the spatial ecology of coral reef fishes · common on coral reefs. however, the response of...

196
This file is part of the following reference: Welsh, Justin Quentin (2014) The spatial ecology of coral reef fishes. PhD thesis, James Cook University. Access to this file is available from: http://researchonline.jcu.edu.au/40758/ The author has certified to JCU that they have made a reasonable effort to gain permission and acknowledge the owner of any third party copyright material included in this document. If you believe that this is not the case, please contact [email protected] and quote http://researchonline.jcu.edu.au/40758/ ResearchOnline@JCU

Upload: others

Post on 07-Jul-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

This file is part of the following reference:

Welsh, Justin Quentin (2014) The spatial ecology of coral

reef fishes. PhD thesis, James Cook University.

Access to this file is available from:

http://researchonline.jcu.edu.au/40758/

The author has certified to JCU that they have made a reasonable effort to gain

permission and acknowledge the owner of any third party copyright material

included in this document. If you believe that this is not the case, please contact

[email protected] and quote

http://researchonline.jcu.edu.au/40758/

ResearchOnline@JCU

The Spatial Ecology of Coral Reef

Fishes

Thesis submitted by

Justin Quentin Welsh Bsc (Hons) James Cook University

August 2014

for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Marine Biology School of Marine and Tropical Biology and

ARC Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies, James Cook University, Townsville, Queensland, Australia

i

Statement of Access

I, the undersigned, author of this thesis, understand that James Cook University will make this

thesis available for the use within the University Library and, via the Australian Digital Thesis

Network, for use elsewhere.

I understand that as unpublished work a thesis has significant protection under Copyright Act

and;

I do not wish to put any further restriction on access to this thesis.

__________________________________ ___________________

Signature Date

ii

Declaration of Ethics

This research presented and reported in this thesis was conducted in compliance with the

National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Australian Code of Practice for the

Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes, 7th Edition, 2004 and the Qld Animal Care

and Protection Act, 2001. The proposed research study received animal ethics approval from

the JCU Animal Ethics Committee Approval Number #A1700.

__________________________________ ___________________

Signature Date

iii

Statement of Sources

I declare that this thesis is my own work and has not been submitted in any form for another

degree or diploma at my university or other institution of tertiary education. Information

derived from the published or unpublished work of others has been acknowledged in the text

and a list of references is given.

__________________________________ ___________________

Signature Date

iv

Electronic Copy Declaration

I, the undersigned and author of this work, declare that the electronic copy of this thesis

provided to James Cook University Library is an accurate copy of the print thesis submitted,

within the limits of technology available.

__________________________________ ___________________

Signature Date

v

Statement on the Contribution of Others

This thesis includes some collaborative work with my supervisor Professor David

Bellwood (James Cook University) as well as Dr Rebecca J Fox (James Cook

University), Dr Christopher HR Goatley and Dale Webber (Vemco, Canada). While

undertaking these collaborations, I was responsible for the project concept and design,

the majority of data collection, analyses and interpretation, as well as the final synthesis

of results into a form suitable for publication. My collaborators provided intellectual

guidance, equipment, financial support, technical instruction, statistical advice and

editorial assistance. Data for chapter 4 was derived from previously unpublished data

collected by Prof. David Bellwood, however, I was responsible for the data analysis,

interpretation and final synthesis of the results into a form suitable for publication.

Financial support for the project was provided by the Australian Museum and

Lizard Island Reef Research Foundation, the James Cook University Graduate Research

Scheme and the Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef

Studies. Stipend support was provided by a James Cook University International

Research Scholarship. Financial support for conference travel was provided by the

Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies.

vi

Acknowledgements

I would like to start by acknowledge my supervisor, Professor David Bellwood.

Throughout my career as a researcher you have been an unwavering source of

encouragement, guidance and support. Words cannot express my gratitude to you for

everything you have done for me.

Furthermore, I would like to extend a special thanks to all my collaborators;

Christopher Goatley, Kirsty Nash, Dale Webber, Rebecca Fox and Roberta Bonaldo.

All of you have been an inspiration to me throughout my candidature and I will forever

appreciative for your support, advice and patience.

As with any project involving fieldwork, this would not have been possible

without the tireless efforts of many field assistants. Thank you to Roberta Bonaldo,

Yolly Bosiger, Simon Brandl, David Duchene, Christopher Goatley, Sophie Gordon,

Jess Hopf, Simon Hunt, Michael Kramer, Susannah Leahy, Carine Lefèvre, Oona

Lonnstedt, Mat Mitchell, Chiara Pisapia, Chris Pickens, Justin Rizarri, Derek Sun, Tara

Stephens, John Welsh and Johanna Werminghausen for your time, energy,

encouragement and friendship during countless hours of data collection. I am also very

grateful to the staff at the Lizard Island Research Station, and Orpheus Island Research

Station; Lyle Vale, Anne Hoggett, Lance and Marianne Pearce, Bob and Tanya Lamb,

Stuart and Kym Pulbrook, Kylie and Rob Eddie, Lachin and Louise Wilkins and Haley

Burgess. Your assistance and support made this project possible.

