,66) = 3.37, p = .07.

    Concern With Others PerceptionsWe also expected that participants exposed to images

    reflecting appearance norms would feel more concernedabout others judgments of them than would controlparticipants. A one-way ANOVA with public self-consciousness as the dependent measure revealed thatparticipants who viewed the thin commercials were sig-nificantly more concerned with others opinions (M =2.96, SD = 0.55) than were participants who viewed theneutral commercials (M = 2.65, SD = 0.63), F(1, 66) =4.14, p = .05, 2 = .06.

    Test of the Process ModelOur proposed mediation model that specifies the

    sociocultural norm manipulation as the predictor,appearance-contingent self-worth as the mediator, andbody satisfaction and concern with others perceptionsas the outcome variables was tested using path analyses.We used structural equation modeling (SEM; specifi-cally, Amos 6.0; Arbuckle & Wothke, 1999) to increasethe sensitivity of the analyses (see Spencer, Zanna, & Fong,2005, for a discussion of the utility of this approach). Themodel is reported in Figure 2 and allows for associationsbetween the outcome variables. Consistent with resultsreported previously, salience of sociocultural norms hada significant direct effect on both outcome variables:body satisfaction and concern for others opinions.Consistent with our hypothesized mediation model, partic-ipants reported that their self-worth was more contingenton appearance when sociocultural norms were salient. In

    turn, the more participants based their self-worth onappearance, the lower their body satisfaction was andthe more concerned they were with others perceptionsof them. When the mediator was included, both directeffects were reduced to nonsignificance. Sobel tests indi-cate that appearance-contingent self-worth was a signif-icant mediator for both the body satisfaction (z = 1.95,p = .05) and concern with others opinions (z = 1.95,p = .05) pathways (Sobel, 1982). This path analysis sug-gests that the effect of sociocultural norms for appear-ance on body satisfaction and concern with othersperceptions is partly mediated by appearance-contingentself-worth.

    SummaryFindings from Study 1 suggest that the sociocultural

    norms for appearance have a significant impact onwomens dissatisfaction with their bodies and the extentto which they are concerned with other peoples per-ceptions of them. Furthermore, exposure to imagesreflecting these norms led women to base their self-worth more strongly on their appearance, which in turnled them to feel less satisfied with their bodies and moreconcerned with others opinions.

    In the first study, we suggested that exposure to ide-alized media images makes salient sociocultural normsfor appearance. In Study 2, rather than increasingsalience of sociocultural norms, we attempted to reduceacceptance of norms by designing an intervention chal-lenging these beliefs. If we find that people who receivedthe challenging-norms intervention base their self-esteem less strongly on appearance, which in turn leadsthem to feel more satisfied with their bodies and lessconcerned with others perceptions, we will have con-vergent evidence of the causal role of socioculturalnorms and further support for our process model.

    e1

    .23* (.08, ns)

    .27* (.15, ns)

    Appearance-ContingentSelf-Worth

    Body Satisfaction

    e2

    e3

    .25*

    Concern with Other Peoples

    Perceptions

    .57**

    .47**

    .26*Salience of Images

    ReflectingSociocultural

    Norms

    Figure 2 Study 1: Effect of exposure to ideal images on concernwith others perceptions and body satisfaction mediatedby changes in appearance-contingent self-worth.

    *p < .05. **p < .01.

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • STUDY 2

    In Study 2, a field study conducted at local publicschools, we test the causal role of sociocultural normsby delivering an intervention to reduce their impact.Adolescents received either the challenging-norms inter-vention or a control intervention. Dependent measureswere collected 1 week later. To ensure that socioculturalnorms were salient to participants before completingthe dependent variables, we exposed all participants toidealized images at the beginning of that session.

    We expected that adolescents who received the inter-vention in which the sociocultural norm was challengedwould base their self-worth less strongly on appearance,which in turn would lead to more body satisfaction andless concern with others opinions. We also expectedthese findings to be more pronounced for female ado-lescents. This expectation is based on past research thathas demonstrated that females (a) feel greater pressurethan males to look thin and attractive (Rodin et al.,1985), (b) believe that the sociocultural norm for beautyis legitimate (Crocker et al., 1993), and (c) are moreinclined to derive their self-esteem from their appear-ance than males (Harter, 1999).

    The notion of conducting interventions to target bodyimage cognitions and behaviors is not new. Many inter-ventions using a wide range of procedures and theoreticalgoals have been tested, often with limited success (seeAustin, 2000, for a review). Several more recent inter-ventions found positive changes in attitudes aboutappearance or eating (ODea & Abraham, 2000). Forexample, one intervention with some conceptual similar-ities to our approach had women who were high in inter-nalization of the thin ideal voluntarily argue against thevalue of the thin ideal in our society (Stice, Mazotti,Weibel, & Agras, 2000). This intervention reduced thin-ideal internalization, body dissatisfaction, and dietingbehavior. Our primary goal, however, was to create anintervention that would allow us to test our theoreticalarguments about the impact of sociocultural norms forideal appearance. Therefore, we do not review the litera-ture on body image interventions in detail here.

