the subjective distinction between the basic emotions of

46
Is Tenderness a Basic RUNNING HEAD: Tenderness Basic Emotion Is Tenderness a Basic Emotion? Juan Pablo Kalawski University of Louisville This article was originally published in Motivation and Emotion: Volume 34, Issue 2 (2010), Page 158, DOI: 10.1007/s11031-010- 9164-y. The original publication is available at http://www.springerlink.com/openurl.asp? genre=article&id=doi:10.1007/s11031-010-9164-y 1

Upload: docong

Post on 11-Feb-2017

220 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

RUNNING HEAD: Tenderness Basic Emotion

Is Tenderness a Basic Emotion?

Juan Pablo Kalawski

University of Louisville

This article was originally published in Motivation and Emotion: Volume 34, Issue 2 (2010),

Page 158, DOI: 10.1007/s11031-010-9164-y. The original publication is available at

http://www.springerlink.com/openurl.asp?genre=article&id=doi:10.1007/s11031-010-9164-y

1

Page 2: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Abstract

The article presents the case to consider tenderness a basic emotion, using the criteria proposed

by Ekman (1999). The first part of the article reviews the relationship between tenderness and

the related concepts of love and empathy. The next section reviews evidence concerning whether

tenderness meets some of Ekman’s criteria. The last section reports an experiment testing

whether tenderness meets Ekman’s criterion of distinct subjective experience. Participants

watched scenes designed to induce different emotions. Results showed that a scene could induce

high levels of tenderness and low levels of joy if that scene also induced high levels of sadness.

These results suggest that the subjective experience of tenderness is distinct from that of joy.

2

Page 3: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Is Tenderness a Basic Emotion?

Emotional phenomena are as pervasive as they are varied. Over a century ago, William

James complained that,

As far as "scientific psychology" of the emotions goes, I may have been surfeited by too

much reading of classic works on the subject, but I should as lief read verbal descriptions

of the shapes of the rocks on a New Hampshire farm as toil through them again. They

give one nowhere a central point of view, or a deductive or generative principle. They

distinguish and refine and specify in infinitum without ever getting on to another logical

level. (1890, p.448)

Since James’ complaint, several scholars have proposed theories that attempt to bridge that gap

in abstraction. Of those theories, the basic emotions approach gained increased support during

the second half of the twentieth century. There are several basic emotions theories, but they all

share some basic assumptions. Basic emotion theories propose that the array of human affective

phenomena arise out of a handful of basic emotions (e.g., Ekman, 1999; Izard, 1991, 2007).

Basic emotions are “those emotions that have been characterized as having evolutionarily old

neurobiological substrates, as well as an evolved feeling component and capacity for expressive

and other behavioral actions of evolutionary origin” (Izard, 2007, p. 261). Through their

interaction with an individual’s experiences and cognitions, basic emotions give rise to a variety

of complex and idiosyncratic affective experiences. While these complex emotional reactions

can be understood in terms of more basic components, basic emotions cannot. In this sense, they

are irreducible, and hence basic. Furthermore, basic emotion theorists proposed that most of the

daily emotional experiences of human adults are not basic emotions. “Theorists generally agree

that basic emotions are few in number, relatively infrequent, and short in duration and that

3

Page 4: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

nonbasic emotions (emotion schemas) are virtually infinite in number and usually longer in

duration (cf. Damasio, 1999; James, 1890/1950; Kagan, 1978).” (Izard, 2007, p. 265) Thus, basic

emotions include many experiences that laypeople call emotions, but not all of those

experiences. For instance, laypeople probably consider jealousy an emotion. Basic emotion

theorists would propose that, although jealousy is a real emotional experience, it is not a basic

emotion. They would argue that jealousy is a complex phenomenon that arises out of the

interaction of basic emotions (especially anger) and specific cognitive processes.

One area of disagreement among different basic emotion approaches is over the list of

basic emotions. Shaver, Morgan, and Wu (1996) have summarized the basic emotion lists

proposed by several scholars. A review of those lists shows that there is consensus that joy,

anger, sadness, and fear are basic emotions. Other emotions like shame and contempt are more

controversial. In this article, I will argue that tenderness should be considered a basic emotion.2

In order to understand what tenderness is, I will first discuss its relationships with related

concepts.

Tenderness, love, and empathy

One of the concepts related to tenderness is love. Love is conspicuously absent from

basic emotion lists. Shaver et al. (1996) have presented several arguments for the inclusion of

love as a basic emotion. However, they agreed with Ekman (1999) that the word “love” often

does not refer to an emotion but to a disposition to respond emotionally. Shaver et al. pointed

out, however, that the emotional disposition of love is punctuated by “surges” of emotion. These

surges are temporally discrete in the way other basic emotions are.

In addition, Shaver et al. (1996) noted that the term “love” means different things under

different circumstances. In other words, there are several varieties of love. Based on attachment

4

Page 5: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

theory, Shaver et al. distinguished among love as attachment, love as caregiving, and love as

sexual attraction. Each of these three types of love may occur by itself, or it may combine with

one or both other types. Shaver et al. suggested that different types of emotion “surges” may

correspond to the different types of love.