My thanks are also extended to a number of other people who were invaluable

along the way. I would like to thank; Sean Connolly, Jess Hopf, Marcus Sheaves,

Richard Rowe, Steven Swearer and Vinay Udyawer for analytical and statistical advice;

Roberta Bonaldo, Simon Brandl, Howard Choat, Christopher Goatley, Jen Hodge, Jess

Hopf, Charlotte Johansson, Zoe Loffler, Roland Muñoz, Jenn Tanner and one

anonymous reviwer for helpful discussion and comments on the text; Michelle Heupel

and Colin Simpfendorfer for their valuable insights and for providing data from

AANIMS receivers. Furthermore, I would like to extend a special thanks to all those

who worked behind the scenes to make research possible; G. Bailey and V. Pullella for

IT support, R. Abom, G. Bailey, G. Ewels, P. Osmond and J. Webb for boating, diving

and logistical support; J. Fedorniak and K. Wood for travel assistance; D. Ford and S.

Frances for a number of matters; and S. Manolis for purchasing.

vii

This research was made possible due to the generous support of many funding

bodies. A special thanks goes out to the Australia Museum and Lizard Island, who

supported this research through a Lizard Island Research Station Doctoral Fellowship.

Valuable funding was also provided by the James Cook University Internal Research

Awards and Graduate Research Scheme, as well as the Australian Research Council

through Prof. David Bellwood. Furthermore, many thanks are extended to James Cook

University who provided a Postgraduate Research Scholarship for the duration of my

candidature.

I am also very grateful to my close friends Karina, Jen, Ingrid, Jess, Charlotte, Jo,

Zoe and Derek. You were not only a source of happiness and kindness during my

candidature but also endless inspiration and encouragement. I am also extremely

thankful to the members of the Reef Fish Lab. A. Hoey, B. Fox, C. Lefèvre, C.

Johansson, C. Goatley, C. Heckathorn, M. Young, M. Kramer, J. Johansen, J. Hodge, J.

Tanner, K. Chong-Seng, K. Nash, P. Cowman, S. Brandl and Z. Loffler for all your

shared expertise and enthusiasm. A very special thank you goes out to my two families

in Australia, The Bellwoods (Dave, Orpha, Hannah and Oli) and Hunts (Alan, Kerry,

Kym, Annie, Emily and Simon). You truly have made me feel at home on the other side

of the world. To my family back in Canada, dad, mom and Jazzy, I cannot thank you

enough for your words of wisdom and kindness and most of all, for always being there

for me. It is because of you that I was able to realize my dream.

Last but definitely not least I would like to thank my partner, Simon Hunt. The

support, friendship, patience and especially love you have shown me have been my

greatest source of motivation. With you beside me, anything is possible.

viii

Abstract

Movement is a fundamental component of a species’ ecology and the study of space use

in organisms has a long-standing history as a conservation tool. Within an ecosystem,

numerous functional processes are conferred by taxa, and are essential to maintain

stable ecosystem processes. The application of functional roles is, however, bound by

the home ranges of the taxa responsible, and thus, the spatial ecology of organisms is of

great significance to ecosystem health. Coral reefs are among the most vulnerable

ecosystems to degradation and yet, the spatial ecology of key species which support

coral reef resilience remain largely unknown. In this thesis I therefore endeavoured to

quantify the spatial ecology of functionally important coral reef herbivores to further

our understanding of ecological processes.

Passive acoustic receivers are commonly used to remotely monitor animal

movements in the marine environment. The detection range and diel performance of

acoustic receivers was assessed using two parallel lines of 5 VR2W receivers spanning

125 m, deployed on the reef base and reef crest. The working detection range (distance

within which > 50% of detections are recorded) for receivers was found to be

approximately 90 m on the reef base and 60 m on the reef crest. No diel patterns in

receiver performance or detection capacities were detected. These results are in contrast

to those in non-reef environments, with coral reefs presenting a unique and challenging

environment for the use of acoustic telemetry.

Using a dense array of passive acoustic receivers, the maximum potential areas

occupied by the schooling herbivorous fish, Scarus rivulatus, was quantified over 7

months. Despite schooling, all S. rivulatus were site attached. On average, the

maximum potential home range of individuals was 24,440 m2 and ranges overlapped

extensively in individuals captured from the same school. The area shared by all

members of the same school was smaller than that of individual’s average home range,

measuring 21,652 m2. This suggests that school fidelity in this species may be low and

while favourable, schooling represents a facultative behavioural association. However,

schooling was found to have a beneficial influence on ecosystem processes, with

feeding rates in schooling S. rivulatus being double those of non-schooling individuals.

Despite adult parrotfish being largely site attached, the ontogeny of these fishes’

home range expansion is not yet known. This study therefore assessed the home range

size of three different parrotfish species at every stage of development following

ix

settlement onto the reef. With masses spanning five orders of magnitude, from the early

post-settlement stage through to adulthood, no evidence of a response to predation risk,

dietary shifts or sex change on home range expansion rates was found. Instead, a

distinct ontogenetic shift in home range expansion with sexual maturity was

documented. Juvenile parrotfishes displayed rapid home range growth until reaching

approximately 100 - 150 mm long. Thereafter, the relationship between home range and

mass broke down. This shift reflected changes in colour patterns, social status and

reproductive behaviour associated with the transition to adult stages.

The majority of herbivorous reef fishes are regarded as ‘roving herbivores’,

despite new evidence recording these taxa as being highly site attached. The extents to

which site-attached behaviour is prevalent in herbivorous reef fishes was assessed by

quantifying the movements of a largely overlooked family of functionally important

coral reef browsers, the Kyphosidae, and comparing their movements to other coral reef

herbivores. Kyphosus vaigiensis exhibited regular, large-scale (> 2 km). Each day

individual K. vaigiensis cover, on average, 2.5 km of reef (11 km maximum). A meta-

analysis of home range data from other herbivores found a consistent relationship

between home range size and body length. Only K. vaigiensis departs significantly from

the expected home-range body size relationship, with home range sizes more

comparable to large pelagic predators rather than other reef herbivores. These large-

scale movements of K. vaigiensis suggest that this species is a mobile link, providing

functional connectivity, and helping to support functional processes across habitats and

spatial scales.