    In this study, we tested the effectiveness of an interven-tion that challenged the legitimacy of the socioculturalnorms dictating appearance. Each activity was specificallydesigned to contest the idea that to be accepted in society,women need to be thin and beautiful and men need to betall and muscular. Although all activities were consistentwith this theoretical framework, we recognize that byusing multiple activities, we sacrifice some precision toensure a higher impact intervention. In other words,because we thought it was important to have studentsengage in multiple, hands-on, dynamic activities, we can-not determine the exact impact of each activity.

    We wanted to ensure that any treatment effects wefound were a result of the content of the intervention.To rule out the possibility that our findings could beattributed to the fact that intervention participantsreceived special treatment and attention, we included acontrol intervention. Participants in the control condi-tion received an intervention of the same length and for-mat designed to promote positive attitudes towardvolunteering. In addition, to reduce the possibility ofdemand characteristics, we waited 1 week before collect-ing the dependent measures, had different research assis-tants collect those measures, and used a cover story todisguise the purpose of the final session. We included asuspicion probe at the end of the session to directly assessstudents beliefs about whether the sessions were related.

    Consistent with the findings from our first study, weexpected that participants (especially girls) who receivedthe experimental intervention that challenged the socio-cultural norm of appearance would base their self-worth less strongly on appearance, which in turn wouldlead them to feel more satisfied with their bodies andless concerned with others perceptions.

    Method

    ParticipantsParticipants were 146 students (73 females, 73 males)

    recruited from three local schools located in demo-graphically similar areas with respect to socioeconomicand cultural composition. Mean age was 12.5 years.Three male participants were excluded from the analy-ses because they received the questionnaire intended forgirls. Random assignment was used to assign conditionsto the various schools.3 Participants from two of thethree schools received the experimental interventionthat challenged sociocultural norms about appearance.Participants from the third school received the controlintervention that promoted volunteerism. Consent wasobtained from the school principals, the students, andtheir parents.

    ProcedureThe experimental intervention consisted of two 80-

    min group sessions delivered over a 2-week period. Onemale and one female research assistant led all sessions.Each activity was designed to challenge socioculturalnorms for ideal appearance that convey the notion thatpeople need to look like the unattainable ideals por-trayed in the media to be accepted and successful in life.A cover story was used to increase students engagementwithout increasing demand characteristics. Students wereinformed that we were currently developing a program toencourage young people to question messages in the

    Strahan et al. / HOW SOCIOCULTURAL NORMS INFLUENCE WOMEN 293

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • media and that we wanted their help and feedback onthe program we would deliver in the future to studentstheir age.

    Session 1 began with an interactive discussion ofideal body types for men and women, aided byPowerPoint slides depicting idealized media images.Next, facilitators presented images reflecting historicalchanges in ideal bodies from the Renaissance period tothe present and discussed the flexibility of these ideals.Students nominated various sources that convey socio-cultural appearance norms (e.g., the media, ones friendsand family). Then, in small groups, students engaged inproblem solving in response to scenarios in which anindividual was described as feeling pressure to look a cer-tain way. Subsequently, the entire class discussed thesecoping strategies and incorporated them into a toolboxrepresenting techniques that students could use to com-bat the negative effects of sociocultural norms.

    Next, facilitators conveyed how unrealistic and unat-tainable appearance ideals can be by breaking down thetime and effort required to prepare a magazine photo(i.e., hairstyling, wardrobe, makeup, airbrushing), illus-trated by contrasting real-life pictures of celebrities withtheir perfected media images. Finally, facilitatorshelped students identify risks associated with pursuingsociocultural ideals to deglamorize these norms andreduce their appeal.

    In Session 2, students were given the opportunity toapply what they learned in two activities: a debate andthe creation of a poster. For the debate, pairs ofstudents from each of two teams argued against each of10 statements supporting sociocultural norms, earningpoints for the strength of their arguments. Next,students were provided with materials (including maga-zine images, markers, glue, etc.) and encouraged to cre-ate posters challenging the sociocultural norms aboutappearance. Finally, facilitators summarized the mainpoints from both sessions and thanked the students fortheir participation.

    The control intervention promoting volunteerism mir-rored the procedure of the experimental intervention intwo 80-min sessions. Because the content is not relevantto the current focus, it will not be discussed in detail.