I conceptualize tenderness as the momentary experience, that is, the emotion “surge,” that

corresponds to love as caregiving. Frijda (1986) proposed that, “tenderness can be regarded as

the impulse toward tender—that is, caregiving—behavior; or else as the acute act of recognition

of an object as a fit object for such behavior” (p. 83). To summarize the conceptual relationship

between tenderness and love, whereas love is a disposition to respond emotionally, tenderness is

a momentary emotional experience. And whereas there are different varieties of love, tenderness

relates specifically to caregiving.

Another concept that is related to tenderness is the concept of empathy. Clinical and

counseling psychologists may be familiar with Rogers’ (1957) definition of empathy: “to sense

the client’s private world as if it were your own, but without ever losing the ‘as if’ quality” (p.

99). In the context of this definition, the word “sense” probably means two things: a cognitive

understanding and a feeling. Batson and Shaw (1991) suggest using the term “perspective

taking” to refer to the cognitive aspect. They reserve the term “empathy” for the emotional

feeling that may arise from taking someone else’s perspective. In this sense, Batson, Lishner,

Cook, and Sawyer (2005) state that,

Empathy (…) refers to an other-oriented emotional response congruent with the

perceived welfare of another (…). Empathic feelings for someone in need include

sympathy, compassion, tenderness, and the like. These feelings have been found to be a

potent source of motivation to help relieve the empathy-inducing need. (p.15)

5

Page 6: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Lishner (2003) reported some evidence consistent with the idea that tenderness and

sympathy are distinct empathic feelings. In particular, his results suggest that perceiving

someone’s vulnerability elicits tenderness, whereas perceiving someone’s current need elicits

sympathy. In addition to differences in what elicits tenderness versus sympathy, it appears that

laypeople categorize tenderness apart from sympathy. Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson, and O’Connor

(1987) found that participants organized the term “tenderness” into a love category and

“sympathy” into a sadness category. To summarize the relationship between tenderness and

empathy, tenderness is an empathic feeling, but it is more specific, and distinct from sympathy,

which appears more closely related to the basic emotion of sadness.

Both love and empathy are complex reactions and therefore they would not qualify as

basic emotions. I hypothesize, however, that tenderness probably does qualify as a basic

emotion. Ekman (1999) proposed 11 characteristics that any basic emotion must have. They are:

(a) distinctive universal signals; (b) distinctive physiology, (c) automatic appraisal, tuned to (d)

distinctive universals in antecedent events; (e) distinctive appearance developmentally; (f)

presence in other primates; (g) quick onset; (h) brief duration; (i) unbidden occurrence; (j)

distinctive thoughts, memories, images; and (k) distinctive subjective experience (p. 56). Ekman

admitted that research had not yet established that any emotion fulfills all the criteria he

proposed.

The present article reports an experiment designed to test whether tenderness meets the

criterion of distinctive subjective experience. In particular, given that tenderness is a pleasant,

positive emotion (Shaver et. al, 1987), I looked into the subjective distinction between tenderness

and joy. If tenderness is not subjectively distinct from joy, that may indicate that tenderness,

rather than a basic emotion, can be explained in terms of the more basic emotion of joy. Before

6

Page 7: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

presenting the rationale for the experiment, I will review some evidence concerning whether

tenderness meets some of the other criteria for basic emotions proposed by Ekman.

Distinctive universal signals and physiology

Researchers have found evidence for the physiological and expressive distinctiveness of

tenderness in a variety of cultures. In studies with Chilean and Danish participants, Bloch and

her colleagues (Bloch, Lemeignan, & Aguilera-T, 1991; Bloch, Orthous, & Santibañez-H, 1987;

Santibáñez-H & Bloch, 1986) found that tenderness was associated with a specific breathing

pattern, body posture, and facial expression, which were different from those of joy, anger,

sadness, fear, and sexual arousal. In particular, breathing in tenderness has a slow and even

rhythm, inhalation and exhalation are done through the nose, and there is a slight pause at the

end of the exhalation. The general muscular tone is relaxed, leaning slightly forward. One of the

most distinctive characteristics of the tenderness display is that the head is tilted sideways.

Finally, in tenderness, there is a slight smile, and the gaze is directed at an object. Santibáñez-H

and Bloch (1986) also found that whereas joy, anger, sadness, fear, and sexual arousal increased

heart rate, tenderness decreased it. In another study, French observers were able to effectively

distinguish the postural and facial expressions of tenderness from those of joy, anger, sadness,

fear, and sexual arousal (Lemeignan, Aguilera-Torres, & Bloch, 1992).

Automatic appraisal tuned to distinctive universals in antecedent events

There is little on the appraisals and antecedents that elicit tenderness. Lishner (2003)

reported a reanalysis of data from a previous experiment showing that reading a story about a

human adult led to less tenderness than reading the same story about a child, a dog, or a puppy.

In two subsequent experiments, Lishner found that watching a video of a human infant led to

high levels of tenderness, regardless of the infant’s emotional display or reported health state.