Habitat degradation in the form of macroalgal outbreaks is becoming increasingly

common on coral reefs. However, the response of herbivores to algal outbreaks has

never been evaluated in a spatial context. Therefore, the spatial response of herbivorous

reef fishes was assessed with a combination of acoustic and video monitoring, to

quantify changes in the movements and abundances, respectively, of coral reef

herbivores following a simulated outbreak. An unprecedented accumulation of

functionally important herbivorous taxa was found in response to the algae. Herbivore

abundances increased by 267%, but only where algae were present. This pattern was

driven entirely by the browsing species, Naso unicornis and K. vaigiensis, which were

over 10x more abundant at the sites of simulated degradation. Resident individuals at

the site of the degradation exhibited no change in their movements. Instead, analysis of

the size classes of the responding individuals indicates that the increase in the

x

abundance of functionally important individuals occurred as large non-resident

individuals changed their movement patterns to feed on the algae.

Overall, the site attached nature of coral reef fish spatial ecology highlights a

spatial limitation to the scale of functional processes, and the vulnerability of reefs to

localized impacts. Indeed, the movements of the most mobile known herbivore, K.

vaigiensis, while extensive, were restricted to a single island, despite distances of only

250 m between islands. This suggests that functional connectivity provided by mobile

adults may be limited, and that processes occurring within-reefs are highly important.

Even resident taxa may be unwilling to shift their spatial patterns to consume algal

outbreaks, leaving reefs vulnerable to a patchwork of algal establishment. Such fixed

spatial patterns in coral reef fish emphasize the importance of large mobile taxa.

However, these larger individuals are often the most highly targeted by extractive

activities and can easily move beyond the boundaries of marine protected areas

(MPAs). Therefore, to protected highly important individuals, management initiatives

are required beyond small-scale reserves. Species specific management may be

required.

xi

Table of Contents Page

Statement of Access i

Declaration of Ethics ii

Statement of Sources iii

Electronic Copy Declaration iv

Statement on the Contribution of Others v

Acknowledgements vi

Abstract viii

Table of Contents xi

List of Figures xiii

List of Tables xvii

Chapter 1: General Introduction 1

Chapter 2: Performance of remote acoustic receivers within a coral reef habitat:

implications for array design 9

2.1. Introduction 9

2.2. Materials and Methods 11

2.3 Results 19

2.4 Discussion 25

Chapter 3: How far do schools of roving herbivores rove? A case study using

Scarus rivulatus 32

3.1 Introduction 32

3.2 Materials and Methods 35

3.3 Results 42

3.4 Discussion 50

Chapter 4: The ontogeny of home ranges: evidence from coral reef fishes 59

4.1 Introduction 59

xii

4.2 Materials and Methods 61

4.3 Results 65

4.4 Discussion 68

Chapter 5: Herbivorous fishes, ecosystem function and mobile links on coral reefs

75

5.1 Introduction 75

5.2 Materials and Methods 78

5.3 Results 83

5.4 Discussion 88

Chapter 6: Local degradation triggers a large-scale response on coral reefs 95

6.1 Introduction 95

6.2 Materials and Methods 98

6.3 Results 105

6.4 Discussion 112

Chapter 7: Concluding Discussion 118

References 123

Appendix A: Supplementary Materials for Chapter 2 150

Appendix B: Supplementary Materials for Chapter 4 151

Appendix C: Supplementary Materials for Chapter 5 155

Appendix D: Supplementary Materials for Chapter 6 163

Appendix E: Assessment of the impact of acoustic tagging on fish mortality and

behaviour 172

Appendix F: Publications arising from thesis 175

xiii

List of Figures Page

Fig. 1.1 Number of studies evaluating the home range size in reef fishes using visual

estimations, acoustic telemetry (active and passive combined) and other methods

(Modified from Nash et al. in review). 4

Fig. 1.2 a) VR2W acoustic receiver mooring b) acoustic transmitter implanted into

visceral cavity c) study species used in Chapter 2, Scarus rivulatus and d) study

species used in Chapter 5, Kyphosus vaigiensis. 6

Fig. 2.1 Study site. Pioneer Bay, Orpheus Island, Great Barrier Reef. a) Map showing

location of range testing array within Pioneer Bay, b) locations of remote

acoustic receivers along reef base contour (grey squares) and reef crest contour

(black squares), fixed delay test transmitters (Vemco, V9-1L) were moored 0.5

m above the substratum at opposite ends of the array at deep (grey cross) and

shallow (black cross) positions, and c) an illustration of the depth at which the

receivers were placed as well as the reef profile (please note, receivers and

transmitters are not to scale, horizontal axis is truncated; receivers are 25 m

apart). 13

Fig. 2.2 a) Relationship between the probability of detection and distance from the

receiver for a transmitter moored on the reef base during diurnal hours (grey

line, linear regression, slope = -0.52, constant = 94.56, P < 0.001, r2 = 0.52) and

nocturnal hours (black line, linear regression, slope = -0.49, constant = 90.92, P

< 0.001, r2 = 0.48) and b) relationship between the number of successful versus

unsuccessful detections and distance from the receiver for a transmitter moored

on the reef crest during diurnal hours (grey line, logistic regression, slope = -

