toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...fernandino, stephen b. simons, mario...

46
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pcgn20 Download by: [University of South Carolina ] Date: 19 February 2017, At: 16:22 Cognitive Neuropsychology ISSN: 0264-3294 (Print) 1464-0627 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcgn20 Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation Jeffrey R. Binder, Lisa L. Conant, Colin J. Humphries, Leonardo Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai To cite this article: Jeffrey R. Binder, Lisa L. Conant, Colin J. Humphries, Leonardo Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 33:3-4, 130-174, DOI: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1147426 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2016.1147426 View supplementary material Published online: 16 Jun 2016. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 596 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 4 View citing articles

Upload: others

Post on 22-Jan-2021

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

Full Terms amp Conditions of access and use can be found athttpwwwtandfonlinecomactionjournalInformationjournalCode=pcgn20

Download by [University of South Carolina ] Date 19 February 2017 At 1622

Cognitive Neuropsychology

ISSN 0264-3294 (Print) 1464-0627 (Online) Journal homepage httpwwwtandfonlinecomloipcgn20

Toward a brain-based componential semanticrepresentation

Jeffrey R Binder Lisa L Conant Colin J Humphries Leonardo FernandinoStephen B Simons Mario Aguilar amp Rutvik H Desai

To cite this article Jeffrey R Binder Lisa L Conant Colin J Humphries LeonardoFernandino Stephen B Simons Mario Aguilar amp Rutvik H Desai (2016) Toward a brain-basedcomponential semantic representation Cognitive Neuropsychology 333-4 130-174 DOI1010800264329420161147426

To link to this article httpdxdoiorg1010800264329420161147426

View supplementary material

Published online 16 Jun 2016

Submit your article to this journal

Article views 596

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles 4 View citing articles

Toward a brain-based componential semantic representationJeffrey R Bindera Lisa L Conanta Colin J Humphriesa Leonardo Fernandinoa Stephen B SimonsbMario Aguilarb and Rutvik H Desaic

aDepartment of Neurology Medical College of Wisconsin Milwaukee WI USA bTeledyne Scientific LLC Durham NC USA cDepartment ofPsychology University of South Carolina Columbia SC USA

ABSTRACTComponential theories of lexical semantics assume that concepts can be represented by sets offeatures or attributes that are in some sense primitive or basic components of meaning Thebinary features used in classical category and prototype theories are problematic in that thesefeatures are themselves complex concepts leaving open the question of what constitutes aprimitive feature The present availability of brain imaging tools has enhanced interest in howconcepts are represented in brains and accumulating evidence supports the claim that theserepresentations are at least partly ldquoembodiedrdquo in the perception action and other modal neuralsystems through which concepts are experienced In this study we explore the possibility ofdevising a componential model of semantic representation based entirely on such functionaldivisions in the human brain We propose a basic set of approximately 65 experiential attributesbased on neurobiological considerations comprising sensory motor spatial temporal affectivesocial and cognitive experiences We provide normative data on the salience of each attributefor a large set of English nouns verbs and adjectives and show how these attribute vectorsdistinguish a priori conceptual categories and capture semantic similarity Robust quantitativedifferences between concrete object categories were observed across a large number ofattribute dimensions A within- versus between-category similarity metric showed much greaterseparation between categories than representations derived from distributional (latent semantic)analysis of text Cluster analyses were used to explore the similarity structure in the dataindependent of a priori labels revealing several novel category distinctions We discuss howsuch a representation might deal with various longstanding problems in semantic theory suchas feature selection and weighting representation of abstract concepts effects of context onsemantic retrieval and conceptual combination In contrast to componential models based onverbal features the proposed representation systematically relates semantic content to large-scale brain networks and biologically plausible accounts of concept acquisition

ARTICLE HISTORYReceived 14 June 2015Revised 24 November 2015Accepted 21 January 2016

KEYWORDSSemantics conceptrepresentation embodiedcognition cognitiveneuroscience

Introduction

Theories of semantic representation generally beginwith analyses of conceptual content This analyticalor componential approach assumes that conceptscan ultimately be represented by sets of features orattributes that are in some sense primitive or basiccomponents of meaning The present availability ofbrain imaging tools has enhanced interest in how con-cepts are represented in human neural systems andaccumulating evidence supports the claim that theserepresentations are at least partly ldquoembodiedrdquo in theperception action and other modal neural systemsthrough which concepts are acquired (Binder ampDesai 2011 Kiefer amp Pulvermuumlller 2012 MeteyardRodriguez Cuadrado Bahrami amp Vigliocco 2012) In

this paper we explore the possibility of devising a suf-ficiently explanatory componential model of semanticrepresentation based entirely on such functional div-isions in the human brain

An essential starting point is to define what types ofentities are the components of such a representationand how these differ from those of more standardsemantic theories In classical category theory con-cepts are defined by the presence or absence ofbinary features that are necessary and sufficient forcategory identification (Aristotle 1995350 BCE) Theconcept of bird for example is composed of such fea-tures as wings feathers beak flying egg-laying and soon An important extension of classical theory was therealization that most categories have somewhat fuzzy

copy 2016 Informa UK Limited trading as Taylor amp Francis Group

CONTACT Jeffrey R Binder jbindermcweduSupplemental data for this article can be accessed at httpdxdoiorg1010800264329420161147426

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 2016VOL 33 NOS 3ndash4 130ndash174httpdxdoiorg1010800264329420161147426

boundaries due to varying degrees of prototypicalityamong members (Rosch Mervis Gray Johnson ampBoyes-Braem 1976) Thus a penguin could still beidentified (though more slowly) as a bird despite thefact that it does not fly through a process of jointlyweighing all of its features Extensive work usingfeature generation tasks has documented whatpeople think of as the features of entities and howthese features rank in importance for a givenconcept (Cree amp McRae 2003 Garrard LambonRalph Hodges amp Patterson 2001 McRae de Sa ampSeidenberg 1997 Vinson Vigliocco Cappa amp Siri2003) Such data provide a powerful means of asses-sing degrees of similarity between concepts and ofgrouping concepts into hierarchical categories onthe basis of this similarity structure

A limitation that applies to these standardapproaches however is that the features theyemploy to define conceptual content are typicallyalso complex concepts Physical features like wingsfeathers and beaks are components in the sensethat they are parts of a larger entity but they are nomore primitive than the larger entities they defineLike the larger entities these parts are concepts thatmust be learned through perceptual and verbalexperience and each has its own set of defining fea-tures which in turn have their own defining featuresand so on The resulting combinatorial explosion pre-sents a bracing problem for constraining feature-based theories Across even the closed set of knownphysical objects the possible set of physical featuresis extremely large and the same is true for motionand action features such as flying swimmingrunning jumping eating singing barking howlingand so on The inability of standard verbal feature-based theories to locate a set of atomic featuresfrom which all others are derived places hard limitson the explanatory value of such approaches inseveral areas

One of these limitations and the one of centralconcern here is the lack of any known relationshipbetween verbal features and neurobiological mechan-isms There are no neural systems specifically dedi-cated to representing feathers wings beaks orflight nor is it plausible that such dedicated neuralsystems exist for every conceivable feature Even if itturns out that the brain representation of featuresand other complex concepts includes neurons orlocal neuron ensembles dedicated to a single

concept (Barlow 1972 Bowers 2009) the mere exist-ence of such ldquograndmother cellsrdquo in itself provides noaccount of the means by which external or internalstimuli lead to their activation Without such amechanistic account the claim that each possibleconcept is represented in the brain by a cell orgroup of cells adds nothing to our understanding ofconcept representations beyond a mere listing ofthe concepts What we seek instead is an understand-ing of why concepts are organized in the brain in theway that they actually are organized What facts aboutthe brain determine whether a concept is representedby one group of cells rather than another group Aclosely related problem for which feature-based con-ceptual representations are similarly limited in provid-ing an answer is the question of how features andother concepts come to be learned by the brain Theidea that concepts are represented in the brain byabstract ldquoconcept cellsrdquo also fails to address thedeeper question of how concepts can be understoodwithout a ldquogroundingrdquo in sensoryndashmotor experiencethat enables reference to the external environmentand the phenomenological qualia of consciousthought (Harnad 1990)

The limitations of traditional feature-based seman-tic theories for understanding concept representationin the brain are however a direct reflection of whatthese theories aim to achieve which is to describethe things that exist their similarity structure andtheir groupings into categories The question of howsuch information is organized in the brain is outsidethe domain of these aims so it should not be surpris-ing that these theories have the limitations they haveIn contrast embodiment theories of knowledge rep-resentation provide a fairly straightforward analysisof conceptual content in terms of sensory motoraffective and other experiential phenomena andtheir corresponding modality-specific neural represen-tations In the theory outlined here these neurobiolo-gically defined ldquoexperiential attributesrdquo provide a setof primitive features for the analysis of conceptualcontent while simultaneously grounding concepts inexperience and providing a mechanistic account ofconcept acquisition

Defining conceptual features in terms of neural pro-cesses represents a radical departure from traditionalverbal feature analysis Prior work along theselines includes the Conceptual Semantics program ofJackendoff (Jackendoff 1990) which conceives of

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 131

semantic content in terms of experiential primitivesemphasizing in particular the role of space timeevent and causality phenomena in understandingpropositional content Other related early workincludes the sensoryndashfunctional theory proposed byWarrington and colleagues to account for category-related object processing impairments in neurologicalpatients (Warrington amp Shallice 1984) In this theorythe relevant semantic content of physical entities isessentially reduced to two experiential attributes pro-cessed in two distinct brain networks Subsequentwork has generally recognized the limitations of asimple sensoryndashfunctional dichotomy (Allport 1985Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) leading to investi-gation of an ever-expanding list of sensory motorand affective attributes of concepts and the neuralcorrelates of these attributes

Studies of modality-specific contributions to con-ceptual knowledge have usually focused on a singleattribute or small set of attributes Lynott andConnell collected normative ratings for a sample of423 concrete adjectives that describe object proper-ties (Lynott amp Connell 2009) and 400 nouns (Lynottamp Connell 2013) on strength of association witheach of the five primary sensory modalities Partici-pants were asked to rate how strongly they experi-enced a particular concept by hearing tastingfeeling through touch smelling and seeing Gainottiet al (Gainotti Ciaraffa Silveri amp Marra 2009 GainottiSpinelli Scaricamazza amp Marra 2013) expanded onthis set by dividing the visual domain into threedimensions (shape colour and motion) and addingmanipulation experience as a motor dimensionRatings were obtained on 49 animal plant and arte-fact concepts revealing highly significant differencesbetween categories in the perceived relevance ofthese sensoryndashmotor dimensions to each conceptUsing a similar set of dimensions Hoffman andLambon Ralph (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013)obtained strength of association ratings for a set of160 object nouns Participants were asked ldquoHowmuch do you associate this item with a particular(colour visual form observed motion sound tactilesensation taste smell action)rdquo The resulting attri-bute ratings predicted lexicalndashsemantic processingspeed more accurately than verbal feature-based rep-resentations (McRae Cree Seidenberg amp McNorgan2005) and they supported a novel conceptual distinc-tion between mechanical devices (eg vehicles) and

other non-living objects in that the former hadstrong sound and motion characteristics that madethem more similar to animals

Although prior work in this area has focused over-whelmingly on concrete object and action conceptsmore recent studies explore dimensions of experiencethat may be more important in the representation ofabstract concepts Several authors have emphasizedthe role of affective and social experiences in abstractconcept acquisition and embodiment (Borghi FluminiCimatti Marocco amp Scorolli 2011 Kousta ViglioccoVinson Andrews amp Del Campo 2011 ViglioccoMeteyard Andrews amp Kousta 2009 Wiemer-Hastingsamp Xu 2005 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) Crutch et al(Crutch Williams Ridgway amp Borgenicht 2012)obtained ratings for 200 abstract and 200 concretenouns on the relatedness of each word to conceptsof time space quantity emotion polarity (positiveor negative) social interaction morality andthought in addition to the more physical dimensionsof sensation and action These representations suc-cessfully predicted performance by a severelyaphasic patient on an abstract noun comprehensiontask in which semantic similarity between target andfoil responses was manipulated whereas similaritymetrics (latent semantic analysis cosine similarity)based on patterns of word usage (Landauer ampDumais 1997) were not predictive (Crutch TrocheReilly amp Ridgway 2013) Troche et al (TrocheCrutch amp Reilly 2014) provide further analyses ofthese representations identifying three latent factors(labelled perceptual salience affective associationand magnitude) that accounted for 81 of the var-iance Abstract nouns were rated higher than concretenouns on the dimensions of emotion polarity socialinteraction morality and space

Building on this prior work our aim in the presentstudy was to develop a more comprehensive concep-tual representation based on known modalities ofneural information processing This more comprehen-sive representation would ideally capture aspects ofexperience that are central to the acquisition ofevent concepts as well as object concepts andabstract as well as concrete concepts The resultingrepresentation described in the following sectioncontains entries corresponding to specializedsensory and motor processes affective processessystems for processing spatial temporal and causalinformation social cognition processes and abstract

132 J R BINDER ET AL

cognitive operations We hope this approach stimu-lates further empirical and theoretical efforts towarda comprehensive brain-based semantic theory

Neural components of experience

The following section briefly describes the proposedcomponents of a brain-based semantic represen-tation which are listed in Tables 1 and 2 Each ofthese components is defined by an extensive bodyof physiological evidence that can only be hinted athere Examples are provided of how each componentapplies to a range of concepts Two fundamental prin-ciples guided selection of the components First weassume that all aspects of mental experience can con-tribute to concept acquisition and therefore conceptcomposition These ldquoaspects of mental experiencerdquoinclude not only sensory perceptions but also

affective responses to entities and situations experi-ence with performing motor actions perception ofspatial and temporal phenomena perception of caus-ality experience with social phenomena and experi-ence with internal cognitive phenomena and drivestates Second we focus on experiential phenomena

Table 1 Sensory and motor components organized by domainDomain Component Description (with reference to nouns)

Vision Vision something that you can easily seeVision Bright visually light or brightVision Dark visually darkVision Colour having a characteristic or defining colourVision Pattern having a characteristic or defining visual texture

or surface patternVision Large large in sizeVision Small small in sizeVision Motion showing a lot of visually observable movementVision Biomotion showing movement like that of a living thingVision Fast showing visible movement that is fastVision Slow showing visible movement that is slowVision Shape having a characteristic or defining visual shape or

formVision Complexity visually complexVision Face having a human or human-like faceVision Body having human or human-like body partsSomatic Touch something that you could easily recognize by

touchSomatic Temperature hot or cold to the touchSomatic Texture having a smooth or rough texture to the touchSomatic Weight light or heavy in weightSomatic Pain associated with pain or physical discomfortAudition Audition something that you can easily hearAudition Loud making a loud soundAudition Low having a low-pitched soundAudition High having a high-pitched soundAudition Sound having a characteristic or recognizable sound or

soundsAudition Music making a musical soundAudition Speech someone or something that talksGustation Taste having a characteristic or defining tasteOlfaction Smell having a characteristic or defining smell or smellsMotor Head associated with actions using the face mouth or

tongueMotor Upper limb associated with actions using the arm hand or

fingersMotor Lower limb associated with actions using the leg or footMotor Practice a physical object YOU have personal experience

using

Table 2 Spatial temporal causal social emotion drive andattention componentsDomain Name Description (with reference to nouns)

Spatial Landmark having a fixed location as on a mapSpatial Path showing changes in location along a

particular direction or pathSpatial Scene bringing to mind a particular setting or

physical locationSpatial Near often physically near to you (within easy

reach) in everyday lifeSpatial Toward associated with movement toward or into youSpatial Away associated with movement away from or out

of youSpatial Number associated with a specific number or amountTemporal Time an event or occurrence that occurs at a typical

or predictable timeTemporal Duration an event that has a predictable duration

whether short or longTemporal Long an event that lasts for a long period of timeTemporal Short an event that lasts for a short period of timeCausal Caused caused by some clear preceding event action

or situationCausal Consequential likely to have consequences (cause other

things to happen)Social Social an activity or event that involves an

interaction between peopleSocial Human having human or human-like intentions

plans or goalsSocial Communication a thing or action that people use to

communicateSocial Self related to your own view of yourself a part of

YOUR self-imageCognition Cognition a form of mental activity or a function of the

mindEmotion Benefit someone or something that could help or

benefit you or othersEmotion Harm someone or something that could cause harm

to you or othersEmotion Pleasant someone or something that you find pleasantEmotion Unpleasant someone or something that you find

unpleasantEmotion Happy someone or something that makes you feel

happyEmotion Sad someone or something that makes you feel

sadEmotion Angry someone or something that makes you feel

angryEmotion Disgusted someone or something that makes you feel

disgustedEmotion Fearful someone or something that makes you feel

afraidEmotion Surprised someone or something that makes you feel

surprisedDrive Drive someone or something that motivates you to

do somethingDrive Needs someone or something that would be hard

for you to live withoutAttention Attention someone or something that grabs your

attentionAttention Arousal someone or something that makes you feel

alert activated excited or keyed up ineither a positive or negative way

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 133

for which there are likely to be corresponding dis-tinguishable neural processors drawing on evidencefrom animal physiology brain imaging and neurologi-cal studies In particular we focus on macroscopicneural systems that can be distinguished with invivo human imaging methods We do not require orpropose that these neural processors are self-con-tained ldquomodulesrdquo or that they are highly localized inthe brain only that they process information that pri-marily represents a particular aspect of experience

A detailed listing of the queries used to elicit associ-ation ratings for each component is available for down-load (see link prior to the References section)

Visual components

Visual and other sensory components are listed inTable 1 Components of visual experience for whichthere are likely to be corresponding distinguishableneural processors include luminance size colourvisual texture visual shape visual motion and biologi-cal motion Within the visual shape perceptionnetwork are distinguishable networks that primarilyprocess faces human body parts and three-dimen-sional spaces

Luminance is a basic property of vision but is alsorelevant to such concepts as sun light gleam shineink night dark black and so on that are defined bybrightness or darkness Luminance is an example ofa scalar quantitymdashthat is one that represents a con-tinuous one-dimensional variable Linguistic represen-tation of scalars tends to focus on ends of thecontinuum (eg brightdark hotcold heavylightloudsoft etc) which raises a general question ofwhether or to what extent opposite ends of a scalar(eg bright and dark) are represented in distinguish-able neural processors Are there non-identicalneural networks that encode high and low luminanceThe answer depends to some degree on the spatialscale in question It is known that some neuronsrespond somewhat selectively to stimuli with highluminance while others respond more to stimuli withlow luminance (ie ON and OFF cells in retina andV1) resulting in two networks that are distinguishableat a microscopic scale If these and other luminance-tuned neurons intermingle within a single networkhowever the high- and low-luminance networksmight be indistinguishable at least at the macroscopicscale of in vivo imaging

In contrast the scalar quantity visual size is linked tolarge-scale retinotopic organization throughout thevisual cortical system and has been proposed as amajor determinant of medial-to-lateral organizationeven at the highest levels of the ventral visualstream (Hasson Levy Behrmann Hendler amp Malach2002) At these higher levels medial-to-lateral organiz-ation appears not to depend on the actual retinalextent of a given object exemplar which varies withdistance from the observer but rather on a ldquotypicalrdquoor generalized perceptual representation Images ofbuildings for example produce stronger activationin medial regions than do images of insects and theopposite pattern holds for lateral regions even whenthe images are the same physical size (Konkle ampOliva 2012) By extension the general theory that con-cepts are represented partly in perceptual systemswould predict similar medialndashlateral activation differ-ences in the ventral visual stream for concepts repre-senting objects that are reliably large or reliably small

There is now strong evidence not only for a fairlyspecialized colour perception network in the humanbrain (Bartels amp Zeki 2000 Beauchamp HaxbyJennings amp DeYoe 1999) but also for activation inor near this processor by concepts with colourcontent (Hsu Frankland amp Thompson-Schill 2012Hsu Kraemer Oliver Schlichting amp Thompson-Schill2011 Kellenbach Brett amp Patterson 2001 MartinHaxby Lalonde Wiggs amp Ungerleider 1995Simmons et al 2007) The question of whether thereare distinguishable neural processors activated by par-ticular colour concepts (eg red vs green vs blue) hasnot been studied but this appears unlikely Colour-sensitive neurons in the monkey inferior temporalcortex form a network of millimetre-sized ldquoglobsrdquowithin which there is evidence for spatial clusteringof neurons by colour preference (Conway amp Tsao2009) However the spatial scale of these clusters ison the order of 50ndash100 microm beyond the spatial resol-ution of current in vivo imaging methods More impor-tantly it is not clear that the spatial organization ofcolour concept representations should necessarilyreflect this perceptual organization Division of thevisible light frequency spectrum into colour conceptsvaries considerably across cultures (Berlin amp Kay1969) whereas the spatial organization of colour-sen-sitive neurons presumably does not As with the attri-bute of luminance we have assumed for these reasonsthat a single neural processor encodes the colour

134 J R BINDER ET AL

content of concepts regardless of the particular colourin question A concept like lemon that is reliably associ-ated with a particular colour is expected to activatethis neural processor to approximately the samedegree as a concept like coffee that is reliably associ-ated with a different colour

Visual motion perception involves a relativelyspecialized neural processor known as MT or V5 (Alb-right Desimone amp Gross 1984 Zeki 1974) Responsesin MT vary with motion velocity and degree of motioncoherence (Lingnau Ashida Wall amp Smith 2009 ReesFriston amp Koch 2000 Tootell et al 1995) Motion vel-ocity is relevant for our purposes as it is strongly rep-resented at a conceptual level For examplemovements may be relatively fast or slow in velocityand this scalar quantity is central to action conceptslike run dash zoom creep ooze walk and so on aswell as entity concepts like bullet jet hare snail tor-toise sloth and so on Movements may also have aspecific pattern or quality as illustrated for exampleby the large number of verbs that express manner ofmotion (turn bounce roll float spin twist etc) Weare not aware of any evidence for large-scale spatialorganization in the cortex according to type ormanner of motion therefore the proposed semanticrepresentation is designed to capture strength ofassociation with visually observable characteristicmotion in general regardless of specific type Theexception to this rule is the perception of biologicalmotion which has been linked with a neural processorthat is clearly distinct from MT located in the posteriorsuperior temporal sulcus (STS Grossman amp Blake2002 Pelphrey Morris Michelich Allison amp McCarthy2005) Biological motion contains higher order com-plexity that is characteristic of face and body partactions Perception of biological motion plays acentral role in understanding face and body gesturesand thus the affective and intentional state of otheragents (ie theory of mind Allison Puce amp McCarthy2000) Biological motion is thus one of several keyattributes distinguishing intentional agents (boy girlwoman man etc) from inanimate entities

Visual shape perception involves an extensiveregion of extrastriate cortex including a large lateralextrastriate area known as the lateral occipitalcomplex (Malach et al 1995) and an adjacent areaon the ventral occipitalndashtemporal surface known asVOT (Grill-Spector amp Malach 2004) In human func-tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies

these areas respond more strongly to images ofobjects than to images in which shape contours ofthe objects have been disrupted (ldquoscrambledrdquoimages) similar to response patterns observed inprimate ventral temporal neurons (Tanaka SaitoFukada amp Moriya 1991) Shape information is gener-ally the most important attribute of concrete objectconcepts and distinguishes concrete concepts froma range of abstract (eg affective social and cogni-tive) entities and event concepts Variation in the sal-ience of visual shape also occurs within the domainof concrete entities Shape is typically not a definingfeature of substance nouns (metal plastic water) butit has relevance for some substance-like mass nouns(butter coffee rice) Concrete objects also vary inshape complexity Animals for example tend tohave more complex shapes than plants or tools (Snod-grass amp Vanderwart 1980) We hypothesize that bothshape salience and shape complexity determine thedegree of activation in shape processing regionsduring concept retrieval

More focal areas within or adjacent to these shapeprocessing regions show preferential responses tohuman faces (Kanwisher McDermott amp Chun 1997Schwarzlose Baker amp Kanwisher 2005) and bodyparts (Downing Jiang Shuman amp Kanwisher 2001)A specialized mechanism for face perception is con-sistent with the central role of face recognition insocial interactions Visual perception of body partsprobably plays a crucial role in understandingobserved actions and mapping these onto internalrepresentations of the body schema during actionlearning These processors would be differentially acti-vated by concepts pertaining to faces (nose mouthportrait) and body parts (hand leg finger) As a firstapproximation we propose that both of these neuralprocessors are activated to some degree by all con-cepts referring to human beings which by necessityinclude a face and other human body parts Activationof the face processor may be minimal in the case ofconcepts that represent general human types androles (boy man nurse etc) as these concepts do notinclude information about any specific face whereasconcepts referencing specific individuals (brotherfather Einstein) might include specific facial featureinformation and therefore activate the face processorto a greater degree

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 135

parahippocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998Epstein Parker amp Feiler 2007) Although this processoris typically studied using visual stimuli and is oftenconsidered a high-level visual area it more likely inte-grates visual proprioceptive and perhaps auditoryinputs all of which provide correlated informationabout three-dimensional space Further discussion ofthis component of experience is provided in thesection below on Spatial Components (Table 2)

Somatosensory components

Somatosensory networks of the cortex represent bodylocation (somatotopic representation) body positionand joint force (proprioception) pain surface charac-teristics of objects (texture and temperature) andobject shape (Friebel Eickhoff amp Lotze 2011Hendry Hsiao amp Bushnell 1999 Lamm Decety ampSinger 2011 Longo Azanon amp Haggard 2010McGlone amp Reilly 2010 Medina amp Coslett 2010Serino amp Haggard 2010) Somatotopy is an inherentlymulti-modal phenomenon because the body locationtouched by a particular object is typically the samebody location as that used during exploratory ormanipulative motor actions involving the object Con-ceptual and linguistic representations are more likelyto encode the action as the salient characteristic ofthe object rather than the tactile experience (we sayldquoI threw the ballrdquo to describe the event not ldquothe balltouched my handrdquo) therefore somatotopic knowledgewas encoded in the proposed representation usingaction-based rather than somatosensory attributes(see Motor Components)

Temperature tactile texture and pain wereincluded as components of the representation aswas a component referring to the salience of objectweight Weight is perceived mainly via proprioceptiverepresentations and is a salient attribute of objectswith which we interact Temperature and weight aretwo more examples of scalar entities that may ormay not have a macroscopic topography in thebrain (Hendry et al 1999 Mazzola Faillenot BarralMauguiere amp Peyron 2012) That is although thereis evidence that these types of somatosensory infor-mation are distinguishable from each other no evi-dence has yet been presented for a scalartopography that separates for example hot fromcold representations Tactile texture can refer to avariety of physical properties we operationalized this

concept as a continuum from smooth to rough andthus the texture component is another scalar entityFor all three of these scalars we elected to representthe entire continuum as a single component Thatis the temperature component is intended tocapture the salience of temperature to a conceptregardless of whether the associated temperature ishot or cold

The published literature on conceptual processingof somatosensory information has focused almostexclusively on impaired knowledge of body parts inneurological patients (Coslett Saffran amp Schwoebel2002 Kemmerer amp Tranel 2008 Laiacona AllamanoLorenzi amp Capitani 2006 Schwoebel amp Coslett2005) though no definitive localization has emergedfrom these studies A number of functional imagingstudies suggest an overlap between networksinvolved in real pain perception and those involvedin empathic pain perception (perception of pain inothers) and perception of ldquosocial painrdquo (Eisenberger2012 Lamm et al 2011) suggesting that retrieval ofpain concepts involves a simulation process To ourknowledge no studies have yet examined the concep-tual representation of temperature tactile texture orweight

Finally object shape is an attribute learned partlythrough haptic experiences (Miqueacutee et al 2008) butthe degree to which haptic shape is learned indepen-dently from visual shape is unclear Some objects areseen but not felt (ie very large and very distantobjects) but the converse is rarely true at least insighted individuals It was therefore decided that thevisual shape query would capture knowledge aboutshape in both modalities and that a separate queryregarding extent of personal manipulation experience(see Motor Components) would capture the impor-tance of haptic shape relative to visual shape

Auditory components

The auditory cortex includes low-level areas tuned tofrequency (tonotopy) amplitude and spatial location(Schreiner amp Winer 2007) as well as higher levelsshowing specialization for auditory ldquoobjectsrdquo such asenvironmental sounds (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010)and speech sounds (Belin Zatorre Lafaille Ahad ampPike 2000 Liebenthal Binder Spitzer Possing ampMedler 2005) Corresponding auditory attributes ofthe proposed conceptual representation capture the

136 J R BINDER ET AL

degree to which a concept is associated with a low-pitched high-pitched loud (ie high amplitude)recognizable (ie having a characteristic auditoryform) or speech-like sound The attribute ldquolow inamplituderdquo was not included because it was con-sidered to be a salient attribute of relatively fewconceptsmdashthat is those that refer specifically to alow-amplitude variant (eg whisper) Concepts thatfocus on the absence of sound (eg silence quiet)are probably more accurately encoded as a nullvalue on the ldquoloudrdquo attribute as fMRI studies haveshown auditory regions where activity increases withsound intensity but not the converse (Roumlhl amp Uppen-kamp 2012)

A few studies of sound-related knowledge(Goldberg Perfetti amp Schneider 2006 Kellenbachet al 2001 Kiefer Sim Herrnberger Grothe ampHoenig 2008) reported activation in or near the pos-terior superior temporal sulcus (STS) an auditoryassociation region implicated in environmentalsound recognition (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010 J WLewis et al 2004) Trumpp et al (Trumpp KlieseHoenig Haarmeier amp Kiefer 2013) described apatient with a circumscribed lesion centred on theleft posterior STS who displayed a specific deficit(slower response times and higher error rate) invisual recognition of sound-related words All ofthese studies examined general environmentalsound knowledge nothing is yet known regardingthe conceptual representation of specific types ofauditory attributes like frequency or amplitude

Localization of sounds in space is an importantaspect of auditory perception yet auditory perceptionof space has little or no representation in languageindependent of (multimodal) spatial concepts thereare no concepts with components like ldquomakessounds from the leftrdquo because spatial relationshipsbetween sound sources and listeners are almostnever fixed or central to meaning Like shape attri-butes of concepts spatial attributes and spatial con-cepts are learned through strongly correlatedmultimodal experiences involving visual auditorytactile and motor processes In the proposed semanticrepresentation model spatial attributes of conceptsare represented by modality-independent spatialcomponents (see Spatial Components)

A final salient component of human auditoryexperience is the domain of music Music perceptionwhich includes processing of melody harmony and

rhythm elements in sound appears to engage rela-tively specialized right lateralized networks some ofwhich may also process nonverbal (prosodic)elements in speech (Zatorre Belin amp Penhune2002) Many words refer explicitly to musical phenom-ena (sing song melody harmony rhythm etc) or toentities (instruments performers performances) thatproduce musical sounds Therefore the model pro-posed here includes a component to capture theexperience of processing music

Gustatory and olfactory components

Gustatory and olfactory experiences are each rep-resented by a single attribute that captures the rel-evance of each type of experience to the conceptThese sensory systems do not show clear large-scaleorganization along any dimension and neuronswithin each system often respond to a broad spec-trum of items Both gustatory and olfactory conceptshave been linked with specific early sensory andhigher associative neural processors (Goldberg et al2006 Gonzaacutelez et al 2006)

Motor components

The motor components of the proposed conceptualrepresentation (Table 1) capture the degree to whicha concept is associated with actions involving particu-lar body parts The neural representation of motoraction verbs has been extensively studied withmany studies showing action-specific activation of ahigh-level network that includes the supramarginalgyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus (BinderDesai Conant amp Graves 2009 Desai Binder Conantamp Seidenberg 2009) There is also evidence for soma-totopic representation of action verb representation inprimary sensorimotor areas (Hauk Johnsrude ampPulvermuller 2004) though this claim is somewhatcontroversial (Postle McMahon Ashton Meredith ampde Zubicaray 2008) Object concepts associated withparticular actions also activate the action network(Binder et al 2009 Boronat et al 2005 Martin et al1995) and there is evidence for somatotopicallyspecific activation depending on which body part istypically used in the object interaction (CarotaMoseley amp Pulvermuumlller 2012) Following precedentin the neuroimaging literature (Carota et al 2012Hauk et al 2004 Tettamanti et al 2005) a three-

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 137

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 2: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

Toward a brain-based componential semantic representationJeffrey R Bindera Lisa L Conanta Colin J Humphriesa Leonardo Fernandinoa Stephen B SimonsbMario Aguilarb and Rutvik H Desaic

aDepartment of Neurology Medical College of Wisconsin Milwaukee WI USA bTeledyne Scientific LLC Durham NC USA cDepartment ofPsychology University of South Carolina Columbia SC USA

ABSTRACTComponential theories of lexical semantics assume that concepts can be represented by sets offeatures or attributes that are in some sense primitive or basic components of meaning Thebinary features used in classical category and prototype theories are problematic in that thesefeatures are themselves complex concepts leaving open the question of what constitutes aprimitive feature The present availability of brain imaging tools has enhanced interest in howconcepts are represented in brains and accumulating evidence supports the claim that theserepresentations are at least partly ldquoembodiedrdquo in the perception action and other modal neuralsystems through which concepts are experienced In this study we explore the possibility ofdevising a componential model of semantic representation based entirely on such functionaldivisions in the human brain We propose a basic set of approximately 65 experiential attributesbased on neurobiological considerations comprising sensory motor spatial temporal affectivesocial and cognitive experiences We provide normative data on the salience of each attributefor a large set of English nouns verbs and adjectives and show how these attribute vectorsdistinguish a priori conceptual categories and capture semantic similarity Robust quantitativedifferences between concrete object categories were observed across a large number ofattribute dimensions A within- versus between-category similarity metric showed much greaterseparation between categories than representations derived from distributional (latent semantic)analysis of text Cluster analyses were used to explore the similarity structure in the dataindependent of a priori labels revealing several novel category distinctions We discuss howsuch a representation might deal with various longstanding problems in semantic theory suchas feature selection and weighting representation of abstract concepts effects of context onsemantic retrieval and conceptual combination In contrast to componential models based onverbal features the proposed representation systematically relates semantic content to large-scale brain networks and biologically plausible accounts of concept acquisition

ARTICLE HISTORYReceived 14 June 2015Revised 24 November 2015Accepted 21 January 2016

KEYWORDSSemantics conceptrepresentation embodiedcognition cognitiveneuroscience

Introduction

Theories of semantic representation generally beginwith analyses of conceptual content This analyticalor componential approach assumes that conceptscan ultimately be represented by sets of features orattributes that are in some sense primitive or basiccomponents of meaning The present availability ofbrain imaging tools has enhanced interest in how con-cepts are represented in human neural systems andaccumulating evidence supports the claim that theserepresentations are at least partly ldquoembodiedrdquo in theperception action and other modal neural systemsthrough which concepts are acquired (Binder ampDesai 2011 Kiefer amp Pulvermuumlller 2012 MeteyardRodriguez Cuadrado Bahrami amp Vigliocco 2012) In

this paper we explore the possibility of devising a suf-ficiently explanatory componential model of semanticrepresentation based entirely on such functional div-isions in the human brain

An essential starting point is to define what types ofentities are the components of such a representationand how these differ from those of more standardsemantic theories In classical category theory con-cepts are defined by the presence or absence ofbinary features that are necessary and sufficient forcategory identification (Aristotle 1995350 BCE) Theconcept of bird for example is composed of such fea-tures as wings feathers beak flying egg-laying and soon An important extension of classical theory was therealization that most categories have somewhat fuzzy

copy 2016 Informa UK Limited trading as Taylor amp Francis Group

CONTACT Jeffrey R Binder jbindermcweduSupplemental data for this article can be accessed at httpdxdoiorg1010800264329420161147426

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 2016VOL 33 NOS 3ndash4 130ndash174httpdxdoiorg1010800264329420161147426

boundaries due to varying degrees of prototypicalityamong members (Rosch Mervis Gray Johnson ampBoyes-Braem 1976) Thus a penguin could still beidentified (though more slowly) as a bird despite thefact that it does not fly through a process of jointlyweighing all of its features Extensive work usingfeature generation tasks has documented whatpeople think of as the features of entities and howthese features rank in importance for a givenconcept (Cree amp McRae 2003 Garrard LambonRalph Hodges amp Patterson 2001 McRae de Sa ampSeidenberg 1997 Vinson Vigliocco Cappa amp Siri2003) Such data provide a powerful means of asses-sing degrees of similarity between concepts and ofgrouping concepts into hierarchical categories onthe basis of this similarity structure

A limitation that applies to these standardapproaches however is that the features theyemploy to define conceptual content are typicallyalso complex concepts Physical features like wingsfeathers and beaks are components in the sensethat they are parts of a larger entity but they are nomore primitive than the larger entities they defineLike the larger entities these parts are concepts thatmust be learned through perceptual and verbalexperience and each has its own set of defining fea-tures which in turn have their own defining featuresand so on The resulting combinatorial explosion pre-sents a bracing problem for constraining feature-based theories Across even the closed set of knownphysical objects the possible set of physical featuresis extremely large and the same is true for motionand action features such as flying swimmingrunning jumping eating singing barking howlingand so on The inability of standard verbal feature-based theories to locate a set of atomic featuresfrom which all others are derived places hard limitson the explanatory value of such approaches inseveral areas