    Dependent variable collection. One week after thesecond intervention session, a different malefemale pairof research assistants (blind to condition) collected thedependent variables. No connection was made to theintervention sessions, leading students to believe that thiswas an unrelated study. They were told that the purposeof the session was to find out their impressions of somemagazine advertisements and to gather some generalinformation about students personalities, values, self-views, and opinions. The first three pages of the booklet

    contained advertisements and asked for students evalu-ations. All students first evaluated a neutral ad for tooth-paste, then evaluated one ad featuring a male model andone ad featuring a female model. Boys received these adsordered such that the ideal male image came last (tomake it most salient as they completed other measures);females saw the ideal female ad last.4

    Participants then completed a booklet of question-naires that included our dependent measures mixed withfiller questionnaires including measures of volunteeringattitudes and intentions. Finally, students completed awritten suspicion probe assessing thoughts about anyconnection between this session and previous interven-tion sessions. No one identified the connection betweenthe two studies. Participants were then fully debriefed.

    MeasuresRejection of sociocultural norms. We expected that

    participants in the experimental intervention wouldchallenge the sociocultural norms for appearance to agreater extent than would participants in the controlintervention; therefore, we created a two-item measure ofperceived legitimacy of norms, r(139) = .33, p < .001(e.g., The way women look in magazines, TV, andmovies is unrealistic. It isnt like real life.). Participantsresponded to these items on 5-point scales (1 =completely disagree, 5 = completely agree).

    Thin-ideal internalization. We also included the inter-nalization component of Heinberg, Thompson, andStormers (1995) Sociocultural Attitudes TowardAppearance Questionnaire (SATAQ) as a second manip-ulation check (e.g., I believe that clothes look better onthin models; = .82). Participants responded to theseseven items on a 5-point scale (1 = completely disagree,5 = completely agree). Different versions of this scalewere given to male and female participants with ques-tions reflecting the ideals for their gender. The remainingdependent measures were the same as those described inStudy 1, except for the modifications noted next.5

    Contingencies of self-worth. Crocker and Wolfes(2001) Contingencies of Self-Worth Scale was shortenedto 18 items. Six of the items measured our mediator,appearance-contingent self-worth ( = .69). The remain-ing items measured other contingency domains (academiccompetence, family support, others approval, virtue).

    Appearance self-esteem. The State Self-Esteem Scale(Heatherton & Polivy, 1991) was shortened to 16items, including 4 items measuring appearance self-esteem ( = .68). We included 1 additional item directlyassessing body satisfaction: How satisfied are you with

    294 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • the shape of your body? on a 7-point scale (1 = notsatisfied at all, 7 = completely satisfied). Responses fromthese two measures were standardized and aggregated( = .77) to create a composite measure of body satis-faction. The other items assessed performance andsocial self-esteem.

    Public self-consciousness. We used three items fromthe Self-Consciousness Scale (Fenigstein et al., 1975),which measures concern with others perceptions ofones appearance ( = .59).

    Results and Discussion

    Preliminary AnalysesFirst, we examined whether school and classroom

    within the school should be included as a control factorwhen estimating intervention and gender effects. In thesepreliminary models, gender, intervention, and their inter-action were always treated as fixed effects. We estimatedthe contribution of school and classroom to total residualvariance using mixed-effects models treating school andclassroom as random effects. For all of the dependentmeasures under consideration, school contributed lessthan 3% to total variance (p > .28) and classroom con-tributed less than 4% to total variance (p > .33). We alsofit a nested fixed-effects ANOVA treating school withinclassroom as a nested factor. This factor was not statisti-cally significant for any of the dependent measures (p >.12). Therefore, we pooled responses over school andclassroom for all results reported in this study.

    Manipulation Checks The experimental intervention should have led par-

    ticipants to challenge sociocultural norms for idealappearance more than the control intervention. First,we directly measured participants tendency to chal-lenge the legitimacy of the sociocultural norms with thetwo-item scale created by the authors. Second, we testedwhether people who received the experimental interven-tion would report less internalization of the thin ideal(Heinberg et al., 1995).

    Rejection of sociocultural norms. We conducted a 2(condition) 2 (gender) ANOVA with acceptance of thesociocultural norm as the dependent variable. Bothmale and female participants who received the experi-mental intervention rejected the legitimacy of sociocul-tural norms (M = 4.23, SE = .10) to a greater extentthan participants who received the control intervention(M = 3.52, SE = .11), F(1, 138) = 23.11, p < .001, 2 =.14. Girls were marginally more likely to reject thenorms than boys, F(1, 138) = 3.54, p = .06, 2 = .02.