0.084, constant = 2.35, P < 0.001, Nagelkerke r2 = 0.71) and nocturnal hours

(black line, logistic regression, slope = -0.067, constant = 4.08, P < 0.001,

Nagelkerke r2 = 0.64). Detection probabilities are shown for each 6-h period of

the 7-day test and are classified as diurnal (0601-1800 h) (grey circles) or

nocturnal (1801-0600 h) (black circles). Nocturnal data points have been shifted

slightly left on the y-axis to eliminated significant overlap with diurnal data

points. 22

xiv

Fig. 2.3 Diel detection frequency (mean detections per hourly bin over the 7-day test

period ± SE) across the entire array for the two test transmitters. Shading

indicates nocturnal hours (1801-0600 h). 24

Fig. 3.1 Map of the placement of acoustic receivers (VR2W) within Pioneer Bay on

Orpheus Island, Great Barrier Reef. Dark grey boxes represent the placement of

shallow water receivers on the sub-tidal reef crest, and light grey boxes

represent deep water receivers moored on the reef base. The circles around each

receiver represent the receiver’s estimated detection range. 37

Fig. 3.2 Plot representing the relative proportion of total diurnal detections for each

individual Scarus rivulatus at each receiver for individuals in a) school 1, c)

school 2 and e) school 3. The diameter of each circle is in proportion to the

number of detections at a receiver. Circles provided in the legend box are

examples only. Receiver numbers in black represent reef crest receivers and

those in bold represent receivers on the reef base. The maximum area of reef

occupied by b) school 1, d) school 2 and f) school 3 within Pioneer Bay are

represented by grey areas. The solid black line represents the reef crest and the

site of capture and release of the school is represented by the black dot. Only

receivers with > 5% of individuals or schools detections are shown. 44

Fig. 3.3 Frequency histogram of the average school size that individual Scarus rivulatus

were associated with over a 5-min observational period (n = 60). The dashed

line approximates the school size frequencies that are expected following a

Poisson distribution. 47

Fig. 3.4 Feeding rate (bites min-1; mean ± SE) of a) Scarus rivulatus and b) other

scarids (which include S. ghobban, S. Schlegeli, S. niger, S. flavipectoralis and

Hipposcarus longiceps) recorded for both schooling and solitary individuals at

four discrete periods of the day at Pioneer Bay, Orpheus Island, Great Barrier

Reef. 48

Fig. 3.5 Conceptual model of the degradation in the feeding rate of schooling

individuals as individuals’ density declines and areas of home range overlap

decrease relative to non-schooling individuals, based on a doubling of feeding

rates in schools. Home range overlap was created using the random allocation of

five home ranges to a fixed area. 57

xv

Fig. 4.1 Relationship between body mass (g) and home range size (m2) for Scarus

frenatus, S. niger and Chlorurus sordidus. Triangles represent juvenile

individuals and squares and circles represent initial phase and terminal phase

individuals respectively. Grey triangles indicate juveniles, which are

predominantly omnivores/carnivores (following Bellwood 1988). The dotted

vertical line indicates the maximum body mass achieved before S. frenatus and

S. niger undergo juvenile to adult colour changes, and C. sordidus shifts from

solitary to schooling behaviour. 66

Fig. 4.2 Scale-independent relationship between body mass and home range size. To

remove the effects of scale, body mass was cube-root transformed and home

range areas were square-root transformed. Data are presented for all species;

Scarus frenatus, S. niger and Chlorurus sordidus, with the dotted line

representing the size at maturity. The fitted line is a logarithmic regression, see

text for details. 67

Fig. 5.1 Orpheus Island receiver (VR2W, Vemco) deployment sites. Black circles mark

the location of each receiver. Filled in circles represent receivers with > 5% of at

least one individual Kyphosus vaigiensis’ detections; open circles had no

significant detections. a) Map of Australia showing the location of Orpheus

Island along the Queensland coast. b) Array in Pioneer Bay showing depth

contours and numbered stars representing capture and release sites of individuals

(capture site 1: K40, K41, K42, K43, K44; capture site 2: K31, K32, K35;

capture site 3: K36, K37, K38, K39; capture site 4: K33, K34). Dotted lines

represent reef area. 80

Fig. 5.2 Relationship between home range length (m) and fish body length (√fork

length; mm) for carnivorous and herbivorous taxa with 95% confidence intervals

for each trendline. Species with a Cook’s distance value of > 0.5 are labelled

and have solid circles. 87

Fig. 6.1 Visual representation of the simulated macroalgal outbreak depicting initial

macroalgal deployment density with acoustic receiver placement. 98

Fig. 6.2 The average abundance of a) total herbivore community and b) browsing

herbivore community before (white), during (black) and after (grey) a simulated

phase shift to macroalgae. 107

xvi

Fig. 6.3 Size frequency distribution of key browsing taxa; Kyphosus vaigiensis, Naso

unicornis, K. cinerascens and Siganus doliatus before (white), during (black)

and following (grey) a simulated phase shift to macroalgae. 108

Fig. 6.4 Non-metric multidimensional scaling (nMDS) analysis showing the

relationship between 8 sites at three different treatment levels (pre, during and

post algal treatment) based on the abundance of herbivorous fish taxa. During

algal deployment, sites are designated as either control or treatment. Ellipses

represent significant groupings identified by ANOSIM. Vector lengths indicate

the relative contribution of each species to the observed pattern. Grey triangles

() represent control sites while algae were present, inverted black triangles ()

represent treatment sites while algae were present, unfilled triangles () represent

all sites prior to algal treatment and black triangles () represent all sites post-

algal treatment. 2D stress: 0.18. 110

Fig. 6.5 Change in individual detections rates (# detections.day-1 during algal

deployment - detections.day-1 prior to deployment) for all Signaus vulpinus (n =

6), S. corallinus (n = 6), Scarus schlegeli (n = 17) and Naso unicornis (n = 3).