One of these limitations and the one of centralconcern here is the lack of any known relationshipbetween verbal features and neurobiological mechan-isms There are no neural systems specifically dedi-cated to representing feathers wings beaks orflight nor is it plausible that such dedicated neuralsystems exist for every conceivable feature Even if itturns out that the brain representation of featuresand other complex concepts includes neurons orlocal neuron ensembles dedicated to a single

concept (Barlow 1972 Bowers 2009) the mere exist-ence of such ldquograndmother cellsrdquo in itself provides noaccount of the means by which external or internalstimuli lead to their activation Without such amechanistic account the claim that each possibleconcept is represented in the brain by a cell orgroup of cells adds nothing to our understanding ofconcept representations beyond a mere listing ofthe concepts What we seek instead is an understand-ing of why concepts are organized in the brain in theway that they actually are organized What facts aboutthe brain determine whether a concept is representedby one group of cells rather than another group Aclosely related problem for which feature-based con-ceptual representations are similarly limited in provid-ing an answer is the question of how features andother concepts come to be learned by the brain Theidea that concepts are represented in the brain byabstract ldquoconcept cellsrdquo also fails to address thedeeper question of how concepts can be understoodwithout a ldquogroundingrdquo in sensoryndashmotor experiencethat enables reference to the external environmentand the phenomenological qualia of consciousthought (Harnad 1990)

The limitations of traditional feature-based seman-tic theories for understanding concept representationin the brain are however a direct reflection of whatthese theories aim to achieve which is to describethe things that exist their similarity structure andtheir groupings into categories The question of howsuch information is organized in the brain is outsidethe domain of these aims so it should not be surpris-ing that these theories have the limitations they haveIn contrast embodiment theories of knowledge rep-resentation provide a fairly straightforward analysisof conceptual content in terms of sensory motoraffective and other experiential phenomena andtheir corresponding modality-specific neural represen-tations In the theory outlined here these neurobiolo-gically defined ldquoexperiential attributesrdquo provide a setof primitive features for the analysis of conceptualcontent while simultaneously grounding concepts inexperience and providing a mechanistic account ofconcept acquisition

Defining conceptual features in terms of neural pro-cesses represents a radical departure from traditionalverbal feature analysis Prior work along theselines includes the Conceptual Semantics program ofJackendoff (Jackendoff 1990) which conceives of

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 131

semantic content in terms of experiential primitivesemphasizing in particular the role of space timeevent and causality phenomena in understandingpropositional content Other related early workincludes the sensoryndashfunctional theory proposed byWarrington and colleagues to account for category-related object processing impairments in neurologicalpatients (Warrington amp Shallice 1984) In this theorythe relevant semantic content of physical entities isessentially reduced to two experiential attributes pro-cessed in two distinct brain networks Subsequentwork has generally recognized the limitations of asimple sensoryndashfunctional dichotomy (Allport 1985Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) leading to investi-gation of an ever-expanding list of sensory motorand affective attributes of concepts and the neuralcorrelates of these attributes

Studies of modality-specific contributions to con-ceptual knowledge have usually focused on a singleattribute or small set of attributes Lynott andConnell collected normative ratings for a sample of423 concrete adjectives that describe object proper-ties (Lynott amp Connell 2009) and 400 nouns (Lynottamp Connell 2013) on strength of association witheach of the five primary sensory modalities Partici-pants were asked to rate how strongly they experi-enced a particular concept by hearing tastingfeeling through touch smelling and seeing Gainottiet al (Gainotti Ciaraffa Silveri amp Marra 2009 GainottiSpinelli Scaricamazza amp Marra 2013) expanded onthis set by dividing the visual domain into threedimensions (shape colour and motion) and addingmanipulation experience as a motor dimensionRatings were obtained on 49 animal plant and arte-fact concepts revealing highly significant differencesbetween categories in the perceived relevance ofthese sensoryndashmotor dimensions to each conceptUsing a similar set of dimensions Hoffman andLambon Ralph (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013)obtained strength of association ratings for a set of160 object nouns Participants were asked ldquoHowmuch do you associate this item with a particular(colour visual form observed motion sound tactilesensation taste smell action)rdquo The resulting attri-bute ratings predicted lexicalndashsemantic processingspeed more accurately than verbal feature-based rep-resentations (McRae Cree Seidenberg amp McNorgan2005) and they supported a novel conceptual distinc-tion between mechanical devices (eg vehicles) and

other non-living objects in that the former hadstrong sound and motion characteristics that madethem more similar to animals

Although prior work in this area has focused over-whelmingly on concrete object and action conceptsmore recent studies explore dimensions of experiencethat may be more important in the representation ofabstract concepts Several authors have emphasizedthe role of affective and social experiences in abstractconcept acquisition and embodiment (Borghi FluminiCimatti Marocco amp Scorolli 2011 Kousta ViglioccoVinson Andrews amp Del Campo 2011 ViglioccoMeteyard Andrews amp Kousta 2009 Wiemer-Hastingsamp Xu 2005 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) Crutch et al(Crutch Williams Ridgway amp Borgenicht 2012)obtained ratings for 200 abstract and 200 concretenouns on the relatedness of each word to conceptsof time space quantity emotion polarity (positiveor negative) social interaction morality andthought in addition to the more physical dimensionsof sensation and action These representations suc-cessfully predicted performance by a severelyaphasic patient on an abstract noun comprehensiontask in which semantic similarity between target andfoil responses was manipulated whereas similaritymetrics (latent semantic analysis cosine similarity)based on patterns of word usage (Landauer ampDumais 1997) were not predictive (Crutch TrocheReilly amp Ridgway 2013) Troche et al (TrocheCrutch amp Reilly 2014) provide further analyses ofthese representations identifying three latent factors(labelled perceptual salience affective associationand magnitude) that accounted for 81 of the var-iance Abstract nouns were rated higher than concretenouns on the dimensions of emotion polarity socialinteraction morality and space

Building on this prior work our aim in the presentstudy was to develop a more comprehensive concep-tual representation based on known modalities ofneural information processing This more comprehen-sive representation would ideally capture aspects ofexperience that are central to the acquisition ofevent concepts as well as object concepts andabstract as well as concrete concepts The resultingrepresentation described in the following sectioncontains entries corresponding to specializedsensory and motor processes affective processessystems for processing spatial temporal and causalinformation social cognition processes and abstract

132 J R BINDER ET AL

cognitive operations We hope this approach stimu-lates further empirical and theoretical efforts towarda comprehensive brain-based semantic theory

Neural components of experience

The following section briefly describes the proposedcomponents of a brain-based semantic represen-tation which are listed in Tables 1 and 2 Each ofthese components is defined by an extensive bodyof physiological evidence that can only be hinted athere Examples are provided of how each componentapplies to a range of concepts Two fundamental prin-ciples guided selection of the components First weassume that all aspects of mental experience can con-tribute to concept acquisition and therefore conceptcomposition These ldquoaspects of mental experiencerdquoinclude not only sensory perceptions but also

affective responses to entities and situations experi-ence with performing motor actions perception ofspatial and temporal phenomena perception of caus-ality experience with social phenomena and experi-ence with internal cognitive phenomena and drivestates Second we focus on experiential phenomena

Table 1 Sensory and motor components organized by domainDomain Component Description (with reference to nouns)

Vision Vision something that you can easily seeVision Bright visually light or brightVision Dark visually darkVision Colour having a characteristic or defining colourVision Pattern having a characteristic or defining visual texture

or surface patternVision Large large in sizeVision Small small in sizeVision Motion showing a lot of visually observable movementVision Biomotion showing movement like that of a living thingVision Fast showing visible movement that is fastVision Slow showing visible movement that is slowVision Shape having a characteristic or defining visual shape or

formVision Complexity visually complexVision Face having a human or human-like faceVision Body having human or human-like body partsSomatic Touch something that you could easily recognize by

touchSomatic Temperature hot or cold to the touchSomatic Texture having a smooth or rough texture to the touchSomatic Weight light or heavy in weightSomatic Pain associated with pain or physical discomfortAudition Audition something that you can easily hearAudition Loud making a loud soundAudition Low having a low-pitched soundAudition High having a high-pitched soundAudition Sound having a characteristic or recognizable sound or

soundsAudition Music making a musical soundAudition Speech someone or something that talksGustation Taste having a characteristic or defining tasteOlfaction Smell having a characteristic or defining smell or smellsMotor Head associated with actions using the face mouth or

tongueMotor Upper limb associated with actions using the arm hand or

fingersMotor Lower limb associated with actions using the leg or footMotor Practice a physical object YOU have personal experience

using

Table 2 Spatial temporal causal social emotion drive andattention componentsDomain Name Description (with reference to nouns)

Spatial Landmark having a fixed location as on a mapSpatial Path showing changes in location along a

particular direction or pathSpatial Scene bringing to mind a particular setting or

physical locationSpatial Near often physically near to you (within easy

reach) in everyday lifeSpatial Toward associated with movement toward or into youSpatial Away associated with movement away from or out

of youSpatial Number associated with a specific number or amountTemporal Time an event or occurrence that occurs at a typical

or predictable timeTemporal Duration an event that has a predictable duration

whether short or longTemporal Long an event that lasts for a long period of timeTemporal Short an event that lasts for a short period of timeCausal Caused caused by some clear preceding event action

or situationCausal Consequential likely to have consequences (cause other

things to happen)Social Social an activity or event that involves an

interaction between peopleSocial Human having human or human-like intentions

plans or goalsSocial Communication a thing or action that people use to

communicateSocial Self related to your own view of yourself a part of

YOUR self-imageCognition Cognition a form of mental activity or a function of the

mindEmotion Benefit someone or something that could help or

benefit you or othersEmotion Harm someone or something that could cause harm

to you or othersEmotion Pleasant someone or something that you find pleasantEmotion Unpleasant someone or something that you find

unpleasantEmotion Happy someone or something that makes you feel

happyEmotion Sad someone or something that makes you feel

sadEmotion Angry someone or something that makes you feel

angryEmotion Disgusted someone or something that makes you feel

disgustedEmotion Fearful someone or something that makes you feel

afraidEmotion Surprised someone or something that makes you feel

surprisedDrive Drive someone or something that motivates you to

do somethingDrive Needs someone or something that would be hard

for you to live withoutAttention Attention someone or something that grabs your

attentionAttention Arousal someone or something that makes you feel

alert activated excited or keyed up ineither a positive or negative way

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 133

for which there are likely to be corresponding dis-tinguishable neural processors drawing on evidencefrom animal physiology brain imaging and neurologi-cal studies In particular we focus on macroscopicneural systems that can be distinguished with invivo human imaging methods We do not require orpropose that these neural processors are self-con-tained ldquomodulesrdquo or that they are highly localized inthe brain only that they process information that pri-marily represents a particular aspect of experience

A detailed listing of the queries used to elicit associ-ation ratings for each component is available for down-load (see link prior to the References section)

Visual components

Visual and other sensory components are listed inTable 1 Components of visual experience for whichthere are likely to be corresponding distinguishableneural processors include luminance size colourvisual texture visual shape visual motion and biologi-cal motion Within the visual shape perceptionnetwork are distinguishable networks that primarilyprocess faces human body parts and three-dimen-sional spaces

Luminance is a basic property of vision but is alsorelevant to such concepts as sun light gleam shineink night dark black and so on that are defined bybrightness or darkness Luminance is an example ofa scalar quantitymdashthat is one that represents a con-tinuous one-dimensional variable Linguistic represen-tation of scalars tends to focus on ends of thecontinuum (eg brightdark hotcold heavylightloudsoft etc) which raises a general question ofwhether or to what extent opposite ends of a scalar(eg bright and dark) are represented in distinguish-able neural processors Are there non-identicalneural networks that encode high and low luminanceThe answer depends to some degree on the spatialscale in question It is known that some neuronsrespond somewhat selectively to stimuli with highluminance while others respond more to stimuli withlow luminance (ie ON and OFF cells in retina andV1) resulting in two networks that are distinguishableat a microscopic scale If these and other luminance-tuned neurons intermingle within a single networkhowever the high- and low-luminance networksmight be indistinguishable at least at the macroscopicscale of in vivo imaging

In contrast the scalar quantity visual size is linked tolarge-scale retinotopic organization throughout thevisual cortical system and has been proposed as amajor determinant of medial-to-lateral organizationeven at the highest levels of the ventral visualstream (Hasson Levy Behrmann Hendler amp Malach2002) At these higher levels medial-to-lateral organiz-ation appears not to depend on the actual retinalextent of a given object exemplar which varies withdistance from the observer but rather on a ldquotypicalrdquoor generalized perceptual representation Images ofbuildings for example produce stronger activationin medial regions than do images of insects and theopposite pattern holds for lateral regions even whenthe images are the same physical size (Konkle ampOliva 2012) By extension the general theory that con-cepts are represented partly in perceptual systemswould predict similar medialndashlateral activation differ-ences in the ventral visual stream for concepts repre-senting objects that are reliably large or reliably small

There is now strong evidence not only for a fairlyspecialized colour perception network in the humanbrain (Bartels amp Zeki 2000 Beauchamp HaxbyJennings amp DeYoe 1999) but also for activation inor near this processor by concepts with colourcontent (Hsu Frankland amp Thompson-Schill 2012Hsu Kraemer Oliver Schlichting amp Thompson-Schill2011 Kellenbach Brett amp Patterson 2001 MartinHaxby Lalonde Wiggs amp Ungerleider 1995Simmons et al 2007) The question of whether thereare distinguishable neural processors activated by par-ticular colour concepts (eg red vs green vs blue) hasnot been studied but this appears unlikely Colour-sensitive neurons in the monkey inferior temporalcortex form a network of millimetre-sized ldquoglobsrdquowithin which there is evidence for spatial clusteringof neurons by colour preference (Conway amp Tsao2009) However the spatial scale of these clusters ison the order of 50ndash100 microm beyond the spatial resol-ution of current in vivo imaging methods More impor-tantly it is not clear that the spatial organization ofcolour concept representations should necessarilyreflect this perceptual organization Division of thevisible light frequency spectrum into colour conceptsvaries considerably across cultures (Berlin amp Kay1969) whereas the spatial organization of colour-sen-sitive neurons presumably does not As with the attri-bute of luminance we have assumed for these reasonsthat a single neural processor encodes the colour

134 J R BINDER ET AL

content of concepts regardless of the particular colourin question A concept like lemon that is reliably associ-ated with a particular colour is expected to activatethis neural processor to approximately the samedegree as a concept like coffee that is reliably associ-ated with a different colour

Visual motion perception involves a relativelyspecialized neural processor known as MT or V5 (Alb-right Desimone amp Gross 1984 Zeki 1974) Responsesin MT vary with motion velocity and degree of motioncoherence (Lingnau Ashida Wall amp Smith 2009 ReesFriston amp Koch 2000 Tootell et al 1995) Motion vel-ocity is relevant for our purposes as it is strongly rep-resented at a conceptual level For examplemovements may be relatively fast or slow in velocityand this scalar quantity is central to action conceptslike run dash zoom creep ooze walk and so on aswell as entity concepts like bullet jet hare snail tor-toise sloth and so on Movements may also have aspecific pattern or quality as illustrated for exampleby the large number of verbs that express manner ofmotion (turn bounce roll float spin twist etc) Weare not aware of any evidence for large-scale spatialorganization in the cortex according to type ormanner of motion therefore the proposed semanticrepresentation is designed to capture strength ofassociation with visually observable characteristicmotion in general regardless of specific type Theexception to this rule is the perception of biologicalmotion which has been linked with a neural processorthat is clearly distinct from MT located in the posteriorsuperior temporal sulcus (STS Grossman amp Blake2002 Pelphrey Morris Michelich Allison amp McCarthy2005) Biological motion contains higher order com-plexity that is characteristic of face and body partactions Perception of biological motion plays acentral role in understanding face and body gesturesand thus the affective and intentional state of otheragents (ie theory of mind Allison Puce amp McCarthy2000) Biological motion is thus one of several keyattributes distinguishing intentional agents (boy girlwoman man etc) from inanimate entities

Visual shape perception involves an extensiveregion of extrastriate cortex including a large lateralextrastriate area known as the lateral occipitalcomplex (Malach et al 1995) and an adjacent areaon the ventral occipitalndashtemporal surface known asVOT (Grill-Spector amp Malach 2004) In human func-tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies

these areas respond more strongly to images ofobjects than to images in which shape contours ofthe objects have been disrupted (ldquoscrambledrdquoimages) similar to response patterns observed inprimate ventral temporal neurons (Tanaka SaitoFukada amp Moriya 1991) Shape information is gener-ally the most important attribute of concrete objectconcepts and distinguishes concrete concepts froma range of abstract (eg affective social and cogni-tive) entities and event concepts Variation in the sal-ience of visual shape also occurs within the domainof concrete entities Shape is typically not a definingfeature of substance nouns (metal plastic water) butit has relevance for some substance-like mass nouns(butter coffee rice) Concrete objects also vary inshape complexity Animals for example tend tohave more complex shapes than plants or tools (Snod-grass amp Vanderwart 1980) We hypothesize that bothshape salience and shape complexity determine thedegree of activation in shape processing regionsduring concept retrieval

More focal areas within or adjacent to these shapeprocessing regions show preferential responses tohuman faces (Kanwisher McDermott amp Chun 1997Schwarzlose Baker amp Kanwisher 2005) and bodyparts (Downing Jiang Shuman amp Kanwisher 2001)A specialized mechanism for face perception is con-sistent with the central role of face recognition insocial interactions Visual perception of body partsprobably plays a crucial role in understandingobserved actions and mapping these onto internalrepresentations of the body schema during actionlearning These processors would be differentially acti-vated by concepts pertaining to faces (nose mouthportrait) and body parts (hand leg finger) As a firstapproximation we propose that both of these neuralprocessors are activated to some degree by all con-cepts referring to human beings which by necessityinclude a face and other human body parts Activationof the face processor may be minimal in the case ofconcepts that represent general human types androles (boy man nurse etc) as these concepts do notinclude information about any specific face whereasconcepts referencing specific individuals (brotherfather Einstein) might include specific facial featureinformation and therefore activate the face processorto a greater degree

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 135

parahippocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998Epstein Parker amp Feiler 2007) Although this processoris typically studied using visual stimuli and is oftenconsidered a high-level visual area it more likely inte-grates visual proprioceptive and perhaps auditoryinputs all of which provide correlated informationabout three-dimensional space Further discussion ofthis component of experience is provided in thesection below on Spatial Components (Table 2)

Somatosensory components

Somatosensory networks of the cortex represent bodylocation (somatotopic representation) body positionand joint force (proprioception) pain surface charac-teristics of objects (texture and temperature) andobject shape (Friebel Eickhoff amp Lotze 2011Hendry Hsiao amp Bushnell 1999 Lamm Decety ampSinger 2011 Longo Azanon amp Haggard 2010McGlone amp Reilly 2010 Medina amp Coslett 2010Serino amp Haggard 2010) Somatotopy is an inherentlymulti-modal phenomenon because the body locationtouched by a particular object is typically the samebody location as that used during exploratory ormanipulative motor actions involving the object Con-ceptual and linguistic representations are more likelyto encode the action as the salient characteristic ofthe object rather than the tactile experience (we sayldquoI threw the ballrdquo to describe the event not ldquothe balltouched my handrdquo) therefore somatotopic knowledgewas encoded in the proposed representation usingaction-based rather than somatosensory attributes(see Motor Components)

Temperature tactile texture and pain wereincluded as components of the representation aswas a component referring to the salience of objectweight Weight is perceived mainly via proprioceptiverepresentations and is a salient attribute of objectswith which we interact Temperature and weight aretwo more examples of scalar entities that may ormay not have a macroscopic topography in thebrain (Hendry et al 1999 Mazzola Faillenot BarralMauguiere amp Peyron 2012) That is although thereis evidence that these types of somatosensory infor-mation are distinguishable from each other no evi-dence has yet been presented for a scalartopography that separates for example hot fromcold representations Tactile texture can refer to avariety of physical properties we operationalized this

concept as a continuum from smooth to rough andthus the texture component is another scalar entityFor all three of these scalars we elected to representthe entire continuum as a single component Thatis the temperature component is intended tocapture the salience of temperature to a conceptregardless of whether the associated temperature ishot or cold

The published literature on conceptual processingof somatosensory information has focused almostexclusively on impaired knowledge of body parts inneurological patients (Coslett Saffran amp Schwoebel2002 Kemmerer amp Tranel 2008 Laiacona AllamanoLorenzi amp Capitani 2006 Schwoebel amp Coslett2005) though no definitive localization has emergedfrom these studies A number of functional imagingstudies suggest an overlap between networksinvolved in real pain perception and those involvedin empathic pain perception (perception of pain inothers) and perception of ldquosocial painrdquo (Eisenberger2012 Lamm et al 2011) suggesting that retrieval ofpain concepts involves a simulation process To ourknowledge no studies have yet examined the concep-tual representation of temperature tactile texture orweight

Finally object shape is an attribute learned partlythrough haptic experiences (Miqueacutee et al 2008) butthe degree to which haptic shape is learned indepen-dently from visual shape is unclear Some objects areseen but not felt (ie very large and very distantobjects) but the converse is rarely true at least insighted individuals It was therefore decided that thevisual shape query would capture knowledge aboutshape in both modalities and that a separate queryregarding extent of personal manipulation experience(see Motor Components) would capture the impor-tance of haptic shape relative to visual shape

Auditory components

The auditory cortex includes low-level areas tuned tofrequency (tonotopy) amplitude and spatial location(Schreiner amp Winer 2007) as well as higher levelsshowing specialization for auditory ldquoobjectsrdquo such asenvironmental sounds (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010)and speech sounds (Belin Zatorre Lafaille Ahad ampPike 2000 Liebenthal Binder Spitzer Possing ampMedler 2005) Corresponding auditory attributes ofthe proposed conceptual representation capture the

136 J R BINDER ET AL

degree to which a concept is associated with a low-pitched high-pitched loud (ie high amplitude)recognizable (ie having a characteristic auditoryform) or speech-like sound The attribute ldquolow inamplituderdquo was not included because it was con-sidered to be a salient attribute of relatively fewconceptsmdashthat is those that refer specifically to alow-amplitude variant (eg whisper) Concepts thatfocus on the absence of sound (eg silence quiet)are probably more accurately encoded as a nullvalue on the ldquoloudrdquo attribute as fMRI studies haveshown auditory regions where activity increases withsound intensity but not the converse (Roumlhl amp Uppen-kamp 2012)

A few studies of sound-related knowledge(Goldberg Perfetti amp Schneider 2006 Kellenbachet al 2001 Kiefer Sim Herrnberger Grothe ampHoenig 2008) reported activation in or near the pos-terior superior temporal sulcus (STS) an auditoryassociation region implicated in environmentalsound recognition (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010 J WLewis et al 2004) Trumpp et al (Trumpp KlieseHoenig Haarmeier amp Kiefer 2013) described apatient with a circumscribed lesion centred on theleft posterior STS who displayed a specific deficit(slower response times and higher error rate) invisual recognition of sound-related words All ofthese studies examined general environmentalsound knowledge nothing is yet known regardingthe conceptual representation of specific types ofauditory attributes like frequency or amplitude

Localization of sounds in space is an importantaspect of auditory perception yet auditory perceptionof space has little or no representation in languageindependent of (multimodal) spatial concepts thereare no concepts with components like ldquomakessounds from the leftrdquo because spatial relationshipsbetween sound sources and listeners are almostnever fixed or central to meaning Like shape attri-butes of concepts spatial attributes and spatial con-cepts are learned through strongly correlatedmultimodal experiences involving visual auditorytactile and motor processes In the proposed semanticrepresentation model spatial attributes of conceptsare represented by modality-independent spatialcomponents (see Spatial Components)

A final salient component of human auditoryexperience is the domain of music Music perceptionwhich includes processing of melody harmony and

rhythm elements in sound appears to engage rela-tively specialized right lateralized networks some ofwhich may also process nonverbal (prosodic)elements in speech (Zatorre Belin amp Penhune2002) Many words refer explicitly to musical phenom-ena (sing song melody harmony rhythm etc) or toentities (instruments performers performances) thatproduce musical sounds Therefore the model pro-posed here includes a component to capture theexperience of processing music

Gustatory and olfactory components

Gustatory and olfactory experiences are each rep-resented by a single attribute that captures the rel-evance of each type of experience to the conceptThese sensory systems do not show clear large-scaleorganization along any dimension and neuronswithin each system often respond to a broad spec-trum of items Both gustatory and olfactory conceptshave been linked with specific early sensory andhigher associative neural processors (Goldberg et al2006 Gonzaacutelez et al 2006)

Motor components

The motor components of the proposed conceptualrepresentation (Table 1) capture the degree to whicha concept is associated with actions involving particu-lar body parts The neural representation of motoraction verbs has been extensively studied withmany studies showing action-specific activation of ahigh-level network that includes the supramarginalgyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus (BinderDesai Conant amp Graves 2009 Desai Binder Conantamp Seidenberg 2009) There is also evidence for soma-totopic representation of action verb representation inprimary sensorimotor areas (Hauk Johnsrude ampPulvermuller 2004) though this claim is somewhatcontroversial (Postle McMahon Ashton Meredith ampde Zubicaray 2008) Object concepts associated withparticular actions also activate the action network(Binder et al 2009 Boronat et al 2005 Martin et al1995) and there is evidence for somatotopicallyspecific activation depending on which body part istypically used in the object interaction (CarotaMoseley amp Pulvermuumlller 2012) Following precedentin the neuroimaging literature (Carota et al 2012Hauk et al 2004 Tettamanti et al 2005) a three-

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 137

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 3: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

boundaries due to varying degrees of prototypicalityamong members (Rosch Mervis Gray Johnson ampBoyes-Braem 1976) Thus a penguin could still beidentified (though more slowly) as a bird despite thefact that it does not fly through a process of jointlyweighing all of its features Extensive work usingfeature generation tasks has documented whatpeople think of as the features of entities and howthese features rank in importance for a givenconcept (Cree amp McRae 2003 Garrard LambonRalph Hodges amp Patterson 2001 McRae de Sa ampSeidenberg 1997 Vinson Vigliocco Cappa amp Siri2003) Such data provide a powerful means of asses-sing degrees of similarity between concepts and ofgrouping concepts into hierarchical categories onthe basis of this similarity structure

A limitation that applies to these standardapproaches however is that the features theyemploy to define conceptual content are typicallyalso complex concepts Physical features like wingsfeathers and beaks are components in the sensethat they are parts of a larger entity but they are nomore primitive than the larger entities they defineLike the larger entities these parts are concepts thatmust be learned through perceptual and verbalexperience and each has its own set of defining fea-tures which in turn have their own defining featuresand so on The resulting combinatorial explosion pre-sents a bracing problem for constraining feature-based theories Across even the closed set of knownphysical objects the possible set of physical featuresis extremely large and the same is true for motionand action features such as flying swimmingrunning jumping eating singing barking howlingand so on The inability of standard verbal feature-based theories to locate a set of atomic featuresfrom which all others are derived places hard limitson the explanatory value of such approaches inseveral areas

One of these limitations and the one of centralconcern here is the lack of any known relationshipbetween verbal features and neurobiological mechan-isms There are no neural systems specifically dedi-cated to representing feathers wings beaks orflight nor is it plausible that such dedicated neuralsystems exist for every conceivable feature Even if itturns out that the brain representation of featuresand other complex concepts includes neurons orlocal neuron ensembles dedicated to a single

concept (Barlow 1972 Bowers 2009) the mere exist-ence of such ldquograndmother cellsrdquo in itself provides noaccount of the means by which external or internalstimuli lead to their activation Without such amechanistic account the claim that each possibleconcept is represented in the brain by a cell orgroup of cells adds nothing to our understanding ofconcept representations beyond a mere listing ofthe concepts What we seek instead is an understand-ing of why concepts are organized in the brain in theway that they actually are organized What facts aboutthe brain determine whether a concept is representedby one group of cells rather than another group Aclosely related problem for which feature-based con-ceptual representations are similarly limited in provid-ing an answer is the question of how features andother concepts come to be learned by the brain Theidea that concepts are represented in the brain byabstract ldquoconcept cellsrdquo also fails to address thedeeper question of how concepts can be understoodwithout a ldquogroundingrdquo in sensoryndashmotor experiencethat enables reference to the external environmentand the phenomenological qualia of consciousthought (Harnad 1990)

The limitations of traditional feature-based seman-tic theories for understanding concept representationin the brain are however a direct reflection of whatthese theories aim to achieve which is to describethe things that exist their similarity structure andtheir groupings into categories The question of howsuch information is organized in the brain is outsidethe domain of these aims so it should not be surpris-ing that these theories have the limitations they haveIn contrast embodiment theories of knowledge rep-resentation provide a fairly straightforward analysisof conceptual content in terms of sensory motoraffective and other experiential phenomena andtheir corresponding modality-specific neural represen-tations In the theory outlined here these neurobiolo-gically defined ldquoexperiential attributesrdquo provide a setof primitive features for the analysis of conceptualcontent while simultaneously grounding concepts inexperience and providing a mechanistic account ofconcept acquisition

Defining conceptual features in terms of neural pro-cesses represents a radical departure from traditionalverbal feature analysis Prior work along theselines includes the Conceptual Semantics program ofJackendoff (Jackendoff 1990) which conceives of

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 131

semantic content in terms of experiential primitivesemphasizing in particular the role of space timeevent and causality phenomena in understandingpropositional content Other related early workincludes the sensoryndashfunctional theory proposed byWarrington and colleagues to account for category-related object processing impairments in neurologicalpatients (Warrington amp Shallice 1984) In this theorythe relevant semantic content of physical entities isessentially reduced to two experiential attributes pro-cessed in two distinct brain networks Subsequentwork has generally recognized the limitations of asimple sensoryndashfunctional dichotomy (Allport 1985Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) leading to investi-gation of an ever-expanding list of sensory motorand affective attributes of concepts and the neuralcorrelates of these attributes

Studies of modality-specific contributions to con-ceptual knowledge have usually focused on a singleattribute or small set of attributes Lynott andConnell collected normative ratings for a sample of423 concrete adjectives that describe object proper-ties (Lynott amp Connell 2009) and 400 nouns (Lynottamp Connell 2013) on strength of association witheach of the five primary sensory modalities Partici-pants were asked to rate how strongly they experi-enced a particular concept by hearing tastingfeeling through touch smelling and seeing Gainottiet al (Gainotti Ciaraffa Silveri amp Marra 2009 GainottiSpinelli Scaricamazza amp Marra 2013) expanded onthis set by dividing the visual domain into threedimensions (shape colour and motion) and addingmanipulation experience as a motor dimensionRatings were obtained on 49 animal plant and arte-fact concepts revealing highly significant differencesbetween categories in the perceived relevance ofthese sensoryndashmotor dimensions to each conceptUsing a similar set of dimensions Hoffman andLambon Ralph (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013)obtained strength of association ratings for a set of160 object nouns Participants were asked ldquoHowmuch do you associate this item with a particular(colour visual form observed motion sound tactilesensation taste smell action)rdquo The resulting attri-bute ratings predicted lexicalndashsemantic processingspeed more accurately than verbal feature-based rep-resentations (McRae Cree Seidenberg amp McNorgan2005) and they supported a novel conceptual distinc-tion between mechanical devices (eg vehicles) and

other non-living objects in that the former hadstrong sound and motion characteristics that madethem more similar to animals

Although prior work in this area has focused over-whelmingly on concrete object and action conceptsmore recent studies explore dimensions of experiencethat may be more important in the representation ofabstract concepts Several authors have emphasizedthe role of affective and social experiences in abstractconcept acquisition and embodiment (Borghi FluminiCimatti Marocco amp Scorolli 2011 Kousta ViglioccoVinson Andrews amp Del Campo 2011 ViglioccoMeteyard Andrews amp Kousta 2009 Wiemer-Hastingsamp Xu 2005 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) Crutch et al(Crutch Williams Ridgway amp Borgenicht 2012)obtained ratings for 200 abstract and 200 concretenouns on the relatedness of each word to conceptsof time space quantity emotion polarity (positiveor negative) social interaction morality andthought in addition to the more physical dimensionsof sensation and action These representations suc-cessfully predicted performance by a severelyaphasic patient on an abstract noun comprehensiontask in which semantic similarity between target andfoil responses was manipulated whereas similaritymetrics (latent semantic analysis cosine similarity)based on patterns of word usage (Landauer ampDumais 1997) were not predictive (Crutch TrocheReilly amp Ridgway 2013) Troche et al (TrocheCrutch amp Reilly 2014) provide further analyses ofthese representations identifying three latent factors(labelled perceptual salience affective associationand magnitude) that accounted for 81 of the var-iance Abstract nouns were rated higher than concretenouns on the dimensions of emotion polarity socialinteraction morality and space

Building on this prior work our aim in the presentstudy was to develop a more comprehensive concep-tual representation based on known modalities ofneural information processing This more comprehen-sive representation would ideally capture aspects ofexperience that are central to the acquisition ofevent concepts as well as object concepts andabstract as well as concrete concepts The resultingrepresentation described in the following sectioncontains entries corresponding to specializedsensory and motor processes affective processessystems for processing spatial temporal and causalinformation social cognition processes and abstract

132 J R BINDER ET AL

cognitive operations We hope this approach stimu-lates further empirical and theoretical efforts towarda comprehensive brain-based semantic theory

Neural components of experience

The following section briefly describes the proposedcomponents of a brain-based semantic represen-tation which are listed in Tables 1 and 2 Each ofthese components is defined by an extensive bodyof physiological evidence that can only be hinted athere Examples are provided of how each componentapplies to a range of concepts Two fundamental prin-ciples guided selection of the components First weassume that all aspects of mental experience can con-tribute to concept acquisition and therefore conceptcomposition These ldquoaspects of mental experiencerdquoinclude not only sensory perceptions but also

affective responses to entities and situations experi-ence with performing motor actions perception ofspatial and temporal phenomena perception of caus-ality experience with social phenomena and experi-ence with internal cognitive phenomena and drivestates Second we focus on experiential phenomena

Table 1 Sensory and motor components organized by domainDomain Component Description (with reference to nouns)

Vision Vision something that you can easily seeVision Bright visually light or brightVision Dark visually darkVision Colour having a characteristic or defining colourVision Pattern having a characteristic or defining visual texture

or surface patternVision Large large in sizeVision Small small in sizeVision Motion showing a lot of visually observable movementVision Biomotion showing movement like that of a living thingVision Fast showing visible movement that is fastVision Slow showing visible movement that is slowVision Shape having a characteristic or defining visual shape or

formVision Complexity visually complexVision Face having a human or human-like faceVision Body having human or human-like body partsSomatic Touch something that you could easily recognize by

touchSomatic Temperature hot or cold to the touchSomatic Texture having a smooth or rough texture to the touchSomatic Weight light or heavy in weightSomatic Pain associated with pain or physical discomfortAudition Audition something that you can easily hearAudition Loud making a loud soundAudition Low having a low-pitched soundAudition High having a high-pitched soundAudition Sound having a characteristic or recognizable sound or

soundsAudition Music making a musical soundAudition Speech someone or something that talksGustation Taste having a characteristic or defining tasteOlfaction Smell having a characteristic or defining smell or smellsMotor Head associated with actions using the face mouth or

tongueMotor Upper limb associated with actions using the arm hand or

fingersMotor Lower limb associated with actions using the leg or footMotor Practice a physical object YOU have personal experience

using

Table 2 Spatial temporal causal social emotion drive andattention componentsDomain Name Description (with reference to nouns)

Spatial Landmark having a fixed location as on a mapSpatial Path showing changes in location along a

particular direction or pathSpatial Scene bringing to mind a particular setting or

physical locationSpatial Near often physically near to you (within easy

reach) in everyday lifeSpatial Toward associated with movement toward or into youSpatial Away associated with movement away from or out

of youSpatial Number associated with a specific number or amountTemporal Time an event or occurrence that occurs at a typical

or predictable timeTemporal Duration an event that has a predictable duration

whether short or longTemporal Long an event that lasts for a long period of timeTemporal Short an event that lasts for a short period of timeCausal Caused caused by some clear preceding event action

or situationCausal Consequential likely to have consequences (cause other

things to happen)Social Social an activity or event that involves an

interaction between peopleSocial Human having human or human-like intentions

plans or goalsSocial Communication a thing or action that people use to

communicateSocial Self related to your own view of yourself a part of

YOUR self-imageCognition Cognition a form of mental activity or a function of the

mindEmotion Benefit someone or something that could help or

benefit you or othersEmotion Harm someone or something that could cause harm

to you or othersEmotion Pleasant someone or something that you find pleasantEmotion Unpleasant someone or something that you find

unpleasantEmotion Happy someone or something that makes you feel

happyEmotion Sad someone or something that makes you feel

sadEmotion Angry someone or something that makes you feel

angryEmotion Disgusted someone or something that makes you feel

disgustedEmotion Fearful someone or something that makes you feel

afraidEmotion Surprised someone or something that makes you feel

surprisedDrive Drive someone or something that motivates you to

do somethingDrive Needs someone or something that would be hard

for you to live withoutAttention Attention someone or something that grabs your

attentionAttention Arousal someone or something that makes you feel

alert activated excited or keyed up ineither a positive or negative way

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 133

for which there are likely to be corresponding dis-tinguishable neural processors drawing on evidencefrom animal physiology brain imaging and neurologi-cal studies In particular we focus on macroscopicneural systems that can be distinguished with invivo human imaging methods We do not require orpropose that these neural processors are self-con-tained ldquomodulesrdquo or that they are highly localized inthe brain only that they process information that pri-marily represents a particular aspect of experience

A detailed listing of the queries used to elicit associ-ation ratings for each component is available for down-load (see link prior to the References section)

Visual components

Visual and other sensory components are listed inTable 1 Components of visual experience for whichthere are likely to be corresponding distinguishableneural processors include luminance size colourvisual texture visual shape visual motion and biologi-cal motion Within the visual shape perceptionnetwork are distinguishable networks that primarilyprocess faces human body parts and three-dimen-sional spaces