    Gender did not interact with condition, F(1, 138) =1.84, p = .18, 2 = .01.6

    Thin-ideal internalization. A 2 (condition) 2(gender) ANOVA with the internalization subscale ofthe SATAQ as the dependent variable revealed thatboth male and female participants who received theexperimental intervention (M = 2.11, SE = .10) inter-nalized the norms less did than participants whoreceived the control intervention (M = 2.60, SE = .10),F(1, 142) = 12.71, p < .001, 2 = .08. A main effect forgender indicated that females (M = 2.57, SE = .10) inter-nalized the norm more than did males (M = 2.14, SE =.10), F(1, 142) = 10.16, p = .002, 2 = .07. Gender didnot interact with condition, F(1, 142) < 1, p = .64, 2 =.002. Taken together, manipulation check findings sug-gest that our intervention led both girls and boys tochallenge the legitimacy of the sociocultural norm forappearance and its associated body ideals.

    Appearance as a Basis of Self-Worth Consistent with the first link in our model, we

    expected that participants in the experimental interven-tion would base their self-worth less strongly on appear-ance than would control participants. We also expectedthat this pattern might be particularly evident for girls.A 2 (condition) 2 (gender) ANOVA on appearance-contingent self-esteem revealed a significant Condition Gender interaction, F(1, 124) = 4.11, p = .05, 2 = .03.We conducted planned contrasts on boys and girls sep-arately.7 Boys were not affected by the intervention (F .12). With regard to specific contingency

    Strahan et al. / HOW SOCIOCULTURAL NORMS INFLUENCE WOMEN 295

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • domains, type of intervention did not influence academiccompetence or others approval (ps > .16). However, thevolunteering intervention appeared to increase thedegree to which participants self-esteem was contingenton virtue and family support (Fs > 4.52, ps < .04). It isworth noting that the marginal effect on othersapproval contingency of self-worth found in Study 1was not replicated in Study 2. It appears, then, thatsociocultural norms for appearance primarily targetappearance contingencies of self-worth.

    Body SatisfactionWe predicted that participantsespecially girls

    who received the experimental intervention would feelmore satisfied with their bodies than would participantswho received the control intervention. A 2 (condition) 2 (gender) ANOVA with body satisfaction as the depen-dent variable revealed a significant Condition Genderinteraction, F(1, 124) = 4.29, p = .04, 2 = .03. Boys inthe experimental intervention were equally as satisfiedwith their bodies (M = 0.18, SD = 0.58) as boys in thecontrol intervention (M = 0.09, SD = 0.82), F(1, 61) .11).

    Concern With Others PerceptionsWe also expected participantsparticularly girlsin

    the experimental intervention would be less concernedabout other peoples opinions than would participants inthe control intervention. A 2 (condition) 2 (gender)ANOVA with concern with others opinions as thedependent variable revealed only a main effect of gender,F(1, 124) = 9.36, p = .003, 2 = .07. However, because ofour strong basis for expecting a greater impact on females,we conducted planned contrasts with girls and boys sepa-rately. Boys in the experimental and control interventionswere equally concerned with others perceptions (M = 4.13,

    SD = 1.09 and M = 4.26, SD = 1.43, respectively), F(1,61) < 1, ns. However, girls who received the experimen-tal intervention tended to be less concerned with othersperceptions (M = 4.59, SD = 1.28) than did girls whoreceived the control intervention (M = 5.17, SD = 1.19),F(1, 63) = 3.43, p = .07, 2 = .05.

    In addition, girls in the control condition were moreconcerned with others opinions than were boys in thecontrol condition, F(1, 54) = 6.57, p = .01, 2 = .11.However, boys and girls in the experimental interven-tion did not differ in their concern for others opinions,F(1, 70) = 2.65, p = .11. Hence, challenging the appear-ance norms appeared to reduce the gender difference inpublic self-consciousness.

    Test of the Process ModelAs in Study 1, we tested our proposed mediation

    model using SEM. Because the intervention affectedonly girls on the variables of interest, the model is testedonly for girls. The intervention manipulation was spec-ified as the predictor, appearance-contingent self-worthas the mediator, and body satisfaction and concern withothers perceptions as the outcome variables (see Figure3 for standardized estimates). The model allows forassociations between the outcome variables. The inter-vention condition had a significant direct effect on bodysatisfaction and a marginal effect on concern for othersopinions. Consistent with our hypothesized mediationmodel, girls in the challenging-norms intervention con-dition reported that their self-worth was less contingenton appearance than did girls in the control intervention.In turn, the less strongly girls based their self-worth onappearance, the more satisfied they were with their bod-ies and the less concerned they were with others per-ceptions of them. When the mediator was included inthe analysis, both direct effects were reduced to non-significance. Sobel tests indicate that the mediationpathway was marginally significant for body satisfac-tion (z = 1.82, p < .07) and significant for concern withothers opinions (z = 2.07, p = .04; Sobel, 1982). Highlyconsistent with Study 1, this path analysis suggests thatthe effect of sociocultural norms for appearance onbody satisfaction and concern with others perceptionsis partly mediated by appearance-contingent self-worth.In fact, if the results are meta-analyzed across the twostudies, the Sobel tests for mediation are highly reliablefor both body satisfaction (z = 2.66, p < .01) and forconcern with others perceptions (z = 2.84, p < .01).