Dark grey boxes represent fishes with core areas of detection was at the algal

deployment site (n = 4, 6, 9 and 3 respectively). Box represents mean ± SE and

whiskers represent minimum and maximum values. None of the species’ change

in detection rates following algal deployment were significantly different from

zero. 111

xvii

List of Tables Page

Table 3.1 Summary of detection data and characteristics from 18 tagged Scarus

rivulatus from three separate schools captured in Pioneer Bay, Orpheus Island,

Australia. 45

Table 3.2 Three-way ANOVA comparing the a) rank-transformed feeding rates

for Scarus rivulatus, b) pooled log-transformed feeding rates for Scarus

ghobban, Scarus schegeli, Scarus niger, Scarus flavipectoralis and

Hipposcarus longiceps 49

Table 5.1 Average metrics of Kyphosus vaigiensis movement data separated by

diurnal and nocturnal sampling periods. For individual data, please see

Appendix C. 84

1

Chapter 1: General Introduction

Habitat modification and degradation is occurring at an unprecedented global scale,

with numerous ecological repercussions (Pandolfi et al. 2003; Hoegh-Guldberg et al.

2007; Reyer et al. 2013). The human population is expected to reach 9.3 billion by 2050

(Lee 2011) and thus, an increase in the environmental disturbances caused by

anthropogenic activity is inevitable (Hughes 1994; Bellwood et al. 2012; Cinner et al.

2012). Acting in concert with direct stressors, indirect impacts such as global climate

change are predicted to lead to disturbance events, such as hurricanes and cyclones, of

increased severity, furthering the likelihood of ecosystem degradation (Hoegh-Guldberg

et al. 2007; Bell et al. 2013; Holland and Bruyère 2013). The capacity of an

environment to absorb the effects of deleterious events, and return to a healthy, pre-

disturbance state is described as that environment’s resilience (Hughes et al. 2003,

2005; Dudgeon et al. 2010). However, the resilience of the environment is largely

contingent on several factors that can undermine or support it.

Chronic pressures are among the most significant contributors to reduced

resilience (Nyström et al. 2008; Hughes et al. 2010). Examples of chronic

environmental disturbances include introduced species (Vitousek et al. 1997), nutrient

loading and eutrophication (Smith and Schindler 2009) and overexploitation of natural

populations (Lokrantz et al. 2010). Probably the best example of the effects of multiple

chronic pressures on coral reefs has been reported from the Caribbean. Overfishing

reduced the resilience of Caribbean coral reefs by significantly reducing piscine

herbivore populations, reducing ecological redundancies (Jackson et al. 2001;

Knowlton 2001; Hughes et al. 2003). As a result, the environment was unable to

recover to a pre-disturbance state following the regional scale loss of Diadema

2

antillarum and a range of other local stresses. This produced a large-scale phase-shift

that occurred on Jamaican reefs (Hughes et al. 1994). This phase-shift resulted in the

reef community shifting away from a coral dominated state, to one in which macroalgae

covered the majority of the benthos (Hughes et al. 1994; Connell 1997). Since then,

experimental studies have been able to simulate a similar effect on other tropical reefs

by excluding key species and simulating a scenario where a system’s resilience has

been undermined (e.g. Stephenson and Searles 1960; Hughes et al. 2007; Burkepile and

Hay 2010). Thus, we know how declines in coral reef health are triggered. The

challenge now is to prevent ecosystem decline by identifying and managing ecological

processes that support ecosystem resilience.

Among the key elements in ecosystem resilience are the interactions between taxa

and their environments (Bellwood et al. 2004; Elmqvist et al. 2010). Ecosystems are

reliant on a variety of functions provided by several taxa, which maintain the

environment in a normal, healthy state (Bellwood et al. 2004; Carpenter et al. 2006;

Olds et al. 2012). Examples of such functions include predation, essential for

maintaining stable, diverse populations (Terborgh et al. 2001; Knight et al. 2005);

detritivory, facilitating nutrient cycling (Depczynski and Bellwood 2003); and

herbivory, which controls algal communities (Ledlie et al. 2007; Burkepile and Hay

2010). While the functions are numerous, the species responsible for each function can

be few in number and vary extensively over different spatial scales (Cheal et al. 2012).

Functional redundancy has been suggested to be an essential element of

ecological resilience, in that key functional roles can be fulfilled by various species,

providing insurance for ecosystem functions (Sundstrom et al. 2012). However, recent

evidence suggests that functional redundancy is not as prevalent as previously assumed

(Bellwood et al. 2006; Brandl and Bellwood 2013; Johansson et al. 2013). Due to the

fine-scale niche partitioning that can exist in complex biological systems (a

3

characteristic of the tropics), there are often limited numbers of taxa capable of

conferring essential ecosystem services (Connell 1997; Patterson et al. 2003; Fox and

Bellwood 2013; Mouillot et al. 2013). Coral reefs are among the best examples, with

herbivorous coral reef fish being among the most important for reef resilience (e.g.

