uva-dare (digital academic repository) longitudinal ... · longitudinal research using structural...

277
UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http://dare.uva.nl) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being and personal initiative in East Germany after the unification Garst, G.J.A. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Garst, G. J. A. (2000). Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being and personal initiative in East Germany after the unification. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. Download date: 07 Jul 2020

Upload: others

Post on 25-Jun-2020

8 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http://dare.uva.nl)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants ofpsychological well-being and personal initiative in East Germany after the unification

Garst, G.J.A.

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):Garst, G. J. A. (2000). Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies ofdeterminants of psychological well-being and personal initiative in East Germany after the unification.

General rightsIt is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s),other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Disclaimer/Complaints regulationsIf you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, statingyour reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Askthe Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam,The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible.

Download date: 07 Jul 2020

Page 2: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

Longitudinal research

using Structural Equation Modeling

applied in studies of determinants of

psychological well-being and personal initiative

in East Germany after the unification

Harry Garst

Page 3: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being
Page 4: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

Longitudinal research using Structural Equation Modeling

applied in studies of determinants of

psychological well-being and personal initiative

in East Germany after the unification

ACADEMISCH PROEFSCHRIFT

ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Universiteit van Amsterdam

op gezag van de Rector Magnificus

Prof.dr. J.J.M. Franse

ten overstaan van een door het college voor promoties ingestelde

commissie, in het openbaar te verdedigen in de Aula der Universiteit

op

donderdag 15 juni 2000, te 10.00 uur

door

Gerhard Jan Aalbert Garst

geboren te Warnsveld

Page 5: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

Promotor: Prof. dr. M. Frese

Co-promotor: Prof. dr. P.C.M. Molenaar

Faculteit: Maatschappij- en Gedragswetenschappen

Promotiecommissie: Prof. dr. C.K.W. de Dreu

Prof. dr. H. van der Flier

Prof. dr. J. Hox

Prof. dr. G.J. Mellenbergh

Prof. dr. C.G. Rutte

Page 6: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

Acknowledgements

In the first place I want to thank my promotor, Michael Frese. Although it wasn’t

always easy for both of us and we sometimes spoke different languages, we

succeeded to finish this project. In many ways I learned a lot from Michael. Next, I

want to thank my co-promotor, Peter Molenaar for his methodological support.

Part 1 of this dissertation improved considerably by the helpful comments of Conor

Dolan.

Part 2 of this dissertation is based on data from the project AHUS (Aktives Handeln in

einer Umbruch-Situation - active actions in a radical change situation). This project

was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, No Fr 638/6-5)

(principal investigator: Prof. Frese) from 1990-1998. Continuation of the project was

made possible by the programmagroep work and organizational psychology,

University of Amsterdam. Members of the AHUS project were D. Fay, S. Hilligloh,

C. Speier, T. Wagner and J. Zempel, student members were C. Dormann, M.Erbe-

Heinbokel, T. Hilburger, J. Grefe, M. Kracheletz, K. Leng, K. Plüddemann, V.

Rybowiak, and A. Weike (Giessen) and R. Bamesberger, A. Dehnelt, G. Engstle, M.

Fontin, B. Hartmann, J. Haushofer, B. Immler, E. Kahl, M. Eichholz, S. Kemmler, C.

Lamberts, R. Lautner, A. Röver, B. Schier, S. Schmider, D. Schweighart, H. Simon,

B. Waldhauser, T. Weber, B. Winkler (Munich). Especially, I want to thank Doris

Fay, my former roommate and fellow member of the AHUS project in the last stage.

I thank the following persons for reviewing parts of this dissertation: Kathy Klein,

Elizabeth Morrison, Andreas Utsch, and Dieter Zapf (Chapter 3), Frans Oort, Doris

Fay and Mike Rovine (Chapter 4), Sabine Sonnentag (Chapter 4 and 5), and Jasper

Klapwijk (Summary).

Finally, I owe much to Frans Oort and Wim de Haan who both guided me through the

field of methodology for many years.

Page 7: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being
Page 8: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

Contents Chapter 1 Introduction 1

PART 1

Chapter 2 A General Longitudinal Model 7

PART 2

Chapter 3 Control and Complexity in Work and the Development of

Personal Initiative (PI): A 5-Wave Longitudinal Structural

Equation Model of Occupational Socialization

73

Chapter 4 The Temporal Factor of Change in Stressor-Strain Relationships:

A Growth Curve Model on a Longitudinal Study in East

Germany

119

Chapter 5 Optimism and Subjective Well-being in a Radical Change

Situation in East Germany 163

Chapter 6 Summary and conclusion 221

Appendix A 235

Appendix B 237

Appendix C 245

Appendix D 250

Appendix E 255

Summary in Dutch 263

Page 9: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

Chapter 3 is based on:

Frese, M., Garst, H., Fay, D. Control and complexity in work and the development of

personal initiative (PI): A 5-wave longitudinal structural equation model of

occupational socialization. Manuscript submitted for publication

Chapter 4 of this thesis has been accepted for publication:

Garst, H., Frese, M. & Molenaar P.C.M. (in press). The Temporal Factor of Change in

Stressor-Strain Relationships: A Growth Curve Model on a Longitudinal Study in

East Germany. Journal of Applied Psychology.

Chapter 5 is based on:

Garst, H & Frese, M. Optimism and Subjective Well-being in a Radical Change

Situation in East Germany. Manuscript submitted for publication.

ISBN 90-5470-091-2

Page 10: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

1

Chapter 1

Introduction

Structural Equation Modeling is especially well suited for analyzing longitudinal data since

it allows the inclusion of many repeatedly measured variables into a single model. Moreover, since

both observed and latent variables can be included, relations between latent variables over time can

be studied. Using Structural Equation Modeling the processes of how psychological well-being and

personal initiative unfold over time can now adequately be tested.

Both theory and applications of longitudinal Structural Equation Modeling will be treated

and therefore this dissertation is composed of two parts. Part 1 introduces a new general

longitudinal model and describes how several well-known models can be treated as its special

cases. Part 2 consists of three longitudinal studies on a sample from former East Germany. These

studies are part of a larger project that started immediately after the unification 1990 and stretched a

period of five years with six measurement occasions.

In Part 1 a hierarchy of longitudinal models will be described and it is shown that different

classes of models (autoregressive versus latent growth curve models) are based upon different

assumptions of the underlying change processes.

The data in Part 2 were analyzed using Structural Equation Modeling. Both autoregressive

and latent growth curve models have been tested. Some of the models are innovative by combining

autoregressive and growth curve models into a single model. Also the inclusion of a measurement

model in growth curve models is new in the literature (to the best of my knowledge).

The studies in Part 2 belong to the field of industrial and organizational psychology,

although the third study is also rooted in the field of social psychology.

The data on which the studies in Part 2 are based were gathered in the AHUS project

(AHUS: Aktives Handeln in einer Umbruch-Situation - active actions in a radical change situation).

A representative sample of (former East-German) workers participate in a longitudinal investigation

into the transition from a planned economy to a market economy. Questionnaire and interview data

were obtained from an average of 540 respondents. The 1st wave was directly after economic

unification in July, 1990; the 2nd after the political unification in November and December, 1990;

the 3rd in July, 1991; the 4th in September and October, 1992; and the 5th in August and

September, 1993 and the sixth in august and September 1995.

Page 11: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

2

The central concern of the AHUS study was how much did people cope with the many

changes in these revolutionary times and which people changed the most and what were the major

determinants for these changes? How can we understand that some people changed for the best

while other suffered and grew bitter? Is it fair to speak from winners and losers of the German

unification? Did the winners possess important personality traits? Did they use different coping

styles? Did they use more personal initiative? Or were external circumstances the key determinants?

Other AHUS studies reported on personal initiative (Frese, M., Kring, W., Soose, A. & Zempel, J.

,1996; Speier, C. & Frese, M., 1997; Frese, M., Fay, D., Hilburger, T., Leng, K., Tag, A. 1997);

error orientation (Rybowiak, V., Garst, H., Frese, M. & Batinic, B. (1999), social support

(Dormann, C., & Zapf, D., 1999).

The AHUS data are unique in many respects. First, its longitudinal design: it included six

measurement waves stretching a period of five years. Most field studies in IO psychology use cross-

sectional designs and longitudinal studies are relatively rare and almost always limited to a few

measurement occasions. Further the length of the period of study made it possible to study effects

with a long time lag. Both the number of waves and the length of the period of this study allowed

decomposing changes into state and trait components. Traits are by definition relatively stable, but

may show changes in a long-time frame. Second, the sample size was impressive: 684 subjects

participated at least one measurement occasion. Third, the sample consisted of subjects with various

occupations. Too often studies in the field of organization and work psychology make use of

convenience samples, including subjects with the same occupation or working in the same

company. Generalizing the results of these studies are severely limited. Fourth, in this study a large

array of constructs relevant for industrial and organizational and social psychology were measured.

This made it possible to test for hypothesis controlling for variables known to affect the outcomes

also. Fifth, many constructs in this study have great societal relevance as well. Working

characteristics and work stressors have well documented effects on both performance as many other

psychological constructs (e.g., subjective well-being, strains). Personal initiative, coping styles and

resilience constructs like optimism are central in theories about how to function optimal in an

adverse environment. Sixth, the measurements were not restricted to questionnaires, but also

included interviewer observations by trained interviewers. Especially the measurement of personal

initiative used a situational interview procedure, where in a standardized procedure hypothetical

problems were offered and the responses of the subjects were recorded. Finally, this study took

place in a period in which drastic changes took place. Since human behavior is supposed to be

affected by a great number of determinants the isolated effects of a single determinant is frequently

Page 12: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

3

of modest size. In high change environments many processes are stirred up and this is to some

extent comparable with manipulations in field experiments.

In Chapter 3 an occupational socialization model will be presented. This model describes the

relationships between work characteristics (job control and job complexity), mastery orientation

(including control appraisals, self-efficacy, and control aspirations), and personal initiative (PI).

In Chapter 4 several theoretical models about how stressor-strain relationships unfold in

time will be tested using multivariate latent growth curve models.

In Chapter 3 and 4 work conditions (job characteristics, like job control and job complexity,

and work stressors) were included to explain psychological processes. In Chapter 5 the focus was

on factors within the person: Optimism and pessimism. Several models of the relation between

optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being will be tested and the mediational role of coping

styles will be investigated.

In Chapter 6 the results will be summarized and the overall conclusion will be discussed in

light of the specific political and economical context of this historical period. Chapter 6 ends with a

discussion of some methodological issues.

References

Dormann, C., & Zapf, D. (1999). Social support, social stressors at work and depressive symptoms:

Testing for moderator effects with structural equations in a 3-wave longitudinal study. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 84, 874–884.

Frese, M., Kring, W., Soose, A. & Zempel, J. (1996). Personal initiative at work: Differences

between East and West Germany. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 37-63.

Speier, C. & Frese, M. (1997). Generalized self-efficacy as a mediator and moderator between

control and complexity at work and personal initiative: A longitudinal field study in East Germany.

Human Performance, 10, 171-192.

Frese, M., Fay, D., Hilburger, T., Leng, K., Tag, A. (1997). The concept of personal initiative:

Operationalization, reliability and validity in two German samples. Journal of Organizational and

Occupational Psychology, 70, 139-161.

Rybowiak, V., Garst, H., Frese, M. & Batinic, B. (1999). Error Orientation Questionnaire (EOQ):

Reliability, validity, and different language equivalence. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20.

527-547.

Page 13: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

4

Page 14: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

5

Part 1

Page 15: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

6

Page 16: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

7

Chapter 2 A General Longitudinal Model

Introduction

Over the years many authors have consistently pleaded for making more use of longitudinal

designs. Despite the consensus on the importance of longitudinal studies, the question of how to

study change over time is long debated and have led to much controversy in the social sciences

(Cronbach & Furby, 1970; Rogosa, 1980). Not only has the longitudinal literature been rife with

conflict and controversy, also remarkable advances have been made in developing powerful

methods of analyzing change. Latent Growth Curve Models (Muthén, 1997; Muthén & Curran,

1997) and Hierarchical Linear Models (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1991) are major contributions.

Pioneers like Meredith and Tisak (1990) introduced the Latent Growth Curve Model and authors

like Browne (1993), Muthén (1997), and Willett and Sayer (1994, 1995) wrote influential papers

about this subject. However, the introduction of Latent Growth Curve Models also led to new

controversies (Stoolmiller, 1995) e.g., about the ability to distinguish empirically between the

Latent Growth Curve Model and the more conventional Quasi-Markov Simplex (Rogosa & Willett,

1985, Mandies, Dolan & Molenaar, 1994, Raykov, 1998). Both models and the differences between

them are excellent explained by Curran (1998).

In order to give some oversight to the wide array of existing longitudinal models I present in

this paper a new general model and I will show how existing models can be fit into this framework

by treating them as special cases of this general model.

The general model that will be described in this paper is a higher order factor model. The

relationship between several longitudinal models and factor models is well known in the literature.

Jöreskog (1970) showed that a quasi-simplex can be parameterized as a factor model (see also

Browne, 1992, McCloy, Campbell & Cudeck, 1994). Two decades later Meredith and Tisak (1990)

demonstrated that latent growth models can also be specified as a factor model. Recently Curran

and Bollen (1999) introduced a longitudinal model which can be viewed as a hybrid of the latent

growth curve model and the quasi-simplex.

Although a general longitudinal model will be presented, I will show that a special case of

this model is already sufficiently general in scope to encompass several longitudinal models as

special cases. However, more complex longitudinal models cannot be treated as special cases of this

submodel. These models should be instead considered as submodels of the general longitudinal

Page 17: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

8

factor model. The similarities and differences between all longitudinal submodels will be described

and it will be shown how these models are based upon different assumptions about the underlying

developmental process. Although many well-known longitudinal models can be considered as

descendents of the general longitudinal model, the conventional specification of these models

differs sometimes greatly from the specification of the model as a special case of the general

longitudinal model. In order to study the interrelations of the longitudinal models it is necessary to

translate them in a more general presentation. However, the equivalence between the various

presentations of the models will be demonstrated extensively, although in some cases the

technicalities had to be transferred to the Appendices.

All models include latent variables. It is assumed that an acceptable measurement model is

known and that the requirement of measurement invariance over time holds. The unique terms of

identical items may correlate over time and the covariance matrix of the unique terms may be a

banded or block-diagonal matrix.

In this paper the modeling of the means will not be described as our presentation does not

provide new insights to the mean part and the topic of incorporating structured means in

longitudinal models is already well treated in Bast & Reitsma (1997) and Mandys, Dolan &

Molenaar (1994).

This paper is organized as follows: First the general longitudinal model and the submodel

will be described. Second, the least restricted submodel of the general longitudinal model will be

presented: The Latent Difference Model. It will be shown that linking this model to the time-

dimension yields an equivalent model: the smallest Piecewise Latent Growth Curve Model which

allows the slopes for each individual to be different for each time-interval. Third, constraining the

slopes to be equal for all time-intervals leads to the Linear Growth Curve Model. A special case of

this model is the Random Intercept Model and a further restriction, assuming equal residual

variances, yields the Equal Variance Covariance Model. A special case of the Latent Difference

model is the Quasi-Wiener Simplex Model which can be derived from the Latent Difference Model

by restricting the covariance matrix to be diagonal. Next, the First Order Moving Average

Difference Model will be presented. What follows are three equivalent first order autoregressive

models (further denoted as AR(1) models). An extension of this model is the second order

autoregressive model (AR(2)). A new model will be presented by autoregressing latent differences

on preceding latent differences. It will be demonstrated that this model can also be estimated as a

special case of an AR(2) model. An overview of the General Longitudinal Model and the

submodels is shown in Figure 1. The reader may find it helpful to consult Figure 1 in the next

section where the models will be discussed in detail.

Page 18: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

9

General M

odel

εΘ

ΛΨ

∆Φ

∆Λ

Σ+

+

∏=

−=

−=

''

11

11

qtt

qtt

2

A

R(2) m

odel

subm

odel

∏ −=

11

qtt

ΚΤΗ

∆=

∏ −=

11

qtt

free parameters in row

t and column t –1

()

εΘ

ΛΨ

ΚΤ

ΗΚ

ΤΗΦ

ΛΣ

++

='

''

'2

and in t –2 (except if t = 2).

m

odels Κ = Ι

models Κ

≠ Ι

Figure 1. Family of L

ongitudinal Models

Page 19: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

10

Figure 1. Continued

models Κ

≠ Ι Q

uasi Markov Sim

plex

Autoregressive effects for latent differences

Κ =

Ζ; Η

= Ζ

-1; Ψ= 0

; Φ2 =

diagonal, free

Κ

= ΤΖ

; Η =

Ζ-1; Ψ

= 0; Φ

2 = diagonal, free

Ζ =

diagonal, fixed parameter at value 1 at

first row and first colum

n, all other parameters free:

products of autoregressive coefficients

Page 20: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

11

Figure 1. Continued

models Κ

= Ι

Latent D

ifference Model

Linear G

rowth C

urve Model

First Order M

oving Average

Κ =

Η = Ι ; Ψ

= 0; Φ

2 = sym

metric, free

(conventional version: Ψ= diagonal, free

D

ifference Model

to allow for tim

e-specific disturbances)

Η is q ×q m

atrix with fixed

equal slopes for each subject

elem

ents: 1’s at the

()

εΘ

ΛΨ

ΤΗ

ΤΗΦ

ΛΣ

++

='

''

4

diagonal, -1 at second band below

''Ν

ΗΦ

ΝΗ

Φ1

21

4−

−=

main diagonal

Ν =

fixed matrix of 0’s and 1’s

Ψ= 0

; Φ2 =

diagonal, free, Κ = Ι

R

andom Intercept M

odel ϕ

11 and ϕ21 in Φ

4 fixed to zero

Equal V

ariance Covariance M

odel

In addition: all parameters in Ψ

equal

Page 21: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

12

Figure 1. C

ontinued L

atent Difference M

odel

Sm

allest Piecewise L

inear Grow

th Model

εΘ

ΛΤ

ΗΗ

ΦΤΗ

ΗΛ

Σ+

=−

−'

''

'12

1

Η =

diagonal, fixed timesteps on diagonal

Q

uasi-Wiener Sim

plex

L

inear Grow

th Curve M

odel

Κ =

Η = Ι; Ψ

= 0; Φ

2 = diagonal, free

(restricted version: Ψ

= 0)

equal slopes for each subject

()

εΘ

ΛΤ

ΗΤΗ

ΦΛ

Σ+

='

''

4

''Ν

ΗΦ

ΝΗ

Φ1

21

4−

−=

Ν =

fixed matrix of 0’s and 1’s

Page 22: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

13

After the presentation of the basic longitudinal models we will introduce some more

complex models. First, the hybrid model of Curran and Bollen (1999) will be described as a

synthesis of the AR(1) and Linear Growth Curve Model. These last models were previously

considered as distinct models. Second, multivariate models, describing multiple series of latent

constructs, will be treated hereafter. Finally, for the Linear Growth Models the relationship with

time will be explored. The invariance of the model parameters under a linear transformation of the

time scale will be discussed.

The General Longitudinal Model

All longitudinal models described in this paper refer to latent variables. Therefore we start

with a description of the measurement model. For an arbitrary subject the measurement model for a

single latent construct, repeatedly measured by the same set of items, can be expressed as follows:

ittiiity εηλτ ++= , (1)

where τi refers to the item intercept and λi to the factor loading of item i (i ∈ 1,2,…, p) on factor

ηt at measurement occasion t. Note that times t are discrete (t ∈ 1,2,…, q). Because measurement

invariance (Oort, 1996) is assumed no occasion indices were added to the item intercepts and factor

loading. The unique factor is denoted by εit. To simplify the notation no subject indices are added.

In matrix form (1) can be expressed as

y ++= ΛΛ , (2)

where y is a pq × 1 vector of observed variables, is a pq × 1 vector of item intercepts, Λ is a pq ×

q matrix of factor loadings, η is a q ×1 vector of latent constructs and ε is a pq × 1 zero mean

vector of unique factors. These matrices are specified as follows:

Page 23: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

14

+

+

=

pq

q

p

q

p

p

p

p

p

pq

q

p

y

y

y

y

ε

ε

ε

ε

η

η

λ

λ

λ

λλ

λ

τ

τ

τ

τ

..

..

..

..

..00

....00

..00

........

0..0

0....0

0..0

0..0

0..0..

0..0

..

..

..

..

..

..

1

1

11

1

1

1

1

1

1

1

1

11

The covariance structure for model (2) (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1989) is:

εΘΛΛΦΣ += '1 , (3)

given that Cov[ηη] = Φ1, E[εε] = Θε, and E[ηε] = 0. The covariance matrix of the

unique factors, Θε can be specified as either banded error or as block diagonal (Vonesh &

Chinchilli, 1997).

A second order factor model can be formulated by specifying relations between the first and

the second order factors. This can be described as follows:

ζ+= Γ (4)

In (4) the q × r matrix Γ contains the second order factor loadings and the q × 1 vector ζ containing

unique factors. If we define E[ζζ]= ψ, E[ξξ]= Φ2 then, under the assumption of E[ζε]=

0, E[ξε]= 0, and E[ζξ]= 0 . The covariance matrix of the second order factor model is:

( ) εΘΛΨΓΓΦΛΣ ++= ''2 (5)

Page 24: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

15

Φ2 is the r × r covariance matrix of the second order factors. Finally, Ψ is a q × q matrix of the

unique factors, determining the first order factors. Restricting Γ to equal a q × q identity matrix and

Ψ to a zero matrix reduces (5) to (3).

The structure of higher order factors influencing lower order factors can be used for

longitudinal models. If we define the matrix of second order loadings in (5) as the product of q – 1

matrices, where q is the number of measurement occasions and t = q – j + 1, as follows:

∏=−

=

1

1

q

jt∆Γ , (6)

then our general model can be described as:

εΘΛΨ∆Φ∆ΛΣ +

+

∏=−

=

=

''

1

1

1

1

q

jt

q

jt 2

. (7)

The specification of each of the matrices ∆t is a function of the measurement occasion and

the specific longitudinal model under consideration. The number of waves (denoted as q) minus 1

determines the total number of these matrices.

A submodel of the general model (7) can be obtained by imposing the following restrictions

on the ∆t matrices: Each ∆t can be decomposed into three matrices Αt Μt Ωt, specified as follows.

The matrices Αt and Ωt are both diagonal matrices of order q × q and the matrices Μt can be

specified as q × q identity matrices except for one single element in row r = t and column c = t –1

which is fixed to the value 1. Thus, the matrices Μt are specified as follows:

Page 25: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

16

=

=

= −

100..00

010..00

001..00

............

000..11

000..01

....;;

100..00

011..00

001..00

............

000..10

000..01

;

11..000

01..000

............

00..100

00..010

00..001

21 ΜΜΜ qq

j = 1 j = 2 j = q – 1

t = q – j + 1 = q t = q – j + 1 = q –1 t = q – j + 1 = 2

r = t = q r = t = q –1 r = t = q –1 = 2

c = t – 1= q –1 c = t – 1= q – 2 c = t – 1 = 1

With some algebraic manipulations the product

222333111 .... ΩΜΑΩΜΑΩΜΑΩΜΑ −−− qqqqqq (8)

can be written as

( ) qqqqqq ΥΥΥΥΜΜΜΜΧΧΧΧ 132231132 .......... −−− (9)

All Αt matrices, placed between the Μt matrices, have been replaced by premultiplying with a

suitable matrix Χt. In similar vein, all Ωt matrices, placed between the Μt matrices, have been

replaced by postmultiplying with a suitable matrix Υt. The algebraic derivation of (9) can be found

in Appendix A. The following identity can easily be verified:

ΤΜ =∏−

=

1

1

q

jt (10)

where T is lower q × q triangular matrix with all elements fixed to the value of 1.

Page 26: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

17

=

1..111

..........

0..111

0..011

0..001

Τ (11)

We show the equivalence 2345 ΜΜΜΜΤ = for a 5-wave study:

Μ5 Μ4 Μ3 Μ2 Τ

=

11111

01111

00111

00011

00001

10000

01000

00100

00011

00001

10000

01000

00110

00010

00001

10000

01100

00100

00010

00001

11000

01000

00100

00010

00001

If we define:

qq ΧΧΧΧΚ 132 ... −=

qq ΥΥΥΥΗ 132 ... −=

we can write the covariance structure for this submodel as:

( ) εΘΛΨΚΤΗΚΤΗΦΛΣ ++= ''''2 (12)

If ΚΤΗ is replaced by Γ the model (12) reduces to a second order factor model described in (5).

Central to longitudinal models is the principle that ‘the past influences the present in a

particular manner’ and that ‘innovation’ or ‘change’ may take place. If we define one factor as

representing ‘initial status’ and all subsequent factors as ‘change’ factors, we can interpret a higher

Page 27: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

18

order factor model as a longitudinal model. This concept is displayed as a SEM graph in Figure 2.

The model is a factor model, because higher order factors influence lower order factors (top-down

direction in display). At the same time it is a longitudinal model, because initial status and change

factors influence present and all subsequent lower order factors (time dimension in left-right

direction in display).

Transmission matrix Τ (all elements fixed to 1)

π1 π2 π3 π4 π5

ω1 ω2 ω3 ω4 ω5

initial status factor

change factors

Time

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

Figure 2. Longitudinal model specified as a factor model.

The model can be described with the following equations:

11 ωπ =

212 ωωπ += (13)

3213 ωωωπ ++=

.. .. .. ..

qq ωωωωπ +++= ...321

In a more general form (13) can be expressed as:

∑==

t

jjt

1ωπ

(14)

Page 28: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

19

The matrix representation uses the q × q lower triangular matrix Τ with all elements fixed to

the value of 1.

Τ= , (15)

where π and ω are q × 1 vectors of, respectively, higher and lower order factors. The function of

the matrix Τ is to transmit information to the present and all subsequent waves. The first column

refers to the effects of the first wave to all the other waves (including the first wave itself). The

matrix Τ only accomplishes a one to one transmission of information of higher order factors (initial

status and subsequent changes) to the lower order factors (latent constructs). In order to transform

this information the model have to be extended with two new sets of factors. The relations between

the factors can be specified as:

Η= , (16)

+= ΚΚ , (17)

where ξ and η are q × 1 vectors of higher and lower order factors, respectively, and Η and Κ are

q × q factor loading matrices and ζ is a q × 1 vector of residuals. The matrix ζ introduces additional

time-specific variances at the lowest structural level. Its function will be discussed when describing

the specification of growth curve models. Note that in equations (16) and (17) no vectors of

residuals were added, so only linear transformations of the ξ and ω factors are considered. The

relationship between the highest and lowest order factor is now:

+= ΚΤΗΚΤΗ (19)

The covariance matrix of (19) is the same as (12). The specification of the matrices Κ, Η, Φ2 and

Ψ will yield several longitudinal models presented in this paper.

Page 29: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

20

The submodel (12) of the general model is displayed in Figure 3. The function of the

transformation matrices is visualized by introducing auxiliary variables without residual variances

(similar to phantom variables; Rindkopf, 1984; for a similar SEM display: see Neale (1999, p.112)

explaining the Cholesky decomposition). In Figure 3 the matrices Κ and Η are specified as

diagonal, which will be the case in almost all models. Figure 3 can best be interpreted starting from

top to bottom and from left to right. Initial status information is transmitted to all lower variables,

although possibly transformed by the matrices Κ and Η. During the developmental process new

information can be brought into the system through the change factors (also called innovation

terms). However, these effects may be again transformed and hence will be different for subsequent

lower variables. The transformation process can either have an explicit relationship with the factor

time or the process may only implicitly be related with the time dimension. The innovation terms

have a stochastic nature. In addition to these innovation terms, time-specific residuals reside on the

lowest level of the structural part of the model, only influencing the variable of one measurement

occasion. Only in a small subset of the models to be presented will these residuals be required.

In summary, a general longitudinal model (7) and a submodel (12) of this model were

specified as factor models. In the next section it will be explained that this submodel (12) is itself

sufficiently general to encompass several longitudinal models. However, some longitudinal models

cannot be treated as a special case of the submodel (12), but should be considered as a distinct

submodel of the general model (7) itself.

Page 30: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

21

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

y13 y23 y33

Transmission matrix Τ (all elements fixed to 1)

measument

model Λ

Transformation matrix Η

Transformation matrix Κ

π1 π2 π3 π4 π5

ω1 ω2 ω3 ω4 ω5

Covariance matrix Φ2

ξ1 ξ2 ξ3 ξ4 ξ5

time-specific disturbance term

time-specific unique i tem factors

Figure 3. Submodel (12); measurement model partly shown; covariances between unique factors of

identical items omitted.

Page 31: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

22

Latent Difference Model

A special case of the submodel (7) is the Latent Difference Model. This model can be

obtained from the submodel (7) by specifying Κ and Η as identity matrices and Ψ as a zero

matrix. Strictly, the Latent Difference Model is not a model in the sense of imposing further

restrictions on the (co)variances of the latent constructs, but is in fact only a transformation of those

latent constructs. It differs from the model proposed by Steyer, Eid & Schwenkmezger (1997). They

specified a restricted latent difference model by imposing certain restrictions on the factor loadings.

However, the Latent Difference Model described here, does not concern itself with the specification

of the measurement model.

The following transformation yields a vector of latent difference scores (except for the first

element which remains η1):

1−= ΤΤ (20)

This is immediately apparent if we show the matrices with the elements:

=

=

−1

23

12

1

3

2

1

3

2

1

....

11..00

..........

00..10

00..11

00..01

..

qqqq ηη

ηη

ηη

η

η

η

η

η

ξ

ξ

ξ

ξ

(21)

Premultiplying both sides of (20) with Τ yields:

Τ= (22)

We defined ( ) 2Φ='E , and now we can write

( ) ( ) ( ) '''''' EEE ΤΤΦΤΤΤΤ 2=== (23)

Page 32: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

23

Thus, if the matrices Κ and Η are specified as identity matrices and Ψ is a zero matrix the

submodel (7) reduces to:

( ) εΘΛΤΤΦΛΣ += ''2 (24)

This model is only a linear transformation of the original latent variables and is thus saturated with

respect to the structural part. In Figure 4 the model is shown as a SEM path model1.

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

η1 η2−η1 η3−η2 η4−η3 η5−η4

Covariance matrix Φ2

Transmission matrix Τ (all elements fixed to 1)

y13

y23

y33

Figure 4. Latent Differences Model); measurement model partly shown; covariances between

unique factors of identical items omitted.

The Latent Difference Model can also be specified using the general model (7) by

specifying ∆t = Μt and all matrices Αt and Ωt are identity matrices. Using (10) the present

specification of (7) reduces to the model described in (24).

1 For a two wave model with difference scores: see McArdle & Nesselroade (1994).

Page 33: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

24

Latent Difference Model as a Piecewise Linear Growth Model

Rogosa, Brandt, & Zimowski (1982, p. 730) showed that a difference score can be

considered as the slope (divided by the elapsed time) in a two-wave linear growth model. This can

be extended to more waves as illustrated in Figure 5.

d i ff e re nc e s c or e

t im e : T 3 - T 2

T 2 T 1 T 3 T 4 T 5 T 6

b 0

b 2

η

Figure 5. Score pattern for a single subject;

Note: slope b2 is equal to the ratio difference score/elapsed time

A simple reformulation of the equations of the latent difference model allows us to

demonstrate the similarities between the least restricted case of the Piecewise Linear Growth Model

and the Latent Difference Model. The least restricted Piecewise Linear Growth Model allows the

individual slopes to be different for each time-interval. Here are the equations:

11 ηη =

( )( )1212

1212 tt

tt−

−−+= ηηηη (25)

( )( ) ( )( )2323

2312

12

1213 tt

tttt

tt−

−−+−

−−+= ηηηηηη

… … ... … …

( )( ) ( )( ) ( )( )11

123

23

2312

12

121 ... −

− −−−

++−−−+−

−−+= qq

qq

qqq tt

tttt

tttt

tt

ηηηηηηηη

Page 34: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

25

The matrix Η was specified as an identity matrix in the Latent Difference Model, but if the

specification of Η is modified in:

−−

=

−1

23

12

..000

0........

0..00

0..00

0..001

qq tt

tt

tt

Η, (26)

then (25) can be written in matrix form as follows:

11 −−= ΤΤΤΗΗ (27)

which is equivalent to

−−

−−−−

−−−

−−−

=

−−

1

1

23

23

12

12

1

12312

2312

12

3

2

1

......1

0.........1

0...1

0...01

0...001

...

qq

qqqqq

tt

tt

tt

tttttt

tttt

tt

ηη

ηη

ηηη

η

ηηη

,

whereby the right side of the equation is presented by the product of matrix ΤΗ and vector

11 −− ΤΗ , respectively.

It is straightforward to demonstrate the equivalence between the Latent Difference Model

and the least restricted Piecewise Model. Since in (20) ξ was defined as

1−= ΤΤ , (20)

we can write (27) as:

Page 35: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

26

ΤΤΗΗ == −1, (28)

and the covariance matrix of the least restricted piecewise model is:

εΘΛΤΗΗΦΤΗΗΛΣ +

= −− ''''12

1 (29)

The covariance matrix (29) is of course the same as the covariance structure of the Latent

Difference Model (24):

( ) εΘΛΤΤΦΛΣ += ''2 (24)

Thus, the Latent Difference Model is equivalent to the least restricted version of a Piecewise

Linear Growth Model. No restrictions are imposed on the structural part of the SEM model and

hence the goodness of fit of the Latent Difference Model and the least restricted Piecewise linear

growth model is completely determined by the measurement part of the model.

Linear Growth Curve Model

The slopes in the piecewise model (with different slopes for each time-interval for each

individual) are simple transformations of the latent difference scores. However, if we restrict the

piecewise slopes to be equal (see Figure 6) for each subject and at the same time allow for time-

specific residuals (denoted as ζ t), we obtain the Linear Growth Curve Model. The time-specific

residuals, ζ t , represent the deviations of each subject around his/her linear growth curve.

If we denote ξ1

* as the intercept factor score and ξ

2

* as the slope factor score for an arbitrary

person, the equations can be described as follows:

Page 36: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

27

t1 t0 t2 t3 t4 t5

b0

Figure 6. Linear growth curve for a single subject

1*11 ζξη +=

( ) 212*2

*12 ζξξη +−+= tt

( ) ( ) 323*212

*2

*13 ζξξξη +−+−+= tttt (30)

… … … … …. ….

( ) ( ) ( ) qqqq tttttt ζξξξξη +−++−+−+= −1*223

*212

*2

*1 ...

which can be simplified:

1*11 ζξη +=

( ) 212*2

*12 ζξξη +−+= tt

( ) ( ) 313*2

*132312

*2

*13 ζξξζξξη +−+=+−+−+= tttttt (31)

… … … … …. ….

( ) ( ) qqqqqq tttttttt ζξξζξξη +−+=+−++−+−+= − 1*2

*112312

*2

*1 ...

Page 37: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

28

In matrix formulation we can express the equality restriction on the slopes for each subject

as follows:

( )( )( )( )( )

( )( )

−−

−−−−

=

1

1

23

23

12

12

1

1

...

qq

qq

tt

tt

tt

ηη

ηη

ηηη

Η

==

*2

*2

*2

*1

*

...

ξ

ξ

ξ

ξ

(32)

We now define the covariance matrix for the growth curve factors Φ3 as:

''E 12

1**3

−−=

= ΗΗΦΗΦ (33)

The assumption of equal slopes reduces the rank of the q × q matrix Φ3 in (33) to 2 and the

rank deficiency can be prevented by premultiplying and postmultiplying Φ3 by Ν and Ν

respectively, whereby Ν is a q by 2 matrix of fixed factor loadings, specified as follows:

=

10

....

10

10

01

Ν (34)

Let define the full rank version of Φ3 as Φ4:

'ΝΝΦΦ 43 = (35)

The reducing of the rank of Φ3 can be illustrated by showing the matrices including the elements:

Page 38: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

29

=

=

2222222221

22222221

22222221

12121211

3

..........

..

..

..

φφφφφ

φφφφ

φφφφ

φφφφ

Φ

1..110

0..001

10

....

10

10

01

2221

2111

φφ

φφ (36)

This results in the following model

( ) εΘΛΨΤΗΝΤΗΝΦΛΣ ++= ''''4 (37)

It is straightforward to show that this is the familiar linear latent growth model (with a

measurement model included) by defining ΤΗΝ as T*. Instead of using timesteps the relationship

with time is specified as the length of time that passed from the first measurement occasion.

( ) εΘΛΨΤΦΤΛΣ ++= ''** 4 (38)

ΤΗΝ=

Page 39: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

30

=

−−

− 10

......

10

10

01

...0000

..................

0...000

0...000

0...0001

1...1111

..................

0...0111

0...0011

0...0001

1

23

12

qq tt

tt

tt

−−

=

−−−−

−−−

− 1

13

12

1342312

2312

12

1

......

1

1

01

10

......

10

10

01

...1

..................

0...01

0...001

0...0001

tt

tt

tt

tttttttt

tttt

tt

qqq

(39)

The equivalence of models with the sum of time steps and models with the elapsed time is

demonstrated in Figure 7a and 7b.

1

1 1

1 1

t5-t4 t4-t3 t3-t2 t2-t1

1 1 1 1

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

slope factor

intercept factor

y13 y23 y33

Figure 7a

Page 40: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

31

1

t5-t1

t4-t1

t3-t1

t2-t1

1 1 1

1

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

slope factor intercept factor

y13 y23 y33

∗ ξ1 ∗ ξ2

Figure 7b Figure 7a and b. Two equivalent specifications of a Linear Growth Curve Model: Figure 7a: time

steps; Figure 7b: elapsed time from first measurement occasion. Measurement model partly shown;

covariances between unique factors of identical items omitted.

Equal Variance-Covariance Baseline Model

McArdle & Aber (1990) suggest before testing change models one should first reject the

following baseline model which they call the Equal Variance-Covariance Baseline Model. This

model predicts equal variances and covariances and excludes the existence of change factors. They

note that this model is equivalent with a one-factor model with equal factor loadings and equal

unique variances. The model is a special case of the Linear Growth Curve model described in (37).

This can by shown by fixing both ϕ22

and ϕ21

in (36) to zero. The matrix Ψ in (37) should be

specified as a diagonal residual matrix with all elements restricted to be equal. Freeing in (38) the

elements in T* and Ψ (except those for identification purposes) yields a conventional second

order factor model.

Page 41: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

32

The Equal Variance Covariance Model is also known as the Random Intercept Model (see

Figure 8), although the restriction of equal variances is here usually not made. This model specifies

that each individual trajectory can be described by an individual baseline and uncorrelated time-

specific deviations around this baseline.

The Random Intercept Model is also equivalent with a one factor repeated measures

ANOVA (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992). An advantage of the SEM approach is that the random

intercepts refer to the latent constructs if a measurement model is included.

Figure 8. Random Intercept Model

Note: not shown autocorrelations between unique factors of identical items.

Quasi-Wiener simplex

If the matrix Φ2 in Model (24) is restricted to a diagonal matrix, the resulting model is

known as the Quasi-Wiener simplex (Jöreskog, 1970). This model may be appropriate for a

stochastic process if the only source of change consists of uncorrelated time-specific increments.

The model is displayed in Figure 9.

1 1 1 1 1 1

ξ

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5 η6

Page 42: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

33

1 1 1 1 η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

η5 η4 η3 η2 η1

η2−η1 η1 η3−η2 η4−η3 η5−η4

Figure 9. Quasi-Wiener simplex; top panel reparameterized as a second order factor model, bottom

panel conventional SEM diagram; measurement model partly shown (only for T3); covariances

between unique factors of identical items omitted.

First Order Moving Average Difference Model

McArdle & Aber (1990) describe a model which they attribute to Wold. This is a first order

moving average or random shock model. In this model only adjacent waves share a common factor

Page 43: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

34

and this makes the model highly restrictive, because it predicts a zero covariance between waves

that are more than one measurement occasion apart. A graphical display is shown in Figure 10.

measument model

Transformation matrix Η

Transmission matrix Τ (all elements fixed to 1)

1 1 1 1 1

-1 -1 -1

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

y31 y32 y33

measument model η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

y31 y32 y33

ζ1 ζ2 ζ3 ζ4 ζ5

1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1

1 1 1 1 1

π1 π2 π3 π4 π5

Transformation matrix Κ

ω1 ω2 ω3 ω4 ω5

ξ1 ξ2 ξ3 ξ4 ξ5

Figure 10. First order moving average model; top panel reparametrized as a special case of model

(12); bottom panel conventional diagram; measurement model partly shown (only for T3);

covariances between unique factors of identical items omitted.

Page 44: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

35

By specifying the elements of the matrix Η as follows:

−=

1..1000

............

0..1010

0..0101

0..0010

0..0001

Η

, (40)

the product TH yields the desired permutation matrix:

TΗ =

−−

1 0 0 0 0

1 1 0 0 0

1 1 1 0 0

1 1 1 1 0

1 1 1 1 1

1 0 0 0 0

0 1 0 0 0

1 0 1 0 0

0 1 0 1 0

0 0 0 1 1

...

...

...

...

... ... ... ... ... ...

...

...

...

...

...

... ... ... ... ... ...

...

=

1 0 0 0 0

1 1 0 0 0

0 1 1 0 0

0 0 1 1 0

0 0 0 0 1

...

...

...

...

... ... ... ... ... ...

...

The function of matrix Η is ‘to break the chain’ and hence this model violates the principle that all

subsequent measurement waves are affected by previous measurement occasions.

The covariance for the First Order Moving Average Difference Model is:

( ) εΘΛΤΗΤΗΦΛΣ += '''2 (41)

The model can also be specified as a submodel of the general model (7) by specifying the

matrices ∆t as identity matrices except for the elements below the main diagonal in column t – 1.

These elements are fixed to –1(–1)(r-c), were r denotes the rth row and c the cth column and it

assumed that r > c. An example for a four-wave study is:

Page 45: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

36

∆t = 4 ∆t = 3 ∆t = 2 ΤΗ

=

1100

0110

0011

0001

1001

0101

0011

0001

1010

0110

0010

0001

1100

0100

0010

0001

In this example the matrix to the left, corresponds with t = 4 and column t – 1 = 3. Below the main

diagonal is element 4,3, which is fixed to –1(–1)(4 - 3) = 1

Quasi Markov Simplex

In the Linear Growth Curve Model the piecewise slopes were restricted to be equal for each

subject. An alternative specification is that the piecewise slopes can be predicted by the latent score

on the preceding measurement occasion. If we denote the regressioncoefficients as ct, t-1 and the

disturbance term as ζt , we can write the equations as:

11 ζη = (43)

( )( ) 2121

12

12 ζηηη +=−−

ctt

( )( ) 3232

23

23 ζηηη +=−−

ctt

( )( ) 4343

34

34 ζηηη +=−−

ctt

…. …. …

( )( ) qqqq

qq

qq ctt

ζηηη

+=−−

−−−

−11,

1

1

Page 46: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

37

It is convenient to express (43) in matrix formulation:

C +=−− 11ΤΗ (44)

where is Η defined as in (27) and C as follows:

=

− 0000

..........

0..00

0..00

0..000

1,

32

21

qqc

c

c

C (45)

The reduced form of (44) can be derived as follows:

C =−−− 11ΤΗ

( ) C =−−− 11ΤΗ

( ) C111 −−− −= ΤΤΗ (46)

Equation (46) can also be written as:

( ) C ΗΗΤΗ 1111 −−−− −= (47)

Using ( ) 111 −−− = ΒΑΒΑΒΑ we can write (47) as:

Page 47: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

38

( ) C ΗΤΗΗ111 −−− −= (48)

( ) C ΗΗΤ11 −− −= (49)

It is easy to verify that

( ) ( )*1 CC −=−− ΙΗΤ , (50)

where C* is defined as follows:

( )( )

( )

+−

+−+−

=

−− 01000

..........

0..010

0..001

0..000

11,

1232

0121*

qqqq ttc

ttc

ttc

C (51)

It may be helpful to show the specification of the matrices again:

=− *CΙ

−−

11..00

..........

00..10

00..11

00..01

−−

−1

23

12

..000

0........

0..00

0..00

0..001

qq tt

tt

tt

− 0000

..........

0..00

0..00

0..000

1,

32

21

qqc

c

c

If we now assume that all timesteps have equal length, we can use unit scaling and this turns Η into

an identity matrix. Now we define C** as:

Page 48: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

39

+

++

=

− 01000

..........

0..010

0..001

0..000

1,

32

21**

qqc

c

c

C (52)

and we write (49) as:

( ) C1** −

−= ΙΙ (53)

This is equivalent to the reduced form of the conventional presentation of the quasi Markov

simplex:

( ) 1−−= ΒΒΙ , (54)

where Β is specified as:

=

− 0000

..........

0..00

0..00

0..000

1,

32

21

qqβ

ββ

Β (55)

and now we can conclude that:

**C=Β (56)

Because piecewise slopes can be obtained from the original latent scores by a simple

transformation and the timesteps are given and fixed, the model (43) is only a simple

reparametrization of the Quasi Markov Simplex Model.

Page 49: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

40

Jöreskog (1970) described another interesting reparametrization of the Quasi Markov

Simplex. The submodel (12) reduces to (57) if we specify Ψ as a zero matrix and Κ as Ζ and Η as

Ζ-1:

εΘΛΖΤΖΦΖΤΖΛΣ +

= −− ''''12

1 (57)

The elements of Ζ are specified as follows:

=

=+

1

1,1

3221

21

..000

..........

0..00

0..00

0..001

q

iii

Ζ (58)

The model (57) is similar to Jöreskog’s reparametrization of the Quasi Markov Simplex. The

equivalence between model (57) and the more conventional formulation (Bollen, 1989) in (59)

( ) ( ) εΘΛΒΙΦΒΙΛΣ +

−−= −− ''11

, (59)

where Β is again specified as in (55), can easily be verified by showing the identity between

ΖΤΖ-1and (Ι − Β)

-1. The details are described in Appendix B. Both the AR(1) model in (57) and

(59) are shown in Figure 11.

Page 50: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

41

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

y13 y23 y33

Transmission matrix Τ (all elements fixed to 1)

measument

model Λ

Transformation matrix Ζ

π1 π2 π3 π4 π5

ω1 ω2 ω3 ω4 ω5

ξ1 ξ2 ξ3 ξ4 ξ5

Transformation matrix Ζ−1 1 1/β21 1/β21β32 1/β21β32β43 1/β21β32β43β54

β21β32β43β54 β21β32β43 β21β32 β21 1

η1

η2

η3

η4

η5

y13

y23

y33

β21 β32 β43 β54

Figure 11. First Order Markov Simplex: top panel reparametrized model; bottom panel

conventional diagram; measurement model only shown for T3 and autocorrelation between unique

item factors not shown.

Page 51: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

42

The First Order Autoregressive Model can also be specified using the general model (7) by

specifying ∆t as identity matrices except for element δ in row r = t and column c = t –1 in each

matrix. All δ’s are free parameters which are equal to the βs. A SEM display is shown in Figure 12.

measument model (including first order factor loading matrix)

second order factor loading matrix

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

β21

β32

β43

β54

1 1 1 1 1

1 1 1 1 1

1 1 1 1 1

1 1 1 1 1

third order factor loading matrix

fourth order factor loading matrix

fifth order factor loading matrix

ζ1 ζ2 ζ3 ζ4 ζ5

Figure 12. AR(1) model as a general model (7)

Page 52: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

43

In Appendix B the equivalence between the AR(1) formulation using model (7) and the

conventional formulation will be proven for a five-wave model. Here we only show the equivalence

between both versions of the AR(1) model:

∆5 ∆4 ∆3 ∆2 (60)

=

10000

01000

00100

0001

00001

10000

01000

0010

00010

00001

10000

0100

00100

00010

00001

1000

01000

00100

00010

00001

21

32

43

54

β

β

β

β

r = t = 5 r = t = 4 r = t = 3 r = t = 2

c = t – 1= 4 c = t – 1 = 3 c = t – 1 = 2 c = t – 1 = 1

=

1

01

001

0001

00001

54544343433254433221

434332433221

323221

21

ββββββββββ

ββββββ

βββ

β (61)

It is easily verified that the above matrix is the same as (Ι − Β)-1

if Β is specified as in (55).

Second Order Autoregressive Model

A Second Order Autoregressive Model (further denoted as AR(2)) cannot conveniently be

treated as a special case of submodel (12), but it fits nicely into the framework provided by the

general longitudinal model (7). The disadvantage of submodel (12) is that the transmission matrix is

inflexible and in cases where more complex forms of transmissions are needed there seems to be no

convenient way to treat these models as special cases of submodel (12). Transmission is more

complicated if more than one source of information partly travels along the same paths. Model (7)

Page 53: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

44

is, however, easily adaptable. The second order structure can be incorporated by specifying ∆t as

identity matrices except for the following two elements δs in each matrix: The element in row r = t

and column c = t –1 and secondly the element in column c = t –2 (except if t = 2). All δs are free

parameters and equal to the βs in the conventional representation of the AR(2) model. A SEM

display both for a conventional and the alternative version of the AR(2) model is shown in Figure

13. We show the equivalence of both models for a five-wave study: ∏−

=+−

1

11

q

jjq∆ =

∆5 ∆4 ∆3 ∆2 (62)

=

10000

01000

00100

0001

00001

10000

01000

001

00010

00001

10000

010

00100

00010

00001

100

01000

00100

00010

00001

21

3231

4342

5453

β

ββ

ββ

ββ

r = t = 5 r = t = 4 r = t = 3 r = t = 2

c = t – 1 = 4;

c = t – 2 = 3

c = t – 1 = 3;

c = t – 2 = 2

c = t – 1 = 2;

c = t – 1 = 1

c = t – 1 = 1

It is easily verified that the same result will be obtained from (Ι − Β2)-1

whereby Β2 is

specified as follows:

=

100

010

001

0001

00001

5453

4342

3231

21

2

ββββ

βββ

Β (63)

Thus, ( ) ∏=−−

=+−

− 1

11

12

q

jjq∆ΒΙ

Page 54: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

45

measument model (including first order factor loading matrix)

second order factor loading matrix

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

β21

β31 β32

β42 β43

β54

1 1 1 1 1

1 1 1 1 1

1 1 1 1 1

1 1 1 1 1

third order factor loading matrix

fourth order factor loading matrix

fifth order factor loading matrix

ζ1 ζ2 ζ3 ζ4 ζ5

β53

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

Figure 13. AR(2) model: top panel specified as a special case of general model (7); bottom panel conventional model.

Page 55: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

46

Autoregressive Effects For Latent Differences

In the Linear Growth Model the piecewise slopes of each subject are assumed to be equal:

The growth of every subject is characterized by a constant rate of change. For some data sets this

assumption may be overly restrictive. This assumption may be alleviated by giving up the equality

constraints for the slopes, and instead only assuming that the slopes can be predicted by the slopes

on a previous measurement occasion. This means that changes can be predicted by previous

changes. For identification purposes we have to assume that this is a fixed regression. If the

prediction is perfect, then this model reduces to the Linear Growth Model.

One can decide whether to regress also the first latent difference on the latent scores of the

first measurement occasion or only use latent difference scores as predictors. First we describe the

model which includes an autoregression on the first latent score. Later we will show that only a

small adjustment in the model specification is needed if only autoregression is assumed on previous

latent differences and the first latent score is ignored. If we denote the fixed regressioncoeffients as

α’s and the disturbance term as ζ’s, the equations can be described as follows:

11 ζη = (64)

( )( ) 2121

12

12 ζηαηη +=−−

tt

( )( )

( )( ) 3

12

1232

23

23 ζηηαηη +−−=

−−

tttt

( )( )

( )( ) 4

23

2343

34

34 ζηηαηη +−−=

−−

tttt

…. …. …

( )( )

( )( ) q

qq

qqqq

qq

qq

ttttζ

ηηα

ηη+

−−

=−−

−−

−−−

11

211,

1

1

The dependent variables, the piecewise slopes, can be transformed into latent scores by first

multiplying both sides of all equations (except the first equation) by the time step and next moving

the previous latent score to the right side. We show this for the third equation in (64). Both sides

multiplying by the timestep gives:

Page 56: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

47

( ) ( ) ( )( )

+

−−−=− 3

12

12322323 ζηηαηη

tttt (65)

( )( )( ) ( ) 32312

23

2332 ζηηα tt

tt

tt −+−−−= ,

and moving 1η− to the right side gives:

( )( )

( )( ) ( ) 3231

12

233222

12

23323 ζηαηηαη tt

tt

tt

tt

tt −+−−−+

−−= (66)

To simplify (66) we can write:

*31

*3222

*323 ζηαηηαη +−+= = (67)

( ) *31

*322

*32 1 ζηαηα +−+ ,

where

( )( )12

2332

*32 tt

tt

−−= αα , and ( ) 323

*3 ζζ tt −= . (68)

If we assume that all timesteps have equal length, we can scale each timestep to the value of 1:

11 ζη = (69)

( ) 21212 1 ζηαη ++=

( ) 31322323 1 ζηαηαη +−+=

( ) qqqqqqqq ζηαηαη +−+= −−−− 21,11, 1

For deriving the covariance matrix of η it is more convenient to express (69) in matrix formulation:

+= 2Α , (70)

where Α2 is specified as:

Page 57: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

48

+−

+−+−

+

=

−− 01..0000

................

000..0000

000..010

000..001

000..0001

000..0000

1,1,

4343

3232

21

2

qqqq αα

αααα

α

Α

(71)

The equations in (69) and the specification of Α2 in (71) show a second order autoregressive

structure, although we started in (64) with a first order autoregressive structure. The matrix

formulation of (64) is:

+= −−−− 111

11 ΤΗΑΤΗ , (72)

where Α1 is a matrix of first order autoregressive coefficients, defined as:

=

− 0..000

00..000

............

00..00

00..00

00..000

1,

32

21

1

qqα

αα

Α (73)

The relationship between the first and the second order autoregressive coefficients in this model can

be demonstrated by the following easily verifiable identity:

11

12

−− +−= ΤΤΑΤΙΑ (74)

Page 58: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

49

We first derive the covariance matrix of η by using the first order autoregressive structure

of (74), and later show the equivalent matrix for the second order structure. To obtain the reduced

form of (74) some algebraic manipulations are needed: First the η vector on the right side have to

move to the left side:

=− −−−− 111

11 ΤΗΑΤΗ (75)

This can be simplified to:

( ) =− −− 111 ΤΗΑΙ (76)

Premultiplying both sides with ( ) 11

−− ΑΑΙΤΗ gives:

( ) 1

1−−= ΑΑΙΤΗ (77)

The covariance matrix for η is then:

[ ] ( ) [ ]( ) ''''' EE ΤΗΑΙΑΙΤΗ 11

11

−− −−= (78)

If we again define E[ζζ]= ψ, the covariance for y is then:

( ) ( ) εΘΛΤΗΑΙΨΑΙΛΤΗΣ +−−= −− ''''11

11 (79)

Using again Jöreskog’s parametrization of the first order autoregressive model, whereby Ζα is

defined as:

Page 59: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

50

=

=+

=+

q

ttt

q

ttt

1,1

1

1,1

3221

21

0..000

0..000

............