Luminance is a basic property of vision but is alsorelevant to such concepts as sun light gleam shineink night dark black and so on that are defined bybrightness or darkness Luminance is an example ofa scalar quantitymdashthat is one that represents a con-tinuous one-dimensional variable Linguistic represen-tation of scalars tends to focus on ends of thecontinuum (eg brightdark hotcold heavylightloudsoft etc) which raises a general question ofwhether or to what extent opposite ends of a scalar(eg bright and dark) are represented in distinguish-able neural processors Are there non-identicalneural networks that encode high and low luminanceThe answer depends to some degree on the spatialscale in question It is known that some neuronsrespond somewhat selectively to stimuli with highluminance while others respond more to stimuli withlow luminance (ie ON and OFF cells in retina andV1) resulting in two networks that are distinguishableat a microscopic scale If these and other luminance-tuned neurons intermingle within a single networkhowever the high- and low-luminance networksmight be indistinguishable at least at the macroscopicscale of in vivo imaging

In contrast the scalar quantity visual size is linked tolarge-scale retinotopic organization throughout thevisual cortical system and has been proposed as amajor determinant of medial-to-lateral organizationeven at the highest levels of the ventral visualstream (Hasson Levy Behrmann Hendler amp Malach2002) At these higher levels medial-to-lateral organiz-ation appears not to depend on the actual retinalextent of a given object exemplar which varies withdistance from the observer but rather on a ldquotypicalrdquoor generalized perceptual representation Images ofbuildings for example produce stronger activationin medial regions than do images of insects and theopposite pattern holds for lateral regions even whenthe images are the same physical size (Konkle ampOliva 2012) By extension the general theory that con-cepts are represented partly in perceptual systemswould predict similar medialndashlateral activation differ-ences in the ventral visual stream for concepts repre-senting objects that are reliably large or reliably small

There is now strong evidence not only for a fairlyspecialized colour perception network in the humanbrain (Bartels amp Zeki 2000 Beauchamp HaxbyJennings amp DeYoe 1999) but also for activation inor near this processor by concepts with colourcontent (Hsu Frankland amp Thompson-Schill 2012Hsu Kraemer Oliver Schlichting amp Thompson-Schill2011 Kellenbach Brett amp Patterson 2001 MartinHaxby Lalonde Wiggs amp Ungerleider 1995Simmons et al 2007) The question of whether thereare distinguishable neural processors activated by par-ticular colour concepts (eg red vs green vs blue) hasnot been studied but this appears unlikely Colour-sensitive neurons in the monkey inferior temporalcortex form a network of millimetre-sized ldquoglobsrdquowithin which there is evidence for spatial clusteringof neurons by colour preference (Conway amp Tsao2009) However the spatial scale of these clusters ison the order of 50ndash100 microm beyond the spatial resol-ution of current in vivo imaging methods More impor-tantly it is not clear that the spatial organization ofcolour concept representations should necessarilyreflect this perceptual organization Division of thevisible light frequency spectrum into colour conceptsvaries considerably across cultures (Berlin amp Kay1969) whereas the spatial organization of colour-sen-sitive neurons presumably does not As with the attri-bute of luminance we have assumed for these reasonsthat a single neural processor encodes the colour

134 J R BINDER ET AL

content of concepts regardless of the particular colourin question A concept like lemon that is reliably associ-ated with a particular colour is expected to activatethis neural processor to approximately the samedegree as a concept like coffee that is reliably associ-ated with a different colour

Visual motion perception involves a relativelyspecialized neural processor known as MT or V5 (Alb-right Desimone amp Gross 1984 Zeki 1974) Responsesin MT vary with motion velocity and degree of motioncoherence (Lingnau Ashida Wall amp Smith 2009 ReesFriston amp Koch 2000 Tootell et al 1995) Motion vel-ocity is relevant for our purposes as it is strongly rep-resented at a conceptual level For examplemovements may be relatively fast or slow in velocityand this scalar quantity is central to action conceptslike run dash zoom creep ooze walk and so on aswell as entity concepts like bullet jet hare snail tor-toise sloth and so on Movements may also have aspecific pattern or quality as illustrated for exampleby the large number of verbs that express manner ofmotion (turn bounce roll float spin twist etc) Weare not aware of any evidence for large-scale spatialorganization in the cortex according to type ormanner of motion therefore the proposed semanticrepresentation is designed to capture strength ofassociation with visually observable characteristicmotion in general regardless of specific type Theexception to this rule is the perception of biologicalmotion which has been linked with a neural processorthat is clearly distinct from MT located in the posteriorsuperior temporal sulcus (STS Grossman amp Blake2002 Pelphrey Morris Michelich Allison amp McCarthy2005) Biological motion contains higher order com-plexity that is characteristic of face and body partactions Perception of biological motion plays acentral role in understanding face and body gesturesand thus the affective and intentional state of otheragents (ie theory of mind Allison Puce amp McCarthy2000) Biological motion is thus one of several keyattributes distinguishing intentional agents (boy girlwoman man etc) from inanimate entities

Visual shape perception involves an extensiveregion of extrastriate cortex including a large lateralextrastriate area known as the lateral occipitalcomplex (Malach et al 1995) and an adjacent areaon the ventral occipitalndashtemporal surface known asVOT (Grill-Spector amp Malach 2004) In human func-tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies

these areas respond more strongly to images ofobjects than to images in which shape contours ofthe objects have been disrupted (ldquoscrambledrdquoimages) similar to response patterns observed inprimate ventral temporal neurons (Tanaka SaitoFukada amp Moriya 1991) Shape information is gener-ally the most important attribute of concrete objectconcepts and distinguishes concrete concepts froma range of abstract (eg affective social and cogni-tive) entities and event concepts Variation in the sal-ience of visual shape also occurs within the domainof concrete entities Shape is typically not a definingfeature of substance nouns (metal plastic water) butit has relevance for some substance-like mass nouns(butter coffee rice) Concrete objects also vary inshape complexity Animals for example tend tohave more complex shapes than plants or tools (Snod-grass amp Vanderwart 1980) We hypothesize that bothshape salience and shape complexity determine thedegree of activation in shape processing regionsduring concept retrieval

More focal areas within or adjacent to these shapeprocessing regions show preferential responses tohuman faces (Kanwisher McDermott amp Chun 1997Schwarzlose Baker amp Kanwisher 2005) and bodyparts (Downing Jiang Shuman amp Kanwisher 2001)A specialized mechanism for face perception is con-sistent with the central role of face recognition insocial interactions Visual perception of body partsprobably plays a crucial role in understandingobserved actions and mapping these onto internalrepresentations of the body schema during actionlearning These processors would be differentially acti-vated by concepts pertaining to faces (nose mouthportrait) and body parts (hand leg finger) As a firstapproximation we propose that both of these neuralprocessors are activated to some degree by all con-cepts referring to human beings which by necessityinclude a face and other human body parts Activationof the face processor may be minimal in the case ofconcepts that represent general human types androles (boy man nurse etc) as these concepts do notinclude information about any specific face whereasconcepts referencing specific individuals (brotherfather Einstein) might include specific facial featureinformation and therefore activate the face processorto a greater degree

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 135

parahippocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998Epstein Parker amp Feiler 2007) Although this processoris typically studied using visual stimuli and is oftenconsidered a high-level visual area it more likely inte-grates visual proprioceptive and perhaps auditoryinputs all of which provide correlated informationabout three-dimensional space Further discussion ofthis component of experience is provided in thesection below on Spatial Components (Table 2)

Somatosensory components

Somatosensory networks of the cortex represent bodylocation (somatotopic representation) body positionand joint force (proprioception) pain surface charac-teristics of objects (texture and temperature) andobject shape (Friebel Eickhoff amp Lotze 2011Hendry Hsiao amp Bushnell 1999 Lamm Decety ampSinger 2011 Longo Azanon amp Haggard 2010McGlone amp Reilly 2010 Medina amp Coslett 2010Serino amp Haggard 2010) Somatotopy is an inherentlymulti-modal phenomenon because the body locationtouched by a particular object is typically the samebody location as that used during exploratory ormanipulative motor actions involving the object Con-ceptual and linguistic representations are more likelyto encode the action as the salient characteristic ofthe object rather than the tactile experience (we sayldquoI threw the ballrdquo to describe the event not ldquothe balltouched my handrdquo) therefore somatotopic knowledgewas encoded in the proposed representation usingaction-based rather than somatosensory attributes(see Motor Components)

Temperature tactile texture and pain wereincluded as components of the representation aswas a component referring to the salience of objectweight Weight is perceived mainly via proprioceptiverepresentations and is a salient attribute of objectswith which we interact Temperature and weight aretwo more examples of scalar entities that may ormay not have a macroscopic topography in thebrain (Hendry et al 1999 Mazzola Faillenot BarralMauguiere amp Peyron 2012) That is although thereis evidence that these types of somatosensory infor-mation are distinguishable from each other no evi-dence has yet been presented for a scalartopography that separates for example hot fromcold representations Tactile texture can refer to avariety of physical properties we operationalized this

concept as a continuum from smooth to rough andthus the texture component is another scalar entityFor all three of these scalars we elected to representthe entire continuum as a single component Thatis the temperature component is intended tocapture the salience of temperature to a conceptregardless of whether the associated temperature ishot or cold

The published literature on conceptual processingof somatosensory information has focused almostexclusively on impaired knowledge of body parts inneurological patients (Coslett Saffran amp Schwoebel2002 Kemmerer amp Tranel 2008 Laiacona AllamanoLorenzi amp Capitani 2006 Schwoebel amp Coslett2005) though no definitive localization has emergedfrom these studies A number of functional imagingstudies suggest an overlap between networksinvolved in real pain perception and those involvedin empathic pain perception (perception of pain inothers) and perception of ldquosocial painrdquo (Eisenberger2012 Lamm et al 2011) suggesting that retrieval ofpain concepts involves a simulation process To ourknowledge no studies have yet examined the concep-tual representation of temperature tactile texture orweight

Finally object shape is an attribute learned partlythrough haptic experiences (Miqueacutee et al 2008) butthe degree to which haptic shape is learned indepen-dently from visual shape is unclear Some objects areseen but not felt (ie very large and very distantobjects) but the converse is rarely true at least insighted individuals It was therefore decided that thevisual shape query would capture knowledge aboutshape in both modalities and that a separate queryregarding extent of personal manipulation experience(see Motor Components) would capture the impor-tance of haptic shape relative to visual shape

Auditory components

The auditory cortex includes low-level areas tuned tofrequency (tonotopy) amplitude and spatial location(Schreiner amp Winer 2007) as well as higher levelsshowing specialization for auditory ldquoobjectsrdquo such asenvironmental sounds (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010)and speech sounds (Belin Zatorre Lafaille Ahad ampPike 2000 Liebenthal Binder Spitzer Possing ampMedler 2005) Corresponding auditory attributes ofthe proposed conceptual representation capture the

136 J R BINDER ET AL

degree to which a concept is associated with a low-pitched high-pitched loud (ie high amplitude)recognizable (ie having a characteristic auditoryform) or speech-like sound The attribute ldquolow inamplituderdquo was not included because it was con-sidered to be a salient attribute of relatively fewconceptsmdashthat is those that refer specifically to alow-amplitude variant (eg whisper) Concepts thatfocus on the absence of sound (eg silence quiet)are probably more accurately encoded as a nullvalue on the ldquoloudrdquo attribute as fMRI studies haveshown auditory regions where activity increases withsound intensity but not the converse (Roumlhl amp Uppen-kamp 2012)

A few studies of sound-related knowledge(Goldberg Perfetti amp Schneider 2006 Kellenbachet al 2001 Kiefer Sim Herrnberger Grothe ampHoenig 2008) reported activation in or near the pos-terior superior temporal sulcus (STS) an auditoryassociation region implicated in environmentalsound recognition (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010 J WLewis et al 2004) Trumpp et al (Trumpp KlieseHoenig Haarmeier amp Kiefer 2013) described apatient with a circumscribed lesion centred on theleft posterior STS who displayed a specific deficit(slower response times and higher error rate) invisual recognition of sound-related words All ofthese studies examined general environmentalsound knowledge nothing is yet known regardingthe conceptual representation of specific types ofauditory attributes like frequency or amplitude

Localization of sounds in space is an importantaspect of auditory perception yet auditory perceptionof space has little or no representation in languageindependent of (multimodal) spatial concepts thereare no concepts with components like ldquomakessounds from the leftrdquo because spatial relationshipsbetween sound sources and listeners are almostnever fixed or central to meaning Like shape attri-butes of concepts spatial attributes and spatial con-cepts are learned through strongly correlatedmultimodal experiences involving visual auditorytactile and motor processes In the proposed semanticrepresentation model spatial attributes of conceptsare represented by modality-independent spatialcomponents (see Spatial Components)

A final salient component of human auditoryexperience is the domain of music Music perceptionwhich includes processing of melody harmony and

rhythm elements in sound appears to engage rela-tively specialized right lateralized networks some ofwhich may also process nonverbal (prosodic)elements in speech (Zatorre Belin amp Penhune2002) Many words refer explicitly to musical phenom-ena (sing song melody harmony rhythm etc) or toentities (instruments performers performances) thatproduce musical sounds Therefore the model pro-posed here includes a component to capture theexperience of processing music

Gustatory and olfactory components

Gustatory and olfactory experiences are each rep-resented by a single attribute that captures the rel-evance of each type of experience to the conceptThese sensory systems do not show clear large-scaleorganization along any dimension and neuronswithin each system often respond to a broad spec-trum of items Both gustatory and olfactory conceptshave been linked with specific early sensory andhigher associative neural processors (Goldberg et al2006 Gonzaacutelez et al 2006)

Motor components

The motor components of the proposed conceptualrepresentation (Table 1) capture the degree to whicha concept is associated with actions involving particu-lar body parts The neural representation of motoraction verbs has been extensively studied withmany studies showing action-specific activation of ahigh-level network that includes the supramarginalgyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus (BinderDesai Conant amp Graves 2009 Desai Binder Conantamp Seidenberg 2009) There is also evidence for soma-totopic representation of action verb representation inprimary sensorimotor areas (Hauk Johnsrude ampPulvermuller 2004) though this claim is somewhatcontroversial (Postle McMahon Ashton Meredith ampde Zubicaray 2008) Object concepts associated withparticular actions also activate the action network(Binder et al 2009 Boronat et al 2005 Martin et al1995) and there is evidence for somatotopicallyspecific activation depending on which body part istypically used in the object interaction (CarotaMoseley amp Pulvermuumlller 2012) Following precedentin the neuroimaging literature (Carota et al 2012Hauk et al 2004 Tettamanti et al 2005) a three-

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 137

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 4: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

semantic content in terms of experiential primitivesemphasizing in particular the role of space timeevent and causality phenomena in understandingpropositional content Other related early workincludes the sensoryndashfunctional theory proposed byWarrington and colleagues to account for category-related object processing impairments in neurologicalpatients (Warrington amp Shallice 1984) In this theorythe relevant semantic content of physical entities isessentially reduced to two experiential attributes pro-cessed in two distinct brain networks Subsequentwork has generally recognized the limitations of asimple sensoryndashfunctional dichotomy (Allport 1985Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) leading to investi-gation of an ever-expanding list of sensory motorand affective attributes of concepts and the neuralcorrelates of these attributes

Studies of modality-specific contributions to con-ceptual knowledge have usually focused on a singleattribute or small set of attributes Lynott andConnell collected normative ratings for a sample of423 concrete adjectives that describe object proper-ties (Lynott amp Connell 2009) and 400 nouns (Lynottamp Connell 2013) on strength of association witheach of the five primary sensory modalities Partici-pants were asked to rate how strongly they experi-enced a particular concept by hearing tastingfeeling through touch smelling and seeing Gainottiet al (Gainotti Ciaraffa Silveri amp Marra 2009 GainottiSpinelli Scaricamazza amp Marra 2013) expanded onthis set by dividing the visual domain into threedimensions (shape colour and motion) and addingmanipulation experience as a motor dimensionRatings were obtained on 49 animal plant and arte-fact concepts revealing highly significant differencesbetween categories in the perceived relevance ofthese sensoryndashmotor dimensions to each conceptUsing a similar set of dimensions Hoffman andLambon Ralph (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013)obtained strength of association ratings for a set of160 object nouns Participants were asked ldquoHowmuch do you associate this item with a particular(colour visual form observed motion sound tactilesensation taste smell action)rdquo The resulting attri-bute ratings predicted lexicalndashsemantic processingspeed more accurately than verbal feature-based rep-resentations (McRae Cree Seidenberg amp McNorgan2005) and they supported a novel conceptual distinc-tion between mechanical devices (eg vehicles) and

other non-living objects in that the former hadstrong sound and motion characteristics that madethem more similar to animals

Although prior work in this area has focused over-whelmingly on concrete object and action conceptsmore recent studies explore dimensions of experiencethat may be more important in the representation ofabstract concepts Several authors have emphasizedthe role of affective and social experiences in abstractconcept acquisition and embodiment (Borghi FluminiCimatti Marocco amp Scorolli 2011 Kousta ViglioccoVinson Andrews amp Del Campo 2011 ViglioccoMeteyard Andrews amp Kousta 2009 Wiemer-Hastingsamp Xu 2005 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) Crutch et al(Crutch Williams Ridgway amp Borgenicht 2012)obtained ratings for 200 abstract and 200 concretenouns on the relatedness of each word to conceptsof time space quantity emotion polarity (positiveor negative) social interaction morality andthought in addition to the more physical dimensionsof sensation and action These representations suc-cessfully predicted performance by a severelyaphasic patient on an abstract noun comprehensiontask in which semantic similarity between target andfoil responses was manipulated whereas similaritymetrics (latent semantic analysis cosine similarity)based on patterns of word usage (Landauer ampDumais 1997) were not predictive (Crutch TrocheReilly amp Ridgway 2013) Troche et al (TrocheCrutch amp Reilly 2014) provide further analyses ofthese representations identifying three latent factors(labelled perceptual salience affective associationand magnitude) that accounted for 81 of the var-iance Abstract nouns were rated higher than concretenouns on the dimensions of emotion polarity socialinteraction morality and space

Building on this prior work our aim in the presentstudy was to develop a more comprehensive concep-tual representation based on known modalities ofneural information processing This more comprehen-sive representation would ideally capture aspects ofexperience that are central to the acquisition ofevent concepts as well as object concepts andabstract as well as concrete concepts The resultingrepresentation described in the following sectioncontains entries corresponding to specializedsensory and motor processes affective processessystems for processing spatial temporal and causalinformation social cognition processes and abstract

132 J R BINDER ET AL

cognitive operations We hope this approach stimu-lates further empirical and theoretical efforts towarda comprehensive brain-based semantic theory

Neural components of experience

The following section briefly describes the proposedcomponents of a brain-based semantic represen-tation which are listed in Tables 1 and 2 Each ofthese components is defined by an extensive bodyof physiological evidence that can only be hinted athere Examples are provided of how each componentapplies to a range of concepts Two fundamental prin-ciples guided selection of the components First weassume that all aspects of mental experience can con-tribute to concept acquisition and therefore conceptcomposition These ldquoaspects of mental experiencerdquoinclude not only sensory perceptions but also

affective responses to entities and situations experi-ence with performing motor actions perception ofspatial and temporal phenomena perception of caus-ality experience with social phenomena and experi-ence with internal cognitive phenomena and drivestates Second we focus on experiential phenomena

Table 1 Sensory and motor components organized by domainDomain Component Description (with reference to nouns)

Vision Vision something that you can easily seeVision Bright visually light or brightVision Dark visually darkVision Colour having a characteristic or defining colourVision Pattern having a characteristic or defining visual texture

or surface patternVision Large large in sizeVision Small small in sizeVision Motion showing a lot of visually observable movementVision Biomotion showing movement like that of a living thingVision Fast showing visible movement that is fastVision Slow showing visible movement that is slowVision Shape having a characteristic or defining visual shape or

formVision Complexity visually complexVision Face having a human or human-like faceVision Body having human or human-like body partsSomatic Touch something that you could easily recognize by

touchSomatic Temperature hot or cold to the touchSomatic Texture having a smooth or rough texture to the touchSomatic Weight light or heavy in weightSomatic Pain associated with pain or physical discomfortAudition Audition something that you can easily hearAudition Loud making a loud soundAudition Low having a low-pitched soundAudition High having a high-pitched soundAudition Sound having a characteristic or recognizable sound or

soundsAudition Music making a musical soundAudition Speech someone or something that talksGustation Taste having a characteristic or defining tasteOlfaction Smell having a characteristic or defining smell or smellsMotor Head associated with actions using the face mouth or

tongueMotor Upper limb associated with actions using the arm hand or

fingersMotor Lower limb associated with actions using the leg or footMotor Practice a physical object YOU have personal experience

using

Table 2 Spatial temporal causal social emotion drive andattention componentsDomain Name Description (with reference to nouns)

Spatial Landmark having a fixed location as on a mapSpatial Path showing changes in location along a

particular direction or pathSpatial Scene bringing to mind a particular setting or

physical locationSpatial Near often physically near to you (within easy

reach) in everyday lifeSpatial Toward associated with movement toward or into youSpatial Away associated with movement away from or out

of youSpatial Number associated with a specific number or amountTemporal Time an event or occurrence that occurs at a typical

or predictable timeTemporal Duration an event that has a predictable duration

whether short or longTemporal Long an event that lasts for a long period of timeTemporal Short an event that lasts for a short period of timeCausal Caused caused by some clear preceding event action

or situationCausal Consequential likely to have consequences (cause other

things to happen)Social Social an activity or event that involves an

interaction between peopleSocial Human having human or human-like intentions

plans or goalsSocial Communication a thing or action that people use to

communicateSocial Self related to your own view of yourself a part of

YOUR self-imageCognition Cognition a form of mental activity or a function of the

mindEmotion Benefit someone or something that could help or

benefit you or othersEmotion Harm someone or something that could cause harm

to you or othersEmotion Pleasant someone or something that you find pleasantEmotion Unpleasant someone or something that you find

unpleasantEmotion Happy someone or something that makes you feel

happyEmotion Sad someone or something that makes you feel

sadEmotion Angry someone or something that makes you feel

angryEmotion Disgusted someone or something that makes you feel

disgustedEmotion Fearful someone or something that makes you feel

afraidEmotion Surprised someone or something that makes you feel

surprisedDrive Drive someone or something that motivates you to

do somethingDrive Needs someone or something that would be hard

for you to live withoutAttention Attention someone or something that grabs your

attentionAttention Arousal someone or something that makes you feel

alert activated excited or keyed up ineither a positive or negative way

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 133

for which there are likely to be corresponding dis-tinguishable neural processors drawing on evidencefrom animal physiology brain imaging and neurologi-cal studies In particular we focus on macroscopicneural systems that can be distinguished with invivo human imaging methods We do not require orpropose that these neural processors are self-con-tained ldquomodulesrdquo or that they are highly localized inthe brain only that they process information that pri-marily represents a particular aspect of experience

A detailed listing of the queries used to elicit associ-ation ratings for each component is available for down-load (see link prior to the References section)

Visual components

Visual and other sensory components are listed inTable 1 Components of visual experience for whichthere are likely to be corresponding distinguishableneural processors include luminance size colourvisual texture visual shape visual motion and biologi-cal motion Within the visual shape perceptionnetwork are distinguishable networks that primarilyprocess faces human body parts and three-dimen-sional spaces

Luminance is a basic property of vision but is alsorelevant to such concepts as sun light gleam shineink night dark black and so on that are defined bybrightness or darkness Luminance is an example ofa scalar quantitymdashthat is one that represents a con-tinuous one-dimensional variable Linguistic represen-tation of scalars tends to focus on ends of thecontinuum (eg brightdark hotcold heavylightloudsoft etc) which raises a general question ofwhether or to what extent opposite ends of a scalar(eg bright and dark) are represented in distinguish-able neural processors Are there non-identicalneural networks that encode high and low luminanceThe answer depends to some degree on the spatialscale in question It is known that some neuronsrespond somewhat selectively to stimuli with highluminance while others respond more to stimuli withlow luminance (ie ON and OFF cells in retina andV1) resulting in two networks that are distinguishableat a microscopic scale If these and other luminance-tuned neurons intermingle within a single networkhowever the high- and low-luminance networksmight be indistinguishable at least at the macroscopicscale of in vivo imaging

In contrast the scalar quantity visual size is linked tolarge-scale retinotopic organization throughout thevisual cortical system and has been proposed as amajor determinant of medial-to-lateral organizationeven at the highest levels of the ventral visualstream (Hasson Levy Behrmann Hendler amp Malach2002) At these higher levels medial-to-lateral organiz-ation appears not to depend on the actual retinalextent of a given object exemplar which varies withdistance from the observer but rather on a ldquotypicalrdquoor generalized perceptual representation Images ofbuildings for example produce stronger activationin medial regions than do images of insects and theopposite pattern holds for lateral regions even whenthe images are the same physical size (Konkle ampOliva 2012) By extension the general theory that con-cepts are represented partly in perceptual systemswould predict similar medialndashlateral activation differ-ences in the ventral visual stream for concepts repre-senting objects that are reliably large or reliably small

There is now strong evidence not only for a fairlyspecialized colour perception network in the humanbrain (Bartels amp Zeki 2000 Beauchamp HaxbyJennings amp DeYoe 1999) but also for activation inor near this processor by concepts with colourcontent (Hsu Frankland amp Thompson-Schill 2012Hsu Kraemer Oliver Schlichting amp Thompson-Schill2011 Kellenbach Brett amp Patterson 2001 MartinHaxby Lalonde Wiggs amp Ungerleider 1995Simmons et al 2007) The question of whether thereare distinguishable neural processors activated by par-ticular colour concepts (eg red vs green vs blue) hasnot been studied but this appears unlikely Colour-sensitive neurons in the monkey inferior temporalcortex form a network of millimetre-sized ldquoglobsrdquowithin which there is evidence for spatial clusteringof neurons by colour preference (Conway amp Tsao2009) However the spatial scale of these clusters ison the order of 50ndash100 microm beyond the spatial resol-ution of current in vivo imaging methods More impor-tantly it is not clear that the spatial organization ofcolour concept representations should necessarilyreflect this perceptual organization Division of thevisible light frequency spectrum into colour conceptsvaries considerably across cultures (Berlin amp Kay1969) whereas the spatial organization of colour-sen-sitive neurons presumably does not As with the attri-bute of luminance we have assumed for these reasonsthat a single neural processor encodes the colour

134 J R BINDER ET AL

content of concepts regardless of the particular colourin question A concept like lemon that is reliably associ-ated with a particular colour is expected to activatethis neural processor to approximately the samedegree as a concept like coffee that is reliably associ-ated with a different colour

Visual motion perception involves a relativelyspecialized neural processor known as MT or V5 (Alb-right Desimone amp Gross 1984 Zeki 1974) Responsesin MT vary with motion velocity and degree of motioncoherence (Lingnau Ashida Wall amp Smith 2009 ReesFriston amp Koch 2000 Tootell et al 1995) Motion vel-ocity is relevant for our purposes as it is strongly rep-resented at a conceptual level For examplemovements may be relatively fast or slow in velocityand this scalar quantity is central to action conceptslike run dash zoom creep ooze walk and so on aswell as entity concepts like bullet jet hare snail tor-toise sloth and so on Movements may also have aspecific pattern or quality as illustrated for exampleby the large number of verbs that express manner ofmotion (turn bounce roll float spin twist etc) Weare not aware of any evidence for large-scale spatialorganization in the cortex according to type ormanner of motion therefore the proposed semanticrepresentation is designed to capture strength ofassociation with visually observable characteristicmotion in general regardless of specific type Theexception to this rule is the perception of biologicalmotion which has been linked with a neural processorthat is clearly distinct from MT located in the posteriorsuperior temporal sulcus (STS Grossman amp Blake2002 Pelphrey Morris Michelich Allison amp McCarthy2005) Biological motion contains higher order com-plexity that is characteristic of face and body partactions Perception of biological motion plays acentral role in understanding face and body gesturesand thus the affective and intentional state of otheragents (ie theory of mind Allison Puce amp McCarthy2000) Biological motion is thus one of several keyattributes distinguishing intentional agents (boy girlwoman man etc) from inanimate entities

Visual shape perception involves an extensiveregion of extrastriate cortex including a large lateralextrastriate area known as the lateral occipitalcomplex (Malach et al 1995) and an adjacent areaon the ventral occipitalndashtemporal surface known asVOT (Grill-Spector amp Malach 2004) In human func-tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies

these areas respond more strongly to images ofobjects than to images in which shape contours ofthe objects have been disrupted (ldquoscrambledrdquoimages) similar to response patterns observed inprimate ventral temporal neurons (Tanaka SaitoFukada amp Moriya 1991) Shape information is gener-ally the most important attribute of concrete objectconcepts and distinguishes concrete concepts froma range of abstract (eg affective social and cogni-tive) entities and event concepts Variation in the sal-ience of visual shape also occurs within the domainof concrete entities Shape is typically not a definingfeature of substance nouns (metal plastic water) butit has relevance for some substance-like mass nouns(butter coffee rice) Concrete objects also vary inshape complexity Animals for example tend tohave more complex shapes than plants or tools (Snod-grass amp Vanderwart 1980) We hypothesize that bothshape salience and shape complexity determine thedegree of activation in shape processing regionsduring concept retrieval

More focal areas within or adjacent to these shapeprocessing regions show preferential responses tohuman faces (Kanwisher McDermott amp Chun 1997Schwarzlose Baker amp Kanwisher 2005) and bodyparts (Downing Jiang Shuman amp Kanwisher 2001)A specialized mechanism for face perception is con-sistent with the central role of face recognition insocial interactions Visual perception of body partsprobably plays a crucial role in understandingobserved actions and mapping these onto internalrepresentations of the body schema during actionlearning These processors would be differentially acti-vated by concepts pertaining to faces (nose mouthportrait) and body parts (hand leg finger) As a firstapproximation we propose that both of these neuralprocessors are activated to some degree by all con-cepts referring to human beings which by necessityinclude a face and other human body parts Activationof the face processor may be minimal in the case ofconcepts that represent general human types androles (boy man nurse etc) as these concepts do notinclude information about any specific face whereasconcepts referencing specific individuals (brotherfather Einstein) might include specific facial featureinformation and therefore activate the face processorto a greater degree

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 135

parahippocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998Epstein Parker amp Feiler 2007) Although this processoris typically studied using visual stimuli and is oftenconsidered a high-level visual area it more likely inte-grates visual proprioceptive and perhaps auditoryinputs all of which provide correlated informationabout three-dimensional space Further discussion ofthis component of experience is provided in thesection below on Spatial Components (Table 2)

Somatosensory components

Somatosensory networks of the cortex represent bodylocation (somatotopic representation) body positionand joint force (proprioception) pain surface charac-teristics of objects (texture and temperature) andobject shape (Friebel Eickhoff amp Lotze 2011Hendry Hsiao amp Bushnell 1999 Lamm Decety ampSinger 2011 Longo Azanon amp Haggard 2010McGlone amp Reilly 2010 Medina amp Coslett 2010Serino amp Haggard 2010) Somatotopy is an inherentlymulti-modal phenomenon because the body locationtouched by a particular object is typically the samebody location as that used during exploratory ormanipulative motor actions involving the object Con-ceptual and linguistic representations are more likelyto encode the action as the salient characteristic ofthe object rather than the tactile experience (we sayldquoI threw the ballrdquo to describe the event not ldquothe balltouched my handrdquo) therefore somatotopic knowledgewas encoded in the proposed representation usingaction-based rather than somatosensory attributes(see Motor Components)

Temperature tactile texture and pain wereincluded as components of the representation aswas a component referring to the salience of objectweight Weight is perceived mainly via proprioceptiverepresentations and is a salient attribute of objectswith which we interact Temperature and weight aretwo more examples of scalar entities that may ormay not have a macroscopic topography in thebrain (Hendry et al 1999 Mazzola Faillenot BarralMauguiere amp Peyron 2012) That is although thereis evidence that these types of somatosensory infor-mation are distinguishable from each other no evi-dence has yet been presented for a scalartopography that separates for example hot fromcold representations Tactile texture can refer to avariety of physical properties we operationalized this

concept as a continuum from smooth to rough andthus the texture component is another scalar entityFor all three of these scalars we elected to representthe entire continuum as a single component Thatis the temperature component is intended tocapture the salience of temperature to a conceptregardless of whether the associated temperature ishot or cold

The published literature on conceptual processingof somatosensory information has focused almostexclusively on impaired knowledge of body parts inneurological patients (Coslett Saffran amp Schwoebel2002 Kemmerer amp Tranel 2008 Laiacona AllamanoLorenzi amp Capitani 2006 Schwoebel amp Coslett2005) though no definitive localization has emergedfrom these studies A number of functional imagingstudies suggest an overlap between networksinvolved in real pain perception and those involvedin empathic pain perception (perception of pain inothers) and perception of ldquosocial painrdquo (Eisenberger2012 Lamm et al 2011) suggesting that retrieval ofpain concepts involves a simulation process To ourknowledge no studies have yet examined the concep-tual representation of temperature tactile texture orweight

Finally object shape is an attribute learned partlythrough haptic experiences (Miqueacutee et al 2008) butthe degree to which haptic shape is learned indepen-dently from visual shape is unclear Some objects areseen but not felt (ie very large and very distantobjects) but the converse is rarely true at least insighted individuals It was therefore decided that thevisual shape query would capture knowledge aboutshape in both modalities and that a separate queryregarding extent of personal manipulation experience(see Motor Components) would capture the impor-tance of haptic shape relative to visual shape

Auditory components

The auditory cortex includes low-level areas tuned tofrequency (tonotopy) amplitude and spatial location(Schreiner amp Winer 2007) as well as higher levelsshowing specialization for auditory ldquoobjectsrdquo such asenvironmental sounds (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010)and speech sounds (Belin Zatorre Lafaille Ahad ampPike 2000 Liebenthal Binder Spitzer Possing ampMedler 2005) Corresponding auditory attributes ofthe proposed conceptual representation capture the

136 J R BINDER ET AL

degree to which a concept is associated with a low-pitched high-pitched loud (ie high amplitude)recognizable (ie having a characteristic auditoryform) or speech-like sound The attribute ldquolow inamplituderdquo was not included because it was con-sidered to be a salient attribute of relatively fewconceptsmdashthat is those that refer specifically to alow-amplitude variant (eg whisper) Concepts thatfocus on the absence of sound (eg silence quiet)are probably more accurately encoded as a nullvalue on the ldquoloudrdquo attribute as fMRI studies haveshown auditory regions where activity increases withsound intensity but not the converse (Roumlhl amp Uppen-kamp 2012)

A few studies of sound-related knowledge(Goldberg Perfetti amp Schneider 2006 Kellenbachet al 2001 Kiefer Sim Herrnberger Grothe ampHoenig 2008) reported activation in or near the pos-terior superior temporal sulcus (STS) an auditoryassociation region implicated in environmentalsound recognition (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010 J WLewis et al 2004) Trumpp et al (Trumpp KlieseHoenig Haarmeier amp Kiefer 2013) described apatient with a circumscribed lesion centred on theleft posterior STS who displayed a specific deficit(slower response times and higher error rate) invisual recognition of sound-related words All ofthese studies examined general environmentalsound knowledge nothing is yet known regardingthe conceptual representation of specific types ofauditory attributes like frequency or amplitude

Localization of sounds in space is an importantaspect of auditory perception yet auditory perceptionof space has little or no representation in languageindependent of (multimodal) spatial concepts thereare no concepts with components like ldquomakessounds from the leftrdquo because spatial relationshipsbetween sound sources and listeners are almostnever fixed or central to meaning Like shape attri-butes of concepts spatial attributes and spatial con-cepts are learned through strongly correlatedmultimodal experiences involving visual auditorytactile and motor processes In the proposed semanticrepresentation model spatial attributes of conceptsare represented by modality-independent spatialcomponents (see Spatial Components)

A final salient component of human auditoryexperience is the domain of music Music perceptionwhich includes processing of melody harmony and

rhythm elements in sound appears to engage rela-tively specialized right lateralized networks some ofwhich may also process nonverbal (prosodic)elements in speech (Zatorre Belin amp Penhune2002) Many words refer explicitly to musical phenom-ena (sing song melody harmony rhythm etc) or toentities (instruments performers performances) thatproduce musical sounds Therefore the model pro-posed here includes a component to capture theexperience of processing music

Gustatory and olfactory components

Gustatory and olfactory experiences are each rep-resented by a single attribute that captures the rel-evance of each type of experience to the conceptThese sensory systems do not show clear large-scaleorganization along any dimension and neuronswithin each system often respond to a broad spec-trum of items Both gustatory and olfactory conceptshave been linked with specific early sensory andhigher associative neural processors (Goldberg et al2006 Gonzaacutelez et al 2006)

Motor components

The motor components of the proposed conceptualrepresentation (Table 1) capture the degree to whicha concept is associated with actions involving particu-lar body parts The neural representation of motoraction verbs has been extensively studied withmany studies showing action-specific activation of ahigh-level network that includes the supramarginalgyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus (BinderDesai Conant amp Graves 2009 Desai Binder Conantamp Seidenberg 2009) There is also evidence for soma-totopic representation of action verb representation inprimary sensorimotor areas (Hauk Johnsrude ampPulvermuller 2004) though this claim is somewhatcontroversial (Postle McMahon Ashton Meredith ampde Zubicaray 2008) Object concepts associated withparticular actions also activate the action network(Binder et al 2009 Boronat et al 2005 Martin et al1995) and there is evidence for somatotopicallyspecific activation depending on which body part istypically used in the object interaction (CarotaMoseley amp Pulvermuumlller 2012) Following precedentin the neuroimaging literature (Carota et al 2012Hauk et al 2004 Tettamanti et al 2005) a three-

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 137

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 5: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

cognitive operations We hope this approach stimu-lates further empirical and theoretical efforts towarda comprehensive brain-based semantic theory