Hughes et al 2007). Within the herbivores, several contrasting functions exist and each

is dominated by a limited number of taxa (Bellwood et al. 2004; Burkepile and Hay

2008; Hoey and Bellwood 2009). Furthermore, when assessed using bioassays and

manipulative experiments, the rates at which the functional processes are applied and

the primary species driving them are highly variable at a range of spatial scales (Bennett

and Bellwood 2011; Vergés et al. 2011; Johansson et al. 2013).

The spatial scales over which functions are applied, is inherently bound by the

home ranges of those that moderate the process. In this sense, a great deal of the

observed variability in functional processes on coral reefs may result from the spatial

biology of key taxa (Fox and Bellwood 2011; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a).

Traditionally, the home ranges of animals have been assessed to estimate the

effectiveness of protected areas (e.g. Meyer and Holland 2005; Afonso et al. 2009;

Bryars et al. 2012), or nature reserves (e.g. Eloff 1959; Broomhall et al. 2003), and to

understand migration pathways of charismatic or commercially important species (e.g.

Berger 2004; Hedger et al. 2008). However, few studies have considered the

importance of interactions between organisms and their environment, in the context of

home ranges (but see Cooke et al. 2004; Owen-Smith et al. 2010; Fox and Bellwood

2011; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). It is surprising that a factor such as movement,

which is intrinsic to the application of functional process, has been largely overlooked

on coral reefs, one of the most threatened environments.

Given the logistical constraints of assessing the home ranges of fishes, spatial

studies in the marine environment have historically lagged behind their terrestrial

4

counterparts. The methods associated with quantifying movement in terrestrial systems

have evolved over time from visual observations and mapping, to radio telemetry

(Harris et al. 1990; Laver & Kelly 2008) and satellite tagging (Jouventin &

Weimerskirch 1990). In the case of marine species, especially fishes, before the late 90s

studies were largely restricted to visual observations (Kramer & Chapman 1999), due to

the limitations of working in the marine environment (but see Holland et al. 1996;

Zeller 1997). However the application and refinement of acoustic telemetry in the last

few decades has made it possible to accurately monitor the movement of marine species

and to estimate the home range of a broader range of taxa (Fig.1.1; Bolden 2001;

Voegeli et al. 2001; Cooke et al. 2004; Heupel et al. 2006).

Fig. 1.1 Number of studies evaluating the home range size in reef fishes using visual

estimations, acoustic telemetry (active and passive combined) and other methods

(Modified from Nash et al. in review).

0

10

20

30

40

1984−1993

1994−2003

2004−2013

Num

ber o

f stu

dies Visual

Telemetry Other

5

The evolution of acoustic telemetry as a means to monitor the movement of

marine taxa has largely evolved in two directions; active and passive acoustic

monitoring. Active acoustic monitoring is used to collect high-resolution data on the

short-term movements of a focal individual (Meyer and Holland 2005; Fox and

Bellwood 2011; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). While this technique is useful for studies

that require highly detailed data on animal movements, it is limited in that the battery

life of the transmitters is often less than a month, data collection is labour intensive

(Voegeli et al. 2001) and tracking fish from motorized vessels in shallow water may

modify their behavior (Meyer and Holland 2005; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). For

long-term studies, passive acoustic monitoring is often favoured. Using passive acoustic

monitoring, the presence or absence data of many tagged individuals can be collected

by a network of acoustic receivers for a period of months to years (Fig. 1.2a, b; Heupel

et al. 2006; Welsh et al. 2012). Another benefit of this technology is that movements

can be tracked over large spatial scales with minimal upkeep and maintenance of the

receivers (Heupel et al. 2008). Therefore, data can be continuously collected, even in

remote location when continued access to field sites may not be permitted. With the

development of these tracking techniques for the marine environment, the study of coral

reef fish spatial biology has represented a burgeoning field of research. However, the

application of animal movement data to ecological questions has been limited and thus,

our understanding of the ecological implications of reef fish movement remain in its

infancy.

The aim of this thesis, therefore, is to provide a spatial context for ecological

interactions and to evaluate for the importance of spatial biology in ecological research.

More specifically, the studies herein are aimed to place the ecosystem functions of key

6

herbivorous fish taxa on the Great Barrier Reef (GBR) in a spatial context and to assess

to what extent their movement patterns may influence ecosystem resilience.

Fig. 1.2 a) VR2W acoustic receiver mooring b) acoustic transmitter implanted into

visceral cavity c) study species used in Chapter 2, Scarus rivulatus and d) study species

used in Chapter 5, Kyphosus vaigiensis.

We address the objective of the thesis in five data chapters. Each data chapter

either relates to a publication derived from the present work or has been submitted for

review in a scientific journal (Appendix F). The evaluation of spatial patterns in reef

fishes are limited, especially when compared to terrestrial taxa or even temperate or

pelagic fishes. This is partially a result of the difficulties in collecting telemetry data for

coral reef species, even using modern acoustic telemetry. Therefore, the question

remains as to how acoustic receivers perform on coral reefs and whether or not they can

a) b)

c) d)

7

be used as an effective tool to quantify the movements of benthic fish taxa. In Chapter

2 this question is addressed, with an evaluation of the performance of ultrasonic

acoustic receivers on coral reefs (Fig. 1.1a, b). Furthermore, this chapter provides data

to inform the construction of acoustic arrays and information pertaining to the

interpretation of animal movement patterns derived from acoustic telemetry on coral

reefs. With methods established to monitor the movements of fishes on coral reefs using

acoustic telemetry, questions regarding the scale of movements, and thus ecological

interactions, conferred by key taxa can be addressed.