    SummaryIn the second study, we found that an intervention

    that challenged the legitimacy of the socioculturalnorms dictating appearance had a significant impact on

    296 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • female adolescents. Despite being exposed to an idealizedmedia image reflecting sociocultural norms for beauty,girls who received the challenge to the norm interven-tion based their self-worth less strongly on appearance,which in turn led them to feel more satisfied with theirbodies and less concerned with others perceptions(relative to a control condition).

    We attempted to reduce the potential for demandcharacteristics in this study by using a cover story, wait-ing 1 week before the collection of dependent measures,and using different experimenters to collect these mea-sures. Other findings also suggest that demand charac-teristics did not account for our results. First, althoughboth boys and girls in the experimental conditionreduced their acceptance of sociocultural norms, boyscontingencies of self-esteem, body dissatisfaction, andconcern with others perceptions were not affected bythe intervention. If our results were due to experimentaldemand, one might expect both genders to respond insimilar ways. Second, participants were probed for sus-picion in a private questionnaire and nobody linked thedependent variable collection to the earlier interventionsessions. This strengthens our confidence that theresults for the females in this study were due to theintervention. These findings offer convergent evidencefor the causal role of sociocultural norms for appear-ance: Challenging the sociocultural norm appeared toalleviate the vulnerability often displayed by womenand girls in the domain of physical appearance.

    GENERAL DISCUSSION

    Taken together, these studies provide considerable sup-port for our model. In Study 1, women who were exposedto ideal images that reflected sociocultural norms basedtheir self-worth more strongly on appearance than did

    women exposed to neutral images, and they subse-quently felt less satisfied with their bodies and moreconcerned with others perceptions. In Study 2, an inter-vention that challenged sociocultural norms dictatingappearance appeared to alleviate female participantsvulnerability to the effects of sociocultural standards ofappearance. When female adolescents received theintervention that challenged the norms, they based theirself-worth less strongly on appearance, leading toincreased body satisfaction and decreased concern withothers perceptions.

    Gender Specificity of Model

    Notably, although the intervention in Study 2appeared to affect participants of both genders (i.e., girlsand boys in the experimental intervention accepted andinternalized the norms less than controls), only girls inthe experimental condition reported basing their self-worth less strongly on appearance and reported feelingmore satisfied with their bodies and less concerned withothers perceptions. Consistent with past findings (Plineret al.,1990), we find that in the control condition, boysbase their self-worth less strongly on appearance and aremore satisfied with their bodies than are girls (arguablyreflecting the typical gender difference). Thus, an inter-vention that challenges sociocultural norms may affectboys less because they are already relatively immune tothese norms. Alternatively, it is possible that we createdan intervention that more effectively targeted concernspertinent to girls than to boys.

    Even though boys and girls rejection of the legiti-macy of sociocultural norms and their internalization ofthe thin ideal were equally affected by the interventionin Study 2 (i.e., gender and condition did not interact),the differences between boys and girls on these mea-sures that remained despite the intervention were inter-esting. Girls tended to see the sociocultural norms asless legitimate than did boys, and yet girls internalizedthe thin ideal more than did boys. Together these dif-ferences might have made girls more responsive thanboys to the intervention. Because girls were less accept-ing of the legitimacy of the sociocultural norms, theymay have been more receptive than boys to the messagepresented in the intervention. Alternatively, becausethey had greater internalization of the thin ideal, theymay have paid more attention than boys to the inter-vention. In any event, further research is needed tounderstand why the current intervention was effectivefor girls and not for boys.

    It is also possible that the interventions effectivenessfor girls was enhanced by the presence of boys (Austin,2000). If girls were aware that the boys in their socialcircles also questioned sociocultural norms, they might

    Strahan et al. / HOW SOCIOCULTURAL NORMS INFLUENCE WOMEN 297

    Appearance-ContingentSelf-Worth

    Body Satisfaction

    e2

    e3

    .13

    Concern with Other Peoples

    Perceptions

    .29*

    .30*

    .45**

    .29* (.20, ns)

    .23 (.10, ns)

    e1

    Type of Intervention

    Figure 3 Study 2: Effect of type of intervention on concern withothers perceptions and body satisfaction mediated bychanges in appearance-contingent self-worth.

    p = .06. *p < .05. **p < .01.

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • feel more confident in rejecting the legitimacy of thesenorms. This is consistent with evidence that girls andwomen often believe that others buy into sociocul-tural norms to an even greater extent than themselves;hence, regardless of their own evaluation of the legiti-macy of the norms they believe they will be measuredagainst them by others (Park, 2005).