00..00

00..00

00..001

α

α

αα

α

αΖ

, (80)

we can write:

( ) ( ) =−− −− '11

11 ΑΙΨΑΙ '''

αααα ΖΤΖΦΤΖΖ 14

1 −− (81)

Thus, (79) can be written as:

εαααα ΘΛΤΗΖΤΖΦΤΖΛΤΗΖΣ += −− ''''''14

1 (82)

The model (82) is displayed in Figure 14.

Page 60: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

51

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

y13 y23 y33 measument

model Λ

Transformation matrix Ζ−1

Transformation matrix Ζ

Transmission matrix Τ (all elements fixed to 1)

π1 π2 π3 π4 π5

ω1 ω2 ω3 ω4 ω5

ξ1 ξ2 ξ3 ξ4 ξ5

ρ1 ρ2 ρ3 ρ4 ρ5

Transmission matrix Τ (all elements fixed to 1)

Transformation matrix Κ

ϖ1 ϖ2 ϖ3 ϖ4 ϖ5

Figure 14. AR(1) structure imposed on Latent Difference Model

Next we show the equivalence between the covariance matrix in (82) using a first order

autoregressive structure and the covariance matrix based upon the second order structure.

We again show the identity in (74):

11

12

−− +−= ΤΤΑΤΙΑ (74)

Page 61: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

52

Moving Α1 to the left side and Α2 to the right side gives:

12

11

−− +−= ΤΤΙΑΤΑ (83)

Postmultiplying both sides with Τ-1 gives:

ΤΤΤΤΑΑ 121

−+−= (84)

If we substitute (84) in ( ) 11

−− ΑΑΙ gives:

( ) =− −11ΑΙ =−+−

−−− 112

1 ΤΤΤΤΑΤΤ

( ) =−+−−−− 11

21 ΤΤΙΑΤ

( ) =− −12 ΤΑΙ

( ) 12

1 −− − ΑΑΙΤ (85)

Substituting this result into (79) with an additional assumption of equal time steps (Η = Ι), we can

write:

( ) ( ) εΘΛΤΤΑΙΨΑΙΛΤΤΣ +−−= −−−− '''' 112

12

1, (86)

which simplifies to:

( ) ( ) εΘΛΑΙΨΑΙΛΣ +−−= −− ''12

12 (87)

Formula (87) describes the covariance matrix for a second order autoregressive model.

Page 62: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

53

So far we have discussed the option where the slopes of the first time interval are regressed

on the initial score. If we change this option into an alternative assumption which states that the first

slopes are only allowed to be correlated with the initial score, only some small adjustment have to

be made. The second equation in (64) has to be changed to:

( )( ) 2

12

12 ζηη =−−

tt (88)

Consequently, in (79) the matrices Τand Ζα have to be replaced by respectively:

=

1..110

..........

0..110

0..010

0..001

*Τ (89)

and

∏=

=+

=+

q

ttt

q

ttt

1,1

1

1,1

32*

0..000

0..000

............

00..00

00..010

00..001

α

α

α

αΖ

.

The matrix Φ4 is no longer diagonal, since the parameter cov(ζ1, ζ2) has to be estimated.

Page 63: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

54

Bollen and Curran Hybrid model:

A Synthesis between AR(1) and the Latent Growth Model

Recently Bollen & Curran (in press) introduced a synthesis between the AR(1) and the

Latent Growth Curve Model. The model is shown in Figure 15.

η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

y15 y25 y35

I S

1 1 1 1 1

t2

t3 t4 t5

Slope factor Intercept factor

Figure 15. Synthesis between Quasi Markov Simplex and Linear Growth Curve Model;

measurement model only shown for T3.

The Bollen and Curran hybrid model can also be specified using parts of submodel (12).

Since the hybrid model integrates two models based upon different concepts of change it is obvious

that it is not possible to treat this hybrid model as a special case of submodel (12). However, all that

is needed is a small modification of submodel (12): Only one additional equation is required. For

ease of presentation we present the linear growth curve specification in the simple form using ξ* as

in (32) and T* as in (38) and (39).

Page 64: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

55

The relations between the factors for the hybrid model can be defined as:

+= **Τ , (91)

where ρ is a q × 1 vector of latent factors, T* is a q × 2 matrix of fixed basis coefficients2 as in

(39), ξ* is a 2 × 1 vector of growth curve factors (intercept and slope factor, respectively), and ξ is

a q × 1 vector of latent factors, necessary for the AR(1) specification. Equation (91) is the

modification needed to incorporate both models (note that the right side of (91) adds two vectors,

one for the growth part and one for the AR(1) part). The second equation is:

1−= ΖΖ , (92)

where ω is a q × 1 vector of latent factors as in (15) and Ζ is as a q × q diagonal matrix of products

of first order autoregressive coefficients in (58). The third equation is the same as in (15):

Τ= , (15)

where π is q × 1 vector of latent factors and T is lower q × q triangular matrix with all elements

fixed to the value of 1 as in (11). Finally, if we now specify ζ as a q × 1 zero vector, the last

equation changes to:

Ζ= (93)

Substituting (91) and (92) in (93) gives:

( ) += − **1 ΤΖΤΖ (94)

2 basis coefficients is the term used by Meredith & Tisak (1990) for denoting the factor loadings for growth curve models.

Page 65: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

56

Assuming that the growth curve factors in ξ* are independent of the autoregressive factors in ξ the

covariance matrix of η is as follows:

( ) '''''Cov ΖΤΖΦΤΦΤΖΤΖ 12

*4

*1 −−

+= (95)

where Φ4 is a 2 × 2 covariance matrix of the growth curve factors as in (36) and Φ2 is a q × q

diagonal matrix of residual3 variances pertaining to the AR(1) part of the model as in (57).

The covariance matrix for the observed variables is:

( )( ) '' CovCov yy ++== ΛΛΣ =

εΘΛΖΤΖΦΤΦΤΖΤΖΛ +

+ −− ''''' 12

*4

*1 (96)

The derivation of (96) can be found in Appendix C. In Appendix C the equivalence between the

covariance structure of (96) and the conventional specification of the model (as shown in Figure 15)

is demonstrated.

The alternative specification of the Bollen and Curran Hybrid Model is displayed in Figure

16.

3 except that element (1,1) in (57) is the variance of the first latent construct of the AR(1) series and not a residual variance as in (95).

Page 66: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

57

1/β21β32β43

β21β32β43

η5

Transmission

matrix Τ (all elements fixed to 1)

Transformation

matrix Ζ

π5

ω5

ρ5

Transformation

matrix Ζ−1

1/β21β32β43β54

β21β32β43β54

y13 y23 y33

η1

π1

ω1

ρ1

1

1

1/β21

η2

π2

ω2

ρ2

β21

η3

π3

ω3

ρ3

1/β21β32

β21β32

η4

π4

ω4

ρ4

1 1

1 1

t2-t1

t3-t1 t4-t1 t5-t1

Slope factor

ξ1∗

ξ2∗ Intercept factor

ξ2 ξ1 ξ4 ξ3 ξ5 1

1 1 1 1

1

Figure 16. Alternative specification of Bollen & Curran Model; measurement model only shown for T3.

Page 67: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

58

Multivariate Models

Simple multivariate models, including more than one series of latent constructs, can be

formulated by extending the submodel (12). It is convenient to make use of partioned matrices. For

example, a cross-domain latent growth curve model, including two growth curves, can be specified

by replacing ΤΗΝ by:

222

111

ΝΗΤΝΗΤ

0

0 (94)

All other matrices should be partioned accordingly. The covariance matrix between the growth

curve parameters appear in the off-diagonal partition of Φ3.

However, more complex multivariate models are more easily specified by extending the

general model (7). Again, some new complexities, created by specifying relationships between the

sets of latent constructs, do not seem to fit well into submodel (12). Although it is possible to extend

submodel (12) for limited classes of relationships between the series, the causal structures needed

for many models are not easily specified as extension of model (12). The explanation for the

inflexibility of submodel (12) is the use of a direct form of transmission of information from earlier

to subsequent waves. Many complex models require indirect forms of transmission which can be

accomplished by dividing the transmission into multiple paths. In this way also relationships can be

specified which use only parts of the transmission.

To extend model (7) to a multivariate model only the matrix ∆ need to be partioned into

suitable specified submatrices. On the main diagonal the matrices for the univariate time series are

placed and on below the main diagonal the matrices specifying the paths between the series are

located. For a multivariate model, consisting of two time series, the model can be described as:

εΘΛ∆∆

∆Φ

∆∆∆

ΛΣ +

=

=

=

''

q

t tt

tq

t tt

t 1

12

1

1 2221

11

2221

1100

(95)

Page 68: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

59

two time series with lagged effects for one of the variables. The model is shown in Figure 17 and in

Figure 18 as a conventional lagged AR(1) model.

ω1 ω2 ω3 ω4 ω5 ω6 ω7 ω8

1

ξ1 ξ2 ξ3 ξ4

1 1 1

ξ5 ξ6 ξ7 ξ8

1

β87

1 1

η1 η2 η3 η4

y13 y23 y33

η5 η6 η7 η8

y13 y23 y33

1 1 1 1

π1 π2 π3 π4 π5 π6 π7 π8

1 1 1

β21

β32

1

β43

β65

β76

1

β61

β72

β83

φ51

Figure 17. Model with two First Order Markov Simplexes with lagged effects of one variable;

measurement model only partly shown; η1 and η5 refer to T1, η2 and η6 refer to T2, etc.

Page 69: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

60

β76 β87 β98 β10,9 η6 η7 η8 η9 η10

β21 β32 β43 β54 η1 η2 η3 η4 η5

β71 β82 β93 β10,4

Figure 18. Conventional SEM diagram of model including two AR(1) series with lagged effects;

measurement model only partly shown; η1 and η6 refer to T1, η2 and η7 refer to T2, etc.

Because the matrices on the main diagonal are specified as AR(1) matrices and equal to (60), and

given that the inverse of triangular matrices has a special structure, we only show the specification

of the off-diagonal matrices.

212121 234 ∆∆∆

0000

0000

000

0000

0000

000

0000

0000

000

0000

0000

0000

61

72

83

β

β

β

(96)

To demonstrate the equivalence between the model (95) and the conventional parametrization we

denoted the coefficients in (96) as betas. In Appendix D the equivalence of model (95) and the

conventional model is demonstrated.

Page 70: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

61

Invariance of parameters in Latent Growth Curve Models

A problem of the specification of a Linear Growth Curve Model is that for most applications

in the social sciences the scaling of the time axis (fixing the basis coefficients) is arbitrary: Both the

origin and the metric is not inherently tied to the real timeframe for the data of the study. In the

following the (in)variance of the growth parameters for a cross-domain model (Linear Growth

Curve Model consisting of two growth curves, cf. Willett & Sayer, 1995) is investigated under a

linear transformation of the timescale.

jj tt βα +=* (97)

The implied covariance structure of the original scaling is (using a conventional specification and

ignoring the matrix of residuals):

'ΛΨΛΣ = , (98)

which matrices are specified as follows:

4321

4321

44434241

43333231

42322221

41312111

4

3

2

1

4

3

2

1

0000

11110000

0000

00001111

100

100

100

100

001

001

001

001

tttt

tttt

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

ψψψψψψψψψψψψψψψψ

(99)

A linear transformation of basis coefficients yields:

++++

++++

=

4

3

2

1

4

3

2

1

4

3

2

1

4

3

2

1

100

100

100

100

001

001

001

001

000

100

000

001

100

100

100

100

001

001

001

001

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

δγδγδγδγ

βαβαβαβα

δγ

βα

(100)

Page 71: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

62

If we denote P as follows:

=

δ

γ

β

α

000

100

000

001

Ρ, (101)

Because estimation methods like Maximum Likelihood and Generalized Least Squares are scale-

free (Long, 1984, p.58) transformations of the factor loadings yield the same expected covariance

matrix, because the scale transformations can completely be absorbed by corresponding changes in

the factor (co)variances. Therefore, we can write:

''''' 1111 −−−− =⇒== ΨΡΨΡΡΦΛΡΨΡΛΡΡΛΨΛΣ (102)

where

1

1 0 0

10 0 0

0 0 1

10 0 0

α

β

β

γ

δ

δ

=

(103)

and the elements of this final matrix are: '11 −−= ΨΡΨΡΡΦ

Page 72: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

63

Φ =

−−

+−+−+−−

+−

244

44243424241

442

2

4333423242413231

222

22221

222

2

2111

111

21

1

2

δψψ

δγψ

δψ

βδψ

βδαψ

δ

ψδγψ

δγψψ

βδγψ

βψ

δγ

βαψ

δγψ

βαψ

βψψ

βαψ

β

ψβαψ

βαψ

(104)

Of course one can transform the new parameters in Φ to obtain the old ones.

== 'ΡΦΡΨ

( )( )

( )

++++++++

+++

442

4443424241

442

4333423242413231

222

2221

222

2111

2

2

φδγφφδβδφδαφδφφγγφφγφφβγαφγφαφφ

φβαφφβφααφφ

(105)

In the case one is only interested in focusing on another zero timepoint, the matrix, used for

the linear transformation simplifies to:

=

1000

100

0010

001

α

α

Ρ (106)

and

=−

1000

100

0010

001

1

α

α

Ρ

(107)

so that:

Page 73: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

64

++++

++++

=

4

3

2

1

4

3

2

1

4

3

2

1

4

3

2

1

100

100

100

100

001

001

001

001

1000

100

0010

001

100

100

100

100

001

001

001

001

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

t

αααα

αααα

α

α

(108)

Φ =

−−

+−+−+−−

+−

444443424241

442

43334232422

413231

222221

222

2111

2

2

ψαψψψαψψ

ψααψψαψψψααψαψψ

ψαψψ

ψααψψ (109)

As one can see the variances of both slopes are not affected by a translation on the time axis,

neither is the covariance between the slopes. Only if the variance of the slope factor is nonzero, the

covariance between the intercept factor and the slope factor changes by a translation on the time-

axis (cf. Rovine & Molenaar, 1998). This makes sense because the slope factor scores are constants

for each subject. However, the relative positions of the subjects change over time in the case there is

variance in slopes. So the subjects change into different directions and given a constant slope, the

covariance has to change as well. Equivalently, one can start reasoning with a given fixation

scheme of the basis coefficients, for instance fixing the first basiscoefficient to the value of zero.

Next one is interested in finding the covariance between the intercept factor and the slope factor at

an arbitrary time point t. The covariance equals COV(η1+tη2, η2) = COV(η1, η2) + tVAR(η2, η2) =

ψ21 + tψ22. The same result can be obtained by a translation on the time axis by α = −t. The

covariance is ψ21 − αψ22 = ψ21 − (−t)ψ22 = ψ21 + tψ22. In case the growths curves are not parallel,

the covariance between the intercept factor and slope factor can only be conditionally interpreted:

each time point yields a different covariance and represents the state of affairs at that particular

time. In the case of nonparallel slopes it is misleading to speak of the covariance between level and

shape, as sometimes appears in the literature: the level changes constantly if there are

interindividual differences in growth trajectories, so does the covariance.

Page 74: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

65

Discussion

In this paper I presented a general longitudinal model and described how special cases of

this model yielded several well-known longitudinal models. In Figure 1 this family of longitudinal

models is displayed. The submodel (12) encompasses all models except the AR(2) model, which

should be regarded as a direct descendent of the general longitudinal model (7). The second order

autoregressive structure needs a mechanism of transmission which is more complicated than the

submodel (12) can provide. Therefore the broader model (7) was used.

The submodel can be divided into two categories: models that need the matrix Κ and

models and models that restrict this matrix to an identity matrix. Autoregressive models need the

matrix Κ in order to model the decaying of information over time (also called entropy by Dwyer,

1987). The effect of the same source of variance (initial or innovation) on a variable depends upon

the passing of time. Although time is not explicitly modeled in the AR models, it is shown that the

factor time can be incorporated if the piecewise slopes are used as dependent variables. Whatever

the exact functional relationship with time may look like, in many applications the secant line (a

line connecting two points on a curve; mathematical term for piecewise slope) might be a

reasonable approximation of the real curve (Rogosa, Brandt & Zimowski, 1982, p. 728; Willett,

1989). Although to the best of my knowledge autoregression for piecewise slopes has not been

described in the literature, this approach might be fruitful in cases where the time steps differ

among subjects. In this case the difference score divided by the elapsed time between the occasions

on which the subjects has been observed, may be a better estimate for modeling the developmental

process.

Another model which is, again to the best of my knowledge, unknown in the literature is the

autoregression on latent changes. Previous changes may predict later changes and this may or may

not be independent of the level of the score (note that two versions of this model have been

proposed). A developmental process, in which change is directional, may better be modeled by

predicting changes by previous changes. This model argues that change is both directional and

(partly) predictable. These characteristics are shared with the growth curve models, but unlike these

models random changes can be incorporated into the true scores and no functional relationship with

time has to be made. Directional change contrasts with the conventional AR models in which the

innovations are normally treated as independent of each other (but see Finkel, 1996).

Page 75: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

66

The models that restricted the matrix Κ to an identity matrix were the Latent Difference

Model, the Linear Growth Curve Model and the First Order Moving Difference Model. One could

question if the Latent Difference Model (and also the equivalent Smallest Piecewise Linear Growth

Model) should be considered to be a model, since no further restriction are imposed and only a

transformation of the scores is obtained. This may be true, but the ‘model’ provides some major

advantages over more restricted models. First it does not depend on characteristics of data and they

can be used if the developmental process is unknown. In contrast, Growth Curve Models and first-

order Autoregressive Models make strong assumptions about the developmental process. Growth

Curve Models assume that the explicit relationship with time is tenable, and autoregressive models

assume that the innovations are stochastically independent of each other. Second, as Arminger

(1987, p. 339) argues that difference scores protect against misspecification of the model by

omitting stable variables: the estimation of the model is still consistent even though the model is

partly specified. This an important advantage of panel studies, since omitted variable bias is one of

the most severe threats for the validity of any model (Dormann, 1999). However, Arminger warns

(on p. 340) that this protection is no longer present if the lagged dependent variable is included in

the regression model (as in AR models). Third, latent difference scores may be better predictors

than the original scores in many panel studies. Models including multiple repeatedly measured

variables are often modeled as autoregressive models with an additional cross-lagged structure

explaining the causal effects upon each other. By partialling out the stabilities the predictors explain

residual changes. However the predictor itself is in this model not a change variable, but instead the

latent score is used. For some data sets it might be better to use the changes in one variable to

predict (residual) changes in another variable.

In the Latent Difference Model the covariance matrix was unconstrained, but restricting this

matrix as a diagonal matrix yields the Wiener Simplex. An interesting application of the Wiener

Simplex can be found in modeling individual change (Mellenbergh & Van de Brink, 1998).

The Smallest Piecewise Growth Curve Model is equivalent to the Latent Difference Model.

Restricting the slopes of each subject as equal assumes a constant rate of change for each subject

and this results in a restricted version of the Linear Growth Curve Model. This model is overly

restrictive, because it does not allow for deviations around the curves. Thus, the conventional

presentation of the Linear Growth Curve Model is not nested in the Latent Difference Model, but is

a special case of the submodel (12). One aspect of the Linear Growth Curve Model is its reduction

of dimensions: only two common factors (an intercept and a slope factor) are necessary to describe

the systematic part of the growth. However, for every measurement occasion time-specific residuals

Page 76: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

67

are specified. In order to reduce the number of dimensions the matrix Ν is used: the submodel (12)

provides as many factors as there are measurement occasions, but the Linear Growth Curve Model

and also the First Order Moving Difference Model only need a few. So a provision is made in the

submodel to reduce the dimensions. A further reduction of dimensions of Linear Growth Curve

Model yields the Random Intercept Model: both the variance and the mean of the slope factor are

fixed to the value of zero. Of course these new restrictions make the relationship with the factor

time redundant.

A pedagogical advantage of presenting longitudinal models as factor models is that these

representations focus more clearly on the exogenous sources of variance which underlie all the

relationships between the latent construct over time. All longitudinal models start with initial

variance, which will is passed to present and future measurement occasions. Subsequently new

sources of information come into play and these have to be transferred again. The models differ in

how they picture these forms of input and how they transmit this information to the present and

subsequent waves.

The relation between longitudinal models and factor models has another interesting feature:

all longitudinal models reduce to one-factor models if the variance(s) of the change factor(s)

(innovation terms in the AR models and slope factor in the growth models) become(s) zero. In this

case the single common factor represents the initial scores. This one-factor model may include

time-specific disturbance terms which turns it into a random intercept model.

Paradoxically, although in the Latent Growth Curve Model the level 1 parameters are treated

as random and the autoregressive coefficients are fixed in the population, the assumptions about the

underlying developmental processes in both models are opposite: growth curves models assume a

constant, fixed process for each subject, whereas the autoregressive models assume a stochastic

process, where random changes are incorporated into the true scores for each subject.

Recently a synthesis between the seemingly antagonistic AR and Latent Growth Curve

Models were made. Interestingly, it was not possible to use submodel (12) to include both structures

into a single hybrid model, although both models could be fitted separately within this framework.

This can be explained by the different functions the factors have in the AR and Latent Growth

Curve Model. In the AR model the first factor represents the initial scores and all subsequent factors

are innovation terms. In contrast the first factor in the Latent Growth Curve Model represents the

start4 of the growth trajectories for each person and this is only one of the two components for the

initial score (the second is the time-specific residual). The slope factor is quite different from the

4 Assuming a conventional fixation scheme of fixing the first basis coefficient to “0”.

Page 77: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

68

innovation terms in two ways: First it is tied to the time dimension and secondly it is usually

correlated with the intercept factor. Innovations terms have no relationship with the time factor and

are assumed to be independent of each other.

The interpretation of the Bollen & Curran model in terms of the underlying developmental

process seems rather complicated. Since two simultaneously operating processes are involved, the

interpretation of each process should account for the existence of the other. For ease of reasoning

one consider a two-stage procedure: One can either start from a Latent Growth Curve Model and

subsequently extend the model with an autoregressive structure or one can start with a first order

Autoregressive Model and later add an intercept and slope factor. Lets start with a linear Growth

Curve Model. Provided that a measurement model is included and the growth curves refer to the

true variates, for each subject the time-specific deviations around his/her growth curve can be

interpreted as caused by the particular state the subject was in at that moment (Garst, Frese,

Molenaar, in press; see also McArdle & Woodcock, 1997). Although states are by definition

transient they may show a regular pattern over time (e.g., oscillatory) and may influence the

outcome at the next measurement occasion. Thus, one interpretation of the Bollen & Curran model

is that development of the state components can be characterized by an autoregressive nature and in

this model stochastic time-specific changes can have a lasting (although decaying) effect on later

outcomes. If one compares Figures 15 and 16 one can notice that the disturbance terms in Figure 15

have been replaced by the autoregressive factors on top of Figure 16.

Alternatively, one can first conceive a first order autoregressive model and in a second stage

one can cancel the independence assumption for the innovation term by adding an intercept and a

slope factor. Innovation terms are deviations for each subject from the regression line (which is

assumed as fixed in the population). For each subject these innovations are now partly predictable

by his/her linear growth curve. Thus, the second interpretation of the Bollen & Curran hybrid model

is that it is a growth curve model for the innovation terms of a first order AR model.

In summary, the hybrid model decomposes change into three components: First, a

component which refers to a continuous underlying growth process which is constant for all

measurement occasions for each individual, and secondly, an autoregressive process which is time-

specific and constant for all subjects and finally a stochastic component which is both time-specific

and different for each subject.

The final question is: How flexible is the factor framework for longitudinal models? For

instance, in the multivariate models: Can also cross-lagged, synchronous and reciprocal effects be

specified? We think that the multivariate extension of the general model is flexible enough to fit

recursive models. However, in models including reciprocal relationships the matrix of structural

Page 78: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

69

regression coefficients cannot be rearranged into a triangular form and it seems that these models go

beyond the structure of factor models as described in this paper.

References Bast, J.W., & Reitsma, P. (1997). Matthew effects in reading: A comparison of latent growth curve

models and simplex models with structured means. Multivariate Behavior Research, 32, 135-167.

Browne, M. W. (1993). Structured latent curve models. In C.M. Cuadras & C.R. Rao (Eds.),

Multivariate analysis: Future directions 2, (pp.171–197). New York: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Bryk, A.S. & Raudenbush, S.W. (1992). Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and data

analysis methods. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

Browne (1982). Covariance structures. In D.M. Hawkins (Ed.). Topics in Applied Multivariate

Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Cronbach, L.J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure "change" - or should we?

Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68-80.

Curran, P & Bollen, K. (in press). The best of both worlds: combining autoregressive and latent

curve models. To appear in A. Sayer & L.Collins (Eds.). New Methods for the Analysis of Change.

Dormann C. (1999). Modeling unmeasured third variables in longitudinal studies on organizational

stress. The case of social stressors at work, irritation, and depression. Doctoral Dissertation.

Johann Wolfgang Goethe Universität Frankfurt am Main.

Jöreskog, K.G. (1970). Estimation and testing of simplex models. British Journal of Mathematical

and Statistical Psychology, 23, 121-145.

Kenny, D.A., & Campbell, D.T. (1989). On the measurement of stability in over-time data. Journal

of Personality, 57, 445-481.

Long, S.C. (1984). Confirmatory factor analysis. Sage Universities Paper series on Quantitative

Applications in the Social Sciences, series no. 07-033.Beverly Hills and London: Sage Pubns.

Mandys, F., Dolan, C.V., & Molenaar, P.C.M. (1994). Two aspects of the simplex model:

Goodness of fit to linear growth curve structures and the analysis of mean trends. Journal of

Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 19, 201-215.

McArdle, J.J., & Nesselroade, J.R. (1994). Using multivariate data to structure developmental

change. In: S.H. Cohen & H.W. Reese (Eds.). Life-span developmental psychology: methodological

contributions (pp. 223-267). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Page 79: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

70

Mellenbergh, G.J., & Van den Brink, W.P. (1998). The measurement of individual change.

Psychological Methods, 3, 470-485.

McArdle, J.J., & Aber, M.S. (1990). Patterns of change within Latent Variable Structural Equation

Models. In: A. Von Eye (Ed.). Statistical methods in longitudinal research. Volume 1. Principles

and structuring change.(pp. 151-224). Academic Press: Boston

McArdle, J.J., & McDonald, R.P. (1984). Some algebraic properties of the Reticular Action Model

for moment structures. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 37, 234-251.

McArdle, J.J., & Woodcock, R.W. (1997). Expanding test-retest designs to include developmental

time-lag components. Psychological Methods, 2, 403-435.

McCloy, R.A., Campbell, J.P., & Cudeck, R. (1994). A confirmatory test of a model of performance

determinants. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 493-505.

Meredith, W., & Tisak, J. (1990). Latent curve analysis. Psychometrika, 55, 107-122.

Muthén, B.O., & Curran, P.J. (1997). General longitudinal modeling of individual differences in

experimental designs: A latent variable framework for analysis and power estimation.

Psychological Methods, 2, 371-402.

Rindskopf, D. (1984). Using phantom and imaginary latent variables to parameterize constraints in

linear structural models. Psychometrika, 49, 37-47.

Rogosa, D.R, Brandt, D., & Zimowski, M. (1982). A growth curve approach to the measurement of

change. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 726-748.

Rogosa, D., & Willett, J.B. (1985). Satisfying a simplex structure is simpler than it should be.

Journal of Educational Statistics, 10, 99-107.

Rovine, M.J. & Molenaar, P.C.M. (1998). The covariance between level and shape in the latent

growth curve model with estimated basis vector coefficients. Methods of Psychological Research,

3, 95-107.

Steyer, R., Eid, M., & Schwenkmezger, P. (1997). Modeling true intraindividual change: true

change as a latent variable. Methods of Psychological Research Online, 2, 21-33.

Stoolmiller, M. (1995). Autoregressive effects in structural equation models: Autoregressive effects

in structural equation models: We see some problems. In J.M. Gottmann (Ed). The analysis of

change (pp. 261-276). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Vonesh E.F., & Chinchilli V.M. (1997). Linear and nonlinear models for the analysis of repeated

measurements. New York: Marcel Dekker.

Willett, J.B. (1989). Some results on reliability for the longitudinal measurement of change:

Implications for the design of studies of individual growth. Education and Psychological

Measurement, 49, 587-602.

Page 80: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

71

Willett, J.B., & Sayer, A.G. (1994). Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates and

predictors of individual change over time. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 363-381.

Willett, J.B., & Sayer, A.G. (1995). Cross-domain analysis of change over time: Combining growth

curve modeling and covariance structure analysis. In G.A. Marcoulides & R.E. Schumacker (Eds.).

Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques, (pp. 125-157). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Page 81: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

72

Page 82: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

73

Part 2

Chapter 3 Control and Complexity in Work and the Development of Personal Initiative

(PI): A 5-Wave Longitudinal Structural Equation Model of Occupational

Socialization

Introduction

Personal initiative (PI) is a relatively new concept which we assume will become more

important in the future. Many companies are moving from stable structures to change oriented

organizations and the issue of PI is of high importance in any change process. People who just react

to change situations and just do what they have been ordered to do or what is necessary but do not

go “the extra mile” will not be able to carry changes actively forward and to make them work. To

do this they need initiative. Performance concepts that emphasize a proactive and self-starting

orientation are increasingly important with the advent of new production systems (e.g., Just in

Time, Total Quality Management, Advanced Manufacturing Technology), with new responsibilities

Page 83: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

74

assigned to rank and file workers (Wall & Jackson, 1995; Taira, 1996), with reduced supervision in

these new production systems (Womack, Jones, & Roos, 1990), with increased participation in

continuous improvement programs (Imai, 1994; Spreitzer, 1995), and with changes of the job

concept that require a more active orientation in the labor market (Bridges, 1995). Thus, industry

pushes employees to become more involved and active in their work (Lawler, 1992). Moreover, for

individuals, careers will depend more and more on initiative. The concept of PI may be an

important prerequisite for the issue of employability.

A practical and theoretically important question is which factors contribute to taking PI at

the work place. We would like to answer this question within a large five-wave longitudinal study

in East Germany. The longitudinal study allows us to study the development of PI as a function of

work place and personal characteristics. Moreover, it also makes it possible to study reciprocal

effects, such as the effects of work characteristics on PI and the effects of PI on changes in work

characteristics. An occupational socialization framework (Frese, 1982) is used to study these

developments. East Germany is a particularly interesting region to study PI, because a high amount

of changes in work and work places occurred and this lets us look at circular effects as well.

The Concept of Personal Initiative

PI consists of the following aspects (Frese et al., 1996, Frese, Fay, Hilburger, Leng & Tag,

1997): First, it is self-starting which means that goals are developed without external pressure, role

requirements, or instruction. Thus, PI is the pursuit for self-set goals in contrast to assigned goals.

An example is a blue-collar worker who attempts to fix a broken machine although this is not part

of his or her job description. Second, it is pro-active that is to prepare oneself for negative events

and prevent these from happening, for example when a blue collar worker attempts to prevent

breakdowns of the machine in the future. Third, it overcomes barriers on the way to the goals, that

is, goal pursuit is not stopped prematurely because problems appeared on the way towards the goal.

PI, as conceptualized for this article, consists of motivated behavior that leads in the long

term to positive outcomes for the individual and for the company. The long-term effects are

important here because in the short term, PI may even be negatively sanctioned by the organization

or by the supervisor because, initiatives tend to “rock the boat” and produce changes that are not

always welcome. If one defines motivation with (Ford, 1992), that it directs, energizes, and

regulates goal directed behavior, PI is reserved for those behaviors that are based on self-set goals

and that are energized to overcome many difficulties and that change the environment at least to a

certain extent. As any performance it should be affected by personality traits (for example proactive

Page 84: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

75

personality, Crant, 1995), specific attitudes and orientations, and by situational parameters. A model

of how it develops with regard to work characteristics will be presented below.

Validity studies have been carried out and the validity of the PI concept is discussed next. PI

was shown to be related to the impression of PI by life partners (Frese et al., 1997). Another validity

issue was to distinguish the concept from organizational citizenship behavior. Conceptually, PI is an

aspect of contextual performance (Motowidlo, Borman, & Schmitt, 1997) because it does not

usually belong to the formalized job requirements of technical core performance. Organizational

citizenship behavior is the core concept of contextual performance. The major dimensions that

differentiate PI from organizational citizenship behavior is its proactive and self-starting nature.

Two aspects – altruism and generalized compliance have been studied in most detail within the

Organizational Citizenship Behavior paradigm (Smith, Organ, & Near, 1983). Fay (1998) has

shown that generalized compliance has a very different nomological net than PI; however there is

some overlap between altruism and PI. However, altruism can be shown in two forms: One is to

react to requests and obvious needs. In this case, there is an obvious conceptual difference of

altruism to PI. Another form of altruism requires initiative and is self-starting because it is not

salient for the other person involved that help is required. Thus, this form of organizational

citizenship behavior is self-starting. Thus, clearly, there is no overlap with generalized compliance

but there is one form of altruism – the one based on PI – that shows some overlap with PI. By

showing the usefulness of PI, this may also contribute to differentiating the concept of

organizational citizenship into forms that are based on PI and those that are not.

PI is a motivation concept that is action oriented and that should lead to higher

achievements. PI is, therefore, related to need for achievement but it is not identical to it (r=.20,

Frese et al., 1997). Conceptually, need for achievement can also be differentiated into a form that

relates to self-set goal and one related to other-set goals. Action orientation (Kuhl, 1992) is also

related but the two concepts are differentiated from each other as seen by their low correlation

(r=.20 in an East and r=.14 in a West German sample, Frese et al., 1997).

PI has been shown to be lawfully related to certain behaviors and to personal and firm

success in these validity studies: PI is related to developing career plans and, more importantly,

with executing them at a later stage (Frese et al., 1997). Moreover, unemployed with a high degree

of PI find a job more quickly than those with a lower degree of PI – PI was measured prior to

becoming unemployed (Frese et al., 1997). In a training, in which the task is to learn from

exploration, those students who have shown a higher degree of PI in their studies, seek less help and

reassurance from the trainer and overcome problems by themselves (Fay & Frese, 1998a). PI was

also measured in small scale entrepreneurs and was shown to be related to firm success in Uganda

Page 85: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

76

and Zimbabwe (Koop, De Reu & Frese, in press; Krauss, Frese & Friedrich, 1999) and in East

Germany (Zempel, 1999). Finally, a pro-initiative climate in companies, measured as a general

climate factor, is strongly related to the profitability of medium sized German firms; in addition,

there are interactions with process innovations: Those with many process innovations (e.g., total

quality management, re-engineering) are only profitable if they also have a pro-initiative climate in

their firm (Baer & Frese, 1999).

Occupational Socialization

Occupational socialization is defined as a developmental perspective that emphasizes "the

changes that take place in the person as a function of the job." (Frese, 1982, p. 209). Occupational

socialization theory (Frese, 1982; Kohn & Schooler, 1978; Semmer & Schallberger, 1996; Volpert,

1977) is concerned with the influence of control and complexity on long-range changes in people.

Examples of occupational socialization are the effects of non-control and stress on ill-health

(Gardell, 1971; Karasek, 1979, Karasek & Theorell, 1990), the effects of work complexity on

intellectual flexibility (Kohn & Schooler, 1978), on intelligence (Schallberger, 1988), and on values

and self-concept (Mortimer & Lorence, 1979a and b), and the effects of job characteristics on work

motivation (Hackman & Oldham, 1975).

There are three interfaces between the organization and the individual: The colleagues,

managers, and the work characteristics (including rules and procedures). The latter constitute the

substance of occupational socialization. Complexity and control at work are seen as

materializations of organizational decisions which have an influence on the development of the

person (Volpert, 1977). Occupational socialization is, thus, related to work design issues and

socialization of organizations is accomplished via work characteristics. Therefore, work

characteristics are used as independent variables. In contrast, organizational socialization (Chao,

O'Leary-Kelly, Wolf, Klein, & Gardner, 1994) is concerned with the other interfaces of the

organization and looks directly at what the organization does to socialize the person into the

organization, for example, socialization strategies that "break people in" (van Maanen, 1976).

We want to study PI within the occupational socialization framework and, therefore, look at

the effects of work characteristics on PI. Both organizational and occupational socialization

research emphasize that people are not just passive recipients of organizational and work input but

are also actively changing and selecting work environments. Thus, there are reciprocal relationships

between socialization and selection (Kohn & Schooler, 1978; Semmer & Schallberger, 1996) and

people are active participants ("...socialization is a process affected not only by organizational

Page 86: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

77

initiatives, but also by newcomer initiatives", Morrison, 1993, p. 173). Thus, PI should not only be

affected by the tasks at work but should in turn also influence which kinds of tasks are selected for a

person and by a person.

On a more general level Bandura (1997, p. 6) has argued with his concept of reciprocal

determinism that "internal personal factors in the form of cognitive, affective, and biological events,

behavior, and the environmental events all operate as interacting determinants that influence one

another bidirectionally." We would like to discuss theoretically and show empirically that PI

constitutes one important mechanism by which people change their (work-) environment. Because

of this, a concept of PI is central to the notion of reciprocal determinism.

The Theoretical Model

We would look at how occupational characteristics (in our case, control at work and

complexity of work) lead to personal initiative and vice versa. Our theoretical model is presented in

Figure 19.

Figure 19: Occupational Socialization Model of Initiative

A general occupational socialization framework (Frese, 1982; Semmer & Schallberger,

1996) is used to understand the development of initiative at work and hypothesized reciprocal

relations make it possible that PI increases control and complexity in one's works tasks (Kohn &

Schooler, 1978) and changes one's mastery orientation. In the following we shall walk along Figure

19 and start with the control and complexity.

Work characteristics

(Control, Complexity)

Mastery

Orientation

Initiative

Page 87: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

78

Control and Complexity as Work Characteristics. We restrict the analysis to the two work

characteristics control and complexity because they are related and are the core to occupational

socialization theory. A person has control at work when he or she "has an influence over his or her

actions and over the conditions" of work in accordance with his or her goals (Frese, 1989, p. 108).

Complexity has been defined by the number of elements that have to be considered in decisions

(Frese, 1987; Wood, 1986). Control and complexity are conceptually and empirically related as

both refer to decision making possibilities. Control and complexity are empirically related; for

example in one study the respective correlations between these two variables were .42 (control and

complexity measured on the level of job incumbents) and .70 (measured by observers’ ratings)

(Semmer, 1982). Control and complexity are, therefore, often combined into one factor (e.g., by

Frankenhaeuser & Gardell, 1976; Karasek, 1979). A more pragmatic reason to restrict the analysis

to two variables was that we needed to reduce the number of variables in the complex analysis of a

longitudinal study.

The notion that control and complexity are important work characteristics has been

theoretically and empirically supported by many authors (Gardell, 1971; Greenberger & Strasser,

1986; Karasek & Theorell, 1990; Kohn & Schooler, 1978; Kohn et al., 1997; Kornhauser, 1965 and

Spector, 1986) and is central to occupational socialization theory (Frese, 1982; Hacker, 1986; Kohn

& Schooler, 1978; Volpert, 1977). Job changes such as job enrichment or autonomous work groups

have emphasized changes in the level of control and complexity (Emery & Thorsrud, 1969;

Gulowsen, 1972; Jackson, et al., 1993; Wall & Clegg, 1981). The job characteristics model by

Hackman and Oldham (1975) presents convergent evidence. While this model is made up of five

variables, the central variables are autonomy and skill variety. Autonomy - control at work in our

terminology - is central to the 5 variables since it has the highest relationship with the overall job

motivation potential in Hackman and Oldham's (1975, r=.80) and in Wall, Clegg, and Jackson

(1978, r=.79) and with job satisfaction (Loher, 1985). Skill variety in Hackman and Oldham's

(1975) - a concept that is similar albeit not the same as complexity of work - is highly correlated

with autonomy (Wall et al., 1978) and it has been argued that job complexity is the core of

Hackman & Oldham's model (Gerhart, 1988).

The Influence of Control and Complexity on Mastery Orientation. Control and complexity

have been suspected to be direct predictors of performance (Frese, et al., 1996; Greenberger,

Strasser, Cummings, & Dunham, 1989; Wall & Jackson, 1995; Karasek & Theorell, 1990; Kohn &

Schooler, 1982; Spector, 1986). In contrast, our model (Figure 19) specifies that control and

Page 88: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

79

complexity influence PI via mediators. There are three interrelated mediating mechanisms that

produce the relationship between control at work and PI: control aspirations (or lack of control

aspirations as in the case of helplessness), control appraisal, and self-efficacy.

First, whenever control is thwarted, helplessness appears. This implies negative

motivational consequences because the organism stops trying to control the environment when it

does not expect any positive outcomes from such attempts (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995; Seligman,

1975; White, 1959). Seligman (1975) has argued that helplessness is related to control appraisals

and Abramson, Seligman, and Teasdale (1978) have shown that these expectations of non-control

can be generalized widely. Opposite ideas also exist: Lack of control may actually lead to higher

control aspirations because one is motivated to be in control again (cf. also Greenberger & Strasser,

1991). Reactance should follow when control is low. Wortman & Brehm (1975) have combined

reactance and helplessness theories. In the short term, lack of control can actually increase the

aspirations for control, as reactance theory suggests (Wicklund, 1974). However, if the attempts to

increase control get thwarted, learned helplessness develops (Wortman & Brehm, 1975). Thus, lack

of control leads to giving up wanting to have control and reduces control aspirations. A low level of

control aspirations leads people to show little PI.

Second, control appraisals relate to believing that one is able to influence decisions at work

(Folkman, 1984). If one has control at work, it is likely that one also expects the situation to be

controllable. If people expect that they can influence things at work, they are more likely to show

PI.

Third, Bandura (1997) argued that mastery experiences lead to higher self-efficacy which is

the expectancy that one is able to perform a certain action effectively. Control at work makes it

possible to have such mastery experiences which lead to self-efficacy. If people have a high degree

of self-efficacy, they will be more likely to show a high degree of PI because they have to rely on

their own effective actions when taking initiative.

As discussed above, there is theoretical and empirical overlap of control and complexity.

Complexity should also help in the development of these three mediators because complexity

increases the chances to have mastery experiences at work. Self-efficacy as perceived competence

requires the possibility to use skills and knowledge which can only be shown if a certain task

complexity exists (Bandura, 1997).

We measured all three variables but combined them into one general concept – mastery

orientation – for theoretical and practical reasons. The practical reason was to decrease the number

of variables in our complex LISREL modeling. The theoretical idea is that there is a common theme

of the three concepts – control aspirations, control appraisal, and self-efficacy are all related to

Page 89: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

80

being able to master and to want mastery at work. We call it, therefore, mastery orientation.

Orientation (like an attitude) includes affective, conative, and cognitive components (Eagly &

Chaiken, 1993) (in terms of Hackman & Oldham, 1975, they are critical psychological states, albeit

Hackman & Oldham did not study these specific psychological states). The term orientation

signifies that it is a concept of medium specificity. It is not a very specific attitude but also not a

general personality trait. With Fishbein & Aijzen (1975) and Rotter (1972), we think that all person

concepts can be differentiated along the dimension of generality and that the generality of the

concept should fit the research question. We deal with an intermediate level of specificity as all

concepts are supposed to predict PI across a number of domains within the work setting. Thus, all

concepts used in our study are specific in so far as they refer to the work setting and mid-range as

they refer to broad categories of work, person, and behavior in the job.

We do not yet know enough about the time trajectory for these effects to appear. Control

and complexity can influence a person only given a certain exposure time. However, we do not

know whether these processes take months, half a year or a full year; therefore we shall contrast

models with short and long time lags (synchronous and lagged effects). In general, the timing of

effects due to working conditions is an issue that is very complicated and far from being resolved

theoretically or empirically (cf. Frese & Zapf, 1988 on time problems in a similar area).

Hypothesis 1: Control and complexity increase mastery orientation which consists of control

aspirations, control appraisals, and self-efficacy.

The Effects of Mastery Orientation on Initiative. A higher mastery orientation should

contribute to a more active approach and, thus, more PI. Potential processes are: People with high

mastery orientation should have a stronger sense of responsibility (Hackman & Oldham, 1975), they

should not give up easily when problems appear (Bandura, 1997; Folkman, 1984), they should

search more for opportunities to act (Bandura, 1997; Folkman, 1984), they should have higher

hopes for success and therefore take a long term perspective in goal setting and planning

(Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995), and should actively search for information (Ashford & Tsui, 1991),

which leads to better knowledge of where to show initiative. Indeed, self-efficacy has been shown

to be related to performance (Stajkovic & Luthans, 1998). All of these effects are supposed to be

immediate because attitudes and cognitions should have an immediate regulatory function on

actions (Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960). The impact of mastery orientation on PI should,

therefore, be synchronous.

Thus, our hypothesis 2 is: Mastery orientation leads to a higher degree of personal initiative

synchronously.

Page 90: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

81

Hypothesis 3: Mastery orientation functions as a mediator between work characteristics and

PI.

The Effects of Personal Initiative on Mastery Orientation. In the sense of reciprocal

determinism, PI should not only be determined by mastery orientation but should in turn also

determine mastery orientation (Bandura, 1997). Showing initiative provides the experience that one

has mastered difficulties and problems that appear after one has shown PI. For example, if a person

has implemented an idea to produce better quality with a new procedure, he or she will think of

him- or herself to be effective, to have control over the environment, and he or she will be

encouraged to assert control again. Therefore, such experiences function as a mastery experiences

and lead to higher mastery orientation (in the sense of higher expectations that one can master the

world and one's actions and that it is worthwhile). For this reason, Figure 1 displays reciprocal paths

between PI and mastery orientation.

Hypothesis 4: There is an effect of PI on mastery orientation.

The Effects of Personal Initiative on Control and Complexity. The reciprocal relationships

between work and behavior stands in contrast to some older approaches of occupational

socialization which assumed an influence of work on the person only (e.g., Frese, 1982 or Van

Maanen, 1976). However, the newcomer in a job changes the roles and the job content (Ashford &

Black, 1996; Ilgen & Hollenbeck, 1991; Staw & Boettger, 1990) and is, therefore, able to change

the job as well.

Thus, PI as an active approach to work, should eventually have an influence on work

characteristics. Two mechanisms may play a role here: First, people with high PI may produce some

added complexity and control in their given jobs. The tasks of a job are not completely fixed, once

and for all. There are always emergent elements to be developed (Hacker, 1986, Ilgen &

Hollenbeck, 1991). For example, by developing initiatives to improve productivity, the given job is

changed and control and complexity are increased. Work then becomes more interesting and one is

further encouraged to change it by developing better work procedures. Another examples shows

that the superiors can also be involved in this process: A secretary might have been originally hired

to be a typist; if she or he takes over more and more tasks in the organization of the group, the

superior will rely on him or her and in this way she or he actually increased control and complexity

of the job.

Page 91: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

82

Second, another mechanism goes via job change. People with higher PI should use job

changes to get more challenging jobs. People with higher PI should also be more successful in

finding those jobs. Challenging jobs include tasks with a higher degree of control and complexity.

In the following we use the term job change to signify both mechanisms. Both mechanisms

need a certain amount of time to unfold. It is a slow process for the secretary described above to

convince the superiors that he or she should be approached for organizational tasks (and not just for

typing). Certain events, like reorganizations, new supervisors, etc. may help to speed up the process.

Similarly, giving up a job (or losing a job) and searching for another one is normally not a frequent

event and, thus, takes time to unfold. Kohn and Schooler (1978) found a lagged selection effect

with a time lag of 10 years. In a different area, Wilk, Desmarais, and Sackett (1995) found that

people gravitate to jobs commensurate with their ability within a five year period. We assume that

the stability of the economic situation of a country also influences the effective time lags (because

stability leads to more stable jobs). The time lags of 10 years (Kohn & Schooler, 1978) and 5 years

(Wilk et al., 1995) have been found in relatively stable economies. In a transitional economy, such

as East Germany, in which many more job changes occur (Frese et al., 1996), the time lag may be

much shorter.

Thus hypothesis 5 states: PI increases the degrees of control and complexity in the long run.

Hypothesis 6: PI is a mediator between mastery orientation and job change effects.

The Setting of the Study

Because of global competition and technological and organizational innovations, jobs

change their nature in today's Western economies (Bridges, 1995). Still, Western economies are

relatively stable in comparison to transitional ones which changed from socialist to market driven

economies (Kohn et al., 1997). We wanted to do this study in a high change situation and we picked

East Germany. In East Germany, many people have lost their jobs and had to find other ones. Even

those who did not lose their jobs experienced drastic changes in the technical lay-out and

organization of their jobs (and they could influence this process if they showed PI). In nearly every

company, new technology and new organizational structures were introduced. Thus, it is a situation

of revolutionary job change which affected nearly every East German (Nickel, Kühl & Schenk,

1994). Because of its dynamic situation, East Germany is a particularly good area to do research on

occupational socialization and selection effects. Occupational socialization research is better when

there is a natural "zero point", e.g., when all subjects start a given job. The natural "zero point" in

Page 92: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

83

this study is the beginning of the transition from socialism to capitalism which started in Each

Germany at the time of unification of Germany. Additionally, PI is low in East Germany; in a

comparison of East and West Germans' PI, Frese et al. (1996) suggested that the differences in

control and complexity were important reasons for the higher degree of initiative in West Germany.

To test an occupational socialization model described in Figure 19, a longitudinal study in a

high change situation is needed. Ideally, the longitudinal study should have at least three

measurement points because identification problems are then reduced (Finkel, 1995). It is even

better to have more measurement points because then there is a chance to replicate the effects

several times and to study the exact nature of the effects. Additionally, it allows testing rather

complex models. In our case, we have used five waves.

Finally, we wanted to have as many sources of information as possible, not just

questionnaire responses. Unfortunately, it was not possible to observe people at work (companies

would not have given us their consent to do research at a time when they were scrambling to

survive; this was true at least for the first four years after the change over from socialism to a

market economy). A combination of behavioral interviews, questionnaire research, and ratings by

the interviewers was chosen.

Methods

Sample

The sample was drawn from Dresden, a large city in the south of East Germany; it is the

capital of Saxonia, houses a large Technical University and is relatively well-off in comparison to

other East German cities which are often quite poor. A "random walk sampling" was used by

randomly selecting streets, selecting every third house and in each house, every fourth apartment (in

smaller houses every third one). All people in this household between the ages of 18 and 65 with

full-time employment at T1 were asked to participate (thus, we sometimes had more than one

person per family). The refusal rate of 33% was quite low for a study of this kind. Confidentiality

was assured; if subjects preferred anonymity, this was done with the help of a personal code word.

In wave one (T1 for time 1) (July 1990), 463 people participated in Dresden. At wave two

(T2) (November, December, 1990) 202 additional people were asked to participate5. At wave three

(T3) (September 1991), the N was 543, at wave four (T4) (September 1992) the N was 506, at wave

5 Additional people were added to ascertain whether repeated participation had an influence on initiative. This is a worry in some developmental studies (Schaie, 1973). As we found that there was no such influence for T1 and T2, we discontinued to add further people in the other waves.

Page 93: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

84

five (T5) (September 1993), N=478, at wave six (T6) (September 1995), N=4896. We did not

include wave one (T1) in our analysis and restricted our analyses to the five waves from T2 to T6.

We did this because we had added more subjects at T2 and, therefore, could use a larger N.

Experimental mortality did not prove to change the make-up of the sample. There were no

significant differences in the initiative variables between dropouts from T1 to T3 and full

participants. The sample is representative of the Dresden population on the relevant parameters (for

example, for age, social class, male/female percentage at work).

This article is based on a broad longitudinal study. Other publications of this study have

looked at the differences between East and West Germany (Frese et al., 1996), at the validity of PI

(Frese et al., 1997), at the relationship of conservatism and PI (Fay & Frese, in press), at the

function of self-efficacy at T3 and T4 of this study (Speier & Frese, 1997), at the relationship

between stressors and strain (Garst, Frese & Molenaar, in press) and at the moderating function of

social support on the stress process (Dormann & Zapf, in press). None of these papers have a

conceptually or methodologically done what the present paper attempts to achieve.

Interview Procedures

Structured interviews were used to measure personal initiative, with additional prompts

adapted by the interviewer to the particular answers provided. The interviews were carried out by

psychology and business students from Munich, Giessen, and Dresden trained during a two-day

course.

Subjects' answers were written down by the interviewers in a short form that was later

typed7 and used as the basis for a numerical coding system. After the short transcripts of the

interviews had been typed, they were coded by the interviewer him- or herself and by a second

coder. The coding system was either factual (for example, subject is unemployed or not - a

dichotomous variable), or it involved some kind of judgment (for example, to what extent does a

certain answer constitute initiative; usually a five point scale was used in these cases). Examples

were provided for the end points as anchors of the scales. All interviewers went to a two-day

training course that taught them the interview procedure and the use of the coding system.

6 N was higher at T6 because we made an extra effort to get at least questionnaire responses from those subjects who had moved to other parts of Germany. 7 For reasons of research economy, we did not use verbatim transcripts of the interviews. This was not necessary because the coding system was developed beforehand and the interviewers knew which answers had to be written down to make coding possible. However, the interviewers were also trained to write down the relevant responses as verbatim as possible; therefore, the records were not just a shorthand for coding.

Page 94: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

85

After the interview, the subjects were given the questionnaires to fill them out at their

leisure (usually they were picked up one or two weeks afterwards). The work characteristics and the

mastery orientation variables were measured with the questionnaire. Immediately following the

interview, the interviewer evaluated the subject on a number of dimensions - this was deliberately

used as a subjective interviewer's response to the interviewee in question (we call it interviewer

evaluation and one of the measures of PI was taken from this). For this reason, no inter-rater

reliability was calculated here.

Measures

All measures were in German. In the questionnaire scales the response alternatives were

from 1 - 5 throughout; to make the scales comparable, the scale values were divided by the number

of items.

The work characteristics Control and Complexity were measured with four questionnaire

items each (Semmer, 1984; Zapf, 1993). Item examples are "Can you determine how you do your

work?" for control and "Can you learn new things in your work?" for complexity. Means (divided

by number of items) were 3.57 for control and 3.46 for complexity on average across the five

waves; the standard deviations were on average .80 for control and .68 for complexity across the

five waves. Complexity and control can be measured by questionnaires well because both variables

show high relationships of job incumbents’ self-reports with other people's judgments (Hackman &

Oldham, 1971; Semmer, 1982; Spector, 1992; Zapf, 1989).