Neural components of experience

The following section briefly describes the proposedcomponents of a brain-based semantic represen-tation which are listed in Tables 1 and 2 Each ofthese components is defined by an extensive bodyof physiological evidence that can only be hinted athere Examples are provided of how each componentapplies to a range of concepts Two fundamental prin-ciples guided selection of the components First weassume that all aspects of mental experience can con-tribute to concept acquisition and therefore conceptcomposition These ldquoaspects of mental experiencerdquoinclude not only sensory perceptions but also

affective responses to entities and situations experi-ence with performing motor actions perception ofspatial and temporal phenomena perception of caus-ality experience with social phenomena and experi-ence with internal cognitive phenomena and drivestates Second we focus on experiential phenomena

Table 1 Sensory and motor components organized by domainDomain Component Description (with reference to nouns)

Vision Vision something that you can easily seeVision Bright visually light or brightVision Dark visually darkVision Colour having a characteristic or defining colourVision Pattern having a characteristic or defining visual texture

or surface patternVision Large large in sizeVision Small small in sizeVision Motion showing a lot of visually observable movementVision Biomotion showing movement like that of a living thingVision Fast showing visible movement that is fastVision Slow showing visible movement that is slowVision Shape having a characteristic or defining visual shape or

formVision Complexity visually complexVision Face having a human or human-like faceVision Body having human or human-like body partsSomatic Touch something that you could easily recognize by

touchSomatic Temperature hot or cold to the touchSomatic Texture having a smooth or rough texture to the touchSomatic Weight light or heavy in weightSomatic Pain associated with pain or physical discomfortAudition Audition something that you can easily hearAudition Loud making a loud soundAudition Low having a low-pitched soundAudition High having a high-pitched soundAudition Sound having a characteristic or recognizable sound or

soundsAudition Music making a musical soundAudition Speech someone or something that talksGustation Taste having a characteristic or defining tasteOlfaction Smell having a characteristic or defining smell or smellsMotor Head associated with actions using the face mouth or

tongueMotor Upper limb associated with actions using the arm hand or

fingersMotor Lower limb associated with actions using the leg or footMotor Practice a physical object YOU have personal experience

using

Table 2 Spatial temporal causal social emotion drive andattention componentsDomain Name Description (with reference to nouns)

Spatial Landmark having a fixed location as on a mapSpatial Path showing changes in location along a

particular direction or pathSpatial Scene bringing to mind a particular setting or

physical locationSpatial Near often physically near to you (within easy

reach) in everyday lifeSpatial Toward associated with movement toward or into youSpatial Away associated with movement away from or out

of youSpatial Number associated with a specific number or amountTemporal Time an event or occurrence that occurs at a typical

or predictable timeTemporal Duration an event that has a predictable duration

whether short or longTemporal Long an event that lasts for a long period of timeTemporal Short an event that lasts for a short period of timeCausal Caused caused by some clear preceding event action

or situationCausal Consequential likely to have consequences (cause other

things to happen)Social Social an activity or event that involves an

interaction between peopleSocial Human having human or human-like intentions

plans or goalsSocial Communication a thing or action that people use to

communicateSocial Self related to your own view of yourself a part of

YOUR self-imageCognition Cognition a form of mental activity or a function of the

mindEmotion Benefit someone or something that could help or

benefit you or othersEmotion Harm someone or something that could cause harm

to you or othersEmotion Pleasant someone or something that you find pleasantEmotion Unpleasant someone or something that you find

unpleasantEmotion Happy someone or something that makes you feel

happyEmotion Sad someone or something that makes you feel

sadEmotion Angry someone or something that makes you feel

angryEmotion Disgusted someone or something that makes you feel

disgustedEmotion Fearful someone or something that makes you feel

afraidEmotion Surprised someone or something that makes you feel

surprisedDrive Drive someone or something that motivates you to

do somethingDrive Needs someone or something that would be hard

for you to live withoutAttention Attention someone or something that grabs your

attentionAttention Arousal someone or something that makes you feel

alert activated excited or keyed up ineither a positive or negative way

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 133

for which there are likely to be corresponding dis-tinguishable neural processors drawing on evidencefrom animal physiology brain imaging and neurologi-cal studies In particular we focus on macroscopicneural systems that can be distinguished with invivo human imaging methods We do not require orpropose that these neural processors are self-con-tained ldquomodulesrdquo or that they are highly localized inthe brain only that they process information that pri-marily represents a particular aspect of experience

A detailed listing of the queries used to elicit associ-ation ratings for each component is available for down-load (see link prior to the References section)

Visual components

Visual and other sensory components are listed inTable 1 Components of visual experience for whichthere are likely to be corresponding distinguishableneural processors include luminance size colourvisual texture visual shape visual motion and biologi-cal motion Within the visual shape perceptionnetwork are distinguishable networks that primarilyprocess faces human body parts and three-dimen-sional spaces

Luminance is a basic property of vision but is alsorelevant to such concepts as sun light gleam shineink night dark black and so on that are defined bybrightness or darkness Luminance is an example ofa scalar quantitymdashthat is one that represents a con-tinuous one-dimensional variable Linguistic represen-tation of scalars tends to focus on ends of thecontinuum (eg brightdark hotcold heavylightloudsoft etc) which raises a general question ofwhether or to what extent opposite ends of a scalar(eg bright and dark) are represented in distinguish-able neural processors Are there non-identicalneural networks that encode high and low luminanceThe answer depends to some degree on the spatialscale in question It is known that some neuronsrespond somewhat selectively to stimuli with highluminance while others respond more to stimuli withlow luminance (ie ON and OFF cells in retina andV1) resulting in two networks that are distinguishableat a microscopic scale If these and other luminance-tuned neurons intermingle within a single networkhowever the high- and low-luminance networksmight be indistinguishable at least at the macroscopicscale of in vivo imaging

In contrast the scalar quantity visual size is linked tolarge-scale retinotopic organization throughout thevisual cortical system and has been proposed as amajor determinant of medial-to-lateral organizationeven at the highest levels of the ventral visualstream (Hasson Levy Behrmann Hendler amp Malach2002) At these higher levels medial-to-lateral organiz-ation appears not to depend on the actual retinalextent of a given object exemplar which varies withdistance from the observer but rather on a ldquotypicalrdquoor generalized perceptual representation Images ofbuildings for example produce stronger activationin medial regions than do images of insects and theopposite pattern holds for lateral regions even whenthe images are the same physical size (Konkle ampOliva 2012) By extension the general theory that con-cepts are represented partly in perceptual systemswould predict similar medialndashlateral activation differ-ences in the ventral visual stream for concepts repre-senting objects that are reliably large or reliably small

There is now strong evidence not only for a fairlyspecialized colour perception network in the humanbrain (Bartels amp Zeki 2000 Beauchamp HaxbyJennings amp DeYoe 1999) but also for activation inor near this processor by concepts with colourcontent (Hsu Frankland amp Thompson-Schill 2012Hsu Kraemer Oliver Schlichting amp Thompson-Schill2011 Kellenbach Brett amp Patterson 2001 MartinHaxby Lalonde Wiggs amp Ungerleider 1995Simmons et al 2007) The question of whether thereare distinguishable neural processors activated by par-ticular colour concepts (eg red vs green vs blue) hasnot been studied but this appears unlikely Colour-sensitive neurons in the monkey inferior temporalcortex form a network of millimetre-sized ldquoglobsrdquowithin which there is evidence for spatial clusteringof neurons by colour preference (Conway amp Tsao2009) However the spatial scale of these clusters ison the order of 50ndash100 microm beyond the spatial resol-ution of current in vivo imaging methods More impor-tantly it is not clear that the spatial organization ofcolour concept representations should necessarilyreflect this perceptual organization Division of thevisible light frequency spectrum into colour conceptsvaries considerably across cultures (Berlin amp Kay1969) whereas the spatial organization of colour-sen-sitive neurons presumably does not As with the attri-bute of luminance we have assumed for these reasonsthat a single neural processor encodes the colour

134 J R BINDER ET AL

content of concepts regardless of the particular colourin question A concept like lemon that is reliably associ-ated with a particular colour is expected to activatethis neural processor to approximately the samedegree as a concept like coffee that is reliably associ-ated with a different colour

Visual motion perception involves a relativelyspecialized neural processor known as MT or V5 (Alb-right Desimone amp Gross 1984 Zeki 1974) Responsesin MT vary with motion velocity and degree of motioncoherence (Lingnau Ashida Wall amp Smith 2009 ReesFriston amp Koch 2000 Tootell et al 1995) Motion vel-ocity is relevant for our purposes as it is strongly rep-resented at a conceptual level For examplemovements may be relatively fast or slow in velocityand this scalar quantity is central to action conceptslike run dash zoom creep ooze walk and so on aswell as entity concepts like bullet jet hare snail tor-toise sloth and so on Movements may also have aspecific pattern or quality as illustrated for exampleby the large number of verbs that express manner ofmotion (turn bounce roll float spin twist etc) Weare not aware of any evidence for large-scale spatialorganization in the cortex according to type ormanner of motion therefore the proposed semanticrepresentation is designed to capture strength ofassociation with visually observable characteristicmotion in general regardless of specific type Theexception to this rule is the perception of biologicalmotion which has been linked with a neural processorthat is clearly distinct from MT located in the posteriorsuperior temporal sulcus (STS Grossman amp Blake2002 Pelphrey Morris Michelich Allison amp McCarthy2005) Biological motion contains higher order com-plexity that is characteristic of face and body partactions Perception of biological motion plays acentral role in understanding face and body gesturesand thus the affective and intentional state of otheragents (ie theory of mind Allison Puce amp McCarthy2000) Biological motion is thus one of several keyattributes distinguishing intentional agents (boy girlwoman man etc) from inanimate entities

Visual shape perception involves an extensiveregion of extrastriate cortex including a large lateralextrastriate area known as the lateral occipitalcomplex (Malach et al 1995) and an adjacent areaon the ventral occipitalndashtemporal surface known asVOT (Grill-Spector amp Malach 2004) In human func-tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies

these areas respond more strongly to images ofobjects than to images in which shape contours ofthe objects have been disrupted (ldquoscrambledrdquoimages) similar to response patterns observed inprimate ventral temporal neurons (Tanaka SaitoFukada amp Moriya 1991) Shape information is gener-ally the most important attribute of concrete objectconcepts and distinguishes concrete concepts froma range of abstract (eg affective social and cogni-tive) entities and event concepts Variation in the sal-ience of visual shape also occurs within the domainof concrete entities Shape is typically not a definingfeature of substance nouns (metal plastic water) butit has relevance for some substance-like mass nouns(butter coffee rice) Concrete objects also vary inshape complexity Animals for example tend tohave more complex shapes than plants or tools (Snod-grass amp Vanderwart 1980) We hypothesize that bothshape salience and shape complexity determine thedegree of activation in shape processing regionsduring concept retrieval

More focal areas within or adjacent to these shapeprocessing regions show preferential responses tohuman faces (Kanwisher McDermott amp Chun 1997Schwarzlose Baker amp Kanwisher 2005) and bodyparts (Downing Jiang Shuman amp Kanwisher 2001)A specialized mechanism for face perception is con-sistent with the central role of face recognition insocial interactions Visual perception of body partsprobably plays a crucial role in understandingobserved actions and mapping these onto internalrepresentations of the body schema during actionlearning These processors would be differentially acti-vated by concepts pertaining to faces (nose mouthportrait) and body parts (hand leg finger) As a firstapproximation we propose that both of these neuralprocessors are activated to some degree by all con-cepts referring to human beings which by necessityinclude a face and other human body parts Activationof the face processor may be minimal in the case ofconcepts that represent general human types androles (boy man nurse etc) as these concepts do notinclude information about any specific face whereasconcepts referencing specific individuals (brotherfather Einstein) might include specific facial featureinformation and therefore activate the face processorto a greater degree

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 135

parahippocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998Epstein Parker amp Feiler 2007) Although this processoris typically studied using visual stimuli and is oftenconsidered a high-level visual area it more likely inte-grates visual proprioceptive and perhaps auditoryinputs all of which provide correlated informationabout three-dimensional space Further discussion ofthis component of experience is provided in thesection below on Spatial Components (Table 2)

Somatosensory components

Somatosensory networks of the cortex represent bodylocation (somatotopic representation) body positionand joint force (proprioception) pain surface charac-teristics of objects (texture and temperature) andobject shape (Friebel Eickhoff amp Lotze 2011Hendry Hsiao amp Bushnell 1999 Lamm Decety ampSinger 2011 Longo Azanon amp Haggard 2010McGlone amp Reilly 2010 Medina amp Coslett 2010Serino amp Haggard 2010) Somatotopy is an inherentlymulti-modal phenomenon because the body locationtouched by a particular object is typically the samebody location as that used during exploratory ormanipulative motor actions involving the object Con-ceptual and linguistic representations are more likelyto encode the action as the salient characteristic ofthe object rather than the tactile experience (we sayldquoI threw the ballrdquo to describe the event not ldquothe balltouched my handrdquo) therefore somatotopic knowledgewas encoded in the proposed representation usingaction-based rather than somatosensory attributes(see Motor Components)

Temperature tactile texture and pain wereincluded as components of the representation aswas a component referring to the salience of objectweight Weight is perceived mainly via proprioceptiverepresentations and is a salient attribute of objectswith which we interact Temperature and weight aretwo more examples of scalar entities that may ormay not have a macroscopic topography in thebrain (Hendry et al 1999 Mazzola Faillenot BarralMauguiere amp Peyron 2012) That is although thereis evidence that these types of somatosensory infor-mation are distinguishable from each other no evi-dence has yet been presented for a scalartopography that separates for example hot fromcold representations Tactile texture can refer to avariety of physical properties we operationalized this

concept as a continuum from smooth to rough andthus the texture component is another scalar entityFor all three of these scalars we elected to representthe entire continuum as a single component Thatis the temperature component is intended tocapture the salience of temperature to a conceptregardless of whether the associated temperature ishot or cold

The published literature on conceptual processingof somatosensory information has focused almostexclusively on impaired knowledge of body parts inneurological patients (Coslett Saffran amp Schwoebel2002 Kemmerer amp Tranel 2008 Laiacona AllamanoLorenzi amp Capitani 2006 Schwoebel amp Coslett2005) though no definitive localization has emergedfrom these studies A number of functional imagingstudies suggest an overlap between networksinvolved in real pain perception and those involvedin empathic pain perception (perception of pain inothers) and perception of ldquosocial painrdquo (Eisenberger2012 Lamm et al 2011) suggesting that retrieval ofpain concepts involves a simulation process To ourknowledge no studies have yet examined the concep-tual representation of temperature tactile texture orweight

Finally object shape is an attribute learned partlythrough haptic experiences (Miqueacutee et al 2008) butthe degree to which haptic shape is learned indepen-dently from visual shape is unclear Some objects areseen but not felt (ie very large and very distantobjects) but the converse is rarely true at least insighted individuals It was therefore decided that thevisual shape query would capture knowledge aboutshape in both modalities and that a separate queryregarding extent of personal manipulation experience(see Motor Components) would capture the impor-tance of haptic shape relative to visual shape

Auditory components

The auditory cortex includes low-level areas tuned tofrequency (tonotopy) amplitude and spatial location(Schreiner amp Winer 2007) as well as higher levelsshowing specialization for auditory ldquoobjectsrdquo such asenvironmental sounds (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010)and speech sounds (Belin Zatorre Lafaille Ahad ampPike 2000 Liebenthal Binder Spitzer Possing ampMedler 2005) Corresponding auditory attributes ofthe proposed conceptual representation capture the

136 J R BINDER ET AL

degree to which a concept is associated with a low-pitched high-pitched loud (ie high amplitude)recognizable (ie having a characteristic auditoryform) or speech-like sound The attribute ldquolow inamplituderdquo was not included because it was con-sidered to be a salient attribute of relatively fewconceptsmdashthat is those that refer specifically to alow-amplitude variant (eg whisper) Concepts thatfocus on the absence of sound (eg silence quiet)are probably more accurately encoded as a nullvalue on the ldquoloudrdquo attribute as fMRI studies haveshown auditory regions where activity increases withsound intensity but not the converse (Roumlhl amp Uppen-kamp 2012)

A few studies of sound-related knowledge(Goldberg Perfetti amp Schneider 2006 Kellenbachet al 2001 Kiefer Sim Herrnberger Grothe ampHoenig 2008) reported activation in or near the pos-terior superior temporal sulcus (STS) an auditoryassociation region implicated in environmentalsound recognition (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010 J WLewis et al 2004) Trumpp et al (Trumpp KlieseHoenig Haarmeier amp Kiefer 2013) described apatient with a circumscribed lesion centred on theleft posterior STS who displayed a specific deficit(slower response times and higher error rate) invisual recognition of sound-related words All ofthese studies examined general environmentalsound knowledge nothing is yet known regardingthe conceptual representation of specific types ofauditory attributes like frequency or amplitude

Localization of sounds in space is an importantaspect of auditory perception yet auditory perceptionof space has little or no representation in languageindependent of (multimodal) spatial concepts thereare no concepts with components like ldquomakessounds from the leftrdquo because spatial relationshipsbetween sound sources and listeners are almostnever fixed or central to meaning Like shape attri-butes of concepts spatial attributes and spatial con-cepts are learned through strongly correlatedmultimodal experiences involving visual auditorytactile and motor processes In the proposed semanticrepresentation model spatial attributes of conceptsare represented by modality-independent spatialcomponents (see Spatial Components)

A final salient component of human auditoryexperience is the domain of music Music perceptionwhich includes processing of melody harmony and

rhythm elements in sound appears to engage rela-tively specialized right lateralized networks some ofwhich may also process nonverbal (prosodic)elements in speech (Zatorre Belin amp Penhune2002) Many words refer explicitly to musical phenom-ena (sing song melody harmony rhythm etc) or toentities (instruments performers performances) thatproduce musical sounds Therefore the model pro-posed here includes a component to capture theexperience of processing music

Gustatory and olfactory components

Gustatory and olfactory experiences are each rep-resented by a single attribute that captures the rel-evance of each type of experience to the conceptThese sensory systems do not show clear large-scaleorganization along any dimension and neuronswithin each system often respond to a broad spec-trum of items Both gustatory and olfactory conceptshave been linked with specific early sensory andhigher associative neural processors (Goldberg et al2006 Gonzaacutelez et al 2006)

Motor components

The motor components of the proposed conceptualrepresentation (Table 1) capture the degree to whicha concept is associated with actions involving particu-lar body parts The neural representation of motoraction verbs has been extensively studied withmany studies showing action-specific activation of ahigh-level network that includes the supramarginalgyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus (BinderDesai Conant amp Graves 2009 Desai Binder Conantamp Seidenberg 2009) There is also evidence for soma-totopic representation of action verb representation inprimary sensorimotor areas (Hauk Johnsrude ampPulvermuller 2004) though this claim is somewhatcontroversial (Postle McMahon Ashton Meredith ampde Zubicaray 2008) Object concepts associated withparticular actions also activate the action network(Binder et al 2009 Boronat et al 2005 Martin et al1995) and there is evidence for somatotopicallyspecific activation depending on which body part istypically used in the object interaction (CarotaMoseley amp Pulvermuumlller 2012) Following precedentin the neuroimaging literature (Carota et al 2012Hauk et al 2004 Tettamanti et al 2005) a three-

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 137

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 6: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

for which there are likely to be corresponding dis-tinguishable neural processors drawing on evidencefrom animal physiology brain imaging and neurologi-cal studies In particular we focus on macroscopicneural systems that can be distinguished with invivo human imaging methods We do not require orpropose that these neural processors are self-con-tained ldquomodulesrdquo or that they are highly localized inthe brain only that they process information that pri-marily represents a particular aspect of experience

A detailed listing of the queries used to elicit associ-ation ratings for each component is available for down-load (see link prior to the References section)

Visual components

Visual and other sensory components are listed inTable 1 Components of visual experience for whichthere are likely to be corresponding distinguishableneural processors include luminance size colourvisual texture visual shape visual motion and biologi-cal motion Within the visual shape perceptionnetwork are distinguishable networks that primarilyprocess faces human body parts and three-dimen-sional spaces

Luminance is a basic property of vision but is alsorelevant to such concepts as sun light gleam shineink night dark black and so on that are defined bybrightness or darkness Luminance is an example ofa scalar quantitymdashthat is one that represents a con-tinuous one-dimensional variable Linguistic represen-tation of scalars tends to focus on ends of thecontinuum (eg brightdark hotcold heavylightloudsoft etc) which raises a general question ofwhether or to what extent opposite ends of a scalar(eg bright and dark) are represented in distinguish-able neural processors Are there non-identicalneural networks that encode high and low luminanceThe answer depends to some degree on the spatialscale in question It is known that some neuronsrespond somewhat selectively to stimuli with highluminance while others respond more to stimuli withlow luminance (ie ON and OFF cells in retina andV1) resulting in two networks that are distinguishableat a microscopic scale If these and other luminance-tuned neurons intermingle within a single networkhowever the high- and low-luminance networksmight be indistinguishable at least at the macroscopicscale of in vivo imaging

In contrast the scalar quantity visual size is linked tolarge-scale retinotopic organization throughout thevisual cortical system and has been proposed as amajor determinant of medial-to-lateral organizationeven at the highest levels of the ventral visualstream (Hasson Levy Behrmann Hendler amp Malach2002) At these higher levels medial-to-lateral organiz-ation appears not to depend on the actual retinalextent of a given object exemplar which varies withdistance from the observer but rather on a ldquotypicalrdquoor generalized perceptual representation Images ofbuildings for example produce stronger activationin medial regions than do images of insects and theopposite pattern holds for lateral regions even whenthe images are the same physical size (Konkle ampOliva 2012) By extension the general theory that con-cepts are represented partly in perceptual systemswould predict similar medialndashlateral activation differ-ences in the ventral visual stream for concepts repre-senting objects that are reliably large or reliably small

There is now strong evidence not only for a fairlyspecialized colour perception network in the humanbrain (Bartels amp Zeki 2000 Beauchamp HaxbyJennings amp DeYoe 1999) but also for activation inor near this processor by concepts with colourcontent (Hsu Frankland amp Thompson-Schill 2012Hsu Kraemer Oliver Schlichting amp Thompson-Schill2011 Kellenbach Brett amp Patterson 2001 MartinHaxby Lalonde Wiggs amp Ungerleider 1995Simmons et al 2007) The question of whether thereare distinguishable neural processors activated by par-ticular colour concepts (eg red vs green vs blue) hasnot been studied but this appears unlikely Colour-sensitive neurons in the monkey inferior temporalcortex form a network of millimetre-sized ldquoglobsrdquowithin which there is evidence for spatial clusteringof neurons by colour preference (Conway amp Tsao2009) However the spatial scale of these clusters ison the order of 50ndash100 microm beyond the spatial resol-ution of current in vivo imaging methods More impor-tantly it is not clear that the spatial organization ofcolour concept representations should necessarilyreflect this perceptual organization Division of thevisible light frequency spectrum into colour conceptsvaries considerably across cultures (Berlin amp Kay1969) whereas the spatial organization of colour-sen-sitive neurons presumably does not As with the attri-bute of luminance we have assumed for these reasonsthat a single neural processor encodes the colour

134 J R BINDER ET AL

content of concepts regardless of the particular colourin question A concept like lemon that is reliably associ-ated with a particular colour is expected to activatethis neural processor to approximately the samedegree as a concept like coffee that is reliably associ-ated with a different colour

Visual motion perception involves a relativelyspecialized neural processor known as MT or V5 (Alb-right Desimone amp Gross 1984 Zeki 1974) Responsesin MT vary with motion velocity and degree of motioncoherence (Lingnau Ashida Wall amp Smith 2009 ReesFriston amp Koch 2000 Tootell et al 1995) Motion vel-ocity is relevant for our purposes as it is strongly rep-resented at a conceptual level For examplemovements may be relatively fast or slow in velocityand this scalar quantity is central to action conceptslike run dash zoom creep ooze walk and so on aswell as entity concepts like bullet jet hare snail tor-toise sloth and so on Movements may also have aspecific pattern or quality as illustrated for exampleby the large number of verbs that express manner ofmotion (turn bounce roll float spin twist etc) Weare not aware of any evidence for large-scale spatialorganization in the cortex according to type ormanner of motion therefore the proposed semanticrepresentation is designed to capture strength ofassociation with visually observable characteristicmotion in general regardless of specific type Theexception to this rule is the perception of biologicalmotion which has been linked with a neural processorthat is clearly distinct from MT located in the posteriorsuperior temporal sulcus (STS Grossman amp Blake2002 Pelphrey Morris Michelich Allison amp McCarthy2005) Biological motion contains higher order com-plexity that is characteristic of face and body partactions Perception of biological motion plays acentral role in understanding face and body gesturesand thus the affective and intentional state of otheragents (ie theory of mind Allison Puce amp McCarthy2000) Biological motion is thus one of several keyattributes distinguishing intentional agents (boy girlwoman man etc) from inanimate entities

Visual shape perception involves an extensiveregion of extrastriate cortex including a large lateralextrastriate area known as the lateral occipitalcomplex (Malach et al 1995) and an adjacent areaon the ventral occipitalndashtemporal surface known asVOT (Grill-Spector amp Malach 2004) In human func-tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies

these areas respond more strongly to images ofobjects than to images in which shape contours ofthe objects have been disrupted (ldquoscrambledrdquoimages) similar to response patterns observed inprimate ventral temporal neurons (Tanaka SaitoFukada amp Moriya 1991) Shape information is gener-ally the most important attribute of concrete objectconcepts and distinguishes concrete concepts froma range of abstract (eg affective social and cogni-tive) entities and event concepts Variation in the sal-ience of visual shape also occurs within the domainof concrete entities Shape is typically not a definingfeature of substance nouns (metal plastic water) butit has relevance for some substance-like mass nouns(butter coffee rice) Concrete objects also vary inshape complexity Animals for example tend tohave more complex shapes than plants or tools (Snod-grass amp Vanderwart 1980) We hypothesize that bothshape salience and shape complexity determine thedegree of activation in shape processing regionsduring concept retrieval

More focal areas within or adjacent to these shapeprocessing regions show preferential responses tohuman faces (Kanwisher McDermott amp Chun 1997Schwarzlose Baker amp Kanwisher 2005) and bodyparts (Downing Jiang Shuman amp Kanwisher 2001)A specialized mechanism for face perception is con-sistent with the central role of face recognition insocial interactions Visual perception of body partsprobably plays a crucial role in understandingobserved actions and mapping these onto internalrepresentations of the body schema during actionlearning These processors would be differentially acti-vated by concepts pertaining to faces (nose mouthportrait) and body parts (hand leg finger) As a firstapproximation we propose that both of these neuralprocessors are activated to some degree by all con-cepts referring to human beings which by necessityinclude a face and other human body parts Activationof the face processor may be minimal in the case ofconcepts that represent general human types androles (boy man nurse etc) as these concepts do notinclude information about any specific face whereasconcepts referencing specific individuals (brotherfather Einstein) might include specific facial featureinformation and therefore activate the face processorto a greater degree

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 135

parahippocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998Epstein Parker amp Feiler 2007) Although this processoris typically studied using visual stimuli and is oftenconsidered a high-level visual area it more likely inte-grates visual proprioceptive and perhaps auditoryinputs all of which provide correlated informationabout three-dimensional space Further discussion ofthis component of experience is provided in thesection below on Spatial Components (Table 2)

Somatosensory components

Somatosensory networks of the cortex represent bodylocation (somatotopic representation) body positionand joint force (proprioception) pain surface charac-teristics of objects (texture and temperature) andobject shape (Friebel Eickhoff amp Lotze 2011Hendry Hsiao amp Bushnell 1999 Lamm Decety ampSinger 2011 Longo Azanon amp Haggard 2010McGlone amp Reilly 2010 Medina amp Coslett 2010Serino amp Haggard 2010) Somatotopy is an inherentlymulti-modal phenomenon because the body locationtouched by a particular object is typically the samebody location as that used during exploratory ormanipulative motor actions involving the object Con-ceptual and linguistic representations are more likelyto encode the action as the salient characteristic ofthe object rather than the tactile experience (we sayldquoI threw the ballrdquo to describe the event not ldquothe balltouched my handrdquo) therefore somatotopic knowledgewas encoded in the proposed representation usingaction-based rather than somatosensory attributes(see Motor Components)

Temperature tactile texture and pain wereincluded as components of the representation aswas a component referring to the salience of objectweight Weight is perceived mainly via proprioceptiverepresentations and is a salient attribute of objectswith which we interact Temperature and weight aretwo more examples of scalar entities that may ormay not have a macroscopic topography in thebrain (Hendry et al 1999 Mazzola Faillenot BarralMauguiere amp Peyron 2012) That is although thereis evidence that these types of somatosensory infor-mation are distinguishable from each other no evi-dence has yet been presented for a scalartopography that separates for example hot fromcold representations Tactile texture can refer to avariety of physical properties we operationalized this

concept as a continuum from smooth to rough andthus the texture component is another scalar entityFor all three of these scalars we elected to representthe entire continuum as a single component Thatis the temperature component is intended tocapture the salience of temperature to a conceptregardless of whether the associated temperature ishot or cold

The published literature on conceptual processingof somatosensory information has focused almostexclusively on impaired knowledge of body parts inneurological patients (Coslett Saffran amp Schwoebel2002 Kemmerer amp Tranel 2008 Laiacona AllamanoLorenzi amp Capitani 2006 Schwoebel amp Coslett2005) though no definitive localization has emergedfrom these studies A number of functional imagingstudies suggest an overlap between networksinvolved in real pain perception and those involvedin empathic pain perception (perception of pain inothers) and perception of ldquosocial painrdquo (Eisenberger2012 Lamm et al 2011) suggesting that retrieval ofpain concepts involves a simulation process To ourknowledge no studies have yet examined the concep-tual representation of temperature tactile texture orweight

Finally object shape is an attribute learned partlythrough haptic experiences (Miqueacutee et al 2008) butthe degree to which haptic shape is learned indepen-dently from visual shape is unclear Some objects areseen but not felt (ie very large and very distantobjects) but the converse is rarely true at least insighted individuals It was therefore decided that thevisual shape query would capture knowledge aboutshape in both modalities and that a separate queryregarding extent of personal manipulation experience(see Motor Components) would capture the impor-tance of haptic shape relative to visual shape

Auditory components

The auditory cortex includes low-level areas tuned tofrequency (tonotopy) amplitude and spatial location(Schreiner amp Winer 2007) as well as higher levelsshowing specialization for auditory ldquoobjectsrdquo such asenvironmental sounds (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010)and speech sounds (Belin Zatorre Lafaille Ahad ampPike 2000 Liebenthal Binder Spitzer Possing ampMedler 2005) Corresponding auditory attributes ofthe proposed conceptual representation capture the

136 J R BINDER ET AL

degree to which a concept is associated with a low-pitched high-pitched loud (ie high amplitude)recognizable (ie having a characteristic auditoryform) or speech-like sound The attribute ldquolow inamplituderdquo was not included because it was con-sidered to be a salient attribute of relatively fewconceptsmdashthat is those that refer specifically to alow-amplitude variant (eg whisper) Concepts thatfocus on the absence of sound (eg silence quiet)are probably more accurately encoded as a nullvalue on the ldquoloudrdquo attribute as fMRI studies haveshown auditory regions where activity increases withsound intensity but not the converse (Roumlhl amp Uppen-kamp 2012)

A few studies of sound-related knowledge(Goldberg Perfetti amp Schneider 2006 Kellenbachet al 2001 Kiefer Sim Herrnberger Grothe ampHoenig 2008) reported activation in or near the pos-terior superior temporal sulcus (STS) an auditoryassociation region implicated in environmentalsound recognition (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010 J WLewis et al 2004) Trumpp et al (Trumpp KlieseHoenig Haarmeier amp Kiefer 2013) described apatient with a circumscribed lesion centred on theleft posterior STS who displayed a specific deficit(slower response times and higher error rate) invisual recognition of sound-related words All ofthese studies examined general environmentalsound knowledge nothing is yet known regardingthe conceptual representation of specific types ofauditory attributes like frequency or amplitude

Localization of sounds in space is an importantaspect of auditory perception yet auditory perceptionof space has little or no representation in languageindependent of (multimodal) spatial concepts thereare no concepts with components like ldquomakessounds from the leftrdquo because spatial relationshipsbetween sound sources and listeners are almostnever fixed or central to meaning Like shape attri-butes of concepts spatial attributes and spatial con-cepts are learned through strongly correlatedmultimodal experiences involving visual auditorytactile and motor processes In the proposed semanticrepresentation model spatial attributes of conceptsare represented by modality-independent spatialcomponents (see Spatial Components)

A final salient component of human auditoryexperience is the domain of music Music perceptionwhich includes processing of melody harmony and

rhythm elements in sound appears to engage rela-tively specialized right lateralized networks some ofwhich may also process nonverbal (prosodic)elements in speech (Zatorre Belin amp Penhune2002) Many words refer explicitly to musical phenom-ena (sing song melody harmony rhythm etc) or toentities (instruments performers performances) thatproduce musical sounds Therefore the model pro-posed here includes a component to capture theexperience of processing music

Gustatory and olfactory components

Gustatory and olfactory experiences are each rep-resented by a single attribute that captures the rel-evance of each type of experience to the conceptThese sensory systems do not show clear large-scaleorganization along any dimension and neuronswithin each system often respond to a broad spec-trum of items Both gustatory and olfactory conceptshave been linked with specific early sensory andhigher associative neural processors (Goldberg et al2006 Gonzaacutelez et al 2006)

Motor components

The motor components of the proposed conceptualrepresentation (Table 1) capture the degree to whicha concept is associated with actions involving particu-lar body parts The neural representation of motoraction verbs has been extensively studied withmany studies showing action-specific activation of ahigh-level network that includes the supramarginalgyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus (BinderDesai Conant amp Graves 2009 Desai Binder Conantamp Seidenberg 2009) There is also evidence for soma-totopic representation of action verb representation inprimary sensorimotor areas (Hauk Johnsrude ampPulvermuller 2004) though this claim is somewhatcontroversial (Postle McMahon Ashton Meredith ampde Zubicaray 2008) Object concepts associated withparticular actions also activate the action network(Binder et al 2009 Boronat et al 2005 Martin et al1995) and there is evidence for somatotopicallyspecific activation depending on which body part istypically used in the object interaction (CarotaMoseley amp Pulvermuumlller 2012) Following precedentin the neuroimaging literature (Carota et al 2012Hauk et al 2004 Tettamanti et al 2005) a three-

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 137

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 7: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

content of concepts regardless of the particular colourin question A concept like lemon that is reliably associ-ated with a particular colour is expected to activatethis neural processor to approximately the samedegree as a concept like coffee that is reliably associ-ated with a different colour

Visual motion perception involves a relativelyspecialized neural processor known as MT or V5 (Alb-right Desimone amp Gross 1984 Zeki 1974) Responsesin MT vary with motion velocity and degree of motioncoherence (Lingnau Ashida Wall amp Smith 2009 ReesFriston amp Koch 2000 Tootell et al 1995) Motion vel-ocity is relevant for our purposes as it is strongly rep-resented at a conceptual level For examplemovements may be relatively fast or slow in velocityand this scalar quantity is central to action conceptslike run dash zoom creep ooze walk and so on aswell as entity concepts like bullet jet hare snail tor-toise sloth and so on Movements may also have aspecific pattern or quality as illustrated for exampleby the large number of verbs that express manner ofmotion (turn bounce roll float spin twist etc) Weare not aware of any evidence for large-scale spatialorganization in the cortex according to type ormanner of motion therefore the proposed semanticrepresentation is designed to capture strength ofassociation with visually observable characteristicmotion in general regardless of specific type Theexception to this rule is the perception of biologicalmotion which has been linked with a neural processorthat is clearly distinct from MT located in the posteriorsuperior temporal sulcus (STS Grossman amp Blake2002 Pelphrey Morris Michelich Allison amp McCarthy2005) Biological motion contains higher order com-plexity that is characteristic of face and body partactions Perception of biological motion plays acentral role in understanding face and body gesturesand thus the affective and intentional state of otheragents (ie theory of mind Allison Puce amp McCarthy2000) Biological motion is thus one of several keyattributes distinguishing intentional agents (boy girlwoman man etc) from inanimate entities

Visual shape perception involves an extensiveregion of extrastriate cortex including a large lateralextrastriate area known as the lateral occipitalcomplex (Malach et al 1995) and an adjacent areaon the ventral occipitalndashtemporal surface known asVOT (Grill-Spector amp Malach 2004) In human func-tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies

these areas respond more strongly to images ofobjects than to images in which shape contours ofthe objects have been disrupted (ldquoscrambledrdquoimages) similar to response patterns observed inprimate ventral temporal neurons (Tanaka SaitoFukada amp Moriya 1991) Shape information is gener-ally the most important attribute of concrete objectconcepts and distinguishes concrete concepts froma range of abstract (eg affective social and cogni-tive) entities and event concepts Variation in the sal-ience of visual shape also occurs within the domainof concrete entities Shape is typically not a definingfeature of substance nouns (metal plastic water) butit has relevance for some substance-like mass nouns(butter coffee rice) Concrete objects also vary inshape complexity Animals for example tend tohave more complex shapes than plants or tools (Snod-grass amp Vanderwart 1980) We hypothesize that bothshape salience and shape complexity determine thedegree of activation in shape processing regionsduring concept retrieval