7

Page 8: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

This pattern was more reliable among women than men. In a third experiment, he found that, for

both men and women, imagining a child led to more tenderness than imagining an adult. Lishner

proposed that appraising vulnerability in a target elicits tenderness and that,

Perceptions of vulnerability will likely occur when an observer makes certain inferences

about a target such as whether he or she can control his or her situation, whether the

target is naïve or not, and possibly whether the target displays heuristic vulnerability cues

(e.g., childlike features, attributes, or behaviors; Eibl-Ebesfeldt, 1971; Lorenz, 1971).

Furthermore, if current need is perceived, then the likelihood of perceiving vulnerability

increases because the existence of current need indicates that some dimension of well-

being is not sufficiently regulated. (p.73)

The little extant evidence thus suggests that an appraisal of vulnerability is associated

with tenderness. Observing an animal, an infant, or a child appears to be a common antecedent of

tenderness. However, not all infants may elicit the same level of tenderness. Langlois, Ritter,

Casey, and Sawin (1995) reported that mothers of attractive infants displayed more interactions

that were affectionate towards their children than did mothers of unattractive infants.

Presence in other primates

As previously discussed, tenderness is related to caregiving. Caregiving behavior is

present not only among primates, but among mammals more generally. Panksepp (2000)

described a “CARE/nurturance” emotional system in mammals, mediated by the anterior

cingulate, bed nucleus of stria terminalis, preoptic area, ventral tegmental area, and

periaqueductal gray. Oxytocin, prolactin, dopamine, and opioids are the key neuromodulators of

this system. Bell (2001) proposed a theory which traces the evolution of caregiving behavior

8

Page 9: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

back to the premammalian reptiles. Bell provided an especially compelling argument for the

evolutionary primitiveness of caregiving:

Attachment theory has described how an attachment behavioral control system could

provide survival advantages to primates and humans within the environment of

evolutionary adaptedness. However, as Bowlby’s (1982) analysis showed but did not

emphasize, the evolutionary advantages of attachment can only be realized if there is a

preexisting caregiving behavioral control system (Bell & Richard, 2000). Crying, one of

the prototypical attachment behaviors, would be an evolutionary disadvantage if there

were no caregiver to respond because crying attracts predators. Crying is only an

advantage to the extent that it usually attracts the caregiver faster than it attracts the

predator. As a result, any evolutionary analysis of attachment must account for the prior

emergence of the caregiving behavioral control system as a prerequisite for the

emergence of an attachment behavioral control system (p. 226).

Bell argued that attachment theory had focused excessively on the cognitive aspects of

caregiving at the expense of the emotional impulse towards caregiving. Bell’s “caring emotion”

appears to be equivalent to tenderness.

Quick onset, brief duration, and unbidden occurrence

As previously discussed, it is important to distinguish the emotion of tenderness from the

emotional disposition of love. Tenderness probably meets the criteria of quick onset and brief

duration, although research is needed in this area. In addition, the emotion of tenderness cannot

be willed. The feeling that arises upon seeing, for instance, a cute baby, is automatic.

Distinctive thoughts, memories, images

9

Page 10: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

As previously discussed, Bloch et al. (1987; 1991; Santibáñez-H & Bloch, 1986) found

that tenderness was associated with a specific breathing pattern, body posture, and facial

expression. These authors also discovered that they could teach participants the “effector

patterns” of each emotion, and that the voluntary reproduction of these patterns induced the

corresponding subjective feeling in the participant. In Chile, I conducted a study exploring the

use of these patterns in psychotherapy (Kalawski, 1997). In one of the sessions studied, the

client/participant was talking about a conflict with her boyfriend. Her discourse was generally

one of blaming him without understanding his perspective. Then I gave her instructions to

reproduce the pattern of tenderness, but without naming the target emotion. During this exercise,

I asked her to think about her boyfriend. After the exercise, the participant reported having felt

tenderness. She added that when I asked her to think about her boyfriend she was able to see the

whole situation in a different way, understanding his position. Also concerning thoughts

associated with tenderness, Lishner (2003) reported that tenderness was positively correlated to

feeling “protective” and to valuing the welfare of the tenderness-inducing target.

Distinctive subjective experience and the present experiment

An affective phenomenon cannot be considered a basic emotion if it “feels” like another

emotion. An example of this is jealousy, which does not have a specific subjective feeling.

Often, a jealous person may feel anger or another basic emotion, but there is no “jealous” feeling

distinct from the way basic emotions feel. With regards to tenderness, it could not be considered

a basic emotion unless it has its own, unique subjective experience. Lishner’s results suggest that

the experience of tenderness is distinct from the experience of sympathy. However, I am not

aware of any studies directly testing whether the subjective experience of tenderness is distinct

from that of other basic emotions. Given tenderness’ pleasant quality, it is possible that persons