The mediators were the questionnaire scales control appraisal, control aspiration, and

self-efficacy which were collapsed into one second order latent factor (see Introduction for our

theoretical and Results for the empirical arguments for collapsing these three variables into one

construct). Control appraisal measures how high one perceives one’s control with regard to work

with three items (Personally, my chance to influence ... things at the work place in general are very

good .... not at all good; ... climate in my department; ... decisions by the shop stewards [shop

stewards - Betriebsrat - is a decision body by law in all German firms]. Control aspirations was

developed by Frese (1984, printed in Frese et al., 1996 – then it was called control rejection; it is

now reversed scored) (7 items). Frese (1984) reported that he attempted at first to measure control

aspirations directly; but this resulted in little variance and a highly skewed distribution. Most people

at work wanted more control. He, therefore, measured whether or not people also accept the

potential negative consequences of control as well (e.g. higher responsibility for errors). This led to

a normal distribution and a more meaningful content of the scale (example, "I would rather be told

exactly what I have to do. Then I make fewer mistakes"). Self-efficacy is a work related generalized

Page 95: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

86

scale with 5 items (similar to the one used by Schwarzer, Baessler, Kwiatek, Schroeder, & Zhang,

1997 and highly correlated with it) and shown to be useful in our context by Speier & Frese (1997).

The items are presented by Frese et al (1996) (sample item: "If I want to achieve something, I can

overcome setbacks without giving up my goal."). The Means (Standard Deviations) were 2.5 (.70)

for control appraisal on average across the five waves, 3.90 (.68) for control aspiration, and 3.55

(.51) for self-efficacy. Alphas were on average .61, .87, and.69 for control appraisal, control

aspiration, and self-efficacy respectively.

Personal Initiative was ascertained via a standardized interview and with an interviewer

evaluation at the end of the interview. We used three measures – Interviewer evaluation,

Overcoming barriers, and Active approach – with the latter two being condensed into one scale by

combining two items into one (so-called item parcels, March, Hau, Balla & Grayson, 1998). The 8-

item measure interviewer evaluation was filled out by the interviewer directly after the interview

as a subjective account of the degree of initiative that was shown by the particular interviewee

(degree of being active, goal oriented, independent, etc.). The interviewers were trained to use this

measure. The Alphas of this scale were around .92 for the five time periods, the mean was 3.6 on

average across the five waves and the SD was .9.

Overcoming barriers was based on a sort of situational interview (Latham & Saari, 1984).

The interviewees were asked to imagine having a certain problem, for example, a colleague who

always did his or her work sloppily, requiring additional effort from the interviewee. After the

interviewees suggested a way to deal with this situation, the interviewer would then present reasons

why that was not possible, thus presenting new barriers. After the third barrier (the question was

counted to be the first one), the respondents were asked whether they could think of additional

solutions. In this way we measured how many barriers the respondents were able to overcome.

Interrater agreement was r =.80 at T3. To overcome potential testing effects (Cook & Campbell,

1979 p. 52), we changed the problems three times, so that a repeat of the problem of T1 would

appear in T4, etc.

The scale Active approach was also based on this situational interview. Overcoming

barriers may be done more or less actively. Since proactivity is a core of personal initiative, it was

important to find out whether interviewees would just delegate a problem to other people (e.g., the

supervisor) or personally do something about the problems. The interviewers were asked (and

trained) to code how active an interviewee's propositions to overcoming barriers were. Overcoming

barriers and active approach were combined - in a scale called Situational interview; this

combined scale is based on 8 items and has an average alpha of .72 with a mean of 3.0 averaged

across the five waves and an SD of .71.

Page 96: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

87

In principle we also ascertained other measures of PI in the interview who were shown to

correlate well with the measures used here (Frese et al., 1997, e.g., it is shown that these measures

also correlated well with partners’ estimates of PI and with a self-reported measure, as well with

giving suggestions at the work place and with participating in continuous education). We restricted

this analysis to three measures of PI. This was done because these three measures were consistently

used in all waves. Further, they are the best measures and they represent a good mix because they

combine an interview based performance measure (overcoming barriers) and two interviewer

estimates. Since they are based either on performance in the interview or on the interviewers'

judgments, they constitute a separate source from the questionnaire responses used for the

independent and moderator variables.

Models

Our overall model is displayed in Figure 19. From a methodological point of view, we have an

enormously complex array of potentially analyzable models because we have 5 different

measurement points and two levels of variables (first order constructs, second order latent

constructs) and several different causal time lags. In principle, one can always invent further

arrangements of the variables to produce other models. Therefore, we had to make certain decisions

to reduce the number of potential models.

In the following we shall first discuss the measurement models. Work characteristics are

frequently measured as latent constructs. Factor models assume that a latent common construct

determines the observed variables, that is the covariance among the observed variables (the items)

can be explained by this latent construct. However, the items of the work characteristics measures

control and complexity can also be conceived as the causes of a latent construct (Bollen & Lennox,

1991). A factor model implies, for example, that a change in the control over the timing of rest

periods is related to an equivalent change in the control over selecting one's methods of doing the

job. This does not have to be case. Therefore, a more reasonable model would consider both aspects

of control as causes for the latent control variable. In this "causal indicator model" a change in one

variable is not necessarily accompanied by a change in the other ones. The latent variable is then

only an abstraction of control in the sense that each specific instance of control added together leads

to overall higher control at work. Cohen, Cohen, Teresi, Marchi, and Velez (1990) criticized the

inappropriate use of confirmatory factor models by arguing similarly that in cases such as ours, one

should not develop latent construct to determine the observed variables. Therefore, the work

Page 97: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

88

characteristics variables will not be fitted with a confirmatory factor analysis, because we prefer to

see them as causal indicators and not as effect indicators (Bollen and Lennox, 1991); we shall also

not calculate internal consistencies for the same reason (MacCallum & Browne, 1993)8. The causal

indicator model was calculated but it led to identification problems. Because we do not have good

theoretical reasons to weigh control and complexity, we used an equally weighted summation of the

two variables (McDonald, 1996). For reasons to keep a good variable to N ratio, it was also

necessary to decrease the number of variables in the model.

For mastery orientation, we chose a "banded error structure", which means that all unique

factors of identical items are correlated over time (Vonesh & Chinchilli, 1997). After the three

mastery orientation variables were treated as longitudinal measurement models9, the three measures

of mastery orientation - control appraisals, self-efficacy, and control aspirations - were modeled as a

second order construct. This was done for four reasons: First, the three mastery orientation variables

were significantly correlated (averaging across waves, the cross-sectional intercorrelations were .

34. Second, there is a coherent theme in these variables that makes it theoretically useful to

combine them (see our arguments above). Third, it was necessary to reduce the number of variables

for the structural equation analyses and, therefore, it was warranted to reduce these three mediators

to one. Fourth, the three variables produce a well fitting second order measurement model.

The PI scales were taken from different interviewers at different times; therefore, there is no

necessity for a banded error structure. In the structural model, these scales were also modeled as a

second order construct. This makes sense because they are well correlated (cf. also Frese, Fay et al.,

1997) and because a second order construct model has a good fit with the data.

To test for measurement invariance, we estimated models with free factor loadings and

alternative models with equal factor loadings over time (Pentzt & Chou, 1994).

The following structural models will be investigated (all of them shown in Figure 20). The

structural one-directional models are signified as I-models, and the reciprocal models as II- and III-

models. We used A, B, and C, to signify different time lags and arabic numbering for different lags

for the job change effects.

The Baseline Stability Model assumes that there are no causal relationships between the

variables except stabilities. It is used as a baseline model to test further structural causal models

(Hertzog & Nesselroade, 1987; Marsh 1993, Kenny & Campbell, 1989). Here each longitudinally

8 In earlier publications we did not yet think of this problem and have, therefore, calculated internal consistencies. The Alphas were for control .78 and for complexity .67 in Frese et al. (1996). 9 This means that the factor model is done for every time point within one model.

Page 98: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

89

measured variable is repeatedly regressed on the same variable measured one wave before (Plewis,

1996).

The Fully Synchronous Socialization Model (I-A) is a longitudinal model in which

the working conditions impact on the mediating latent construct mastery orientation which in turn

impact on PI. Thus, it is an occupational socialization model because it assumes that work

conditions change person characteristics. It is fully synchronous because all the causal paths are

assumed to work at the same time. In this model the previous values of the dependent variables are

held constant so that we actually measure residual changes, as we do in all further models.

The Lagged-Synchronous Socialization Model (I-B) models a lagged effect from control

and complexity of work on mastery orientation and a synchronous effect of mastery orientation on

PI.

The Fully Lagged Socialization Model (I-C) is a model with time lags from work on mastery

orientation and from mastery orientation on PI.

From the 3 socialization models just described, we selected the empirically best fitting

model. This was then used a starting point for Models II and III which also involve reverse paths.

Model II is a Socialization Plus Job Change Model with reversed and direct paths from PI to work

characteristics. The reversed paths are assumed to be the result of job changes due to changing the

job or changing the content of one's work. We hypothesized this to be a slow effect. This implies

that this effect should be lagged. To be on the safe side, we calculated three models with a 3 to 4

years reverse effects lag, a two years lag and1 year lags (signified 3, 2, and 1).

From this we took the best II Model and used it as a starting point for Model III. The

Socialization Plus Job Change and Reciprocation Model (Model III) allows an additional

reverse path from PI to the mediators mastery orientation. The theoretical argument is that (at least

successful) personal initiative will lead to higher mastery orientation.

Page 99: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

90

Baseline Stability Model

I-A Fully Synchronous Socialization Model

I-B Lagged Synchronous Socialization Model

I-C Fully Lagged Socialization Model

II-A-3 Socialization Plus Job Change Model (3 yr.)

II-A-2 Socialization Plus Job Change Model (2 yr.)

II-A-1 Socialization Plus Job Change Model (1 yr.)

III-A-1 Reciprocal Socialization Plus Job Change Model (1 yr.)

Figure 20: Structural Models (on Top, There is Personal Initiative, in the Middle Mastery Orientation, and at the Bottom Work Characteristics; from Left to Right: T2 to T6)

Page 100: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

91

Statistical Analysis Method

Latent-variable models were first used to analyze the scales and in a second step (Anderson

& Gerbing, 1988, Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993b p.113) structural equation models with simultaneous

estimates of the measurement and structural part of the model were done (Williams &.Podsakoff,

1989 p. 272) using LISREL VIII (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993a). Our models are complex not only

because they are longitudinal, but also because they test for mediation. Although many researchers

have used the Baron and Kenny's (1986) and James and Brett's (1984) approaches to test for

mediation, the use of structural equation modeling is a better strategy. The latter uses a simultaneous

estimate of the complete model and can deal with measurement error and nonrecursive parts of the

model as well (Brown, 1997).

Including the complete measurement model in the structural models would result in an

unfavorable subjects to variables ratio (Bentler and Chou,1987). To reduce the size of the structural

models factor scores and item parcels (Marsh, Hau, Balla, & Grayson, in press) were used. The

regression coefficients for the calculation of the factor scores were based upon longitudinal

measurement models after testing for equivalence of factor structure across time.

Model fit was assessed by the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA; Browne

and Cudeck, 1993) and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI, Bentler, 1990). Values of the RMSEA

lower than .05 indicate a good model fit and value of the CFI higher than .90 are desirable. Chi-

square values and degrees of freedom are also provided. We used the chi square difference test for

comparing nested models and the Expected Cross Validation Index (ECVI: Browne and Cudeck,

1993) and the Akaike (AIC, Akaike, 1987) for the non-nested models since these indicators show

how easy it is to cross-validate the models.

Treatment of Missing Cases

The N varies across the analyses. We, therefore, used covariance matrix based upon pairwise

deletion of the data was analyzed. Pairwise deletion is a more efficient method than list-wise

deletion and is preferred to listwise deletion (Roth, 1994). One reason for this procedure was

missing data as they appear in all studies of this type. However, a more important reason was that

people underwent strong changes and this led often to periods of unemployment, sabbaticals,

educational years, extended holidays, maternity leaves, extended lay-offs without being fired from

the job, etc. For example, of the 471 who had a job at wave 2, 97 did not work at wave 3 and of

those 428 with a job at wave 3, 91 did not have a job at wave 4. It was necessary to have a job to

answer the questions on the work related items. Given this picture across the different waves and

given that most people who lost a job at wave 3 had a job again at wave 4 (of those 97 not working

at T3, 46 had a job again at wave 4), a listwise missing data procedure would have led to a high loss

Page 101: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

92

in N. Moreover, we would have lost precisely those people whose further fate was affected by PI,

for example, because they searched for a different job and got a job with higher (or lower) control

and complexity.

The sample size for the analyses was estimated as the median of different sample sizes used

for estimating the variances and the covariances. This leads to an N between 428 and 470 in the

measurement models and an N of 311 in the structural models. Whenever there were no data

available for the working conditions at a specific time (e.g. because of unemployment or maternal

leave), the data for PI and mastery orientation were ignored for this person for this wave. This was

done to prevent confounding with effects of unemployment.

Results

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Table 1 provides the resulting fit indices of the longitudinal LISREL measurement models,

tested separately for free and equal factor loadings over time. All of the fit indices were very good.

There were no significant differences on the chi2 tests between free and equal factor loadings for the

first order mastery orientation variables: control appraisal, self-efficacy, and control aspiration. This

means that we can assume measurement invariance across time. These three variables were then

collapsed into one second order latent variable - mastery orientation - in the later models. This

second order latent variable also has good fit indices (chi2= 53.66, df= 58, p =.25).

Measurement equivalence tests were more difficult to do for the PI constructs, first because

the situational interview asked different questions at different times (and therefore, we cannot

assume complete measurement invariance for them) and there were only two repeats for them (T2

and T5, and T3 and T6 used the same items). In those cases that used the same items, the results

allow one to assume measurement equivalence.

For the interviewer evaluation of PI the equal loadings model had a worse fit than the free

loading model (significant difference). This is not surprising given the fact that the interviewer

evaluation is based on subjective interpretations and that different interviewers were used at

different waves. Partial measurement invariance is usually considered a sufficient condition for

using the measurement models in a longitudinal structural model (Pentzt & Chou, 1994).

Page 102: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

93

Table 1

Goodness of F

it Measures of L

ISR

EL

Longitudinal M

easurement M

odels

M

odel χ

2 χ

2diff

d.f. ∆

d.f. RM

SEA

E

CV

I A

IC

CFI

Mastery

orientation

Control

Factor loadings free 37.49

50

.0000

.416 177.49

1.000 expectation

Equal factor loadings

42.35

58

.0000 .390

166.35 1.000

D

ifference

4.86

8

S

elf -efficacy Factor loadings free 321.32

215

.0325

1.154 541.32

.977

Equal factor loadings

342.93

231

.0321 1.132

530.93 .976

D

ifference

21.61

16

C

ontrol Factor loadings free

750.80

480

.0347 2.241

1050.80 .971

rejection E

qual factor loadings 778.31

504

.0341

2.200 1030.31

.971 D

ifference

27.51

24

Initiative

S

ituational Factor loadings free

190.58

160

.0206 .647

290.58 .985

interview

Pairs equal T

2=T

5; T3=T

6 194.44

166

.0195

.629 282.44

.986

Difference

3.86

6

Interview

er Factor loadings free

1283.74

720

.0428 3.467

1483.74 .952

evaluation E

qual factor loadings 1354.99

748

.0435

3.502 1498.99

.949 D

ifference

71.25*

28

*p < .05 (for difference χ

2 test).

Page 103: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

94

Structural Models

Table 2 shows the intercorrelations of all the constructs/variables used in the structural

models (from the LISREL correlation matrix of Eta). This Table allows to do a first test of a

mediation effect for mastery orientation. All the prerequisites of Baron and Kenny (1986) are met

for all waves: There are sizeable correlations between work characteristics and the mediator mastery

orientation, between mastery orientation and PI and between work characteristics and PI. Further,

one can see that the correlations between work characteristics and PI are smaller than the ones

between work characteristics and the mastery orientation, and between mastery orientation and

initiative. Of course, the mediation test is done with LISREL in the following structural analyses.

Table 3 shows the fit indices for the structural models. The Baseline Model fits well but

clearly can be improved by allowing theoretically specified paths between the constructs.

Table 3 shows the best I - Model to be the Fully Synchronous Socialization Model (Model I-

A). Thus, the effects from work characteristics to mastery orientation and from mastery orientation

to PI are synchronous. This should not be interpreted to mean that there are no time lags for the

development of these effects. As Dwyer (1983, p.397) points out: "... the effects that are modeled as

synchronous are actually cross-lagged effects for which the appropriate lag is much shorter than the

period between waves of observation." Thus, strictly speaking it is not possible to prove

synchronous effects and we can only conclude from these results that the time lags are smaller than

1 year. Second, the model is a full mediation model: Mastery orientation completely mediates the

effects of work characteristics on PI. The modification indices showed no indication that there

should be a direct path from work characteristics to personal initiative.

Page 104: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

95

Table 2

Correlations betw

een Latent C

onstructs for Maxim

um S

tructural Model

SD

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

8 9

10 11

12 13

14 15

1 T2 C

ontrol cognitions 0.26

1.00

2 T

3 Control cognitions

0.30 0.82

1.00

3 T4 C

ontrol cognitions 0.30

0.67 0.79

1.00

4 T

5 Control cognitions

0.28 0.60

0.64 0.82

1.00

5 T6 C

ontrol cognitions 0.30

0.56 0.54

0.72 0.72

1.00

6 T

2 Work characteristics

0.70 0.69

0.52 0.41

0.36 0.36

1.00

7 T3 W

ork characteristics 0.70

0.50 0.59

0.43 0.32

0.29 0.55

1.00

8 T

4 Work characteristics

0.72 0.49

0.54 0.73

0.51 0.54

0.46 0.47

1.00

9 T5 W

ork characteristics 0.66

0.38 0.44

0.62 0.66

0.55 0.41

0.42 0.67

1.00

10 T

6 Work characteristics

0.72 0.40

0.42 0.50

0.52 0.72

0.32 0.35

0.43 0.52

1.00

11 T2 Initiative

0.30 0.52

0.45 0.40

0.38 0.39

0.37 0.38

0.30 0.31

0.30 1.00

12 T3 Initiative

0.43 0.44

0.49 0.45

0.39 0.37

0.26 0.38

0.31 0.25

0.27 0.58

1.00

13 T4 Initiative

0.34 0.44

0.47 0.62

0.48 0.41

0.24 0.23

0.39 0.32

0.29 0.43

0.46 1.00

14 T5 Initiative

0.28 0.32

0.33 0.45

0.52 0.42

0.22 0.18

0.34 0.40

0.33 0.49

0.52 0.52

1.00

15 T6 Initiative

0.35 0.32

0.40 0.47

0.46 0.59

0.20 0.23

0.32 0.35

0.45 0.48

0.54 0.44

0.55 1.00

Page 105: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

96

Table 3

Goodness of Fit M

easures of LISR

EL

Structural M

odels

Model

χ2

χ2

diff d.f.

∆d.f. R

MSE

A

EC

VI

AIC

C

FI

B

aseline Stability M

odel 1650.52

1125

.039

6.29 1950.52

.933 I-A

Fully S

ynchronous Socialization

1352.14

1117

.026 5.38

1668.14 .970

D

ifference Baseline Stability M

odel and I-A

298.38*

8

I-B

Lagged-Synchronous S

ocialization 1431.09

1117

.030

5.63 1747.09

.960

Difference B

aseline Stability Model and I-B

219.43*

8

I-C

Fully L

agged Socialization

1600.62

1117

.037 6.18

1916.62 .938

D

ifference Baseline Stability M

odel and I-C

49.90*

8

II-A-1

Socialization P

lus Job Change

1313.51

1113

.024 5.28

1637.51 .975

D

ifference I-A and II-A

-1 (1 yr.)

38.63*

4

II-A

-2 S

ocialization Plus Job C

hange 1331.57

1114

.025

5.33 1653.57

.972

Difference I-A

and II-A-2 (2 yr.)

20.57*

3

II-A-3

Socialization Plus Job C

hange 1336.26

1115

.025

5.34 1656.26

.972

Difference I-A

and II-A-3 (3 yr.)

15.88*

2

III-A-1 R

eciprocal Socialization Plus Job C

hange 1290.15

1109

.023

5.23 1622.15

.977

Difference II-A

-1 and III-A-1

23.36*

4

Note. N

= 311 for all models.

p < .00 for χ

2 test for all models.

*p < .05 (for difference χ

2 test).

Page 106: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

97

The results up to this point suggest that we should use the Fully Synchronous Socialization

Model from here on. The next set of models includes the job change effects. This implies that

people with high initiative will eventually move to more responsible jobs with higher control and

complexity or create these kinds of jobs by changing the job content. These Socialization Plus Job

Change Models were tested with different time lags (note, that the time difference between T5 and

T6 is two years while all other time differences between the waves are 1 year each). All

Socialization Plus Job Change Models (II Models) were significantly better than our best

Socialization Model. This speaks for the reciprocal determinism concept in which both job change

and socialization effects can be observed. The decision which of these four Job Change Plus

Socialization Models is the best one is a "close call." Two arguments speak for Model II-A-1: First,

the job change effects stayed significant even when we assume the lag of only one wave. There

were four significant job change paths from T2 to T3, from T3 to T4, from T4 to T5, and from T5

to T6. This implies that the change situation in East Germany was strong enough to make it possible

for people to change their working conditions enough to produce an effect within a year. Second,

the fit indices including AIC and ECVI were slightly better for Model II-A-1 than for Models II-A-2

and 3. While the differences are not very large, this speaks again for taking the II-A-1 model as a

starting point for the last additional path.

Our last model - the Socialization Plus Job Change and Reciprocation Model (III-A-1) adds

one more path at each time period - the reciprocal path from personal initiative to mastery

orientation. This model was significantly better than the best fitting Model II.

The Best Fitting Structural Model: Reciprocal Socialization Plus Job Change

The best fitting model Reciprocal Socialization Plus Job Change is displayed in Figure 21.

The results are highly regular across time. While not each of the hypothesized paths was significant,

all of them were in the expected direction. There was the hypothesized effect of work characteristics

on mastery orientation which was significant in each case and rather large (standardized path

coefficients of .40 and above). Thus, occupational socialization has an important effect on mastery

orientation.

Further, the hypothesized effects of mastery orientation on personal initiative were

significant in three of the four cases with Betas of .32, .31 and .19 (one-sided significance tests

Page 107: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

Figure 21. Reciprocal socialization plus job change m

odel (app: control appraisals; s-e: self efficacy; asp: control aspirations)

R2 = .49

R

2 = .65

R2 = .67

R

2 = .56

R2 = .70

R

2 = .87

R2 = .72

R

2 = .66

R2 = .35

R

2 = .26

R2 = .46

R

2 = .31

.46* .40*

.59* .43*

.53*

.32* .18*

.20* .45*

.56* .51*

.68* .52*

.19* .32*

.16 .31*

.40* .55*

.46*

WO

RK

CH

AR

AC

TE

RIST

ICS

MA

STE

RY

OR

IEN

TA

TIO

N

T2

T3

T4

T5

T6

T2

T3

T4

T5

T6

T6

T5

T4

T3

T2

.23* .20*

.16* .22*

.16* .38*

.35* .11

.60*

.38*

.75*

PE

RS

ON

AL

INIT

IAT

IVE

app s-e

asp app

s-e

asp

app

s-e

asp

app

s-e

asp

app

s-e asp

.55* .43*

.51*.54*

.51*

.61*

98

Page 108: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

99

used). The hypothesized job change effect was also significant in all four waves, although it was not

as sizeable as the socialization effects - the paths were around .20. There was a synchronous

reciprocal effect in three of the waves (.38, .35 and .16). This means that PI also leads to higher

mastery orientation. This effect was about as large as the effect from mastery orientation to PI.

Figure 21 also shows the hypothesized mediation effect of mastery orientation. None of the

modification indices indicated that one would still need a direct path from work characteristics to

PI. This confirms that the mediation model fully explains that part of the correlations between work

characteristics and PI that was due to the socialization effect (of course, there is still the job change

effect from PI on work characteristics).

As one can see from Figure 21, the direct stabilities (as path coefficients) of work

characteristics signify that there were changes. The stabilities between T2 and T3 and T3 and T4

were lower than the stability for T4 to T5. This squares quite well with the informal observations

that work place changes were most dramatic directly after German unification (at T2) and then

leveled off two years later. The stability between T5 and T6 appears to be a little lower. Here one

has take into consideration that this time period is 2 years (in contrast to all other time lags which

are 1 year) (Arminger, 1987).

When interpreting the relatively low direct stability paths for the mediator mastery

orientation, one has to take into account that there were many indirect paths that also contributed to

the general stability. Table 2 shows that the stability-correlations for mastery orientation were

higher than the respective stability-paths in Figure 21.

Our model is able to explain a good part of the change processes. Figure 21 presents the

explained variance R2 coefficients. Between 66 and 87% of the variance of the mediator mastery

orientation was explained by our model. Between one-half and two thirds of the variance of

Personal Initiative was explained. This is certainly a high degree of explained variance in the social

sciences. Obviously, our model also includes stabilities and the stabilities make up a large part of

the explained variance.

Work characteristics showed the lowest degree of explained variance. Since we assume that

these variables are determined by a whole set of predictors in the outside world not modeled here

Page 109: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

100

(e.g. organizational structure, career paths, management, delegation by supervisor, political /

economic developments, etc.), we are not surprised about this result.

Discussion

The model described in Figure 19 has fared quite well empirically. Most of the relationships

were synchronous. Our hypotheses were supported quite well. First, mastery orientation was

affected by the two work characteristics, control and complexity. Second, mastery orientation

(consisting of control appraisal, self-efficacy, and control aspiration) had an effect on PI and third,

mastery orientation mediated the socialization process. Fourth, there were lagged job change effects

of PI on work characteristics. Fifth, there were reciprocal effects of PI and mastery orientation. The

latter two findings confirmed the idea of reciprocal determinism (Bandura, 1997; Kohn & Schooler,

1978).

There were both synchronous as well as lagged processes. The effects of mastery orientation

on PI and from PI on mastery orientation were synchronous while the job change effect of PI on

work was lagged as hypothesized. It is important to keep in mind that a synchronous effect does not

mean that the effect is immediate. The synchronous effects found may be actually working within

the time frame of up to a year (the time between two waves) although we assume them to work

much faster from a theoretical perspective.

The predictive power of the model was high. Prediction of initiative was not just a matter of

the stability of initiative. This is encouraging because it suggests that one can change initiative by

changing the job content (control and complexity) and by increasing mastery orientation.

The data signify a vicious or benign cycle (depending upon one's perspective). Those who

show initiative eventually get complex and controllable work which in turn increases initiative. This

implies that those with little initiative may be on a downward spiral. These mechanisms should

eventually lead to a polarization into winners and losers in East Germany.

Page 110: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

101

Limitations and Strengths

Obviously, one limitation of our study is that we do not have objective measures of work

characteristics. On the other hand, there is good theoretical thinking and empirical data in this area.

Theoretically, one can differentiate between different types of task characteristics. Hackman (1970)

and Wood (1986) have differentiated between tasks qua task, tasks as behavior requirements, tasks

as behavior descriptions, or as ability requirements. Underlying this differentiation is the dimension

from the most objective task characteristic (measured without any regard to the job incumbent) to

the most subjective one (measured based on the job incumbents' feelings). We conceptualize control

as opportunities and complexity as behavior requirements. "Because behavior requirements ... are a

relatively stable property of a given task, they can be described independently of the characteristics

of task performers." (Wood, 1986, p. 63). This implies that there is a certain objectivity to the task

situation.

Empirically, the two work characteristics studied show substantial correlations between job

incumbents and other raters of the task characteristics (cf. the meta-analysis by Spector, 1992).

Complexity ascertained by a questionnaire correlated .67 with observers' judgments and the

respective correlation for control at work was .58 (Semmer, 1982). Hackman & Lawler (1971)

similarly showed in their overview, that employees and their supervisors agreed highly when

judging variety (a concept similar to complexity) and autonomy; both showed the highest

correlations of all of Hackman & Lawler's variables; the correlations were on average around .90

for variety and .80 for autonomy (averaged by us after r-to-z transformation)(cf. also Gerhart,

1988). Zapf (1989) involving trained observers and job incumbents has shown that job incumbents'

perceptions of control and complexity were not influenced by ill-health variables in contrast to

stressors. Thus, for all practical purposes, control and complexity can be measured more objectively

than other work descriptors (such as stressors) (Zapf, 1989).

Our longitudinal design strengthens the conclusions to be drawn from this study. Since we

designed it with more than two waves, different time lags can be estimated and models with

reciprocal paths can be used without identification problems or non-theoretical constraints. In some

way, a long-term longitudinal study allows replicating the findings within its design - the same

relationships hold across different waves and, similarly, the measurement models can be upheld at

Page 111: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

102

different time points. The longitudinal study allows to study processes and we hope to have shown

both (self-) selection and socialization processes in this study. However, we could not study micro-

processes with our design, for example, what happens immediately after a new supervisor is

introduced to a work place who supports PI or what happens, if somebody attempts to show

initiative and fails. It is of particular importance to look at failed initiatives. For example, we do not

think that the relationship between PI and increase of mastery orientation holds if an initiative fails.

The reason, why the relationship appears may be due to actual positive experiences that people have

once they showed PI or with positive bias memory effects. These micro-processes still await study;

there are some suggestions from recent studies on the psychology of volition that there is

considerable perseverance, once a person has set the course on showing initiative (Gollwitzer,

1993).

The longitudinal study also overcomes some of the problems of common method variance

because earlier levels of the variables were held constant; at least constant sources of common

method variance (e.g., negative affectivity) are ruled out in this way. Within wave common method

(e.g., state of mind or quickly fluctuating mood), may produce higher concurrent correlations.

Given the arguments on how well work characteristics can be described by people and given the

fact that there are also significant correlations between work characteristics and mastery orientation

across time, we think that the problem of within wave common method variance is probably of

minor importance for our design. This kind of within wave common method variance could only

influence the correlations between work characteristics and mastery orientation because PI

(measured by performance and interviewer) does not have common method variance with work

characteristics and mastery orientation (measured in the questionnaire).

The second feature of our study - the variable overcoming barriers which is a performance

variable ascertained within the interview - also works against percept-percept biases. Even the

subjective interviewer impressions (such as interviewer evaluation of PI) help to overcome the

percept-percept bias of questionnaire studies. Since the interviewers were trained and had a

common anchor point across different subjects, they do not have the problem of differential anchor

points that besets questionnaire research. Most of the interviewers errors (e.g., a halo effect) would

most likely work to decrease the relationships. On the other hand, the interviewer errors are

Page 112: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

103

probably not very high because we found relatively high stabilities for PI (cf. Figure 21 – PI has

higher stabilities than mastery orientation and work characteristics) although different interviewers

did the interviews at different time points. Obviously our interviewer training was geared towards

keeping interviewer effects small.

The use of LISREL modeling has been quite useful in our study although it necessitated

some compromises. Rogosa (1995) and Stoolmiller (1995) criticized the autoregressive model. The

last author argues that if the dependent variable is highly stable, it is difficult to detect causal

relations with other variables. While this observation is certainly correct, the radical change

situation in East Germany - the site for this study - probably made it easier to detect significant

paths. The stability coefficients in our study were relative low – all lower than .70. Thus, the

historical change situation allowed us to use the autoregressive model in this case profitably.

One could argue that the specific historical situation of East Germany cannot be generalized

to the more stable market economies in Western Europe and in the U.S.A. And indeed, the specific

historical changes were reflected in our results. So, for example, the stabilities of work

characteristics are lower during the time of most rapid changes in the work places right after the

political unification of Germany (our waves T2 and T3). However, the relationships in our model

are regular across time suggesting that they would also hold (albeit maybe not as strongly and more

slowly) when the change situation is not quite so radical. One evidence for this is that the cross

sectional correlations between some of the variables discussed in our model are similar in East and

West Germany (Frese et al., 1996). Moreover, we think that even the more stable Western

economies are becoming more and more like East Germany with its high degree of turmoil on the

labor market and job changes. Obviously, our results bear on the situation of Asian countries at the

time of writing this article. Finally, all those countries that have recently changed from a more state

driven economy to a market economy (former Eastern bloc countries but also many developing

countries) find themselves in a similar situation as our study site (e.g., Kohn et al., 1997)

Theoretical Implications

The results confirm the view that occupational socialization has to incorporate both

processes: People actively change their work but the type of work they do also changes them. This

Page 113: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

104

is of high theoretical importance because it calls into question all one-dimensional views which

assume either a simple influence of the person on the type of work one gets and of the work

imprinting the person without any activity from the side of the person.

Our results have some bearing on some influential theories on the effects of control. First,

they reinforce that the popular, however rarely studied, concepts of reciprocal determinism Bandura

(1997) or efficacy- performance cycles (Lindsley, Brass, & Thomas, 1995) are useful and operative

at the individual level. Second, Bandura (1997) argues that reciprocal determinism works via self-

efficacy. While our study was not concerned with self-efficacy per se (as discussed, we developed a

second order latent factor from the three mediators which included self-efficacy), our results

support the importance of the mediator PI. As shown in Figure 21, PI can be seen to mediate the

relationship of mastery orientation (which includes self-efficacy) with work characteristics. This

implies that simply having mastery orientation does not help one to change one's environment - one

has to show PI. For this reason, we think that the concept of PI may be an important "missing link"

to agency and self-efficacy theory. It may pay off to systematically integrate PI into these type of

theories. PI may be of particular importance to understand positive cycles of efficacy and

performance (Lindsley et al., 1995)

Third, Stajkovic & Luthans (1998) show that the relationship between self-efficacy and

performance is lowest in complex tasks. Our study points to some other issue of task complexity:

Task complexity itself can increase mastery orientation (self-efficacy) and, thereby, contribute to

higher performance. Moreover, mastery experience (self-efficacy) contributes to job changes that

lead to higher task complexity (via higher PI). This leads to the interesting hypothesis that self-

efficacy might become less and less effective to produce high performance as real life career

trajectories unfold.

Fourth, our results also bear on the interpretation of the control needs by Greenberger and

Strasser (1991). They argued that there is a consistent desire to have control that is independent of

the actual control one receives. In case control is lower than desired, compensatory areas of control

are sought; for example, an assembly line worker with little control at work would seek to have

control in his home life. While our study was not meant to test this hypothesis directly (our

longitudinal study started before Greenberger & Strasser's paper was published), our data show that

Page 114: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

105

work characteristics change mastery orientation (and control motives are a part of mastery

orientation). Greenberger and Strasser (1991) did not directly discuss initiative, but their model

implies that lack of control at work should increase the degree of initiative because initiative is a

method to reassert control again. Our data show that this is not the case because lack of control and

complexity eventually decrease initiative. Thus, our data suggest that one should be skeptical

towards compensatory mechanisms.

Up to this point we have refrained from discussing PI as a personality construct; we simply

framed PI as a behavior. It makes sense that this behavior is related to such concepts as proactive

personality (Crant, 1995) or to a high activity level (Buss & Plomin, 1984). However, we do not

think that PI is a non-changeable trait. But there is no doubt that there are personality processes that

help in the development of showing PI-behavior. Personality processes could and should be

analyzed in the future within the 5-unit personality system suggested by Mischel & Shoda (1995).

One would look at the following processes: First, encoding of events, of situations and people can

be done from a self-starting and proactive viewpoint or from a purely order- or situation-driven and

reactive point of view. Perceiving action opportunities within the job situation is an important factor

that contributes to PI. We also think that the work characteristics control and complexity allow and

encourage the perception of action opportunities. Expectancies and beliefs (the second unit of the

personality system by Mischel & Shoda, 1995) have been in the foreground in this study. We do not

yet know a lot about the importance of affect (the third unit of Mischel & Shoda) for showing PI.

However, we assume that it will not be a purely positive affective state that contributes to PI-

behavior; first empirical results show that stressors actually increase the occurrence of PI (Fay,

1998). In terms of goals and values (the fourth unit), we assume that people who want to help the

company in the long run and who have long range goals and who have wider and more proactive

goals at the work place will show more PI at the work place (Parker, Wall & Jackson, 1997).

Finally, one needs specific competencies and self-regulatory plans (the fifth personality system unit

of Mischel & Shoda, 1995) to be able to show PI. We assume that it is important to deal with the

frustrations because of problems that appear whenever one attempts to show PI and persistence is

one aspect of PI. We do not know yet which competencies are important for PI but we assume that a

combination of cognitive ability (Fay, 1998) and practical intelligence (Sternberg & Wagner, 1986)

Page 115: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

106

is important to show PI. We think of this article as one step in describing the dynamics of the

development of PI - behavior. It goes without saying that some situations are obviously more

conducive to showing PI-behavior than others and that people can differentially apply PI in different

areas of their life (e.g. social or achievement areas).

Practical Implications

We started out to be interested in this variable because East Germans had difficulties showing PI

(Frese et al., 1996). However, we are now convinced that the general add-on value of this variable

for industry and service is even more important than the study of this particular historical situation.

Many companies are moving from stable structures to change oriented organizations and the issue

of PI is of high importance in any change process. The concept of PI may also be an important

prerequisite for the issue of employability.

Our results have important practical implications. If managers want to achieve a higher

degree of PI, they have to break the vicious cycle of low control and complexity at work, low

mastery orientation, low PI, leading to lower control and complexity at work. Probably the best

strategy is to change the work conditions to increase control and complexity and at the same time to

increase control appraisal, control motives and self-efficacy. The latter can be changed by

reinforcing mastery experiences and by letting the employees know how much one can rely on

them. Additionally, training procedures can be developed to increase control appraisal, control

motives, and self-efficacy, mainly those that increase self-regulatory processes.

Our data have important implications for what companies can do to increase initiative.

Many companies that introduced lean production have told employees to be more daring. However,

the work characteristics of control and complexity were not necessarily changed much (Remdisch,

1998). An example is to keep the assembly line intact without any changes but to introduce quality

circles that are supposed to present initiatives to improve quality. Here control and complexity of

the work itself are not changed but people are encouraged to show initiative. This strategy may be

effective to a certain extent (and we see that mastery orientation and personal initiative feed upon

each other). However, as long as the work conditions stay constant, there will be a limit to this

strategy (Lawler, 1992). People who take more initiative may leave the work place to find other

Page 116: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

107

work that allows more control and complexity (note, there is a path from PI to mastery orientation

which implies that one wants to have more control and responsibilities if one shows PI). Others may

not take initiative because they do not have enough mastery experiences in their jobs. We saw that

the influence of the work characteristics on mastery orientation was stronger and more consistent

than the other paths.

Our results support a pluralistic approach to encouraging initiative. There are various "entry

points" to change the cycles described. This may explain why different approaches to enhance

productivity report results of similar magnitude (Guzzo, Jette, & Katzell, 1985). One can start out

with improving the work characteristics, one can improve control appraisal, self-efficacy, and

enhance control aspirations, one can encourage initiative - since all of the paths feed upon each

other, the end result may be rather similar.

References

Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans:

Critique and reformulations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49-74.

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317- 332.

Anderson, J.C., & Gerbing, D.W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and

recommended two step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 411-423.

Arminger G. (1987). Misspecification, asymptotic stability, and ordinal variables in the analysis of

panel data. Sociological Methods & Research, 15, 336–348.

Ashford, S. J., & Black, J. S. (1996). Proactivity during organizational entry: The role of desire for

control. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 199-214.

Ashford, S. J., & Tsui, A. S. (1991). Self-regulation for managerial effectiveness: The role of active

feedback seeking. The Academy of Management Journal, 34, 251-280.

Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and

content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338-375.

Page 117: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

108

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.:

Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social theory. American Psychologist, 44, 1175-1184.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman and Co.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social

psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Bentler, P.M.(1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107,

238-246.

Bentler, P.M., & Chou, C. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociological Methods and

Research, 16, 78-117.

Bollen, K. & Lennox, R. (1991). Conventional wisdom on measurement: a structural equation

perspective. Psychological Bulletin,110, 305-314.

Borman, W. C., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1993). Expansing the criterion domain to include elements of

contextual performance. In N. Schmitt & W. C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel selection in

organizations (pp. 71-98). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Braverman, H. (1974). Labor and monopoly capital. New York: Monthly Review Press.

Bridges, W. (1995). Jobshift. London: Allen & Unwin.

Brief, A. P., Burke, M. J., George, J. M., Robinson, B. S., & Webster, J. (1988). Should negative

affectivity remain an unmeasured variable in the study of job stress? Journal of Applied

Psychology, 73, 193-198.

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J.

S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-62). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Brown, R.L. (1997). Assessing specific mediational effects in complex theoretical models.

Structural Equation Modeling 4,142-156.

Buss, A.H. & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, N.J.:

Erlbaum.

Page 118: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

109

Campbell, J. P., McCloy, R. A., Oppler, S., & Sager, C. E. (1993). A theory of performance. In N.

Schmitt, W. C. Borman, & ass. (Eds.), Personnel selection in organizations (pp. 35-79). San

Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Chao, G. T., O'Leary-Kelly, A. M., Wolf, S., Klein, H. J., & Gardner, P. D. (1994). Organizational

socialization: Its content and consequences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 730-743.

Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Teresi, J., Marchi, M., Velez, C.N.(1990). Problems in the measurement of

latent variables in structural equations causal models. Applied Psychological Measurement, 14,

183-196.

Cook T.D., Campbell D.T. (1979). Quasi- Experimentation. Design & analysis issues for field

settings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company.

Crant, J. M. (1995). The proactive personality scale and objective job performance among real

estate agents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 532-537.

Dwyer, J.H. (1983). Statistical models for the social and behavioral sciences. New York: Oxford

University Press.

Emery, F. E., & Thorsrud, E. (1969). Form and content in industrial democracy. London:

Tavistock.

Fay, D. (1998). Personal Initiative: Construct validation of a new concept of performance at work.

Doctoral Dissertation. Amsterdam: Faculty of Psychology, Univ. of Amsterdam.

Finkel, S.E. (1995). Causal analysis with panel data. Sage University series in Quantitative

Applications in the Social Sciences, 07-105. Thousand Oaks, Ca: Sage.

Fishbein, M. & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to theory

and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Folkman, S. (1984). Personal control and stress and coping processes: A theoretical analysis.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 839-852.

Ford, M.E. (1992). Motivating humans. Newbury Park, California: Sage

Frankenhaeuser, M., & Gardell, B. (1976). Underload and overload in working life: Outline of a

multidisciplinary approach. Journal of Human Stress, 35-46.

Frese, M. (1982). Occupational socialization and psychological development: An underemphasized

research perspective in industrial psychology. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 55, 209-224.

Page 119: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

110

Frese, M. (1984). Do workers want control at work or don't they: Some results on denial and

adjustment. Berlin. Institut für Humanwissenschaft in Arbeit und Ausbildung der Technischen

Universitaet Berlin .

Frese, M. (1987). A theory of control and complexity: Implications for software design and

integration of computer system into the work place. In M. Frese, E. Ulich, & W. Dzida (Eds.),

Psychological issues of human-computer interaction at the work place (pp. 313-337). Amsterdam:

North-Holland.

Frese, M. (1989). The concept of personal initiative: Operationalization, reliability and validity in

two German samples. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 70, 139-161.

Frese, M., Fay, D., Hilburger, T., Leng, K., & Tag, A. (1997). The concept of personal initiative:

Operationalization, reliability and validity in two German samples. Journal of Occupational and

Organizational Psychology, 70, 139-161.

Frese, M., Kring, W., Soose, A., & Zempel, J. (1996). Personal Initiative at work: Differences

between East and West Germany. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 37-63.

Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1988). Methodological issues in the study of work stress: Objective vs

subjective measurement of work stress and the question of longitudinal studies. In C. L. Cooper &

R. Payne (Eds.), Causes, coping and consequences of stress at work (pp. 375-411). Chichester:

Wiley.

Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1994). Action as the core of work psychology; A German approach. In H. C.

Triandis, M. D. Dunnette, & L. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and Organizational

Psychology (pp. 271-340). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Gardell, B. (1971). Alienation and mental health in the modern industrial environment. In L. Levi

(Ed.), Society, stress, and disease. vol. 1: The psychosocial environment and psychosomatic

diseases London: Oxford University Press.

Gerhart, B. (1988). Sources of variance in incumbent perceptions of job complexity. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 73, 154-162.

Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone

(Eds.), European review of social psychology (pp. 141-185). London: Wiley.

Page 120: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

111

Graham, J.W., & Donaldson, S.I. (1993). Evaluating interventions with differential attrition: the

importance of nonresponse mechanisms and use of follow-up data. Journal of Applied Psychology,

78, 119-128.

Greenberger, D. B., & Strasser, S. (1986). Development and application of a model of personal

control in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 11, 164-177.

Greenberger, D. B., & Strasser, S. (1991). The role of situational and dispositional factors in the

enhancement of personal control in organizations. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.),

Research in Organizational Behavior (pp. 111-146). Greenwich, Conn.: JAI Press.

Greenberger, D. B., Strasser, S., Cummings, L. L., & Dunham, R. B. (1989). The impact of

personal control on performance and satisfaction. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 43, 29-51.

Gulowsen, J. (1972). A measure of work-group autonomy. In L. E. Davis & J. C. Taylor (Eds.),

Design of jobs (pp. 374-390). Harmondsworth: Penguin.

Guzzo, R. A., Jette, R. D., & Katzell, R. A. (1985). The effects of psychologically based

intervention programs on work productivity: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 38, 275-291.

Hacker, W. (1986). Arbeitspsychologie. Bern: Huber.

Hacker, W. (1992). Expertenkönnen. Erkennen und Vermitteln. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Hackman, J. R. (1970). Tasks and task performance in research on stress. In J. E. McGrath Ed.),

Social and psychological factors in stress pp. 202-237). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Hackman, J. R., & Lawler, E. E. (1971). Employee reactions to job characteristics. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 55, 259-286.

Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1975). Development of the job diagnostic survey. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 60, 259-270.

Hayduk, L. A. (1987). Structural equation modeling with LISREL: Essentials and advances,

Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102,

284-304.

Page 121: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

112

Hertzog, C., & Nesselroade, J.R.(1987). Beyond autoregressive models: some implications of the

trait-state distinction of the structural modeling of developmental change. Child Development, 58,

93-109.

Hisrich, R. D. (1990). Entrepreneurship/intrapreneurship. American Psychologist, 45, 209-222.

Ilgen, D. R., & Hollenbeck, J. R. (1991). The structure of work: Job design and roles. In M. D.

Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 165-

208). Palo Alto, Calif.: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Imai, M. (1994). Kaizen. Berlin: Ullstein.

Jackson, P. R., Wall, T. D., Martin, R., & Davids, K. (1993). New measures of job control,

cognitive demand, and production responsibility. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 753-762.

James, L. R., & Brett, J. M. (1984). Mediators, moderators and tests for mediation. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 69, 307-321.

Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (1989). LISREL 7: A guide to the program and applications (2nd

ed.). Chicago:SPSS.

Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (1993a). LISREL8 user’s reference guide. Chicago: Scientific

Software International, Inc.

Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (1993b). LISREL8: Structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS

command language. Hilldale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Kahn, R. L., & Byosiere, P. (1992). Stress in organizations. In M. D. Dunnette & L. Hough (Eds.),

Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 571-650). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting

Psychologists Press.

Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude and mental strain: Implications for job

redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24, 385-408.

Karasek, R. A., & Theorell, T. (1990). Healthy work. Stress, productivity, and the reconstruction of

working life. New York: Basic Books.

Kenny D.A. & Campbell D.T. (1989). On the measurement of stability in over-time data. Journal of

Personality, 57, 445-481.

Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1978). The reciprocal effects of the substantive complexity of work

and intellectual flexibility: A longitudinal assessment. American Journal of Sociology, 84, 24-52.

Page 122: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

113

Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1982). Job conditions and personality: A longitudinal assessment of

their reciprocal effects. American Journal of Sociology, 87, 1257-1286.

Kohn, M. L., Slomczynski, K. M., Janicka, K., Khmelko, V., Mach, B. W., Paniotto, V.,

Zaborowski, W., Gutierrez, R., & Heyman, C. (1997). Social structure and personality under

conditions of radical social change: A comparative analysis of Poland and Ukraine. American

Sociological Review, 62, 614-638.

Kornhauser, A. (1965). Mental health of the industrial worker. New York: Wiley.

Kuhl, J. (1992). A theory of self-regulation: Action versus state orientation, self-discrimination, and

some applications. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 41, 97-129.

Latham, G. P., & Saari, L. M. (1984). Do people do what they say? Further studies on the

situational interview. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 569-573.

Lawler, E. E., III (1992). The ultimate advantage: Creating the high involvement organization. San

Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer.

Lindsley, D.H., Brass, D.J. & Thomas, J.B. (1995). Efficacy-performance spirals: A multilevel

perspective. Academy of Management Review, 20, 645-678.

Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood

Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Loher, B.T.,Noe, R.A.,Moeller, N.L. Fitzgerald, M.P.(1985). A meta-analysis of the relation of job

characteristics to job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 280-289

MacCallum, R.C. & Browne, M.C. (1993). The use of causal indicators in covariance structure

models: some practical issues. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 533-541.

Marsh, H.W. (1993). Stability of individual differences in multiwave panel studies: comparison of

simplex models and one factor models. Journal of Educational Measurement, 30, 157-183.

Marsh, H.W., Hau, K., Balla, J.R. & Grayson, D. (1998). Is more ever too much? The number of

indicators per factor in confirmatory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavior Behavioral Research,

33, 181-220.

McDonald, R.P.(1996). Path analysis with composite variables. Multivariate Behavioral Research,

31, 239-270.

Page 123: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

114

Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. London:

Holt.

Mischel, W. & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality:

Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure.

Psychological Review, 102, 246-268.

Morrison, E. W. (1993). Longitudinal study of the effects of information seeking on newcomer

socialization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 173- 183.

Mortimer, J.T. & Lorence, J. (1997a). Work experience and occupational value socialization: A

longitudinal study. American Journal of Socioloy, 84, 1361-1387.

Mortimer, J.T. & Lorence, J. (1997b). Occupational experience and and the self-concept: A

longitudinal study. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42, 307-323.

Motowidlo, S. J., Borman, W. C., & Schmit, M. J. (1997). A theory of individual differences in task

and contextual performance. Human Performance, 10, 71-84.

Mueller, R.O. (1996. Basic principles of structural equation modeling. An introduction to LISREL

and EQS. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Muthén, B. (1989). Latent variable modeling in heterogeneous populations. Psychometrika. 54,

557-585.

Nickel, H. M., Kühl, J., & Schenk, S. (Eds.). (1994). Erwerbsarbeit und Beschäftigung im

Umbruch. Berlin: Akademie Verlag.

Organ, D. (1988). Organizational citizenship behavior: The good soldier syndrome. Lezington,

MA: Lexington Books.

Organ, D. W. (1997). Organizational Citizenship Behavior: It's construct clean-up time. Human

Performance, 10, 85-98.

Parker, S.K., Wall, T.D. & Jackson, P.R. (1997). “That’s not my job”: Developing flexible

employee work orientations. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 899-929.

Peters, T. J., & Waterman, R. H. (1982). In search of excellence. New York: Harper & Row.

Pentzt, M.A. & Chou C.P. (1994). Measurement invariance in longitudinal research assuming

change from development and intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,62,

450-462.

Page 124: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

115

Plewis, I. (1985). Analysing change. Chichester: Wiley.

Plewis, I. (1996). Statistical methods for understanding cognitive growth: a review, a synthesis and

an application. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 49, 25–42.

Raffalovich, L.E. & Bohrnstedt,G.W. (1987). Common, specific, and error variance components of

factor models. Sociological Methods & Research.15, 385-405.

Remdisch, S. (1998). Gruppenarbeit in der Automobilindustrie: Die Rolle der Gruppensprecher

und Meister. PhD, Univ. of Giessen, Germany.

Roth, P.L. (1994). Missing data: a conceptual review for applied psychologists. Personnel

Psychology, 47, 537-560.

Rogosa, R. (1995). Myths and Methods: “Myths about Longitudinal research” plus Supplemental

Questions. In J.M. Gottmann (Ed). The analysis of change (pp.3-66). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rotter, J. B. (1972). An introduction to social learning theory. In J. B. Rotter, J. E. Chance, & E. J.

Phares (Eds.), Applications of a social learning theory of personality. New York: Holt.

Rothbaum, F. W., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the world and changing the self:

A two-process model of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 5-37.

Rovine, J.M. & Molenaar, P.C.M. (1998). A LISREL model for the analysis of repeated measures

with a patterned covariance matrix. Structural Equation Modeling, 5, 318-384.

Schaie, K.W. (1973). Methodological problems in descriptive developmental research on adulthood

and aging. In J.R. Nesselroade & H.W. Reese (Eds.). Life-span development psychology:

methodological issues (pp. 253-280). New York: Academic.

Schallberger, U. (1988). Berufsausbildung und Intelligenz (occupational education and

intelligence). In K. Häfeli, U. Kraft & U. Schallberger (Eds.) Berufsausbildung und

Persönlichkeitsentwicklung: Eine Längsschnittuntersuchung (Occupational education and

personality development: A longitudinal study) (pp. 148-167). Bern: Huber.

Schwarzer, R., Baessler, J., Kwiatek, P., Schroeder, K., & Zhang, J. X. (1997). The assessment of

optimistic self-beliefs: Comparison of the German, Spanish, and Chinese versions of the General

Self-Efficacy Scale. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46, 69-88.

Seligman, M. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San Francisco:

Freeman.

Page 125: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

116

Seligman, M. E. P., & Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory style as a predictor of productivity and

quitting among life insurance sales agents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 432-

438.

Semmer, N. (1982). Stress at work, stress in private life and psychological well-being. In W.

Bachmann & I. Udris (Eds.), Mental load and stress in activity: European approaches (pp. 42-55).

Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Semmer, N. (1984). Streßbezogene Tätigkeitsanalyse: Psychologische Untersuchungen zur Analyse

von Streß am Arbeitsplatz. Weinheim: Beltz.

Semmer, N., & Schallberger, U. (1996). Selection, socialisation, and mutual adaptation: Resolving

discrepancies between people and work. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 263-288.

Spector, P. E. (1986). Perceived control by employees: A meta-analysis of studies concerning

autonomy and participation at work. Human Relations, 11, 1005-1016.

Spector, P. E. (1992). A consideration of the validity and meaning of self-report measures of job

conditions. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and

organizational psychology 1992 (pp. 123-151). Chichester: Wiley.

Smith, C. A., Organ, D. W., & Near, J. P. (1983). Organizational citizenship behavior: its nature

and antecedents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 653-663.

Speier, C., & Frese, M. (1997). Generalized self-efficacy as a mediator and moderator between

control and complexity at work and personal initiative: A longitudinal field study in East Germany.

Human Performance, 10, 171-192.

Spreitzer, G. M. (1995). Psychological empowerment in the workplace: Dimensions, measurement,

and validation. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 1442-1465.

Staw, B. M., & Boettger, R. D. (1990). Task revision: A neglected form of work performance.