More focal areas within or adjacent to these shapeprocessing regions show preferential responses tohuman faces (Kanwisher McDermott amp Chun 1997Schwarzlose Baker amp Kanwisher 2005) and bodyparts (Downing Jiang Shuman amp Kanwisher 2001)A specialized mechanism for face perception is con-sistent with the central role of face recognition insocial interactions Visual perception of body partsprobably plays a crucial role in understandingobserved actions and mapping these onto internalrepresentations of the body schema during actionlearning These processors would be differentially acti-vated by concepts pertaining to faces (nose mouthportrait) and body parts (hand leg finger) As a firstapproximation we propose that both of these neuralprocessors are activated to some degree by all con-cepts referring to human beings which by necessityinclude a face and other human body parts Activationof the face processor may be minimal in the case ofconcepts that represent general human types androles (boy man nurse etc) as these concepts do notinclude information about any specific face whereasconcepts referencing specific individuals (brotherfather Einstein) might include specific facial featureinformation and therefore activate the face processorto a greater degree

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 135

parahippocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998Epstein Parker amp Feiler 2007) Although this processoris typically studied using visual stimuli and is oftenconsidered a high-level visual area it more likely inte-grates visual proprioceptive and perhaps auditoryinputs all of which provide correlated informationabout three-dimensional space Further discussion ofthis component of experience is provided in thesection below on Spatial Components (Table 2)

Somatosensory components

Somatosensory networks of the cortex represent bodylocation (somatotopic representation) body positionand joint force (proprioception) pain surface charac-teristics of objects (texture and temperature) andobject shape (Friebel Eickhoff amp Lotze 2011Hendry Hsiao amp Bushnell 1999 Lamm Decety ampSinger 2011 Longo Azanon amp Haggard 2010McGlone amp Reilly 2010 Medina amp Coslett 2010Serino amp Haggard 2010) Somatotopy is an inherentlymulti-modal phenomenon because the body locationtouched by a particular object is typically the samebody location as that used during exploratory ormanipulative motor actions involving the object Con-ceptual and linguistic representations are more likelyto encode the action as the salient characteristic ofthe object rather than the tactile experience (we sayldquoI threw the ballrdquo to describe the event not ldquothe balltouched my handrdquo) therefore somatotopic knowledgewas encoded in the proposed representation usingaction-based rather than somatosensory attributes(see Motor Components)

Temperature tactile texture and pain wereincluded as components of the representation aswas a component referring to the salience of objectweight Weight is perceived mainly via proprioceptiverepresentations and is a salient attribute of objectswith which we interact Temperature and weight aretwo more examples of scalar entities that may ormay not have a macroscopic topography in thebrain (Hendry et al 1999 Mazzola Faillenot BarralMauguiere amp Peyron 2012) That is although thereis evidence that these types of somatosensory infor-mation are distinguishable from each other no evi-dence has yet been presented for a scalartopography that separates for example hot fromcold representations Tactile texture can refer to avariety of physical properties we operationalized this

concept as a continuum from smooth to rough andthus the texture component is another scalar entityFor all three of these scalars we elected to representthe entire continuum as a single component Thatis the temperature component is intended tocapture the salience of temperature to a conceptregardless of whether the associated temperature ishot or cold

The published literature on conceptual processingof somatosensory information has focused almostexclusively on impaired knowledge of body parts inneurological patients (Coslett Saffran amp Schwoebel2002 Kemmerer amp Tranel 2008 Laiacona AllamanoLorenzi amp Capitani 2006 Schwoebel amp Coslett2005) though no definitive localization has emergedfrom these studies A number of functional imagingstudies suggest an overlap between networksinvolved in real pain perception and those involvedin empathic pain perception (perception of pain inothers) and perception of ldquosocial painrdquo (Eisenberger2012 Lamm et al 2011) suggesting that retrieval ofpain concepts involves a simulation process To ourknowledge no studies have yet examined the concep-tual representation of temperature tactile texture orweight

Finally object shape is an attribute learned partlythrough haptic experiences (Miqueacutee et al 2008) butthe degree to which haptic shape is learned indepen-dently from visual shape is unclear Some objects areseen but not felt (ie very large and very distantobjects) but the converse is rarely true at least insighted individuals It was therefore decided that thevisual shape query would capture knowledge aboutshape in both modalities and that a separate queryregarding extent of personal manipulation experience(see Motor Components) would capture the impor-tance of haptic shape relative to visual shape

Auditory components

The auditory cortex includes low-level areas tuned tofrequency (tonotopy) amplitude and spatial location(Schreiner amp Winer 2007) as well as higher levelsshowing specialization for auditory ldquoobjectsrdquo such asenvironmental sounds (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010)and speech sounds (Belin Zatorre Lafaille Ahad ampPike 2000 Liebenthal Binder Spitzer Possing ampMedler 2005) Corresponding auditory attributes ofthe proposed conceptual representation capture the

136 J R BINDER ET AL

degree to which a concept is associated with a low-pitched high-pitched loud (ie high amplitude)recognizable (ie having a characteristic auditoryform) or speech-like sound The attribute ldquolow inamplituderdquo was not included because it was con-sidered to be a salient attribute of relatively fewconceptsmdashthat is those that refer specifically to alow-amplitude variant (eg whisper) Concepts thatfocus on the absence of sound (eg silence quiet)are probably more accurately encoded as a nullvalue on the ldquoloudrdquo attribute as fMRI studies haveshown auditory regions where activity increases withsound intensity but not the converse (Roumlhl amp Uppen-kamp 2012)

A few studies of sound-related knowledge(Goldberg Perfetti amp Schneider 2006 Kellenbachet al 2001 Kiefer Sim Herrnberger Grothe ampHoenig 2008) reported activation in or near the pos-terior superior temporal sulcus (STS) an auditoryassociation region implicated in environmentalsound recognition (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010 J WLewis et al 2004) Trumpp et al (Trumpp KlieseHoenig Haarmeier amp Kiefer 2013) described apatient with a circumscribed lesion centred on theleft posterior STS who displayed a specific deficit(slower response times and higher error rate) invisual recognition of sound-related words All ofthese studies examined general environmentalsound knowledge nothing is yet known regardingthe conceptual representation of specific types ofauditory attributes like frequency or amplitude

Localization of sounds in space is an importantaspect of auditory perception yet auditory perceptionof space has little or no representation in languageindependent of (multimodal) spatial concepts thereare no concepts with components like ldquomakessounds from the leftrdquo because spatial relationshipsbetween sound sources and listeners are almostnever fixed or central to meaning Like shape attri-butes of concepts spatial attributes and spatial con-cepts are learned through strongly correlatedmultimodal experiences involving visual auditorytactile and motor processes In the proposed semanticrepresentation model spatial attributes of conceptsare represented by modality-independent spatialcomponents (see Spatial Components)

A final salient component of human auditoryexperience is the domain of music Music perceptionwhich includes processing of melody harmony and

rhythm elements in sound appears to engage rela-tively specialized right lateralized networks some ofwhich may also process nonverbal (prosodic)elements in speech (Zatorre Belin amp Penhune2002) Many words refer explicitly to musical phenom-ena (sing song melody harmony rhythm etc) or toentities (instruments performers performances) thatproduce musical sounds Therefore the model pro-posed here includes a component to capture theexperience of processing music

Gustatory and olfactory components

Gustatory and olfactory experiences are each rep-resented by a single attribute that captures the rel-evance of each type of experience to the conceptThese sensory systems do not show clear large-scaleorganization along any dimension and neuronswithin each system often respond to a broad spec-trum of items Both gustatory and olfactory conceptshave been linked with specific early sensory andhigher associative neural processors (Goldberg et al2006 Gonzaacutelez et al 2006)

Motor components

The motor components of the proposed conceptualrepresentation (Table 1) capture the degree to whicha concept is associated with actions involving particu-lar body parts The neural representation of motoraction verbs has been extensively studied withmany studies showing action-specific activation of ahigh-level network that includes the supramarginalgyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus (BinderDesai Conant amp Graves 2009 Desai Binder Conantamp Seidenberg 2009) There is also evidence for soma-totopic representation of action verb representation inprimary sensorimotor areas (Hauk Johnsrude ampPulvermuller 2004) though this claim is somewhatcontroversial (Postle McMahon Ashton Meredith ampde Zubicaray 2008) Object concepts associated withparticular actions also activate the action network(Binder et al 2009 Boronat et al 2005 Martin et al1995) and there is evidence for somatotopicallyspecific activation depending on which body part istypically used in the object interaction (CarotaMoseley amp Pulvermuumlller 2012) Following precedentin the neuroimaging literature (Carota et al 2012Hauk et al 2004 Tettamanti et al 2005) a three-

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 137

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 8: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

parahippocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998Epstein Parker amp Feiler 2007) Although this processoris typically studied using visual stimuli and is oftenconsidered a high-level visual area it more likely inte-grates visual proprioceptive and perhaps auditoryinputs all of which provide correlated informationabout three-dimensional space Further discussion ofthis component of experience is provided in thesection below on Spatial Components (Table 2)

Somatosensory components

Somatosensory networks of the cortex represent bodylocation (somatotopic representation) body positionand joint force (proprioception) pain surface charac-teristics of objects (texture and temperature) andobject shape (Friebel Eickhoff amp Lotze 2011Hendry Hsiao amp Bushnell 1999 Lamm Decety ampSinger 2011 Longo Azanon amp Haggard 2010McGlone amp Reilly 2010 Medina amp Coslett 2010Serino amp Haggard 2010) Somatotopy is an inherentlymulti-modal phenomenon because the body locationtouched by a particular object is typically the samebody location as that used during exploratory ormanipulative motor actions involving the object Con-ceptual and linguistic representations are more likelyto encode the action as the salient characteristic ofthe object rather than the tactile experience (we sayldquoI threw the ballrdquo to describe the event not ldquothe balltouched my handrdquo) therefore somatotopic knowledgewas encoded in the proposed representation usingaction-based rather than somatosensory attributes(see Motor Components)

Temperature tactile texture and pain wereincluded as components of the representation aswas a component referring to the salience of objectweight Weight is perceived mainly via proprioceptiverepresentations and is a salient attribute of objectswith which we interact Temperature and weight aretwo more examples of scalar entities that may ormay not have a macroscopic topography in thebrain (Hendry et al 1999 Mazzola Faillenot BarralMauguiere amp Peyron 2012) That is although thereis evidence that these types of somatosensory infor-mation are distinguishable from each other no evi-dence has yet been presented for a scalartopography that separates for example hot fromcold representations Tactile texture can refer to avariety of physical properties we operationalized this

concept as a continuum from smooth to rough andthus the texture component is another scalar entityFor all three of these scalars we elected to representthe entire continuum as a single component Thatis the temperature component is intended tocapture the salience of temperature to a conceptregardless of whether the associated temperature ishot or cold

The published literature on conceptual processingof somatosensory information has focused almostexclusively on impaired knowledge of body parts inneurological patients (Coslett Saffran amp Schwoebel2002 Kemmerer amp Tranel 2008 Laiacona AllamanoLorenzi amp Capitani 2006 Schwoebel amp Coslett2005) though no definitive localization has emergedfrom these studies A number of functional imagingstudies suggest an overlap between networksinvolved in real pain perception and those involvedin empathic pain perception (perception of pain inothers) and perception of ldquosocial painrdquo (Eisenberger2012 Lamm et al 2011) suggesting that retrieval ofpain concepts involves a simulation process To ourknowledge no studies have yet examined the concep-tual representation of temperature tactile texture orweight

Finally object shape is an attribute learned partlythrough haptic experiences (Miqueacutee et al 2008) butthe degree to which haptic shape is learned indepen-dently from visual shape is unclear Some objects areseen but not felt (ie very large and very distantobjects) but the converse is rarely true at least insighted individuals It was therefore decided that thevisual shape query would capture knowledge aboutshape in both modalities and that a separate queryregarding extent of personal manipulation experience(see Motor Components) would capture the impor-tance of haptic shape relative to visual shape

Auditory components

The auditory cortex includes low-level areas tuned tofrequency (tonotopy) amplitude and spatial location(Schreiner amp Winer 2007) as well as higher levelsshowing specialization for auditory ldquoobjectsrdquo such asenvironmental sounds (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010)and speech sounds (Belin Zatorre Lafaille Ahad ampPike 2000 Liebenthal Binder Spitzer Possing ampMedler 2005) Corresponding auditory attributes ofthe proposed conceptual representation capture the

136 J R BINDER ET AL

degree to which a concept is associated with a low-pitched high-pitched loud (ie high amplitude)recognizable (ie having a characteristic auditoryform) or speech-like sound The attribute ldquolow inamplituderdquo was not included because it was con-sidered to be a salient attribute of relatively fewconceptsmdashthat is those that refer specifically to alow-amplitude variant (eg whisper) Concepts thatfocus on the absence of sound (eg silence quiet)are probably more accurately encoded as a nullvalue on the ldquoloudrdquo attribute as fMRI studies haveshown auditory regions where activity increases withsound intensity but not the converse (Roumlhl amp Uppen-kamp 2012)

A few studies of sound-related knowledge(Goldberg Perfetti amp Schneider 2006 Kellenbachet al 2001 Kiefer Sim Herrnberger Grothe ampHoenig 2008) reported activation in or near the pos-terior superior temporal sulcus (STS) an auditoryassociation region implicated in environmentalsound recognition (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010 J WLewis et al 2004) Trumpp et al (Trumpp KlieseHoenig Haarmeier amp Kiefer 2013) described apatient with a circumscribed lesion centred on theleft posterior STS who displayed a specific deficit(slower response times and higher error rate) invisual recognition of sound-related words All ofthese studies examined general environmentalsound knowledge nothing is yet known regardingthe conceptual representation of specific types ofauditory attributes like frequency or amplitude

Localization of sounds in space is an importantaspect of auditory perception yet auditory perceptionof space has little or no representation in languageindependent of (multimodal) spatial concepts thereare no concepts with components like ldquomakessounds from the leftrdquo because spatial relationshipsbetween sound sources and listeners are almostnever fixed or central to meaning Like shape attri-butes of concepts spatial attributes and spatial con-cepts are learned through strongly correlatedmultimodal experiences involving visual auditorytactile and motor processes In the proposed semanticrepresentation model spatial attributes of conceptsare represented by modality-independent spatialcomponents (see Spatial Components)

A final salient component of human auditoryexperience is the domain of music Music perceptionwhich includes processing of melody harmony and

rhythm elements in sound appears to engage rela-tively specialized right lateralized networks some ofwhich may also process nonverbal (prosodic)elements in speech (Zatorre Belin amp Penhune2002) Many words refer explicitly to musical phenom-ena (sing song melody harmony rhythm etc) or toentities (instruments performers performances) thatproduce musical sounds Therefore the model pro-posed here includes a component to capture theexperience of processing music

Gustatory and olfactory components

Gustatory and olfactory experiences are each rep-resented by a single attribute that captures the rel-evance of each type of experience to the conceptThese sensory systems do not show clear large-scaleorganization along any dimension and neuronswithin each system often respond to a broad spec-trum of items Both gustatory and olfactory conceptshave been linked with specific early sensory andhigher associative neural processors (Goldberg et al2006 Gonzaacutelez et al 2006)

Motor components

The motor components of the proposed conceptualrepresentation (Table 1) capture the degree to whicha concept is associated with actions involving particu-lar body parts The neural representation of motoraction verbs has been extensively studied withmany studies showing action-specific activation of ahigh-level network that includes the supramarginalgyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus (BinderDesai Conant amp Graves 2009 Desai Binder Conantamp Seidenberg 2009) There is also evidence for soma-totopic representation of action verb representation inprimary sensorimotor areas (Hauk Johnsrude ampPulvermuller 2004) though this claim is somewhatcontroversial (Postle McMahon Ashton Meredith ampde Zubicaray 2008) Object concepts associated withparticular actions also activate the action network(Binder et al 2009 Boronat et al 2005 Martin et al1995) and there is evidence for somatotopicallyspecific activation depending on which body part istypically used in the object interaction (CarotaMoseley amp Pulvermuumlller 2012) Following precedentin the neuroimaging literature (Carota et al 2012Hauk et al 2004 Tettamanti et al 2005) a three-

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 137

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 9: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

degree to which a concept is associated with a low-pitched high-pitched loud (ie high amplitude)recognizable (ie having a characteristic auditoryform) or speech-like sound The attribute ldquolow inamplituderdquo was not included because it was con-sidered to be a salient attribute of relatively fewconceptsmdashthat is those that refer specifically to alow-amplitude variant (eg whisper) Concepts thatfocus on the absence of sound (eg silence quiet)are probably more accurately encoded as a nullvalue on the ldquoloudrdquo attribute as fMRI studies haveshown auditory regions where activity increases withsound intensity but not the converse (Roumlhl amp Uppen-kamp 2012)

A few studies of sound-related knowledge(Goldberg Perfetti amp Schneider 2006 Kellenbachet al 2001 Kiefer Sim Herrnberger Grothe ampHoenig 2008) reported activation in or near the pos-terior superior temporal sulcus (STS) an auditoryassociation region implicated in environmentalsound recognition (Leaver amp Rauschecker 2010 J WLewis et al 2004) Trumpp et al (Trumpp KlieseHoenig Haarmeier amp Kiefer 2013) described apatient with a circumscribed lesion centred on theleft posterior STS who displayed a specific deficit(slower response times and higher error rate) invisual recognition of sound-related words All ofthese studies examined general environmentalsound knowledge nothing is yet known regardingthe conceptual representation of specific types ofauditory attributes like frequency or amplitude

Localization of sounds in space is an importantaspect of auditory perception yet auditory perceptionof space has little or no representation in languageindependent of (multimodal) spatial concepts thereare no concepts with components like ldquomakessounds from the leftrdquo because spatial relationshipsbetween sound sources and listeners are almostnever fixed or central to meaning Like shape attri-butes of concepts spatial attributes and spatial con-cepts are learned through strongly correlatedmultimodal experiences involving visual auditorytactile and motor processes In the proposed semanticrepresentation model spatial attributes of conceptsare represented by modality-independent spatialcomponents (see Spatial Components)

A final salient component of human auditoryexperience is the domain of music Music perceptionwhich includes processing of melody harmony and

rhythm elements in sound appears to engage rela-tively specialized right lateralized networks some ofwhich may also process nonverbal (prosodic)elements in speech (Zatorre Belin amp Penhune2002) Many words refer explicitly to musical phenom-ena (sing song melody harmony rhythm etc) or toentities (instruments performers performances) thatproduce musical sounds Therefore the model pro-posed here includes a component to capture theexperience of processing music

Gustatory and olfactory components

Gustatory and olfactory experiences are each rep-resented by a single attribute that captures the rel-evance of each type of experience to the conceptThese sensory systems do not show clear large-scaleorganization along any dimension and neuronswithin each system often respond to a broad spec-trum of items Both gustatory and olfactory conceptshave been linked with specific early sensory andhigher associative neural processors (Goldberg et al2006 Gonzaacutelez et al 2006)

Motor components

The motor components of the proposed conceptualrepresentation (Table 1) capture the degree to whicha concept is associated with actions involving particu-lar body parts The neural representation of motoraction verbs has been extensively studied withmany studies showing action-specific activation of ahigh-level network that includes the supramarginalgyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus (BinderDesai Conant amp Graves 2009 Desai Binder Conantamp Seidenberg 2009) There is also evidence for soma-totopic representation of action verb representation inprimary sensorimotor areas (Hauk Johnsrude ampPulvermuller 2004) though this claim is somewhatcontroversial (Postle McMahon Ashton Meredith ampde Zubicaray 2008) Object concepts associated withparticular actions also activate the action network(Binder et al 2009 Boronat et al 2005 Martin et al1995) and there is evidence for somatotopicallyspecific activation depending on which body part istypically used in the object interaction (CarotaMoseley amp Pulvermuumlller 2012) Following precedentin the neuroimaging literature (Carota et al 2012Hauk et al 2004 Tettamanti et al 2005) a three-

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 137

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 10: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

way partition into head-related (face mouth ortongue) upper limb (arm hand or fingers) andlower limb (leg or foot) actions was used to assesssomatotopic organization of action concepts A refer-ence to eye actions was felt to be confusingbecause all visual experiences involve the eyes inthis sense any visible object could be construed asldquoassociated with action involving the eyesrdquo

Finally the extent to which action attributes arerepresented in action networks may be partly deter-mined by personal experience (Calvo-Merino GlaserGrezes Passingham amp Haggard 2005 JaumlnckeKoeneke Hoppe Rominger amp Haumlnggi 2009) Mostpeople would rate ldquopianordquo as strongly associatedwith upper limb actions but most people also haveno personal experience with these specific actionsBased on prior research it is likely that the action rep-resentation of the concept ldquopianordquo is more extensivein the case of pianists Therefore a separate com-ponent (called ldquopracticerdquo) was created to capture thelevel of personal experience the rater has with per-forming the actions associated with the concept

Spatial components

Spatial and other more abstract components are listedin Table 2 Neural systems that encode aspects ofspatial experience have been investigated at manylevels (Andersen Snyder Bradley amp Xing 1997Burgess 2008 Kemmerer 2006 Kranjec CardilloSchmidt Lehet amp Chatterjee 2012 Woods et al2014) As mentioned above the acquisition of basicspatial concepts and knowledge of spatial attributesof concepts generally involves correlated multimodalinputs The most obvious spatial content in languageis the set of relations expressed by prepositionalphrases which have been a major focus of study in lin-guistics and semantic theory (Landau amp Jackendoff1993 Talmy 1983) Lesion correlation and neuroima-ging studies have implicated various parietal regionsparticularly the left inferior parietal lobe in the proces-sing of spatial prepositions and depicted locativerelations (Amorapanth Widick amp Chatterjee 2010Noordzij Neggers Ramsey amp Postma 2008 Tranel ampKemmerer 2004 Wu Waller amp Chatterjee 2007) Pre-positional phrases appear to express most if not all ofthe explicit relational spatial content that occurs inEnglish a semantic component targeting this type ofcontent therefore appears unnecessary (ie the set

of words with high values on this component wouldbe identical to the set of spatial prepositions) Thespatial components of the proposed representationmodel focus instead on other types of spatial infor-mation found in nouns verbs and adjectives

Perception of three-dimensional spaces involves aspecialized neural processor in the posterior parahip-pocampal region (Epstein amp Kanwisher 1998 Epsteinet al 2007) The perception of three-dimensionalspace is based largely on visual input but also involvescorrelated proprioceptive information wheneverthree-dimensional space is experienced via move-ment of the body through space Spatial localizationin the auditory modality also probably contributes tothe experience of three-dimensional space Some con-cepts refer directly to interior spaces such as kitchenfoyer bathroom classroom and so on and seem toinclude information about general three-dimensionalsize and layout as do concepts about types of build-ings (library school hospital etc) More generallysuch concepts include the knowledge that they canbe physically entered navigated and exited whichmakes possible a range of conceptual combinations(entered the kitchen ran through the hall went out ofthe library etc) that are not possible for objectslacking large interior spaces (entered the chair) Thesalient property of concepts that determineswhether they activate a three-dimensional represen-tation of space is the degree to which they evoke aparticular ldquoscenerdquomdashthat is an array of objects in athree-dimensional space either by direct referenceor by association Space names (kitchen etc) andbuilding names (library etc) refer directly to three-dimensional scenes Many other object conceptsevoke mental representations of scenes by virtue ofthematic relations such as the evocation of kitchenby oven An object concept like chair would probablyfail to evoke such a representation as chairs are notlinked thematically with any particular scene Thusthere is a graded degree of mental representation ofthree-dimensional space evoked by different con-cepts which we hypothesize results in a correspond-ing variation in activation of the three-dimensionalspace neural processor

Large-scale (topographical) spatial information iscritical for navigation in the environment Entitiesthat are large and have a fixed location such as geo-logical formations and buildings can serve as land-marks within a mental map of the environment We

138 J R BINDER ET AL

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 11: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

hypothesize that such concepts are partly encoded inenvironmental navigation systems located in the hip-pocampal parahippocampal and posterior cingulategyri (Burgess 2008 Maguire Frith Burgess Donnettamp OrsquoKeefe 1998 Wallentin Oslashstergaarda LundOslashstergaard amp Roepstorff 2005) and we assess the sal-ience of this ldquotopographicalrdquo attribute by inquiring towhat degree the concept in question could serve asa landmark on a map

Spatial cognition also includes spatial aspects ofmotion particularly direction or path of motionthrough space (location change) Some verbs thatdenote location change refer directly to a directionof motion (ascend climb descend rise fall) or type ofpath (arc bounce circle jump launch orbit swerve)Others imply a horizontal path of motion parallel tothe ground (run walk crawl swim) while othersrefer to paths toward or away from an observer(arrive depart leave return) Some entities are associ-ated with a particular type of path through space(bird butterfly car rocket satellite snake) Visualmotion area MT features a columnar spatial organiz-ation of neurons according to preferred axis ofmotion however this organization is at a relativelymicroscopic scale such that the full 180deg of axis ofmotion is represented in 500 microm of cortex (Albrightet al 1984) In human fMRI studies perception ofthe spatial aspects of motion has been linked with aneural processor in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) andsurrounding dorsal visual stream (Culham et al1998 Wu Morganti amp Chatterjee 2008)

A final aspect of spatial cognition relates to periper-sonal proximity Intuitively the coding of peripersonalproximity is a central aspect of action representationand performance threat detection and resourceacquisition all of which are neural processes criticalfor survival Evidence from both animal and humanstudies suggests that the representation of periperso-nal space involves somewhat specialized neural mech-anisms (Graziano Yap amp Gross 1994 Makin Holmes ampZohary 2007) We hypothesize that these systems alsopartly encode the meaning of concepts that relate toperipersonal spacemdashthat is objects that are typicallywithin easy reach and actions performed on theseobjects Given the importance of peripersonal spacein action and object use two further componentswere included to explore the relevance of movementaway from or out of versus toward or into the selfSome action verbs (give punch tell throw) indicate

movement or relocation of something away fromthe self whereas others (acquire catch receive eat)indicate movement or relocation toward the selfThis distinction may also modulate the representationof objects that characteristically move toward or awayfrom the self (Rueschemeyer Pfeiffer amp Bekkering2010)

Mathematical cognition particularly the represen-tation of numerosity is a somewhat specialized infor-mation processing domain with well-documentedneural correlates (Dehaene 1997 Harvey KleinPetridou amp Dumoulin 2013 Piazza Izard PinelBihan amp Dehaene 2004) In addition to numbernames which refer directly to numerical quantitiesmany words are associated with the concept ofnumerical quantity (amount number quantity) orwith particular numbers (dime hand egg) Althoughnot a spatial process per se since it does not typicallyinvolve three dimensions numerosity processingseems to depend on directional vector represen-tations similar to those underlying spatial cognitionThus a component was included to capture associ-ation with numerical quantities and was consideredloosely part of the spatial domain

Temporal and causal components

Event concepts include several distinct types of infor-mation Temporal information pertains to the durationand temporal order of events Some types of eventhave a typical duration in which case the durationmay be a salient attribute of the event (breakfastlecture movie shower) Some event-like conceptsrefer specifically to durations (minute hour dayweek) Typical durations may be very short (blinkflash pop sneeze) or relatively long (childhoodcollege life war) Events of a particular type may alsorecur at particular times and thus occupy a character-istic location within the temporal sequence of eventsExamples of this phenomenon include recurring dailyevents (shower breakfast commute lunch dinner)recurring weekly or monthly events (Mondayweekend payday) recurring annual events (holidaysand other cultural events seasons and weatherphenomena) and concepts associated with particularphases of life (infancy childhood college marriageretirement) The neural correlates of these types ofinformation are not yet clear (Ferstl Rinck amp vonCramon 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Grondin

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 139

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 12: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,

2010 Kranjec et al 2012) Because time seems to bepervasively conceived of in spatial terms (Evans2004 Kranjec amp Chatterjee 2010 Lakoff amp Johnson1980) it is likely that temporal concepts share theirneural representation at least in part with spatial con-cepts Components of the proposed conceptual rep-resentation model are designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a charac-teristic duration the degree to which a concept isassociated with a long or a short duration and thedegree to which a concept is associated with a particu-lar or predictable point in time

Causal information pertains to cause and effectrelationships between concepts Events and situationsmay have a salient precipitating cause (infection killlaugh spill) or they may occur without an immediateapparent cause (eg some natural phenomenarandom occurrences) Events may or may not havehighly likely consequences Imaging evidencesuggests involvement of several inferior parietal andtemporal regions in low-level perception of causality(Blos Chatterjee Kircher amp Straube 2012 Fonlupt2003 Fugelsang Roser Corballis Gazzaniga ampDunbar 2005 Woods et al 2014) and a few studiesof causal processing at the conceptual level implicatethese and other areas (Kranjec et al 2012 Satputeet al 2005) The proposed conceptual representationmodel includes components designed to capture thedegree to which a concept is associated with a clearpreceding cause and the degree to which an eventor action has probable consequences

Social components

Theory of mind (TOM) refers to the ability to infer orpredict mental states and mental processes in othervolitional beings (typically though not exclusivelyother people) We hypothesize that the brainsystems underlying this ability are involved whenencoding the meaning of words that refer to inten-tional entities or actions because learning these con-cepts is frequently associated with TOM processingEssentially this attribute captures the semanticfeature of intentionality In addition to the query per-taining to events with social interactions as well as thequery regarding mental activities or events weincluded an object-directed query assessing thedegree to which a thing has human or human-likeintentions plans or goals and a corresponding verb

query assessing the degree to which an action reflectshuman intentions plans or goals The underlyinghypothesis is that such concepts which mostly referto people in particular roles and the actions theytake are partly understood by engaging a TOMprocess

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in TOM These regionsmost consistently include the medial prefrontalcortex posterior cingulateprecuneus and the tem-poroparietal junction (Schilbach et al 2012 SchurzRadua Aichhorn Richlan amp Perner 2014 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014) withseveral studies also implicating the anterior temporallobes (Ross amp Olson 2010 Schilbach et al 2012Schurz et al 2014) The temporoparietal junction(TPJ) is an area of particular focus in the TOM literaturebut it is not a consistently defined region and mayencompass parts of the posterior superior temporalgyrussulcus supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus Collapsing across tasks TOM or intentionalityis frequently associated with significant activation inthe angular gyri (Carter amp Huettel 2013 Schurz et al2014) but some variability in the localization of peakactivation within the TPJ is seen across differentTOM tasks (Schurz et al 2014) In addition whilesome studies have shown right lateralization of acti-vation in the TPJ (Saxe amp Wexler 2005) severalmeta-analyses have suggested bilateral involvement(Schurz et al 2014 Spreng Mar amp Kim 2009 VanOverwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance 2014)

Another aspect of social cognition likely to have aneurobiological correlate is the representation of theself There is evidence to suggest that informationthat pertains to the self may be processed differentlyfrom information that is not considered to be self-related Some behavioural studies have suggestedthat people show better recall (Rogers Kuiper ampKirker 1977) and faster response times (Kuiper ampRogers 1979) when information is relevant to theself a phenomenon known as the self-referenceeffect Neuroimaging studies have typically implicatedcortical midline structures in the processing of self-referential information particularly the medial pre-frontal and parietal cortices (Araujo Kaplan ampDamasio 2013 Northoff et al 2006) While both self-and other-judgments activate these midline regionsgreater activation of medial prefrontal cortex for self-trait judgments than for other-trait judgments has

140 J R BINDER ET AL

been reported with the reverse pattern seen in medialparietal regions (Araujo et al 2013) In addition thereis evidence for a ventral-to-dorsal spatial gradientwithin medial prefrontal cortex for self- relative toother-judgments (Denny Kober Wager amp Ochsner2012) Preferential activation of left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex and left insula for self-judgments andof the bilateral temporoparietal junction and cuneusfor other-judgments has also been seen (Dennyet al 2012) The relevant query for this attributeassesses the degree to which a target noun isldquorelated to your own view of yourselfrdquo or the degreeto which a target verb is ldquoan action or activity that istypical of you something you commonly do andidentify withrdquo

A third aspect of social cognition for which aspecific neurobiological correlate is likely is thedomain of communication Communication whetherby language or nonverbal means is the basis for allsocial interaction and many noun (eg speech bookmeeting) and verb (eg speak read meet) conceptsare closely associated with the act of communicatingWe hypothesize that comprehension of such items isassociated with relative activation of language net-works (particularly general lexical retrieval syntacticphonological and speech articulation components)and gestural communication systems compared toconcepts that do not reference communication orcommunicative acts

Cognition component

A central premise of the current approach is that allaspects of experience can contribute to conceptacquisition and therefore concept composition Forself-aware human beings purely cognitive eventsand states certainly comprise one aspect of experi-ence When we ldquohave a thoughtrdquo ldquocome up with anideardquo ldquosolve a problemrdquo or ldquoconsider a factrdquo weexperience these phenomena as events that are noless real than sensory or motor events Many so-called abstract concepts refer directly to cognitiveevents and states (decide judge recall think) or tothe mental ldquoproductsrdquo of cognition (idea memoryopinion thought) We propose that such conceptsare learned in large part by generalization acrossthese cognitive experiences in exactly the same wayas concrete concepts are learned through generaliz-ation across perceptual and motor experiences

Domain-general cognitive operations have beenthe subject of a very large number of neuroimagingstudies many of which have attempted to fractionatethese operations into categories such as ldquoworkingmemoryrdquo ldquodecisionrdquo ldquoselectionrdquo ldquoreasoningrdquo ldquoconflictsuppressionrdquo ldquoset maintenancerdquo and so on No clearanatomical divisions have arisen from this workhowever and the consensus view is that there is alargely shared bilateral network for what are variouslycalled ldquoworking memoryrdquo ldquocognitive controlrdquo orldquoexecutiverdquo processes involving portions of theinferior and middle frontal gyri (ldquodorsolateral prefron-tal cortexrdquo) mid-anterior cingulate gyrus precentralsulcus anterior insula and perhaps dorsal regions ofthe parietal lobe (Badre amp DrsquoEsposito 2007 DerrfussBrass Neumann amp von Cramon 2005 Duncan ampOwen 2000 Nyberg et al 2003) We hypothesizethat retrieval of concepts that refer to cognitivephenomena involves a partial simulation of thesephenomena within this cognitive control networkanalogous to the partial activation of sensory andmotor systems by object and action concepts

Emotion and evaluation components

There is strong evidence for the involvement of aspecific network of regions in affective processing ingeneral These regions principally include the orbito-frontal cortex dorsomedial prefrontal cortex anteriorcingulate gyri (subgenual pregenual and dorsal)inferior frontal gyri anterior temporal lobes amygdalaand anterior insula (Etkin Egner amp Kalisch 2011Hamann 2012 Kober et al 2008 Lindquist WagerKober Bliss-Moreau amp Barrett 2012 Phan WagerTaylor amp Liberzon 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) Acti-vation in these regions can be modulated by theemotional content of words and text (Chow et al2013 Cunningham Raye amp Johnson 2004 Ferstlet al 2005 Ferstl amp von Cramon 2007 Kuchinkeet al 2005 Nakic Smith Busis Vythilingam amp Blair2006) However the nature of individual affectivestates has long been the subject of debate One ofthe primary families of theories regarding emotionare the dimensional accounts which propose thataffective states arise from combinations of a smallnumber of more fundamental dimensions most com-monly valence (hedonic tone) and arousal (Russell1980) In current psychological construction accountsthis input is then interpreted as a particular emotion

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 141

using one or more additional cognitive processessuch as appraisal of the stimulus or the retrieval ofmemories of previous experiences or semantic knowl-edge (Barrett 2006 Lindquist Siegel Quigley ampBarrett 2013 Posner Russell amp Peterson 2005)Another highly influential family of theories character-izes affective states as discrete emotions each ofwhich have consistent and discriminable patterns ofphysiological responses facial expressions andneural correlates Basic emotions are a subset of dis-crete emotions that are considered to be biologicallypredetermined and culturally universal (Hamann2012 Lench Flores amp Bench 2011 Vytal amp Hamann2010) although they may still be somewhat influ-enced by experience (Tracy amp Randles 2011) Themost frequently proposed set of basic emotions arethose of Ekman (Ekman 1992) which include angerfear disgust sadness happiness and surprise

There is substantial neuroimaging support for dis-sociable dimensions of valence and arousal withinindividual studies however the specific regionsassociated with the two dimensions or their endpointshave been inconsistent and sometimes contradictoryacross studies (Anders Eippert Weiskopf amp Veit2008 Anders Lotze Erb Grodd amp Birbaumer 2004Colibazzi et al 2010 Gerber et al 2008 P A LewisCritchley Rotshtein amp Dolan 2007 Nielen et al2009 Posner et al 2009 Wilson-Mendenhall Barrettamp Barsalou 2013) With regard to the discreteemotion model meta-analyses have suggested differ-ential activation patterns in individual regions associ-ated with basic emotions (Lindquist et al 2012Phan et al 2002 Vytal amp Hamann 2010) but there isa lack of specificity Individual regions are generallyassociated with more than one emotion andemotions are typically associated with more thanone region (Lindquist et al 2012 Vytal amp Hamann2010) Overall current evidence is not consistentwith a one-to-one mapping between individual brainregions and either specific emotions or dimensionshowever the possibility of spatially overlapping butdiscriminable networks remains open Importantlystudies of emotion perception or experience usingmultivariate pattern classifiers are providing emergingevidence for distinct patterns of neural activity associ-ated with both discrete emotions (Kassam MarkeyCherkassky Loewenstein amp Just 2013 Kragel ampLaBar 2014 Peelen Atkinson amp Vuilleumier 2010Said Moore Engell amp Haxby 2010) and specific

combinations of valence and arousal (BaucomWedell Wang Blitzer amp Shinkareva 2012)

The semantic representation model proposed hereincludes separate components reflecting the degree ofassociation of a target concept with each of Ekmanrsquosbasic emotions The representation also includes com-ponents corresponding to the two dimensions of thecircumplex model (valence and arousal) There is evi-dence that each end of the valence continuum mayhave a distinctive neural instantiation (Anders et al2008 Anders et al 2004 Colibazzi et al 2010 Posneret al 2009) and therefore separate components wereincluded for pleasantness and unpleasantness