10

Page 11: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

reporting feeling tenderness will also report feeling joy. This intuitive hypothesis may be one of

the reasons why theorists do not consider tenderness a basic emotion. I assumed that a positive

correlation between tenderness and joy would be likely. However, it is possible that this

correlation is not due to tenderness’ lack of a distinct subjective experience. Instead, a positive

correlation between joy and tenderness may reflect the fact that many situations call for both

tenderness and joy. It is also possible that people use words such as “joy” to refer to pleasant

feelings in general, but, if required, are able to tell joy from tenderness. Therefore, an experiment

that uses stimuli that selectively preclude the experience of either joy or tenderness may have a

better chance of finding a subjective distinction between the two, if such distinction exists. The

question then is what kinds of stimuli can selectively preclude either joy or tenderness. A

possible answer to this question may come from the intuitive notions that joy is the opposite of

sadness and that tenderness is the opposite of anger. Some research findings are consistent with

this possibility. Lorr & McNair, for instance, (1988) reported the discovery of six bipolar mood

factors. One factor had “elated” (joy) and “depressed” (sadness) as their poles. Another factor’s

poles were “agreeable” (tenderness) and “hostile” (anger). There are several ways to interpret

this kind of results. The first interpretation is that the two poles of each factor represent opposite

extremes of a single process. Another interpretation is that emotions such as joy and depression

result from different processes that somehow inhibit each other. Thirdly, one might view these

results as reflecting the fact that situations that induce one polar emotion normally do not induce

the emotion considered its opposite. Under this interpretation, sadness is not necessarily opposite

to joy; they just tend to occur in different situations. Whatever the interpretation, it appears that

stimuli that induce certain emotions also tend to preclude the experience of other specific

emotions. If this were the case, and in particular, if anger precludes tenderness and sadness

11

Page 12: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

precludes joy, adding anger-inducing features to a joyful stimulus and sad features to a tender

stimulus would increase the contrast between joy and tenderness. The present study employed

five types of stimuli, as presented in Table 1. The stimuli varied along two hypothetical

dimensions: sadness-inducing vs. joy-inducing, and anger-inducing vs. tenderness-inducing. For

the purposes of the present study, I called “pitiful” a stimulus expected to induce both tenderness

and sadness, whereas a “cute” stimulus would induce tenderness but not sadness. Both are

“tender” stimuli. Similarly, I called “ironic”, a stimulus expected to induce both joy/amusement

and anger, and “funny” one expected to induce joy/amusement but no anger. Both are “amusing”

stimuli. The main hypothesis was that tender stimuli would lead to higher levels of tenderness

and lower levels of joy as compared to the amusing stimuli. Another hypothesis was that tender

stimuli would lead to higher tenderness than a “neutral” stimulus would. Likewise, amusing

stimuli would lead to higher joy/amusement than the neutral stimulus would.

In addition to finding out what types of stimuli differentially induce tenderness versus

joy, it was important to know what "tenderness" meant to participants. Thus, they rated

tenderness on three dimensions: passive-active, unpleasant-pleasant, and relaxed-tense. The

research and theory previously reviewed are consistent with a view of tenderness as active,

relaxed, and pleasant. It is active as it provides the impulse toward caregiving behavior. It is

relaxed according to research on its physiology and expression. Finally, previously reviewed

research by Shaver et al. (1987) showed that people categorize tenderness as a pleasant emotion.

Method

Participants

One hundred forty seven undergraduates (86 female) from the research pool of the

12

Page 13: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

University of North Texas psychology department participated in the study. Their mean age was

22.15 years (SD = 5.92). The modal age was 19 years. Separate schedules for males and females

randomly assigned each participant to one stimulus condition (ironic, funny, pitiful, cute, or

neutral).

Design

As previously stated, the main hypothesis was that tender stimuli would lead to higher

levels of tenderness and lower levels of joy than amusing stimuli would. To test this hypothesis, I

compared emotion ratings across levels of two variables: the main emotional characteristic of the

stimuli (tender or amusing, a between-subjects variable) and the type of emotion measured

(tenderness or joy, a within-subjects variable). If joy and tenderness are in fact distinct emotions,

as hypothesized, participants in the joy-inducing conditions would score higher on the joy

measure, and participants in the tenderness-inducing conditions would score higher on the

tenderness measure. This means that there would be an interaction between the type of emotional

stimulus and the emotion measured.

An additional between-subjects factor was the presence or absence of features to preclude

either joy or tenderness. That is, sadness-inducing features in a tenderness-inducing stimulus

would preclude joy. Conversely, anger-inducing features in a joy-inducing stimulus would

preclude tenderness. There was no prediction as to whether the presence or absence of these

features would moderate the hypothesized interaction between the type of emotional stimulus

and the emotion measured. This stimulus x measure interaction could occur across the board, or

it could occur only when the precluding features were present.

Materials

13

Page 14: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Emotional stimuli. The “cute” scene was a segment from the movie Animal Bloopers (Ingle &

Heck, 1994), lasting approximately 1 min and 45 s. In it, a zookeeper explains how he takes care

of a baby Asian otter while images show the animal in several activities. The expectation was

that this scene would elicit tenderness as the baby otter is vulnerable, and thus an object for care-

giving behavior.

The “pitiful” scene was a segment from The Bear (Annaud, 1989), lasting approximately

1 min and 34 s. The scene shows a cub bear and its mother. A big rock falls on top of the mother,

causing its death. The cub tries to rescue the mother unsuccessfully. As in the case of the baby

otter, the cub is vulnerable and thus an object for care-giving behavior, which should induce

tenderness. Additionally, both the death of a family member and failure to achieve an important

goal (in this case, saving the cub’s mother) tend to elicit sadness (Izard, 1991), so if participants

identified with the cub they would experience those emotions too.