Academy of Management Journal, 33, 534-559.

Stajkovic, A.D. & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and work-related performance: A meta-analysis.

Psychological Bulletin, 124, 240-261.

Sternberg, R.J. & Wagner, R.K. (1986). Practical intelligence. Cambridge, England: Cambridge

Univ. Press.

Page 126: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

117

Stoolmiller, M. (1995). Autoregressive effects in structural equation models: we see some

problems. In J.M. Gottmann (Ed). The analysis of change (pp. 261-276). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Taira, K. (1996). Compatibility of human resource management, industrial relations, and

engineering under mass production and lean production: An exploration. Applied Psychology: An

International Review, 45, 97-117.

Van Maanen, J. (1976). Breaking in: Socialization to work. In R. Dubin (Ed.), Handbook of work,

organization, and society (pp. 67-130). Chicago: Rand McNally.

Volpert, W. (1977). Einleitung zum Neudruck von Grundsätze der wissenschaftlichen

Betriebsführung. In W. Volpert & R. Vahrenkamp (Eds.), Frederick W. Taylor: Grundsätze der

wissenschaftlichen Betriebsführung. Neuauflage Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.

Vonesh E.F., & Chinchilli V.M. (1997). Linear and nonlinear models for the analysis of repeated

measurements. New York: Marcel Dekker.

Wall, T. D., & Clegg, C. W. (1981). A longitudinal study of group work redesign. Journal of

Occupational Psychology, 2, 31-49.

Wall, T. D., Clegg, C. W., & Jackson, P. R. (1978). An evaluation of the Job Characteristics Model.

Journal of Occupational Psychology, 51, 183-196.

Wall, T. D., Jackson, P. R., Mullarkey, S., & Parker, S. (1996). The demands-control model of job

strain: A more specific test. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 69, 153-166.

Wall, T. D., & Jackson, P. S. (1995). New manufacturing initiatives and shop floor job design. In

A. Howard (Ed.), The changing nature of work (pp. 139-174). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

West, M. A., & Farr, J. L. (Eds.. (1990). Innovation and creativity at work: Psychological and

organizational strategies. Chichester, England: Wiley.

White, R. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66,

267-333.

Wicklund, R. A. (1974). Freedom and reactance. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Wilk, S. L., Desmarais, L. B., & Sackett, P. R. (1995). Gravitation to jobs commensurate with

ability: Longitudinal and cross-sectional tests. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 79-85.

Willett, J.B. Sayer, A.G. (1994). Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates of

individual change over time. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 363–381.

Page 127: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

118

Willett, J.B. Sayer, A.G. (1995). Cross-domain analysis of change over time: Combining growth

curve modeling and covariance structure analysis. In G.A. Marcoulides & R.E. Schumacker (Eds).

Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp. 125-157). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Williams, L.J. &. Podsakoff, P.M. (1989). Longitudinal field methods for studying reciprocal

relationships in organizational behavior research: toward improved causal analysis. Research in

Organizational Behavior, 11, 247-292.

Womack, J. P., Jones, D. T., & Roos, D. (1990). The machine that changed the world. New York:

Rawson.

Wood, R. E. (1986). Task complexity: Definition of the construct. Organizational Behavior and

Human Decision Processes, 37, 60-82.

Wortman, C. B., & Brehm, J. W. (1975). Responses to uncontrollable outcomes: An integration of

reactance theory and the learned helplessness model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in

experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press.

Zapf, D. (1989). Selbst-und Fremdbeobachtung in der psychologischen Arbeitsanalyse. Göttingen:

Hogrefe.

Zapf, D. (1993). Stress-oriented job analysis of computerized office work. The European Work and

Organizational Psychologist, 3, 85-100.

Page 128: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

119

Chapter 4

The Temporal Factor of Change in Stressor-Strain Relationships:

A Growth Curve Model on a Longitudinal Study in East Germany

In this chapter we examine the stressor-strain relationships using longitudinal data (six

measurement points) in a radical change situation - the situation in East Germany after the collapse

of communism in 1990. This article contributes to the literature in the following ways: First, the

stress literature is vast, but there is a lack of longitudinal studies (Zapf, Dormann & Frese, 1996).

Many authors have called for longitudinal studies. Second, stress effects unfold in time. However,

stress research has not been explicit in discussing this process. In this article we shall discuss

alternative models on time lags necessary for stressors to have an effect on strain. Third, there has

been a call in stress research to look for intraindividual differences in these processes (Frese &

Zapf, 1988). In this chapter we make a first attempt to focus on the relationships between within-

person changes in stressors and within-person changes in strain. For example, some individuals may

react to a decrease of a stressor with an immediate decline in their strain level, whereas others take a

much longer time to react. Fourth, the focus on intraindividual changes over a long time frame

enabled us to decompose changes in slow moving trendlike changes and short-term statelike

fluctuations. Fifth, to do this, we had to use a methodological procedure not frequently applied in

stress research: the growth curve model. Sixth, Kasl (1978) argued that stress research should

capitalize on naturally occurring events that have an impact on stress. East German society and

workplaces changed completely from socialism to capitalism after the introduction of the West

German D-Mark in mid 1990. This is a natural starting point for stressor changes. Finally, this study

used a representative sampling procedure, so that the stress process in multiple worksites or

industries and the role of occupational self-selection and drift can be studied (recommended by

Murphy, Hurrell and Quick (1992)).

Stressor-Strain Models

The issue of how stressor-strain relationships unfold in time is of fundamental importance

for stress research although it has not been studied systematically. The theoretical framework for

our models was inspired by the stressor-strain models presented by Frese and Zapf (1988) and the

Page 129: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

120

interpretation of change by Nesselroade (1991). Nesselroade (1991, p. 96) distinguishes three kinds

of variability: (1) intraindividual variability (relatively rapid, more or less reversible changes, such

as states), (2) intraindividual change (relatively slow changes reflecting processes, such as

development, labeled as ‘trait change’) and (3) inter-individual changes (highly stable, even over

long periods, denoted as ‘traits’). The stressor-strain models of Frese & Zapf (1988) explain the

various ways exposure to stressors may lead to psychological and psychosomatic dysfunctioning in

the course of time.

From these two sources we distilled six theoretical models that were tested in this study;

they are summarized in Table 4. Table 4 also presents differences between the models with regard

to their predictions, time perspectives, and causal agents. The last column on statistical predictions

is described in more detail later. These predictions range from perfect stability to very short-term

effects of stressors on strain. An additional model is the Reverse Causation Model, which argues for

the opposite direction of strain effects on stressors.

1) Strain Stability Model. There are two types of stability. One relates to the stability of the

means of stressors and strains (mean stability). The other one is the stability of individual

differences. The stability of individual differences implies that the relative position of the subjects

scores does not change over time. For instance, all people can move in the same direction, which

implies a mean change, but the relative position of persons might remain unaltered. Theoretically, it

has been argued that strain is a function of negative affectivity and that negative affectivity is

genetically determined (Brief, Burke, George, Robinson & Webster, 1988; Burke, Brief & George,

1993; Spector, Zapf, Chen & Frese, in press). In its strong form, this hypothesis implies that strain

is completely stable both in terms of means and in terms of individual differences. This means that

a person’s psychological health is not affected by changes in the outside world; strain is

conceptualized as a stable trait despite changing circumstances.

On the stressor side, we hypothesized changes because East Germany is in a radical change

situation, which should translate into changes in the levels of stressors. This implies that there

should be clear changes in the means and probably in the individual differences, because not all

people are equally affected by the shifts in stressor levels.

2) Interindividual Differences Model. In contrast to the Strain Stability Model, the

Interindividual Difference Model predicts that stressors and strains should be related. Furthermore,

the Interindividual Difference Model does not expect the strains to be completely stable, although

Page 130: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

121

this model refers to the stable component in the strains. In addition, this model argues that there is

also a stable component in the stressors and that the stable parts of stressors and strains should be

related. We call it the Interindividual Differences Model, because the covariation between stressors

and strains in all measurement waves can be fully explained by differences between people. Two

processes may be responsible for the relationship between stressors and strains. First, there may be a

fit between personal characteristics (e.g., strain) and situational parameters (e.g., stressors) that are

constantly adjusting to each other. This leads to a highly stable, mutually reinforcing equilibrium

that would imply that relationships between variables do not change. Second, the model may also

come about because one underlying ultrastable personality trait causes both stressors and strains.

This could be a result of negative affectivity (Brief, et al., 1988; Burke et al., 1993). Negative

affectivity implies that a stable negative affectivity trait produces spurious correlations between

reports of stressors and strains (Brief et al., 1988). Thus, equilibrium processes or a stable third

variable could cause high stability of interindividual differences in both stressors and strains.

3) Stressor-Strain Trend Model. In contrast to the previous models this model does not

refer to the completely stable components, but instead to the relatively slow moving changes in

stressors and strains. The Stressor-Strain Trend Model implies that long-term changes in one

stressor lead to corresponding changes in the strain variables. Thus, the trends of stressors and strain

are related. This is, for example, the case when time pressures in the job gradually increases. People

do not react immediately to the accompanying daily fluctuations in time pressure, but instead

gradually develop visible psychosomatic symptoms. This model allows for a waxing and waning in

the symptoms as well, which might be related to the daily fluctuations in stress levels.

4) Reverse Causation Model. Most stress models focus on the effects of stressors, but

some models argue for a reverse causation: Initial strain levels may determine later exposure to

stressors. Two mechanisms can explain the impact on later stressors: selection and direct effects.

Selection effects can have benign or detrimental effects on later stressors. Kohn (1973) and Frese

(1985) have argued that one legitimate hypothesis is that people with a high degree of strain tend to

fall back to less desirable jobs or get assigned more stressful tasks within their jobs (drift model).

The reason is that they, either cannot cope well with the job and, therefore, do not receive more

desirable assignments, or because of a high degree of absenteeism are relegated to more stressful

tasks (or vice versa, those who can cope better get better tasks). However, initial strain levels may

also have opposite effects on later stressor levels: The selection mechanism with an opposite

Page 131: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

122

continuation Table 4

Reverse

Causation

long-term

influence of strains on stressors

lagged strains

correlation betw

een strain intercept factor and stressor slope factor

Sleeper-E

ffect M

odel long term

influences of stressors on strains

lagged stressors

positive correlation betw

een stressor intercept factors and strain slope factors

Short-Term

R

eaction M

odel

short-term

continuous effects of stressors on strains

synchronous stressors

significant

positive covariates

Page 132: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

123

Table 4. C

haracteristics and Statistical Predictions for Theoretical M

odels T

ested in: M

odel Prediction

Tim

e perspective

Causal agent

Spurious m

odel L

atent Grow

th m

odel M

easurement

model

Hybrid m

odel

Strain S

tability M

odel a) m

ean stability b) stability of individual differences

stable means

of strains, but stressors m

eans change no change in the relative position of strain scores

long time

stability for strains

stable trait

equality constraints for m

eans low

stability coefficients

Interindivi-dual D

ifference M

odel

Stable parts of stressors and strains are related

long time-

period stable trait

one perfectly stable latent variable explains the covariances of all stressors and strains

Stressor-

Strain Trend

Model

continuous influence of stressors on strains

trends over a long tim

e-period

stressors

positive correlation betw

een stressor slopes factors and strain slope factors

Page 133: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

124

outcome is the Refuge Model. Employees suffering from high strain may seek new jobs (or diffe-

rent tasks within the same job) so that they can reduce their stress level. This kind of selection effect

is called the Refuge Model, because employees retreat from the tough jobs and look for the less

stressful jobs (and the other way around, some people may look for challenges when their strain

level is low). The Drift Model and the Refuge Model differ in their prediction for the development

of later stressors. The Drift Model predicts a positive relationship between initial strain and later

stressors, because of the worsening of the working conditions, whereas a Refuge Model predicts a

negative relationship, because workers are successful in reducing the level of stress to which they

are exposed. Direct effects can also be either positive or negative. The extent to which coping

efforts are successful is crucial. Positive effects can be expected if problem-focused coping reduces

chronic stressors. An example of negative effects is the true strain-stressor10 hypothesis (Zapf,

Dormann & Frese, 1996). For instance, software designers who cannot cope with time pressure,

may become too anxious, resulting in reduced cognitive abilities, and this may result in more errors.

Correcting these errors increases the workload even further.

5) Sleeper Effect Model. A sleeper effect occurs when stressors do not have an immediate

effect but need some “incubation” time (Nesselroade, 1991, Frese & Zapf, 1988). An analogy is

post-traumatic stress disorder (DSM IV American Psychiatric Association, 1994) or burnout (Glass

& McKnight, 1996; Maslach, 1998). For our purposes, the question is whether there are long-term

lagged effects of stressors that appear much later. For example, social stressors may lead to a

cautious and even hostile attitude toward colleagues that contributes to later depression. In this case,

the hostile attitude acts like a slow-acting virus. An alternative mechanism for this effect is the

accumulation model with a threshold. Such a model has been argued for the results of night work

and shift work (Frese & Okonek, 1984). Only after a certain threshold (breaking point) is reached

do long-term effects of shift work appear. These effects do not disappear even with the cessation of

the shift work.

6) Short-Term Reaction Model. Stressors can have an immediate effect on strain (Frese &

Zapf, 1988, refer to this as an initial impact model). Thus, there is an immediate reaction to a

stressor that may subside shortly thereafter if there is no exposure to this stressor any longer. Thus,

10 In the context of negative affectivity a similar hypothesis is called the ‘stressor creation hypothesis’ by Spector, Chen,

Zapf & Frese (in press).

Page 134: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

125

strain fluctuates directly with the level of stressors involved. Although this model sounds like a

simple stimulus-response model and, therefore, is reminiscent of the stress-strain models of the

stress research in the 50s and 60s, it is also possible to posit some intermediate coping processes

(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). However, in contrast to the sleeper effect these processes should occur

relatively quickly.

In summary, the first two models (the Strain Stability Model and the Individual Differences

Model) can be regarded as personality models and therefore predict high stability in strains. The

difference between the Strain Stability Model and the Individual Differences Model is that the first

treats the measurement of the stressors as reflecting objective characteristics of the work

environment, which can change of course, although these changes do not affect the strains. In

contrast, the Individual Difference Model treats the stressor reports as strongly confounded with

personality and does not predict that the residual changes (thus, after the stable trait has been

partialled out) in stressors and strains are related to each other. The Reverse Causation Model and

the Sleeper Effect Model are both lagged models, but they differ in their causal direction: Initial

strain predicts later stressor levels or vice versa, earlier levels of stressors have a lagged effect on

strains. The Sleeper Effect Model, the Stressor-Strain Trend Model and the Short-Term Reaction

Model are traditional stress models in the sense that they consider stressors as the causal agents.

The last two models differ because they focus on different aspects of the data: The slow moving

systematic changes versus the rapid fluctuations. An analogy is the effects of trends of air pollution

on climate versus the short-term effects of air pollution on weather conditions.

The Situation in East Germany

Our approach in looking at growth curves is particularly interesting in a country that has

changed dramatically in terms of working conditions and social makeup. All East European

countries share this dramatic change. We concentrate on East Germany during the 5 years after

unification.

Table 5 shows a few dates to emphasize the historical context in which our study was done.

As the short description in Table 5 shows, the economic situation in East Germany changed most

radically in late 1990 - 1991. Democracy and capitalism came to East Germany mid 1990. Because

the workplaces did not become more democratic until late 1990 and in 1991, our T1 data were

collected at a time when socialist practices were still widespread in the state-run companies.

Page 135: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

126

Although all respondents knew that changes were imminent, most did not anticipate the quality and

level of the impending changes.

Table 5 Historical context in East Germany

Time Historical events Study waves

October-November 1989 Mass demonstrations in Leipzig, Dresden and Berlin November 1989 The Berlin wall opens March 1990 First free election in East Germany July 1990 Economic unification; the DM (the West German

currency) is introduced in East Germany; the first changes appear at the work places; East German companies are start to be sold off, mainly to West German firms; workplaces are still very much like they were under socialism

T1

October 1990 Political unification November 1990 Workplaces start to be changed T2 December 1990 First general election in all of Germany 1991 Serious economic crisis in East Germany, many work

related education programs started by government

August-September 1991 Dramatic changes in workplaces; many people change jobs

T3

1992-1993 The economic crisis in East Germany deepens; wages increase to ca. 70 - 80% of Western level; many government programs to stimulate growth; more and more resentment toward West German managers among East Germans

August-September 1992 T4 August-September 1993 T5 1994-1995 The economic situation in East Germany stabilizes on a

low level; unemployment is high, in some towns approaching 50%; there are pockets of very high productivity in the East; however, average productivity of East German workers is about 70% of those in the West; most industrial jobs have been lost; West Germany slides into an economic recession with high unemployment

August-September 1995 T6

With regard to the stressors, the following hypotheses are plausible. Under socialism,

unemployment was virtually unknown. This was still the case in 1990, during the first wave of our

study (we only included employed people at T1 in our sample). People were aware that the

introduction of capitalism would mean layoffs. Moreover, they had few illusions about the

Page 136: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

127

competitive strength of their companies. It was obvious to everyone how badly work was organized

and how many investments were needed to bring productivity and product quality up to modern

standards. Thus, they knew that many jobs would be lost and, therefore, job insecurity should be

high from the start. However, at the same time optimism with regard to the labor market prevailed.

People expected that even if they lost their jobs, it would be easy to find new ones. We

hypothesized that fear of unemployment would peak at T3 and then level off. Unemployment

increases were higher in the beginning years of economic change, particularly 1991, because the

firms had to lay off staff to trim their companies and make them ready for sale (nearly all state-

owned companies were sold until 1995). Although the rate of unemployment still continued to

increase, those who had a job in 1993 could feel much more secure than in 1991.

At T1, work life was still socialist, which implied that people could easily leave the

workplace to go shopping and that work was slow and comparatively nondemanding. However,

there were many organizational problems: obtaining needed supplies, trying to complete tasks with

inadequate tools, etc. In other words, time pressure was low and organizational problems were high.

It was our hypothesis that during the 5 years of our study, time pressure would increase because of

the work pressures of modern management systems. On the other hand, organizational problems

would decrease as the tools of production became more modern and the introduction of supplies

became better planned.

Socialist East Germany has been described as a "niche society" in which friendships were

very important and where comradeship at work was high. Newspaper reports and psychotherapists

(e.g., Maaz, 1992) have argued that with the introduction of capitalism the social climate has

become rougher because competition has increased (e.g., for workplaces, for better jobs, for a

career). This would suggest that in 1990, social stressors should be lower and that they should

increase linearly with time.

Work requirements were not clearly laid down in socialist times; thus, it was not quite clear

which job requirements one had to fulfill and which ones not (Pearce, Branyicki & Bukacsi, 1994).

It is reasonable to assume that this led to role ambiguity and role conflicts. We assume that the

modern management methods introduced in 1991 would gradually give the employees a clearer

sense of what was expected of them, thus reducing uncertainty (which is a conglomerate of role

ambiguity and conflict).

One could interpret the radical change situation as a stressful life event. Although many

changes were, of course, most welcome, others might be perceived to be negative. The social

Page 137: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

128

atmosphere would become harder and more competitive - the niche society could not be upheld. On

the other hand the introduction of modern management would lead to the decrease of organizational

problems and uncertainty. Thus, we hypothesized that some of the work stressors would increase,

whereas others would decrease, and that the combined effects of all the work stressors would

produce a more or less constant level of strain during the period of our study.

Method

The data in this study were gathered in the AHUS project. AHUS is a German acronym for

“active actions in a radical change situation”. The goal of the project was to study the effects of the

drastic changes that took place after the unification of East and West Germany, and one of the

research questions was which people could cope better with the many stressors they encountered.

This study used all 6 waves over a five year time period. Other publications of this study

concentrated on personal initiative (Frese, Kring, Soose, & Zempel, 1996; Frese, Fay, Hilburger,

Leng, & Tag, 1997; Frese, Garst, & Fay, 1998) and social support as a moderator of stressors

(Dormann & Zapf, 1999). None of the data reported here have been published before.

Sample

A representative sample was drawn in Dresden, a large city in the south of East Germany; it

is the capital of Saxonia, houses a large technical university, and is relatively well-off (e.g.,

compared with cities in the north of East Germany). The sampling was done by randomly selecting

streets, then selecting every third house, and then in each house, every fourth apartment (in smaller

houses every third one). All people between the ages of 17 and 65 with full-time employment at T1

participated (thus, we sometimes had more than one participant per family). The refusal rate of 33%

was quite low for a study of this kind. Confidentiality was assured; if participants preferred

anonymity, they were assigned a personal code word.

At T1 (July 1990), 463 people participated in Dresden. At T2 (November and December,

1990) 202 additional people were asked to participate11. At T3 (September 1991), the N was 543, at

T4 (September 1992) the N was 506, at T5 (September 1993), N = 478, at T6 (September 1995), N

11 Additional people were added to ascertain whether repeated participation had an influence on the variables of the

questionnaire. This was not the case.

Page 138: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

129

= 489. Experimental mortality did not change the makeup of the sample. There were no significant

differences in the stressor variables between dropouts from T1 to T3 and full participants. The

sample is representative of the Dresden population on the relevant parameters (e.g., for age, social

class, male and female percentage at work). Fifty-three percent of the participants were male and

47% were female. At T3 ages ranged from 17 to 65 years (M = 39, SD = 11.4). Most participants

worked in public or private services (35,9%), followed by those who were employed in trade or

manufacturing enterprises (30.9%). Of the office workers in the study 18.9% had jobs that required

minimal qualifications, whereas 27.4% were either managers or professionals positions calling for

higher qualifications. Higher level public servants, mostly employed in schools and universities

made up 12,5% of the participants. The study also contained skilled (16.5%) and unskilled blue-

collar workers (14.9%). At the start of the study none of the participants were unemployed, but the

unemployment figures for the subsequent waves were n = 42 (7%) at T2, n = 59 (11%) at T3, n = 38

(7.8%) at T4, n = 35 (7.5%) at T5 and n = 37 (8.1%) at T6. After the first wave, some of the

participants had no job for reasons other than involuntary unemployment (e.g., retirement,

schooling, parental leave). The items on the stressor scales were not administered to those people

who did not have a job at the time of the assessment.

Measures

All stressor and strain measures were ascertained with a questionnaire.

Strain Variables. The strain variables, depression, psychosomatic complaints, irritation and

worrying, are adaptations of Mohr’s (1986) scales – a group of well-validated scales that are used

frequently in Germany.

Depression (four items) was originally adapted from Zung (1965), and all of those items that

referred to physical problems (e.g., not being able to sleep) were excluded to reduce the overlap

with psychosomatic complaints. Items were “A good deal seems senseless to me” and “I have sad

moods”. A 7-point Likert scale was used and the extreme response categories were described as

“almost always” and “never”.

Psychosomatic complaints (eight items) was originally adapted from Fahrenberg (1975) and

related to aches and other negative bodily sensations, that are commonly regarded as strain

symptoms. The respondents can easily detect the symptoms and no medical assistance is needed for

its diagnosis. The contents of some items were: “Do you feel pain in your shoulders?” and “Do you

Page 139: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

130

have feelings of dizziness?”. A 5-point Likert scale was used with response categories ranging

from ‘almost daily’ to ‘never’.

Irritation (five items) and Worrying (three items) were both derivatives of the scale of

irritation and strain developed by Mohr (1986), because preliminary analysis indicated that two

(only moderately correlated) factors could be distinguished.

Worrying referred to the preoccupation of work-related problems in one’s spare time (“Even

during holidays I think a lot about problems at my work”). The scope of the original irritation scale

was narrowed down to feelings of irritation and nervousness (“I’m easily agitated”). A 7-point

Likert scale was used.

Stressors. The stressors used a 5-point Likert-type answer scale and have all been adapted from

Semmer (1982; 1984) and Zapf (1991) with the exception of social stressors (Frese & Zapf, 1987).

The scale development of the stressors was influenced by Caplan, Cobb, French, van Harrison and

Pinneau (1975). All of these scales are frequently used in German studies and have been well

validated.

Job insecurity (four items) asked about how secure the job was. Questions referred to the

probability of becoming unemployed or of the chance of finding a new job in case one became

unemployed.

Time pressure (five items) included several aspects of mental efforts (concentration,

vigilance, long working hours and time-pressure) (e.g., “How often do you experience time

pressure?”).

Organizational problems (eight items) was longer than the original Semmer scale, because

we wanted to include more East-German-specific items. It measured whether the material, the

supplies, and the tools were adequate so that work could be done without interruptions. In a prior

study it had been shown to be one of the stressors most strongly related to psychosomatic

complaints (Semmer, 1984). The construct is similar to the organizational constraints described in

Peters, O’Connor, and Rudolf (1980). They defined the construct as ‘facilitating and inhibiting

conditions not under the control of the individual’, and they found a relation not only with

performance but also with affective responses.

Social stressors (six items) referred to several aspects of work relationships e.g., a negative

group climate, conflicts with coworkers and supervisors, and social animosities.

Uncertainty (five items) combined role conflict and role ambiguity by asking for unclear and

conflicting commands and the problem that a mistake may lead to damages.

Page 140: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

131

Modeling Strategy

Our modeling strategy consisted of two steps. In the first step we tested the measurement

models. The second step was used to test the structural models (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988).

Measurement Models

Strain variables. The strategy of the measurement modeling involved three basic steps. In

the first step (Model 1) a longitudinal measurement model was tested.

The assumption that the same construct was measured across all time points is crucial

(Plewis 1996; Kenny & Campbell 1989). Therefore, steps two and three tested measurement

invariance (Little 1997; Meredith, 1993). The second step (Model 2) tested for equality of factor

loadings over time. Changes in relationships of the latent construct and the items over time is an

indication of a gamma change (Golembiewski, Billingsley & Yeager, 1976), which implies a

change in the respondent’s interpretation of the item content (Oort, 1996). If there is a sizeable

gamma change, comparisons of the relevant constructs over time are impossible. In a third step

(Model 3), the equivalence of item intercepts over time was tested. If all factor loadings are equal, a

change in the item intercepts indicates a general change in the level of the item response. This

implies that the item is more or less attractive and, that this shift cannot be explained by a change in

the latent trait. This phenomenon is called beta change (also called a response shift) and occurs if a

respondent changes his or her meaning of the item response scale’s value (Oort, 1996). Some

authors (Byrne, Shavelson & Muthén, 1989; Pentz & Chou, 1994) have argued that in practice a

few violations can be tolerated and that partial measurement invariance is a more realistic goal.

Stressors. The stressor variables were not fitted with a confirmatory factor analysis, and

internal consistencies were not calculated. The reason for this is that we prefer to see the stressor

items as a mixture of causal and effect indicators12 (Bollen & Lennox, 1991; Cohen, Cohen, Teresi,

Marchi & Velez 1990; MacCallum & Browne, 1993; Spector & Jex, 1998). In general, variables

can be conceived either as causes or as effects of a latent construct (Blalock, 1967, pp. 163-164). In

a factor model, the latent construct is conceived as the cause for the observed variables (e.g.,

12 Causal indicators are also known as ‘formative’ indicators, while effect indicators are often called ‘reflective’

indicators.

Page 141: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

132

responses to the items of a questionnaire) that are called effect indicators. In contrast, a causal

indicator model assumes that the observed variables determine the latent construct. However,

sometimes both models seem to be at least partly valid, and this poses a problem for constructing a

measurement model. This was the case in our study, where the stressor variables were based upon a

questionnaire, in which the items focused on external events the respondent encountered. Semmer,

Zapf, and Greif (1996) showed that our measures of the stressors were at least partly related to the

“objective” work situation. Therefore, it is best to consider the stressor variables as representing

both subjective and objective features of the work situation (Ilgen & Hollenback, 1992, Spector,

1998, p.161). However, the subjective and the objective interpretations have different implications

for both the direction of the paths between the latent construct and the indicators, as well as the

form of the covariance matrix of the indicators of the stressor scales. For practical purposes a

reasonable solution is the use of an equal weighting scheme for the items, because no information is

available about more appropriate weighting (McDonald, 1996).

One could argue that the same reasoning would also apply to some of the strain measures,

for example, for psychosomatic complaints. Although there are pros and cons for both views, we

preferred the effect indicator model because we did not want to measure psychosomatic symptoms

per se, but the underlying strain which manifests itself in various forms of bodily discomfort. We

acknowledge that for each single complaint there are manifold causes, but one common cause is the

strain level of the person.

The measurement models were also used to test the Strain Stability Model (cf. Table 4). As

described in Table 4, mean stability required that the latent means of the strain variables were

restricted to be equal in the measurement models. This can be tested with a difference chi-square

test, because this is a nested model (Bollen, 1989). The stressors were not modeled as latent

variables, and hence we used paired t-tests to test for the equality of means. The stability of the

individual differences model is correct, if the correlations of the latent constructs are very high and

are equally high across two adjacent time points and 5 time points (for example, T1-T2 stability

should be similarly as high as the T1-T6 stability); (again, for the strain variables we used the latent

constructs, for the stressors variables, the observed constructs).

Structural models

To test the structural models, it was necessary to fit separate models for each combination of

a strain and a stressor variable. Both restrictions of the software and the limited sample size

Page 142: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

133

(Bentler & Chou, 1987) forced us to use this modeling strategy. Table 4 explains the relationship

between the theoretical models and the statistical tests. As previously described, the Strain Stability

Model was tested by imposing constraints on the measurement models.

The Interindividual Differences model was tested with the spurious model. This is a one-

factor model for all the stressor and strain variables. This factor represents a stable construct that

explains all the covariation between all the stressor and the strain variables (see Figure 22).

Figure 22. Spurious Model: one perfectly stable construct explains all the covariation between stressors and strain variables.

To test all the other models in Table 4, we needed to use growth curve models (compare the

Appendix for a short introduction to the growth curve modeling), although they are not typically

used in the stress literature. Briefly, growth curve models focus on intraindividual changes and

interindividual differences in change patterns. Therefore, they allow us to test (stressor) trend –

(strain) trend correlations (slope-slope correlations) and correlations between initial values

(intercepts) and change patterns (slopes). In addition, we also used one hybrid model (to be

explained later).

The next model in Table 4 – the Stressor-Strain Trend Model – was tested within a specific

growth curve model. This growth curve model can either be linear or nonlinear (depending upon

which one has the best fit). If growth cannot be considered as linear over time, some of the slope

factor loadings can be estimated (more on this in the Appendix). This can be done for both the

stressors and the strain variables. A convenient strategy (Meredith & Tisak, 1990; McArdle, 1988)

T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6

T6T5T4T3T2T1

Stressor Variables

Strain Variables

Page 143: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

134

is to fix the slope factor loading for the first measurement wave to the value 0 and for the second

measurement wave to the value of 1. The other factor loadings are then estimated. From these

growth models, we derived the slope-slope correlations. Figure 23 explains this. There are two

factors for both strains and stressors: Slope and Intercept. The slope factor S is a latent construct

that represents the slope coefficients for each individual (as deviations from the mean slope). A

high factor score for S means that the slope for that individual deviates strongly from the mean

slope. If the mean slope is zero (no mean changes over time), a high positive (negative) factor score

implies a strong positive (negative) change for that person and a low positive (negative) value

means that there is little positive (negative) change. Thus, S tells us something about the

interindividual differences in change processes (more on this in the Appendix). The correlation

between the two slopes of stressors and strain tells us something about how individual differences

in the stressor trajectories are related to individual differences in strain trajectories. It is convenient

to fix the slope factor loading for the first measurement occasion to zero. In that case, the intercept

factor can be interpreted as representing the expected values for the first measurement wave (initial

status; see Appendix). A high positive (negative) factor score on the intercept factor (denoted as I in

Figure 2) means that the growth curve starts at a higher (lower) initial value than the average growth

curve. A positive intercept-intercept correlation means that people with higher (lower) initial values

on one variable also tend to have higher (lower) initial values on the other variable.

The fourth model in Table 4 – Reverse Causation Model – was be tested by looking at how

strongly earlier levels of strains were predictive of later developmental trajectories of stressors. We

fixed the slope factor loading for the first measurement wave to the value 0 (cf. Figure 23) and now

we could then interpret the intercept factor score as the expected initial value (T1) for a particular

subject. The Reverse Causation Model was tested by the intercept-slope correlation of strain and

stressors within the linear or nonlinear growth curve model (depending upon which one had the best

fit).

The Sleeper Effect Model was tested with the same statistical procedure as the Reverse

Causation Model (cf. Figure 23); however, this time we looked for the relationship between earlier

levels of stressor on later strain developments. Technically, this means that the stressor intercept

factor is correlated with the strain slope factor.

The final model in Table 4 – the Short-Term Reaction Model – was be tested with a hybrid

Page 144: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

135

Figure 23. (Non)linear grow

th curve model of stressors and strain w

ith a measurem

ent model included for the strain variables.

Note: I = intercept; S

= slope; not shown autocorrelations betw

een unique factors of strain items.

1

11

11

1

IS

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

T6

IS

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

T6

11

1

1

11

11

11

11

Stressors

Strains

‘slope stressor-slope strain’correlation tested inStressor-S

train Trend Model

‘intercept strain-slope stressor’correlation tested inR

everse-Causal M

odel

‘intercept stressor-slope strain’correlation tested inSleeper E

ffect Model

Page 145: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

136

model that is a combination of a latent growth curve model and an autoregressive model10. (The

autoregressive model assumes that the immediately preceding variable has a path on the next

without regard to time. This means that stressors at the first measurement point predict the stressors

at the next measure). This combination is more appropriate for the short-term reaction effect

because it allows synchronous effects from stressors on strain (unlike a combination of two growth

curve models). Figure 24 describes this model. The introduction of time-varying covariates will

change the interpretation of the growth curve itself: the growth curve describes the adjusted values

after the influence of the covariates (the stressors) is taken into account. Or, equivalently, the

stressor explains the state variance in the strain, because the trendlike changes are already partialled

out. In the hybrid model, the covariates are unrelated to the intercept and slope factor and the time-

specific residual (cf. Figure 24). Thus, the variance of each strain variable can be partitioned into

three nonoverlapping components: explained variance by the growth curve, the covariate, and a

time-specific residual.

All of the described models were tested against a maximal model that did not place any

constraints on the structural relations of the variables; this is called the correlated model, and is

used as a baseline model.

To evaluate all the models, covariance matrices and means were estimated with the

computer program NORMS (Schafer, 1997), and these matrices and mean vectors were used as

input for LISREL (Version 8.14, Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993). The NORMS program is specifically

designed for handling missing data problems. We used the EM algorithm of NORMS. The EM

algorithm (Dempster, Laird & Rubin, 1977) is a general technique for finding maximum-likelihood

estimates for models with partial missingness. It is based upon the assumption that data are

"missing at random" (MAR), which is a much milder assumption than the assumption that

"missingness occurs completely at random" (MCAR). MAR only requires that the missing values

behave like a random sample of all values within subclasses defined by observed data (Schafer,

1997, p. 11). The sample size used for a particular LISREL analysis was calculated by the mean of

the different sample sizes of the input matrix (N = 448 for depression; N = 445 for psychosomatic

complaints; N = 447 for irritation; N = 447 for worrying).

10 Actually, we also did an extensive comparison of the autoregressive and the growth curve model for all of the models

described in this study. For space constraints this part was left out.

Page 146: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

137

Figure 24. Hybrid m

odel: combination of (non)linear grow

th curve model (above) for the strain variables

(measurem

ent model included) and a first order autoregressive m

odel (below) for the stressor variables.

Note: I = intercept; S

= slope; not shown autocorrelations betw

een unique factors of strain items.

1

11

11

1

IS

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

T6

T6

T5

T4

T3

T2

T1

Strains

Stressors

regression coefficientstested inShort-T

erm R

eactionM

odel

Page 147: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

138

To evaluate the overall fit of the models, we report the chi-square statistic, the Akaike

Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987) and the comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, 1990). One

disadvantage with the chi-square statistic in comparative model fitting is that it always decreases

when parameters are added to the model. Therefore, we also report the AIC index, because it takes

parsimony (in the sense of as few parameters as possible) as well as fit into account (Jöreskog &

Sörbom, 1993). However, if two models were nested, we report the chi-square difference test

(Bollen, 1989). The CFI is based on a comparison of the fit of the hypothesized model to the fit of

the null baseline model and most researchers consider values greater than .90 as an indication for a

good fit, although recent research suggests a cutoff value close to .95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

To evaluate effect sizes, we report the parameters of interest, the standard errors, and the z

values. Unfortunately, in the multivariate nonlinear latent growth curve models (with freely

estimated factor loadings), we could not test the significance of several growth curve parameter

estimates. The reason is that the z values for those estimates are not invariant under different

fixation schemes (more about this in the Appendix E).

Results

Descriptive Data

In Table 6 to 11 the means, standard deviations, and cross-sectional intercorrelations of the

summated scores of the stressor and strain scales for each measurement wave are presented

separately.

In Table 12, the zero-order intercorrelations of the strains and stressors scale scores for all

time periods are shown. Many of the correlations between stressors and strains are small to

moderate in size.

Page 148: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

139

Table 6

Means, Standard D

eviations, and Intercorrelations at T1

Subscale M

S

D

1 2

3 4

5 6

7 8

9 S

tressors

1. Job insecurity 2.80

.73

2. T

ime pressure

3.10 .86

.07

3. Organizational constraints

2.80 .77

-.04 .11

4. Social stressors 2.05

.65 .16*

.22** .27**

5. U

ncertainty 2.42

.77 .00

.36** .40**

.41**

S

trains

6. Depression

2.70 .90

.31** .01

.15* .26**

.15*

7. Psychosomatic com

plaints 2.03

.84 .17*

.24** .11

.23** .12

.35**

8. Irritation

3.22 1.29

.20** .15*

.10 .34**

.23** .47**

.37**

9. Worrying

3.29 1.48

.16* .28**

.00 .22**

.18* .29**

.32** .51**

Note. N

= 179 (listw

ise deletion); * p < .05. ** p <

.01. T

able 7 M

eans, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations at T

2 Subscale

M

SD

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

8 9

Stressors

1. Job insecurity 3.06

.78

2. T

ime pressure

3.28 .77

-.08

3. Organizational constraints

2.34 .73

.03 .09

4. Social stressors 2.00

.70 .10*

.12* .32**

5. U

ncertainty 2.27

.68 -.02

.29** .36**

.40**

S

trains

6. Depression

2.73 .90

.17** -.09

.15** .21**

.11*

7. Psychosomatic com

plaints 2.10

.77 .09*

.10* .08

.12** .17**

.46**

8. Irritation

3.19 1.18

.10* .03

.09 .20**

.16** .48**

.49**

9. Worrying

3.56 1.50

.13** .25**

.00 .08

.10* .22**

.26** .40**

Note. N

= 440 (listw

ise deletion); * p < .05. ** p <

.01.

Page 149: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

140

Table 8

Means, Standard D

eviations, and Intercorrelations at T3

Subscale M

S

D

1 2

3 4

5 6

7 8

9 Stressors

1. Job insecurity

2.77 .72

2. Tim

e pressure 3.36

.74 -.09

3. O

rganizational constraints 2.03

.69 .14**

-.05

4. Social stressors

1.98 .69

.08 .04

.29**

5. Uncertainty

2.26 .67

-.04 .17**

.26** .49**

Strains

6. D

epression 2.68

.93 .22**

-.05 .15**

.27** .11*

7. Psychosom

atic complaints

2.15 .78

.12* .13*

.11* .09

.10 .40**

8. Irritation 3.28

1.12 .11*

.08 .16**

.25** .23**

.46** .49**

9. W

orrying 3.77

1.41 .15**

.27** .01

.07 .09

.24** .26**

.41**

N

ote. N =

362 (listwise deletion); * p <

.05. ** p < .01.

Table 9

Means, Standard D

eviations, and Intercorrelations at T4

Subscale

M

SD

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

8 9

Stressors

1. Job insecurity 2.71

.73

2. T

ime pressure

3.47 .75

-.16**

3. Organizational constraints

1.92 .67

.18** -.02

4. Social stressors 1.97

.72 .19**

.10 .37**

5. U

ncertainty 2.19

.64 -.02

.13* .34**

.43**

S

trains

6. Depression

2.67 .98

.30** -.15**

.23** .30**

.14*

7. Psychosomatic com

plaints 2.21

.80 .24**

.09 .10

.13* .06

.36**

8. Irritation

3.23 1.16

.21** .05

.22** .24**

.12* .50**

.48**

9. Worrying

3.84 1.46

.13* .31**

.01 .12*

.06 .21**

.29** .41**

Note. N

= 332 (listw

ise deletion); * p < .05. ** p <

.01.

Page 150: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

141

Table 10

Means, Standard D

eviations, and Intercorrelations at T5

Subscale

M

SD

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

8 9

Stressors

1. Job insecurity 2.71

.67

2. T

ime pressure

3.49 .69

-.05

3. Organizational constraints

1.81 .66

.15** -.04

4. Social stressors 2.00

.70 .22**

.14* .36**

5. U

ncertainty 2.18

.62 .07

.23** .40**

.39**

S

trains

6. Depression

2.60 .94

.22** -.07

.26** .34**

.23**

7. Psychosomatic com

plaints 2.23

.80 .17**

.13* .09

.15** .13*

.42**

8. Irritation

3.17 1.12

.15** .07

.19** .27**

.13* .52**

.48**

9. Worrying

3.87 1.44

.08 .25**

.07 .15*

.10 .23**

.32** .41**

Note. N

= 304 (listw

ise deletion); * p < .05. ** p <

.01. T

able 11 M

eans, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations at T

6 S

ubscale M

S

D

1 2

3 4

5 6

7 8

9 Stressors

1. Job insecurity

2.74 .67

2. Tim

e pressure 3.50

.68 -.02

3. O

rganizational constraints 1.77

.62 .11*

.09

4. Social stressors

2.02 .72

.16** .19**

.33**

5. Uncertainty

2.21 .64

.05 .33**

.34** .51**

Strains

6. D

epression 2.59

.92 .25**

-.03 .24**

.29** .25**

7. Psychosom

atic complaints

2.19 .77

.14* .11

.08 .20**

.15** .42**

8. Irritation 3.16

1.09 .10

.11* .25**

.27** .19**

.42** .36**

9. W

orrying 3.86

1.41 .12*

.24** .07

.20** .19**

.28** .28**

.46**

N

ote. N =

316 (listwise deletion); * p <

.05. ** p < .01.

Page 151: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

142

Table 12

Correlation M

atrix of Strain with Stressor V

ariables

D

epression

Psychosom

atic complaints

Irritation

Worrying

Stressors

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

T6

T

1 T

2 T

3 T

4 T

5 T

6

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

T6

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

T6

Job insecurity T

1 .22*

.15* .16* .25* .26*

.21* .11* .10

.11 .11

.11 .05

.17* .08

.10 .07

.12 .09

.13* .12*

.15* .10 .08

.11

T2

.22* .19* .19*

.22* .22* .17*

.16* .12* .13* .18*

.14* .08

.14* .11* .17* .13*

.14* .08

.14* .11* .15* .10

.08 .09

T

3 .20*

.21* .24* .30* .29*

.22* .16* .16*

.14* .18* .11* .08

.17* .09

.12* .14* .14* .04

.09

.14* .13* .08

.03 .06

T

4 .24*

.17* .15* .28* .23*

.17* .15* .15*

.13* .22* .18* .13*

.13* .12* .14* .17*

.14* .08

.09 .10*

.07 .09

.04 .09

T

5 .29*

.16* .16* .25* .24*

.19* .19* .13*

.15* .21* .17* .18*

.19* .06 .15* .18*

.15* .12*

.13* .07 .07

.07 .04

.08

T6

.23* .19* .22*

.26* .25* .28*

.14* .11* .13* .18*

.11* .16* .12* .03

.15* .17* .12* .13*

.19* .04

.08 .08

.02 .07

Tim

e pressure T

1 .00

-.01 -.08

-.12* -.10 -.07

.15* .08

.10 .03

.09 -.08

.15* .08

.05 .07

.08 .08

.31* .27*

.20* .23* .26* .14*

T

2 -.02

-.10* -.07 -.09

-.04 .00

.15* .10*

.14* .14* .16* .09

.12* .06

.02 .07

.09 .02

.31* .28*

.27* .34* .24* .18*

T

3 -.02

-.11* -.09 -.09

-.04 -.06

.10

.06 .10* .12*

.11* .09

.10* .05 .07

.06 .08

.05

.27* .26* .28* .31*

.24* .15*

T4

-.07 -.14* -.10* -.13* -.09

-.07

.07 .07

.06 .08

.09 .04

.04

.02 .02

.08 .05

.03

.20* .18* .19* .31*

.23* .18*

T5

-.06 -.14* -.12* -.15* -.08

-.12* .08

.04 .06

.08 .10

.06

.02 -.01

-.05 .01

.05 .00

.17* .17*

.14* .24* .25* .20*

T

6 -.07

-.15* -.13* -.15* -.15* -.05

.06 -.02

.02 .04

.06 .07

.01

-.04 -.03

.01 .01

.10

.12* .13* .10* .19*

.14* .27* O

rganizational T

1 .17*

.11 .18*

.05 .20*

.18*

.11 .00

.05 -.06

-.04 -.08

.09

.10 .09

.08 .11

.18*

.02 .01

.10 -.04

.05 .04

problems

T2

.13 .15* .15*

.15* .20* .19*

.13

.08 .11* .10

.05 .04

.11

.09* .10

.15* .11* .13*

.04

.00 .00

.00 .09

.04

T3

.08 .11* .18*

.18* .20* .19*

.06

.08 .13* .08

.11* .06

.12 .11*

.18* .19* .22* .18*

.11

-.02 .03

.00 .07

.02

T4

.08 .11* .19*

.23* .22* .22*

.08

.08 .08

.08 .07

.06

.10 .08

.17* .21* .19* .17*

-.03 -.05

.01 .01

.05 .03

T

5 .06

.10* .16* .17* .25*

.25*

.11 .06

.09 .05

.09 .07

.09

.12* .15* .15*

.18* .22* -.08

-.07 -.08

-.06 .04

.01

T6

.04 .07

.14* .15* .19*

.26*

.08 .01

.10 .09

.03 .08

.14

.07 .14* .17*

.14* .25*

.07 .04

.09 .03

.06 .06

Social stressors T

1 .21*

.12* .17* .19* .20*

.07

.21* .03 .06

-.02 .09

.07

.27* .16* .19* .20*

.14* .21*

.17* .10 .09

.14* .05

.14*

T2

.20* .19* .18*

.16* .18* .12*

.18* .10*

.09 .05

.07 .08

.23* .21*

.19* .11* .09

.11*

.14* .07 .06

.05 .05

.04

T3

.20* .16* .25*

.22* .20* .12*

.18* .05

.10* .06 .08

.08

.24* .17* .24* .19*

.16* .20*

.09 -.03

.06 .07

.04 .05

T

4 .19*

.11* .25* .29* .21*

.19*

.11 .10*

.11* .11* .13* .08

.16* .12*

.20* .24* .17* .17*

.03

.02 .05

.11* .05

.07

T5

.23* .16* .27*

.25* .30* .23*

.15* .06

.14* .11 .15* .13*

.18* .14*

.22* .24* .27* .24*

.14* .02

.08 .08

.14* .12*

T6

.25* .15* .24*

.25* .18* .29*

.18* .05

.10* .12* .13* .22*

.23* .15*

.24* .26* .17* .29*

.14* .10

.15* .16* .11

.20* U

ncertainty T

1 .11*

.04 .08

.07 .17*

.14*

.11* .11* .08

.03 .10

.08

.21* .20* .16* .17*

.19* .18*

.23* .11* .13* .08

.14* .10

T2

.15* .08

.09 .09

.14* .15*

.10

.15* .12* .09

.11* .13*

.18* .16* .12* .05

.06 .10

.21* .10*

.05 -.01

.01 .06

T

3 .12*

.04 .10*

.11* .17* .16*

.09

.11* .11* .09

.10 .11*

.19* .14*

.23* .16* .13* .20*

.12* .05

.10* .02 .06

.14*

T4

.12* .01

.13* .13* .17*

.21*

.08 .10*

.09 .07

.04 .10

.16* .06

.12* .15* .10

.13*

.12 .02

.02 .09

.11* .15*

T5

.10 .05

.15* .07

.20* .19*

.10

.08 .17* .07

.10 .12*

.08

.08 .19* .07

.12* .13*

.14* .02 .09

.03 .09

.11*

T6

.12* .07

.09 .12* .16*

.24*

.14* .03 .14* .07

.10 .15*

.18* .13*

.18* .16* .12* .19*

.18* .07

.13* .03

.09 .19*

Note. * p <

.05. N varies because of m

issing data (range N: 159-526, m

ean N =

345).

Page 152: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

143

Strain Measurement Models

The goodness-of-fit measures of the measurement models are shown in Table 13. The Null -

Model assumes complete independence between the items. The unconstrained model (Model 1) is a

longitudinal measurement model that is unconstrained, allowing different factor loadings and item

intercepts over time. Model 2 is a restricted model, with equal factor loadings over time. Model 3 is

more restrictive and additionally assumes equal item intercepts over time. Model 4 will be

discussed later and tests for equal means of the latent constructs over time. Measurement invariance

across time is one prerequisite interpreting the constructs to be comparable over time. Measurement

invariance can be assumed to exist if equal factor loadings and item intercepts do not lead to a

significantly worse fit of the model. Only a few parameters needed to be freed for depression and

psychosomatic complaints. Although, after these modifications, the results of the chi-square

difference tests of irritation and psychosomatic complaints remained significant, further freeing of

parameters led to estimates which were only trivially different from the restricted ones.

Thus, for irritation and worrying we found full measurement invariance, and for depression

and psychosomatic complaints we found partial measurement invariance. This is not a problem

because partial measurement invariance is sufficient (Byrne et al., 1989; Muthén & Muthén, 1998;

Pentz & Chou, 1994). We note that for the modified constrained measurement models all values of

the CFI were above .94, which can be considered a good fit.

Structural Models

The goodness-of-fit measures of our model tests are shown in Table 14. The first model

(“correlated”) is an unconstrained structural model that can be used as a baseline for the growth

curve models. The second model is the Interindividual Difference Model (“spurious model”) in

which the correlations of all the stressor and strain variables can be explained by one common,

unmeasured factor, which is assumed to be perfectly stable over time.

The next two models are latent growth curve models (linear and nonlinear) to test for the

relevant parameters for the Stressor-Strain Trend Model, the Reverse Causation Model and Sleeper-

Effect Model. The last model is a hybrid model (to test for the Short-Term Reaction Model), which

combines a latent growth curve model for the strain variables and a first-order autoregressive model

for the stressor variables that act as synchronous covariates for the strain variables.

Page 153: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

144

Table 13

Goodness of Fit M

easures for Measurem

ent Models and M

ean Stability M

odel

D

epression P

sychosomatic com

plaints Irritation

Worrying

Model

χ2

df. A

IC

CF

I χ

2 df.

AIC

C

FI

χ2

df. A

IC

CF

I χ

2 df.

AIC

C

FI

Null 6 908.50

276 7079.00

16485.87 1128 16581.97

9656.21

435 9716.21

6053.92

153 6089.92

1. Unconstrained

298.75 177

544.75 .982

1369.00 699

1777.00 .944

628.64 315

928.64 .966

125.07 75

317.07 .992

2. Equal factor loadings 311.00

190 531.00

.982 1409.87

729 1757.87

.943 652.30

335 912.30

.966 139.78

85 311.78

.991

Difference of 2 and 1

30.28* 17

40.87

30

23.66

20

14.71

10

3. Equal loadings and

intercepts

383.07 203

625.07 .973

1469.29 758

1843.29 .941

686.55 355

966.55 .964

161.71 95

349.71 .989

Difference of 3 and 2

72.07* 13

59.42* 29

34.25 20

21.93 10

Mean stability

4. Equal latent m

eans 391.87

208 623.87

.972 1494.44

763 1858.44

.939 695.79

360 965.79

.964 178.33

100 356.33

.987

Difference of 4 and 3

8.80 5

25.15* 5

9.24

5

16.62* 5

Note.

* Chi-square difference test w

as significant (α =

0.01)

N =

448 for depression; N =

445 for psychosomatic com

plaints; N =

447 for irritation; N =

447 for worrying

Page 154: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

145

Table 14

Goodness of Fit M

easures for Structural Models

D

epression Psychosom

atic complaints

Irritation W

orrying

χ2

df A

IC

CF

I χ

2 df

AIC

C

FI

χ2

df A

IC

CF

I χ

2 df

AIC

C

FI

Job insecurity

correlated 669.54

315 1029.54

.959 1863.66

974 2363.66

.936 1007.06

499 1413.06

.955 305.68

167 619.68

.982 spurious

1413.06 354

1635.06 .876

3145.69 999

3499.69 .846

2352.33 533

2618.33 .837

1438.00 211

1616.00 .840

linear 1005.07

373 1249.07

.927 2238.52

1032 2622.52

.914 1333.55

557 1623.55

.931 648.08

225 846.08

.945 nonlinear

811.64 365

1071.64 .948

1992.74 1024

2392.74 .931

1134.91 549

1440.91 .948

431.82 217

645.82 .972

hybrid 765.11

363 1029.11

.953 1937.16

1022 2341.16

.935 1066.38

547 1376.38

.954 362.41

215 580.41

.981 T

ime pressure

correlated

666.55 315

1026.55 .958

1947.28 974

2447.28 .930

973.35 499

1379.35 .957

268.49 167

582.49 .987

spurious 1675.44

367 1871.44

.843 3258.62

999 3612.62

.837 2328.94

533 2594.94

.837 1351.46

211 1529.46

.851 linear

837.13 373

1081.13 .945

2136.97 1032

2520.97 .921

1154.15 557

1444.15 .946

472.38 225

670.38 .968

nonlinear 770.34

365 1030.34

.952 2052.54

1024 2452.54

.926 1059.39

549 1365.39

.954 367.75

217 581.75

.980 hybrid

753.00 363

1017.00 .954

2029.58 1022

2433.58 .927

1035.88 547

1345.88 .956

356.53 215

574.53 .982

Organizational problem

s

correlated 676.63

315 1036.63

.956 2052.97

974 2552.97

.921 987.52

499 1393.52

.955 322.68

167 636.68

.979 spurious

1441.52 367

1637.52 .868

3357.89 999

3711.89 .827

2239.29 533

2505.29 .841

1463.29 211

1641.29 .828

linear 1172.26

373 1416.26

.905 2604.77

1032 2988.77

.887 1461.40

557 1751.40

.917 835.17

225 1033.17

.919 nonlinear

796.22 365

1056.22 .947

2209.21 1024

2609.21 .913

1107.25 549

1413.25 .948

472.86 217

686.86 .965

hybrid 745.74

363 1009.74

.953 2179.29

1022 2583.29

.915 1085.47

547 1395.47

.950 438.21

215 656.21

.969 Social stressors

correlate 663.27

315 1023.27

.957 1819.21

974 2319.21

.937 927.77

499 1333.77

.960 321.97

167 635.97

.979 spurious

1600.02 367

1796.02 .847

3089.33 999

3443.33 .844

2081.75 533

2347.75 .854

1462.15 211

1640.15 .827

linear 838.80

373 1082.80

.943 1979.97

1032 2363.97

.929 1071.41

557 1361.41

.952 514.91

225 712.91

.960 nonlinear

748.51 365

1008.51 .953

1880.50 1024

2280.50 .936

982.49 549

1288.49 .959

409.80 217

623.80 .973

hybrid 795.27

363 1059.27

.947 1882.60

1022 2286.60

.936 1000.63

547 1310.63

.957 439.00

215 657.00

.969 U

ncertainty

correlate 648.18

315 1008.18

.958 1893.29

974 2393.29

.932 983.17

499 1389.17

.955 318.14

167 632.14

.979 spurious

1474.49 367

1670.49 .860

3102.29 999

3456.29 .843

2198.82 533

2464.82 .844

1405.80 211

1583.80 .835

linear 790.75

373 1034.75

.948 2051.73

1032 2435.73

.924 1137.93

557 1427.93

.946 506.00

225 704.00

.961 nonlinear

743.73 365

1003.73 .952

1980.67 1024

2380.67 .929

1082.57 549

1388.57 .950

437.47 217

651.47 .970

hybrid 742.52

363 1006.52

.952 1974.50

1022 2378.50

.929 1081.33

547 1391.33

.950 444.38

215 662.38

.968 N

OT

E. N

= 448 for depression; N

= 445 for psychosom

atic complaints; N

= 447 for irritation; N

= 447 for w

orrying correlated =

all constructs were allow

ed to correlate without further restrictions im

posed spurious =

one factor model for all stressor and strain variables

linear = linear grow

th model for stressor and strain variables

nonlinear = nonlinear grow

th model for stressor and strain variables

hybrid = nonlinear grow

th curve for strains with stressors as tim

e-varying covariates with a first order autoregressive structure

Page 155: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

146

The AIC values displayed in Table 14 show that for the combination of strains with the

stressors, job insecurity, time pressure, and organizational problems were better (i.e. lowest) for the

hybrid than for the latent growth curve models, although the differences were not very high. For

social stressors and uncertainty, the nonlinear latent growth curve model yielded the lowest AIC

values. There was only one exception: The combination of psychosomatic complaints and

uncertainty gave a slightly lower AIC value for the hybrid model, although the difference in AIC

with the nonlinear latent growth curve model was negligible. Again, we note that AIC value

differences between alternative models were sometimes quite small. In those cases, in which the

differences between the growth curve and the hybrid models were relatively small, we could

continue to test hypotheses with either of those models.