Chapter 3 evaluates the link between social systems and home range extent in

parrotfishes. This question is addressed by quantifying the movement patterns of Scarus

rivulatus (Fig. 1.1c), an important reef herbivore on the GBR, and their foraging

schools, placing the term ‘roving herbivore’ in a spatial context. In Chapter 4 the rate

of ontogenetic home range expansion is assessed for a number of different parrotfish

species as they grow in body mass over five orders of magnitude. The resulting pattern

is then compared to that of higher vertebrates. Despite a growing body of literature on

the movements of coral reef fishes, the true maximum of mobility in herbivorous coral

reef fishes is yet to be assessed, and the key question of ‘what is a true roving

herbivore’ remains. Chapter 5 assesses the movements of a browsing herbivore

Kyphosus vaigiensis (Fig. 1.1d) over large spatial scales to address this question. The

movements of this species were then compared to all available studies conducted on

reef fishes in order to create a context by which large-scale movements can be

identified. Finally, Chapter 6 presents a manipulative experiment in which habitat

degradation is simulated on a coral reef and the spatial response of resident and non-

resident coral reef herbivores is assessed. This thesis is ends with a concluding

discussion which examines the importance of the spatial biology of reef fishes in

8

relation to ecosystem functioning and provides a summary of the studies available on

the movements of coral reef fishes.

9

Chapter 2: Performance of remote acoustic receivers within

a coral reef habitat: implications for array design Published in Coral Reefs 2012 31: 693-702

2.1. Introduction

Investigations of the movement patterns and site fidelity of aquatic species are now

increasingly being carried out using passive (remote) acoustic monitoring, where focal

individuals are tagged with coded transmitters and are monitored at automated listening

stations (receivers) (Afonso et al. 2009; Semmens et al. 2010; Simpfendorfer et al.

2011). Of all peer-reviewed studies carried out using remote acoustic telemetry, more

than one-third have been published in the last 3 years. Passive acoustic monitoring,

therefore, represents a burgeoning field, presenting the opportunity to track the

movement of individuals over periods of months (Egli and Babcock 2004; March et al.

2010) or years (Afonso et al. 2008; Meyer et al. 2010), and giving researchers the

opportunity to test hypotheses relating to long-term habitat usage and site fidelity. The

technology has been most frequently employed within estuarine (e.g. Hartill et al. 2003;

Heupel et al. 2006), riverine (e.g. Winter et al. 2006) or deep-water oceanic habitats

(e.g. Clements et al. 2005). Increasingly, however, the methodology is being utilized

within the coral reef environment, particularly to answer important questions relating to

the site fidelity and habitat use of harvested reef fish species (e.g. Meyer et al. 2010;

O’Toole et al. 2011).

Despite the remarkable technological advances that have facilitated the increased

ease and flexibility of use of remote acoustic monitoring, the interpretation of data

collected by automated listening stations is still a developing area of research (Lacroix

and Voegeli 2000; Clements et al. 2005; Simpfendorfer et al. 2008). Critical to the

10

interpretation of detections made by an acoustic array is an understanding of both the

detection range (Klimley et al. 1998) and the performance (sensu Simpfendorfer et al.

2008) of receivers within that array. Ultimately, the coverage yielded by the array at

any given time will determine whether the data collected represents either a minimum

or complete estimate of the animal’s movement range. Detection ranges are all too

frequently assumed, rather than tested. Where range tests are undertaken and reported

for individual studies, detection ranges can deviate from the value reported in

manufacturers’ product specifications, highlighting the discrepancy in listening range

for receivers within different aquatic habitats (Voegeli and Pincock 1996; Heupel et al.

2006). Both the detection range and performance of individual monitoring stations have

been shown to be highly variable on temporal and spatial scales (Simpfendorfer et al.

2008; Payne et al. 2010). Without a full understanding of this variability in

performance, the behaviour of the organisms being studied can be grossly

misinterpreted (e.g. Payne et al. 2010).

The constraints of the technology, and the potential for variability in the detection

performance of monitoring stations highlights the importance of properly evaluating

receiver performance prior to and during each individual study (Heupel et al. 2006).

However, there is currently a paucity of studies focusing on the acoustic equipment and

its performance, especially on coral reefs (Heupel et al. 2008). As information on

equipment performance in any given environment is integral to understanding telemetry

results, variability in detection ranges between different environments should be a

consideration in data analysis and interpretation. This is particularly important on coral

reefs, which represent a relatively new and potentially difficult environment for the

acoustic technology. Coral reefs are extremely noisy environments with a plethora of

reef noise generated by the feeding, mating and territorial displays of invertebrates and

fish taxa (e.g. Cato 1978; McCauley and Cato 2000; Simpson et al. 2008a, b). Reef

11

noise, coupled with the high topographic complexity of coral reefs, may result in a

highly variable acoustic receiver detection range, unique to the reef environment. The

synergistic effects of the aforementioned obstacles when working on coral reefs stand to

significantly affect the performance of acoustic receivers, with median detection ranges

being reported as low as 108 m with a minimum value of 55 m (Meyer et al. 2010), well

below manufacturer’s specifications.

Recently, several performance metrics such as code detection efficiency, rejection

coefficients, and noise quotients have become available, making it possible to evaluate

the performance of receivers individually. The availability of performance metrics at

the scale of the individual receiver has created the potential to better understand how

the complexity and acoustic environment of coral reefs are influencing the receiver’s

capacity to detect acoustic transmitters, ultimately leading to an ameliorated capacity to

interpret telemetry data (Simpfendorfer et al. 2008).