The “funny” scene was a segment from Bill Cosby: Himself (Cosby & Cosby, 1982),

lasting approximately 2 min and 13 s. In this scene, Bill Cosby humorously describes his

experiences with dentists. As this scene is from a popular comedian, the expectation was that it

would induce joy/amusement.

The “ironic” scene was a segment from the movie Brazil (Gilliam, 1985), lasting

approximately 1 min and 13 s. The character played by Jonathan Pryce is in a minuscule office

receiving documents though a tube, which he must then insert into another tube. The documents

come in faster than he can manage. When he finds a way to connect the two tubes, someone pulls

his desk from the adjacent office. Finally, the tube explodes, causing a rain of paper. The

expectation was that this ridiculous situation would elicit joy/amusement. However, participants

would also experience anger if they identified with the frustration endured by the character.

14

Page 15: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

The “neutral” scene was from an interview with B. F. Skinner (Miller, 1977), lasting

approximately 1 min and 20 s. In it, Skinner explains operant conditioning. The expectation was

that this scene would not induce high levels of any emotion.

Before beginning the present study, I showed all the segments, except the one from Bill

Cosby, to graduate psychology students and measured their emotional reactions with the same

scales described in the Measures section, to assess whether their reactions matched the expected

ones. All of the graduate students reported the predicted pattern of emotions to the cute, pitiful,

and neutral scenes. In response to the ironic scene, one person reported feeling anger and

joy/amusement, as expected. Another one reported only anger, and a third person reported

sadness.

Measures. There was a set of standard demographic questions. The emotion questionnaire had

the following instructions: “Below are four emotions. Please circle how much of each emotion

you felt in response to the video. The number 0 means that you did not feel the emotion at all.

The number 10 represents an extreme level of that emotion. You can circle any number.” There

were four separate lines labeled “Joy/Amusement”, “Tenderness”, “Sadness”, and “Anger”.

Thus, the questionnaire yielded four separate scores, ranging from 0 to 10, for each participant

on each emotion, equal to the number circled on each scale. Following the emotion questionnaire

on a separate page, participants answered a questionnaire with the following instructions: “One

of the emotions we asked you about in the previous page was TENDERNESS. Regardless of

how much tenderness you felt, we want to know what tenderness means to you. For each item

below, please circle the number that you think represents tenderness. There is no right or wrong

answer.” After those instructions, there were three semantic differential scales. Each scale had a

0-10 range. The anchors were passive-active, unpleasant-pleasant, and relaxed-tense.

15

Page 16: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Procedure

Students individually participated in this experiment. After providing informed consent,

participants completed the demographic questionnaire. An experimenter then started the

videotape and then went to the back of the room to avoid giving the participant any cues

regarding reactions to the scene. Once the scene was over, the experimenter handed the

participant the questionnaires to complete. For participants watching the pitiful scene, before

starting the video, the experimenter added this statement: “The scene you will watch is not from

a documentary. It is from a fictional movie”. The reason for this explanation is that one pilot

participant reported feeling angry that someone would film the death of an animal without

intervening.

Results

Table 2 presents the mean ratings for each emotion and experimental condition.

Anger and Sadness

Before testing the hypotheses regarding tenderness and joy, the first set of analyses was a

manipulation check for the effectiveness of the features meant to preclude those emotions. The

first step in this process was testing whether the ironic and pitiful scenes had actually induced

anger and sadness, respectively. The next step was testing the correlation between anger and

tenderness on the one hand, and sadness and joy on the other. If anger precluded tenderness and

sadness precluded joy, both correlations would be negative.

An ANOVA comparing anger scores among the five experimental conditions (cute,

pitiful, funny, ironic, and neutral) yielded a significant gender main effect, F(1, 137) = 4.84,

MSE = 3.07, Partial η2 = .59,. There was also a significant scene x gender interaction, F(4, 137)

= 2.66, MSE = 3.07, Partial η2 = .07. Therefore, the next analyses were tests of simple effects

16

Page 17: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

for males and females. Among males, the scene watched had a significant effect on anger scores,

F(4, 56) = 6.66, MSE = 3.78, Partial η2 = .32, as expected. Paired Bonferroni comparisons

showed that the ironic scene, M = 3.62 (SD = 2.90), led to the higher anger scores than the

neutral, M = .69 (SD = 2.21), cute, M = .30 (SD = .95), and funny, M = .04 (SD = .20), scenes

did. The scores from the pitiful condition, M = 1.66 (SD = 1.66), did not differ significantly from

those of any other condition. These results were consistent with my expectations, except that the

anger scores in the pitiful condition were not statistically different from those in the ironic

condition. The scene watched had a significant effect on anger scores for females, F(4, 81) =

4.29, MSE = 2.58, Partial η2 = .18. Contrary to my expectations, paired Bonferroni comparisons

did not yield a significant difference between the ironic condition scores, M = 1.17 (SD = 2.17),

and those of the other conditions. The scores for the pitiful condition, M = 1.80 (SD = 2.48),

were higher than those for the neutral, M = .22 (SD = .73), cute, M = .07 (SD = .26), and funny,

M = .07 (SD = .26), conditions, which did not differ from each other.

The next ANOVA compared sadness scores among the five experimental conditions.