Testing of the Theoretical Models

We now describe how the results bear on the theoretical models that are described in Table

4.

The Strain Stability Model.

This model argues that there is no change over time for strains, despite changes in the

stressors. The answer to this model can be split into questions of mean and individual differences

stabilities.

Mean stability for strain variables. The strain means were rather stable, as one can see in

Table 15. However, there were statistical differences for psychosomatic complaints, (∆χ2 = 25.15,

∆df = 5, p < 0.001; N = 445) and worrying, (∆χ2 = 16.62, ∆ df = 5, p = 0.005; N = 447), but not for

irritation, (∆χ2 = 9.24, ∆ df = 5, p = 0.10; N = 445) and depression, (∆χ2 = 8.8, ∆ df = 5, p = 0.12; N

= 448). From a practical perspective the differences in means for psychosomatic complaints and

worrying were not high.

Mean stability for stressor variables. The stressor variables were measured by the

unweighted summated scores, and the hypothesis of the stability of the means were tested by series

of paired t-tests. The results are shown in Table 16. There were no significant differences for social

stressors only. For uncertainty, there was only a significant result for the first waves, but if we apply

a Bonferroni adjustment to correct for multiple testing, we can conclude that no significant

differences could be detected. The most drastic changes were shown in the consistent decrease of

organizational problems (see Table 16). It seems there was a leveling off, noting the consistent

Page 156: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

147

Table 15 Means of Latent Strains for all Measurement Waves

T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Depression 2.75

(0.053) 2.83

(0.054) 2.87

(0.052) 2.85

(0.053) 2.81

(0.052) 2.80

(0.054) Psychosomatic

complaints 1.57

(0.097) 1.62

(0.098) 1.67

(0.100) 1.70

(0.101) 1.67

(0.099) 1.69

(0.100) Irritation 3.09

(0.055) 3.11

(0.053) 3.14

(0.051) 3.11

(0.052) 3.04

(0.051) 3.05

(0.052) Worrying 3.70

(0.080) 3.74

(0.080) 3.92

(0.075) 3.93

(0.075) 3.90

(0.075) 3.92

(0.075) Note. Standard errors between parentheses Table 16 Means, Mean Differences and t-Tests for all Measurement Waves M ti M ti+1 M difference SE of M t df p

Job insecurity T1- T2 2.855 3.000 -.140 .039 -4.127 300 .000 T2- T3 3.018 2.746 .273 .032 8.626 380 .000 T3- T4 2.729 2.668 .061 .031 1.987 334 .048 T4- T5 2.644 2.653 -.008 .027 -.297 316 .766 T5- T6 2.635 2.684 -.049 .028 -1.778 305 .076

Time pressure T1-T2 3.215 3.225 -.010 .0304 -.343 335 .732 T2-T3 3.310 3.385 -.075 .0306 -2.451 394 .015 T3-T4 3.416 3.508 -.092 .0305 -3.021 332 .003 T4-T5 3.534 3.512 .022 .0327 .668 319 .505 T5-T6 3.526 3.545 -.019 .0312 -.608 307 .543

Organizational problems

T1-T2 2.792 2.480 .3121 .0480 6.492 169 .000 T2-T3 2.319 2.012 .3071 .0312 9.837 357 .000 T3-T4 2.016 1.889 .1275 .0328 3.887 297 .000 T4-T5 1.883 1.794 .0881 .0300 2.939 289 .004 T5-T6 1.806 1.750 .0561 .0301 1.868 285 .063

Social stressors T1-T2 1.935 1.925 .0104 .0318 .315 296 .753 T2-T3 2.000 1.986 .0141 .0328 .429 365 .668 T3-T4 1.979 1.971 .0075 .0369 .204 303 .839 T4-T5 1.945 1.969 -.0238 .0340 -.702 295 .483 T5-T6 1.972 2.022 -.0501 .0379 -1.319 277 .188

Uncertainty T1-T2 2.357 2.279 .0787 .0356 2.211 293 .028 T2-T3 2.254 2.248 .0564 .0301 .187 369 .852 T3-T4 2.263 2.223 .0406 .0373 1.087 301 .278 T4-T5 2.223 2.182 .0434 .0345 1.257 295 .210 T5-T6 2.197 2.222 -.0250 .0342 -.731 272 .466

Page 157: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

148

decrease in mean differences, although the difference between T5 and T6 was not significant any

longer. The means of time pressure increased after the second wave, but after the fourth wave

stopped to change significantly. The mean of job insecurity increased in T2, which took place 3

months after the start of the study. After this increase there was a downward trend.

Stability of individual differences. The correlations of the latent constructs in Table 17 show

that the hypothesis of stability of individual differences is to be rejected. If one compares the last

column of Table 17 with all the other columns, it becomes clear that the stabilities across the 6

waves (a five year period) were much lower than the stabilities across adjacent waves.

The stabilities of the stressors are also shown in Table 17. Again, a comparison of the one-

wave lag with the six-wave lagged stabilities revealed a low degree of stabilities for the stressor

variables. In line with our expectation, more changes in organizational problems were present in the

first half of our study: In the last three years the scores were more stable than in the first two years.

Changes in social stressors took place in the period from T2 to T4, and again it stabilized in the last

years.

Thus, these results imply that the mean stability for the stressors was not really low (see

Table 17), but rather was lower than the stability for the strains, and that there was little stability in

individual differences across a long time frame.

Table 17 Stability Coefficients of Strains and Stressors

T1-T2 T2-T3 T3-T4 T4-T5 T5-T6 T1-T6 Strains

Depression .82 .78 .74 .77 .73 .56 Psychosomatic

complaints .87 .85 .80 .83 .82 .67

Irritation .70 .72 .76 .70 .74 .59 Worrying .73 .74 .73 .75 .68 .50

Stressors Job insecurity .82 .75 .87 .94 .88 .44 Time pressure .97 .88 .86 .87 .90 .58

Organizational problems

.73 .85 83 .92 .95 .45

Social Stressors .95 .84 .78 .93 .89 .51 Uncertainty .88 .93 .82 .97 .93 .60

NOTE. All time lags are 1 year, except T1-T2 lag (4 months) and T5-T6 lag (2 years)

Page 158: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

149

Interindividual Differences Model

The Interindividual Differences Model was tested by the spurious model. In Table 14 the

goodness-of-fit measures for the spurious models are shown. In all cases the fit measures were poor.

The hypothesis that all the relationships between stressors and strains can fully be explained by one

stable construct has to be rejected.

Stressor-Strain Trend Model

The stressor-strain trend model was tested by the correlation of the stressor slope factor with

the strain slope factor in the nonlinear growth model (see again Figure 23). These correlations are

displayed in the first column of Table 18. If we concentrate on the combinations of social stressors

and uncertainty with all the strain variables, we notice that there were sizeable Slope Stressor-

Slope Strain correlations. Out of the eight correlations, 6 were higher than .20 and two were higher

than .30. This means that long-term changes in social stressors and uncertainty were accompanied

with corresponding changes in the strain variables. Remarkably, job insecurity slope factor had no

sizeable correlation with any of the strain variable slope factors. The slope factors of the other

stressors - time pressure and organizational problems - showed meaningful relationships with most

strain variables slopes.

Reverse Causation Model

This model was tested by the correlation of the Intercept Strain with Slope Stressor in the

nonlinear growth model (see again Figure 23). The correlations were by and large rather low (see

the second column in Table 15); however, almost all were negative, which is directly counter to the

drift model or the true strain hypothesis and in line with the Refuge Model or models with direct

positive effects due to successful problem-focused coping.

Sleeper Effect Model

This model was tested with the correlations of the intercept of the stressor with the slope of

strain in the nonlinear growth model (see, again, Figure 23), and the correlations are shown in the

third column of Table 15. There is little evidence for sleeper effects because nearly all correlations

were below .20, and instead of being positive, they were almost all negative.

Page 159: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

150

Table 18

Correlations between Intercept Factors and Slope Factors of Strains and Stressors

Slope Stressor

Slope Strain

Intercept Strain

Slope Stressor

Intercept Stressor

Slope Strain

Depression

Job insecurity 0.122 -0.007 0.058

Time pressure -0.126 0.051 -0.064

Organizational problems 0.267 -0.026 -0.101

Social stressors 0.179 -0.021 0.065

Uncertainty 0.250 -0.033 -0.067

Psychosomatic Complaints

Job insecurity 0.118 -0.066 0.075

Time pressure 0.247 -0.124 -0.083

Organizational problems 0.154 -0.247 -0.020

Social stressors 0.335 -0.265 -0.059

Uncertainty 0.219 -0.147 -0.069

Irritation

Job insecurity 0.115 -0.121 -0.033

Time pressure 0.143 -0.041 -0.125

Organizational problems 0.207 -0.005 -0.126

Social stressors 0.250 -0.133 -0.087

Uncertainty 0.206 -0.163 -0.161

Worrying

Job insecurity -0.086 -0.053 -0.058

Time pressure 0.343 -0.180 -0.293

Organizational problems 0.080 0.098 -0.107

Social stressors 0.158 -0.079 -0.058

Uncertainty 0.493 -0.194 -0.229

NOTE.

Estimates are taken from the nonlinear latent growth models with correlated residuals

Page 160: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

151

Short-Term Reaction Model

The Short-Term Reaction Model could be well modeled as a hybrid model and could be

tested by looking at the synchronous paths from stressors to strain variables. Table 19 shows the

standardized regression coefficients of the solution presented in Table 14; many of them were

significant. The strongest paths occurred for time pressure and worrying. Social stressors and

uncertainty were related to all strain variables with similar magnitude. Organizational problems

were related to the strain variables except to worrying.

Discussion

We tested several stressor-strain models. First, the Strain Stability Model has been shown to

be wrong for the stability of individual differences of strains, but there was a high degree of stability

of the means of strain variables. As predicted, meaningful differences in stressors could be detected

across the five-year period. Job insecurity peaked somewhat earlier than we predicted (at T2 and not

at T3), but we anticipated the leveling off: after T2 job insecurity decreased, to remain at a more or

less constant level. One has to keep in mind that job insecurity measured the fear of becoming

unemployed and should not be equated with the stressor of being unemployed itself. For a particular

wave, the people who had lost their jobs, the items of this scale were not included. It might be that

for some respondents with initial high scores on job insecurity their fears turned out to be realistic

and they indeed lost their jobs, which resulted in missing values for subsequent waves. Thus,

selection effects can partly explain the changes in the means of job insecurity. The means of time

pressure increased after T2 as expected, because Western production norms soon pervaded job

requirements and set the pace at higher standards. The monotonic decrease of the means of

organizational problems was also in line with our expectations. Although any transitional period

will create its own organizational troubles, apparently the new work systems run more smoothly,

and in the first year a decrease in organizational problems can already be detected.

The stability of the means for social stressors was unexpected, because we had originally

thought that social cohesion at the workplace would be reduced and competition would increase.

The stressor uncertainty showed a small decrease. This was in line with our expectations, because

more efficient organizations describe work requirements unambiguously, and this reduces role

conflicts and uncertainty.

Page 161: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

152

Table 19

Regression C

oefficients for the Regression of the Strain V

ariables on the Stressors in the Hybrid M

odels

D

epression Psychosom

atic complaints

Irritation

Worrying

T

1 T

2 T

3 T

4 T

5 T

6 T

1 T

2 T

3 T

4 T

5 T

6 T

1 T

2 T

3 T

4 T

5 T

6 T

1 T

2 T

3 T

4 T

5 T

6

Job insecurity .14* .15* .18* .19*

.21* .21* .04

.08* .12* .14* .17* .15* .06 .08* .09* .09* .09* .09* .04

.06 .05

.01 -.01

-.01

Tim

e pressure -.01

.01 -.02

-.05 -.06

-.07 .11* .11* .10* .09* .06

.07* .12* .12* .13* .11* .08 .09* .37* .35* .37* .33*

.29* .29*

Organizational probl.

.17* .20* .22* .25* .24* .23*

.10* .12* .15* .16* .14* .14* .10* .13* .15* .17* .17* .15* .04 .05

.07 .05

.03 .04

Social stressors .17* .20* .23* .28*

.30* .31* .16* .19* .22* .24* .24* .25* .18* .24* .25* .26* .30* .30* .13* .13* .17* .18*

.17* .18*

Uncertainty

.15* .15* .15* .16* .18* .19*

.12* .14* .16* .15* .16* .16* .21* .20* .21* .17* .16* .18* .25* .23* .25* .22* .23*

.24*

NO

TE

. * z > 1.96 (based on unstandardized solution).

Regression coefficients taken from

LISR

EL

’s completely standardized solution.

N =

448 for depression; N =

445 for psychosomatic com

plaints; N =

447 for irritation; N =

447 for worrying.

Page 162: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

153

The mean changes in stressors suggest that East Germany gradually moved in the direction

of a Western economy. There were higher work requirements, as indicated by more time pressure,

and smoother and more efficient organization and task design, as reflected by lower organizational

problems and lower levels of uncertainty. However, there were no signs of higher costs in the sense

of higher levels of strains.

The means of the strain variables remained almost stable, but there was no stability of

interindividual differences. This means that there were considerable changes in the relative

positions of people (as indicated by moderate stability coefficients). Thus, people changed in

different ways, with some people improving and some deteriorating (winners and losers of German

unification).

The fact that there were mean changes in stressors but not in strain should not be interpreted

to mean that there were no causal effects of stressors on strain. Because some stressors increased

over time (e.g., time pressure) and others decreased (e.g., organizational problems), the net effect on

strain may be the same.

Second, the Interindividual Differences Model could be clearly rejected. There was no stable

factor, be it negative affectivity or some other nonmeasured factor, that could explain all common

variance between stressors and strains. Because we only tested for a complete Interindividual

Differences model, there may still be some partial impact (e.g., of negative affectivity), that was not

captured in this model (Spector et al, in press).

Third, the Stressor – Strain Trend Model was supported by half of the possible combinations

of stressors and strains (see Table 18, first column): Uncertainty was related to all the strains

(depression, psychosomatic complaints, irritation, and worrying). Uncertainty seems to be one of

the most consistent and important stressors; this replicates other reports on the importance of role

ambiguity and conflict (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992).

Social stressors showed slope-slope correlations above .20 with psychosomatic complaints

and irritation. Time pressure was related to psychosomatic complaints and to worrying, and

organizational problems were related to irritation and depression. Interestingly, job insecurity was

not related to any of the strains within the constraints of this model.

Some of the slope-slope correlations were quite sizeable, such as social stressors with

psychosomatic complaints (.34) and time pressure (.34) and uncertainty (.49) with worrying. More

specifically, one can see a fit in the content of stressor and strain relationships. Worrying refers to

worrying about work after working time (mood spillover); thus, there is a delayed effect of time

Page 163: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

154

pressure and uncertainty. One potential mechanism is that, with time pressure, a person does not

have time to worry about things during working hours and, therefore, does it outside of work.

Uncertainty was most highly related to worrying. Uncertainty leads to confusion and internal

conflict that takes a long time to be resolved and, therefore, carries over into nonwork time.

It is important to note that these correlations cannot be interpreted to be due to some stable

third variable (such as negative affectivity), because only the change part of stressors and strains is

related in the slope-slope correlation and the constant part is statistically held constant. From this

perspective, the size of the correlations is quite high.

An alternative explanation for stressor – strain relations is an overlap in the item content of

stressor and strain scales (cf. ‘the triviality trap’; Kasl, 1978). But an inspection of the items of

these scales led to the conclusion that this was not the case.

Fourth, the models that assume reverse causation were tested as latent growth curve and not

as hybrid models. The hybrid model, the best-fitting model for most stressor-strain combinations,

did not allow to test for lagged effects of the intercept parameter of the strain growth curve on the

stressor covariates. The Drift Model was not supported. Reverse Causation Models, which

hypothesized reduced stressors as a result of prior strain levels, were in line with the results. Both

positive selection mechanisms as well as positive direct effects can explain this result. Thus the

Refuge Model was supported. Since there was a radical change situation, many job movements

could occur within a short time. Therefore, people with high strain found jobs with less stressors,

and people with low strain found jobs with more challenges. Thus, a person with a high degree of

psychosomatic complaints attempted to find a job with less social and organizational stressors.

However, the effects were quite small and should not be overinterpreted. Additionally, several

Reverse Causation mechanisms might be valid only for subgroups, and this contributes to only

small correlations.

Fifth, the Sleeper-Effect Model was not supported. A methodological problem in detecting

lagged effects is that the presumed causal agents are constantly changing as well. Determining the

exact time length of the lagged effects is an unresolved methodological problem in longitudinal

research, especially if both short-term and long-term effects are present.

Sixth, the Short-Term Reaction Model is well supported by the results of the hybrid models.

In nearly every case, there were significant relationships between stressors and strains (the only real

exceptions being relationships of time pressure with depression and organizational problems and

job insecurity with worrying). For some stressors, the effects were quite high and suggest a

Page 164: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

155

specificity effect. For instance, the strongest synchronous effects were detected for the relationship

of the stressor time pressure with worrying (with correlations around .33), but time pressure was

unrelated to depression. Time pressure does not depress people, but makes them active at work.

However, they worry about the job after working hours. General effects on strains were noticeable

for social stressors, and uncertainty. Note that the latent growth curve was partialled from the strain

variables. Thus, the results of Table 19 really present the immediate strain reactions to the stressors,

holding the slow moving trait change (the overall trend) for each individual constant.

The overall results can be interpreted in this way: There are two effects side by side. One is

the overall relationships between individual trends in stressors and strains (Stressor-Strain Trend

Model). In a way, this reflects the overall long-term effect of stressors on the slowly changing

component of strain (this has been called trait component by Nesselroade, 1991). The other effect is

the Short-Term Reaction effect, which means that there is a direct and immediate effect of stressors

on strains (synchronous). This is unrelated to the general trend and, therefore, is to be interpreted as

a clear state effect. This means that both components of strain - in Nesselroade’s terminology, state

and trait - are affected by the stressors.

As with any study, our research also has some limitations. One relates to the issue of

causality. Although we used a longitudinal study, the Stressor-Strains Trend Model cannot be

convincingly interpreted causally. One prerequisite for interpreting something as causal is the time

order effect. However, for example, the slope-slope correlations give up the time order because they

look at the general trends of stressors and strain over the full time range. Thus, these correlations

can also be the result of a causal effect of strain on stressors or a third variable explaining the

variance in both slope factors. The causal argument can be maintained more strongly for the hybrid

model that we used to test the Short-Term Reaction Model. Here the intercept and the slope factor

of the dependent variable strain were partialled out which means that there is some indication for a

causal influence of the stressor on strain even though the effect was synchronous.

A second limitation is that we could not discriminate between subgroups for which

differential models may hold (Frese & Zapf, 1988). Although this is true of most studies in the

field, it is potentially possible to use growth curve models for multiple groups. However, both

sample size limitations and software restrictions forbade using this procedure in this study.

Promising software developments have been announced, making it possible to integrate latent class

analysis and structural equation modeling (Muthén, in press).

Page 165: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

156

The strengths of this study should not be overlooked. There are very few stressor-strain

studies with more than three waves in the literature (Zapf et al., 1996). There is no doubt that this is

a unique study in this regard. Another feature is that it took place in a unique historical period.

From one perspective this may be a limitation, because it may imply that one cannot generalize the

results. But from another perspective it means that one can model complex relationships in a radical

change situation more easily because more changes happen overall, therefore speeding up the

processes. Thus, similar to the laboratory situation, the “manipulation” is strong and compressed in

time (Moeller & Strauss, 1997).

Another design feature is that we used multiple measures of stressors and strains. This was

particularly important for the description of the mean changes of stressors in East Germany, because

we could show that there was a characteristic picture of some stressors increasing during the time of

the study, some stressors decreasing, and one not changing at all.

Another strength relates to our use of the growth curve models. There are two advantages.

We could look at the long-term changes from an overall trend point of view (trait perspective).

Moreover, it was possible to differentiate the trait and the state perspective on strain, because we

could look at the immediate effects of stressors on strain and at the long-term trends of the

relationships between stressors and strains. We found that there were stressor – strain relationships

appearing for different time frames side by side. This would have gone undetected with alternative

approaches (e.g., with an autoregressive model approach).

Another advantage of the growth curve analysis is that some of the relationships are much

stronger than the relationships shown by the zero-order relationships of the stressor and strain

variables (although even these relationships were already disattenuated, because the strain variables

were latent; cf. Table 12).

One important contribution of this article is its analysis strategy. To our knowledge, both

factor models within a growth curve approach and hybrid models are infrequently or never used in

the literature. The use of the factor models made it possible to test for measurement equivalence

over time, to ensure that the meaning of the latent constructs remained the same. The advantage of

the hybrid model was that we could adjust the growth curves for nontrendlike influences.

Introducing time-specific determinants into the model makes latent growth curve modeling a more

flexible strategy and partly compensates for the lack of stochastical variation which presumably is a

part of many psychological developmental processes (cf. Bock, 1991 p. 127).

Page 166: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

157

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317-332.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Anderson, J.C., & Gerbing, D.W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and

recommended two step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 411-423.

Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107,

238-246.

Bentler, P.M., & Chou, C. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociological Methods and

Research, 16, 78-117.

Blalock, H.M., Jr. (1967). Causal inferences in nonexperimental research. Chapel Hill, North

Carolina: University of North Carolina Press

Bock, R.D. (1991). Prediction of growth. In L. M. Collins & J.L. Horn (Eds.), Best methods for the

analysis of change: recent advances, unanswered questions, future directions (pp. 126 -136).

Washington, DC: American Psychological.Association.

Bollen, K.A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.

Bollen, K., & Lennox, R. (1991). Conventional wisdom on measurement: A structural equation

approach. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 305-314.

Brief, A.P., Burke, M.J., George, J.M., Robinson, B., & Webster, J. (1988). Should negative

affectivity remain an unmeasured variable in the study of job stress? Journal of Applied

Psychology, 73, 193-198.

Burke, M.J., Brief, A.P., & George, J.M. (1993). The role of negative affectivity in understanding

relations between self-reports of stressors and strains: A comment on the applied psychology

literature. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 402-412.

Byrne, B.M., Shavelson, R.J., & Muthén, B. (1989). Testing for the equivalence of factor

covariance and mean structures: The issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychological

Bulletin, 105, 456-466.

Caplan, R. D., Cobb, S., French, J. R. P. Jr., van Harrison, R., & Pinneau, S. R., Jr. (1975). Job

demands and worker health. Washington DC: National Institute of Occupational Safety and

Health.

Page 167: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

158

Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Teresi, J., Marchi, M., & Velez, C.N. (1990). Problems in the measurement of

latent variables in structural equations causal models. Applied Psychological Measurement, 14,

183-196.

Curran, P.J. (in press). A latent curve framework for the study of developmental trajectories in

adolescent substance use. In J. Rose, L. Chassin, C. Presson, & J. Sherman (Eds.), Multivariate

applications in substance use research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Dormann, C., Zapf, D. (1999). Social support, social stressors at work and depressive symptoms:

testing for moderating effects with structural equations in a 3-wave longitudinal study. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 84, 874-884.

Dempster, A.P., Laird, N.M., & Rubin, D.B.(1977). Maximum likelihood estimation from

incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 39, 1-38.

Fahrenberg, J. (1975). Die Freiburger Beschwerdeliste (FBL) [The Freiburg Complaints List].

Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, 4, 79-100.

Frese, M. (1985). Stress at work and psychosomatic complaints: A causal interpretation. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 70, 314-328.

Frese, M., Fay, D., Hilburger, T., Leng, K., & Tag, A. (1997). The concept of personal initiative:

Operationalization, reliability and validity in two German samples. Journal of Occupational and

Organizational Psychology, 70, 139-161.

Frese, M., Garst, H., Fay, D. (1998). Control and complexity in work and the development of

personal initiative (PI): A 5-wave longitudinal structural equation model of occupational

socialization. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Frese, M., Kring, W., Soose, A., & Zempel, J. (1996). Personal Initiative at work: Differences

between East and West Germany. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 37-63.

Frese, M., & Okonek, K. (1984). Reasons to leave shiftwork and psychological and psychosomatic

complaints of former shiftworkers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 500-514.

Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1988) Methodological issues in the study of work stress: Objective vs.

subjective measurement of work stress and the question of longitudinal studies. In C.L. Cooper &

R. Payne (Eds.), Causes, coping and consequences of stress at work (pp. 375-411). Chichester,

England: Wiley.

Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1987). Eine Skala zur Erfassung von sozialen Stressoren am Arbeitsplatz [A

scale to assess social stressors at work]. Zeitschrift für Arbeitswissenschaft, 41, 134-142.

Page 168: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

159

Glass, D.C., & McKnight, J.D. (1996). Perceived control, depressive symptomatology, and

professional burnout: A review of the evidence. Psychology and Health, 11, 23-48.

Golembiewski, R.T., Billingsley, K., & Yeager, S. (1976). Measuring change and persistence in

human affairs: Types of change generated by OD designs. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science,

12, 133-157.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:

Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55.

Ilgen, D. R., & Hollenbeck, J. R. (1992). The structure of work: Job design and roles. In M. D.

Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 165-

208). Palo Alto, Ca.: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL8 user’s reference guide. Chicago: Scientific

Software International.

Kahn, R. L., & Byosiere, P. (1992). Stress in organizations. In M. D. Dunnette

& L. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology, Vol. 3, (pp. 571-650).

Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Kasl, S.V. (1978). Epidemiological contributions to the study of work stress. In C.L. Cooper & R.

Payne (Eds.), Stress at work (pp. 3-48). Chicester, England: Wiley.

Kenny, D.A., & Campbell, D.T. (1989). On the measurement of stability in over-time data. Journal

of Personality, 57, 445-481.

Kohn, M.L. (1973). Social class and schizophrenia: A critical review and a reformulation.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, 7, 60-79.

Lazarus, R.S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping, New York: Springer.

Little, T.D. (1997). Mean and covariance structures (MACS) analysis of cross-cultural data:

Practical and theoretical issues. Multivariate Behavior Research, 32, 53-76.

Maaz, H.J. (1992). Interview of the Frankfurter Rundschau of July 6, 1992.

MacCallum, R.C., & Browne, M.W. (1993). The use of causal indicators in covariance structure

models: Some practical issues. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 533-541.

MacCallum, R.C., Kim, C., Malarkey, W.B., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K. (1997). Studying multivariate

change using multilevel models and latent curve models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32,

215-253.

Maslach, C. (1998). A multidimensional theory of burnout. In G.J.Cooper (Ed.), Theories of

organizational stress. New York: Oxford University Press.

Page 169: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

160

McArdle, J.J. (1988). Dynamic but structural equation modeling of repeated measures data. In J.R.

Nesselroade & R.B. Cattell (Eds.), Handbook of multivariate experimental psychology (pp. 561-

614). New York: Plenum.

McArdle, J.J., & Woodcock, R.W. (1997). Expanding test-retest designs to include developmental

time-lag components. Psychological Methods, 2, 403-435.

McDonald, R.P. (1996). Path analysis with composite variables. Multivariate Behavior Research,

31, 239-270.

Meredith, W. (1993). Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance.

Psychometrika, 58, 525-543.

Meredith, W., & Tisak, J. (1990). Latent curve analysis. Psychometrika, 55, 107-122.

Moeller, J., & Strauss, B. (1997). Before and after the German reunification: Changes in observers'

commentaries on achievements in a natural experiment. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27,

75-93.

Mohr, G. (1986). Die Erfassung psychischer Befindensbeeinträchtigungen bei Industriearbeitern

[Measuring disturbances of psychological well-being in industrial blue collar workers]. Frankfurt

Germany: Peter Lang.

Murphy, L.R., Hurrell, J.J., Jr, Quick, J.C (1992). Work and well-being: Where do we go from

here? In J.C. Quick, L.R. Murphy & J.J. Hurrell, Jr. (Eds.) Stress & well-being at work. Washington

DC.: American Psychological Association.

Muthén, B. (in press). Second-generation structural equation modeling with a combination of

categorical and continuous latent variables: New opportunities for latent class/latent growth

modeling. In A. Sayer & L. Collins (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change. Washinton

D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Muthén, B. (1997). Latent variable modeling with longitudinal and multilevel data. In A. Raftery

(Ed.), Sociological Methodology (pp. 453-480). Boston: Blackwell Publishers.

Muthén, B. & Muthén, L. (1998). Training session Stockholm August 1998 on: Analysis of

longitudinal data: Growth modeling using latent variables.

Muthén, B.O., & Curran, P.J. (1997). General longitudinal modeling of individual differences in

experimental designs: A latent variable framework for analysis and power estimation.

Psychological Methods, 2, 371-402.

Page 170: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

161

Nesselroade, J.R. (1991). Interindividual differences in intraindividual change. In L. M. Collins &

J.L. Horn (Eds.), Best methods for the analysis change: Recent advances, unanswered questions,

future directions (pp. 92-105). Washington, DC.: American Psychological Association.

Oort, F.J.O. (1996). Using restricted factor analysis in test construction. Unpublished Dissertation

University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

Pearce, J., Branyicki, I., & Bukacsi, G. (1994). Person-based reward systems: A theory of

organizational reward practices in reform-communist organizations. Journal of Organizational

Behavior, 15, 261-282.

Pentz, M.A., & Chou, C.P. (1994). Measurement invariance in longitudinal clinical research

assuming change from development and intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 62, 450-462.

Peters, L.H., O’Connor, E.J., & Rudolf, C.J. (1980). The behavioral and affective consequences of

performance-relevant situational variables. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 25,

79-96.

Plewis, I. (1996). Statistical methods for understanding cognitive growth: A review, a synthesis and

an application. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 49, 25–42.

Rovine, M.J. & Molenaar, P.C.M. (1998). The covariance between level and shape in the latent

growth curve model with estimated basis vector coefficients. Methods of Psychological Research,

3, 95-107

Schafer, J.L. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data, London: Chapman & Hall.

Semmer, N. (1982). Stress at work, stress in private life and psychological well-being. In W.

Bachmann & I. Udris (Eds.), Mental load and stress in activity: European approaches (pp. 42-55).

Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Semmer, N. (1984). Stressbezogene Tätigkeitsanalyse: Psychologische Untersuchungen zur

Analyse von Stress am Arbeitsplatz [Stress oriented analysis of work: Psychological studies on the

analysis of stress at work]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.

Semmer, N., Zapf, D., & Greif, S. (1996). ‘Shared job strain’: A new approach for assessing the

validity of job stress measurements. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 69,

293-310.

Spector, P.E. (1998). A control theory of the job stress process. In G.J. Cooper (Ed.), Theories of

organizational stress. New York: Oxford University Press.

Page 171: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

162

Spector, P.E., & Jex, S.M. (1998). Development of four self-report measures of job stressors and

strain: Interpersonal Conflicts at Work Scale, Organizational Constraints Scale, Quantitative

Workload Inventory, and Physical Symptoms Inventory. Journal of Occupational Health

Psychology, 3, 356-367.

Spector, P.E., Zapf, D., Chen, P.Y., & Frese, M. (in press). Don’t throw out the baby with the bath

water: The role of negative affectivity in reports of job stressors and strains. Journal of

Organizational Behavior.

Willett, J.B. (1989). Some results on reliability for the longitudinal measurement of change:

Implications for the design of studies of individual growth. Educational and Psychological

Measurement, 49, 587-602.

Willett, J.B., & Sayer, A.G. (1994). Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates and

predictors of individual change over time. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 363-381.

Willett, J.B., & Sayer, A.G. (1995). Cross-domain analysis of change over time: Combining growth

curve modeling and covariance structure analysis. In G.A. Marcoulides & R.E. Schumacker (Eds.),

Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp. 125-157). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Zapf, D. (1991). Stressbezogene Arbeitsanalyse bei der Arbeit mit unterschiedlichen Bü-

rosoftwaresystemen. [Stress-related job-analysis for various office software-systems]. Zeitschrift für

Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 35, 2-14.

Zapf, D., Dormann, C., & Frese, M. (1996). Longitudinal studies in organizational stress research:

A review of the literature with reference to methodological issues. Journal of Occupational Health

Psychology, 1, 145-169.

Zung, W. W. K. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 63-70.

Page 172: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

163

Chapter 5

Optimism and Subjective Well-being in a Radical Change Situation in

East Germany

Does being an optimist help to deal with a prolonged stressful period and can these

beneficial effects of optimism be explained by the effective coping strategies used by optimists?

Researchers have pleaded for more longitudinal designs in order to gain insight into the causal

ordering of optimism, coping, and subjective well-being (Carver et al., 1993; Diener et al., 1999, p.

277; Lazarus, 1992, p. 245). Our study investigates the relation of optimism/pessimism and

subjective well-being and the mediating role of coping styles in a five year, five wave longitudinal

sample of East German inhabitants after the unification of East and West Germany. This study adds

to our knowledge in the following ways: First, it is one of the few longitudinal studies and the only

study with more than two or three measurement waves testing the effects of optimism and

pessimism on subjective well-being and the mediating effects of coping. Second, it has been argued

that subjective well-being has trait- and state like properties (Diener, Suh, Lucas & Smith, 1999, p.

280). However, the implications of this have not been spelled out theoretically and empirically. By

using growth curve methodology, we can systematically differentiate fast and slow moving changes

in optimism and subjective well-being.

Conceptualization of change

Most psychological constructs can be differentiated into three components, a completely

stable part, a slowly changing trend-like part, and a fast changing state-like component. Even

dispositions may show changes over time (Kenny & Campbell, 1989, Mischel & Shoda, 1998; in

the domain of coping: Lazarus, 1993). With the completely stable part, we mean a genetic or early

childhood predisposition which only changes because of biological parameters (e.g., dementia). The

slow moving trend may imply a developmental change. For example, in East Germany, people may

slowly discover the implications of having freedom, for example, in choosing a job or becoming an

entrepreneur. This is not an idea that changes quickly (within the framework of a few months) but

may take years to develop. Finally, fast moving changes are not day-by-day transient changes but

Page 173: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

164

are state-components which can change from week to week or month to month. For example,

having just found a new job increases one’s optimism and subjective well-being for several months

(e.g., honeymoon effect). Dispositional optimism, by definition regarded as a personality trait, but

also subjective well-being can have these three components (Diener et al, 1999, p.281). The high

stability of subjective well-being is well documented (Headey & Wearing, 1989, Ormel &

Schaufeli, 1991). Our theoretical model is inspired by the interpretation of change by Nesselroade

(1991). Nesselroade (1991, p. 96) distinguished three kinds of variability: (1) intra-individual

variability (relatively rapid, more or less reversible changes like states), (2) intra-individual change

(relatively slow changes reflecting processes such as development, labeled as ‘trait change’) and (3)

inter-individual changes (highly stable, denoted as ‘traits’).

Many studies on the effects of optimism used either cross-sectional designs or longitudinal

designs with only two wave studies and these designs are not able to differentiate between these

three change components. Even though the interest in the trait-state distinction is high (Mischel &

Shoda, 1998, Kenny & Campbell, 1989) there are no studies demonstrating differential effects of

the completely stable, slowly changing and rapidly changing components of optimism on subjective

well-being. The availability of longitudinal data including more than two waves is a prerequisite to

separate these components.

Differentiation between optimism and pessimism

In this study we concentrate on optimism, defined as the tendency to have positive general

outcome expectancies (Scheier & Carver, 1985) and the effects of optimism on subjective well-

being.

In recent years the construct validity of optimism has been debated. Many studies used the

Life Orientation Test (LOT, Scheier & Carver, 1985) for measuring optimism and it was

consistently found that a two-factor model fitted the data better than a one-factor model (Cheng &

Hamid, 1997; Marshall, Wortman, Kusulas, Hervig & Vickers, 1992; Mook, Kleijn , Van der Ploeg,

1992; Mroczek, Spiro, Aldwin, Ozer & Bossé, 1993; Räikkönen, Mattthews, Flory, Owens &

Gump, 1999; Robinson-Whelen, Kim, MacCallum, Kieholt-Glaser, 1997; Scheier, Carver &

Bridges, 1994; Schwarzer, 1994, p. 170). In these two-factor solutions all positively formulated

items loaded on one factor (optimism) and all negatively formulated items loaded on the other

factor (pessimism). Carver and Scheier (1985) argued that response tendencies could be blamed for

the emergence of two factors and that for most purposes the LOT could be considered as measuring

Page 174: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

165

optimism as a one-dimensional construct. From this perspective optimism and pessimism are

opposites of one underlying continuum. However, there are findings that optimism and pessimism

have different relationships with other variables and have different predictive values. For example,

Schwarzer (1994), Marshall et al. (1992) claimed that pessimism is associated with neuroticism and

negative affect, whereas optimism is linked to extraversion and positive affect. Although the debate

on whether the LOT is bi-dimensional is not completely settled (Spector, Van Katwijk, Brannick &

Chen, 1997), we decided to treat optimism and pessimism as separate constructs in order to explore

their potential differential effects on coping styles and subjective well-being.

Effects of optimism/pessimism on subjective well-being

Optimism/pessimism has been discussed within the frameworks of stress research and

subjective well-being. Optimism is related to symptoms of physiological and psychological health

in many stress studies (for excellent reviews of the literature see Scheier & Carver, 1992 and Taylor

& Aspinwall, 1996). In the stress literature optimism is interpreted as a resistance factor and

pessimism is treated as a vulnerability factor (Kessler, Price & Wortman, 1985, p. 541; Taylor &

Aspinwall, 1996). The reasons for these consistent relationships are thought to lie in differential

primary and secondary appraisal (Chang, 1998; Khoo & Bishop, 1996), in better social networks for

optimists (Geers, Reilly & Dember, 1998), or in psychoneuroimmunological factors (Abraham,

1994, p. 184). The most frequent mentioned mediator is coping which will be discussed below.

However the role of optimism as a major predictor of subjective well-being is not without

debate. Diener et al (1999, p. 281) argued that it is difficult to disentangle whether the cognitive

processes of optimism are the cause or the result of higher well-being. DeNeve and Cooper (1998)

discarded optimism in their meta-analysis by arguing (on footnote p. 199) that there is a conceptual

overlap between optimism and subjective well-being which makes showing relationships between

these variables trivial. A similar argument has been made for pessimism to be part of negative

affectivity (e.g., neuroticism: Smith, Pope, Rhodewalt & Poulton, 1989; and self-mastery: Marshall

& Lang, 1990). Evidence against this argument has been provided by several researchers (Chang,

1998; Räikkönen, Mattthews, Flory, Owens & Gump 1999; Scheier, Carver & Bridges, 1994;

Williams, 1992), showing the unique contribution of optimism in predicting outcome variables over

and beyond neuroticism.

Page 175: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

166

This debate can only be resolved by using longitudinal studies that show two things. First,

that optimism/pessimism is indeed related to subjective well-being, and second that it remains

significant after controlling for initial level of subjective well-being. Once, these two questions are

answered within a longitudinal study, the issue of mediation will be addressed.

This study is both unique in its design as well in its sample size. To our knowledge such a

large scale longitudinal study has not been published yet.

The mediating role of coping

The most dominant theory argues for indirect effects mediated by coping strategies:

Optimists make more use of efficient coping strategies like problem-oriented coping, whereas

pessimists use less effective strategies like emotional-focused strategies (Carver, Scheier &

Weintraub, 1989). Carver and Scheier (1992) used self-regulation theory to explain why optimists

are using more effective coping strategies. Self-regulation theory states that people persist in

achieving their goals as long as they believe that the outcomes would be positive. Because optimists

believe that they can expect more positive outcomes than pessimists, they are more strongly

motivated to retain their level of efforts. However, there is far more evidence that optimists use

different types of coping strategies, than that coping mediates the relation between optimism and

well-being (Carver et al, 1993). Moreover most of the available studies established only concurrent

relations and did not use a longitudinal design to predict future well-being (partialling out initial

well-being). Up to this point the prospective mediation hypothesis has not been adequately tested

(Carver et al, 1993).

The hypothesis that coping mediates the effects of optimism introduces theoretical

complexities from a transactional stress theory point of view. On the micro level stress and coping

are dynamic in time so that both appraisals (primary and secondary) and coping reactions can

rapidly change across the stages of a stressful transaction (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Therefore,

problem and emotion-focused coping can be used in rapid interchange during several stages of the

coping process and also can occur simultaneously to enhance their effectiveness (Scheier & Carver,

1994). Lazarus, in his transactional stress approach, contends that coping is a dynamic process and

that ways of coping can rapidly change in accordance with the demands of the appraisal of the

stressors at the time. No coping strategy is always effective and no coping strategy is always

ineffective. On the other hand, to make coping at all researchable within our context, it is necessary

to collapse concrete coping strategies across time (Lazarus, 1993). As Lazarus (1992, p. 243) says:

Page 176: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

167

“To study coping over time and across diverse sources of stress in the same persons in sufficient

number to address both its process and trait aspects, and to do this with an appreciation of the whole

person, calls for complex, long-term research designs.” In the coping literature a distinction

between coping strategies and coping styles is often made. For some research purposes the concept

of a coping style (or sometimes called dispositional or trait coping) may be useful to characterizes

the typical way a person acts in many stressful situations across many times (Ben-Zur, 1998;

Bijttebier & Vertommen, 1997, p. 848; Carver & Scheier, 1994, p.185; Schwarz, Neale, Marco,

Shiffman, & Stone, 1999; Taylor & Aspinwall, 1996; Terry, 1994). Recently, Schwarz et al (1999)

used a sophisticated longitudinal design (ecological momentary assessment) and found that

individual differences in coping styles exist, accounting for 15-40% of the variance in momentary

coping. In this paper we concentrate on the stable parts of coping and will investigate the mediating

role of coping styles in the relationship between optimism and subjective well-being.

The situation in East Germany

The setting of this study is particularly interesting, because drastic changes have taken place

during the time of the study. Immediately after the fall of the Berlin wall the atmosphere was

euphoric, but after a year East Germany slid into an economic recession and unemployment rates

rose dramatically. It slowly became apparent that the promised land of wealth and prosperity was

still a long way to go. This setting, characterized by strong political, economic, and social turmoil is

a good place to study the effects of optimism. The situation in East Germany after the unification in

1990 created both new challenges as well as threats, which were never experienced before. Chances

to become entrepreneurs increased side by side with unknown problems like long-term

unemployment. Bureaucratic procedures changed, stressors, for example, time pressure at work

increased, new supervisory principles appeared, and the old social structures vanished (Fay & Frese;

in press). All of this happened in a relatively short time. There were winners and losers in this

transition from socialism to market capitalism. If there is any situation that should produce drastic

changes in optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being, this should be one. The events of East

Germany during the 5 years after unification – the period or our study – are presented in Table 20.

As the short description in Table 20 shows, the economic situation in East Germany changed most

radically in the years late 1990 to 1991. Democracy and capitalism came mid 1990. The work

places were not transformed immediately, this happened at the end of 1990 and in 1991.

Page 177: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

168

Table 20 Historical context in East Germany

Time Historical Events Study Waves

October, November 1989 Mass demonstrations in Leipzig, Dresden and Berlin November 1989 The Berlin wall opened March 1990 First free election in East Germany July 1990 Economic unification, the DM (the West German

currency) is introduced in East Germany; the first changes appear at the work places, East German companies are started to be sold off, mainly to West German firms work places are still very much like they were under socialism

T0

October 1990 Political unification November 1990 work places started to be changed T1 December 1990 First general election in all of Germany Year of 1991 Serious economic crisis in East Germany, many work

related education programs started by government

August, September 1991 dramatic changes in work places, many people had to change jobs

T2

Years of 1992 and 1993 The economic crisis in East Germany deepened; wages increased to ca. 70 - 80% of Western level; many government programs to stimulate growth; more and more resentment towards West German managers among East Germans

August, September 1992 T3 August, September 1993 T4 Years of 1994 and 1995 The economic situation in East Germany stabilizes on

a low level; unemployment is high, in some towns it approaches 50%; there are pockets of very high productivity in the East, however, average productivity of East German workers is about 70% of those in the West; most industrial jobs have been lost; West Germany also slides into an economic recession with high unemployment

August, September 1995 T5

Theoretical Predictions

We will describe both a direct and a mediating model. These models are shown in Figure 25

and it is described to which “change” component the hypothesis is referring.

Page 178: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

169

__________________________________________________________________________________________________________________ O

ptimism

/pessimism

Subjective W

ell-being __________________________________________________________________________________________________________________

H

ypothesis: Stable components of

optimism

/pessimism

are related to stable com

ponents of subjective well-being

Hypothesis: T

rends in optimism

/pessimism

are related to trends in subjective w

ell-being which

can be explained by effectiveness of coping styles H

ypothesis: Fast changes in optimism

/pessimism

and fast changes in subjective w

ell-being are related

Figure 25. Theoretical hypothesis and the relation w

ith several parts of optimism

/pessimism

and subjective well-being

time

subject 1

subject 2

subject 3

subject 4

mean

com

pletely stab

le parts

time

subject 1

subject 3

subject 2

subject 4

mean

slow system

atic changes

time

subject 5

trend

line

ge

fast an

ch

s

time

subject 5

trend

line

ge

fast

an

ch

s

time

subject 1

subject 2

subject 3

subject 4

mean

complete

ly stable

parts

time

subject 1

subject 3

subject 2

subject 4

mean

slow system

atic changes

slow

changes in subjective w

ell-being

Hypothesis: Initial

optimism

/pessimism

is related to

coping styles as m

edia-tors

Page 179: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

170

Relationships of the Stable Components of Optimism/Pessimism and Subjective Well-

Being. Our hypothesis that there is a relationship of the stable components of optimism/pessimism

and subjective well-being is based on the idea that all three variables have a strong genetic

component. This has been shown to be true for subjective well-being particularly (Diener et al,

1999, p. 279).

Relationships of Initial Optimism/Pessimism to Changes in Well-Being. This hypothesis

tests if being optimistic (pessimistic) at the start of the period of this study will lead to long term

positive (negative) changes in well-being. This follows from self-regulation theory (Scheier &

Carver, 1985). People who see positive outcomes in the future will continue to strive to achieve

them. Although occasionally disengagement from one’s goal may have adaptive value, particularly

in situations where continued effort is futile (Carver, Scheier & Pozo, 1992), we think that showing

persistence and remaining committed to one goals is the better strategy than disengagement.

Ultimately, not giving up may lead to success and it may provide mastery experiences and create

new opportunities. This process may feed upon itself and thus explains a benign cycle for optimists

and a vicious cycle for pessimists. The time order of this model satisfies one of the prerequisites for

a causal interpretation: the initial status of optimism precedes later changes in well-being. The time

lag is necessary because it takes time to deal with the new challenges and stressors of the post-

communist situation and it needs time to gradually translate these into changes in well-being.

Relationships of Trends in Optimism/Pessimism and Well-Being. This hypothesis refers to

the slowly changing components of both optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being and it

hypothesizes that these changes are related. Explanation of this relationship may be that an effective

coping style leads to both psychological as well as environmental changes. For example, continuing

one’s efforts in looking for a job under mass unemployment or studying hard for a new education

may be successful in the end. Both can be considered as mastery of stressors and both changes

one’s relationship to the environment (e.g., labor market). Conversely, an ineffective coping style

like wishful thinking combined with premature giving up one’s attempts to find a job may lead to

long-term unemployment which decreases one’s well-being and increases one’s pessimism in the

long run. In summary, changes in optimism/pessimism and changes in subjective well-being can

both be considered as outcome variables which are related because they are generated by the same

causes (e.g., effective coping style).

Relationships between Fast Changes of Optimism/Pessimism and Fast Changes of Well-

Being. A sort of honeymoon effect occurs when a positive event (for example, getting some good

news) produces positive effects on optimism and well-being. This is a short-lived effect. However,

Page 180: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

171

it is not an uninteresting effect because cross-sectional studies may actually capitalize on this effect

to get a high correlation between optimism/pessimism and well-being.

Mediation. This model implies that optimism/pessimism – subjective well-being

relationships are mediated by coping. Such a mediation process is not relevant for the completely

stable parts of optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being, because it does not assume a causal

relationship between them (genetic predisposition and previous life experiences are third variables

producing a spurious correlation between optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being). We did

not test the mediation hypothesis for the fast changes because we do not have adequate process data

for such a procedure.

Therefore, the mediation hypothesis is only relevant for one hypothesized relationship: The

relationship of the stable components of optimism/pessimism and changes in subjective well-being.

Optimism at the start of an episode will be related differentially to coping styles. Coping styles, in

turn, are differentially related to subjective well-being. There are several coping styles and therefore

a simple mediation model is inappropriate. As Figure 26 shows there can be several mediational

paths and we hypothesize that all effects of optimism on subjective well-being via coping styles

should be positive: Optimists have more effective coping styles, which in turn have a positive

influence on their well-being14. In contrast pessimists can be characterized by having an ineffective

coping style, which will negatively affect their well-being. In particular, we expect that there are

positive relations between optimism and problem-focused coping, which includes trying to get help

from social networks and planning to deal with one’s problems. Pessimists have an emotionally

focused coping style and will use more wishful thinking and self-criticism.

Coping may not only be determined by optimism/pessimism, it may also affect future

changes in optimism/pessimism. Thus, coping may have a reciprocal relationship with

optimism/pessimism. In a way, problem-focused coping is likely to lead to more mastery

experiences and, therefore, will lead to a higher degree of optimism. If effective coping styles

would result in both positive changes in optimism/pessimism as well as in subjective well-being,

14 In the general case, complex mediational models can have paths with opposite effects and theoretically the total effect

can be zero. Thus, compensational mechanisms can produce suppressor effects (Gully, Frone, Edwards, 1998). Thus, if

direct effects in a model without mediators are absent, this does not necessarily imply that mediational effects are

absent. As Bollen stated (1989):”The old saying that correlation does not prove causation should be complemented by

the saying that a lack of correlation does not disprove causation (p.52).

Page 181: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

172

the coping style variables act as a set of third variables (partly) explaining the relationship between

changes in optimism/pessimism and changes in subjective well-being.

Figure 26. Simple and complex mediation.

Independent variable

mediator Dependent variable

+ +

Initial optimism Changes in subjective well-being

+ +

- -

Effective coping style

Simple full mediation model

Complex full mediational model

Page 182: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

173

Method

The data in this study were gathered in the AHUS project. AHUS is a German acronym for

“active actions in a radical change situation”. The goal of the project was to study the effects of

drastic changes that took place after the unification of East and West Germany and one of the

research questions was which people could cope better with the many stressors they encountered.

This study used only the last five waves over a five year time period (the data of the first wave were

not used; more on this later). Other publications of this study concentrated on personal initiative

(Frese, Kring, Soose, & Zempel, 1996; Frese, Fay, Hilburger, Leng, & Tag, 1997; Frese, Garst, &

Fay, 1998; Speier & Frese, 199) and stressors and strains (Dormann, Zapf & Speier, in press; Garst,

Frese & Molenaar, in press). The issue of optimism/pessimism has not been dealt with in any other

publication.

Sample

A representative sample was drawn in Dresden, a large city in the south of East Germany; it

is the capital of Saxonia, houses a large Technical University and is relatively well-off (for

example, compared with cities in the north of East Germany). The sampling was done by randomly

selecting streets, selecting every third house and in each house, every fourth apartment (in smaller

houses every third one). People between the ages of 18 and 65 with full-time employment at the

start of the study participated (thus, we sometimes had more than one person per family). The

refusal rate of 33% was quite low for a study of this kind. Confidentiality was assured; if subjects

preferred anonymity, this was done with the help of a personal code word.

In wave 1, (July 1990), 463 people participated in Dresden (the first wave, T0, was not

included in this study, more on this will be discussed later). At T1 (November, December, 1990)

202 additional people were asked to participate15. At T2 (September 1991), the N was 543, at T3

(September 1992) the N was 506, at T4 (September 1993), N = 478, at T5 (September 1995), N =

489. Experimental mortality did not prove to change the make-up of the sample. The sample is

representative of the Dresden population on the relevant parameters (for example, for age, social

class, male/female percentage at work). Fifty-three percent of the sample was male and 47% female.

At T2 age ranged from 17 to 65 years (M = 39, SD = 11.4). Most subjects worked in the service

15 Additional people were added to ascertain whether repeated participation had an influence on the variables of the

questionnaire. This was not the case.