    Does Our Model Apply to All Women?

    We did not find evidence that individual differences,such as restrained eating status or BMI, moderated oureffects in Study 1, supporting the idea that womensconcern with their weight is a normative discontent(Rodin et al., 1985). We suggest that almost all womenare vulnerable to sociocultural norms, especially whenthey are very salient. However, we do allow for the pos-sibility that other individual differences might moderateour model. For example, women with strong feministbeliefs or women who are chronically high in thin-idealinternalization might respond differently to sociocul-tural norms, thereby either reducing or exacerbating thenegative impact of ideal images.

    However, although the degree to which women inter-nalize these norms varies (Heinberg et al., 1995), we sug-gest that most women at least implicitly accept thelegitimacy of these norms. For instance, because weight isseen as controllable, women tend to believe that theyshould be able to achieve a more ideal body as long asthey are disciplined (Major & Crocker, 1993; Quinn &Crocker, 1999). Additionally, women are aware that theycan be negatively evaluated based on their weight andappearance, and they seem to think that such an evalua-tion is warranted if they do not conform to societys stan-dards for these attributes (Crocker et al., 1993).

    This widespread acceptance of the norms legitimacymay help account for why norms can directly influencecontingencies of self-worth. We argue that contextual fac-tors (such as salience of norms) can have a causal effect onself-worth contingencies. However, presumably contex-tual factors should only affect individuals who accept thatit is legitimate to base ones self-worth on that domain.For example, a situational factor such as the presence of aBible could increase the extent to which self-worth isbased on Gods love, but only if that person already hasfaith in God. Salience of sociocultural norms for appear-ance may play a powerful role in the degree to whichpeoples self-esteem is contingent on appearance becausemost women presume the legitimacy of these norms.

    Media and Sociocultural Norms for Appearance

    In the current studies we used media images to makesalient the messages conveyed in sociocultural norms

    for appearance. We deliberately chose ads that reflectedvarious features of the cultural ideal for attractiveness,thinness, height, and body shape. These ads are cer-tainly multifaceted, but so are the sociocultural normsfor ideal appearance. Women in these ads are undoubt-edly sexualized, objectified, and idealized, and we donot claim to disentangle precisely which features of theads drive our effects. Future research could examine theimpact of specific ad features, as well as investigatingother routes by which these norms are communicated.

    Relation of Our Findings toSelf-Objectification Theory

    Our findings are consistent with the contention ofself-objectification theory that women are socialized totake a third-person perspective on their own bodies andthat media depictions of women as objects lead them tofeel self-conscious and to habitually monitor their out-ward appearance (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997).Accordingly, we found that women exposed to idealmedia images were more concerned with others per-ceptions than were women not exposed to these images.In addition, the current results expand on this theoreti-cal perspective by demonstrating that appearance-con-tingent self-worth mediates the relation betweensociocultural norms and womens appearance self-consciousness. This is consistent with Fredricksons per-spective, but it has never been tested empirically.Fredrickson has also argued that self-objectification candisrupt and limit an individuals mental resources(Fredrickson, Roberts, Noll, Quinn, & Twenge, 1998).The time and energy that women devote to worryingabout their appearance limits the mental resources theycould be using for (arguably) more important activities,such as academics, career, or athletics.

    Mechanisms Accounting for Body Image Effects

    The current research identifies appearance-contingentself-worth as a previously unexplored mechanism thathelps account for the relation between socioculturalmessages and outcomes including body dissatisfactionand concern with others perceptions. It is important toemphasize that our research does not imply a criticismof previous process models (i.e., social comparison andthin-ideal internalization), nor do we mean to supersedethem. Previous models make important contributions toour understanding about the psychology of body image;we believe that our research adds to this understanding.Specifically, our research focuses on broader self andidentity implications of sociocultural norms for appear-ance. Not only do these sociocultural norms influencewomens beliefs about what they should look like, but

    298 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • they may have the power to affect their more core feel-ings about where they derive their feelings of self-worth.

    Implications of Sociocultural Norms

    One of the most important and novel findings fromthese studies is the strong causal link, for women andgirls, between sociocultural norms for ideal appearanceand appearance-contingent self-worth. This finding hasimportant implications for women: When women areexposed to these sociocultural messages, appearancewill become a larger basis of their self-esteem. Mostwomen cannot live up to the standards set by thesesociocultural norms; therefore, they come to base theirself-worth on an aspect of their self that will be inher-ently threatening. In the current research, it appearedthat simply viewing images that reflect the socioculturalnorms reinforced womens belief that they should mea-sure their self-worth with their appearance.