Drive components

Drives are central associations of many concepts andare conceptualized here following Mazlow (Mazlow1943) as needs that must be filled to reach homeosta-sis or enable growth They include physiological needs(food restsleep sex emotional expression) securitysocial contact approval and self-development Driveexperiences are closely related to emotions whichare produced whenever needs are fulfilled or fru-strated and to the construct of reward conceptual-ized as the brain response to a resolved or satisfieddrive Here we use the term ldquodriverdquo synonymouslywith terms such as ldquoreward predictionrdquo and ldquorewardanticipationrdquo Extensive evidence links the ventralstriatum orbital frontal cortex and ventromedial pre-frontal cortex to processing of drive and reward states(Diekhof Kaps Falkai amp Gruber 2012 Levy amp Glimcher2012 Peters amp Buumlchel 2010 Rolls amp Grabenhorst2008) The proposed semantic components representthe degree of general motivation associated with thetarget concept and the degree to which the conceptitself refers to a basic need

Attention components

Attention is a basic process central to information pro-cessing but is not usually thought of as a type of infor-mation It is possible however that some concepts(eg ldquoscreamrdquo) are so strongly associated with atten-tional orienting that the attention-capturing eventbecomes part of the conceptual representation Acomponent was included to assess the degree towhich a concept is ldquosomeone or something thatgrabs your attentionrdquo

142 J R BINDER ET AL

Attribute salience ratings for 535 Englishwords

Ratings of the relevance of each semantic componentto word meanings were collected for 535 Englishwords using the internet crowdsourcing survey toolAmazon Mechanical Turk ( httpswwwmturkcommturk) The aim of this work was to ascertain thedegree to which a relatively unselected sample ofthe population associates the meaning of a particularword with particular kinds of experience We refer tothe continuous ratings obtained for each word asthe attributes comprising the wordrsquos meaning Theresults reflect subjective judgments rather than objec-tive truth and are likely to vary somewhat with per-sonal experience and background presence ofidiosyncratic associations participant motivation andcomprehension of the instructions With thesecaveats in mind it was hoped that mean ratingsobtained from a sufficiently large sample would accu-rately capture central tendencies in the populationproviding representative measures of real-worldcomprehension

As discussed in the previous section the attributesselected for study reflect known neural systems Wehypothesize that all concept learning depends onthis set of neural systems and therefore the sameattributes were rated regardless of grammatical classor ontological category

The word set

The concepts included 434 nouns 62 verbs and 39adjectives All of the verbs and adjectives and 141 ofthe nouns were pre-selected as experimentalmaterials for the Knowledge Representation inNeural Systems (KRNS) project (Glasgow et al 2016)an ongoing multi-centre research program sponsoredby the US Intelligence Advanced Research ProjectsActivity (IARPA) Because the KRNS set was limited torelatively concrete concepts and a restricted set ofnoun categories 293 additional nouns were addedto include more abstract concepts and to enablericher comparisons between category types Table 3summarizes some general characteristics of the wordset Table 4 describes the content of the set in termsof major category types A complete list of thewords and associated lexical data are available fordownload (see link prior to the References section)

Query design

Queries used to elicit ratings were designed to be asstructurally uniform as possible across attributes andgrammatical classes Some flexibility was allowedhowever to create natural and easily understoodqueries All queries took the general form of headrelationcontent where head refers to a lead-inphrase that was uniform within each word classcontent to the specific content being assessed andrelation to the type of relation linking the targetword and the content The head for all queriesregarding nouns was ldquoTo what degree do you thinkof this thing as rdquo whereas the head for all verbqueries was ldquoTo what degree do you think of thisas rdquo and the head for all adjective queries wasldquoTo what degree do you think of this propertyas rdquo Table 5 lists several examples for each gram-matical class

For the three scalar somatosensory attributesTemperature Texture and Weight it was felt necess-ary for the sake of clarity to pose separate queriesfor each end of the scalar continuum That is ratherthan a single query such as ldquoTo what degree do youthink of this thing as being hot or cold to thetouchrdquo two separate queries were posed about hotand cold attributes The Temperature rating wasthen computed by taking the maximum rating forthe word on either the Hot or the Cold query Similarlythe Texture rating was computed as the maximumrating on Rough and Smooth queries and theWeight rating was computed as the maximum ratingon Heavy and Light queries

A few attributes did not appear to have a mean-ingful sense when applied to certain grammaticalclasses The attribute Complexity addresses thedegree to which an entity appears visuallycomplex and has no obvious sensible correlate in

Table 3 Characteristics of the 535 rated wordsCharacteristic Mean SD Min Max Median IQR

Length in letters 595 195 3 11 6 3Log(10) orthographicfrequency

108 080 minus161 305 117 112

Orthographic neighbours 419 533 0 22 2 6Imageability (1ndash7 scale) 517 116 140 690 558 196

Note Orthographic frequency values are from the Westbury Lab UseNetCorpus (Shaoul amp Westbury 2013) Orthographic neighbour counts arefrom CELEX (Baayen Piepenbrock amp Gulikers 1995) Imageability ratingsare from a composite of published norms (Bird Franklin amp Howard 2001Clark amp Paivio 2004 Cortese amp Fugett 2004 Wilson 1988) IQR = interquar-tile range

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 143

Table 4 Cell counts for grammatical classes and categories included in the ratings studyType N Examples

Nouns (434)Concrete objects (275)Living things (126)Animals 30 crow horse moose snake turtle whaleBody parts 14 finger hand mouth muscle nose shoulderHumans 42 artist child doctor mayor parent soldierHuman groups 10 army company council family jury teamPlants 30 carrot eggplant elm rose tomato tulip

Other natural objects (19)Natural scenes 12 beach forest lake prairie river volcanoMiscellaneous 7 cloud egg feather stone sun

Artifacts (130)Furniture 7 bed cabinet chair crib desk shelves tableHand tools 27 axe comb glass keyboard pencil scissorsManufactured foods 18 beer cheese honey pie spaghetti teaMusical instruments 20 banjo drum flute mandolin piano tubaPlacesbuildings 28 bridge church highway prison school zooVehicles 20 bicycle car elevator plane subway truckMiscellaneous 10 book computer door fence ticket window

Concrete events (60)Social events 35 barbecue debate funeral party rally trialNonverbal sound events 11 belch clang explosion gasp scream whineWeather events 10 cyclone flood landslide storm tornadoMiscellaneous 4 accident fireworks ricochet stampede

Abstract entities (99)Abstract constructs 40 analogy fate law majority problem theoryCognitive entities 10 belief hope knowledge motive optimism witEmotions 20 animosity dread envy grief love shameSocial constructs 19 advice deceit etiquette mercy rumour truceTime periods 10 day era evening morning summer year

Verbs (62)Concrete actions (52)Body actions 10 ate held kicked laughed slept threwLocative change actions 14 approached crossed flew left ran put wentSocial actions 16 bought helped met spoke to stole visitedMiscellaneous 12 broke built damaged fixed found watched

Abstract actions 5 ended lost planned read usedStates 5 feared liked lived saw wanted

Adjectives (39)Abstract properties 13 angry clever famous friendly lonely tiredPhysical properties 26 blue dark empty heavy loud shiny

Table 5 Example queries for six attributesAttribute Query relation content

ColourNoun having a characteristic or defining colorVerb being associated with color or change in colorAdjective describing a quality or type of color

TasteNoun having a characteristic or defining tasteVerb being associated with tasting somethingAdjective describing how something tastes

Lower limbNoun being associated with actions using the leg or footVerb being an action or activity in which you use the leg or footAdjective being related to actions of the leg or foot

LandmarkNoun having a fixed location as on a mapVerb being an action or activity in which you use a mental map of your environmentAdjective describing the location of something as on a map

HumanNoun having human or human-like intentions plans or goalsVerb being an action or activity that involves human intentions plans or goalsAdjective being related to something with human or human-like intentions plans or goals

FearfulNoun being someone or something that makes you feel afraidVerb being associated with feeling afraidAdjective being related to feeling afraid

144 J R BINDER ET AL

the verb class The attribute Practice addresses thedegree to which the rater has personal experiencemanipulating an entity or performing an actionand has no obvious sensible correlate in the adjec-tive class Finally the attribute Caused addressesthe degree to which an entity is the result of someprior event or an action will cause a change tooccur and has no obvious correlate in the adjectiveclass Ratings on these three attributes were notobtained for the grammatical classes to which theydid not meaningfully apply

Survey methods

Surveys were posted on Amazon Mechanical Turk(AMT) an internet site that serves as an interfacebetween ldquorequestorsrdquo who post tasks and ldquoworkersrdquowho have accounts on the site and sign up to com-plete tasks and receive payment Requestors havethe right to accept or reject worker responses basedon quality criteria Participants in the present studywere required to have completed at least 10000 pre-vious jobs with at least a 99 acceptance rate and tohave account addresses in the United States these cri-teria were applied automatically by AMT Prospectiveparticipants were asked not to participate unlessthey were native English speakers however compli-ance with this request cannot be verified Afterreading a page of instructions participants providedtheir age gender years of education and currentoccupation No identifying information was collectedfrom participants or requested from AMT

For each session a participant was assigned a singleword and provided ratings on all of the semantic com-ponents as they relate to the assigned word A sen-tence containing the word was provided to specifythe word class and sense targeted Two such sen-tences conveying the most frequent sense of theword were constructed for each word and one ofthese was selected randomly and used throughoutthe session Participants were paid a small stipendfor completing all queries for the target word Theycould return for as many sessions as they wantedAn automated script assigned target words randomlywith the constraint that the same participant could notbe assigned the same word more than once

For each attribute a screen was presented with thetarget word the sentence context the query for thatattribute an example of a word that ldquowould receive

a high ratingrdquo on the attribute an example of aword that ldquomight receive a medium ratingrdquo on theattribute and the rating options The options werepresented as a horizontal row of clickable radiobuttons with numerical labels ranging from 0 to 6(left to right) and verbal labels (Not At All SomewhatVery Much) at appropriate locations above thebuttons An example trial is shown in Figure S1 ofthe Supplemental Material An additional buttonlabelled ldquoNot Applicablerdquo was positioned to the leftof the ldquo0rdquo button Participants were forewarned thatsome of the queries ldquowill be almost nonsensicalbecause they refer to aspects of word meaning thatare not even applicable to your word For exampleyou might be asked lsquoTo what degree do you thinkof shoe as an event that has a predictable durationwhether short or longrsquo with movie given as anexample of a high-rated concept and concert givenas a medium-rated concept Since shoe refers to astatic thing not to an event of any kind you shouldindicate either 0 or ldquoNot Applicablerdquo for this questionrdquoAll ldquoNot Applicablerdquo responses were later converted to0 ratings

General results

A total of 16373 sessions were collected from 1743unique participants (average 94 sessionsparticipant)Of the participants 43 were female 55 were maleand 2 did not provide gender information Theaverage age was 340 years (SD = 104) and averageyears of education was 148 (SD = 21)

A limitation of unsupervised crowdsourcing is thatsome participants may not comply with task instruc-tions Informal inspection of the data revealedexamples in which participants appeared to beresponding randomly or with the same response onevery trial A simple metric of individual deviationfrom the group mean response was computed by cor-relating the vector of ratings obtained from an individ-ual for a particular word with the group mean vectorfor that word For example if 30 participants wereassigned word X this yielded 30 vectors each with65 elements The average of these vectors is thegroup average for word X The quality metric foreach participant who rated word X was the correlationbetween that participantrsquos vector and the groupaverage vector for word X Supplemental Figure S2shows the distribution of these values across the

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 145

sample after Fisher z transformation A minimumthreshold for acceptance was set at r = 5 which corre-sponds to the edge of a prominent lower tail in thedistribution This criterion resulted in rejection of1097 or 669 of the sessions After removing thesesessions the final data set consisted of 15268 ratingvectors with a mean intra-word individual-to-groupcorrelation of 785 (median 803) providing anaverage of 286 rating vectors (ie sessions) perword (SD 20 range 17ndash33) Mean ratings for each ofthe 535 words computed using only the sessionsthat passed the acceptance criterion are availablefor download (see link prior to the References section)

Exploring the representations

Here we explore the dataset by addressing threegeneral questions First how much independent infor-mation is provided by each of the attributes and howare they related to each other Although the com-ponent attributes were selected to represent distincttypes of experiential information there is likely to beconceptual overlap between attributes real-worldcovariance between attributes and covariance dueto associations linking one attribute with another inthe minds of the raters We therefore sought tocharacterize the underlying covariance structure ofthe attributes Second do the attributes captureimportant distinctions between a priori ontologicaltypes and categories If the structure of conceptualknowledge really is based on underlying neural pro-cessing systems then the covariance structure of attri-butes representing these systems should reflectpsychologically salient conceptual distinctionsFinally what conceptual groupings are present inthe covariance structure itself and how do thesediffer from a priori category assignments

Attribute mutual information

To investigate redundancy in the informationencoded in the representational space we computedthe mutual information (MI) for all pairs of attributesusing the individual participant ratings after removalof outliers MI is a general measure of how mutuallydependent two variables are determined by the simi-larity between their joint distribution p(XY) and fac-tored marginal distribution p(X)p(Y) MI can range

from 0 indicating complete independence to 1 inthe case of identical variables

MI was generally very low (mean = 049)suggesting a low overall level of redundancy (see Sup-plemental Figure S3) Particular pairs and groupshowever showed higher redundancy The largest MIvalue was for the pair PleasantndashHappy (643) It islikely that these two constructs evoked very similarconcepts in the minds of the raters Other isolatedpairs with relatively high MI values included SmallndashWeight (344) CausedndashConsequential (312) PracticendashNear (269) TastendashSmell (264) and SocialndashCommuni-cation (242)

Groups of attributes with relatively high pairwise MIvalues included a human-related group (Face SpeechHuman Body Biomotion mean pairwise MI = 320) anattention-arousal group (Attention Arousal Surprisedmean pairwise MI = 304) an auditory group (AuditionLoud Low High Sound Music mean pairwise MI= 296) a visualndashsomatosensory group (VisionColour Pattern Shape Texture Weight Touch meanpairwise MI = 279) a negative affect group (AngrySad Disgusted Unpleasant Fearful Harm Painmean pairwise MI = 263) a motion-related group(Motion Fast Slow Biomotion mean pairwise MI= 240) and a time-related group (Time DurationShort Long mean pairwise MI = 193)

Attribute covariance structure

Factor analysis was conducted to investigate potentiallatent dimensions underlying the attributes Principalaxis factoring (PAF) was used with subsequentpromax rotation given that the factors wereassumed to be correlated Mean attribute vectors forthe 496 nouns and verbs were used as input adjectivedata were excluded so that the Practice and Causedattributes could be included in the analysis To deter-mine the number of factors to retain a parallel analysis(eigenvalue Monte Carlo simulation) was performedusing PAF and 1000 random permutations of theraw data from the current study (Horn 1965OrsquoConnor 2000) Factors with eigenvalues exceedingthe 95th percentile of the distribution of eigenvaluesderived from the random data were retained andexamined for interpretability

The KaiserndashMeyerndashOlkin Measure of SamplingAccuracy was 874 and Bartlettrsquos Test of Sphericitywas significant χ2(2016) = 4112917 p lt 001

146 J R BINDER ET AL

indicating adequate factorability Sixteen factors hadeigenvalues that were significantly above randomchance These factors accounted for 810 of the var-iance Based on interpretability all 16 factors wereretained Table 6 lists these factors and the attributeswith the highest loadings on each

As can be seen from the loadings the latent dimen-sions revealed by PAF mostly replicate the groupingsapparent in the mutual information analysis The mostprominent of these include factors representing visualand tactile attributes negative emotion associationssocial communication auditory attributes food inges-tion self-related attributes motion in space humanphysical attributes surprise characteristics of largeplaces upper limb actions reward experiences positiveassociations and time-related attributes

Together the mutual information and factor ana-lyses reveal a modest degree of covariance betweenthe attributes suggesting that a more compact rep-resentation could be achieved by reducing redun-dancy A reasonable first step would be to removeeither Pleasant or Happy from the representation asthese two attributes showed a high level of redun-dancy For some analyses use of the 16 significantfactors identified in the PAF rather than the completeset of attributes may be appropriate and useful On theother hand these factors left sim20 of the varianceunexplained which we propose is a reflection of theunderlying complexity of conceptual knowledgeThis complexity places limits on the extent to which

the representation can be reduced without losingexplanatory power In the following analyses wehave included all 65 attributes in order to investigatefurther their potential contributions to understandingconceptual structure

Attribute composition of some major semanticcategories

Mean attribute vectors representing major semanticcategories in the word set were computed by aver-aging over items within several of the a priori cat-egories outlined in Table 4 Figures 1 and 2 showmean vectors for 12 of the 20 noun categories inTable 4 presented as circular plots

Vectors for three verb categories are shown inFigure 3 With the exception of the Path and Social attri-butes which marked the Locative Action and SocialAction categories respectively the threeverbcategoriesevoked a similar set of attributes but in different relativeproportions Ratings for physical adjectives reflected thesensorydomain (visual somatosensory auditory etc) towhich the adjective referred

Quantitative differences between a prioricategories

The a priori category labels shown in Table 4 wereused to identify attribute ratings that differ signifi-cantly between semantic categories and thereforemay contribute to a mental representation of thesecategory distinctions For each comparison anunpaired t-test was conducted for each of the 65 attri-butes comparing the mean ratings on words in onecategory with those in the other It should be kept inmind that the results are specific to the sets ofwords that happened to be selected for the studyand may not generalize to other samples from thesecategories For that reason and because of the largenumber of contrasts performed only differences sig-nificant at p lt 0001 will be described

Initial comparisons were conducted between thesuperordinate categories Artefacts (n = 132) LivingThings (N = 128) and Abstract Entities (N = 99) Asshown in Figure 4 numerous attributes distinguishedthese categories Not surprisingly Abstract Entitieshad significantly lower ratings than the two concretecategories on nearly all of the attributes related tosensory experience The main exceptions to this were

Table 6 Summary of the attribute factor analysisF EV Interpretation Highest-Loading Attributes (all gt 35)

1 1281 VisionTouch Pattern Shape Texture Colour WeightSmall Vision Dark Bright

2 1071 Negative Disgusted Unpleasant Sad Angry PainHarm Fearful Consequential

3 693 Communication Communication Social Head4 686 Audition Sound Audition Loud Low High Music

Attention5 618 Ingestion Taste Head Smell Toward6 608 Self Needs Near Self Practice7 586 Motion in space Path Fast Away Motion Lower Limb

Toward Slow8 533 Human Face Body Speech Human Biomotion9 508 Surprise Short Surprised10 452 Place Landmark Scene Large11 450 Upper limb Upper Limb Touch Music12 449 Reward Benefit Needs Drive13 407 Positive Happy Pleasant Arousal Attention14 322 Time Duration Time Number15 237 Luminance Bright16 116 Slow Slow

Note Factors are listed in order of variance explained Attributes with positiveloadings are listed for each factor F = factor number EV = rotatedeigenvalue

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 147

Figure 1 Mean attribute vectors for 6 concrete object noun categories Attributes are grouped and colour-coded by general domainand similarity to assist interpretation Blackness of the attribute labels reflects the mean attribute value Error bars represent standarderror of the mean [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 2 Mean attribute vectors for 3 event noun categories and 3 abstract noun categories [To view this figure in colour please seethe online version of this Journal]

148 J R BINDER ET AL

the attributes specifically associated with animals andpeople such as Biomotion Face Body Speech andHuman for which Artefacts received ratings as low asor lower than those for Abstract Entities ConverselyAbstract Entities received higher ratings than the con-crete categories on attributes related to temporal andcausal experiences (Time Duration Long ShortCaused Consequential) social experiences (SocialCommunication Self) and emotional experiences(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal) In all 57 of the 65 attributes distin-guished abstract from concrete entities

Living Things were distinguished from Artefacts bythe aforementioned attributes characteristic ofanimals and people In addition Living Things onaverage received higher ratings on Motion and Slowsuggesting they tend to have more salient movement

qualities Living Things also received higher ratingsthan Artefacts on several emotional attributes(Angry Disgusted Fearful Surprised) although theseratings were relatively low for both concrete cat-egories Conversely Artefacts received higher ratingsthan Living Things on the attributes Large Landmarkand Scene all of which probably reflect the over-rep-resentation of buildings in this sample Artefacts werealso rated higher on Temperature Music (reflecting anover-representation of musical instruments) UpperLimb Practice and several temporal attributes (TimeDuration and Short) Though significantly differentfor Artefacts and Living Things the ratings on the tem-poral attributes were lower for both concrete cat-egories than for Abstract Entities In all 21 of the 65attributes distinguished between Living Things andArtefacts

Figure 3 Mean attribute vectors for 3 verb categories [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Figure 4 Attributes distinguishing Artefacts Living Things and Abstract Entities Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indi-cated by the coloured squares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 149

Figure 5 shows comparisons between the morespecific categories Animals (n = 30) Plants (N = 30)and Tools (N = 27) Relatively selective impairmentson these three categories are frequently reported inpatients with category-related semantic impairments(Capitani Laiacona Mahon amp Caramazza 2003Forde amp Humphreys 1999 Gainotti 2000) The datashow a number of expected results (see Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 for previous similar comparisonsbetween these categories) such as higher ratingsfor Animals than for either Plants or Tools on attri-butes related to motion higher ratings for Plants onTaste Smell and Head attributes related to their pro-minent role as food and higher ratings for Tools andPlants on attributes related to manipulation (TouchUpper Limb Practice) Ratings on sound-related attri-butes were highest for Animals intermediate forTools and virtually zero for Plants Colour alsoshowed a three-way split with Plants gt Animals gtTools Animals however received higher ratings onvisual Complexity

In the spatial domain Animals were viewed ashaving more salient paths of motion (Path) and Toolswere rated as more close at hand in everyday life(Near) Tools were rated as more consequential andmore related to social experiences drives and needsTools and Plants were seen as more beneficial thanAnimals and Plants more pleasant than ToolsAnimals and Tools were both viewed as having morepotential for harm than Plants and Animals had

higher ratings on Fearful Surprised Attention andArousal attributes suggesting that animals areviewedas potential threatsmore than are the other cat-egories In all 29 attributes distinguished betweenAnimals and Plants 22 distinguished Animals andTools and 20 distinguished Plants and Tools

Figure 6 shows a three-way comparison betweenthe specific artefact categories Musical Instruments(N = 20) Tools (N = 27) and Vehicles (N = 20) Againthere were several expected findings includinghigher ratings for Vehicles on motion-related attri-butes (Motion Biomotion Fast Slow Path Away)and higher ratings for Musical Instruments on thesound-related attributes (Audition Loud Low HighSound Music) Tools were rated higher on attributesreflecting manipulation experience (Touch UpperLimb Practice Near) The importance of the Practiceattribute which encodes degree of personal experi-ence manipulating an object or performing anaction is reflected in the much higher rating on thisattribute for Tools than for Musical Instrumentswhereas these categories did not differ on the UpperLimb attribute The categories were distinguishedby size in the expected direction (Vehicles gt Instru-ments gt Tools) and Instruments and Vehicles wererated higher than Tools on visual Complexity

In the more abstract domains Musical Instrumentsreceived higher ratings on Communication (ldquoa thing oraction that people use to communicaterdquo) and Cogni-tion (ldquoa form of mental activity or function of themindrdquo) suggesting stronger associations with mental

Figure 5 Attributes distinguishing Animals Plants and Tools Pairwise differences significant at p lt 0001 are indicated by the colouredsquares above the graph [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

150 J R BINDER ET AL

activity but also higher ratings on Pleasant HappySad Surprised Attention and Arousal suggestingstronger emotional and arousal associations Toolsand Vehicles had higher ratings than Musical Instru-ments on both Benefit and Harm attributes andTools were rated higher on the Needs attribute(ldquosomeone or something that would be hard for youto live withoutrdquo) In all 22 attributes distinguishedbetween Musical Instruments and Tools 21 distin-guished Musical Instruments and Vehicles and 14 dis-tinguished Tools and Vehicles

Overall these category-related differences are intui-tively sensible and a number of them have beenreported previously (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainottiet al 2009 Gainotti et al 2013 Hoffman amp LambonRalph 2013 Tranel Logan Frank amp Damasio 1997Vigliocco Vinson Lewis amp Garrett 2004) They high-light the underlying complexity of taxonomic categorystructure and illustrate how the present data can beused to explore this complexity

Category effects on item similarity Brain-basedversus distributional representations

Another way in which a priori category labels areuseful for evaluating the representational schemeis by enabling a comparison of semantic distancebetween items in the same category and items indifferent categories Figure 7A shows heat maps ofthe pairwise cosine similarity matrix for all 434nouns in the sample constructed using the brain-

based representation and a representation derivedvia latent semantic analysis (LSA Landauer ampDumais 1997) of a large text corpus (LandauerKintsch amp the Science and Applications of LatentSemantic Analysis (SALSA) Group 2015) Vectors inthe LSA representation were reduced to 65 dimen-sions for comparability with the brain-based vectorsthough very similar results were obtained with a300-dimensional LSA representation Concepts aregrouped in the matrices according to a priori categoryto highlight category similarity structure The matricesare modestly correlated (r = 31 p lt 00001) As thefigure suggests cosine similarity tended to be muchhigher for the brain-based vectors (mean = 55 SD= 18) than for the LSA vectors (mean = 19 SD = 17p lt 00001) More importantly cosine similarity wasgenerally greater for pairs in the same a priori cat-egory than for between-category pairs and this differ-ence measured for each word using Cohenrsquos d effectsize was much larger for the brain-based (mean =264 SD = 109) than for the LSA vectors (mean =109 SD = 085 p lt 00001 Figure 7B) Thus both thebrain-based vectors and LSA vectors capture infor-mation reflecting a priori category structure and thebrain-based vectors show significantly greater effectsof category co-membership than the LSA vectors

Data-driven categorization of the words

Cosine similarity measures were also used to identifyldquoneighbourhoodsrdquo of semantically close concepts

Figure 6 Attributes distinguishing Musical Instruments Tools and Vehicles [To view this figure in colour please see the online versionof this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 151

without reference to a priori labels Table 7 showssome representative results (a complete list is avail-able for download see link prior to the Referencessection) The results provide further evidence thatthe representations capture semantic similarityacross concepts although direct comparisons ofthese distance measures with similarity decision andpriming data are needed for further validation

A more global assessment of similarity structurewas performed using k-means cluster analyses withthe mean attribute vectors for each word as input Aseries of k-means analyses was performed with thecluster parameter ranging from 20ndash30 reflecting theapproximate number of a priori categories inTable 4 Although the results were very similar acrossthis range results in the range from 25ndash28 seemedto afford the most straightforward interpretations

Results from the 28-cluster solution are shown inTable 8 It is important to note that we are not claimingthat this is the ldquotruerdquo number of clusters in the dataConceptual organization is inherently hierarchicalinvolving degrees of similarity and the number of cat-egories defined depends on the type of distinctionone wishes to make (eg animal vs tool canine vsfeline dog vs wolf hound vs spaniel) Our aim herewas to match approximately the ldquograin sizerdquo of thek-means clusters with the grain size of the a priorisuperordinate category labels so that these could bemeaningfully compared

Several of the clusters that emerged from the ratingsdata closely matched the a priori category assignmentsoutlined in Table 4 For example all 30 Animals clusteredtogether 13of the14BodyParts andall 20Musical Instru-ments One body part (ldquohairrdquo) fell in the Tools and

Figure 7 (A) Cosine similarity matrices for the 434 nouns in the study displayed as heat maps with words grouped by superordinatecategory The matrix at left was computed using the brain-based vectors and the matrix at right was computed using latent semanticanalysis vectors Yellow indicates greater similarity (B) Average effect sizes (Cohenrsquos d ) for comparisons of within-category versusbetween-category cosine similarity values An effect size was computed for each word then these were averaged across all wordsError bars indicate 99 confidence intervals BBR = brain-based representation LSA = latent semantic analysis [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

Table 7 Representational similarity neighbourhoods for some example wordsWord Closest neighbours

actor artist reporter priest journalist minister businessman teacher boy editor diplomatadvice apology speech agreement plea testimony satire wit truth analogy debateairport jungle highway subway zoo cathedral farm church theater store barapricot tangerine tomato raspberry cherry banana asparagus pea cranberry cabbage broccoliarm hand finger shoulder leg muscle foot eye jaw nose liparmy mob soldier judge policeman commander businessman team guard protest reporterate fed drank dinner lived used bought hygiene opened worked barbecue playedavalanche hailstorm landslide volcano explosion flood lightning cyclone tornado hurricanebanjo mandolin fiddle accordion xylophone harp drum piano clarinet saxophone trumpetbee mosquito mouse snake hawk cheetah tiger chipmunk crow bird antbeer rum tea lemonade coffee pie ham cheese mustard ketchup jambelch cough gasp scream squeal whine screech clang thunder loud shoutedblue green yellow black white dark red shiny ivy cloud dandelionbook computer newspaper magazine camera cash pen pencil hairbrush keyboard umbrella

152 J R BINDER ET AL

Furniture cluster and three additional sound-producingartefacts (ldquomegaphonerdquo ldquotelevisionrdquo and ldquocellphonerdquo)clustered with the Musical Instruments Ratings-basedclusteringdidnotdistinguishediblePlants fromManufac-tured Foods collapsing these into a single Foods clusterthat excluded larger inedible plants (ldquoelmrdquo ldquoivyrdquo ldquooakrdquoldquotreerdquo) Similarly data-driven clustering did not dis-tinguish Furniture from Tools collapsing these into asingle cluster that also included most of the miscella-neousmanipulated artefacts (ldquobookrdquo ldquodoorrdquo ldquocomputerrdquo

ldquomagazinerdquo ldquomedicinerdquo ldquonewspaperrdquo ldquoticketrdquo ldquowindowrdquo)as well as several smaller non-artefact items (ldquofeatherrdquoldquohairrdquo ldquoicerdquo ldquostonerdquo)

Data-driven clustering also identified a number ofintuitively plausible divisions within categories Basichuman biological types (ldquowomanrdquo ldquomanrdquo ldquochildrdquoetc) were distinguished from human occupationalroles and human individuals or groups with negativeor violent associations formed a distinct cluster Com-pared to the neutral occupational roles human type

Table 8 K-means cluster analysis of the entire 535-word set with k = 28Animals Body parts Human types Neutral human roles Violent human roles Plants and foods Large bright objects

n = 30 n = 13 n = 7 n = 37 n = 6 n = 44 n = 3

chipmunkhawkduckmoosehamsterhorsecamelcheetahpenguinpig

armfingerhandshouldereyelegnosejawfootmuscle

womangirlboyparentmanchildfamily

diplomatmayorbankereditorministerguardbusinessmanreporterpriestjournalist

mobterroristcriminalvictimarmyriot

apricotasparagustomatobroccolispaghettijamcheesecabbagecucumbertangerine

cloudsunbonfire

Non-social places Social places Tools and furniture Musical instruments Loud vehicles Quiet vehicles Festive social events

n = 22 n = 20 n = 43 n = 23 n = 6 n = 18 n = 15

fieldforestbaylakeprairieapartmentfencebridgeislandelm

officestorecathedralchurchlabparkhotelembassyfarmcafeteria

spatulahairbrushumbrellacabinetcorkscrewcombwindowshelvesstaplerrake

mandolinxylophonefiddletrumpetsaxophonebuglebanjobagpipeclarinetharp

planerockettrainsubwayambulance(fireworks)

boatcarriagesailboatscootervanbuscablimousineescalator(truck)

partyfestivalcarnivalcircusmusicalsymphonytheaterparaderallycelebrated

Verbal social events Negativenatural events

Nonverbalsound events

Ingestive eventsand actions

Beneficial abstractentities

Neutral abstractentities

Causal entities

n = 14 n = 16 n = 11 n = 5 n = 20 n = 23 n = 19

debateorationtestimonynegotiatedadviceapologypleaspeechmeetingdictation

cyclonehurricanehailstormstormlandslidefloodtornadoexplosionavalanchestampede

squealscreechgaspwhinescreamclang(loud)coughbelchthunder

fedatedrankdinnerbarbecue

moraltrusthopemercyknowledgeoptimismintellect(spiritual)peacesympathy

hierarchysumparadoxirony(famous)cluewhole(ended)verbpatent

rolelegalitypowerlawtheoryagreementtreatytrucefatemotive

Negative emotionentities

Positive emotionentities

Time concepts Locative changeactions

Neutral actions Negative actionsand properties

Sensoryproperties

n = 30 n = 22 n = 16 n = 14 n = 23 n = 16 n = 19

jealousyireanimositydenialenvygrievanceguiltsnubsinfallacy

jovialitygratitudefunjoylikedsatireawejokehappy(theme)

morningeveningdaysummernewspringerayearwinternight

crossedleftwentranapproachedlandedwalkedmarchedflewhiked

usedboughtfixedgavehelpedfoundmetplayedwrotetook

damageddangerousblockeddestroyedinjuredbrokeaccidentlostfearedstole

greendustyexpensiveyellowblueusedwhite(ivy)redempty

Note Numbers below the cluster labels indicate the number of words in the cluster The first 10 items in each cluster are listed in order from closest to farthestdistance from the cluster centroid Parentheses indicate words that do not fit intuitively with the interpretation

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 153

concepts had significantly higher ratings on attributesrelated to sensory experience (Shape ComplexityTouch Temperature Texture High Sound Smell)nearness (Near Self) and positive emotion (HappyTable 9) Places were divided into two groups whichwe labelled Non-Social and Social that correspondedroughly to the a priori distinction between NaturalScenes and PlacesBuildings except that the Non-Social Places cluster included several manmadeplaces (ldquoapartmentrdquo fencerdquo ldquobridgerdquo ldquostreetrdquo etc)Social Places had higher ratings on attributes relatedto human interactions (Social Communication Conse-quential) and Non-Social Places had higher ratings onseveral sensory attributes (Colour Pattern ShapeTouch and Texture Table 10) In addition to dis-tinguishing vehicles from other artefacts data-drivenclustering separated loud vehicles from quiet vehicles(mean Loud ratings 530 vs 196 respectively) Loudvehicles also had significantly higher ratings onseveral other auditory attributes (Audition HighSound) The two vehicle clusters contained four unex-pected items (ldquofireworksrdquo ldquofountainrdquo ldquoriverrdquo ldquosoccerrdquo)probably due to high ratings for these items onMotion and Path attributes which distinguish vehiclesfrom other nonliving objects

In the domain of event nouns clustering divided thea priori category Social Events into two clusters that welabelled Festive Social Events and Verbal Social Events(Table 11) Festive Events had significantly higherratings on a number of sensory attributes related tovisual shape visual motion and sound and wererated as more Pleasant and Happy Verbal SocialEvents had higher ratings on attributes related tohuman cognition (Communication Cognition

Consequential) and interestingly were rated as bothmore Unpleasant and more Beneficial than FestiveEvents A cluster labelled Negative Natural Events cor-responded roughly to the a priori category WeatherEventswith the additionof several non-meteorologicalnegative or violent events (ldquoexplosionrdquo ldquostampederdquoldquobattlerdquo etc) A cluster labelled Nonverbal SoundEvents corresponded closely to the a priori categoryof the samename Finally a small cluster labelled Inges-tive Events and Actions included several social eventsand verbs of ingestion linked by high ratings on theattributes Taste Smell Upper Limb Head TowardPleasant Benefit and Needs

Correspondence between data-driven clusters anda priori categories was less clear in the domain ofabstract nouns Clustering yielded two abstract

Table 9 Attributes that differ significantly between human typesand neutral human roles (occupations)Attribute Human types Neutral human roles

Bright 080 (028) 037 (019)Small 173 (119) 063 (029)Shape 396 (081) 245 (055)Complexity 258 (050) 178 (038)Touch 222 (083) 050 (025)Temperature 069 (037) 034 (014)Texture 169 (101) 064 (024)High 169 (112) 039 (022)Sound 273 (058) 130 (052)Smell 127 (061) 036 (028)Near 291 (077) 084 (054)Self 271 (123) 101 (077)Happy 350 (081) 176 (091)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 10 Attributes that differ significantly between non-socialplaces and social placesAttribute Non-social places Social places

Colour 271 (109) 099 (051)Pattern 292 (082) 105 (057)Shape 389 (091) 250 (078)Touch 195 (103) 047 (037)Texture 276 (107) 092 (041)Time 036 (042) 134 (092)Consequential 065 (045) 189 (100)Social 084 (052) 331 (096)Communication 026 (019) 138 (079)Cognition 020 (013) 103 (084)Disgusted 008 (006) 049 (038)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 11 Attributes that differ significantly between festive andverbal social eventsAttribute Festive social events Verbal social events

Vision 456 (093) 141 (113)Bright 150 (098) 010 (007)Large 318 (138) 053 (072)Motion 360 (100) 084 (085)Fast 151 (063) 038 (028)Shape 140 (071) 024 (021)Complexity 289 (117) 048 (061)Loud 389 (092) 149 (109)Sound 389 (115) 196 (103)Music 321 (177) 052 (078)Head 185 (130) 398 (109)Consequential 161 (085) 383 (082)Communication 220 (114) 533 (039)Cognition 115 (044) 370 (077)Benefit 250 (053) 375 (058)Pleasant 441 (085) 178 (090)Unpleasant 038 (025) 162 (059)Happy 426 (088) 163 (092)Angry 034 (036) 124 (061)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

154 J R BINDER ET AL

entity clusters mainly distinguished by perceivedbenefit and association with positive emotions(Table 12) labelled Beneficial and Neutral which com-prise a variety of abstract and mental constructs Athird cluster labelled Causal Entities comprises con-cepts with high ratings on the Consequential andSocial attributes (Table 13) Two further clusters com-prise abstract entities with strong negative and posi-tive emotional meaning or associations (Table 14)The final cluster of abstract entities correspondsapproximately to the a priori category Time Periodsbut also includes properties (ldquonewrdquo ldquooldrdquo ldquoyoungrdquo)and verbs (ldquogrewrdquo ldquosleptrdquo) associated with time dur-ation concepts