Page 183: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

174

industry (35,9%) and as employees in trade or manufacturing enterprises (30.9%). Overall, there

were 18.9% office workers on jobs that required little qualification, 27.4% worked as managers or

professionals with high qualification requirements, 12.5% were higher level public servants mostly

employed in schools and universities (e.g., university professors are public servants in Germany),

and 16.5% worked in skilled respectively 14.9% unskilled blue collar positions. At the start of the

study there were no unemployed in our sample, but later unemployment rose leading to the

following unemployment figures for the subsequent waves: n = 42 (7%) at T1, n = 59 (11%) at T2,

n = 38 (7.8%) at T3, n = 35 (7.5%) at T4 and n = 37 (8.1%) at T5. At later waves some subjects also

did not have a job for reasons other than unemployment (e.g., retirement, schooling, parental leave).

Measures

All optimism and subjective well-being measures were ascertained with a questionnaire. The

coping style variables were also measured by a questionnaire but were prepared within an interview

setting. Planning was measured in the interview.

Optimism and Pessimism. The Life Orientation Test (LOT) of Carver & Scheier (1985) was

used. The LOT was fully administered in five waves. Unfortunately due to a clerical error in the

first wave one of the items of the LOT was not included in the questionnaire. Therefore, the first

wave was not included in the study. As the LOT scale was part of an extended questionnaire we did

not include the filler items. Optimism included all the positive items, pessimism all the negative

ones.

Subjective Well-Being Variables. Diener, Suh, Lucas & Smith (1999) argue that subjective

well-being should not be treated as a monolithic entity, but as consisting of separate components

that exhibit unique relations with other variables of its nomological network. Hence, in this article

we used depression, irritation, worrying and psychosomatic complaints as separate constructs.

Diener et al (1999, p. 278) argue that subjective well-being depends on reactions in multiple

physiological and psychological systems, and psychosomatic complaints refer more to physiology,

whereas worrying is rooted in the cognitive system. The measures, depression, psychosomatic

complaints, irritation and worrying, were adaptations of Mohr’s (1986) scales – a group of

frequently used scales in Germany, because they are well validated. Garst, Frese & Molenaar (in

press) demonstrated that these scales possessed measurement invariance for a period of at least five

years.

Depression (4 items) was originally adapted from Zung (1965) and all of those items that

referred to physical problems (e.g., not being able to sleep) were excluded to reduce the overlap

Page 184: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

175

with psychosomatic complaints. Items were “A good deal seems senseless to me” and “I have sad

moods”. This depression scale attempts to measure mild forms of depression and probably even

persons with high scores would not be diagnosed as suffering from depression in a clinical sense

(Coyne & Downey, 1991, p. 405-406). A seven point Likert scale was used and the extreme

response categories were described as “almost always” to “never”. The internal consistencies for the

five waves were .72, .81, .80, .79 and .82.

Psychosomatic complaints (8 items) was originally adapted from Fahrenberg (1975) and

related to aches and other negative bodily sensations. The respondents can easily detect the

symptoms and no medical assistance is needed for its diagnosis. The contents of some items were:

“Do you feel pain in your shoulders?” and “Do you have feelings of dizziness?”. A five point

Likert scale was used with response categories, which ranged from ‘almost daily’ to ‘never’. The

alphas ranged from .83 to .85. To reduce the size of the measurement models we constructed 3

item-parcels out of the 8 items. An item-parcel is a combination of the scores of several items so

that a much smaller number of measured variables have to be included into the model (Marsh, Hau,

Balla & Grayson, 1998). The allocation of the items to the parcels was based on the results of an

exploratory factor analysis.

Irritation (5 items) and worrying (3 items) were both derivatives of the scale irritation

developed by Mohr (1986) because preliminary analysis indicated that two (moderately correlated)

factors could be distinguished. The internal consistencies for the irritation scales ranged from .84 to

.85.

Worrying referred to the preoccupation of work-related problems in one’s spare time (“Even

during holidays I think a lot about problems at my work”). The scope of the original irritation scale

was narrowed down to feelings of irritation and nervousness (I’m easily agitated”). A seven point

Likert scale was used. Alphas ranged from .83 to .87.

Coping styles consisted of a German translation of the Ways of Coping Checklist (Folkman,

Lazarus, Dunkel-Schetter, DeLongis, & Gruen, 1986) and the following scales were administered:

Problem-focused coping, wishful thinking, emotional focused coping, seeking social support and

self-criticism. Our procedure was a combination of state and style approaches. At each wave, the

interviewer asked the respondent to name a specific stressor at work that had happened within a 7-

day period. This stressor was then used as stimulus material for the Ways of Coping Checklist. All

items were summed for waves T1 to T5 to estimate the coping style. The coefficient alpha values

for these scales were .87 for problem-focused coping, .86 for wishful thinking, .86 for emotional-

focused coping, .83 for seeking social support, and .71 for self-criticism. Our approach is in line

Page 185: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

176

with the suggestion by Lazarus (1992) and is an attempt to measure the typical preference to

approach problems in particular ways (Kessler, Price & Wortman, 1985, p.551).

Planning was ascertained by an interviewer rating after the interview. The score was based

on the sum of three items with regard to level of concreteness and detail of plans and their long-

term orientation. The rating was done after an extensive interview, which included questions on

personal initiative, professional career, and future plans. Planning should be seen as one specific

instance of problem focused coping. The scale planning was measured with a different procedure

and, therefore, does not carry the risk of common variance with optimism/pessimism, well-being,

and the other coping factors. Coefficient alpha for the scale, aggregated over five waves, was .86.

To evaluate all the models covariance matrices and means were estimated with the computer

program NORMS (Schafer, 1997) and these matrices and mean vectors were used as input for

LISREL (Version 8.30, Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993). The NORMS program is specifically designed

for handling missing data problems. We used the EM Algorithm of NORMS. The EM algorithm

(Dempster, Laird & Rubin, 1977) is a general technique for finding maximum-likelihood estimates

for models with partial missingness. It is based upon the assumption that data are "missing at

random" (MAR), which is a much milder assumption than the assumption that "missingness occurs

completely at random" (MCAR). MAR only requires that the missing values behave like a random

sample of all values within subclasses defined by observed data” (Schafer, 1997, p. 11). The sample

size used for a particular LISREL analyze was calculated by the mean of the different sample sizes

of the input matrix (N = 423 for depression; N = 420 for psychosomatic complaints; N = 421 for

irritation; N = 416 for worrying).

To evaluate the overall fit of the models, we report the chi-square statistic, the Akaike index

(AIC; Akaike, 1987) Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA; Browne & Cudeck,

1993) and the comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, 1990). One disadvantage of using the chi-square

statistic in comparative model fitting is that it decreases when parameters are added to the model.

Therefore we also report the AIC index, because it takes parsimony (in the sense of as few

parameters as possible) as well as fit into account (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993). However, if two

models are nested, we report the difference chi-square test (Bollen, 1989). Browne & Cudeck

(1993) suggested using Steiger's Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) as a

measure of discrepancy per degree of freedom with a value of 0.05 indicating a close fit and values

up to 0.08 representing reasonable errors of approximation in the population. The CFI is based upon

a comparison of the fit of the hypothesized model to the fit of the null baseline model and most

Page 186: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

177

researchers consider values greater than .90 as an indication for a good fit, although recent research

suggests a cutoff value close to .95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

In order to evaluate effect sizes we will report the parameters of interest, the standard errors

and the z values. Since we have directional hypotheses we used one-sided tests of significance: If

the ratio of a parameter estimate and its standard error exceeds the value of 1.65 the parameter will

be called significant. However, many researchers using structural equation modeling call a

parameter significant if the ratio exceeds the value of 1.96, so we will also report if this is the case.

Modeling Strategy

Our modeling strategy consisted of three parts. In the first part we tested the measurement

models. In the second part we investigated the stability of the means and individual differences. The

third part dealt with testing the structural models. For all three parts the latent means were required,

hence all estimated models simultaneously analyzed the covariance matrix and the mean vector.

Measurement Models

A description of the measurement models for the subjective well-being variables can be

found in Garst, Frese & Molenaar (in press), so here we only describe the measurement models for

optimism and pessimism. The strategy of the measurement modeling involved three basic steps. In

the first step (Model 1) a longitudinal measurement model with a banded error16 structure was

tested. Models with optimism as one single construct can be compared with a two-factor model

with the optimism and pessimism items loading on two correlated constructs. The one and two-

factor solutions are nested and the difference chi-square test (Bollen, 1989) could therefore be used

for selecting the best model.

16 In the measurement model a common latent factor, an item-specific factor and a random error term can predict an

item response. In cross-sectional models it is not possible to separate the specific item variance and the random error

variance. Both are summed into the unique variance of the item (Raffalovich & Bohrnstedt, 1987). But in longitudinal

studies the specific item factor can be specified as the correlation between the residuals of identical items measured at

different occasions. Identical and repeatedly administered items invoke specific responses for a person and the unique

components are therefore allowed to correlate over time. Vonesh and Chinchilli (1997) and Steyer, Ferring, and Schmitt

(1992) describe several error structures; in this study the time between adjacent measurements varied and, therefore, we

preferred the unrestricted banded error variant. In a banded error structure there are covariances specified between the

unique factors of identical items (measured at different occasions).

Page 187: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

178

The next two steps tested measurement invariance (Little 1997; Meredith, 1993). For growth

models the assumption that across all time points the same construct has been measured is crucial

(Plewis 1996; Kenny & Campbell 1989). Thus, the second step (Model 2) tested for equality of

factor loadings over time. Factor loadings are regression coefficients where the observed variable is

regressed upon an unobserved latent factor. A change in relationships of the latent construct and the

items over time is an indication of a Gamma change (Golembiewski, Billingsley & Yeager, 1976)

which implies a change in the respondent’s interpretation of the item content (Chan, 1998; Oort,

1996). If there is a sizeable gamma change, comparisons of the relevant constructs over time are

impossible. In a third step (Model 3) the equivalence of item intercepts over time were tested. If all

factor loadings of identical items are equal over time a change in the item intercepts indicates a

general change in the level of the item response. This implies that the item is more or less attractive

and this shift cannot be explained by a change in the latent trait. This phenomenon is called beta

change (also called a response shift) and occurs if a respondent changes his or her meaning of the

item response scale’s value (Oort, 1996). Testing the equivalence of item intercepts requires that

both the covariance matrix and the vector of means should be analyzed. Some authors (Byrne,

Shavelson & Muthén, 1989; Pentz & Chou, 1994, Muthén, 1998) argue that it is sufficient to have

indicators for each construct with invariant measurement parameters. These items function as the

anchor items and keep the latent scale at a comparable level (cf. vertical equating in Item Response

Theory). So, a few violations can be tolerated and partial measurement invariance is a more realistic

goal.

Stability of the means and individual differences. Before testing longitudinal models it is

useful to inspect the pattern of the latent means and the stability of the individual differences. If the

goodness of fit is acceptable we can extract this information from the measurement models.

Comparing the mean trajectories of several latent constructs informs about the parallelism of the

development in these constructs. Stability coefficients inform about the degree to which there are

changes in the latent constructs over time. If there is a fair amount of change this justifies the fitting

of structural models in the next section in order to explain those changes.

Structural models

Appendix E provides a short description of the latent growth curve models used. Briefly,

growth curve models focus on intra-individual changes and interindividual differences in change

patterns. An obvious example is that some school children may start at a lower level of reading but

have steeper learning curves than other children. Thus, both the starting positions and the learning

curves may be different for different pupils. Figure 27 describes slopes and intercepts.

Page 188: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

179

Figure 27. Growth Curve Model with correlations between growth curve factors. Note: I = intercept factor; S= slope factor; not shown autocorrelations between unique factors of items; a: Correlation between slope factors tested in hypothesis: relationships of trends in pessimism and worrying. The slope factor S is a latent construct that represents the slope coefficient for each individual (as

deviation from the mean slope). A high factor score for S means that the slope for that individual

I S

T1 T2 T3 T4 T5

11

1 11Pessimism

I

1111

1S

T1 T2 T3 T4 T5Worrying

a

Page 189: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

180

deviates strongly from the mean slope. If the mean slope is zero (no mean changes over time) a high

positive factor score implies a strong positive change for that person and a low positive value means

that there is little positive change. Thus, S tells us something about the interindividual differences in

change processes (more on this in the Appendix). Therefore, changes in optimism/pessimism and

changes in subjective well-being can be represented by the slope factors. However, in a linear

growth curve model the slope factor only captures the linear change over time for each person.

The intercept factor signifies the starting point of the growth trajectories for each person

(one statistical prerequisite is to fix the time scaling to the value of 0 for the first measurement wave

– more on this in the Appendix). The initial status for optimism/pessimism and subjective well-

being can be represented by the intercept factors. However, the intercept factor is not equal to the

latent construct itself at the first measurement wave, because the last is also determined by state

influences (note the time-specific disturbance term in Figure 27).

Because the sample size in this study is too small to specify models including all our

variables we will report models which consist of subsets of variables. Therefore, for each of the

four well-being variables we had two separate models for optimism and pessimism. In this way

differential effects of optimism and pessimism on well-being can be studied. For

optimism/pessimism as well as for the well-being variables the measurement models were included,

so that we deal with latent variables; therefore, the growth curves are not confounded by

measurement error.

The first step was to model a maximum model. The maximum model does not impose

restrictions on the structural relations and thus maximally accounts for the covariation between

latent constructs. Its goodness of fit refers only to the appropriateness of the measurement models

included. This model will be used as a baseline for comparing the fit of more restricted models.

In a second modeling step, linear growth curve models with latent variables were estimated

for each combination of optimism/ pessimism and one of the well-being variables. Three growth

models will be estimated: An unspecified model which allows the growth factors to freely covary

(see Figure 27), a direct model, which specifies paths between both intercept and the slope factors

(see Figure 28), and a mediator models whereby the coping style variables mediate the relation-

ships between the intercept and the slope factors (see Figure 29). Each of the Figures 27 to 29

displays the model for pessimism and one well-being variable – worrying (the other models are

equivalent).

Page 190: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

181

Figure 28. Direct effects model for growth curve factors of pessimism and worrying. Note: I = intercept; S= slope; not shown autocorrelations between unique factors of items; only two coping styles variables displayed. a: Correlation between intercept factors tested in hypothesis tested in hypothesis: relationship between stable components of pessimism and worrying. b: Coefficient from path intercept factor pessimism to slope factor worrying tested in hypothesis: relationship of initial pessimism and changes in worrying. c: Partial correlation between slope factors tested in hypothesis: relationships of trends in pessimism and worrying.

I S

T1 T2 T3 T4 T5

11

1 11Pessimism

I

1111

1S

T1 T2 T3 T4 T5Worrying

ca

b

Page 191: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

182

S

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

1

1

1

1

1

Pessimism

Worrying

I

1

1

1

1

1

S

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

I

C1

C2

C3

C4

C5

C6

Figure 29. Mediation model with coping styles variables as mediators (C1: planning; C2: self-criticism; C3: emotional-focused coping; C4: seeking social support: C4: problem-focused coping; C5: wishful thinking). The measurement model is not shown.

Page 192: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

183

It is important to note that all these three models will yield the same goodness of fit

measures. This follows from the specification of the direct and mediator models: All covariances

are accounted for and no additional restrictions will be imposed, so the fit will be the same as the

unspecified model.

A third modeling step estimated linear growth curve models with an additional set of

correlated residuals (see Figure 30). These models are the same as the previous models except that

they allow the residuals of the latent constructs of optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being to

be correlated. The small arrows at T1, T2, etc. of these latent constructs (also called disturbances)

signify the deviations for each person from their individual growth curve for a particular time-point.

The correlations of the residuals of pessimism and the residuals of worrying at T1, T2 etc. test

whether there is a relationship of the fast moving changes in optimism/pessimism and the fast

changes in the subjective well-being variables at each particular wave. Thus, we test if there is a

tendency that a deviation from the individual growth curve of optimism/pessimism will be

accompanied by a deviation in the same direction from the individual growth curve of subjective

well-being. The significance of the correlations among the residuals refers to the relations between

the non-stable non-trend-like changes in optimism/pessimism and well-being.

The growth curves models are based on linear curves. If the fit of linear curves is not

satisfactory, quadratic or even higher order polynomials models can be fitted, although the

interpretation of these models is more difficult (Muthén and Muthén, 1998). It is important to note

that all models can only be conceived as approximations to reality (Browne & Cudeck, 1993;

Cudeck, 1991) and a linear growth model is parsimonious and still capturing important aspects of

the change over time even if in reality there are some deviations from linearity (Rogosa, Brandt &

Zimowski, 1982, p. 728; Willet, 1989).

In growth curve models there are other indicators of the usefulness of the models next to the

goodness of fit particularly the amount of explained variance of the growth curves and the

significance of the growth curve parameters. If only a small portion of the variance can be explained

by the linear growth curve, the major part would consist of residual variance referring to deviations

around the individual growth curves. This indicates that state variance would prevail over the

systematic changes. Also, the significance of the growth parameters needs to be inspected. If one of

the growth parameters does not reach the level of significance, this implies that a simpler model

would suffice. For instance, if both the estimates of the mean and the variance of the slope

Page 193: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

184

Figure 30. Model with correlated disturbance terms (Model 3 in Table 31).

I S

T1 T2 T3 T4 T5

11

1 11

Pessimism

I

1111

1S

T1 T2 T3 T4 T5

Worrying

correlationsbetweendisturbanceterms

Page 194: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

185

factor are not significant, a more parsimonious random intercept model would be more appropriate.

This last model does not contain a slope factor, but only has an intercept factor, representing

constant baselines for each individual. This highly restricted model argues that no systematic

changes over time occur, but for each subject there are only state fluctuations around the subject’s

constant baseline.

In summary, after reporting the goodness of fit measures, we will present both the amount of

explained variance of the growth curves and the significance of the growth curve parameters.

There are four relationships on which we have hypotheses: The relationship between the

intercepts, the relationship between the intercepts and the slopes and the relationships between the

slopes and finally the relationships between the time-specific residuals. The first three hypotheses

will be tested by parameters estimated in the linear growth curve model and the last hypothesis will

be tested by comparing the fit of the linear growth curve model with the fit of a model which adds

covariances between the disturbances (this last model will be described later).

Thus, the following parameters will be tested in the first linear growth model: First, the

relationships between the stable components will be tested by the correlation between the intercept

factors of optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being in the unspecified model (see Figure 27).

In the model including pessimism and worrying the correlation between both intercept factors

means that the starting points of both trajectories are correlated. Thus, if a person is above the mean

in pessimism, the person tends to be above the mean in worrying.

Second, the relationship between initial optimism/pessimism and slow changes in subjective

well-being (see Direct Model in Figure 28) will be tested by the regression coefficient of the path

from the intercept factor of optimism (pessimism) to the slope factor of subjective well-being. This

means that in the model including pessimism and worrying the starting point of pessimism predicts

the rate of change for worrying. In other words a high level of pessimism leads to a higher than

average increase in worrying. In the Direct Model there are also paths from the intercept factors of

pessimism and worrying to their respective slope factors, which implies that pre-existing

differences are partialled out.

Third, the relationship of two trends – the trend for optimism/pessimism and the trend for

subjective well-being – can be tested by the correlation of both slope factors (see Figure 27).

However, it is better to control for pre-existing differences and to adjust for potential bottom- and

ceiling effects and to test the partial correlation (correlation between the residuals in Figure 28). In

the pessimism-worrying model the partial correlation between both slope factors tells us something

about how individual differences in the pessimism trajectories are related to individual differences

Page 195: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

186

in worrying trajectories (after controlling for the starting points of the pessimism and worrying

trajectories). An example may be that some people are steadily increasing in both in their pessimism

and in their worrying, while other people may change in the opposite direction for both variables.

The final model includes the coping style variables as mediators between the intercept

factors and slope factors (see Figure 29). In addition to mediational effects this model also contain

direct effects; therefore these indirect models are in fact partial mediation models. The disturbances

among the mediators are allowed to freely covary, because there is no theoretical justification that

the independent variables (both intercept factors) should fully explain the covariances between the

coping style variables. As a consequence of these specifications no additional restrictions will be

imposed and the fit of the model will be the same as the model without the mediators.

The mediation hypothesis can be tested in two ways: The first way is to look at the

significance of the separate paths from the intercept factor of optimism/pessimism to coping and

from coping to the slope factor of subjective well-being. The second test is a global test and looks at

the significance of the indirect effects of the intercept factor of optimism/pessimism on the slope

factor of subjective well-being. This is equivalent to testing the significance of the reduction of the

size of the direct effect after including the mediators into the model (Kenny, Kashy & Bolger,

1998). In the latter case, we do not know the specific workings of the relationships.

Results

Descriptive Data

In Table 21 to 25 the means, standard deviations and the cross-sectional intercorrelations of

the summated scores of all the scales are presented for each measurement occasion separately. In

Table 26 the zero order correlations between optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being scale

scores for all time periods are shown. These correlations show the familiar pattern of negative

correlations between optimism and pessimism; the negative correlations of optimism with the

subjective well-being variables (which are of course; ill-health variables) and positive correlations

of pessimism with the subjective well-being variables. All hypotheses will be tested by looking at

the analyses of the latent constructs and the variables constructed with the help of growth curve

modeling.

Page 196: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

187

Table 21

Means, Standard D

eviations, and Intercorrelations at T1

Subscale M

SD

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

Optim

ism/Pessim

ism

1. O

ptimism

(subscale) 5.34

1.09

2. Pessim

ism (subscale)

3.74 1.06

-.23**

3. Optim

ism (com

plete scale; pessim

ism item

s recoded) 4.80

.84 .79**

-.78**

Subjective Well-being

4. D

epression 2.74

.92 -.26**

.35** -.39**

5. Psychosom

atic complaints

2.13 .80

-.04 .19**

-.15** .46**

6. Irritation 3.21

1.17 -.14**

.27** -.26**

.48** .49**

7. W

orrying 3.63

1.54 -.13**

.16** -.18**

.22** .27**

.39**

N

ote. N =

592 (listwise deletion); * p <

.05. ** p < .01.

Table 22

Means, Standard D

eviations, and Intercorrelations at T2

Subscale M

SD

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

Optim

ism/Pessim

ism

1. O

ptimism

(subscale) 5.13

1.04

2. Pessim

ism (subscale)

3.63 1.04

-.31**

3. Optim

ism (com

plete scale; pessim

ism item

s recoded) 4.75

.84 .81**

-.81**

Subjective Well-being

4. D

epression 2.71

.97 -.22**

.39** -.37**

5. Psychosom

atic complaints

2.18 .80

-.02 .20**

-.13** .48**

6. Irritation 3.28

1.10 -.10*

.30** -.25**

.46** .49**

7. W

orrying 3.80

1.41 -.08

.17** -.15**

.23** .28**

.43**

N

ote. N =

537 (listwise deletion); * p <

.05. ** p < .01.

Page 197: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

188

Table 23

Means, Standard D

eviations, and Intercorrelations at T3

Subscale M

SD

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

Optim

ism/Pessim

ism

1. O

ptimism

(subscale) 5.12

1.08

2. Pessim

ism (subscale)

3.65 1.04

-.29**

3. Optim

ism (com

plete scale; pessim

ism item

s recoded) 4.73

.85 .81**

-.79**

Subjective Well-being

4. D

epression 2.70

.98 -.38**

.39** -.48**

5. Psychosom

atic complaints

2.22 .81

-.05 .21**

-.16** .39**

6. Irritation 3.25

1.13 -.17**

.30** -.29**

.46** .47**

7. W

orrying 3.82

1.44 -.10*

.13** -.14**

.22** .31**

.45**

N

ote. N =

490 (listwise deletion); * p <

.05. ** p < .01.

Table 24

Means, Standard D

eviations, and Intercorrelations at T4

Subscale M

SD

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

Optim

ism/Pessim

ism

1. O

ptimism

(subscale) 5.06

1.05

2. Pessim

ism (subscale)

3.54 1.03

-.27**

3. Optim

ism (com

plete scale; pessim

ism item

s recoded) 4.76

.83 .80**

-.79**

Subjective Well-being

4. D

epression 2.66

.94 -.29**

.48** -.48**

5. Psychosom

atic complaints

2.22 .79

-.09* .28**

-.23** .43**

6. Irritation 3.16

1.09 -.22**

.35** -.36**

.49** .49**

7. W

orrying 3.81

1.45 -.15**

.09 -.15**

.20** .31**

.42**

N

ote. N =

467 (listwise deletion); * p <

.05. ** p < .01.

Page 198: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

189

Table 25

Means, Standard D

eviations, and Intercorrelations at T5

Subscale M

SD

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

Optim

ism/Pessim

ism

1. O

ptimism

(subscale) 5.11

1.01

2. Pessim

ism (subscale)

3.49 1.05

-.29**

3. Optim

ism (com

plete scale; pessim

ism item

s recoded) 4.81

.83 .79**

-.81**

Subjective Well-being

4. D

epression 2.64

.98 -.32**

.44** -.47**

5. Psychosom

atic complaints

2.22 .79

-.07 .17**

-.15** .45**

6. Irritation 3.17

1.11 -.18**

.33** -.32**

.49** .43**

7. W

orrying 3.82

1.44 -.12*

.10* -.13**

.29** .28**

.47**

N

ote. N =

482 (listwise deletion); * p <

.05. ** p < .01.

Table 26

Correlation m

atrix of scale scores of optimism

and pessimism

with subjective w

ell-being variables

D

epression

Psychosomatic com

plaints

Irritation

Worrying

T

1 T

2 T

3 T

4 T

5

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

T

1 T

2 T

3 T

4 T

5

T1

T2

T3

T4

T5

Optim

ism

T1

-.28* -.22* -.24* -.26* -.22* -.03

.01 .00

-.02 -.05

-.13* -.10* -.06 -.12* -.14*

-.12* -.10* -.10* -.14* -.10*

T2

-.25* -.23* -.28* -.24* -.23* -.06

-.04 -.05

-.06 -.06

-.09 -.08

-.10 -.16* -.10*

-.07 -.05

-.07 -.12* -.04

T

3 -.30* -.26* -.39* -.28* -.29*

-.07 .02

-.05 -.04

-.04 -.19* -.17* -.18* -.16* -.13*

-.11* -.11* -.09 -.13* -.09

T

4 -.28* -.26* -.33* -.30* -.28*

-.16* -.12* -.12* -.12* -.15* -.19* -.17* -.22* -.25* -.25*

-.12* -.10* -.10 -.15* -.14*

T

5 -.28* -.22* -.26* -.26* -.32*

-.08 -.07

-.07 -.06

-.08 -.12* -.14* -.12* -.18* -.19*

-.10* -.12* -.08 -.15* -.12*

Pessimism

T

1 .36* .34* .36* .36* .34*

.17* .15* .15* .17* .17* .22* .21* .24* .22* .23*

.10* .15* .14* .13* .09

T2

.32* .39* .36* .35* .34* .22* .22* .22* .21* .19*

.24* .28* .28* .25* .23* .07

.14* .12* .10 .08

T

3 .31* .33* .38* .39* .40*

.14* .14* .18* .26* .23* .20* .25* .31* .28* .26*

.07 .11* .11* .15* .11*

T

4 .24* .28* .36* .47* .41*

.20* .22* .19* .29* .26* .16* .25* .27* .31* .34*

.05 .09

.06 .07

.08

T5

.28* .28* .39* .41* .47* .19* .15* .16* .18* .21*

.21* .27* .30* .30* .36* .02

.09 .10* .09

.12* N

ote. N =

423 for depression; N =

420 for psychosomatic com

plaints; N =

421 for irritation; N =

416 for worrying

Page 199: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

190

Measurement models for optimism/pessimism

For a description of the well-being measurement models we refer the reader to Garst, Frese

& Molenaar (in press). In Table 28 the goodness-of-fit measures of the optimism/pessimism

measurement models are shown. A five-wave longitudinal factor model combining optimism and

pessimism into one latent construct (Model 1 in Table 28) did not fit very well. A longitudinal

model with separate constructs for the optimism and the pessimism (Model 2 in Table 28) was

superior in its fit indexes (∆χ2 = 563.23, ∆df = 35, p < 0.00; RMSEA = 0.032; CFI =0.960).

Imposing equality constraints on the factor loadings (Model 3) did not lead to a significant worse

chi-square (∆χ2 = 27.33, ∆df = 24, p < 0.289). However, further restrictions by constraining the

item intercepts to be equal over time (Model 4) produced a significant worse chi-square (∆χ2 =

65.14, ∆df = 24, p < 0.00). The freeing of only two item intercepts in T4 and T5 was sufficient to

obtain a model (Model 4a) which did not differ significantly from the model with completely equal

factor loadings (Model 3). In summary, full measurement invariance was not reached, but only

minor violations were noticed and partial measurement is a sufficient condition to make the

optimism/pessimism constructs comparable over time.

Table 28

Goodness-of-Fit Measures for Measurement Models Optimism/pessimism

Model χ2 p df RMSEA AIC CFI

1 One factor model 1601.93 0.000 650 0.062 2021.93 0.882

2 Two factor model 851.45 0.000 615 0.032 1341.45 0.960

Difference of 2 and 1 750.48* 0.000 35

3 Equal factor loadings 884.19 0.000 639 0.032 1326.19 0.960

Difference of 3 and 2 32.74 0.110 24

4 Equal loadings and intercepts 949.37 0.000 663 0.034 1343.37 0.953

Difference of 4 and 3 65.18* 0.000 24

4a Equal loadings and intercepts 921.06 0.000 661 0.032 1319.06 0.957

Difference of 4a and 3 36.87 0.024 22

5 Equal latent means 972.59 0.000 669 0.035 1354.59 0.952

Difference of 5 and 4a 51.53* 0.000 8

Note: * Chi square difference test is significant (α = 0.01)

Page 200: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

191

Stability of the latent means and individual differences

A model (Model 5 in Table 28) restricting the latent means of optimism and pessimism to be

equal over time could be rejected (∆χ2 = 51.53, ∆df = 8, p < 0.00). In Table 29 the means and

standard deviations of the latent constructs optimism and pessimism are shown.

Table 29

Means and Standard Deviations of Latent Constructs Optimism and Pessimism

Optimism Pessimism

Mean SD Mean SD

T1 5.18

(.31)

1.01 2.32

(.23)

.59

T2 4.98

(.30)

.94 2.26

(.22)

.58

T3 4.95

(.30)

.97 2.29

(.23)

.58

T4 4.93

(.30)

.91 2.23

(.22)

.56

T5 5.02

(.30)

.88 2.18

(.22)

.58

Note: Standard errors between parentheses.

The means of optimism consecutively decreased over time; only in T5 there was a small increase.

However, the changes in means seem small in relation to the standard deviations of latent optimism.

Remarkably, the latent means of pessimism also decreased over time (except in T2), but again the

effect sizes were rather modest in comparison with the variances of latent pessimism. The latent

means of the subjective well-being variables were almost stable (reported in Garst et al, in press).

In Table 30 the correlations between the latent constructs optimism and pessimism are given

for all measurement occasions. The synchronous correlations between optimism and pessimism fall

within the range -.32 and -.47. Not only can optimism and pessimism be regarded as independent

constructs, the correlations were also moderate and do not warrant a treatment as a single construct.

Table 30 also presents the stability coefficients of optimism and pessimism. The latent correlation

between T1 and T5 is .62 for optimism and .53 for pessimism. The stability coefficients for the

same time-interval for the subjective well-being variables were .57 for depression, .76 for

Page 201: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

192

psychosomatic complaints, .62 for irritation and .48 for worrying. It seems that the construct

psychosomatic complaints was more stable than either optimism or pessimism, whereas worrying

seems less stable. Overall, there was little stability in individual differences across a long time

frame. This information is important because growth curve models require changes in the relative

positions of subjects over time and it is quite clear from Table 30 that over the T1 – T5 (four years

plus nine months) period many changes have taken place.

Table 30

Correlations between Latent Optimism and Pessimism Constructs

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10)

(1) T1 optimism 1.00

(2) T1 pessimism -0.32 1.00

(3) T2 optimism 0.72 -0.36 1.00

(4) T2 pessimism -0.28 0.64 -0.46 1.00

(5) T3 optimism 0.74 -0.36 0.77 -0.37 1.00

(6) T3 pessimism -0.24 0.63 -0.35 0.79 -0.41 1.00

(7) T4 optimism 0.62 -0.39 0.71 -0.45 0.77 -0.48 1.00

(8) T4 pessimism -0.20 0.48 -0.28 0.63 -0.33 0.77 -0.40 1.00

(9) T5 optimism 0.62 -0.33 0.72 -0.39 0.73 -0.37 0.77 -0.32 1.00

(10) T5 pessimism -0.19 0.53 -0.21 0.59 -0.32 0.66 -0.40 0.70 -0.47 1.00

Goodness of fit of growth curve models

In Table 31 the goodness-of-fit measures of the growth curve models are shown. Each

model includes either optimism or pessimism and one of the well-being constructs. The first model

in each case is the maximum model and functions as a baseline model, because all the latent

constructs are allowed to covary freely and the only restrictions arise from the specifications of the

measurement model. All maximum models fit very well: all CFI values are at least .95 (Hu &

Bentler, 1999) and all values of the RMSEA are below .035.

Page 202: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

193

Table 31

Goodness-of-fit Measures for Structural Models.

Optimism Pessimism

χ2 df RMSEA AIC CFI χ2 df RMSEA AIC CFI

Depression 1 Maximum 1287.74 842 .033 1857.74 .959 1266.33 842 .032 1836.33 .958 2 Linear LGC 1450.66 919 .034 1866.66 .951 1398.33 919 .033 1814.33 .953 Difference 2 and 1 162.92* 77 132.00* 77 3 Correlated resid. 1421.95 914 .034 1847.95 .953 1351.43 914 .031 1777.43 .956 Difference 3 and 2 28.71* 5 46.90* 5 Difference 3 and 1 134.21* 72 85.10 72 Psychosomatic C. 1 Maximum 927.70 635 .031 1461.70 .966 866.10 635 .027 1400.10 .969 2 Linear LGC 1061.32 712 .032 1441.32 .961 987.68 712 .028 1367.68 .964 Difference 2 and 1 133.62* 77 121.58* 77 3 Correlated resid. 1052.85 707 .032 1442.85 .961 977.58 707 .028 1367.58 .964 Difference 3 and 2 8.47 5 10.10 5 Difference 3 and 1 125.15* 72 111.48* 72 Irritation 1 Maximum 1728.86 1076 .035 2330.86 .949 1598.87 1076 .031 2200.87 .956 2 Linear LGC 1883.70 1153 .036 2331.70 .944 1697.11 1153 .031 2145.11 .954 Difference 2 and 1 154.84* 77 98.24 77 3 Correlated resid. 1874.91 1148 .036 2332.91 .945 1684.61 1148 .031 2142.61 .955 Difference 3 and 2 8.79 5 12.50 5 Difference 3 and 1 146.05* 72 85.74 72 Worrying 1 Maximum 987.43 635 .034 1521.43 .963 942.37 635 .031 1476.37 .965 2 Linear LGC 1120.49 712 .034 1500.49 .958 1042.93 712 .031 1422.93 .962 Difference 2 and 1 133.06* 77 100.56 77 3 Correlated resid. 1116.95 707 .034 1506.95 .958 1039.18 707 .031 1429.18 .962 Difference 3 and 2 3.54 5 3.75 5 Difference 3 and 1 129.52* 72 96.81 72 NOTE: Linear LGC: linear latent growth curve model refers to unspecified model (Figure 27), direct model (Figure 28), and mediational model (Figure 29). Correlated resid.: model with time-specific correlations between residuals of optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being variables (see Figure 30). * Chi-square Difference test is significant (alpha = 0.01). N = 423 for models including depression; N = 420 for psychosomatic complaints; N = 421 for irritation; N = 416 for worrying.

The second model displayed in Table 31 is a linear growth model. As noted before the unspecified,

direct and mediational models (see again Figure 27, 28, and 29) yielded the same fit measures, so

the fit measures of the second model in Table 31 refers to all three models. The chi-square

difference tests are all significant, which indicate that they are worse than the maximum model.

This is not surprising given the fact that the maximum model imposes no restrictions on the

Page 203: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

194

structural relationships. However, the hypotheses based models are more parsimonious. This has

positive effects on the AIC and so many of these hypotheses based models show lower values of the

AIC. The other fit indices, for example the RMSEA and the CFI make it possible to conclude that a

linear growth model can be considered a reasonable solution. Furthermore, we note that the linear

growth curves can explain a large amount of the total variance in the latent constructs. The lowest

amount of explained variance was .72 for worrying and pessimism and the highest one was .84 for

psychosomatic complaints

The third model displayed in Table 31 is equivalent to the second model except that it

allows the disturbances for each measurement occasion to covary (compare Figure 30). The chi-

square difference tests indicate that the disturbances are necessary only for the models that include

depression. We come back to this point, when we discuss the hypotheses.

In summary, growth curve models yielded acceptable goodness of fit measures and

specifying additional covariances for the disturbances was only necessary for models including

depression.

Prerequisites For Testing the Hypothesis

In addition to yield acceptable goodness of fit measures, the models should be parsimonious

and not contain too many parameters. Most important is that the variances of the slope factors are

significant, because this implies that there are differences between people in the rate they change. In

Table 32 the estimates of the variances and the mean of the slope factors are shown. All variances

are significant. However, two estimates of the mean of the slope factors were not significant (for

psychosomatic complaints and worrying), indicating that the population growth curve for these

variables could be flat (population slope is zero) and no mean changes could be detected. The mean

slopes of both optimism and pessimism were negative, indicating that the means decreased, a

pattern that is consistent with the results found in the measurement models.

Page 204: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

195

Table 32

Variance and Mean estimates of slope factors.

Variance Mean

Optimism 0.68** -0.23**

Pessimism 0.36** -0.24**

Depression 1.14** -0.21**

Psychosomatic complaints 0.30** 0.07

Irritation 0.70** -0.15**

Worrying 3.14** 0.12

** z value > 1.96 (z value: ratio between parameter estimate

and its standard error)

* 1.96 < z value > 1.65

Because the relationships between optimism/pessimism and the subjective well-being

variables are central in this paper it is useful to take a look at the zero order correlations before

discussing the specific hypothesis. The correlations of the latent variables for all time periods of our

study are shown in Table 33. Remarkable are the high synchronous correlations between pessimism

and depression, psychosomatic complaints, and irritation. High are also the negative synchronous

correlations between optimism and depression.

Hypotheses Tested With Direct Models

Relationships of the Stable Components of Optimism/Pessimism and Subjective Well-Being. It is

hypothesized that the stable components of optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being are

related. The stable components are represented by the intercept factors, which represent the starting

values of the linear growth curves at the first measurement wave and Table 34 shows that the

intercepts of optimism and pessimism were significantly correlated with the well-being factors

(with the exception of one). The correlations in Table 34 were higher than the synchronous

Page 205: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

196

Table 33

Correlation M

atrix of Latent C

onstructs Optim

ism and Pessim

ism w

ith Subjective Well-being V

ariables

Depression

Psychosom

atic

complaints

Irritation

W

orrying

T1 T

2 T3 T

4 T5

T

1 T2 T

3 T4 T

5

T1 T

2 T3 T

4 T5

T

1 T2 T

3 T4 T

5

Optim

ism

T1

-.35 -.23 -.29 -.31 -.29 -.06 .01 -.04 -.03 -.11

-.16 -.14 -.09 -.15 -.19 -.14 -.15 -.12 -.16 -.12

T

2 -.27 -.26 -.32 -.27 -.27

-.07 -.04 -.10 -.07 -.09 -.08 -.12 -.14 -.19 -.16

-.07 -.09 -.10 -.15 -.07

T

3 -.33 -.29 -.46 -.33 -.38

-.12 -.03 -.15 -.08 -.13 -.16 -.18 -.20 -.17 -.17

-.11 -.13 -.09 -.12 -.09

T

4 -.31 -.30 -.38 -.37 -.36

-.19 -.13 -.18 -.17 -.23 -.18 -.20 -.23 -.28 -.32

-.13 -.12 -.11 -.15 -.16

T

5 -.32 -.24 -.32 -.32 -.43

-.10 -.10 -.10 -.08 -.15 -.10 -.15 -.13 -.19 -.24

-.10 -.14 -.10 -.15 -.15

Pessimism

T

1 .43 .41 .41 .44 .39

.25 .23 .23 .23 .23 .33 .27 .31 .29 .30

.14 .21 .19 .15 .10

T

2 .40 .47 .47 .47 .42

.29 .27 .26 .21 .19 .31 .34 .33 .29 .27

.11 .21 .16 .13 .09

T

3 .36 .38 .47 .49 .48

.23 .23 .30 .37 .32 .25 .27 .33 .31 .29

.08 .14 .13 .16 .09

T

4 .29 .34 .44 .63 .49

.25 .27 .24 .35 .31 .24 .30 .28 .39 .40

.06 .10 .05 .06 .04

T

5 .33 .30 .46 .52 .56

.29 .24 .24 .26 .28 .21 .26 .28 .32 .38

-.02 .05 .06 .05 .06

Note. N

= 423 for depression; N

= 420 for psychosom

atic complaints; N

= 421 for irritation; N

= 416 for w

orrying.

Page 206: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

197

correlations in T1 in Table 33. This can be explained by the decomposition in systematic and state

variance, which had a similar effect as the well-known attenuation due to uncorrelated measurement

error. The highest correlations in Table 34 involved pessimism and particularly the correlations

between the intercepts of pessimism and depression (r = .54) and irritation (r = .44) were high.

Thus, there is some support for our first hypothesis that there is a correlation between the highly

stable components of optimism/pessimism and well-being. This speaks for genetic or early

childhood components that are not changed much in later life.

Table 34

Correlations between intercept factors of optimism/pessimism and

intercept factors of subjective well-being variables.

Optimism Pessimism

Depression -.36** .54**

Psychosomatic complaints -.06 .33**

Irritation -.19** .44**

Worrying -.15** .26**

** z value > 1.96 (z value: ratio between parameter estimate and

its standard error).

Relationships of Initial Optimism/Pessimism to Changes in Subjective Well-Being. Were initial

levels of optimism and pessimism predictive for changes in subjective well-being? The results are

shown in Table 35 (first column, “total effects”).

There were significant effects of initial optimism (as represented by the intercept factor) on

the slope factors of depression (-.19) and psychosomatic complaints (-.20). Both effects were

negative, which means that people with higher starting points of their trajectories of optimism

tended to have smaller (than the average) slopes for depression and psychosomatic complaints.

Furthermore, there was a significant negative effect (-.21) of initial pessimism on the slope factor of

worrying. The direction of this effect was not anticipated: it means that pessimists at T1 tended to

have a decrease in worrying (compared to the average). At first sight this suggests that worrying

helped in some way to deal with pessimism (we return to this later). The other paths were not

significant.

Page 207: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

198

It is important to note that these are partial regression coefficients, because there is also a

path from the intercept factor of the specific well-being variable to the well-being slope. This means

that pre-existing differences in well-being are controlled for.

The paths from the intercept factor of well-being to the slope factor of well-being were

negative (just one is not significant) which means that people with a low degree of well-being

increased their well-being (compared to the average person). This may be a result of a floor and

ceiling effect or the result of some kind of adaptation process which for example also underlies

cyclical mood swings (Solomon & Corbit, 1974).

Relationships of Trends in Optimism and Subjective Well-Being. Were systematic changes in

optimism and pessimism related to systematic changes in well-being? The systematic changes are

represented in a linear growth curve model by the slope factor. Table 36 shows the correlations

between the slope factors of optimism/pessimism and well-being (first and fourth column, headed

“Unspecified Model”).

Table 36

(Partial) correlations between slope factors of optimism/pessimism and

slope factors of subjective well-being

Optimism Pessimism

Unspecified

Model

Direct

Model

Mediation

Model

Unspecified

Model

Direct

Model

Mediation

Model

Depression -.15 -.23 -.20 .83** .91** .93**

Psychosomatic

complaints

.21 .00 .07 .36* .31* .29

Irritation -.19 -.34** -.33** .71** .73** .78**

Worrying -.08 -.15 -.17 .28* .26* .29**

** z value > 1.96 (z value: ratio between parameter estimate and its standard error).

The correlations were estimated in the unspecified models (see again Figure 27). None of the

correlations between the slope factors of optimism and the slope factors of the well-being variables

were significant. This is different for correlations between changes in pessimism and changes in

well-being. All of the correlations were significant and positive. In the direct model (see Figure 28)

pre-existing differences in optimism/pessimism and well-being are controlled for and the partial

Page 208: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

199

correlations between the slope factors were tested (second and fifth column, headed “Direct

Model”). There was only one significant partial correlation between the slopes factor of optimism

and the slope factors of the well-being variables (for irritation: -.34). All partial correlations

between the slope factors of pessimism and well-being were positive and significant. There were

very strong significant partial correlations between slope factor of pessimism and the slope factors

of depression (.91), and irritation (.73). This speaks for a relationship between the changeable parts:

The trajectories of pessimism seem very parallel with the trajectories of both depression and

irritation. The third and the sixth column of Table 36 will be discussed later when the results of the

spurious model will be reported.

Relationships between Fast Changes of Optimism/Pessimism and Fast Changes of Well-Being.

Were short-term changes in optimism/pessimism related to short term changes in well-being? As

mentioned before, there is evidence in Table 31 only for models including depression. However, in

these models the pattern of correlations did not show a high consistency and for some measurement

occasions the correlations were not significant. For the other well-being variables there was no

significant improvement of fit when these short-term changes correlations were added into the

models. Overall there seems not much support for the hypothesis that fast changes of

optimism/pessimism and well-being were related.

Mediational Model.

The mediational model (see again Figure 29) includes all paths from the intercept factors

(the independent variables in the mediating model) to the coping style variables (mediators) and all

paths from the mediators to the two slope factors (the outcome variables). The covariance between

the disturbance terms of the slope factor was estimated. All covariances between the disturbances of

the coping style variables were also estimated. Additionally, the direct effect of the intercept factor

on the slope factor was estimated, so in fact the model only specifies a partial mediational model.

This model is not imposing further restrictions compared with the direct linear growth curve model.

Therefore, the fit is exactly the same as the direct growth curve model (see Model 2 in Table 31).

The results of the mediational model consist of two parts: first the indirect effects will be

described, and next the direct effects will be discussed.

Page 209: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

200

Indirect effects. We will first describe the separate paths and then we will present the results

of the global test of the indirect effects. In Tables 37 to 40 the regression coefficients are shown for

the mediator models. The first entry of Table 37 implies that the path from the intercept factor of

optimism to planning is -.04 within the model that includes depression (note that there are different

paths depending upon which well-being variable is included in the model). All our models

controlled for pre-existing subjective well-being.

The results show that optimism had significant paths on emotional-focused coping and on

problem-focused coping (regardless of which well-being variable is included in the model). Thus

controlling for initial well-being produced a significant effect of optimism on emotional focused

coping. It is remarkable that both forms of coping that are usually conceptualized to work

differentially were positively affected by optimism.

Pessimism had a negative path on planning and positive paths on emotional coping and

wishful thinking (again regardless of which well-being variable was included in the model). These

are all passive forms of coping: little planning, emotional focused and wishful thinking. This was

expected from coping theory and literature.

In Table 38 the standardized regression coefficients are shown for the paths from the

intercept factor of subjective well-being to the coping style variables. Initial well-being was a strong

predictor of self-criticism, emotional-focused coping, seeking social support, and wishful thinking

over and beyond the effects of initial optimism/pessimism. These effects were higher for depression

and irritation and lower for psychosomatic complaints and worrying.

Worrying showed a different pattern, however, that may be very important. Worrying

showed a positive effect on planning and on problem-focused coping. Worrying can be interpreted

to be an activating emotion and therefore worrying affects that people become more active in their

coping styles (again, controlling for initial pessimism/optimism effects).

Initial depression, psychosomatic complaints and irritation had strong effects on wishful

thinking and to a lesser extent on emotional-focused coping. The standardized effects for initial

depression on wishful thinking were .49 and .35 in the optimism and pessimism model respectively.

The smaller effect in the pessimism model can be explained by the inclusion of the pessimism

intercept factor, which presumably had shared predictive power. The effects seemed smaller in the

pessimism models, probably because pessimism had a stronger affective component and partialling

out the effects of initial pessimism remove some of the impact of initial well-being. However, these

effects of initial well-being on coping style did not occur in the worrying model.

Page 210: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

201

Table 37

Standardized Regressioncoefficients of Paths from

Intercept Factors Optim

ism/Pessim

ism to C

oping Style Variables.

Coping Style V

ariables O

ptimism

Pessim

ism

M

odel

Depression

Model

Psychosom

Model

Irritation

Model

Worrying

Model

Depression

Model

Psychosom

Model

Irritation

Model

Worrying

Planning -0.04

-0.02 -0.01

0.03 -0.20**

-0.18** -0.23**

-0.27**

Self-criticism

-0.04 -0.08

-0.06 -0.08

-0.03 0.03

-0.02 0.04

Em

otional-focused coping 0.27**

0.17** 0.22**

0.16** 0.17**

0.17** 0.18**

0.27**

Seeking social support 0.10*

0.06 0.08

0.08 -0.06

-0.03 -0.04

-0.03

Problem-focused coping

0.16** 0.16**

0.17** 0.20**

-0.03 -0.08

-0.08 -0.11*

Wishful thinking

0.20** 0.05

0.11** 0.05

0.13* 0.22**

0.21** 0.32**

Note: Psychosom

: psychosomatic com

plaints; the effects of intercept factors optimism

/pessimism

on coping style variables were estim

ated in

separate models each containing either optim

ism or pessim

ism variables

** z value > 1.96 (z value: ratio betw

een parameter estim

ate and its standard error).

Page 211: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

202

Table 38

Standardized Regressioncoefficients of Paths from

Intercept Factors Subjective Well-being to C

oping Style Variables.

Coping Style V

ariables D

epression Psychosom

Irritation

Worrying

M

odel

Optim

ism

Model

Pessimism

Model

Optim

ism

Model

Pessimism

Model

Optim

ism

Model

Pessimism

Model

Optim

ism

Model

Pessimism

Planning -0.06

0.06 -0.03

0.03 0.02

0.12** 0.30**

0.37**

Self-criticism

0.14** 0.18**

0.08 0.07

0.16** 0.17**

0.07 0.08

Em

otional-focused coping 0.31**

0.13* 0.28**

0.21** 0.29**

0.16** -0.03

-0.12**

Seeking social support 0.16**

0.16** 0.16**

0.17** 0.14**

0.14** 0.19**

0.19**

Problem-focused coping

-0.01 -0.05

0.07 0.09

0.07 0.07

0.22** 0.22**

Wishful thinking

0.49** 0.35**

0.41** 0.33**

0.40** 0.29**

0.14** 0.06

Note: Psychosom

: psychosomatic com

plaints; the effects of the intercepts factor on coping style variables were estim

ated both in models

containing either optimism

or pessimism

.

* 1.96 < z value >

1.65 (z value: ratio between param

eter estimate and its standard error)

** z value > 1.96

Page 212: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

203

Table 39

Standardized Regressioncoefficients of Paths from

Coping Style V

ariables to Slope Factors of Subjective Well-being.

Planning

Self-criticism

Em

otion-focused Social support

Problem-focused

Wishful thinking

M

odel

Optim

Model

Pessim

Model

Optim

Model

Pessim

Model

Optim

Model

Pessim

Model

Optim

Model

Pessim

Model

Optim

Model

Pessim

Model

Optim

Model

Pessim

Depression

-0.13* -0.15**

0.00 0.01

0.19** 0.15*

0.09 0.09

-0.13* -0.15*

0.11 0.07

Psychosom

-0.14 -0.15

0.04 0.09

0.07 0.01

0.10 0.11

-0.10 -0.15

-0.12 -0.13

Irritation -0.04

-0.06 0.05

0.05 0.02

0.01 -0.06

-0.06 -0.11

-0.12 -0.02

0.01

Worrying

0.08 0.05

0.06 0.08

0.14* 0.16**

0.03 -0.01

-0.01 -0.03

-0.07 -0.02

Note: O

ptim: optim

ism; pessim

: pessimism

; psychosom: psychosom

atic complaints; the effects of coping style variables on slope factors of

subjective well-being w

ere estimated in m

odels containing either optimism

or pessimism

.

* 1.96 < z value >

1.65 (z value: ratio between param

eter estimate and its standard error)

** z value > 1.96

Page 213: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

204

Table 40

Standardized Regressioncoefficients of Paths from Coping Style Variables

to Slope Factors Optimism/pessimism.

Coping style variables

Model

Depression

Model

Psychosom

Model

Irritation

Model

Worrying

Planning

Optimism 0.20** 0.22** 0.20** 0.24**

Pessimism -0.37** -0.35** -0.37** -0.35**

Self-criticism

Optimism 0.05 0.02 0.04 0.03

Pessimism -0.05 -0.03 -0.02 -0.02

Emotional-focused coping

Optimism -0.15 -0.13 -0.09 -0.14

Pessimism 0.06 0.03 -0.01 0.03

Seeking social support

Optimism -0.11 -0.11 -0.10 -0.10

Pessimism 0.11 0.12 0.12 0.12

Problem-focused coping

Optimism 0.10 0.12 0.12 0.12

Pessimism -0.02 -0.07 -0.07 -0.06

Wishful thinking

Optimism 0.23* 0.23** 0.18* 0.16

Pessimism 0.03 0.11 0.12 0.08

Note: Psychosom: psychosomatic complaints; the effects of coping style

variables on slope factors of optimism/pessimism subjective well-being

were estimated in separate models each containing one of the subjective

well-being variables.

* 1.96 < z value > 1.65 (z value: ratio between parameter estimate and

its standard error)

** z value > 1.96

Page 214: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

205

Initial depression and irritation had significant positive effects on self-criticism. Initial well-being is

also a significant predictor for seeking social support (for all well-being variables).

Table 39 shows the paths from coping to the well-being slope factors – the second part of

the mediator analysis. The first entry in this table -.13 means that planning led to a reduction in the

change of depression over time (compared with the average person). In general, planning affected

depression negatively, emotional focused coping affected it positively and problem-focused coping

negatively. In other words, more active forms of coping – such as planning and problem-focused

coping – reduced depression, while an attempt to cope with emotions increased depression.

Psychosomatic complaints and irritation were not significantly affected by coping styles. Worrying

was affected by emotional-focused coping. Apparently, people are not really dealing with their

emotions with this coping style but tend increasingly to think and worry about the stressors

involved. Thus, there were mediator effects for the three coping styles: Planning, emotion-focused,

and problem-focused coping. All three were affected by optimism and pessimism and affected in

turn the development of depression and worrying. An interesting result was that both optimism and

pessimism affected emotional-focused coping positively.