    To our knowledge this is the first set of studies to treatcontext as a significant determinant of self-worth contin-gencies (rather than treating contingencies of self-worthas a chronic individual difference). Specifically, exposureto images conveying sociocultural norms increasedappearance-contingent self-worth, and challenging thosenorms decreased it. If temporarily increasing salience ofthese norms in the lab can influence the domains on whichwomen base their self-worth, one can only imagine howrepeated exposure to these norms in our society mightinfluence the bases of womens self-esteem. Crocker(2002) suggests that any external contingencies of self-worth have significant costs to well-being. Women whobase their self-worth on appearance will look outwardfor validation that may result in an unstable self-view, aswell as a number of unhealthy or even self-destructivebehaviors (Sanchez & Crocker, 2005).

    Our finding that women value appearance more afterbeing exposed to ideal images can be contrasted withpast research that suggests that people will devalue ordisidentify with a domain when faced with threateninginformation about the self. For example, when facedwith a threatening social comparison, people may pro-tect themselves by reducing the self-relevance of thatcomparison dimension (Tesser & Campbell, 1982).Similarly, when people are reminded of a negativeingroup stereotype, they may disidentify with thestereotyped dimension to protect their self-esteem (e.g.,Major, Spencer, Schmader, Wolfe, & Crocker, 1998;Steele, 1997). Thus, it appears that people tend to basetheir self-esteem on things that bolster their self-image,not crush it. In contrast, our findings present a dramaticexception to this general rule. In Study 1, women werefaced with images of the unattainable socioculturalideal, but instead of disidentifying, they actually cared

    more about their weight and appearance despite thethreat these ideals present to the self.

    This failure to protect the self may well represent a rel-atively common phenomenon that has received littleattention by the field. In the current studies women areplaced in a difficult predicament. They must acknowl-edge that they do not measure up to the strong sociocul-tural norms for beauty or they must abandon this idealwith the possible unsettling implication of interpersonalor societal rejection. Given this dilemma, it is perhaps notsurprising that women embrace the sociocultural normsin spite of their negative self-image implications.

    Those mired in poverty in American culture mightwell face a similar predicament: Having to acknowledgethat one does not measure up to the American Dreamor abandon this ideal with the possible unsettling impli-cation that there is no hope. Similarly, women in abu-sive relationships must acknowledge that they do notlive up to the ideal of marital bliss or abandon this idealwith the unsettling implication that they will be leftalone and unable to fend for themselves.

    It is our view that these predicaments in which peopleface acknowledging that they do not measure up to asociocultural idealas unattainable as it may beor riskabandoning this ideal with the negative consequences andostracism that such abandonment might entail are impor-tant and powerful situations that merit further study.

    Although in the present research we study sociocul-tural norms associated with appearance (in the form inwhich they exist in Western society), this is just one ofmany sociocultural norms with implications forpeoples identity and behavior. Sociocultural normsexist across a variety of domains in different culturesand will be differentially pervasive for various groupswithin society. For example, sociocultural norms formodesty may be especially common within Japaneseculture (Heine, Toshitake, & Lehman, 2000) and socio-cultural norms for machismo may be particularly per-vasive for young men in cultures of honor (Cohen,Nisbett, Bowdle, & Schwarz, 1996). The specific conse-quences of not living up to cultural standards will varydepending on the content of the norm, but the processof shifting ones contingencies of self-worth may be con-sistent across a variety of domains and cultures.

    Conclusions

    It seems clear that exposure to societal messages thatreflect the sociocultural norm for ideal appearance hasa negative effect on women. We suggest that repeatedexposure to such images in our society leads womento measure their self-worth by their appearance,accounting for their widespread body dissatisfactionand self-consciousness. Given the pervasiveness of these

    Strahan et al. / HOW SOCIOCULTURAL NORMS INFLUENCE WOMEN 299

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • 300 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

    media messages, the results from Study 1 can be seen asdisheartening. However, the findings from Study 2 aremore hopeful. They suggest that challenging the legitimacyof these sociocultural norms may help break the cyclein which these norms increase appearance-contingentself-esteem, thereby allowing women to become moreaccepting of and less self-conscious about their ownphysical appearance.

    NOTES

    1. This study was conducted before more recent Dove commer-cials that present women of varying weights and sizes.

    2. We tested whether restrained eating (Herman & Polivy, 1980),which was obtained in an earlier mass testing session, and body massindex (BMI) were significant covariates for any of the dependent mea-sures. Restrained eating was not a significant covariate and did notinteract with or reduce the effect of the ideal images on our mediatorand outcome variables (Fs

  • Strahan et al. / HOW SOCIOCULTURAL NORMS INFLUENCE WOMEN 301

    Mills, J. S., Polivy, J., Herman, C. P., & Tiggemann, M. (2002).Effects of exposure to thin media images: Evidence of self-enhancement among restrained eaters. Personality and SocialPsychology Bulletin, 28, 1687-1699.