Three clusters consisted mainly of verbs Theseincluded a cluster labelled Locative Change Actionswhich corresponded closely with the a priori categoryof the same name and was defined by high ratings onattributes related to motion through space (Table 15)The second verb cluster contained a mixture ofemotionally neutral physical and social actions Thefinal verb-heavy cluster contained a mix of verbs andadjectives with negative or violent associations

The final cluster emerging from the k-meansanalysis consisted almost entirely of adjectivesdescribing physical sensory properties including

colour surface appearance tactile texture tempera-ture and weight

Hierarchical clustering

A hierarchical cluster analysis of the 335 concretenouns (objects and events) was performed toexamine the underlying category structure in moredetail The major categories were again clearlyevident in the resulting dendrogram A number ofinteresting subdivisions emerged as illustrated inthe dendrogram segments shown in Figure 8Musical Instruments which formed a distinct clustersubdivided further into instruments played byblowing (left subgroup light blue colour online) andinstruments played with the upper limbs only (rightsubgroup) the latter of which contained a somewhatsegregated group of instruments played by strikingldquoPianordquo formed a distinct branch probably due to its

Table 12 Attributes that differ significantly between beneficialand neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Beneficial abstract entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 342 (083) 222 (095)Self 361 (103) 119 (102)Cognition 403 (138) 219 (136)Benefit 468 (070) 231 (129)Pleasant 384 (093) 145 (078)Happy 390 (105) 132 (076)Drive 376 (082) 170 (060)Needs 352 (116) 130 (099)Arousal 317 (094) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 13 Attributes that differ significantly between causalentities and neutral abstract entitiesAttribute Causal entities Neutral abstract entities

Consequential 447 (067) 222 (095)Social 394 (121) 180 (091)Drive 346 (083) 170 (060)Arousal 295 (059) 176 (083)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 14 Attributes that differ significantly between negativeand positive emotion entitiesAttribute Negative emotion entities Positive emotion entities

Vision 075 (049) 152 (081)Bright 006 (006) 062 (050)Dark 056 (041) 014 (016)Pain 209 (105) 019 (021)Music 010 (014) 057 (054)Consequential 442 (072) 258 (080)Self 101 (056) 252 (120)Benefit 075 (065) 337 (093)Harm 381 (093) 046 (055)Pleasant 028 (025) 471 (084)Unpleasant 480 (072) 029 (037)Happy 023 (035) 480 (092)Sad 346 (112) 041 (058)Angry 379 (106) 029 (040)Disgusted 270 (084) 024 (037)Fearful 259 (106) 026 (027)Needs 037 (047) 209 (096)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

Table 15 Attributes that differ significantly between locativechange and abstractsocial actionsAttribute Locative change actions Neutral actions

Motion 381 (071) 198 (081)Biomotion 464 (067) 287 (078)Fast 323 (127) 142 (076)Body 447 (098) 274 (106)Lower Limb 386 (208) 111 (096)Path 510 (084) 205 (143)Communication 101 (058) 288 (151)Cognition 096 (031) 253 (128)

Note Columns give mean for each rating with standard deviations in par-entheses The larger value is indicated in bold font

Differences significant at p lt 0001 p lt 00001

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 155

higher rating on the Lower Limb attribute People con-cepts which also formed a distinct cluster showedevidence of a subgroup of ldquoprivate sectorrdquo occu-pations (large left subgroup green colour online)and a subgroup of ldquopublic sectorrdquo occupations(middle subgroup purple colour online) Two othersubgroups consisted of basic human types andpeople concepts with negative or violent associations(far right subgroup magenta colour online)

Animals also formed a distinct cluster though two(ldquoantrdquo and ldquobutterflyrdquo) were isolated on nearbybranches The animals branched into somewhat dis-tinct subgroups of aquatic animals (left-most sub-group light blue colour online) small land animals(next subgroup yellow colour online) large animals(next subgroup red colour online) and unpleasantanimals (subgroup including ldquoalligatorrdquo magentacolour online) Interestingly ldquocamelrdquo and ldquohorserdquomdashthe only animals in the set used for human transpor-tationmdashsegregated together despite the fact that

there are no attributes that specifically encode ldquousedfor human transportationrdquo Inspection of the vectorssuggested that this segregation was due to higherratings on both the Lower Limb and Benefit attributesthan for the other animals Association with lower limbmovements and benefit to people that is appears tobe sufficient to mark an animal as useful for transpor-tation Finally Plants and Foods formed a large distinctbranch which contained subgroups of beveragesfruits manufactured foods vegetables and flowers

A similar hierarchical cluster analysis was performedon the 99 abstract nouns Three major branchesemerged The largest of these comprised abstractconcepts with a neutral or positive meaning Asshown in Figure 9 one subgroup within this largecluster consisted of positive or beneficial entities (sub-group from ldquoattributerdquo to ldquogratituderdquo green colouronline) A second fairly distinct subgroup consisted ofldquocausalrdquo concepts associated with a high likelihood ofconsequences (subgroup from ldquomotiverdquo to ldquofaterdquo

Figure 8 Dendrogram segments from the hierarchical cluster analysis on concrete nouns Musical instruments people animals andplantsfoods each clustered together on separate branches with evidence of sub-clusters within each branch [To view this figure incolour please see the online version of this Journal]

156 J R BINDER ET AL

blue colour online) A third subgroup includedconcepts associated with number or quantification(subgroup from ldquonounrdquo to ldquoinfinityrdquo light blue colouronline) A number of pairs (adviceapology treatytruce) and triads (ironyparadoxmystery) with similarmeanings were also evident within the large cluster

The second major branch of the abstract hierarchycomprised concepts associated with harm or anothernegative meaning (Figure 10) One subgroup withinthis branch consisted of words denoting negativeemotions (subgroup from ldquoguiltrdquo to ldquodreadrdquo redcolour online) A second subgroup seemed to consistof social situations associated with anger (subgroupfrom ldquosnubrdquo to ldquogrievancerdquo green colour online) Athird subgroup was a triad (sincurseproblem)related to difficulty or misfortune A fourth subgroupwas a triad (fallacyfollydenial) related to error ormisjudgment

The final major branch of the abstract hierarchyconsisted of concepts denoting time periods (daymorning summer spring evening night winter)

Correlations with word frequency andimageability

Frequency of word use provides a rough indication ofthe frequency and significance of a concept We pre-dicted that attributes related to proximity and self-needs would be correlated with word frequency Ima-

geability is a rough indicator of the overall salience ofsensory particularly visual attributes of an entity andthus we predicted correlations with attributes relatedto sensory and motor experience An alpha level of

Figure 9 Neutral abstract branches of the abstract noun dendrogram [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

Figure 10 Negative abstract branches of the abstract noun den-drogram [To view this figure in colour please see the onlineversion of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 157

00001 (r = 1673) was adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests

Word frequency was positively correlated withNear Self Benefit Drive and Needs consistent withthe expectation that word frequency reflects theproximity and survival significance of conceptsWord frequency was also positively correlated withattributes characteristic of people including FaceBody Speech and Human reflecting the proximityand significance of conspecific entities Word fre-quency was negatively correlated with a number ofsensory attributes including Colour Pattern SmallShape Temperature Texture Weight Audition LoudLow High Sound Music and Taste One possibleexplanation for this is the tendency for some naturalobject categories with very salient perceptual featuresto have far less salience in everyday experience andlanguage use particularly animals plants andmusical instruments

As expected most of the sensory and motor attri-butes were positively correlated (p lt 00001) with ima-geability including Vision Bright Dark ColourPattern Large Small Motion Biomotion Fast SlowShape Complexity Touch Temperature WeightLoud Low High Sound Taste Smell Upper Limband Practice Also positively correlated were thevisualndashspatial attributes Landmark Scene Near andToward Conversely many non-perceptual attributeswere negatively correlated with imageability includ-ing temporal and causal components (DurationLong Short Caused Consequential) social and cogni-tive components (Social Human CommunicationSelf Cognition) and affectivearousal components(Unpleasant Sad Angry Disgusted Fearful SurprisedDrive Arousal)

Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables

The large size of the dataset afforded an opportunityto look for unanticipated relationships betweensemantic attributes and non-semantic lexical vari-ables Previous research has identified various phono-logical correlates of meaning (Lynott amp Connell 2013Schmidtke Conrad amp Jacobs 2014) and grammaticalclass (Farmer Christiansen amp Monaghan 2006 Kelly1992) These data suggest that the evolution of wordforms is not entirely arbitrary but is influenced inpart by meaning and pragmatic factors In an explora-tory analysis we tested for correlation between the 65

semantic attributes and measures of length (numberof phonemes) orthographic typicality (mean pos-ition-constrained bigram frequency orthographicneighbourhood size) and phonologic typicality (pho-nologic neighbourhood size) An alpha level of00001 was again adopted to reduce family-wiseType 1 error from the large number of tests and tofocus on very strong effects that are perhaps morelikely to hold across other random samples of words

Word length (number of phonemes) was negativelycorrelated with Near and Needs with a strong trend(p lt 0001) for Practice suggesting that shorterwords are used for things that are near to us centralto our needs and frequently manipulated Similarlyorthographic neighbourhood size was positively cor-related with Near Needs and Practice with strongtrends for Touch and Self and phonological neigh-bourhood size was positively correlated with Nearand Practice with strong trends for Needs andTouch Together these correlations suggest that con-cepts referring to nearby objects of significance toour self needs tend to be represented by shorter orsimpler morphemes with commonly shared spellingpatterns Position-constrained bigram frequency wasnot significantly correlated with any of the attributeshowever suggesting that semantic variables mainlyinfluence segments larger than letter pairs

Potential applications

The intent of the current study was to outline a rela-tively comprehensive brain-based componentialmodel of semantic representation and to exploresome of the properties of this type of representationBuilding on extensive previous work (Allport 1985Borghi et al 2011 Cree amp McRae 2003 Crutch et al2013 Crutch et al 2012 Gainotti et al 2009 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Lynott amp Connell2009 2013 Martin amp Caramazza 2003 Tranel et al1997 Vigliocco et al 2004 Warrington amp McCarthy1987 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) the representationwas expanded to include a variety of sensory andmotor subdomains more complex physical domains(space time) and mental experiences guided by theprinciple that all aspects of experience are encodedin the brain and processed according to informationcontent The utility of the representation ultimatelydepends on its completeness and veridicality whichare determined in no small part by the specific

158 J R BINDER ET AL

attributes included and details of the queries used toelicit attribute salience values These choices arealmost certainly imperfect and thus we view thecurrent work as a preliminary exploration that wehope will lead to future improvements

The representational scheme is motivated by anunderlying theory of concept representation in thebrain and its principal utilitymdashandmain source of vali-dationmdashis likely to be in brain research A recent studyby Fernandino et al (Fernandino et al 2015) suggestsone type of application The authors obtained ratingsfor 900 noun concepts on the five attributes ColourVisual Motion Shape Sound and Manipulation Func-tional MRI scans performed while participants readthese 900 words and performed a concretenessdecision showed distinct brain networks modulatedby the salience of each attribute as well as multi-levelconvergent areas that responded to various subsetsor all of the attributes Future studies along theselines could incorporate a much broader range of infor-mation types both within the sensory domains andacross sensory and non-sensory domains

Another likely application is in the study of cat-egory-related semantic deficits induced by braindamage Patients with these syndromes show differ-ential abilities to process object concepts from broad(living vs artefact) or more specific (animal toolplant body part musical instrument etc) categoriesThe embodiment account of these phenomenaposits that object categories differ systematically interms of the type of sensoryndashmotor knowledge onwhich they depend Living things for example arecited as having more salient visual attributeswhereas tools and other artefacts have more salientaction-related attributes (Farah amp McClelland 1991Warrington amp McCarthy 1987) As discussed by Gai-notti (Gainotti 2000) greater impairment of knowl-edge about living things is typically associated withanterior ventral temporal lobe damage which is alsoan area that supports high-level visual object percep-tion whereas greater impairment of knowledge abouttools is associated with frontoparietal damage an areathat supports object-directed actions On the otherhand counterexamples have been reported includingpatients with high-level visual perceptual deficits butno selective impairment for living things and patientswith apparently normal visual perception despite aselective living thing impairment (Capitani et al2003)

The current data however support more recentproposals suggesting that category distinctions canarise from much more complex combinations of attri-butes (Cree amp McRae 2003 Gainotti et al 2013Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph 2013 Tranel et al 1997)As exemplified in Figures 3ndash5 concrete object cat-egories differ from each other across multipledomains including attributes related to specificvisual subsystems (visual motion biological motionface perception) sensory systems other than vision(sound taste smell) affective and arousal associationsspatial attributes and various attributes related tosocial cognition Damage to any of these processingsystems or damage to spatially proximate combi-nations of them could account for differential cat-egory impairments As one example the associationbetween living thing impairments and anteriorventral temporal lobe damage may not be due toimpairment of high-level visual knowledge per sebut rather to damage to a zone of convergencebetween high-level visual taste smell and affectivesystems (Gainotti et al 2013)

The current data also clarify the diagnostic attributecomposition of a number of salient categories thathave not received systematic attention in the neurop-sychological literature such as foods musical instru-ments vehicles human roles places events timeconcepts locative change actions and social actionsEach of these categories is defined by a uniquesubset of particularly salient attributes and thus braindamage affecting knowledge of these attributescould conceivably give rise to an associated category-specific impairment Several novel distinctionsemerged from a data-driven cluster analysis of theconcept vectors (Table 8) such as the distinctionbetween social and non-social places verbal andfestive social events and threeother event types (nega-tive sound and ingestive) Although these data-drivenclusters probably reflect idiosyncrasies of the conceptsample to some degree they raise a number of intri-guing possibilities that can be addressed in futurestudies using a wider range of concepts

Application to some general problems incomponential semantic theory

Apart from its utility in empirical neuroscienceresearch a brain-based componential representationmight provide alternative answers to some

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 159

longstanding theoretical issues Several of these arediscussed briefly below

Feature selection

All analytic or componential theories of semantic rep-resentation contend with the general issue of featureselection What counts as a feature and how can theset of features be identified As discussed above stan-dard verbal feature theories face a basic problem withfeature set size in that the number of all possibleobject and action features is extremely large Herewe adopt a fundamentally different approach tofeature selection in which the content of a conceptis not a set of verbal features that people associatewith the concept but rather the set of neural proces-sing modalities (ie representation classes) that areengaged when experiencing instances of theconcept The underlying motivation for this approachis that it enables direct correspondences to be madebetween conceptual content and neural represen-tations whereas such correspondences can only beinferred post hoc from verbal feature sets and onlyby mapping such features to neural processing modal-ities (Cree amp McRae 2003) A consequence of definingconceptual content in terms of brain systems is thatthose systems provide a closed and relatively smallset of basic conceptual components Although theadequacy of these components for capturing finedistinctions in meaning is not yet clear the basicassumption that conceptual knowledge is distributedacross modality-specific neural systems offers a poten-tial solution to the longstanding problem of featureselection

Feature weighting

Standard verbal feature representations also face theproblem of defining the relative importance of eachfeature to a given concept As Murphy and Medin(Murphy amp Medin 1985) put it a zebra and a barberpole can be considered close semantic neighbours ifthe feature ldquohas stripesrdquo is given enough weight Indetermining similarity what justifies the intuitionthat ldquohas stripesrdquo should not be given more weightthan other features Other examples of this problemare instances in which the same feature can beeither critically important or irrelevant for defining aconcept Keil (Keil 1991) made the point as follows

polar bears and washing machines are both typicallywhite Nevertheless an object identical to a polarbear in all respects except colour is probably not apolar bear while an object identical in all respects toa washing machine is still a washing machine regard-less of its colour Whether or not the feature ldquois whiterdquomatters to an itemrsquos conceptual status thus appears todepend on its other propertiesmdashthere is no singleweight that can be given to the feature that willalways work Further complicating this problem isthe fact that in feature production tasks peopleoften neglect to mention some features For instancein listing properties of dogs people rarely say ldquohasbloodrdquo or ldquocan moverdquo or ldquohas DNArdquo or ldquocan repro-ducerdquomdashyet these features are central to our con-ception of animals

Our theory proposes that attributes are weightedaccording to statistical regularities If polar bears arealways experienced as white then colour becomes astrongly weighted attribute of polar bears Colourwould be a strongly weighted attribute of washingmachines if it were actually true that washingmachines are nearly always white In actualityhowever washing machines and most other artefactsusually come in a variety of colours so colour is a lessstrongly weighted attribute of most artefacts A fewartefacts provide counter-examples Stop signs forexample are always red Consequently a sign thatwas not red would be a poor exemplar of a stopsign even if it had the word ldquoStoprdquo on it Schoolbuses are virtually always yellow thus a grey orblack or green bus would be unlikely to be labelleda school bus Semantic similarity is determined byoverall similarity across all attributes rather than by asingle attribute Although barber poles and zebrasboth have salient visual patterns they strongly differin salience on many other attributes (shape complex-ity biological motion body parts and motion throughspace) that would preclude them from being con-sidered semantically similar

Attribute weightings in our theory are obtaineddirectly from human participants by averaging sal-ience ratings provided by the participants Ratherthan asking participants to list the most salient attri-butes for a concept a continuous rating is requiredfor each attribute This method avoids the problemof attentional bias or pragmatic phenomena thatresult in incomplete representations using thefeature listing procedure (Hoffman amp Lambon Ralph

160 J R BINDER ET AL

2013) The resulting attributes are graded rather thanbinary and thus inherently capture weightings Anexample of how this works is given in Figure 11which includes a portion of the attribute vectors forthe concepts egg and bicycle Given that these con-cepts are both inanimate objects they share lowweightings on human-related attributes (biologicalmotion face body part speech social communi-cation emotion and cognition attributes) auditoryattributes and temporal attributes However theyalso differ in expected ways including strongerweightings for egg on brightness colour small sizetactile texture weight taste smell and head actionattributes and stronger weightings for bicycle onmotion fast motion visual complexity lower limbaction and path motion attributes

Abstract concepts

It is not immediately clear how embodiment theoriesof concept representation account for concepts thatare not reliably associated with sensoryndashmotor struc-ture These include obvious examples like justice andpiety for which people have difficulty generatingreliable feature lists and whose exemplars do notexhibit obvious coherent structure They also includesomewhat more concrete kinds of concepts wherethe relevant structure does not clearly reside in per-ception or action Social categories provide oneexample categories like male encompass items thatare grossly perceptually different (consider infantchild and elderly human males or the males of anygiven plant or animal species which are certainly

more similar to the female of the same species thanto the males of different species) categories likeCatholic or Protestant do not appear to reside insensoryndashmotor structure concepts like pauper liaridiot and so on are important for organizing oursocial interactions and inferences about the worldbut refer to economic circumstances behavioursand mental predispositions that are not obviouslyreflected in the perceptual and motor structure ofthe environment In these and many other cases it isdifficult to see how the relevant concepts are transpar-ently reflected in the feature structure of theenvironment

The experiential content and acquisition of abstractconcepts from experience has been discussed by anumber of authors (Altarriba amp Bauer 2004 Barsalouamp Wiemer-Hastings 2005 Borghi et al 2011 Brether-ton amp Beeghly 1982 Crutch et al 2013 Crutch amp War-rington 2005 Crutch et al 2012 Kousta et al 2011Lakoff amp Johnson 1980 Schwanenflugel 1991 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Wiemer-Hastings amp Xu 2005 Zdra-zilova amp Pexman 2013) Although abstract conceptsencompass a variety of experiential domains (spatialtemporal social affective cognitive etc) and aretherefore not a homogeneous set one general obser-vation is that many abstract concepts are learned byexperience with complex situations and events AsBarsalou (Barsalou 1999) points out such situationsoften include multiple agents physical events andmental events This contrasts with concrete conceptswhich typically refer to a single component (usually anobject or action) of a situation In both cases conceptsare learned through experience but abstract concepts

Figure 11Mean ratings for the concepts egg bicycle and agreement [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of thisJournal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 161

differ from concrete concepts in the distribution ofcontent over entities space and time Another wayof saying this is that abstract concepts seem ldquoabstractrdquobecause their content is distributed across multiplecomponents of situations

Another aspect of many abstract concepts is thattheir content refers to aspects of mental experiencerather than to sensoryndashmotor experience Manyabstract concepts refer specifically to cognitiveevents or states (think believe know doubt reason)or to products of cognition (concept theory ideawhim) Abstract concepts also tend to have strongeraffective content than do concrete concepts (Borghiet al 2011 Kousta et al 2011 Troche et al 2014 Vig-liocco et al 2009 Zdrazilova amp Pexman 2013) whichmay explain the selective engagement of anteriortemporal regions by abstract relative to concrete con-cepts in many neuroimaging studies (see Binder 2007Binder et al 2009 for reviews) Many so-calledabstract concepts refer specifically to affective statesor qualities (anger fear sad happy disgust) Onecrucial aspect of the current theory that distinguishesit from some other versions of embodiment theory isthat these cognitive and affective mental experiencescount every bit as much as sensoryndashmotor experi-ences in grounding conceptual knowledge Experien-cing an affective or cognitive state or event is likeexperiencing a sensoryndashmotor event except that theobject of perception is internal rather than externalThis expanded notion of what counts as embodiedexperience adds considerably to the descriptivepower of the conceptual representation particularlyfor abstract concepts

One prominent example of how the model enablesrepresentation of abstract concepts is by inclusion ofattributes that code social knowledge (see alsoCrutch et al 2013 Crutch et al 2012 Troche et al2014) Empirical studies of social cognition have ident-ified several neural systems that appear to be selec-tively involved in supporting theory of mindprocesses ldquoselfrdquo representation and social concepts(Araujo et al 2013 Northoff et al 2006 OlsonMcCoy Klobusicky amp Ross 2013 Ross amp Olson 2010Schilbach et al 2012 Schurz et al 2014 SimmonsReddish Bellgowan amp Martin 2010 Spreng et al2009 Van Overwalle 2009 van Veluw amp Chance2014 Zahn et al 2007) Many abstract words aresocial concepts (cooperate justice liar promise trust)or have salient social content (Borghi et al 2011) An

example of how attributes related to social cognitionplay a role in representing abstract concepts isshown in Figure 11 which compares the attributevectors for egg and bicycle with the attribute vectorfor agreement As the figure shows ratings onsensoryndashmotor attributes are generally low for agree-ment but this concept receives much higher ratingsthan the other concepts on social cognition attributes(Caused Consequential Social Human Communi-cation) It also receives higher ratings on several tem-poral attributes (Duration Long) and on the Cognitionattribute Note also the high rating on the Benefit attri-bute and the small but clearly non-zero rating on thehead action attribute both of which might serve todistinguish agreement from many other abstract con-cepts that are not associated with benefit or withactions of the facehead

Although these and many other examples illustratehow abstract concepts are often tied to particularkinds of mental affective and social experiences wedo not deny that language plays a large role in facili-tating the learning of abstract concepts Languageprovides a rich system of abstract symbols that effi-ciently ldquopoint tordquo complex combinations of externaland internal events enabling acquisition of newabstract concepts by association with older ones Inthe end however abstract concepts like all conceptsmust ultimately be grounded in experience albeitexperiences that are sometimes very complex distrib-uted in space and time and composed largely ofinternal mental events

Context effects and ad hoc categories

The relative importance given to particular attributesof a concept varies with context In the context ofmoving a piano the weight of the piano mattersmore than its function and consequently the pianois likely to be viewed as more similar to a couchthan to a guitar In the context of listening to amusical group the pianorsquos function is more relevantthan its weight and consequently it is more likely tobe conceived as similar to a guitar than to a couch(Barsalou 1982 1983) Componential approaches tosemantic representation assume that the weightgiven to different feature dimensions can be con-trolled by context but the mechanism by whichsuch weight adjustments occur is unclear The possi-bility of learning different weightings for different

162 J R BINDER ET AL

contexts has been explored for simple category learn-ing tasks (Minda amp Smith 2002 Nosofsky 1986) butthe scalability of such an approach to real semanticcognition is questionable and seems ill-suited toexplain the remarkable flexibility with which humanbeings can exploit contextual demands to create andemploy new categories on the spot (Barsalou 1991)

We propose that activation of attribute represen-tations is modulated continuously through two mech-anisms top-down attention and interaction withattributes of the context itself The context drawsattention to context-relevant attributes of a conceptIn the case of lifting or preparing to lift a piano forexample attention is focused on action and proprio-ception processing in the brain and this top-downfocus enhances activation of action and propriocep-tive attributes of the piano This idea is illustrated inhighly simplified schematic form on the left side ofFigure 12 The concept of piano is represented forillustrative purposes by set of verbal features (firstcolumn of ovals red colour online) which are codedin the brain in domain-specific neural processingsystems (second column of ovals green colouronline) The context of lifting draws attention to asubset of these neural systems enhancing their acti-vation Changing the context to playing or listeningto music (right side of Figure 12) shifts attention to adifferent subset of attributes with correspondingenhancement of this subset In addition to effectsmediated by attention there could be direct inter-actions at the neural system level between the distrib-uted representation of the target concept (piano) andthe context concept The concept of lifting for

example is partly encoded in action and proprioceptivesystems that overlap with those encoding piano As dis-cussed in more detail in the following section on con-ceptual combination we propose that overlappingneural representations of two or more concepts canproduce mutual enhancement of the overlappingcomponents

The enhancement of context-relevant attributes ofconcepts alters the relative similarity between conceptsas illustrated by the pianondashcouchndashguitar example givenabove This process not infrequently results in purelyfunctional groupings of objects (eg things one canstand on to reach the top shelf) or ldquoad hoc categoriesrdquo(Barsalou 1983) The above account provides a partialmechanistic account of such categories Ad hoc cat-egories are formed when concepts share the samecontext-related attribute enhancement

Conceptual combination

Language use depends on the ability to productivelycombine concepts to create more specific or morecomplex concepts Some simple examples includemodifierndashnoun (red jacket) nounndashnoun (ski jacket)subjectndashverb (the dog ran) and verbndashobject (hit theball) combinations Semantic processes underlyingconceptual combination have been explored innumerous studies (Clark amp Berman 1987 Conrad ampRips 1986 Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute ampMurphy 1996 Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978Medin amp Shoben 1988 Murphy 1990 Rips Smith ampShoben 1978 Shoben 1991 Smith amp Osherson1984 Springer amp Murphy 1992) We begin here by

Figure 12 Schematic illustration of context effects in the proposed model Neurobiological systems that represent and process con-ceptual attributes are shown in green with font weights indicating relative amounts of processing (ie neural activity) Computationswithin these systems give rise to particular feature representations shown in red As a context is processed this enhances neural activityin the subset of neural systems that represent the context increasing the activation strength of the corresponding features of theconcept [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 163

attempting to define more precisely what kinds ofissues a neural theory of conceptual combinationmight address First among these is the general mech-anism by which conceptual representations interactThis mechanism should explain how new meaningscan ldquoemergerdquo by combining individual concepts thathave very different meanings The concept house forexample has few features in common with theconcept boat yet the combination house boat isnevertheless a meaningful and unique concept thatemerges by combining these constituents Thesecond kind of issue that such a theory shouldaddress are the factors that cause variability in howparticular features interact The concepts house boatand boat house contain the same constituents buthave entirely different meanings indicating that con-stituent role (here modifier vs head noun) is a majorfactor determining how concepts combine Othercombinations illustrate that there are specific seman-tic factors that cause variability in how constituentscombine For example a cancer therapy is a therapyfor cancer but a water therapy is not a therapy forwater In our view it is not the task of a semantictheory to list and describe every possible such combi-nation but rather to propose general principles thatgovern underlying combinatorial processes

As a relatively straightforward illustration of howneural attribute representations might interactduring conceptual combination and an illustrationof the potential explanatory power of such a represen-tation we consider the classical feature animacywhich is a well-recognized determinant of the mean-ingfulness of modifierndashnoun and nounndashverb combi-nations (as well as pronoun selection word orderand aspects of morphology) Standard verbalfeature-based models and semantic models in linguis-tics represent animacy as a binary feature The differ-ence in meaningfulness between (1) and (2) below

(1) the angry boy(2) the angry chair

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animateargument for the modifier angry Similarly the differ-ence in meaningfulness between (3) and (4) below

(1) the boy planned(2) the chair planned

is ldquoexplainedrdquo by a rule that requires an animate argu-ment for the verb plan The weakness of this kind oftheoretical approach is that it amounts to little morethan a very long list of specific rules that are in essence arestatement of the observed phenomena Absent fromsuch a theory are accounts of why particular conceptsldquorequirerdquo animate arguments or evenwhyparticular con-cepts are animate Also unexplained is the well-estab-lished ldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo observed within and acrosslanguages in which adult humans are accorded a rela-tively higher value than infants and some animals fol-lowed by lower animals then plants then non-livingobjects then abstract categories (Yamamoto 2006)suggesting rather strongly that animacy is a graded con-tinuum rather than a binary feature

From a developmental and neurobiological per-spective knowledge about animacy reflects a combi-nation of various kinds of experience includingperception of biological motion perception of causal-ity perception of onersquos own internal affective anddrive states and the development of theory of mindBiological motion perception affords an opportunityto recognize from vision those things that moveunder their own power Movements that are non-mechanical due to higher order complexity are simul-taneously observed in other entities and in onersquos ownmovements giving rise to an understanding (possiblymediated by ldquomirrorrdquo neurons) that these kinds ofmovements are executed by the moving object itselfrather than by an external controlling force Percep-tion of causality beginning with visual perception ofsimple collisions and applied force vectors and pro-gressing to multimodal and higher order events pro-vides a basis for understanding how biologicalmovements can effect change Perception of internaldrive states and of sequences of events that lead tosatisfaction of these needs provides a basis for under-standing how living agents with needs effect changesThis understanding together with the recognition thatother living things in the environment effect changesto meet their own needs provides a basis for theory ofmind On this account the construct ldquoanimacyrdquo farfrom being a binary symbolic property arises fromcomplex and interacting sensory motor affectiveand cognitive experiences

How might knowledge about animacy be rep-resented and interact across lexical items at a neurallevel As suggested schematically in Figure 13 wepropose that interactions occur when two concepts

164 J R BINDER ET AL

activate a similar set of modal networks and theyoccur less extensively when there is less attributeoverlap In the case of animacy for example a nounthat engages biological motion face and body partaffective and social cognition networks (eg ldquoboyrdquo)will produce more extensive neural interaction witha modifier that also activates these networks (egldquoangryrdquo) than will a noun that does not activatethese networks (eg ldquochairrdquo)

To illustrate how such differences can arise evenfrom a simple multiplicative interaction we examinedthe vectors that result frommultiplying mean attributevectors of modifierndashnoun pairs with varying degreesof meaningfulness Figure 14 (bottom) shows theseinteraction values for the pairs ldquoangryboyrdquo andldquoangrychairrdquo As shown in the figure boy and angryinteract as anticipated showing enhanced values forattributes concerned with biological motion faceand body part perception speech production andsocial and human characteristics whereas interactionsbetween chair and angry are negligible Averaging theinteraction values across all attributes gives a meaninteraction value of 326 for angryboy and 083 forangrychair

Figure 15 shows the average of these mean inter-action values for 116 nouns divided by taxonomic cat-egory The averages roughly approximate theldquoanimacy hierarchyrdquo with strong interactions fornouns that refer to people (boy girl man womanfamily couple etc) and human occupation roles

(doctor lawyer politician teacher etc) much weakerinteractions for animal concepts and the weakestinteractions for plants

The general principle suggested by such data is thatconcepts acquired within similar neural processingdomains are more likely to have similar semantic struc-tures that allow combination In the case of ldquoanimacyrdquo(whichwe interpret as a verbal label summarizing apar-ticular pattern of attribute weightings rather than an apriori binary feature) a common semantic structurecaptures shared ldquohuman-relatedrdquo attributes that allowcertain concepts to be meaningfully combined Achair cannot be angry because chairs do not havemovements faces or minds that are essential forfeeling and expressing anger and this observationapplies to all concepts that lack these attributes Thepower of a comprehensive attribute representationconstrained by neurobiological and cognitive develop-mental data is that it has the potential to provide a first-principles account of why concepts vary in animacy aswell as a mechanism for predicting which conceptsshould ldquorequirerdquo animate arguments

We have focused here on animacy because it is astrong and at the same time a relatively complexand poorly understood factor influencing conceptualcombination Similar principles might conceivably beapplied however in other difficult cases For thecancer therapy versus water therapy example givenabove the difference in meaning that results fromthe two combinations is probably due to the fact

Figure 13 Schematic illustration of conceptual combination in the proposed model Combination occurs when concepts share over-lapping neural attribute representations The concepts angry and boy share attributes that define animate concepts [To view this figurein colour please see the online version of this Journal]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 165

that cancer is a negatively valenced concept whereaswater and therapy are usually thought of as beneficialThis difference results in very different constituentrelations (therapy for vs therapy with) A similarexample is the difference between lake house anddog house A lake house is a house located at a lakebut a dog house is not a house located at a dogThis difference reflects the fact that both houses andlakes have fixed locations (ie do not move and canbe used as topographic landmarks) whereas this isnot true of dogs The general principle in these casesis that the meaning of a combination is strongly deter-mined by attribute congruence When Concepts A andB have opposite congruency relations with Concept C

the combinations AC and BC are likely to capture verydifferent constituent relationships The content ofthese relationships reflects the underlying attributestructure of the constituents as demonstrated bythe locative relationship located at arising from thecombination lake house but not dog house

At the neural level interactions during conceptualcombination are likely to take the form of additionalprocessing within the neural systems where attributerepresentations overlap This additional processing isnecessary to compute the ldquonewrdquo attribute represen-tations resulting from conceptual combination andthese new representations tend to have addedsalience As a simple example involving unimodal

Figure 15 Average mean interaction values for combinations of angry with a noun for eight noun categories Interactions are higherfor human concepts (people and occupation roles) than for any of the non-human concepts (all p lt 001) Error bars represent standarderror of the mean

Figure 14 Top Mean attribute ratings for boy chair and angry Bottom Interaction values for the combinations angry boy and angrychair [To view this figure in colour please see the online version of this Journal]

166 J R BINDER ET AL

modifiers imagine what happens to the neural rep-resentation of dog following the modifier brown orthe modifier loud In the first case there is residual acti-vation of the colour processor by brown which whencombined with the neural representation of dogcauses additional computation (ie additional neuralactivity) in the colour processor because of the inter-action between the colour representations of brownand dog In the second case there is residual acti-vation of the auditory processor by loud whichwhen combined with the neural representation ofdog causes additional computation in the auditoryprocessor because of the interaction between theauditory representations of loud and dog As men-tioned in the previous section on context effects asimilar mechanism is proposed (along with attentionalmodulation) to underlie situational context effectsbecause the context situation also has a conceptualrepresentation Attributes of the target concept thatare ldquorelevantrdquo to the context are those that overlapwith the conceptual representation of the contextand this overlap results in additional processor-specific activation Seen in this way situationalcontext effects (eg lifting vs playing a piano) areessentially a special case of conceptual combination

Scope and limitations of the theory

We have made a systematic attempt to relate seman-tic content to large-scale brain networks and biologi-cally plausible accounts of concept acquisition Theapproach offers potential solutions to several long-standing problems in semantic representation par-ticularly the problems of feature selection featureweighting and specification of abstract wordcontent The primary aim of the theory is to betterunderstand lexical semantic representation in thebrain and specifically to develop a more comprehen-sive theory of conceptual grounding that encom-passes the full range of human experience

Although we have proposed several general mechan-isms that may explain context and combinatorial effectsmuchmorework isneeded toachieveanaccountofwordcombinations and syntactic effects on semantic compo-sition Our simple attribute-based account of why thelocative relation AT arises from lake house for exampledoes not explain why house lake fails despite the factthat both nouns have fixed locations A plausible expla-nation is that the syntactic roles of the constituents

specify a headndashmodifier = groundndashfigure isomorphismthat requires the modifier concept to be larger in sizethan the head noun concept In principle this behaviourcould be capturedby combining the size attribute ratingsfor thenounswith informationabout their respective syn-tactic roles Previous studies have shown such effects tovary widely in terms of the types of relationships thatarise between constituents and the ldquorulesrdquo that governtheir lawful combination (Clark amp Berman 1987Downing 1977 Gagneacute 2001 Gagneacute amp Murphy 1996Gagneacute amp Shoben 1997 Levi 1978 Medin amp Shoben1988 Murphy 1990) We assume that many other attri-butes could be involved in similar interactions

The explanatory power of a semantic representationmodel that refers only to ldquotypes of experiencerdquo (ie onethat does not incorporate all possible verbally gener-ated features) is still unknown It is far from clear forexample that an intuitively basic distinction like malefemale can be captured by such a representation Themodel proposes that the concepts male and femaleas applied to human adults can be distinguished onthe basis of sensory affective and social experienceswithout reference to specific encyclopaedic featuresThis account appears to fail however when male andfemale are applied to animals plants or electronic con-nectors in which case there is little or no overlap in theexperiences associated with these entities that couldexplain what is male or female about all of themSuch cases illustrate the fact that much of conceptualknowledge is learned directly via language and withonly very indirect grounding in experience Incommon with other embodied cognition theorieshowever our model maintains that all concepts are ulti-mately grounded in experience whether directly orindirectly through language that refers to experienceEstablishing the validity utility and limitations of thisprinciple however will require further study

The underlying research materials for this articlecan be accessed at httpwwwneuromcweduresourceshtml

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors

Funding

This work was supported by the Intelligence AdvancedResearch Projects Activity (IARPA) [grant number FA8650-14-C-7357]