Next we will describe the global tests of the significance of the indirect effects. The results

are shown in the third column of Table 35. There were two significant results: Both initial optimism

and initial pessimism had positive effects on the slope factor of depression (.07 and .06,

respectively). The positive indirect effect of optimism on depression was not anticipated. As

presented above, optimists had a preference for problem-focused coping (-.16) and this lead to a

reduction in depression (-.13). However, this anticipated effect was outweighed by the fact that

optimists also used more emotional-focused coping which increased depression (.27 × .19 = .051).

Furthermore, optimists used also more wishful thinking and although not significant this increased

depression (.20 × .11 = .022). All effects combined were significantly positive. However, the direct

effect of initial optimism on depression was much stronger (-.26) as can be seen in the second

column of Table 35. The direct effects will be discussed later. The second significant indirect effect

was initial pessimism had a significant positive effect on depression (.06). Pessimist planned less

and planning reduced depression (-.20 × -.15 = .03). Furthermore, pessimists had a small preference

for emotional-focused coping and this increased depression (.17 × .15 = .0255). Overall, there was

some evidence that initial optimism/pessimism had indirect effects via coping styles on subjective

well-being.

Direct effects. The mediation models were specified as partial mediation models by also

allowing direct paths from the intercept factors to the slope factors. In the second column of Table

Page 215: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

206

35 the direct effects of the intercept factors optimism/pessimism on the slope factors are displayed.

There were three significant direct effects: initial optimism had negative effects on the slope factors

of depression (-.26) and psychosomatic complaints (-.19). Initial pessimism resulted in an decrease

of worrying (-.24). All three direct effect reported previously in the discussion of the direct models

appeared to also as direct effects in the mediational models.

Summarizing the effects of the mediational models it is clear that the direct effects were

stronger than the indirect effects. A remarkable finding is that initial optimism had both significant

negative direct and significant positive effects indirect effects, resulting in a net effect which was

significantly negative (-.19) in first column of Table 35.

Spurious model

Coping style variables can act as a third variable in explaining the correlation between

changes in optimism/pessimism and changes in subjective well-being. The correlated trend-trend

hypothesis was confirmed for pessimism and all the well-being variables and for the trend in

optimism and irritation. In Table 36 in the third and sixth column the partial correlations between

both slope factors are shown. There was no evidence for a reduction of the size of the partial

correlation in the indirect models in comparison to partial correlation in the direct models (see

second and fifth column). Standard structural equation software does not allow testing for the

significance of the difference between two free parameters (the zero and partial correlations), but in

our case testing was not necessary: Four partial correlations were even higher than the zero order

correlations. Thus, introducing the coping style variables into the models did not reduce the size of

the correlation. This means that a spurious correlation model does not apply to the data. The failure

of the spurious model can be explained by the lack of significant paths from the coping style

variables to both slope factors. It was already shown that most effects of the coping style variables

on the slope factors of well-being were small and nonsignificant, similar results could be noticed

(see Table 40) for the effects of the coping style variables on the slope factors of

optimism/pessimism. There were, however, three exceptions. Planning had a strong negative effect

on the slope factor of pessimism (.36 on average for the four models) and a moderately strong

positive effect on the slope factor of optimism (.22 on average). The third exception was the

positive effects of wishful thinking on the slope factor of optimism (not in model containing

worrying).

Page 216: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

207

Discussion

Consistent with previous research, it was found that a two-factor model for the LOT scales

fitted the data much better than a one-factor model. In addition, different relations with well-being

variables were found: pessimism correlated stronger with depression and irritation than optimism.

Further evidence against a one-dimensional interpretation consisted of the declining trends of the

latent means of both optimism and pessimism. The downward trend of pessimism was not

anticipated, because East Germans were described by newspapers to be initially euphoric and in

spite of the many worries that beset them a strong positive outlook for the future dominated

(Kunhke, 1991). After political unification, disillusionment in eastern Germany rose sharply

(Britannica, 1999). The anticipated rapid economic recovery seemed to be an illusion and

expectations for the future were getting more realistic. This picture is not supported by the trends in

optimism and pessimism. Apparently, optimism and pessimism cannot be conceived as barometers

for the historical climate, but instead measure only personal tendencies that are changed by very

personal situations rather than by the global climate within a country.

Our use of growth curve models allowed us to decompose psychological constructs into

several components, differing in stability. Optimism showed both a large portion of systematic

developmental changes as well as time-specific fluctuations. That there is room for time specific

fluctuations is shown by the relatively low stabilities: The stabilities for the latent constructs

optimism and pessimism were over a period of five years, .62 and .53. Comparable stabilities were

found for the subjective well-being variables, which are not intended to measure traits. Two sources

of evidence could be found that both systematic as well as time-specific changes influenced the

stabilities of optimism and pessimism. First, the pattern of correlations demonstrated that scores

more distant in time correlated lower than observation more proximal in time. This indicated the

presence of systematic changes. Second, growth curve models fitted the data well. We found both

interindividual differences in growth trajectories as well as sizeable state variances for each time

period. The lower than expected stabilities for optimism and pessimism reconciles the trait

interpretation of Carver and Scheier with the ‘outcome’ interpretation of DeNeve.

Optimism/pessimism could both be treated as important predictors for subjective well-being as well

as the changes in optimism/pessimism could be treated as partly predictable outcomes variables.

Systematic slow changes in especially pessimism developed parallel with changes in depression.

Both aspects of optimism and pessimism are supported by the data and growth curve models are

well suited to treat these aspects in an adequate manner.

Page 217: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

208

In our growth curve models we studied the stable aspects, the slowly changing and the fast

changing components of optimism/pessimism. By concentrating on the systematic changes (slope

factor), optimism/pessimism is treated as a potential outcome variable, in the same way as the well-

being variables. In terms of our hypotheses we found the following results.

The stable components of pessimism and all subjective well-being variables, as indicated by

their intercept factors, were significantly positively correlated. Much weaker negative correlations

were found for the stable component of optimism and its correlation with psychosomatic complaints

was almost zero and nonsignificant. This supports the idea that negative affectivity and neuroticism

is related to pessimism (Burke, Brief & George, 1993). Although we do not claim that our measures

of subjective well-being are completely valid measures of either negative affectivity or neuroticism,

we think especially depression and irritation can be considered as reasonable proxies for those

constructs (Burke et al, 1993). The relation between pessimism and negative feelings may be

explained by the theory of Gray (1991, 1994), who argues that there are two different physiological

nervous systems. The first one is the Behavior Approach System and is affected by rewards. The

second system, the Behavior Inhibition System, is stimulated by innate fear stimuli and signals of

punishments. Gray claims that much of the individual differences in personality can be explained by

differences in these two underlying brain system. Pessimism and negative feelings may be related

because both are produced by the part of the nervous system that deals with avoiding negative

stimuli. It is important to note that our finding of the relationship between pessimism and

depression was based on well fitting measurement models which treated pessimism and depression

as independent constructs. So pessimism is not just a manifestation of depression.

Was initial optimism/pessimism predictive for changes in well-being? Initial optimism

predicted both declining tendencies in growth curves for depression and psychosomatic complaints.

Remarkably, although optimism was initially less correlated with both depression and

psychosomatic complaints than pessimism, it was a better predictor for changes in these well-being

variables. This suggests that other mechanisms than the emotional system are involved as causal

agents. This may be related to the fact that optimists tend to be more open to new experiences or

new stimulation (Marshall, Wortman, Kusulas, Hervig, & Vickers, 1992), which suggest that they

deal more actively with stressors. Initial pessimism predicted declining trajectories in worrying, a

result which was not expected.

The hypothesis that the trends in pessimism and well-being were related were supported as

indicated by the very high correlations between the slope factors for pessimism and depression and

irritation. This was not the case for worrying and psychosomatic complaints. Changes in optimism

were not parallel with changes in well-being, only the slope factors of optimism and irritation were

Page 218: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

209

significantly negative correlated. Again the stronger link between negative feelings and pessimism

was supported. However in this case we are not dealing with either genetic or early childhood

causes of the relationship but with current trends that co-occur. Most probably they feed upon each

other in a sort of feedback cycle: The more one is pessimistic; the more one gets depressed and

irritated; the more one gets irritated and depressed; the more one becomes pessimistic (cf: Beck’s

cognitive triad; Beck, 1976).

There was little support for the hypothesis that the fast changes of optimism/pessimism and

fast changes of well-being were related. The models that allowed correlations between disturbances

did not improve the goodness of fit of the models (the only exception was depression).

The dominant explanation for the link between optimism and subjective well-being is that

differences in coping mediates the relationships between optimism/pessimism and well-being. We

tested this with two strategies: First by testing the fit of a mediational model (this is a “global test”)

and by inspecting the significance of the individual paths. The global test revealed two significant

indirect effects: Both initial optimism and initial pessimism had positive significant indirect effects

on changes in depression. Both indirect effects were either canceled out or outweighed by opposite

direct effects. Inspecting the indirect paths taught us that optimists used both more emotional-

focused and problem-focused coping which had contradictory effects on changes in depression.

However, the positive effects of emotional-focused coping on depression outweighed the negative

effects of problem-focused coping, resulting in a positive indirect effect. Pessimists used also more

emotional-focused coping and used less planning. Both resulted in positive changes in depression.

In general the mediating role of coping styles was not as strongly supported as expected. The effects

of coping styles variables on the slope factors of well-being were very small and most coefficients

were not significant. Since in general coping styles neither predicted the slope factors of

optimism/pessimism, coping styles could not be considered as the common cause for explaining the

relations between changes in optimism/pessimism and changes in well-being. However, we found

some evidence for a third variable explanation for planning. This coping style promoted optimism

and decreased pessimism and also reduced depression. The effectiveness of planning is consistent

with the importance of pro-active coping as described by Aspinwall & Taylor (1997). Important

aspects of proactive coping model are the temporal sequencing of coping efforts and the use of

feedback. Planning and proactive coping may be more effective because it facilitates the elicitation

of feedback. Information of what coping attempts were successful and what strategies did not work

can be used for adapting coping strategies. This may result in more positive control expectations,

which may be generalized to a more optimistic outlook for the future.

Page 219: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

210

An alternative explanation for the role of pessimism could be that its predictive power

consisted of shard variance with constructs like neuroticism and negative affectivity. However; this

interpretation is unlikely as all models controlled for pre-existing levels of subjective well-being.

In our studies we also found a clear effect of well-being on coping. Initial subjective well-

being was a good predictor for coping styles: In general feeling bad predicted a strong preference

for wishful thinking and to a lesser extent for emotional-focused coping and seeking social support.

This was not the case for initial worrying that predicted a preference for planning and problem-

focused coping and seeking social support. Apparently, worrying does not just have a negative

effect. One side may be destructive which may be similar to rumination, a concept introduced by

Nolen-Hoeksema (1994). She defined rumination as the preoccupation with one’s depressive

symptoms. Another side of worrying may be more constructive and may be important in the first

stages of proactive coping: recognition of potential stressors and initial appraisal (“Should I be

worried about this?”)(Aspinwall & Taylor, 1997, p.419).

Notwithstanding some significant effects of coping styles on changes in subjective well-

being, the crucial question remains why the effects of coping were rather disappointing. The use of

coping styles instead of coping strategies should make it easier to find strong relations between

optimism/pessimism and well-being. Begley (1998, p. 313) states that “dispositions are not

predictive of single actions, but only of action tendencies”. Optimism, conceived as a disposition,

should be more highly correlated with coping styles (dispositional coping) than with coping

strategies. Fishbein & Ajzen (1975) argued similarly that predictors and criterion should be

measured on the same level of generality.

There are a few notable limitations to this study. First, we did not control for differences in

type and severance/importance of the stressful events respondents selected for providing their

coping responses. There are pros and cons of our approach to measuring coping. As presented

before, we used work stressors that the participants remembered from last week. If the person did

not remember one, we suggested areas which are typically stressful and asked if there were any in

these areas. We had them describe the situation briefly but we did not an analysis of these

descriptions. Our approach of looking at coping styles led us to collapse all the different coping

questionnaires across time into one score. Our measurement models show this to be useful.

However, this approach does not allow to study the details of the coping interaction (Lazarus &

Folkman, 1984), particularly not at the particulars of the stressors and the coping resources

available. Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that the coping responses were determined

by differences in the stressors reported by the subjects. However, because we aggregated the

measures of coping across five occasions we assumed that the stressors are at least representative

Page 220: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

211

for the individual reporting them and, that we, therefore, also measured the typical coping response

of the participants.

The second limitation of this study is that we could not include all variables into one

comprehensive model because this would have exceeded the capacity of the software and would

have produced an unfavorable ratio of sample size to number of free parameters Bentler (1989, p.

6). Also, we could not investigate the additional predictive power of initial optimism over initial

pessimism. On the other hand, we think that even with these limitations, we have been able to

extract interesting findings from these data.

Thirdly, one of the typical problems that beset stress and well-being research is the fact that

most of this research relies on questionnaires and is, therefore, prone to be biased by common

variance (Begley, 1998; Zapf, Dormann & Frese, 1997). However, our growth curve models

reduced this problem in two ways: First, the completely stable part of common method variance

was reduced by partialing out pre-existing values (Dormann, 1999) and second, the time-specific

(unstable) part was modeled by allowing covariances between the disturbance terms (which were

only necessary in models including depression). Theoretically the correlation between linear trends

could be confounded by a linear change in common methods factors. However, this seems very

unlikely.

Finally, a limitation that holds for all passive longitudinal designs is worth mentioning, that

is, that causal inferences cannot be made. Structural Equation Modeling can determine whether a

model is consistent with the data, but there may be other equivalent models that fit the data equally

well. Alternative explanations cannot be ruled out.

Notwithstanding these limitations, this study is unique both in its longitudinal design and

impressive sample: To the best of our knowledge the effects of optimism/pessimism on subjective

well-being have never before been studied over a period stretching five years and including five

measurement waves using a medium sized sample. The longitudinal design allowed us to

decompose changes into several parts, using a sophisticated statistical approach and to formulate

and to test several hypotheses for each change component.

Page 221: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

212

References

Abraham, C.S. (1994). From predictor variables to cognition models in health psychology: A

commentary on Schwarzer's theoretical analysis. Psychology and Health, 9, 181-185.

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317-332.

Anderson, J.C., & Gerbing, D.W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and

recommended two step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 411-423.

Aspinwall, L.G., & Taylor, S.E., (1997). A Stitch in Time: Self-Regulation and Proactive Coping.

Psychological Bulletin, 121, 417-435

Beck, A.T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International

Universities Press.

Begley, T.M. (1998). Coping strategies as predictors of employee distress and turnover after an

organizational consolidation: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Occupational and Organizational

Psychology, 71, 305-329.

Bentler, P. M. (1989). EQS: Structural equations program manual. Los Angeles: BMDP Statistical

Software.

Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107,

238-246.

Ben-Zur, H. (1999). The effectiveness of coping meta-strategies: Perceived efficiency, emotional

correlates and cognitive performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 923-940.

Bollen, K.A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.

Encyclopdia Britannica (1999). Problems of economic unification. [CD-ROM].

Browne, M.W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K.A. Bollen &

J.S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Bijttebier, P., & Vertommen, H. (1999). Coping strategies in relation to personality disorders.

Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 847-856.

Burke, M.J., Brief, A.P., & George, J.M. (1993). The role of negative affectivity in understanding

relations between self-reports of stressors and strains: A comment on the applied psychology

literature. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 402-412.

Byrne, B.M., Shavelson, R.J., & Muthén, B. (1989). Testing for the equivalence of factor

covariance and mean structures: The issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychological

Bulletin, 105, 456-466.

Carver, C.S., Pozo, C., Harris, S.D., Noriega, V., Scheier, M.F., Robinson, D.S., Ketcham, A.S.,

Moffat, F.L., & Clarck, K.C. (1993). How coping mediates the effect of optimism on distress: A

Page 222: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

213

study of women with early stage breast cancer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65,

375-390.

Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (1992). Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-

being: Theoretical overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 201-228.

Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (1994). Situational coping and coping dispositions in a stressful

transaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 184-195.

Carver, C.S., Scheier, M.F., Pozo, C. (1992). Conceptualizing the process of coping with health

problems. In H.S. Friedman (Ed.). Hostility, coping and health. (pp. 167-187). Washington, D.C.:

A.P.A.

Carver, C.S., Scheier, M.F., & Weintraub, J.K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically

based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267-283.

Chan, D. (1998). The conceptualization and analysis of change over time: An integrative approach

incorporating Longitudinal Mean and Covariance Structures analysis (LMACS) and Multiple

Indicator Growth Modeling (MLGM). Organizational research Methods, 1, 421-483.

Chang, E.C. (1998). Dispositional optimism and primary and secondary appraisal of a stressor:

Controlling for confounding influences and relations to coping and psychological and physical

adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1109-1120.

Cheng, S.T., & Hamid, P.N. (1997). Dispositional optimism in Chinese people: What does the Life

Orientation Test measure? International Journal of Psychology, 32, 15-22.

Coyne, J.C., & Downey, G. (1991). Social factors and psychopathology: Stress, social support, and

coping processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 401-425.

Cudeck, R. (1991). Comments on Chapter 15. “Interpreting and estimating indirect effects assuming

time lags really matter.” (Harry F. Gollob and Charles S. Reichardt). In L. M. Collins & J.L. Horn

(Eds.), Best methods for the analysis of change: recent advances, unanswered questions, future

directions (pp. 126 -136). Washington, D.C.: A.P.A.

Curran, P.J. (in press). A latent curve framework for the study of developmental trajectories in

adolescent substance use. In J. Rose, L. Chassin, C. Presson, & J. Sherman (Eds.), Multivariate

Applications in Substance Use Research. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

Dempster, A.P., Laird, N.M., & Rubin, D.B. (1977). Maximum likelihood estimation from

incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 39, 1-38.

DeNeve K.M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The Happy Personality: A Meta-Analysis of 137 Personality

Traits and Subjective Well-Being. Psychological Bulletin, 124. 197-228.

Diener, E., Suh, E.M., Lucas, R.E., & Smith, H.L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of

progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276-302.

Page 223: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

214

Dormann C. (1999). Modeling unmeasured third variables in longitudinal studies on organizational

stress. The case of social stressors at work, irritation, and depression. Doctoral Dissertation.

Johann Wolfgang Goethe Universität Frankfurt am Main.

Dormann, C., & Zapf, D. (1999). Social support, social stressors at work and depressive symptoms:

Testing for moderator effects with structural equations in a 3-wave longitudinal study. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 84, 874–884.

Fahrenberg, J. (1975). Die Freiburger Beschwerdeliste (FBL) (The Freiburger Beschwerderdeliste

(FBL)). Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, 4, 79-100.

Fay, D., & Frese, M. (in press). Self-starting behavior at work: Towards a theory of personal

Initiative. In J.Heckhausen (Ed.) Motivational psychology of human development: Developing

motivation and motivating development.

Fishbein, M. & Ajzen, I. (1974). Attitudes towards objects as predictors of single and multiple

behavioral criteria. Psychological Review 81, 59-74.

Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Dunkel- Schetter, C., DeLongis, A., & Gruen, R. J. (1986). Dynamics

of a Stressfull Encounter: Cognitive Appraisal, Coping and Encounter Outcomes. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 992-1003.

Frese, M., Fay, D., Hilburger, T., Leng, K., & Tag, A. (1997). The concept of personal initiative:

Operationalization, reliability and validity in two German samples. Journal of Occupational and

Organizational Psychology, 70, 139-161.

Frese, M., Garst, H., Fay, D. (1998). Control and complexity in work and the development of

personal initiative (PI): A 5-wave longitudinal structural equation model of occupational

socialization. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Frese, M., Kring, W., Soose, A., & Zempel, J. (1996). Personal Initiative at work: Differences

between East and West Germany. Academy of Management Journal, 39(1), 37-63.

Garst, H., Frese, M. & Molenaar P.C.M. (in press). The Temporal Factor of Change in Stressor-

Strain Relationships: A Growth Curve Model on a Longitudinal Study in East Germany. Journal of

Applied Psychology.

Geers, A.L., Reilley, S.P., & Dember, W.N. (1998). Optimism, pessimism, and friendship. Current

Psychology, 17, 3-19.

Golembiewski, R.T., Billingsley, K., & Yeager, S. (1976). Measuring change and persistence in

human affairs: types of change generated by OD designs. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science,

12, 133-157.

Gray, J.A. (1991). Neural systems, emotions, and personality. In J. Madden IV (Ed.), Neurobiology

of learning, emotion, and affect (pp. 273-306). New York: Raven Press.

Page 224: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

215

Gray, J.A. (1994). Framework for a taxonomy of psychiatric disorders. In S.H.M. van Goozen, N.E.

van de Poll & J.Sergeant (Eds.). Emotions: Essays on emotion theory (pp. 29-59). Hilldale NJ:

Erlbaum.

Gully, S.M., Frone, M.R., & Edwards, J.R. (1998). Problems and pitfalls in mediational tests. Paper

presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Texas.

Headey, B. & Wearing, A. (1989). Personality, life events, and subjective well-being: towards a

dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology., 57, 731-739.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:

Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55.

Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL8 user’s reference guide. Chicago: Scientific

Software International, Inc.

Kenny, D.A., & Campbell, D.T. (1989). On the measurement of stability in over-time data. Journal

of Personality, 57, 445-481.

Kenny, D.A., Kashy, D.A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social psychology. In D.Gilbert,

S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.). Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol 1, pp. 233-265).

Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.

Kessler, R.C., Price, R.H., & Wortman, C.B. (1985). Social factors in psychopathology: Stress,

social support, and coping processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 531-572.

Khoo, S., & Bishop, G.D. (1996). Stress and optimism: Relationships to coping and well-being.

Psychologia, 39, 29-40.

Kuhnke, R. (1991). Optimisten mit viele Sorgen (Optimists with many worries). Spiegel Spezial.

Lazarus, R.S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping, New York: Springer.

Lazarus, R.S. (1992). Coping theory and research: Past, present, and future. Psychosomatic

Medicine, 55, 234-247.

Lazarus, R.S. (1993). From psychological stress to the emotions: A history of changing outlooks.

Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 1-21.

Little, T.D. (1997). Mean and covariance structures (MACS) analysis of cross-cultural data:

Practical and theoretical issues. Multivariate Behavior Research, 32, 53-76.

MacCallum, R.C., Kim, C., Malarkey, W.B., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K. (1997). Studying multivariate

change using multilevel models and latent curve models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32,

215-253.

Mandys, F., Dolan, C.V., & Molenaar, P.C.M. (1994). Two aspects of the simplex model:

Goodness of fit to linear growth curve structures and the analysis of mean trends. Journal of

Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 19, 201-215.

Page 225: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

216

Marsh, H. W., Hau, K-T., Balla, J. R., & Grayson, D. (1998) Is more ever too much? The number of

indicators per factor in confirmatory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33, 181-

220.

Marshall, G.N., & Lang, E.L. (1990). Optimism, self-mastery, and symptoms of depression in

women professionals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 132-139.

Marshall, G.N., Wortman, C.B., Kusulas, J.W., Hervig, L.K., & Vickers Jr., R.R. (1992).

Distinguishing optimism from pessimism: Relations to fundamental dimensions of mood and

personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 1067-1074.

Meredith, W. (1993). Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance.

Psychometrika, 58, 525-543.

Meredith, W., & Tisak, J. (1990). Latent curve analysis. Psychometrika, 55, 107-122.

McArdle, J.J., & Woodcock, R.W. (1997). Expanding test-retest designs to include developmental

time-lag components. Psychological Methods, 2, 403-435.

Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions.

Annual review of Psychology, 49, 229-258.

Mook, J., Kleijn, W.C., & van der Ploeg, H.M. (1992). Positively and negatively worded items in a

self report measure of dispositional optimism. Psychological Reports, 71, 275-278.

Mohr, G. (1986). Die Erfassung psychischer Befindensbeeinträchtigungen bei Industriearbeitern

(Measuring disturbances of psychological well-being in industrial blue collar workers). Frankfurt:

Peter Lang.

Mroczek, D.K., Spiro III, A., Aldwin, C.M., Ozer, D.J., & Bossé, R. (1993). Construct validation of

optimism and pessimism in older men: Findings from the normative aging study. Health

Psychology, 12, 406-409.

Muthén, B. (1997). Latent variable modeling with longitudinal and multilevel data. In A. Raftery

(Ed.), Sociological Methodology (pp. 453-480). Boston: Blackwell Publishers.

Muthén, B.O., & Curran, P.J. (1997). General longitudinal modeling of individual differences in

experimental designs: A latent variable framework for analysis and power estimation.

Psychological Methods, 2, 371-402.

Muthén, B. & Muthén, L. (1998). Training session Stockholm August 1998 on: Analysis of

longitudinal data: Growth modeling using latent variables.

Nesselroade, J.R. (1991). Interindividual differences in intraindividual change. In L. M. Collins &

J.L. Horn (Eds.), Best methods for the analysis change: Recent advances, unanswered questions,

future directions (pp. 92-105). Washington, D.C.: A.P.A.

Page 226: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

217

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L.E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood

following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92-104.

Oort, F.J.O. (1996). Using restricted factor analysis in test construction. Dissertation University of

Amsterdam.

Ormel, J., & Schaufeli, W.B. (1991). Stability and change in psychological distress and their

relationship with self-esteem and locus of control: A dynamic equilibrium model. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 288-299.

Pentz, M.A., & Chou, C.P. (1994). Measurement invariance in longitudinal clinical research

assuming change from development and intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 62, 450-462.

Plewis, I. (1996). Statistical methods for understanding cognitive growth: A review, a synthesis and

an application. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 49, 25-42.

Plomin,R., Scheier, M.F., Bergeman, C.S., Pedersen, N.L., Nesselroade, J.R., & McClearn, G.E.

(1992). Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 921-930.

Raffalovich, L.E., & Bohrnstedt, G.W. (1987). Common, specific, and error variance components

of factor models. Sociological Methods & Research, 15, 385-405.

Räikkönen, K., Matthews, K.A., Flory, J.D., Owens, J.F., & Gump, B.B. (1999). Effects of

optimism, pessimism, and trait anxiety on ambulatory blood pressure and mood during everyday

life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 104-113.

Robinson-Whelen, S., Kim, C., MacCallum, R.C., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K. (1997). Distinguishing

optimism from pessimism in older adults: is it more important to be optimistic or not to be

pessimistic? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1345-1353.

Rogosa, D.R, Brandt, D., & Zimowski, M. (1982). A growth curve approach to the measurement of

change. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 726-748.

Schafer, J.L. (1997). Analysis of Incomplete Multivariate Data, London: Chapman & Hall.

Scheier, M.F., & Carver, C.S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications

of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219-247.

Scheier, M.F., Carver, C.S. (1992). Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-being:

Overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 201-228.

Scheier, M.F., Carver, C.S., & Bridges, M.W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism

(and trait anxiety, self mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063-1078.

Page 227: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

218

Schwarz, J.E., Neale, J., Marco, C., Shiffman, S.S., & Stone, A.A. (1999). Does trait coping exist?

A momentary assessment approach to the evaluation of traits. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 77, 360-369.

Schwarzer, R. (1994). Optimism, vulnerability, and self-beliefs as health-related cognitions: A

systematic overview. Psychology and Health, 9, 161-180.

Smith, T.W., Pope, M.K., Rhodewalt, F., & Poulton, J.L. (1989). Optimism, neurotism, coping, and

symptom reports: An alternative interpretation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 56, 640-648.

Speier, C. & Frese, M. (1997). Generalized self-efficacy as a mediator and moderator between

control and complexity at work and personal initiative: A longitudinal field study in East Germany.

Human Performance, 10, 171-192.

Steyer, R., Ferring, D., & Schmitt, M.J. (1992). States and traits in psychological assessment.

European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 8, 79-98.

Solomon, R.L., & Corbit, J.D. (1974). An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal

dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, 119-143.

Spector, P.E., Van Katwyck, P.T., Brannick, M.T., & Chen, P.Y. (1997). When two factors don't

reflect two constructs: How item characteristics can produce artifactual factors. Journal of

Management, 23, 659-677.

Taylor, S.E., & Aspinwall, L.G. (1996). Mediating and moderating processes in psychosocial stress.

In H.B. Kaplan (Ed.), Psychological stress: Perspectives on structure, theory, life-courses, and

methods. (pp. 71-110) Academic Press, San Diego.

Terry, D.J. (1994). Determinants of Coping: The role of stable and situational factors. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 895-910.

Vickers, K.S., & Vogeltanz, N.D. (2000). Dispositional optimism as a predictor of depressive

symptoms over time. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 259-272.

Willett, J.B. (1989). Some results on reliability for the longitudinal measurement of change:

Implications for the design of studies of individual growth. Education and Psychological

Measurement, 49, 587-602.

Willett, J.B., & Sayer, A.G. (1994). Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates and

predictors of individual change over time. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 363-381.

Willett, J.B., & Sayer, A.G. (1995). Cross-domain analysis of change over time: Combining growth

curve modeling and covariance structure analysis. In G.A. Marcoulides & R.E. Schumacker (Eds.).

Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp. 125-157). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Page 228: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

219

Williams, D.G. (1992). Dispositional optimism, neuroticism, and extraversion. Personality and

Individual Differences, 13, 475-477.

Vonesh, E.F., & Chinchilli, V.M. (1997). Linear and nonlinear models for the analysis of repeated

measurements. New York: Marcel Dekker.

Zapf, D., Dormann, C., & Frese, M. (1996). Longitudinal studies in organizational stress research:

A review of the literature with reference to methodological issues. Journal of Occupational Health

Psychology, 2, 145-169.

Zung, W. W. K. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 63-70.

Page 229: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

220

Page 230: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

221

Chapter 6

Summary and Conclusion

This dissertation addressed the question of how former East Germans coped with the many

changes in the revolutionary times after the unification and which people profited most?

Specifically the following questions were addressed:

1) Were there occupational socialization effects of working characteristics (job control and job

complexity) on personal initiative and was there evidence that these effects were mediated by

control cognitions? Did people who showed more personal initiative eventually receive the

better jobs? (Chapter 1).

2) Did the transition to a free-market affect the level of work stressors and what effect had these

changes on strains? How could the relation between stressors and strains best be modeled

(Chapter 2).

3) Did being an optimist help one to cope with the many adaptations people had to make and, more

specifically, did it affect subjective well-being? Was there evidence that these effects of

optimism/pessimism on subjective well-being were mediated by coping styles? (Chapter 3).

These research questions should be seen in light of the specific historical context: the turbulent

period after the unification. Planned economy, which its centralized economic control, was rapidly

replaced by a free-market economy (Britannica, 1999). As a consequence many companies were

closed or taken over by Western companies. The restructuring of the economy was accompanied by

an enormous increase in unemployment. For those who were lucky enough to keep their job many

adaptations were required. A core element of the old socialistic system was the expected obedience

of community members to the sanctions of their superiors. This strongly contrasts with modern

Western working values where employees have to be much more actively involved in pursuing the

goals of the company. After abandoning planned economy with its decision making and power

highly concentrated on the top, people had to adapt to the different power structure of modern

organizations in which decision making is spread more evenly to all levels of the hierarchy. High

competition ruled out the immobility characteristic of mass organizations and collectives and

pushes companies to high performance with increasingly emphasis on virtues like constant

innovation, providing better customer service, and consistently monitoring changes in consumer

preferences. This had a profound effect on what was expected of employees. No longer was it

Page 231: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

222

sufficient to comply with strictly formalized task requirements. Since it is impossible for

organizations, operating in dynamic environments, to constantly redesign jobs, the gap between

optimal job behavior and formalized job rules must be filled by a new set of task prescriptions and

job attitudes of the employees. These new, more global, task prescriptions should include a more

active orientation and be more guided by the higher than the lower level goals of the organization.

As an example, if an employee working in a distribution center lets him or herself guide by the goal

of keeping customers satisfied, than delivering orders beyond working time or occasionally giving

extra service is in line with customer policies, although formally it may not be part of his or her job

or may even contradict some lower level procedures.

This new interpretation of what is expected of workers in modern dynamic organizations is

nicely captured by the construct of personal initiative. Personal initiative can be defined as a

sequence of actions that are instigated by self-assigned behavioral goals, which go beyond the more

narrowly defined core job requirements and this self-starting behavior is followed by a high degree

of persistence to overcome obstacles that threaten to thwart the accomplishment of these goals.

These behavioral goals have a long-term perspective, are pro-company oriented, and more inspired

by higher than lower level organizational goals.

In Chapter 1 it was hypothesized that by increasing job control and by adding more

complexities to the job, more optimal conditions were created to further stimulate personal

initiative. Thus by including more control and complexity the step towards more autonomous

behavior (like personal initiative) would be smaller. The effects of job characteristics on the

psychological functioning of the person were called occupational socialization. The most important

job characteristics in this socialization process were control at work and the complexity of the job.

Control at work was defined as the amount of influence a person can exercise over his or her actions

and over job conditions in order to fulfill the goals of the job. Complexity at work is related to the

number of elements a person has to take into account in making appropriate work decisions. It was

hypothesized that if a person had more influence over a greater number of elements in order to

accomplish his or her task this would lead to enduring changes within the person. These changes

captured many aspects of a person and thus were defined on an abstract level. This higher-level

construct was called control cognitions. Control cognitions is related to the self-concept of mastery

orientation and affects control aspirations, control expectations and self-efficacy. Control cognitions

were expected to regulate also actions which would fall beyond the core tasks of the person and to

enhance personal initiative. Since successful initiative should expand the domain over which one

can exert control and extending these boundaries should be reflected in higher control cognitions

reciprocal effects between personal initiative and control cognitions should be expected. Finally, it

Page 232: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

223

was hypothesized that personal initiative affected job characteristics because persons high on

initiative would find better jobs or jobs with more enriched tasks. Thus, working characteristics

should affect control cognitions which in turn would have an impact on personal initiative. And to

complete the circle, personal initiative would lead to changes in working characteristics. In Chapter

1 we tested this model against several alternative models and the goodness of fit favored the

proposed model. The length of the period needed for personal initiative to exert its influence on

working characteristics was approximately one year (except the last lag which was two years). All

paths coefficients were in the expected direction and most of them were significant. In Chapter 1 it

was concluded that this model supported the theory in many respects. Although the model is called

an occupational socialization model, it argues for a bi-directional influence: Not only the job shapes

the person, the person can change the job as well by showing personal initiative, resulting in either

finding a new, more autonomous job or by changing the content of the old job.

Although the effects of working characteristics on personal initiative (mediated by control

cognitions) were synchronous the effects should not be interpreted as instantaneous effects. A more

plausible interpretation of synchronous effects is that the effects occurred within the period between

two measurement occasions. Since measurements were discrete these effects could only be

observed in the next wave. How on a micro-level these processes unfolded in time could only have

been studied if process data would have been available. All we can conclude is that within a

relatively short period occupational socialization effects could be established. The time needed for

the effects of personal initiative on working characteristics were longer. Persons who showed more

personal initiative improved working conditions, indicating that they got the better jobs. There were

reciprocal effects: jobs which allowed more control for the worker made people more active and

more in control of their environments: They were becoming more the active agents instead of being

passively reacting to their environments.

The fact that working conditions changed radically in East Germany was confirmed in Chapter

1 by the low stabilities for the work characteristics variables (control and complexity combined). It

was expected that Western production standards soon would pervade the workplace. What effects

could this have for stressors at work and its effects on strains? This was the topic of Chapter 2.

First, we investigated whether the trends in the means of the stressors reflected the general view that

many changes had occurred in the organizations and on the workplace. The following stressors

were included in Chapter 2: Job insecurity, working under time pressure, organizational problems,

social stressors and uncertainty.

It was commonly known that the technological backwardness and low productivity of East

German companies in 1990 would make it impossible to compete on an unrestricted market and this

Page 233: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

224

would inevitably lead to unemployment. However, the dramatic rise of the unemployment figures

was a shock for almost everyone (Britannica, 1999). Especially in the north of East Germany

unemployment rates rose to staggering heights, although in Dresden, the city where the sample was

drawn, unemployment figures were lower. The means of the stressor fear of becoming unemployed

showed an increase after the first measurement occasion, but showed a decrease again already after

the second measurement wave. This drop was earlier than expected and selection effects may have

been responsible for this decrease: People with high fears of becoming unemployed lost indeed their

jobs the next period and produced missing values for the next wave.

Increased efficiency demands invaded the organizational culture and this was reflected by the

upward trend of the means of the stressor time pressure. Adopting Western standards had benefits

for stressors as well: The means of the stressor organizational problems decreased during this five-

year period.

It was expected that competition on the work floor should make the social climate less friendly,

but the means of social stressors were completely stable. Perhaps, these stressors did not function as

a barometer measuring the comradeship, cohesion and the friendliness, which characterized the

social climate in the former East Germany.

It was known that in the former Eastern bloc work requirements were not clearly prescribed

(Pearce, Branyicki & Bukacsi, 1994) and that modern management tools provided less ambiguous

job descriptions. However, the stressor uncertainty showed only a small decrease after the first

wave to remain stable for the rest of the period. Perhaps opposing forces may explain the stability of

the means of the stressor uncertainty: A more active approach was now demanded from East

German workers and this may have created role ambiguity for those who were socialized to strict

obedience. Simply doing what they were told was no longer sufficient for workers. The paradox of

sometimes having to act autonomously implied that they had to trust their own judgment when and

how to act and this may be confusing for many workers.

In summary, some stressors showed an upward trend in the mean levels (time pressure), some

stressors means declined (organizational problems) while the means of social stressors remained

completely stable. What implications did this have for the trends in the means of strains? Some

studies reported strain levels that remained surprisingly stable (Ormel & Schaufeli, 1991; Headey &

Wearing, 1989). The mean levels of depression, psychosomatic complaints, irritation and worrying

were indeed almost stable. Apparently, the opposing trends in specific stressors may have led to

compensatory effects.

Changes in the means reflect trends representative for the population as a whole. On an

individual level considerable variation can occur. The stabilities of both stressors and strains

Page 234: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

225

showed that the relative positions of people changed considerably over a five-year period.

Apparently people changed into different directions: Some improved relatively while others

position worsened.

Since the relative position of stressor and strain scores of people changed, it made sense to test

several models which could explain these changes. First, an interindividual difference model was

tested. This model argues that relationships between self-reports of stressors and strains could be

explained by one common factor (which may be related to negative affectivity). This model could

clearly be rejected. It was noticed, however, that although this model did not fit the data, it did not

exclude the possibility that one common factor (e.g., negative affectivity) partly had influenced both

stressors and strains reports. Since it was very unlikely that it was the sole factor explaining stressor

- strain relationships more substantial models were tested.

Before testing other stressor – strain models the question of how to model change itself, had to

be addressed. Was change best depicted as an explicit function of time in which the direction of

change was constant for the complete period for each individual? Or should the underlying change

process best be modeled as predictable changes combined with time-specific stochastic influences

that were independent among the different measurement occasions? These models are called latent

growth curve models and autoregressive models, respectively. It was found that for the strain

models the fit indices of the growth curve models were slightly better and the stressor models were

best fit with autoregressive models. A hybrid model was introduced combining the best model for

stressors (AR) with the preferred model for strains (growth curves) with the addition of

synchronous paths from stressors to strains.

The relationships between the systematic slowly moving parts of both stressors and strains

could be represented by the correlation between the slope factors of stressors and strains in cross-

domain growth curve models (Willett & Sayer, 1995). This stressor-strain trend model was tested

for each combination of a stressor and a strain and it was found that several sizeable correlations

could be established. Thus, changes in the domain of stressors tended to covary with a parallel

change in the domain of strains for these combinations. Growth trajectories of time pressure and

uncertainty tended to go parallel with the curves of worrying and social stressors with

psychosomatic complaints.

In the stress literature the most prominent models assume a causal relation from stressors to

strains. However, since the stress - strain process can best be modeled as an ongoing process

(Edwards, 1998) also models that assume reverse causation deserve attention. Two alternative

models were proposed: The Drift model assumes that persons initially suffering from high strains

will not be able to cope and gradually fall back to even worse conditions. The Refuge model

Page 235: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

226

predicts an opposite process: People with initial high strain levels will succeed in finding less

demanding jobs. In cross-domain growth curve models reverse causation hypothesis could be tested

by the correlation between the intercept factor of strain variables and the slope factor of stressor

variables. The results supported the Refuge models, but the effects were small and it was concluded

that in the population opposing Reverse Causation mechanisms could co-occur and partly had

cancelled each other out. Using recent developed software (Muthén, 1999) these effects could be

isolated if information about latent classes was available.

A Sleeper Effect Model tested if initial levels of stressors had effect on later changes in strains.

This was tested in the cross-domain growth curve models by the correlation between the intercept

factor of a stressor variable and the slope variable of a strain variable. There was no support the

Sleeper Effect Model.

The last tested model specified short - term effects for stressors on strains. The Short - Term

Reaction Model was tested by the hybrid model. Many significant stressors – strains relationships

were found, and especially for the effects of the stressor time pressure on worrying. These stressor

effects could be interpreted as affecting the state component of strains, because the individual

trendlines were partialled out. Equivalently, this model can be interpreted as specifying trendlines

after controlling for the synchronous effects of the stressors. Thus, a model without time-varying

covariates (synchronous stressor effects) would have confounded the systematic regular trend of

each individual with time-specific stressor effects.

In summary, the result of Chapter 2 showed that work stressors both affected the slowly

changing components (see Stressor-Strain Trend Model) as well as the state component of strains

(see Short-Term Reaction Model).

In Chapter 1 and 2 work conditions (job characteristics, like job control and job complexity,

and work stressors) were included to explain psychological processes. In Chapter 3 the focus was

on factors within the person. The research question addressed in Chapter 3 was how optimism was

related to subjective well-being. The personality trait optimism is described in the literature as a

protective factor (sometimes called a resource or resilience factor) in processes where people have

to cope with severe stressors. In similar vein, pessimism is regarded as a vulnerability factor. A

confusing result, obtained by testing several measurement models, was that the scale intended to

measure optimism, the Life Orientation Test (LOT) as a uni-dimensional construct, fell apart in two

only moderately correlated factors, optimism and pessimism. This result is consistent with the

reports of many other researchers who used the LOT scale. Moreover, the pattern of relations with

other variables was also very different for optimism and pessimism. The latter was strongly related

Page 236: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

227

to depression and somewhat less to irritation. These results favored the treatment of optimism and

pessimism as separate constructs.

Both the means of optimism and pessimism showed a decreasing trend, which can be

considered as further evidence against the treatment of optimism and pessimism as opposite poles

of one underlying continuum. The decline of the means of pessimism was unexpected. Immediately

after the unification many eastern Germans felt euphoric, hoping for freedom, social justice, and a

high standard of living. After political unification, disillusionment in eastern Germany rose sharply.

The massiveness of unemployment came as a shock and as early as the spring of 1991 there were

mass demonstrations against unemployment in the streets of Leipzig. The rebuilding of the Easter

economic stagnated and some spoke of an economic desert. Apparently, the instrument for

measuring pessimism could not be considered as a political or economical barometer, but instead

measured more enduring personal tendencies.

The stabilities for optimism and pessimism were comparable with those of the subjective

well-being variables (called strain variables in Chapter 2). Although the measurement instrument

intended to measure a personality trait, many changes in the relative positions of the scores of

persons could be observed. To explain these changes latent growth curve models were tested and

these models yielded acceptable goodness of fit measures. Both direct and indirect models were

specified. The last models included coping style variables (aggregated over five waves) as

mediators. The stable components of optimism/pessimism and subjective well-being correlated

significantly, which may be explained by genetic and early childhood factors. Initial optimism

(exemplified by the intercept factor) had a significant effect on the slope factor for depression and

psychosomatic complaints. This was most of all due to direct effects. Coping styles played a much

more modest role than we expected. Although some effects were significant, the effect sizes were

very modest. Coping styles could be predicted by pessimism (pessimists planned less, and used

more emotional-focused coping and wishful thinking). Optimists used more problem-focused

coping and surprisingly also more emotional-focused coping.

Much stronger were the effects of initial subjective well-being (represented by the intercept

factors) on coping styles. How people felt at the start of study (immediately after the unification)

could predict moderately well how they would cope during the subsequent period. People who felt

bad (high on depression, psychosomatic complaints and irritation) attempted to alleviate the

negative emotions by wishful thinking, emotional-focused coping and seeking social support. No

relations were found with planning and problem-focused coping. In contrast people who worried

more at the start of the period had a coping style that could be characterized by planning and

problem-focused coping. The constructive element of worrying was not anticipated. The modest

Page 237: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

228

role coping styles played as mediators could to a great extent be explained by the small effects of

coping styles on changes (slope factors) in the subjective well-being variables. Only a few

significant results could be noticed: planning and problem-focused coping led to less than average

slopes in depression, whereas emotion-focused coping increased depression and worrying.

Not only were pessimism and depression correlated from the start (both intercept factors

were correlated), also their growth curves were (r = .91). To a lesser extent the same picture could

be observed for pessimism and irritation. We can conclude that pessimism is strongly related to

negative emotions.

The overall conclusion of this dissertation is a plea for reciprocal determinism (Bandura,

1997). Both environmental characteristics (work characteristics and stressors at work) and personal

characteristics (personal initiative, control cognitions, optimism, and coping styles) determined

outcomes (strain or synonymously subjective well-being) reciprocally. Those who were in

unfavorable conditions (less control and higher stress levels) suffered more than those who could

act more autonomously and were not exposed to severe stressors. While this may be true: People

were able to change their conditions by showing initiative and adequate coping styles (planning and

problem-focused coping). Being optimistic was an advantage.

Methodological concerns

As all studies some shortcomings have to be noticed. Most of the methodological concerns

have been addressed in the respective chapters. However, some issues need to be elaborated.

Nested relationships

For people without a job the work-related variables are not missing (implying that the values

are potentially observable), but not defined. Treating these data as missing would imply that a

counterfactual assumption had to be made: What are the most plausible values for that person if we

assume that the person would have had a job (which we know is not the case). We would have

preferred to include in our models both the employment status and the work-related variables and

its relations with other psychological variables.

A multi-group solution was unfortunately not an option. Since we had longitudinal model

including 5 or 6 waves, there were many small subgroups with identical observed data patterns and

the n of many subgroups was smaller than the number of variables (leading to singular covariance

matrices for these subgroups).

In the econometric literature the problem of semi-continuous distributions or nested relations

is also known and they recommend a two-step regression model with a binary variable

Page 238: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

229

(employment status in our case) and a continuous variable for the variable of interest.

Unfortunately, the possibility to analyze nested relations is not implemented in LISREL. In the near

future suitable software will be available (Muthén, 1999; Neale, 1999). The models described in

Chapter 1 and 2 will then be reanalyzed.

Omitted variable bias

One of the most important threats to the validity of regression models is the occurrence of

‘omitted variable bias’ (Dormann, 1999). Omitted variable bias occurs if some variables are left out

of the model or if important paths are lacking. Misspecification can lead to biased estimates.

Combining the theoretical insights from the first three chapters, one can see that some models are

partly misspecified. In Chapter 2 it was demonstrated that work stressors affected strains, but in

Chapter 3 no stressors were included. In similar vein, Chapter 2 did not include coping and resource

variables (e.g., optimism). In Chapter 2 and 3 it was described that the analyses had been broken

down using subsets of variables. Unfortunately, including all variables into a single model was not

possible. Both restrictions of the capacity of the software as statistical considerations made this both

impossible and unwise.

It is hard to find any disadvantage for longitudinal designs. However, estimating growth

curve models does not easily permit to split the models in small models using only a few

measurement occasions.

Missing values

Most longitudinal studies suffer from attrition. This is a threat for generalizing the results

obtained in the sample to the intended population. If the data are Missing at Random (MAR)(Little

& Rubin, 1987) the probability of response depends only on the observed data (Vonesh &

Chinchilli, p. 40). In case of MAR application of the EM algorithm for estimating the covariance

matrix and the vector of means is a reasonable approach (Graham, 1997). However, this is not the

optimal choice since overfitting occurs and variances are biased downwards. A more sophisticated

method is data augmentation (Schafer, 1997). The last method has not been used in the previous

chapters, because it was not yet fully tested in its implications. If the missing data are nonignorable

(the probability of response depends on the unobserved data (Vonesh & Chinchilli, p. 40), no

adequate missing data strategy is available. It is reasonable to assume that at least to some extent the

missing data are nonignorable, leading to some bias in the estimation of the model parameters. This

is an unresolved problem many studies suffer from and at this moment there seems no easy solution

to become available in the near future.

Page 239: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

230

Advantages of longitudinal research for making causal statements

What are the advantages of longitudinal designs?

For interpreting a relation between a variable X and Y as causal Cook & Campbell (1979) mention

three requisites:

1) Covariation between X and Y

2) Time order: for interpreting X as the cause for Y, X has to precede Y in time.

3) Alternative explanations can be ruled out.

Ad 1) Although it is commonly thought as a necessary condition, covariation can be masked by

suppressor effects. Suppressor effects can be produced by compensational mechanisms (Gully,

Frone, Edwards, 1998). As Bollen stated (1989):”The old saying that correlation does not prove

causation should be complemented by the saying that a lack of correlation does not disprove

causation (p.52)”. However, if the model is correctly specified (e.g., no omitted variable bias), there

should be covariation. It is known from dynamic system theory (Molenaar, 1998) that many

relations are smaller than expected because compensatory mechanisms become effective if a

variable approaches boundary values.

Ad 2) The time order is more helpful if the phenomenon which is considered to be the cause is a

clearly defined event with a clear beginning and ending. Whether or not a person is exposed to the

stimulus or the extent of exposure can in principle be established in a well-defined manner.

However, the data studied in this dissertation are potentially constantly changing and so are the

potential effects. This complicates statements about causality. What precedes what and what is the

time lag between causes and effects? Knowledge of the time order is still helpful, but since the time

lag is not known, causal interpretations are much harder to make. If two variables are repeatedly

measured, the number of potential relationships, each satisfying the time order condition, is much

greater. There can be synchronous effects, reciprocal effects and lagged effects with different

lengths of the lags. The duration and intensity of effects can also greatly vary and to complicate

matters even further: interaction effects could be observed in the case of prolonged exposure to the

stimulus.

Ad 3) The most difficult task for the researcher claiming causality is that all other explanations for

the covariation can be convincingly rejected. The most severe threat for structural equation models

is omitted variable bias (Jöreskog, 1997) that applies whenever the true cause is left out of a model.

This threat of misspecification exists irrespective if the model is longitudinal or cross-sectional.

Here we quote Cudeck (1991, p. 261):”A “correctly specified model” is, always has been, and

always will be a fiction. A more realistic view of models is that they are simplifications of

Page 240: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

231

extremely complicated behavior. It is a mistake to assume that any model actually represents the

underlying process absolutely correctly, even after certain obvious faults have been corrected. All

that can be hoped is that the model captures some reasonable approximation to the truth, serving

perhaps as a descriptive device or summarizing tool”.

Objective versus subjective measurements

In many theories in work and industrial psychology the focus is on aspects of the work

environment. These are considered as important determinants for both the behavior of the workers

as well as for many relevant behavioral outcomes (such as productivity, absenteeism, turn-over,

satisfaction). Some theories emphasize the objective features of the work environment, whereas

other theories ignore the objectivity of these aspects and primarily focus on how they are perceived.

In job redesign and in some stress prevention programs the objective aspects are of crucial interest.

If individual differences are the main concern of the researcher it is more important how these

aspects are perceived.

Because objective measurements are not always available in many studies, questionnaire

(subjective) data are often used as proxies for the objective characteristics. However, measurement

models, developed for measuring traits and abilities, may not always be adequate for testing these

perceptual data sets. In test theory, where the focus is on individual differences, it is required that

test conditions are highly standardized. As a consequence the covariation between the items can be

explained by common factors, since it is plausible that unique item factors are independent of each

other. The factor model is appropriate for measuring individual differences in job perceptions in

cases where all workers have the exactly the same job and work under highly standardized work

conditions. Covariation between items may be explained by individual differences in the tendency

to over- or underreport (c.f. negative affectivity, Burke et al, 1993).

In the previous case the common factor refers to differences between persons. It is also

possible that common factors reside in the external world. In studies of tasks characteristic many

jobs may be studied. Lets suppose that valid objective measurements of these task characteristics

are available. Now individual differences in perceptions are excluded since objective measurement

implies that all observers would come to the same observation. A set of common factors may

explain the covariation between the indicators, since in general jobs consist of a set of well-

coordinated tasks. Again the factor model does a decent job, since there is only one set of common

causes, now residing completely in the external world. However frequently a set of ‘objective’

indicators from a certain domain will not be systematically related and hence the assumptions for

fitting a factor model will be violated.

Page 241: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

232

Next we discuss more complicated cases. What happens if we confound both sources of

variation? Suppose that in the first example job restructuring will be implemented in such a way

that complete standardization is abandoned. Now workers get in varying degrees more control over

both the methods they uses as well as the quality of what they produce. Since the job redesign is set

up in such a way that both control aspects covary, there are now two common sources which can

explain the correlation between both subjective indicators: A subjective factor (tendency for over-

or underreporting) and an objective factor (caused by job redesign) are completely confounded. In a

factor model both items violate the assumption of local independence: The common factor can only

partly explain the covariance between the items.

In the previous example both control aspects were deliberately matched by the management.

However, in empirical data sets the covariance matrix of objective characteristics can have any

form: From completely uncorrelated to strongly positively of negatively correlated. These

complications led us to conclusion that our measurement of work characteristics (Chapter 3) and

stressors at work (Chapter 4) were based upon simultaneously valid, but conflicting measurement

models: A factor model and a causal indicator model. In cases where objective measurements are

not available, it is hard to find a suitable measurement model for these hybrid models and equally

weighting the items seems an acceptable strategy.

Standard errors and goodness of fit measures

The structural equation modeling software that was used (LISREL) requires as input one

single number representing the sample size. Standard errors and almost all goodness of fit measures

are a direct function of the sample size. It is hard to provide the ‘correct’ sample size if the

covariance matrix and the vector of means are estimated with pairwise deletion or alternatively with

the use of the EM algorithm (Schafer, 1997). There is at present no statistical theory for estimating

the correct standard errors and goodness of fit measures if the covariance matrix and mean vector

are estimated either using pair-wise deletion or the EM algorithm. In practice researchers choose

between the following options: The minimum sample size, the mean and the maximum sample size

(Marsh, 1998). A combination may be used as well: provide both the results for the model estimated

with the minimum sample size and the maximum sample size (Molenaar, 1998).

A promising alternative for obtaining correct standard errors if the assumption of Missing at

Random (Little & Rubin, 1987) holds, is maximizing the case-wise likelihood of the observed data.