    National Eating Disorders Association. (2002). National eating disor-ders association statistics: Eating disorders and their precursors.Retrieved June 20, 2005, from http://www.nationaleatingdisorders.org/nedaDir/files/documents/handouts/Stats.pdf.

    ODea, J. A., & Abraham, S. (2000). Improving the body image, eat-ing attitudes, and behaviors of young male and female adolescents:A new educational approach that focuses on self-esteem.International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 43-57.

    Park, S-Y. (2005). The influence of presumed media influenceon womens desire to be thin. Communication Research, 32,594-614.

    Patrick, H., Neighbors, C., & Knee, C. R. (2004). Appearance-relatedsocial comparisons: The role of contingent self-esteem and self-perceptions of attractiveness. Personality and Social PsychologyBulletin, 30, 501-514.

    Pliner, P., Chaiken, S., & Flett, G. (1990). Gender differences in con-cern with body weight and physical appearance over the life span.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 263-273.

    Quinn, D. M., & Crocker, J. (1999). When ideology hurts: Effects ofbelief in the protestant ethic and feeling overweight on the psy-chological well-being of women. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 77, 402-414.

    Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1985). Women andweight: A normative discontent. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.),Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 32, pp. 267-307).Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Sanchez, D. T., & Crocker, J. (2005). How investment in genderideals affects well-being: The role of external contingencies of self-worth. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 63-77.

    Smolak, L. (1996). Methodological implications of a developmentalpsychopathology approach to the study of eating problems. In L.Smolak & M. P. Levine (Eds.), The developmental psychopathologyof eating disorders: Implications for research, prevention, andtreatment (pp. 31-55). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in struc-tural equations models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological method-ology 1982 (pp. 290-312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2005). Establishing acausal chain: Why experiments are often more effective than medi-ational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 89, 845-851.

    Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shapeintellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52,613-629.

    Stice, E., Mazotti, L., Weibel, D., & Agras, W. S. (2000). Dissonanceprevention program decreases thin-ideal internalization, body dis-satisfaction, dieting, negative affect, and bulimic symptoms: A pre-liminary experiment. International Journal of Eating Disorders,27, 206-217.

    Stice, E., Schupak-Nueberg, E., Shaw, H. E., & Stein, R. I. (1994).Relation of media exposure to eating disorder symptomatology:An examination of mediating mechanisms. Journal of AbnormalPsychology, 103, 836-840.

    Stice, E., & Shaw, H. E. (1994). Adverse effects of the media por-trayed thin-ideal on women and linkages to bulimic symptomatol-ogy. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 288-308.

    Strahan, E. J., Wilson, A. E., Cressman, K. E., & Buote, V. M. (2006).Comparing to perfection: How cultural norms for appearanceaffect social comparisons and self-image. Body Image, 3, 211-227.

    Tesser, A., & Campbell, J. L. (1982). Self-definition: The impact of therelative performance and similarity of others. Social PsychologyQuarterly, 43, 341-346.

    Tiggemann, M., & McGill, B. (2004). The role of social comparisonin the effect of magazine advertisements on womens mood andbody dissatisfaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,23, 23-44.

    Received April 3, 2007Revision accepted August 14, 2007

    at UNIV OF WATERLOO on May 20, 2010 http://psp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    /ColorImageDict > /JPEG2000ColorACSImageDict > /JPEG2000ColorImageDict > /AntiAliasGrayImages false /DownsampleGrayImages true /GrayImageDownsampleType /Bicubic /GrayImageResolution 300 /GrayImageDepth -1 /GrayImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50000 /EncodeGrayImages true /GrayImageFilter /DCTEncode /AutoFilterGrayImages true /GrayImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG /GrayACSImageDict > /GrayImageDict > /JPEG2000GrayACSImageDict > /JPEG2000GrayImageDict > /AntiAliasMonoImages false /DownsampleMonoImages true /MonoImageDownsampleType /Bicubic /MonoImageResolution 1200 /MonoImageDepth -1 /MonoImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50000 /EncodeMonoImages true /MonoImageFilter /CCITTFaxEncode /MonoImageDict > /AllowPSXObjects false /PDFX1aCheck false /PDFX3Check false /PDFXCompliantPDFOnly false /PDFXNoTrimBoxError true /PDFXTrimBoxToMediaBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXSetBleedBoxToMediaBox false /PDFXBleedBoxToTrimBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfile (U.S. Web Coated \050SWOP\051 v2) /PDFXOutputCondition () /PDFXRegistryName (http://www.color.org) /PDFXTrapped /Unknown

    /SyntheticBoldness 1.000000 /Description >>> setdistillerparams> setpagedevice