Gender had no main or interaction effects, Fs < 1. Consequently, the analysis presently reported

did not include gender as a factor. As expected, the scene watched had a significant effect on

these scores, F(4, 142) = 148.50, MSE = 2.38, Partial η2 = .81. Paired Bonferroni comparisons

showed that the pitiful scene led to higher sadness scores than any of the other scenes did. The

scores of the cute, ironic, neutral, and funny conditions did not differ from each other. These

results are consistent with my expectation that the pitiful scene would induce more sadness than

all other conditions would.

The next step of the manipulation check was testing the correlation between anger and

tenderness and joy and sadness respectively. The former correlation was not significant, r = .06,

17

Page 18: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

p = .43, contrary to the expectation. The latter correlation was negative, r = -.44, p < .01, as

expected.

To summarize, the ironic condition appeared to succeed at inducing anger in males.

However, among females, the pitiful, not the ironic, condition induced the most anger.

Furthermore, anger did not preclude tenderness, and the pitiful condition, meant to induce

tenderness, led to an unexpectedly high level of anger. The pitiful condition did appear to

succeed at inducing sadness. Sadness, in turn, had a negative correlation with joy.

Ratings of Tenderness versus Joy

The primary analysis was a 2 x 2 x 2 ANOVA according to the design previously described. As

previously explained, the dependent variable was emotion rating as indicated by the score on the

0-10 scale for the corresponding emotion. The factors were type of emotion measured (within

subjects), whether the scene was tender or amusing (between subjects), and the presence or

absence of precluding features in the scene (between subjects). A preliminary analysis yielded no

interaction effects of gender, Fs < 1. Consequently, the analysis presently reported did not

include gender as a factor. There was a significant three-way interaction, F(1, 112) = 24.50, MSE

= 3.04, Partial η2 = .18. This means that the presence or absence of precluding features

moderated the interaction between the emotion measure and the main emotional feature. Further

exploration of this interaction involved running separate tests of simple effects for the

participants who saw the scenes with precluding features and for those who saw the scenes

without those features. Again, gender was not a factor. There was a significant interaction

between the emotion measured and the main emotional characteristic of the scene, both for the

precluding features condition, F(1, 61) = 191.36, MSE = 3.73, Partial η2 = .76, and the no

precluding features condition, F(1, 51) = 73.73, MSE = 2.22, Partial η2 = .59. However, the

18

Page 19: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

interaction took a different form depending on whether or not the scenes included a precluding

feature. In the case of the scenes with the precluding features, the ironic (amusing with anger-

inducing features) scene led to low tenderness and higher joy/amusement, whereas the pitiful

(tender and sad) scene led to high tenderness and low joy/amusement. In the case of the scenes

without the precluding features, the funny scene led to low tenderness and high joy/amusement,

whereas the cute scene led to high scores for both tenderness and joy/amusement. The interaction

between the main emotional feature and the emotion measured was consistent with the main

hypothesis of the present study.

Comparison among All Groups

Given an interaction between the main emotional feature and the emotion measure, a

possible next step could be to run separate tests of simple effects for tenderness and joy as the

dependent variables. As the interaction was significant for conditions both with and without

precluding features, that would yield four separate tests. An alternative, more informative

strategy consisted of running two ANOVAs comparing scores among all five experimental

conditions (cute, pitiful, funny, ironic, and neutral). The dependent variables were the tenderness

and joy scores respectively. Preliminary analyses showed no main or interaction effects of

gender on either variable, Fs < 1.

As expected, the experimental condition significantly affected tenderness scores, F(4,

142) = 93.51, MSE = 3.39, Partial η2 = .72. Paired Bonferroni comparisons showed that

tenderness scores were higher for the cute and pitiful scenes than for the funny, ironic, and

neutral scenes. These results are consistent with my hypotheses.

As expected, the experimental condition significantly affected joy scores, F(4, 142) =

61.81, MSE = 3.97, Partial η2 = .64. Paired Bonferroni comparisons showed that the funny and

19

Page 20: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

cute conditions led to higher joy scores than all other conditions did. The ironic condition, in

turn, led to higher scores than the pitiful and neutral conditions. These results were consistent

with the hypotheses, with the exception that there was no prediction regarding how much joy the

cute scene would induce.

Participants’ Conceptions of Tenderness

The next series of analyses dealt with the participants’ responses to the questionnaire on

the meaning tenderness had for them. Participants’ responses could range from 0 (passive,

unpleasant, or relaxed, depending on the item) to 10 (active, pleasant, or tense, respectively). A

score of 5 meant “neutral”. Participants rated tenderness as neither passive nor active, t(146) =

1.40, p = .16, M = 5.26 (SD = 2.24). This was contrary to my hypothesis that they would rate it

as active. There was no correlation between this item and participants’ tenderness scores, r = .01,

p = .91. Furthermore, the answer to this question did not differ by gender, F(1, 145) = 1.06, MSE

= 5.03, Partial η2 = .01.