In contrast to the LISREL program alternative structural equation modeling software like AMOS

(Arbuckle, 1995) and MX (Neale, Boker, Xie & Maes, 1999), provides the possibility for Full

Information Maximum Likelihood based on the direct maximization of the likelihood of the

Page 242: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

233

observed data (Arbuckle, 1996, Wothke, 1997; Wood, 1997, p. 373). In a simulation study using the

AMOS program Wothke (1997) reported that using FIML estimation the standard errors were only

slightly estimated when the fraction of missing data was moderate or high. MX offers the

opportunity to estimate likelihood-based confidence intervals, which may be preferable over

standard errors in many cases (Neale, Boker, Xie & Maes, 1999, p. 90).

Another alternative for estimating standard errors and goodness of fit measures is

bootstrapping. However, not in all situations the bootstrap procedure performed well (Yung &

Bentler, 1996, p. 223) and further research is needed to provide guidelines in which cases

bootstrapping is the optimal choice in structural equation modeling with missing data.

For practical purposes we choose to use the mean sample size. This is probably not the most

sophisticated method. As a consequence all standard errors and goodness of fit measures cannot be

considered as exact but hopefully as reasonable approximations. The results of a simulation study

by Marsh (1998) are not conclusive.

Validity of correctly specifying growth curves and interpretation of states

We modeled growth as an explicit function of time and we have to admit that the form of

this relationship with time is determined by the number of measurement occasions: a continuous

relation is estimated with discrete data. If we could provide over many more measurement points

changes could be monitored more precisely and more complex growth curves could be estimated.

We defined states as deviations from the individual trajectory. As a consequence our definition of

state fluctuation versus systematic developments would dramatically change. Thus, the definition of

states is dependent on the availability of the number of measurement points within the period of

study. Ideally one would study fluctuations of different time lengths. The shorter the time interval

between consecutive measurements and the longer the period of the study the more information

about the dynamics of the processes could be obtained. However, in psychological research testing

and practice effects would probably severely threaten the validity of interview and questionnaire

data. In this dissertation no time series data were available, but instead a panel study has been

performed. Therefore, the definition of state fluctuation should be seen in light of the long intervals

between measurement occasions (ranging from three months to two years).

Level of Generality

Research can be organized along its level of generality between people (see Revelle on

personality processes, 1995; p. 297). The level of generality ranges from generalizing to all people

Page 243: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

234

to focusing on single individuals. In quantitative research the level of generality can be reduced by

including moderators or specifying different models for subgroups. Relationships are no longer

treated as invariant, but may vary for different groups.

Originally we intended to specify models which allowed the functional form of growth

curves to be different for subgroups. In these models the group membership was not known in

advance, but instead should be estimated from the data. At the start of the study this was not yet

possible using existing software. However, recent software developments allow for heterogeneity

with respect to the influence of antecedents, growths shapes, and outcomes. These General Growth

Mixture Models can now be estimated using the Mplus software (Muthén, in press; Muthén and

Muthén , 1999). In Chapter 4 we reported that the Sleeper Effect was not supported. But the

prevalence of Sleeper Effects may be relatively rare in the population and only occurring if several

causes combine. More advanced statistical tools will give the researcher a potential for specifying

more fine-tuned models in which relatively rare phenomena, not representative for the whole

population will have a higher probability of being detected. The future of Structural Equation

Modeling seems indeed very promising.

References

Arbuckle, J.L.(1996). Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In G.A.

Marcoulides & R.E. Schumacker (Eds.). Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and

techniques, (pp. 243-277). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cook, T.D. & Campbell, D.T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation. Design and analysis for field

settings. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Marsh, H.W. (1998). Pairwise deletion for missing data in structural equation models: Nonpositive

definite matrices, parameter estimates, goodness of fit, and adjusted sample sizes. Structural

Equation Modeling, 5, 22-36.

Molenaar, P.C.M. (1998). Personal communication.

Wood, P.K. (1997). How the state of the art can inform the art of the practice. Structural Equation

Modeling, 4, 370-387.

Wothke, W. (1997). Longitudinal modeling with missing data. Paper presented at the Berlin

Summer School Conference (BSSC 97), June 25-30.

Yung, Y.-F., & Bentler, P.M.(1996). Bootstrapping techniques in the analysis of mean and

covariance structures. In G.A. Marcoulides & R.E. Schumacker (Eds.). Advanced structural

equation modeling: Issues and techniques, (pp. 195-226). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Page 244: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

235

Appendix A

In this Appendix it will be shown that

111222222111 .... −−−−−− qqqqqq ΩΜΑΩΜΑΩΜΑΩΜΑ (A1)

can be written as

( ) 122112211221 .......... ΥΥΥΥΜΜΜΜΧΧΧΧ −−−−−− qqqqqq (A2)

In this Appendix positive definiteness is assumed in cases matrix inversion is necessary.

All Αt matrices, placed between the Μt matrices, have been replaced by premultiplying with a

suitable matrix Χt. The first matrix Α1 and the last Ωq-1 matrix are already on the right position, so

we can write Χ1 = Α1 and Υq-1 = Ωq-1. This results in:

111222222111 .... −−−−−− qqqqqq ΥΜΑΩΜΑΩΜΑΩΜΧ (A3)

The next step is to replace Α2 by an unknown matrix Χ2 so that:

11122111 ΩΜΧΧΑΩΜΧ = (A4)

The unknown matrix Χ2 can be found by postmultiplying both sides with: 11

11

11

−−− ΧΜΩ

11

11

111112

11

11

112111

−−−−−− = ΧΧΜΩΩΜΧΧΧΜΩΑΩΜΧ (A5)

21

11

11

12111 ΧΧΜΩΑΩΜΧ =−−− (A6)

111222221112 .... −−−−−− qqqqqq ΥΜΑΩΜΑΩΜΩΜΧΧ (A7)

In similar vein the remaining Α matrices can be replaced by premultiplying with a suitable matrix

Χ. This results in:

112222111221 ........ −−−−−− qqqqqq ΥΜΩΜΩΜΩΜΧΧΧΧ (A8)

Similarly, we want to replace 112 −−− qqq ΥΜΩ by

211 −−− qqq ΥΥΜ , where Υq-2 is an unknown

matrix. Premultiplying both sides of the identity

112211 −−−−−− = qqqqqq ΥΜΩΥΥΜ (A9)

with 11

11

−−

−− qq ΜΥ gives:

1121

111211

11

11 −−−

−−

−−−−−

−−

−− = qqqqqqqqqq ΥΜΩΜΥΥΥΜΜΥ (A10)

Page 245: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

236

so that

1121

1112 −−−

−−

−−− = qqqqqq ΥΜΩΜΥΥ . (A11)

In the same fashion all diagonal matrices in the middle can be replaced by pre- or

postmultiplying with a suitable matrix. It is important to note that the order of the algebraic

operations will result in different results. For instance, we could have started replacing the Ω

matrices first. If we again define Χ1 = Α1 this results in

122111222211 ........ ΥΥΥΥΜΑΜΑΜΑΜΧ −−−−−− qqqqqq (A12)

The next step is now to replace Α2 by an unknown matrix *2Χ so that:

11*2211 ΜΧΧΑΜΧ = (A13)

The unknown matrices *2Χ can be found by postmultiplying both sides with: 1

11

1−− ΧΜ .

11

1111

*2

11

11211

−−−− = ΧΧΜΜΧΧΧΜΑΜΧ (A14)

and

11

11211

*2

−−= ΧΧΜΑΜΧΧ . (A15)

This is different from the previous solution in (A6):

11

11

1121112

−−−= ΧΧΜΩΑΩΜΧΧ (A6)

Alternatively, the order of the algebraic operations can use a two-stage process: first shifting

theA matrices to the left (but right to 1Μ ) and theΩ matrices to the right but left to1−qΜ ). If we

denote tΡ for the matrices that replace theA matrices and tΖ for the matrices that replaces theΩ

matrices, then the end result of the first stage is:

11232122123211 ............ −−−−−−− qqqqqqq ΩΜΖΖΖΖΜΜΡΡΡΡΜΑ (A16)

To simplify our notation we define 1232

* .... −−= qq ΡΡΡΡΡ and 2321

* .... −−= qq ΖΖΖΖΖ .

(A16) can be written as:

11

*22

*11 .... −−− qqq ΩΜΖΜΜΡΜΑ

In the second stage we want to replace *Ρ and *Ζ by pre- and postmultiplying with suitable

matrices.

1***

1 ΜΧΡΜ =

Page 246: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

237

Appendix B

In this appendix we want to show the equivalence between the following four formulations

of the covariance structure for an AR(1) model:

1) conventional LISREL specification with:

( ) ( ) εΘΛΒΙΦΒΙΛΣ +

−−= −− ''11

(B1)

where in a five-wave model Β is specified as follows:

=

0000

0000

0000

0000

00000

54

43

32

21

β

β

β

β

Β (B2)

2) Factor model using model (12)

εΘΛΖΤΖΦΖΤΖΛΣ +

= −− ''''12

1 (B3)

3) Factor model using model (7)

εΘΛΨ∆Φ∆ΛΣ +

+

∏=

=

=

''1

1

1

1

q

tt

q

tt 2 (B4)

4) Geometric series (McArdle & McDonald (1984)

εΘΛΒΦΒΛΣ +

∑∑=−

=

=

''q

j

jq

j

j1

0

1

0 (B5)

where Β is specified as in(B2).

Page 247: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

238

(B1) and (B5) are well-known results. In this appendix it will first be demonstrated that

model (B3) is equivalent to model (B1). Second, it will be shown that model (B4) can be

reparametrized as model (B5). Finally the equivalence between model (B4) and model (B3) will be

demonstrated.

Equivalence between model (B3) and model (B1)

It is sufficient to show the equivalence between ΖΤΖ-1 and (Ι-Β)-1

.

ΖΤΖ-1 =

∏∏ −

=+

=+ 1

1,1

3221

21

1

1,1

3221

21

1...000

...............

0...1

00

0...01

0

0...001

1...111

...............

0...111

0...011

0...001

...000

...............

0...00

0...00

0...001

q

iii

q

iii

postmultiplying the first with the second matrix yields:

∏∏∏∏∏ −

=+

=+

=+

=+

=+ 1

1,1

3221

21

1

1,1

1

1,1

1

1,1

1

1,1

322132213221

2121

1...000

...............

0...1

00

0...01

0

0...001

...

...............

0...

0...0

0...001

q

iii

q

iii

q

iii

q

iii

q

iii

Multiplying gives the same matrix as ( ) 1−− ΒΒΙ

∏∏∏−

+−

+−

+ 1...

...............

0...1

0...01

0...001

3

,1

2

,1

1

,1

323221

21

q

ii

q

ii

q

ii

(B6)

Page 248: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

239

Equivalence between model (B3) and model (B1)

The identity ( ) ∞∞

=

− ++++==− ∑ ΑΑΑΑΑΑΑΙ ...3210

0

r1

r

is a well known result

(McArdle & McDonald, 1984, p.235). The series ends when Αr ≠ 0 while Αr+1 = 0. We show

that for an AR(1) model the length of the geometric series is determined by the number of waves,

denoted as q. What follows are the matrices for a five-wave AR(1) model (q = 5).

=

10000

01000

00100

00010

00001

=

0000

0000

0000

0000

00000

54

43

32

21

1

β

β

β

β

Β

=

=

0000

0000

0000

00000

00000

0000

0000

0000

0000

00000

0000

0000

0000

0000

00000

5443

4332

3221

54

43

32

21

54

43

32

21

2

ββ

ββ

ββ

β

β

β

β

β

β

β

β

Β

=

=

0000

0000

00000

00000

00000

0000

0000

0000

0000

00000

0000

0000

0000

00000

00000

544332

433221

54

43

32

21

5443

4332

32213

βββ

βββ

β

β

β

β

ββ

ββ

ββΒ

=

=

0000

00000

00000

00000

00000

0000

0000

0000

0000

00000

0000

0000

00000

00000

00000

5443322154

43

32

21

544332

433221

4

βββββ

β

β

β

βββ

βββ

Β

B5 = 0 so the series has 5 terms (it starts from 0). It is easy to generalize these results to

AR(1) models of any number of measurement occasions. The mechanism can be explained by the

definition of matrix multiplication AB = C: each cij is determined by the sum of products from the

Page 249: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

240

elements of the ith row of A and the jth column of B. As shown above all matrices have only

elements below the subdiagonal which implies that the matrix used for premultiplying can only

reach the elements which are placed on lower rows of the right matrix. The elements of the product

matrix are always one band further away from the main diagonal than the matrix used for

premultiplying. The series starts at the main diagonal (identity matrix) and ends at the band which is

farthest away from the main diagonal. So the number of rows (or colums) determines the length of

the series and this equals the number of waves of AR(1) model.

Next we focus on the specification of the ∆t matrices:

=∏−

=

1

1

q

t∆

10000

01000

00100

0001

00001

10000

01000

0010

00010

00001

10000

0100

00100

00010

00001

1000

01000

00100

00010

00001

21

32

43

54

ββ

ββ

If we define 0ij as an 0 matrix except for one single element ij which is nonzero, we can write:

( )( )( )( )21324354

1

10000 ++++=∏

=ΙΙΙΙ∆

q

t

=

( )( )( )213243545443 000000 +++++ ΙΙΙΙ =

( )( )21324354325432434354544332 0000000000000 ++++++++ ΙΙΙ

It follows from the definition of matrix multiplication that 0ij0kl = 0 if j ≠ k, so 325400 = 0 . We

can simplify:

( )( )2132435443543243544332

1

100000000000 +++++++=∏

=ΙΙ∆

q

t

further multiplications gives

++++++++++ 213243324321545421434321323221 000000000000000Ι213243543243542143544354 000000000000 +++

Page 250: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

241

21432154 , 0000 and 214354 000 are all 0 matrices, so the result is:

=∏−

=Ι∆

1

1

q

t

++++++++ 43543243213254433221 0000000000Ι21324354324354213243 0000000000 ++

Finally we show the following identities:

ΙΒ =0

544332211

0000 +++=Β

4354324321322

000000 ++=Β

3243542132433

000000 +=Β

213243544

0000=Β

Equivalence between Model (B3) and Model (B5).

To demonstrate the equivalence between the AR(1) using submodel (12) and the general

model (7), it is necessary to show the following identity:

1−ΖΤΖ = ∏−

=

1

1

q

tt∆ (B7)

We start with the right hand side of (B7). First we will decompose the ∆t matrices into three

matrices. Second, we will rearrange the matrices by a series of algebraic manipulations. This will

result in a new structure of only three matrices. Still one operation is needed to obtain the

formulation on the left side of (B7)

One can decompose all ∆t matrices in three matrices. The matrix in the middle is a fixed

matrix, which has to be pre- and postmultiplied with a coefficient matrix. Here we show the

decomposition for a five-wave study:

Page 251: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

242

∆5 Μ5 Ω5 Α5

=

545454

10000

01000

00100

00010

00001

11000

01000

00100

00010

00001

0000

01000

00100

00010

00001

1000

01000

00100

00010

00001

βββ

∆4 Μ4 Ω4 Α4

=

10000

01

000

00100

00010

00001

10000

01100

00100

00010

00001

10000

0000

00100

00010

00001

10000

0100

00100

00010

00001

434343 βββ

∆3 Μ3 Ω3 Α3

=

10000

01000

001

00

00010

00001

10000

01000

00110

00010

00001

10000

01000

0000

00010

00001

10000

01000

0010

00010

00001

323232 βββ

Page 252: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

243

∆2 Μ2 Ω2 Α2

=

10000

01000

00100

0001

0

00001

10000

01000

00100

00011

00001

10000

01000

00100

0000

00001

10000

01000

00100

0001

00001

212121 βββ

Hence, we can write:

==∏−

=2345

25

0∆∆∆∆∆

tt 222333444555 ΩΜΑΩΜΑΩΜΑΩΜΑ

The Ωt and Αt matrices are diagonal matrices, so we can replace their order without any

consequence:

223234345455 ΩΜΩΑΜΩΑΜΩΑΜΑ (B8)

It is easily verified that ΤΜΜΜΜ =2345 , so our aim is to find a new formulations with

2345 ΜΜΜΜ in the middle and which is equivalent to (B8). To achieve this we have to find the

appropriate set of matrices for pre- and postmultiplying, so that all matrices Αt and Ωt that reside

in the middle of (B8) will vanish. To let Α4 disappear, we have to premultiply Μ5 with an

unknown matrix Χ.

1545455−=⇒= ΜΜΑΜΧΑΜΧΜ (B9)

Substitution in (B9) gives:

22323434555 ΩΜΩΑΜΩΑΜΩΧΜΑ

The same operation is needed for Α3 and Α2 and we will denote the new matrices for

premultiplication Υ and Ζ, respectively:

15

15

1434554553455

−−−=⇒= ΜΜΩΜΑΜΩΜΥΜΩΥΜΑΜΩΜ

15

15

14

14

13234455

34455234455

−−−−−=

⇒=

ΜΩΜΩΜΑΜΩΜΩΜΖ

ΜΩΜΩΖΜΑΜΩΜΩΜ

Page 253: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

244

Substitution gives:

223344555 ΩΜΩΜΩΜΩΧΥΖΜΑ

The reverse procedure is needed for moving out the Ω matrices and the new matrices for

postmultiplication will be denoted as Ε, Ρ and Ν, respectively.

231

2223 ΜΩΜΕΕΜΜΩ −=⇒=

2341

31

223234 ΜΜΩΜΜΡΡΜΜΜΜΩ −−=⇒=

23451

41

31

22342345 ΜΜΜΩΜΜΜΝΝΜΜΜΜΜΜΩ −−−=⇒=

Substitution of these results gives:

223455 ΝΡΕΩΜΜΜΧΥΖΜΑ (B10)

If we define ΧΥΖΑ5=Q and 2ΝΡΕΩ=W and use the earlier stated identity

ΤΜΜΜΜ =2345 , we can write WQΤ as a shorthand for (B10). We now have to replace the

matrix Q with Ζ from (B10) by finding a matrix Π for postmultiplication:

WQWWWQ ΤΖΤΠΠ∆ΤΤ 111 −−−=⇒=

It can be verified that 1−= ΖΖΠW so that 1−= ΖΤΖΖΤΖΠΖΤW

References

McArdle, J.J., & McDonald, R.P. (1984). Some algebraic properties of the Reticular Action Model

for moment structures. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 37, 234-251.

Page 254: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

245

Appendix C

In this Appendix w

e show how

the covariance matrix for the B

ollen and Curran hybrid m

odel can be obtained.

We start w

ith the structural equation: (

)

+

=−

**

1ΤΖΤΖ

(94)

We define again the covariance m

atrix of the unique factors in the measurem

ent model as E

[ εε] =

Θε , further w

e define Φ4 as a 2 × 2

symm

etric covariance matrix of the grow

th curve factors as in (36), 11

214

2122

φφ

φφ

=

, where φ

11 denotes the variance of the intercept factor,

φ22 the variance of the slope factor and, φ

21 the covariance between the intercept and slope factor. Φ

2 is defined as a diagonal q × q matrix of

residual variances (see equation (57)). Also w

e define ΤΗΝ

as in (38) as T* =

− − −

1 13

12

1...

.. 1 10

1

tt

tt

ttq

.

The covariance m

atrix of the observed variables is:

(

)()

'

'C

ovC

ov

yy

++

==

ΛΛ

Σ

Substituting (94) gives: (

)(

)[

](

)(

)[

]

++

++

−−

'C

ov

*

*1

**

ΖΤΖΛ

ΤΖΤΖ

Λ

(C

1)

Using (

) ''

'=

three times, w

e consecutively can write:

Page 255: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

246

()

()

[]

()

()

++

++

−−

''

'C

ov

Λ

ΤΖΤΖ

ΤΖΤΖ

Λ*

*1

**

1,

(C2)

()

()

[]

+

+

++

−−

''

''

''

''

Cov

ΛΖ

ΤΖ

ΤΤ

ΖΤΖΛ

1*

**

*1

,

(C3)

()

()

[]

() '

''

''

''

'C

ovC

ov

+

+

+−

−Λ

ΖΤ

ΖΤ

ΤΖΤΖ

Λ1

**

**

1.

(C4)

Assum

ing that the unique terms in ε are independent of both ξ* and ξ

, and assuming that the grow

th curve factors in ξ* are independent of the

autoregressive factors in ξ, w

e can write:

()

() '

''

''

''

'C

ovC

ovC

ov

+

+

−−

ΛΖ

ΤΖ

ΤΤ

ΖΤΖΛ

1*

**

*1

(C5)

Using the definitions for Φ

2 , Φ4 , and Θ

ε w

e can write:

εΘ

ΛΖ

ΤΖ

ΦΤ

ΦΤ

ΖΤΖΛ

+

+

−−

''

''

'1

2*

4*

1

(C6)

Page 256: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

247

Next w

e show the equivalence betw

een the covariance structure of (C6) and the covariance structure of the hybrid m

odel obtained from

the conventional specification.

1*1

ξη

+=

()

21

2*2

*12

212

ζξ

ξη

βη

+−

++

=t

t

()

31

3*2

*13

323

ζξ

ξη

βη

+−

++

=t

t

(C

7)

….

….

()

qq

qq

qq

tt

ζξ

ξη

βη

+−

++

=−

−1

*2*1

11

,

Lets define Β

again as in (55) and T*

as in (38):

=

−0

00

0

....

....

..

0..

00

0..

00

0..

00

0

1,

32

21

qq

β

ββ

Β

(55)

− − −=

1 13

12

*

1...

... 1 10

1

tt

tt

ttq

Τ

(38)

In matrix form

ulation (C7) can be w

ritten as:

++

=*

Β

(C

8)

Moving Β

η to the left side gives:

Page 257: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

248

+=

−*

Β

(C

9)

Multiplying out η

:

()

+

=−

**

ΤΒ

Ι

(C10)

Premultiplying both sides w

ith ()

1 −−

yields the reduced form:

() (

)

+

−=

−*

*1Τ

ΒΙ

(C

11)

The covariance m

atrix of η is: (

)(

)()

(

)()

+−

+−

=−

−'

'C

ovC

ov

**

1*

*1

ΤΒ

ΙΤ

ΒΙ

(C

12)

Using (

) ''

'=

:

() (

)

()(

)

++

−−

−'

'C

ov1

**

**

ΙΤ

ΤΒ

Ι

,

(C

13)

and again using () '

''

=

:

Page 258: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

249

() (

)(

)

++

−−

−'

''

'C

ov1

**

**

ΙΤ

ΤΒ

Ι

,

(C

14)

which can be w

ritten as:

()

() (

)

+

−−

''

''

Cov

Cov

1*

**

*1

ΒΙ

ΤΤ

ΒΙ

,

(C

15)

and after proper substitutions:

()

()

+

−−

−'

'1

*4

*1

ΒΙ

ΨΤ

ΦΤ

ΒΙ

(C

16)

in the framew

ork of submodel (12) Ψ

is Φ2 :

+−

−'

''

ΤΖ

ΦΤ

ΦΤ

ΖΤΖ1

2*

4*

1

(C17)

Including the measurem

ent model gives (C

6) again gives:

εΘ

ΛΖ

ΤΖ

ΦΤ

ΦΤ

ΖΤΖΛ

+

+

−−

''

''

'1

2*

4*

1

(C6)

Page 259: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

250

Appendix D

In this Appendix we will demonstrate the equivalence between two multivariate longitudinal

models:

εΘΛ∆∆

∆Φ

∆∆∆

ΛΣ +

=

=

=

''

q

j tt

tq

j tt

t 1

12

1

12221

11

2221

1100 (D1)

where t = q – j + 1, and

εΘΛΒΙΒ

ΒΙΦ

ΒΙΒΒΙ

ΛΣ +

−−−

−−−

=−−

''1

2221

112

1

2221

11 00 (D2)

where Β is specified as follows:

=

=

000000

000000

000000

00000000

0000000

0000000

0000000

00000000

8783

7672

6561

43

32

21

2221

11

ββββ

ββ

ββ

β

ΒΒΒ

Β0

(D3)

We will not provide a general proof, but only demonstrate the equivalence for a four-wave

AR(1) model with lagged effects.

If we denote the free parameters in ∆t in (D1) as the equivalent βs in (D3), the ∆t matrices

can be written as:

Page 260: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

251

111111 234 ∆∆∆

1000

0100

001

0001

1000

010

0010

0001

100

0100

0010

0001

21

32

43

ββ

β

(D4)

222222 234 ∆∆∆

1000

0100

001

0001

1000

010

0010

0001

100

0100

0010

0001

65

76

87

ββ

β

(D5)

212121 234 ∆∆∆

0000

0000

000

0000

0000

000

0000

0000

000

0000

0000

0000

61

72

83

β

β

β

(D6)

Β21 from (D2) can be defined as ∑−

=

1

121

q

jt∆

The following identity has to established:

=

1

1 2221

11q

j tt

t

∆∆∆ 0

=

1

2221

11−

−−

ΒΙΒ

ΒΙ 0 (D7)

Page 261: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

252

We start with working out the left hand side of (D7):

=

2221

11

2221

11

2221

11

22

2

33

3

44

4

∆∆∆

∆∆∆

∆∆∆ 000

++

222222212222112122111121

111111

234234234234

234

∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆ 0

(D8)

At this point it is more convenient to adjust the notation: the lower indices reflect which element is

nonzero in the matrix. Let Ιij and 0ij be defined as identity and null matrices, respectively, except

for only one element ij which is a free parameter (i ≠ j). The specific β parameter that is set free is

denoted by a sub-subscript.

Top left quadrant:111111 234 ∆∆∆ =

213243213243 βββ

ΙΙΙ (D9)

Bottom right: 222222 234 ∆∆∆ =

657687213243 βββ

ΙΙΙ

Bottom left quadrant: 212222112122111121 234234234 ∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆∆ ++ =

213283213243 βββ

ΙΙ0 +217287

213243 βββΙΙ 0 +

617687213243 βββ

0ΙΙ

Page 262: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

253

Next we concentrate on model (D2). Using the formula for a partitioned inverse1

( )( ) ( )( ) ( )

−−−−−

−−−−

122

11121

122

111

ΒΙΒΙΒΒΙ

ΒΙ 0 (D10)

The top left quadrant can be written as:( ) =− −111ΒΙ

213243213243 βββ

ΙΙΙ ,

the bottom right quadrant as: ( ) =− −122ΒΙ

657687213243 βββ

ΙΙΙ ,

and the left bottom quadrant as: ( ) ( )( ) =−−−− −− 11121

122 ΒΙΒΒΙ

− (657687

213243 βββΙΙΙ )−(

162738213243 βββ

000 ++ )(213243

213243 βββΙΙΙ ).

Multiplying out the last result in the left bottom quadrant gives:

(657687

213243 βββΙΙΙ )(

3843β

0 )(213243

213243 βββΙΙΙ ) +

(657687

213243 βββΙΙΙ )(

2732β

0 )(213243

213243 βββΙΙΙ ) +

(657687

213243 βββΙΙΙ )(

1621β

0 )(213243

213243 βββΙΙΙ ) (D11)

It is easily derived from the rules of matrix multiplication2 and the definition of the Ιij and 0ij

matrices that each of the three terms of (D10) reduces to a product of matrices containing only

1 Graybill describes the inverse of partitioned matrix as follows.

Β ΒΒ Β

11 12

21 22

1

=Α ΑΑ Α

11 12

21 22

, where

( )Α Β Β Β Β11 11 12 221

21

1= − − −

( )Α Β Β Α Β Β Β Β Β12 111

12 22 111

12 22 111

12

1= − = − −− − − −

( )Α Β Β Β Β Β Β Β Α Β Β22 22 21 111

12

1

221

221

21 11 12 221= − = +− − − − −

( )Α Β Β Α Β Β Β Β Β Β21 221

21 11 221

21 11 12 221

21

1= − = − − =− − − −

Page 263: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

254

matrices which column number equals the row number of the subsequent matrix (for null or identity

matrices except for one free extra parameter matrices Μ: Μj,j-1Μj-1,j-2, Μj-1,j-2….).

For instance, the last term of (D11) (657687

213243 βββΙΙΙ )(

1621β

0 )(213243

213243 βββΙΙΙ )

reduces to 7687

3243 ββΙΙ

3843β

0 . The product of the last three matrices, 213243

213243 βββΙΙΙ , is

a lower triangular matrix with 1’s on the diagonal. Postmultiplying the matrix in the middle (which

has one nonzero element on the second row and the first column) with this product, is the same as

postmultiplying with an identity matrix:

(16

21β0 )(

213243213243 βββ

ΙΙΙ )=16

21β0

and the first term reduces to:

(657687

213243 βββΙΙΙ )(

1621β

0 ).

Premultiplying the last “zero” matrix with the third matrix has the same effect as premultiplying

with and identity matrix:

(65

21βΙ )(

1621β

0 )=16

21β0

Now the third term reduces to:

76873243 ββ

ΙΙ16

21β0 .

Applying the same logic to the first and the second terms, (D10) can be simplified to:

3843β

02132

2132 ββΙΙ +

8743β

Ι27

32β0

2121β

Ι +7687

3243 ββΙΙ

1621β

0 , which is the same as the

structure in the bottom left quadrant of (D9). Hence it has been proved that the alternative

specification (D1) is equivalent to the conventional specification (D2).

2 If rnnmrm ××× = BAC then ∑==

n

kkjikij bac

1

Page 264: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

255

Appendix E

In the last decade, important improvements have been made in the statistical modeling of

longitudinal data. One important class is the random coefficient model and the SEM variant which

is called the latent growth curve model.

Good introductions of latent growth curve models already exist (see Curran (in press),

MacCallum, Kim, Malarkey, Kieholt-Glaser (1997), Muthén (1997), Muthén and Curran (1997),

Willett and Sayer (1994, 1995). However, a short explanation will be given here.

The Latent Growth Curve Model distinguishes a within-person level (individual level or

Level 1) and a between-person level (group level or Level 2). It is easiest to explain the model by

introducing the individual level first. In Figure E1, some data points for an arbitrary participant are

plotted. On the x-axis the time dimension is displayed.

T1 T0 T2 T3 T4 T5

b0

Figure E1. Linear growth curve for a single participant.

If a straight line can reasonably approximate the data, we can specify a linear growth curve:

ηti = β0i + β1i t + εti (E1)

where ηti represents the true score at time t for person i, β0i is the person’s intercept and β1i is the

person’s slope, t is the time of assessment and εti the person’s residual at time t.

Page 265: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

256

We now extend the model to include multiple participants. In Figure E2 (top panel), the

growth curves for four participants are depicted and the bold line represents the population growth

curve.

T1 T0 T2 T3 T4 T5

Participant 1

Participant 2

Participant 4

Participant 3

Y

T1 T0 T2 T3 T4 T5

Participant 1

Participant 2

Participant 4

Participant 3

X

Figure E2. Four individual linear growth curves and the population growth curve (bold) for variable y shown in top panel and for variable x displayed in the bottom panel.

Page 266: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

257

The lines differ in their starting points as well in their slopes. The introduction of multiple

participants may create variation in both the individual intercepts and slopes. In the Structural

Equation Modeling framework the intercepts and slopes are treated as latent variables and hence in

a linear model the observed scores of each participant can be explained by an intercept factor score,

a slope factor score and a time-specific residual. Accordingly, we change our notation and replace

β0i with η0i and β1i with η1i to express that these are treated as latent factors (intercept and slope

factor). In a linear growth model the factor loadings are fixed constants and are proportional to the

elapsed time from the first measurement occasion. In a six-wave study with equispaced data, we

can fix the values of the factor loadings to 0, 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5. We can now formulate a between

model (Level 2) where the individual slopes and intercepts are expressed as deviations from the

population slope and intercept respectively.

ηti = µ0 + η0i + (µ1 + η1i )t + εti (E2)

or, equivalently,

ηti = µ0 + µ1t + (η0i + η1i t + εti ) (E3)

The last formulation shows that the model can be specified as two additive components: a fixed part

(parameters without subject indices) and a random part (subject indices added). If we take the

expectancies of Equation E3 we find that these can be expressed by the parameters of the fixed part

(the mean intercept and the mean slope). The variances and covariances of Equation E3 refer to the

random part and the parameters are the variance of the intercept factor, the variance of the slope

factor, the covariance between both factors, and the variances of the time specific residuals.

It is convenient to fix the factor loading for the first measurement occasion at the value of

zero. In this case the intercept represents the expected initial value for a particular participant. In the

linear model, the model is a factor model with fixed factor loadings (referring to the time elapsed

from the first measurement occasion). A graphical model is shown in Figure E3. Note also that to

estimate the parameters that belong to the fixed part of the model, the vector of observed means has

to be supplied along with as the covariance matrix.

From the perspective of an applied researcher, it is more interesting to go beyond the

description of individual change and include predictors for the differences in individual trajectories.

For instance, if two variables have been measured on several occasions, it might be interesting to

Page 267: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

258

1

t5-t1

t4-t1

t3-t1

t2-t1

1 1 1 1

ηt=1 ηt=2 ηt=3 ηt=4 ηt=5

slope factor intercept factor

η0 η1

Figure E3. Latent Linear Growth Curve Model specified as a factor model with all factor loadings fixed (note that the first slope factor loading is fixed to the value of zero). relate both developments. In these multivariate or cross-domain growth curve models hypotheses

can be formulated in which characteristics of one growth curve may have predictive value for

characteristics for another growth curve. For instance, participants who have a steeper slope in

variable x may also tend to have higher slopes on variable y. In this case changes in both processes

are related. Alternatively, participants who tend to have higher initial values on variable x may have

on average higher slopes on variable y. This is displayed in Figure E2 (variable y is displayed in the

top panel and variable x is shown in the bottom panel). Also, constant background variables may be

used for explaining differences in growth.

If a linear function is not appropriate to describe the data, quadratic or even higher order

polynomials can be used instead. But Willett (1989, p. 590) remarked that in many instances a

linear function might be an acceptable approximation. There are several interpretations for the

existence of time-specific residuals. First, there may be measurement error, but in the case that a

measurement model is included, the growth curve refers to the true variates. A second interpretation

is that the presence of states is responsible for the more irregular short-termed changes. Kenny &

Campbell (1989) argued that many psychological constructs probably have both traitlike and

statelike aspects. The use of growth curves enables a decomposition of intraindividual ‘trait

changes’ (Nesselroade, 1991) and state changes (see also McArdle & Woodcock, 1997). A third

interpretation of the residual variance is related to approximation error: The model is somewhat

Page 268: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

259

misspecified. This might be caused by variables that are omitted from the model. After the

introduction of time-varying covariates to the model, the chosen growth function may better

describe the underlying development over time (after controlling for other time-specific influences).

Therefore, there are many explanations of why the changes in many data sets seem somewhat

erratic, but it may very well be that the underlying developments over time are much smoother and

individual linear trend lines may give a useful approximation of the developmental process.

An alternative way to specify a nonlinear model is to estimate some of the factor loadings

(except those necessary for identification). Statistically, a linear model is still estimated, but the

nonlinear interpretation emerges by relating the estimated factor loadings to the real time frame.

This is displayed in Figure E4.

T1 T0 T2 T3 T4 T5 T2* T3* T4* T5* T1* T0*

Real Timeframe

Estimated Timeframe

Figure E4. Nonlinear growth curve with free estimated factor loadings for T2 to T5.

In this figure, the first factor loading is fixed at zero and the second loading is also fixed (e.g., at the

value of 1). Apparently, by stretching and shrinking the time axis one can simulate an acceleration

and deceleration of the time dimension, again assuming a constant rate of change. Therefore, a new

time frame is estimated and the transformation to the real time frame gives the nonlinear

Page 269: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

260

interpretation. For instance, the estimate of T2 is larger than the real time elapsed, so apparently

more positive change has taken place than would be predicted linearly.

A complication may arise in models containing two or more growth curves. If these curves

are all specified as nonlinear by freeing some of the factor loadings, it is not possible to test the

significance of some growth curve parameters. This can be explained as follows: For each growth

curve at least two factor loadings have to be fixed for identification purposes. However, which

loadings one chooses and to which values the loadings are fixed, the z values of some of the growth

parameter estimates in these multivariate nonlinear latent growth curve models. Because the

fixation schemes of the factor loadings of the growth curve models are arbitrary to some extent, the

unstandardized estimates and the standard errors are arbitrary as well (and their ratio is not

constant). Fortunately, the correlations between the intercept and slope factors are not influenced by

fixation schemes with the same choice for the zero point of the time axis. However, a shift in the

time axis by choosing a different zero point leads to additional complications (Rovine & Molenaar,

1998).

References

Curran, P.J. (in press). A latent curve framework for the study of developmental trajectories in

adolescent substance use. In J. Rose, L. Chassin, C. Presson, & J. Sherman (Eds.), Multivariate

applications in substance use research. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

Kenny, D.A., & Campbell, D.T. (1989). On the measurement of stability in over-time data. Journal

of Personality, 57, 445-481.

MacCallum, R.C., Kim, C., Malarkey, W.B., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K. (1997). Studying multivariate

change using multilevel models and latent curve models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32,

215-253.

McArdle, J.J., & Woodcock, R.W. (1997). Expanding test-retest designs to include developmental

time-lag components. Psychological Methods, 2, 403-435.

Muthén, B. (1997). Latent variable modeling with longitudinal and multilevel data. In A. Raftery

(Ed.), Sociological Methodology (pp. 453-480). Boston: Blackwell Publishers.

Muthén, B.O., & Curran, P.J. (1997). General longitudinal modeling of individual differences in

experimental designs: A latent variable framework for analysis and power estimation.

Psychological Methods, 2, 371-402.

Page 270: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

261

Nesselroade, J.R. (1991). Interindividual differences in intraindividual change. In L. M. Collins &

J.L. Horn, Best methods for the analysis change: Recent advances, unanswered questions, future

directions (pp. 92-105). Washington, DC.: American Psychological Association.

Rovine, M.J. & Molenaar, P.C.M. (1998). The covariance between level and shape in the latent

growth curve model with estimated basis vector coefficients. Methods of Psychological Research,

3, 95-107.

Willett, J.B. (1989). Some results on reliability for the longitudinal measurement of change:

Implications for the design of studies of individual growth. Education and Psychological

Measurement, 49, 587-602.

Willett, J.B., & Sayer, A.G. (1994). Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates and

predictors of individual change over time. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 363-381.

Willett, J.B., & Sayer, A.G. (1995). Cross-domain analysis of change over time: Combining growth

curve modeling and covariance structure analysis. In G.A. Marcoulides & R.E. Schumacker (Eds.).

Advanced structural equation modeling: Issues and techniques, (pp. 125-157). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Page 271: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

262

Page 272: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

263

Samenvatting

Deze dissertatie bestaat uit twee gedeelten. Het eerste gedeelte is theoretisch en beschrijft een

algemeen longitudinaal model dat valt binnen de klasse van structurele vergelijkingsmodellen. Een

speciaal geval van dit algemene longitudinale model is al voldoende algemeen om enige uit de

literatuur bekende longitudinale modellen als bijzondere gevallen te beschouwen. Aan de orde

komen autoregressieve en latente groeimodellen. Echter, om een tweede orde autoregressief model

te beschrijven moet terug gegrepen worden op het meest algemene model. Er wordt beschreven

welke aannamen over hoe verandering tot stand komt ten grondslag liggen aan de verschillende

modellen. Veranderingen kunnen beschreven worden als expliciete functie van de tijd maar als ook

als een proces dat gedeeltelijk voorspelbaar is uit voorafgaande toestanden waaraan steeds

innovaties gebaseerd op toeval worden toegevoegd. Recentelijk werd een hybride model

geïntroduceerd door Bollen en Curran dat te beschouwen is als een samensmelting van een eerste

orde autoregressief model en een latente groeicurve. Een kleine aanpassing van het eerder

genoemde submodel bleek al voldoende te zijn om ook dit hybride model als een speciaal geval

hiervan te beschouwen. Ook bleek het eenvoudig te zijn om het algemene longitudinale model uit te

breiden tot multivariate modellen waarbij meerdere veranderingsprocessen tegelijkertijd beschreven

kunnen worden. Ten slotte werd ingegaan op de schaalinvariantie van latente groeimodellen.

Deel twee bestaat uit drie artikelen, waarbij longitudinale modellen toegepast worden in

vraagstukken uit de Arbeids- en Organisatiepsychologie. Deze hebben betrekking op data uit de

voormalige DDR. Dit onderzoeksproject is gestart onmiddellijk na de Duitse hereniging in 1990.

De centrale vraagstelling van dit project richt zich op het vaststellen van de determinanten van

psychisch welbevinden en het nemen van initiatief in een situatie die gekenmerkt wordt door vele

ingrijpende veranderingen.

Het eerste artikel behandelt een socialiserend-occupatie model waarbij de effecten van kenmerken

van werk werden onderzocht op de neiging van werknemers om initiatieven te ontplooien. Er werd

verondersteld dat de mate van regelmogelijkheden, waarover een werknemer beschikt om zijn/haar

taak uit te voeren en de complexiteit van het werk een socialiserende werking kan hebben. In het

bijzonder werd gesteld dat werk waarbij men weinig invloed kan uitoefenen op de arbeidstaken en

werk dat slechts uit eenvoudige handelingen bestaat, een slechte voedingsbodem vormt voor

opdoen van positieve leerervaringen. Daarentegen geeft werk met regelmogelijkheden en enige

complexiteit de gelegenheid om nieuwe vaardigheden aan te leren en daarbij grenzen te

overschrijden. Dit leidt tot een verandering in de oriëntatie van werknemers. Deze oriëntatie wordt

Page 273: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

264

gekenmerkt door het besef dat men beschikt over een gedragsrepertoire dat tot gewenste uitkomsten

kan leiden, door de overtuiging dat de omgeving beïnvloedbaar is, en door het streven zelf meer

invloed uit te oefenen ondanks de mogelijke nadelen die eraan verbonden zijn (meer

verantwoordelijkheid en kans op falen). Deze op de omgeving gerichte oriëntatie wordt geacht een

positief effect te sorteren op het ontplooien van initiatieven. Initiatief werd gedefinieerd als

autonoom pro-actief gedrag waarbij men zelf doelen stelt die verder gaan dan het eenvoudigweg

uitvoeren van de taak waarvoor men aangesteld is. Deze door de werknemer zelf gestelde doelen

vallen weliswaar binnen de door de organisatie nagestreefde doelstellingen, maar zijn niet

eenvoudigweg afleidbaar uit de direct opgedragen taak. Veelal hebben de zelf gestelde doelen

betrekking op de (middel)lange termijn. Verder werd initiatief zodanig gedefinieerd dat de

werknemer over een zeker doorzettingsvermogen moet tonen indien hindernissen de

verwezenlijking van deze doelen dreigen te belemmeren. Het resultaat van het ontplooien van

initiatieven werd over het algemeen geacht positief te zijn. Mogelijke gevolgen zijn dat werknemers

betere banen verkrijgen die meer invloed met zich mee brengen en een grotere complexiteit met

zich mee dragen. Ook kan het zijn dat initiatiefrijke werknemers een verbeterd takenpakket

toegewezen krijgen met meer regelmogelijkheden. Door initiatief te tonen kan de werknemer de

omgeving en zijn taak deels zelf bepalen. Aan de andere kant gaat het socialiserend occupatie

model ervan uit dat de werknemer ook enigszins gevormd door zijn/haar omgeving. Dit theoretisch

model werd getoetst met verschillende structurele vergelijkingsmodellen. De resultaten gaven aan

dat het best passende model een goede beschrijving gaf van de data. In belangrijke mate werd steun

gevonden voor de gevonden theorie. Helaas dienen deze resultaten als voorlopig te worden

bestempeld, omdat door beperking van de gebruikte software onvoldoende rekening kon worden

gehouden met complicaties die ontstonden doordat vele respondenten niet gedurende de volledige

onderzoeksperiode over werk beschikten. In de getoetste modellen werden de gegevens als

ontbrekend aangemerkt, waardoor impliciete assumpties gemaakt werden over de meest plausibele

populatiewaarden. Echter, aannamen over plausibele waarden in de populatie voor werkgerelateerde

variabelen voor mensen zonder werk zijn onbevredigend, omdat deze waarden ook in de populatie

behoren te ontbreken omdat zij simpelweg niet van toepassing zijn. Recente ontwikkelingen op het

gebied van statistische software waarbij latente klasse modellen en structurele

vergelijkingsmodellen worden geïntegreerd, maken het mogelijk om meer adequate modellen te

toetsen.

Het tweede artikel behandelt de relatie tussen stressoren op het werk en stressklachten. De

volgende stressoren werden gemeten: de onzekerheid over het behoud van werk, het werken onder

tijdsdruk, het omgaan met organisatorische problemen, sociale stressoren en onzekerheid als gevolg

Page 274: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

265

van rolambiguïteit en rolconflicten. De stressklachten omvatten depressieve klachten,

psychosomatisch klachten, gevoelens van irritatie en piekeren over het werk (buiten werkuren).

Verschillende modellen werden getoetst. In het eerste model werd één factor verantwoordelijk

gehouden voor het verklaren van zowel de stressoren als de stressklachten. In de literatuur wordt

negatieve affectiviteit genoemd die zowel de waarneming van stressoren als ook de stressklachten

kunnen verklaren. Er bleek geen empirische steun voor dit model te zijn.

De stressklachten konden goed benaderd worden door groeicurve modellen. In deze modellen wordt

voor iedere persoon een groeicurve opgesteld, die het verloop van de stressklachten als een continue

functie van de tijd weergeeft. Op elk meetmoment kunnen afwijkingen van deze curve optreden als

gevolg van tijdelijke schommelingen. Non-lineaire modellen werden gefit waarbij voor iedere

persoon de verandering in de tijd niet constant was, maar variatie vertoonde. Ook de stressoren

konden goed benaderd worden door groeicurven, hoewel hier de autoregressieve modellen een iets

betere fit te zien gaven. Het lag dan ook voor de hand een hybride model op te stellen waarbij de

stressklachten als groeicurven werden gemodelleerd en de stressoren een autoregressief beeld te

zien gaven. Tegelijkertijd werden er ook modellen gefit waarbij zowel de stressklachten als de

stressoren als groeicurven werden gemodelleerd. Het laatste model bood de mogelijkheid te

onderzoeken of de trends in de stressklachten correleerde met de trends in de stressoren.

Systematische veranderingen in piekeren over het werk bleek samen te gaan met trendmatige

veranderingen in het werken onder tijdsdruk en het ervaren van onzekerheid. De individuele trends

in sociale stressoren bleek in zekere mate parallel te zijn met de trends in psychosomatische

klachten.

In longitudinale modellen kan de tijdsvolgorde van gebeurtenissen gebruikt worden om

uitspraken te doen over oorzaak- gevolg relaties. In een van de opgestelde modellen werd gekeken

of personen met veel stressklachten aan het begin van de onderzoeksperiode een ander verloop van

hun stressoren lieten zien. Daarbij zijn twee theorieën ban belang. De ene theorie gaat ervan uit dat

mensen met veel stressklachten minder presteren en daardoor terugvallen tot steeds slechtere banen,

waardoor ze aan nog meer stressoren blootstaan. De anderen theorie voorspelt het

tegenovergestelde: mensen die veel stressklachten hebben, zullen alles in het werk stellen om zich

beter te voelen en dan ook minder belastend werk gaan zoeken. De laatste theorie werd meer

ondersteund, maar er werd geconcludeerd dat beide mechanismen een rol kunnen hebben gespeeld

voor verschillende subgroepen.

Een ander model dat gebruik maakt van de tijdsvolgorde is het incubatie model. Hier wordt

geponeerd dat het geruime tijd kan duren voordat de gevolgen van stressoren zich manifesteren in

stressklachten. Dit kan getoetst worden door de correlatie tussen het aanvangsniveau van stressoren

Page 275: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

266

en het verloop van de stressklachten. Voor dit model werd geen steun gevonden. Een probleem met

dit model is dat slechts het aanvangsniveau van stressoren bekeken werd, terwijl het verloop van de

stressoren zelf natuurlijk eveneens kortdurende effecten kan hebben op de stressklachten.

Kortdurende effecten konden worden getoetst met het hybride model, waarbij de

stressklachten als groeicurven werden gemodelleerd en de stressoren een autoregressief verloop

hadden. Hierdoor is het mogelijk de tijdspecifieke afwijkingen van de groeicurven te laten verklaren

door het momentane niveau van de stressoren. Dit model gaat ervan uit dat de stressklachten een

trendmatige verandering ondergaan, maar dat op elk moment afwijkingen kunnen ontstaan door een

meer of minder dan gemiddeld niveau van de stressoren. Het bleek dat synchrone effecten van

stressoren redelijk goed deze schommelingen in de stressklachten konden verklaren. Opvallend was

dat de synchrone effecten van het werken onder tijdsdruk sterke effecten hadden op de niet

trendmatige schommelingen in het piekeren over het werk (buiten werkuren). Er werd dan ook

geconcludeerd dat zowel korte termijn effecten als ook lange termijn effecten relaties optraden.

Het derde artikel onderzoekt de effecten van optimisme op het psychische welbevinden. In

de literatuur wordt veelvuldig gesproken over de positieve effecten van de

persoonlijkheidseigenschap optimisme. Optimisme wordt gedefinieerd als het hebben van positieve

toekomstverwachtingen in het algemeen. De gunstige invloed van optimisme zou verklaard kunnen

worden door een effectievere manier om met stress om te gaan. Optimisten zouden meer

probleemgericht omgaan met stress omdat ze in het algemeen verwachten dat dit positief zou

uitpakken. Daarentegen zouden pessimisten meer gericht zijn op het verminderen van de

stressklachten en zich minder bezig houden met het aanpakken van de problemen zelf. In het

algemeen wordt ervan uitgegaan dat een probleemgerichte aanpak effectiever is doordat stressoren

met wortel en al uitgeroeid worden.

In de literatuur wordt onderkend dat de schaal om optimisme te meten (Life Orientation

Test), bij factoranalyse een twee factor oplossing laat zien, waarbij de vier positief gestelde items

laden op de ene factor (optimisme) en de vier negatief geformuleerde items op de tweede factor

(pessimisme). Beide factoren bleken slechts een zwakke tot matige samenhang te vertonen. Verder

bleek dat beide factoren een verschillend patroon van samenhangen vertoonden met andere

constructen. Deze resultaten werden eveneens in deze studie teruggevonden. Sommige

onderzoekers menen dat methodologische artefacten verantwoordelijk zijn voor het uiteenvallen

van de optimisme schaal, maar overtuigende resultaten zijn vooralsnog niet gepubliceerd. Hierdoor

is het aan te bevelen om optimisme en pessimisme voorlopig als afzonderlijke constructen te

behandelen.

Page 276: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

267

Psychisch welbevinden werd gemeten met dezelfde schalen als de stressklachten uit

hoofdstuk 5, maar de term werd aangepast aan de vigerende terminologie uit de optimisme

literatuur.

In dit hoofdstuk werden eveneens groeicurven gemodelleerd en het bleek dat zowel de

veranderingen in optimisme als pessimisme goed te modelleren waren als lineaire groeicurven.

Eveneens werden er verschillen modellen onderzocht om de effecten van

optimisme/pessimisme op psychisch welbevinden te onderzoeken.

Zowel directe als indirecte modellen werden opgesteld. In de indirecte modellen werd de

voor de persoon kenmerkende stijl hoe men met stress omgaat (‘coping style’) als mediërende

variabelen gemodelleerd. De individuele stressaanpak werd gemeten door de afzonderlijke items

voor de gehele periode te sommeren. Door situatiespecifieke effecten uit te middelen werd getracht

de voor de persoon typische coping stijl te meten.

Slechts twee verschillende groeimodellen werden getoetst: een model zonder dat de

tijdsspecifieke residuen van de groeicurven van optimisme/pessimisme met elkaar correleerden en

een model waarbij deze wel correleerden. De betekenis van de correlaties tussen residuen is dat de

niet systematische schommelingen in optimisme (pessimisme) samenhangen met de korte termijn

fluctuaties in psychische welbevinden. Men kan door stemmingswisselingen geneigd zijn een wat

optimistische kijk te hebben en tevens het eigen welbevinden wat hoger in te schatten. Tevens

kunnen de systematische trends in optimisme en psychisch welbevinden parallel verlopen.

Modellen waarin depressie opgenomen was bleken beter te passen met gecorreleerde residuen,

terwijl de modellen met psychosomatische klachten, irritatie en piekeren over het werk beter pasten

zonder deze correlaties tussen de residuen. Echter, het patroon van de correlaties tussen de residuen

in de depressie modellen was zodanig inconsistent, dat een inhoudelijke interpretatie van deze

correlaties verworpen werd.

Verschillende hypothesen konden getoetst worden met behulp van de parameters van de

groeimodellen.

Het aanvangsniveau van vooral pessimisme en depressie en in mindere mate irritatie bleken

significant samen te hangen. Mogelijkerwijze kunnen erfelijke factoren of factoren uit de vroege

kindertijd deze samenhang verklaren.

De trends in pessimisme en de trends in depressie bleken zeer sterk samen te hangen. In wat

mindere mate gold dit voor de samenhang in de trends van pessimisme en irritatie. Trends in

optimisme en trends in psychisch welbevinden hingen nauwelijks samen.

Het aanvangsniveau van optimisme had een significant negatief effect op de verandering in

depressie en psychosomatische klachten. Indirecte effecten bleken hierbij geen rol te spelen. In het

Page 277: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Longitudinal ... · Longitudinal research using structural equation modeling applied in studies of determinants of psychological well-being

268

algemeen werden slechts weinig significante effecten van de coping stijl variabelen op

veranderingen in psychisch welbevinden gevonden. Uitzondering waren de negatieve effecten van

planning en probleemgerichte aanpak op individuele trends in depressie, terwijl een emotiegerichte

aanpak juist de depressie klachten bevorderderden.

Opmerkelijk was dat het omgaan met stress redelijk voorspelbaar was op grond van het

aanvangsniveau van psychisch welbevinden en de initiële status van optimisme en pessimisme.

Mensen die zich aan het begin van de onderzoeksperiode slecht voelden (depressief, veel

psychosomatische klachten, geïrriteerd), maakten meer gebruik van een stressaanpak gericht om de

symptomen te verzachten (emotiegerichte aanpak, dagdromen en wensdenken), terwijl mensen die

aan het begin van de periode veel piekerden over hun werk juist gekenmerkt werden door een

probleemgerichte aanpak (inclusief planning). Optimisten maakten zowel meer gebruik van een

probleem gerichte als ook een emotiegerichte aanpak. Het laatste was geheel onverwacht en niet

bekend uit de literatuur. Pessimisten maakten minder gebruik van planning, maar meer gebruik van

emotiegerichte aanpak en gaven zich over aan dagdromen en wensdenken.

Samenvattend kan geconcludeerd worden dat in deze dissertatie is aangetoond dat

groeicurve modellen voor de toegepaste onderzoeker een grote waarde hebben. Zeker met het

verschijnen van nieuwe software die het mogelijk maakt latente klasse modellen te integreren met

structurele vergelijkings modellen zijn vele nieuwe toepassingsmogelijkheden binnen het bereik van

de onderzoeker gekomen.