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 167

References

Albright T D Desimone R amp Gross C G (1984) Columnarorganization of directionally selective cells in visual areaMT of the macaque Journal of Neurophysiology 51 16ndash31

Allison T Puce A amp McCarthy G (2000) Social perceptionfrom visual cues Role of the STS region Trends in CognitiveSciences 4 267ndash278

Allport D A (1985) Distributed memory modular subsystemsand dysphasia In S K Newman amp R Epstein (Eds) Currentperspectives in dysphasia (pp 207ndash244) EdinburghChurchill Livingstone

Altarriba J amp Bauer L M (2004) The distinctiveness of emotionconcepts A comparison between emotion abstract and con-crete words The American Journal of Psychology 117 389ndash410

Amorapanth P X Widick P amp Chatterjee A (2010) The neuralbasis for spatial relations Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience22 1739ndash1753

Anders S Eippert F Weiskopf N amp Veit R (2008) The humanamygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and soundsirrespective of arousal An fMRI study Social Cognitve andAffective Neuroscience 3 233ndash243

Anders S Lotze M Erb M Grodd W amp Birbaumer N (2004)Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal Aresponse-related fMRI study Human Brain Mapping 23200ndash209

Andersen R A Snyder L H Bradley D C amp Xing J (1997)Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietalcortex and its use in planning movements Annual Review ofNeuroscience 20 303ndash330

Araujo H F Kaplan J amp Damasio A (2013) Cortical midlinestructures and autobiographical-self processes An acti-vation-likelihood estimation meta-analysis Frontiers inHuman Neuroscience 7 548

Aristotle (1995350 BCE) Categories (J L Ackrill Trans) InJ Barnes (Ed) The complete works of Aristotle (pp 3ndash24)Princeton Princeton University Press

Baayen R H Piepenbrock R amp Gulikers L (1995) The CELEXlexical database (CD-ROM) (25 ed) Philadelphia LinguisticData Consortium University of Pennsylvania

Badre D amp DrsquoEsposito M (2007) Functional magnetic reson-ance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization inthe prefrontal cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 192082ndash2099

Barlow H B (1972) Single units and sensation A neuron doc-trine for perceptual psychology Perception 1 371ndash394

Barrett L F (2006) Are emotions natural kinds Perspectives onPsychological Science 1 28ndash58

Barsalou L W (1982) Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts Memory and Cognition10 82ndash93

Barsalou L W (1983) Ad hoc categories Memory amp Cognition11 211ndash227

Barsalou L W (1991) Deriving categories to achieve goals InG H Bower (Ed) The psychology of learning and motivationAdvances in research and theory (Vol 27 pp 1ndash64) San DiegoCA Academic Press

Barsalou L W (1999) Perceptual symbol systems Behavioraland Brain Sciences 22 577ndash660

Barsalou L W amp Wiemer-Hastings K (2005) Situating abstractconcepts In D Pecher amp R Zwaan (Eds) Grounding cognitionThe role of perception and action in memory language andthought (pp 129ndash163) New York Cambridge UniversityPress

Bartels A amp Zeki S M (2000) The architecture of the colourcentre in the human visual brain New results and a reviewEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 12 172ndash193

Baucom L B Wedell D H Wang J Blitzer D N amp ShinkarevaS V (2012) Decoding the neural representation of affectivestates Neuroimage 59 718ndash727

Beauchamp M S Haxby J V Jennings J E amp DeYoe E A(1999) An fMRI version of the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-huetest reveals multiple color-sensitive areas in human ventraloccipitotemporal cortex Cerebral Cortex 9 257ndash263

Belin P Zatorre R J Lafaille P Ahad P amp Pike B (2000)Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex Nature 403309ndash312

Berlin B amp Kay P (1969) Basic color terms Their universality andevolution Berkeley University of California Press

Binder J R (2007) Effects of word imageability on semanticaccess Neuroimaging studies In J Hart amp M A Kraut(Eds) Neural basis of semantic memory (pp 149ndash181)Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press

Binder J R amp Desai R H (2011) The neurobiology of semanticmemory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 527ndash536

Binder J R Desai R Conant L L amp Graves W W (2009)Where is the semantic system A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies CerebralCortex 19 2767ndash2796

Bird H Franklin S amp Howard D (2001) Age of acquisition andimageability ratings for a large set of words including verbsand function words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 33 73ndash79

Blos J Chatterjee A Kircher T amp Straube B (2012) Neuralcorrelates of causality judgment in physical and socialcontext The reversed effects of space and timeNeuroimage 63 882ndash893

Borghi A M Flumini A Cimatti F Marocco D amp Scorolli C(2011) Manipulating objects and telling words a study onconcrete and abstract words acquisition Frontiers inPsychology 2 15

Boronat C B Buxbaum L J Coslett H B Tang K Saffran EM Kimberg D Y amp Detre J A (2005) Distinctions betweenmanipulation and function knowledge of objects Evidencefrom functional magnetic resonance imaging CognitiveBrain Research 23 361ndash373

Bowers J S (2009) On the biological plausibility of grand-mother cells Implications for neural network theories in psy-chology and neuroscience Psychological Review 116 220ndash251

Bretherton I amp Beeghly M (1982) Talking about internalstates The acquisition of an explicit theory of mindDevelopmental Psychology 18 906ndash921

168 J R BINDER ET AL

Burgess N (2008) Spatial cognition and the brain Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences 1124 77ndash97

Calvo-Merino B Glaser D E Grezes J Passingham R E ampHaggard P (2005) Action observation and acquired motorskills An fMRI study with expert dancers Cerebral Cortex15 1243ndash1249

Capitani E Laiacona M Mahon B amp Caramazza A (2003)What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits Acritical review of the clinical evidence CognitiveNeuropsychology 20 213ndash261

Carota F Moseley R amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Body part-specific representations of semantic noun categoriesJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 1492ndash1509

Carter RM ampHuettel S A (2013) A nexusmodel of the temporal-parietal junction Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 328ndash336

Chow H M Mar R A Xu Y Liu S Wagage S amp Braun A R(2013) Embodied comprehension of stories Interactionsbetween language regions and modality-specific neuralsystems Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 279ndash295

Clark E amp Berman R (1987) Types of linguistic knowledgeInterpreting and producing compound nouns Journal ofChild Language 14 547ndash567

Clark J M amp Paivio A (2004) Extensions of the Paivio Yuilleand Madigan (1968) norms Behavior Research MethodsInstruments amp Computers 36 371ndash383

Colibazzi T Posner J Wang Z Gorman D Gerber A Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2010) Neural systems subserving valenceand arousal during the experience of induced emotionsEmotion 10 377ndash389

Conrad F amp Rips L (1986) Conceptual combination and thegiven-new distinction Journal of Memory and Language25 255ndash278

Conway B R amp Tsao D Y (2009) Color-tuned neurons arespatially clustered according to color preference withinalert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of theUnited States of America 106 18034ndash18039

Cortese M J amp Fugett A (2004) Imageability ratings for 3000monosyllabic words Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsand Computers 36 384ndash387

Coslett H B Saffran E M amp Schwoebel J (2002) Knowledgeof the body A distinct semantic domain Neurology 59 359ndash363

Cree G S amp McRae K (2003) Analyzing the factors underlyingthe structure and computation of the meaning of chipmunkcherry chisel cheese and cello (and many other such con-crete nouns) Journal of Experimental Psychology General132 163ndash201

Crutch S J Troche J Reilly J amp Ridgway G R (2013) Abstractconceptual feature ratings the role of emotion magnitudeand other cognitive domains in the organization of abstractconceptual knowledge Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 186

Crutch S J amp Warrington E K (2005) Abstract and concreteconcepts have structurally different representational frame-works Brain 128 615ndash627

Crutch S J Williams P Ridgway G R amp Borgenicht L (2012)The role of polarity in antonym and synonym conceptualknowledge Evidence from stroke aphasia and multidimen-sional ratings of abstract words Neuropsychologia 502636ndash2644

Culham J C Brandt S A Cavanagh P Kanwisher N G DaleA M amp Tootell R B (1998) Cortical fMRI activation producedby attentive tracking of moving targets Journal ofNeurophysiology 80 2657ndash2670

Cunningham W A Raye C L amp Johnson M K (2004) Implicitand explicit evaluation fMRI correlates of valence emotionalintensity and control in the processing of attitudes Journalof Cognitive Neuroscience 16 1717ndash1729

Dehaene S (1997) The number sense How the mind createsmathematics New York Oxford University Press

Denny B T Kober H Wager T D amp Ochsner K N (2012) Ameta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self-and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentaliz-ing in medial prefrontal cortex Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 24 1742ndash1752

Derrfuss J Brass M Neumann J amp von Cramon D Y (2005)Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitivecontrol Meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studiesHuman Brain Mapping 25 22ndash34

Desai R Binder J R Conant L L amp Seidenberg M S (2009)Activation of sensory-motor and visual areas by sentencecomprehension Cerebral Cortex 20 468ndash478

Diekhof E K Kaps L Falkai P amp Gruber O (2012) Therole of the human ventral striatum and the medial orbi-tofrontal cortex in the representation of reward magni-tudemdashAn activation likelihood estimation meta-analysisof neuroimaging studies of passive reward expectancyand outcome processing Neuropsychologia 50 1252ndash1266

Downing P (1977) On the creation and use of English com-pound nouns Language 53 810ndash842

Downing P E Jiang Y Shuman M amp Kanwisher N (2001) Acortical area selective for visual processing of the humanbody Science 293 2470ndash2473

Duncan J amp Owen A M (2000) Common regions of thehuman frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitivedemands Trends in Neurosciences 23 475ndash483

Eisenberger N I (2012) The neural bases of social painEvidence for shared representations with physical painPsychosomatic Medicine 74 126ndash135

Ekman P (1992) An argument for basic emotions Cognitionand Emotion 6 169ndash200

Epstein R amp Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation ofthe local visual environment Nature 392 598ndash601

Epstein R A Parker W E amp Feiler A M (2007) Where am Inow Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenialcortices in place recognition Journal of Neuroscience 276141ndash6149

Etkin A Egner T amp Kalisch R (2011) Emotional processing inanterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex Trends inCognitive Sciences 15 85ndash93

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 169

Evans V (2004) The structure of time Language meaning andtemporal cognition Amsterdam John Benjamins

Farah M J amp McClelland J L (1991) A computational model ofsemantic memory impairment Modality specificity andemergent category specificity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology General 120 339ndash357

Farmer T A Christiansen M H amp Monaghan P (2006)Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence compre-hension Proceedings of the National Academy of SciencesUSA 103 12203ndash12208

Fernandino L Binder J R Desai R H Pendl S L HumphriesC J Gross Whellip Seidenberg M S (2015) Concept rep-resentation reflects multimodal abstraction A frameworkfor embodied semantics Cerebral Cortex published onlineMarch 5 2015 doi101093cercorbhv020

Ferstl E C amp von Cramon D Y (2007) Time space andemotion fMRI reveals content-specific activation duringtext comprehension Neuroscience Letters 427 159ndash164

Ferstl E C Rinck M amp von Cramon D Y (2005) Emotional andtemporal aspects of situation model processing during textcomprehension An event-related fMRI study Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 724ndash739

Fonlupt P (2003) Perception and judgement of physical caus-ality involve different brain structures Cognitive BrainResearch 17 248ndash254

Forde E M E amp Humphreys G W (1999) Category-specificrecognition impairments a review of important casestudies and influential theories Aphasiology 13 169ndash193

Friebel U Eickhoff S B amp Lotze M (2011) Coordinate-basedmeta-analysis of experimentally induced and chronic persist-ent neuropathic pain Neuroimage 58 1070ndash1080

Fugelsang J A Roser M E Corballis P M Gazzaniga M S ampDunbar K N (2005) Brain mechanisms underlying percep-tual causality Cognitive Brain Research 24 41ndash47

Gagneacute C L (2001) Relation and lexical priming during theinterpretation of noun-noun combinations Journal ofExperimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition27 236ndash254

Gagneacute C L amp Murphy G L (1996) Influence of discoursecontext on feature availability in conceptual combinationDiscourse Processes 22 79ndash101

Gagneacute C L amp Shoben E J (1997) Influence of thematicrelations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combi-nations Journal of Experimental Psychology LearningMemory and Cognition 23 71ndash87

Gainotti G (2000) What the locus of brain lesion tells us aboutthe nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders A review Cortex 36 539ndash559

Gainotti G Ciaraffa F Silveri M C amp Marra C (2009) Mentalrepresentation of normal subjects about the sources ofknowledge in different semantic categories and unique enti-ties Neuropsychology 23 803ndash812

Gainotti G Spinelli P Scaricamazza E amp Marra C (2013) Theevaluation of sources of knowledge underlying differentconceptual categories Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7Article 40

Garrard P Lambon Ralph M A Hodges J R amp Patterson K(2001) Prototypicality distinctiveness and intercorrelationAnalyses of the semantic attributes of living and nonlivingconcepts Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 125ndash174

Gerber A J Posner J Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu S Wang Zhellip Peterson B S (2008) An affective circumplex model ofneural systems subserving valence arousal and cognitiveoverlay during the appraisal of emotional facesNeuropsychologia 46 2129ndash2139

Glasgow K Roos M Haufler A Chevillet M amp Wolmetz M(2016) Evaluating semantic models with word-sentencerelatedness arXiv160307253 Retrieved from httparxivorgabs160307253 [csCL]

Goldberg R F Perfetti C A amp Schneider W (2006) Perceptualknowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions Journalof Neuroscience 26 4917ndash4921

Gonzaacutelez J Barros-Loscertales A Pulvermuumlller F MeseguerV Sanjuaacuten A Belloch V amp Aacutevila C (2006) Reading cinna-mon activates olfactory brain regions Neuroimage 32906ndash912

Graziano M S Yap G S amp Gross C G (1994) Coding of visualspace by premotor neurons Science 266 1054ndash1057

Grill-Spector K amp Malach R (2004) The human visual cortexAnnual Review of Neuroscience 27 649ndash677

Grondin S (2010) Timing and time perception A review ofrecent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoreticaldirections Attention Perception and Psychophysics 72 561ndash582

Grossman E D amp Blake R (2002) Brain areas active duringvisual perception of biological motion Neuron 35 1167ndash1175

Hamann S (2012) Mapping discrete and dimensionalemotions onto the brain controversies and consensusTrends in Cognitive Sciences 16 458ndash466

Harnad S (1990) The symbol grounding problem Physica DNonlinear Phenomena 42 335ndash346

Harvey B M Klein B P Petridou N amp Dumoulin S O (2013)Topographic representation of numerosity in the humanparietal cortex Science 6150 1123ndash1128

Hasson U Levy I Behrmann M Hendler T amp Malach R(2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle forhuman high-order object areas Neuron 34 479ndash490

Hauk O Johnsrude I amp Pulvermuller F (2004) Somatotopicrepresentation of action words in human motor and pre-motor cortex Neuron 41 301ndash307

Hendry S H C Hsiao S S amp Bushnell M C (1999) Somaticsensation In M J Zigmond F E Bloom S C Landis J LRoberts amp L R Squire (Eds) Fundamental neuroscience (pp761ndash789) San Diego Academic Press

Hoffman P amp Lambon Ralph M A (2013) Shapes scents andsounds Quantifying the full multi-sensory basis of concep-tual knowledge Neuropsychologia 51 14ndash25

Horn J (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors infactor analysis Psychometrika 30 179ndash185

Hsu N S Frankland S M amp Thompson-Schill S L (2012)Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowl-edge Neuropsychologia 50 327ndash333

170 J R BINDER ET AL

Hsu N S Kraemer D Oliver R Schlichting M L amp Thompson-Schill S L (2011) Color context and cognitive styleVariations in color knowledge retrieval as a function oftask and subject variables Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 23 1ndash14

Jackendoff R (1990) Semantic structures Cambridge MA MITPress

Jaumlncke L Koeneke S Hoppe A Rominger C amp Haumlnggi J(2009) The architecture of the golferrsquos brain PLoS ONE 4e4785

Kanwisher N McDermott J amp Chun M M (1997) The fusiformface area A module in human extrastriate cortex specializedfor face perception Journal of Neuroscience 17 4302ndash4311

Kassam K S Markey A R Cherkassky V L Loewenstein G ampJust M A (2013) Identifying emotions on the basis of neuralactivation PLoS One 8 e66032ndashe66032

Keil F (1991) The emergence of theoretical beliefs as con-straints on concepts In S C Gelman amp R Gelman (Eds)The epigenesis of mind Essays on biology and cognition (pp237ndash256) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Kellenbach M L Brett M amp Patterson K (2001) Large colour-ful or noisy Attribute- and modality-specific activationsduring retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledgeCognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 1 207ndash221

Kelly M H (1992) Using sound to solve syntactic problems Therole of phonology in grammatical category assignmentsPsychological Review 99 349ndash364

Kemmerer D (2006) The semantics of space Integratinglinguistic typology and cognitive neuroscienceNeuropsychologia 44 1607ndash1621

Kemmerer D amp Tranel D (2008) Searching for the elusiveneural substrates of body part terms A neuropsychologicalstudy Cognitive Neuropsychology 25 601ndash629

Kiefer M amp Pulvermuumlller F (2012) Conceptual representationsin mind and brain Theoretical developments current evi-dence and future directions Cortex 48 805ndash825

Kiefer M Sim E-J Herrnberger B Grothe J amp Hoenig K(2008) The sound of concepts Four markers for a linkbetween auditory and conceptual brain systems Journal ofNeuroscience 28 12224ndash12230

Kober H Barrett L F Joseph J Bliss-Moreau E Lindquist Kamp Wager T D (2008) Functional grouping and cortical-sub-cortical interactions in emotion A meta-analysis of neuroi-maging studies Neuroimage 42 998ndash1031

Konkle T amp Oliva A (2012) A real-world size organization ofobject responses in occipitotemporal cortex Neuron 741114ndash1124

Kousta S-T Vigliocco G Vinson D P Andrews M amp DelCampo E (2011) The representation of abstract wordsWhy emotion matters Journal of Experimental PsychologyGeneral 140 14ndash34

Kragel P A amp LaBar K S (2014) Advancing emotion theory withmultivariate pattern classification Emotion Review 6 160ndash174

Kranjec A Cardillo E R Schmidt G L Lehet M amp Chatterjee A(2012) Deconstructing events The neural bases forspace time and causality Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience24 1ndash16

Kranjec A amp Chatterjee A (2010) Are temporal conceptsembodied A challenge for cognitive neuroscienceFrontiers in Psychology 1 1ndash9

Kuchinke L Jacobs A M Grubich C Vo M L H Conrad M ampHerrmann M (2005) Incidental effects of emotional valencein single word processing An fMRI study Neuroimage 281022ndash1032

Kuiper N A amp Rogers T B (1979) Encoding of personal infor-mation self-other differences Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology 37 499ndash514

Laiacona M Allamano N Lorenzi L amp Capitani E (2006) Acase of impaired naming and knowledge of body partsAre limbs a separate category Neurocase 12 307ndash316

Lakoff G amp Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by ChicagoUniversity of Chicago Press

Lamm C Decety J amp Singer T (2011) Meta-analytic evidencefor common and distinct neural networks associated withdirectly experienced pain and empathy for painNeuroimage 54 2492ndash2502

Landau B amp Jackendoff R (1993) What and where in spatiallanguage and spatial cognition Behavioral and BrainSciences 16 217ndash238

Landauer T K amp Dumais S T (1997) A solution to Platorsquosproblem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisitioninduction and representation of knowledge PsychologicalReview 104 211ndash240

Landauer T K Kintsch W amp the Science and Applicationsof Latent Semantic Analysis (SALSA) Group (2015) LSA appli-cations Boulder CO University of Colorado Retrieved fromhttplsacoloradoedu

Leaver A M amp Rauschecker J P (2010) Cortical representationof natural complex sounds Effects of acoustic features andauditory object category Journal of Neuroscience 30 7604ndash7612

Lench H C Flores S a amp Bench S W (2011) Discreteemotions predict changes in cognition judgment experi-ence behavior and physiology A meta-analysis of exper-imental emotion elicitations Psychological Bulletin 137834ndash855

Levi J (1978) The syntax and semantics of complex nominalsNew York Academic Press

Levy D J amp Glimcher P W (2012) The root of all value Aneural common currency for choice Current Opinion inNeurobiology 22 1027ndash1038

Lewis J W Wightman F L Brefczynski J A Phinney R EBinder J R amp DeYoe E A (2004) Human brain regionsinvolved in recognizing environmental sounds CerebralCortex 14 1008ndash1021

Lewis P A Critchley H D Rotshtein P amp Dolan R J (2007)Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affec-tive words Cerebral Cortex 17 742ndash748

Liebenthal E Binder J R Spitzer S M Possing E T amp MedlerD A (2005) Neural substrates of phonemic perceptionCerebral Cortex 15 1621ndash1631

Lindquist K A Siegel E H Quigley K S amp Barrett L F (2013)The hundred-year emotion war are emotions natural kinds

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 171

or psychological constructions Comment on Lench Floresand Bench (2011) Psychological Bulletin 139 255ndash263

Lindquist K A Wager T D Kober H Bliss-Moreau E ampBarrett L F (2012) The brain basis of emotion A meta-ana-lytic review Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35 121ndash143

Lingnau A Ashida H Wall M B amp Smith A T (2009) Speedencoding in human visual cortex revealed by fMRI adap-tation Journal of Vision 9 3

Longo M Azanon E amp Haggard P (2010) More than skindeep Body representation beyond primary somatosensorycortex Neuropsychologia 48 655ndash668

Lynott D amp Connell L (2009) Modality exclusivity norms for423 object properties Behavior Research Methods 41 558ndash564

Lynott D amp Connell L (2013) Modality exclusivity norms for400 nouns The relationship between perceptual experienceand surface word form Behavior Research Methods 45 516ndash526

Maguire E A Frith C D Burgess N Donnett J G amp OrsquoKeefe J(1998) Knowing where things are Parahippocampal involve-ment in encoding object locations in virtual large-scalespace Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 61ndash76

Makin T R Holmes N P amp Zohary E (2007) Is that near myhand Multisensory representation of peripersonal space inhuman intraparietal sulcus Journal of Neuroscience 27731ndash740

Malach R Reppas J B Benson R R Kwong K K Jiang HKennedy W Ahellip Tootell R B (1995) Object-related activityrevealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging inhuman occipital cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 92 8135ndash8139

Martin A amp Caramazza A (2003) Neuropsychological and neu-roimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge An intro-duction Cognitive Neuropsychology 20 195ndash212

Martin A Haxby J V Lalonde F M Wiggs C L amp UngerleiderL G (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowl-edge of color and knowledge of action Science 270 102ndash105

Mazlow A H (1943) A theory of human motivationPsychological Review 50 370ndash396

Mazzola L Faillenot I Barral F G Mauguiere F amp Peyron R(2012) Spatial segregation of somato-sensory and pain acti-vations in the human operculo-insular cortex Neuroimage60 409ndash418

McGlone F amp Reilly D (2010) The cutaneous sensory systemNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 148ndash159

McRae K Cree G S Seidenberg M S amp McNorgan C (2005)Semantic feature norms for a large set of living and nonlivingthings Behavior Research Methods Instruments amp Computers37 547ndash559

McRae K de Sa V R amp Seidenberg M S (1997) On the natureand scope of featural representations of word meaningJournal of Experimental Psychology General 126 99ndash130

Medin D L amp Shoben E J (1988) Context and structure inconceptual combination Cognitive Psychology 20 158ndash190

Medina J amp Coslett H B (2010) From maps to form to spaceTouch and the body schema Neuropsychologia 48 645ndash654

Meteyard L Rodriguez Cuadrado S Bahrami B amp Vigliocco G(2012) Coming of age A review of embodiment and theneuroscience of semantics Cortex 48 788ndash804

Minda J P amp Smith J D (2002) Comparing prototype-basedand exemplar-based accounts of category learning andattentional allocation Journal of Experimental PsychologyLearning Memory and Cognition 28 275ndash292

Miqueacutee A Xerri C Rainville C Anton J L Nazarian B RothM amp Zennou-Azogui Y (2008) Neuronal substrates of hapticshape encoding and matching A functional magnetic reson-ance imaging study Neuroscience 152 29ndash39

Murphy G (1990) Noun phrase interpretation and conceptualcombination Journal of Memory and Language 29 259ndash288

Murphy G amp Medin D (1985) The role of theories in concep-tual coherence Psychological Review 92 289ndash316

Nakic M Smith B W Busis S Vythilingam M amp Blair R J R(2006) The impact of affect and frequency on lexicaldecision The role of the amygdala and inferior frontalcortex Neuroimage 31 1752ndash1761

Nielen M M A Heslenfeld D J Heinen K Van Strien J WWitter M P Jonker C amp Veltman D J (2009) Distinctbrain systems underlie the processing of valence andarousal of affective pictures Brain and Cognition 71 387ndash396

Noordzij M L Neggers S F W Ramsey N F amp Postma A(2008) Neural correlates of locative prepositionsNeuropsychologia 46 1576ndash1580

Northoff G Heinzel A de Greck M Bermpohl F DobrowolnyH amp Panksepp J (2006) Self-referential processing in ourbrainndasha meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroimage 31 440ndash457

Nosofsky R M (1986) Attention similiarity and the identifi-cation-categorization relationship Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Learning Memory and Cognition 115 39ndash57

Nyberg L Marklund P Persson J Cabeza R Forkstam CPetersson K amp Ingvar M (2003) Common prefrontal acti-vations during working memory episodic memory andsemantic memory Neuropsychologia 41 371ndash377

OrsquoConnor B P (2000) SPSS and SAS programs for determiningthe number of components using parallel analysis andVelicerrsquos MAP test Behavior Research Methods Instrumentsamp Computers 32 396ndash402

Olson I R McCoy D Klobusicky E amp Ross L A (2013) Socialcognition and the anterior temporal lobes A review andtheoretical framework Social Cognitive amp AffectiveNeuroscience 8 123ndash133

Peelen M V Atkinson A P amp Vuilleumier P (2010)Supramodal representations of perceived emotions in thehuman brain Journal of Neuroscience 30 10127ndash10134

Pelphrey K A Morris J P Michelich C R Allison T ampMcCarthy G (2005) Functional anatomy of biologicalmotion perception in posterior temporal cortex An fMRIstudy of eye mouth and hand movements Cerebral Cortex15 1866ndash1876

Peters J amp Buumlchel C (2010) Neural representations ofsubjective reward value Behavioural Brain Research 213135ndash141

172 J R BINDER ET AL

Phan K L Wager T Taylor S F amp Liberzon I (2002) Functionalneuroanatomy of emotion A meta-analysis of emotion acti-vation studies in PET and fMRI Neuroimage 16 331ndash348

Piazza M Izard V Pinel P Bihan D L amp Dehaene S (2004)Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the humanintraparietal sulcus Neuron 44 547ndash555

Posner J Russell J A Gerber A Gorman D Colibazzi T Yu Shellip Peterson B S (2009) The neurophysiological bases ofemotion An fMRI study of the affective circumplex usingemotion-denoting words Human Brain Mapping 30 883ndash895

Posner J Russell J A amp Peterson B S (2005) The circumplexmodel of affect An integrative approach to affective neuro-science cognitive development and psychopathologyDevelopment and Psychopathology 17 715ndash734

Postle N McMahon K L Ashton R Meredith M amp deZubicaray G I (2008) Action word meaning representationsin cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cor-tices Neuroimage 43 634ndash644

Rees G Friston K J amp Koch C (2000) A direct quantitativerelationship between the functional properties of humanand macaque V5 Nature Neuroscience 3 716ndash723

Rips L Smith E amp Shoben E (1978) Semantic composition insentence verification Journal of Verbal Learning and VerbalBehavior 17 375ndash401

Rogers T B Kuiper N A amp Kirker W S (1977) Self-referenceand the encoding of personal information Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 35 677ndash688

Roumlhl M amp Uppenkamp S (2012) Neural coding of sound inten-sity and loudness in the human auditory system Journal ofthe Association for Research in Otolaryngology 13 369ndash379

Rolls E T amp Grabenhorst F (2008) The orbitofrontal cortex andbeyond From affect to decision-making Progress inNeurobiology 86 216ndash244

Rosch E Mervis C B Gray W D Johnson D M amp Boyes-Braem D (1976) Basic objects in natural categoriesCognitive Psychology 8 382ndash439

Ross L A amp Olson I R (2010) Social cognition and the anteriortemporal lobes Neuroimage 49 3452ndash3462

Rueschemeyer S-A Pfeiffer C amp Bekkering H (2010) Bodyschematics On the role of the body schema in embodiedlexical-semantic representations Neuropsychologia 48774ndash781

Russell J (1980) A circumplex model of affect Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology 39 1161ndash1178

Said C P Moore C D Engell A D amp Haxby J V (2010)Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressionsin the superior temporal sulcus Journal of Vision 10 1ndash12

Satpute A B Fenker D B Waldmann M R Tabibnia GHolyoak K J amp Lieberman M D (2005) An fMRI study ofcausal judgments European Journal of Neuroscience 221233ndash1238

Saxe R amp Wexler A (2005) Making sense of another mind Therole of the right temporo-parietal junction Neuropsychologia43 1391ndash1399

Schilbach L Bzdok D Timmermans B Fox P T Laird A RVogeley K amp Eickhoff S B (2012) Introspective mindsUsing ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the

neural correlates of emotional processing social amp uncon-strained cognition PLoS One 7 e30920-e30920

Schmidtke D S Conrad M amp Jacobs A M (2014)Phonological iconicity Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 80

Schreiner C E amp Winer J A (2007) Auditory cortex mapmakingPrinciples projections and plasticity Neuron 56 356ndash365

Schurz M Radua J Aichhorn M Richlan F amp Perner J (2014)Fractionating theory of mind A meta-analysis of functionalbrain imaging studies Neuroscience and BiobehavioralReviews 42 9ndash34

Schwanenflugel P (1991) Why are abstract concepts hard tounderstand In P Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 223ndash250) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Schwarzlose R F Baker C I amp Kanwisher N (2005) Separateface and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus Journal ofNeuroscience 25 11055ndash11059

Schwoebel J amp Coslett H B (2005) Evidence for multiple dis-tinct representations of the human body Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 17 543ndash553

Serino A amp Haggard P (2010) Touch and the bodyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 34 224ndash236

Shaoul C amp Westbury C (2013) A reduced redundancy USENETcorpus (2005ndash2011) Edmonton AB University of AlbertaRetrieved from httpwwwpsychualbertaca~westburylabdownloadsusenetcorpusdownloadhtml

Shoben E (1991) Predicating and nonpredicating combi-nations In P J Schwanenflugel (Ed) The psychology ofword meanings (pp 117ndash135) Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum

Simmons W K Ramjee V Beauchamp M S McRae K MartinA amp Barsalou L W (2007) A common neural substrate forperceiving and knowing about color Neuropsychologia 452802ndash2810

Simmons W K Reddish M Bellgowan P S amp Martin A(2010) The selectivity and functional connectivity of theanterior temporal lobes Cerebral Cortex 20 813ndash825

Smith E E amp Osherson D N (1984) Conceptual combinationwith prototype concepts Cognitive Science 8 337ndash361

Snodgrass J G amp Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of260 pictures Norms for name agreement image agreementfamiliarity and visual complexity Journal of ExperimentalPsychology Human Learning and Memory 6 174ndash215

Spreng R N Mar R A amp Kim A S N (2009) The commonneural basis of autobiographical memory prospection navi-gation theory of mind and the default mode A quantitativemeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21 489ndash510

Springer K amp Murphy G (1992) Feature availability in concep-tual combination Psychological Science 3 111ndash117

Talmy L (1983) How language structures space In H Pick amp LAcredolo (Eds) Spatial orientation Theory research andapplication (pp 225ndash282) New York Plenum Press

Tanaka K Saito H Fukada Y amp Moriya M (1991) Codingvisual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of themacaque monkey Journal of Neurophysiology 66 170ndash189

Tettamanti M Buccino G Saccuman M C Gallese V DannaM Scifo Phellip Perani D (2005) Listening to action-relatedsentences activates fronto-parietal motor cicuits Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17 273ndash281

COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY 173

Tootell R B Reppas J B Kwong K K Malach R Born R TBrady T Jhellip Belliveau J W (1995) Functional analysis ofhuman MT and related visual cortical areas using magneticresonance imaging Journal of Neuroscience 15 3215ndash3230

Tracy J L amp Randles D (2011) Four models of basic emotionsa review of Ekman and Cordaro Izard Levenson andPanksepp and Watt Emotion Review 3 397ndash405

Tranel D amp Kemmerer D (2004) Neuroanatomical correlates oflocative prepositions Cognitive Neuropsychology 21 719ndash749

Tranel D Logan C G Frank R J amp Damasio A R (1997)Explaining category-related effects in the retrieval ofconceptual and lexical knowledge for concrete entitiesOperationalization and analysis of factors Neuropsychologia35 1329ndash1339

Troche J Crutch S amp Reilly J (2014) Clustering hierarchicalorganization and the topography of abstract and concretenouns Frontiers in Psychology 5 Article 360

Trumpp N M Kliese D Hoenig K Haarmeier T amp Kiefer M(2013) Losing the sound of concepts Damage to auditoryassociation cortex impairs the processing of sound-relatedconcepts Cortex 49 474ndash486

Van Overwalle F (2009) Social cognition and the brain A meta-analysis Human Brain Mapping 30 829ndash858

van Veluw S J amp Chance S A (2014) Differentiating betweenself and others An ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind Brain Imaging and Behavior8 24ndash38

Vigliocco G Meteyard L Andrews M amp Kousta S (2009)Toward a theory of semantic representation Language andCognition 1 219ndash247

Vigliocco G Vinson D P Lewis W amp Garrett M F (2004)Representing the meanings of object and action wordsThe featural and unitary semantic space hypothesisCognitive Psychology 48 422ndash488

Vinson D P Vigliocco G Cappa S amp Siri S (2003) The break-down of semantic knowledge Insights from a statisticalmodel of meaning representation Brain and Language 86347ndash365

Vytal K amp Hamann S (2010) Neuroimaging support for dis-crete neural correlates of basic emotions A voxel-basedmeta-analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 2864ndash2885

Wallentin M Oslashstergaarda S Lund T E Oslashstergaard L ampRoepstorff A (2005) Concrete spatial language See what Imean Brain and Language 92 221ndash233

Warrington E K amp McCarthy R A (1987) Categories of knowl-edge Further fractionations and an attempted integrationBrain 110 1273ndash1296

Warrington E K amp Shallice T (1984) Category specific seman-tic impairments Brain 107 829ndash853

Wiemer-Hastings K amp Xu X (2005) Content differencesfor abstract and concrete concepts Cognitive Science 29719ndash736

Wilson M D (1988) The MRC Psycholinguistic DatabaseMachine Readable Dictionary Version 2 Behavior ResearchMethods Instruments amp Computers 20 6ndash10

Wilson-Mendenhall C D Barrett L F amp Barsalou L W (2013)Neural evidence that human emotions share core affectiveproperties Psychological Science 24 947ndash956

Woods A J Hamilton R H Kranjec A Minhaus P Bikson MYu J amp Chatterjee A (2014) Space time and causality in thehuman brain Neuroimage 92 285ndash297

Wu D H Morganti A amp Chatterjee A (2008) Neural substratesof processing path and manner information of a movingevent Neuropsychologia 46 704ndash713

Wu D H Waller S amp Chatterjee A (2007) The functionalneuroanatomy of thematic role and locativerelational knowledge Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 191542ndash1555

Yamamoto M (2006) Agency and impersonality Their linguisticand cultural manifestations Amsterdam J Benjamins Pub Co

Zahn R Moll J Krueger F Huey E D Garrido G amp GrafmanJ (2007) Social concepts are represented in the superioranterior temporal cortex Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences USA 104 6430ndash6435

Zatorre R J Belin P amp Penhune V B (2002) Structure andfunction of auditory cortex Music and speech Trends inCognitive Sciences 6 37ndash46

Zdrazilova L amp Pexman P M (2013) Grasping the invisiblesemantic processing of abstract words PsychonomicBulletin and Review 20 1312ndash1318

Zeki S M (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in theposterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesusmonkey The Journal of Physiology 236 549ndash573

174 J R BINDER ET AL

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • Neural components of experience
    • Visual components
    • Somatosensory components
    • Auditory components
    • Gustatory and olfactory components
    • Motor components
    • Spatial components
    • Temporal and causal components
    • Social components
    • Cognition component
    • Emotion and evaluation components
    • Drive components
    • Attention components
      • Attribute salience ratings for 535 English words
        • The word set
        • Query design
        • Survey methods
        • General results
          • Exploring the representations
            • Attribute mutual information
            • Attribute covariance structure
            • Attribute composition of some major semantic categories
            • Quantitative differences between a priori categories
            • Category effects on item similarity Brain-based versus distributional representations
            • Data-driven categorization of the words
            • Hierarchical clustering
            • Correlations with word frequency and imageability
            • Correlations with non-semantic lexical variables
              • Potential applications
                • Application to some general problems in componential semantic theory
                • Feature selection
                • Feature weighting
                • Abstract concepts
                • Context effects and ad hoc categories
                • Conceptual combination
                  • Scope and limitations of the theory
                  • Disclosure statement
                  • References
Page 13: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 14: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 15: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 16: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 17: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 18: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 19: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 20: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 21: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 22: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 23: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 24: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 25: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 26: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 27: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 28: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 29: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 30: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 31: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 32: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 33: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 34: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 35: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 36: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 37: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 38: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 39: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 40: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 41: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 42: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 43: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 44: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 45: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,
Page 46: Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation...Fernandino, Stephen B. Simons, Mario Aguilar & Rutvik H. Desai (2016) Toward a brain-based componential semantic representation,