As expected, participants rated tenderness as pleasant, t(146) = 5.84, p = < .01, M = 6.49

(SD = 3.10). There was no correlation between this item and participants’ tenderness scores, r = -

.07, p = .38. The answer to this question did not differ by gender, F(1, 145) = .14, MSE = 9.64,

Partial η2 < .01. Participants also rated tenderness as relaxed, t(146) = 8.47, p = < .01, M = 3.26

(SD = 2.49). This was consistent with my hypotheses. Again, there was no correlation between

this item and participants’ tenderness scores, r = .13, p = .11. Likewise, the answer to this

question did not differ by gender, F(1, 145) < .01, MSE = 6.26, Partial η2 < .01.

Discussion

The present results may clarify the relationships of empathy, sympathy, and tenderness

with other emotional phenomena. Although I did not directly measure empathy or sympathy,

20

Page 21: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

those feelings would be expected in the pitiful condition, as it presented a target in need (Batson,

Lishner, Cook, & Sawyer, 2005; Batson & Shaw, 1991; Lishner, 2003). The cute condition, on

the other hand, would be expected to induce tenderness but not sympathy, as it presented a target

which was vulnerable but not in current need (Lishner’s 2003). The results show that the pitiful

condition induced tenderness and sadness, while the cute condition induced tenderness but not

sadness. This pattern of results seems to suggest that tenderness and sadness are different

components of sympathy. It may also help explain why laypersons categorize sympathy along

with sadness (Shaver et. al, 1987). A sympathetic observer’s sadness is congruent with the

sadness likely felt by the target in need. Tenderness appears to provide the other-oriented,

empathic emotional component to sympathy.

The present results also show that tenderness is not simply general positive affect, as

different experimental conditions led to either tenderness with joy (the cute condition),

tenderness without joy (the pitiful condition) or joy without tenderness (the funny and ironic

conditions). If tenderness and joy are distinct emotions, they may also have distinct cognitive and

behavioral consequences. There is a wealth of research on the effects of positive affect on social

cognition and behavior (e.g., Crisp & Hewstone, 2000; Isen, 1987; Urada & Miller, 2000).

Future research may focus on the specific behavioral and cognitive effects of tenderness versus

joy.

One interesting finding of the present study was that the ironic scene induced significant

levels of anger only among males. There are several possible explanations for this. The simplest

one would be that males are more prone to experience and/or report anger. Another possibility is

that it was easier for males to identify with the male character in the scene. Finally, it is possible

that women are more likely to think of anger as negative. If this is the case, the fact that the

21

Page 22: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

ironic scene also induced joy might have prevented them from consciously acknowledging

anger. Future research could empirically test these speculations.

With the exception of the previously discussed gender effect, gender showed no other

significant effects. In the present study, gender did not relate either to the overall level of

tenderness or to what conditions led to it. By contrast, Lishner (2003) found that women reported

higher tenderness than did men. One difference between Lishner’s studies and the present

experiment is that Lishner’s targets were human babies and children. In the present experiment,

on the other hand, the tenderness targets were animals. Women’s higher tenderness scores in

Lishner’s studies may be due to women having more extensive socialization with children and

babies. Men and women’s experiences taking care of animals may be more comparable. In the

present study, gender also did not relate to participants’ conceptions of tenderness.

Participants’ conceptions of tenderness were largely consistent with the ones predicted.

They viewed it as pleasant and relaxed. Contrary to my hypothesis, participants rated tenderness

as neither active nor passive. Tenderness may not show a consistent active/passive pattern

because it influences behavior differently depending on whether the target of tenderness has high

or low current need.3 When the target’s current need is high, then tenderness may motivate more

immediate active behavior designed to protect and promote the welfare of the target from

immediate threats to his or her wellbeing. In contrast, when current need is low, feeling

tenderness for a target may result in an impulse to simply be available for the target.

Overall, the present results suggest that tenderness is distinct from both sadness and joy,

and that tenderness may be a necessary component of sympathy and empathy. Previous studies

have also shown that tenderness appears to have distinctive signals and physiology. Furthermore,

both animal research and attachment theory suggest that tenderness is biologically primitive.

22

Page 23: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Together, these findings are consistent with the view of tenderness as a distinct, elementary

emotion, which can be considered in the same category as anger, fear, joy, and sadness.

Obviously, the findings presented here do not settle the issue of whether tenderness is a basic

emotion. Remember that Ekman (1999) acknowledged that research had not shown that any

emotion met all of his criteria. Fully making the case for tenderness as a basic emotion would

necessitate a replication of the present findings, as well as additional empirical and conceptual

work on whether tenderness meets the different criteria for basic emotions. With the present

article, I hope to have shown that it makes sense to think of tenderness as a basic emotion, and

that this issue deserves serious consideration. Viewing tenderness as a basic emotion would help

understand the complex phenomena of empathy and love using the same paradigm applied to

other emotional phenomena.

23

Page 24: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

References

Annaud, J. (Director). (1989). The bear [Motion picture]. United States/France: Tri-Star.

Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., Cook, J., & Sawyer, S. (2005). Similarity and nurturance: Two

possible sources of empathy for strangers. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 15-

25.

Batson, C. D., & Shaw, L. L. (1991). Evidence for altruism: Toward a pluralism of prosocial

motives. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 107-122.

Bell, D. C. (2001). Evolution of parental caregiving. Personality and Social Psychology Review,

5(3), 216-229.

Bloch, S., Lemeignan, M., & Aguilera-T, N. (1991). Specific respiratory patterns distinguish

among human basic emotions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 11, 141-154.

Bloch, S., Orthous, P., & Santibañez-H, G. (1987). Effector patterns of basic emotions: A

psychophysiological method for training actors. Journal of Social and Biological

Structures, 10, 1-19.

Cosby, W. H., Jr. (Producer), & Cosby, B. (Writer/Director). (1982). Bill Cosby, himself [Motion

picture]. United States: Twentieth Century Fox

Crisp, R. J., & Hewstone, M. (2000). Crossed categorization and intergroup bias: The moderating

roles of intergroup and affective context. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36,

357-383.

Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition

and emotion (pp. 45-60). Chichester, West Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons.

Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gilliam, T. (Director). (1985). Brazil [Motion picture]. England: Embassy International Pictures.

24

Page 25: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. Advances in

Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 203-253.

Ingle, R., & Heck, B. (Producers). (1994). Animal bloopers [Motion picture]. United States:

Republic Pictures.

Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum Press.

Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm.

Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 260-280.

James, W. (1890). The principles of Psychology. Retrieved February 19, 2009, from

http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/James/Principles/prin25.htm

Kalawski, J. P. (1997). Aplicación psicoterapéutica de Alba Emoting™: Un método para inducir,

reconocer y regular las emociones básicas [Psychotherapeutic application of Alba

Emoting™: A method for inducing, recognizing, and regulating the basic emotions.]

Unpublished thesis, Central University of Chile, Santiago.

Langlois, J. H., Ritter, J. M., Casey, R. J., & Sawin, D. B. (1995). Infant attractiveness predicts

maternal behaviors and attitudes. Developmental Psychology, 31, 464-472.

Lemeignan, M., Aguilera-Torres, N., & Bloch, S. (1992). Emotional effector patterns:

Recognition of expressions. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of

Cognition, 12, 173-188.

Lishner, D. A. (2003). The components of empathy: Distinguishing between tenderness and

sympathy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas, Lawrence.

Lorr, M., & McNair, D. M. (1988). Manual: Profile of Mood States Bi-Polar Form (POMS-BI)

(2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.

25

Page 26: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Miller, H. J. (Producer/Director). (1977). B. F. Skinner on behaviorism [Television series

episode]. In: Psychology of human relations. Milwaukee, MI: WMVS/WMVT

Television.

Ortony, A. & Turner, T. J. (1990). What's basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97,

315-331.

Panksepp, J. (2000). Emotions as natural kinds within the mammalian brain. In M. Lewis & J.

M. Haviland-Jones, (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 137-156, 2nd ed.). New York:

Guilford.

Rogers, C. R. (1957). The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change.

Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 95-103.

Santibáñez-H, G., & Bloch, S. (1986). A qualitative analysis of emotional effector patterns and

their feedback. The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 21, 108-116.

Shaver, P. R., Morgan, H. J., & Wu, S (1996). Is love a “basic” emotion? Personal

Relationships, 3, 81-96.

Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further

explorations of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,

1061-1086.

Urada, D. I., & Miller, N. (2000). The impact of positive mood and category importance on

crossed categorization effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 417-

433.

26

Page 27: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Footnotes

1 Juan Pablo Kalawski, Counseling Center, University of Louisville. The experiment

reported in the present article was part of the author’s doctoral dissertation at the

University of North Texas. I thank all the members of the dissertation committee for their

input. I also thank an anonymous reviewer of a previous version of this article for telling

me about Lishner’s research. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed

to Juan Pablo Kalawski, Counseling Center, University of Louisville, 2207 South Brook

Street, Louisville, Kentucky 40292. E-mail: [email protected]

2 Of course, some scholars do not subscribe to the basic emotions approach (e.g., Ortony

and Turner, 1990). For them, the question of whether tenderness is a basic emotion would

probably be meaningless. Those scholars might still be interested in the findings on

tenderness discussed in the literature review, as well as those from the present study, even

if those findings are not interpreted in terms of a basic emotions approach.

3 This interpretation was suggested by an anonymous reviewer.

27

Page 28: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Table 1

Emotional stimuli conceptualized on sadness-joy vs. anger-tenderness dimensions.

Tenderness Dimension

Angry Neutral Tender

Joy

Dim

ensi

on

Sad Pitiful

Neutral Neutral Cute

Amusing Ironic Funny

28

Page 29: The Subjective Distinction between the Basic Emotions of

Is Tenderness a Basic

Table 2

Mean ratings by emotion measured and experimental condition.

ConditionEmotion

Cute Pitiful Funny Ironic Neutral

Joy 7.37 (1.88) 1.03 (1.36) 7.19 (1.90) 4.45 (2.74) 1.97 (1.82)

Tenderness 7.22 (1.93) 7.00 (2.02) 2.08 (2.38) .90 (1.56) .71 (1.22)

Sadness 1.33 (1.98) 8.19 (1.79) .04 (.20) 1.16 (1.68) .45 (1.31)

Anger .30 (.95) 1.76 (2.18) .04 (.20) 2.19 (.20) .42 (1.52)

Sample Size 27 32 26 31 31

Note. Numbers in parentheses are standard deviations.

29