The goals of the current study were: first, to investigate the detection range and

performance of ultrasonic acoustic receivers within a specific shallow coral reef

environment and, second, to provide data to inform the design of listening arrays and

interpretation of animal movement patterns within coral reef habitats more generally.

The specific aims of the study were to determine (1) the effective working detection

range of 9-mm acoustic transmitters within a coral reef environment, and (2) the extent

of diel variability in acoustic receiver performance on a coral reef.

2.2. Materials and Methods

The study site was a 1.5-km stretch of fringing reef within Pioneer Bay, Orpheus Island,

a granitic island in the inner-shelf region of the Great Barrier Reef lagoon (Fig. 2.1a).

The leeward stretch of reef within Pioneer Bay is a low-energy environment composed

12

of an extensive reef flat that reaches up to 400 m from the shoreline (details in Fox and

Bellwood 2007). The reef flat has little topographic complexity and is frequently

exposed at low tide. The reef crest is not sharply defined and is composed of many bare

patches of consolidated substratum. The crest gives way to a gentle slope that displays

high topographic complexity in many places near the crest created by large colonies of

Porites spp. and Acropora spp. interspersed with sand and coral rubble areas, which

create gullies and channels in many areas. At a depth of approximately 5 m (below

chart datum) the topographic complexity decreases and the reef slope continues as a

gently sloping sand substratum with occasional low patches of coral before flattening

off at approximately 18 m. Due to its location on the inner part of the continental shelf

and proximity to the mouth of the Herbert River, the reef on the leeward side of

Orpheus Island is in a high sediment environment, with turbidity often resulting in

visibility dropping to less than 2 m. Visibility is usually in the region of 4-10 m. Water

turbidity was consistent throughout the study period, with visibility remaining at

approximately 3 m.

13

Fig. 2.1 Study site. Pioneer Bay, Orpheus Island, Great Barrier Reef. a) Map showing

location of range testing array within Pioneer Bay, b) locations of remote acoustic

receivers along reef base contour (grey squares) and reef crest contour (black squares),

fixed delay test transmitters (Vemco, V9-1L) were moored 0.5 m above the substratum

at opposite ends of the array at deep (grey cross) and shallow (black cross) positions,

and c) an illustration of the depth at which the receivers were placed as well as the reef

profile (please note, receivers and transmitters are not to scale, horizontal axis is

truncated; receivers are 25 m apart).

Pioneer Bay

0.1km

Orpheus Island

(a)

5

4

3

2

1

0

CrestBase

Transmitter

TransmitterReceiver

Receiver

1 m0 m

5 m

N

Dep

th (m

; cha

rt da

tum

)

N

PB1S

PB2S

PB3S

PB4S

PB5S

PB6D

PB7D

PB8D

PB9D

PB10D

25 m

(b)

(c)

14

Transmitter detection-range tests

Maximum detection range

Prior to the commencement of the study, preliminary tests were carried out to determine

the maximum unobstructed detection range of 9-mm acoustic transmitters using fixed

delay transmitters, which have a predictable, and constant, transmission interval

(Vemco, V9-1L, 69 kHz, 5-s repeat rate, power output 146 dB re 1 lPa at 1 m). These

data were then used to estimate effective distance increments between receivers for

temporal detection range evaluations. In these initial tests, a single remote acoustic

receiver (VR2W, Vemco. Ltd., NS, Canada) was moored at a depth of 2 m

(approximately 5 m seaward off the reef crest). A fixed delay transmitter was then

moored for approximately 15 min at a distance of 50 m from the receiver, a sufficient

amount of time for the transmitter to produce more than 100 signal transmissions. After

this time, the transmitter was moved parallel to the reef, maintaining the same depth, to

a distance of 75 m where it was moored for an additional 15 min. The procedure was

repeated at 100, 125 and 150 m fixed distances from the receiver. The detection

efficiency of the receiver at each distance was then calculated based on the number of

recorded detections divided by the number expected over the deployment period at each

distance increment. The value for the expected number of detections could be

calculated from preliminary laboratory tests of the transmitter run prior to the field

deployment, as signals were produced by the transmitter at fixed, non-random time

intervals. The transmission interval was determined to be 8 s as a result of the

approximate 3 s it takes for the transmitter to emit a complete signal pulse train coupled

with the 5-s fixed delay transmission interval, giving an expected detection rate of 7.5

signals min-1.

15

Effective detection range and temporal variation in detection

Between 25th February and 3rd March 2011, 10 VR2W acoustic receivers were

deployed in Pioneer Bay. Based on the results of preliminary tests to determine

maximum detection range within the reef habitat (see above), the receivers were

positioned in parallel lines following two distinct reef zones. Each line along the reef

consisted of 5 VR2W receivers and was configured with the first two receivers spaced

50 m apart and the remaining 3 receivers spaced at 25 m increments (i.e. 0, 50, 75, 100

and 125 m from start point respectively; Fig. 2.1b). This deployment configuration is

designed to achieve high detection area coverage to estimate various spatial attributes of

site attached fish such as their home range (e.g. Marshell et al. 2011) or the median

distance travelled (Murchie et al. 2010). One line of receivers was positioned just

shoreward of the reef crest while the other receiver line followed the reef base contour

(Fig. 2.1b). Moorings for the receivers on the reef crest were placed at a depth of

approximately 1 m (below chart datum) and consisted of a 50 cm metal pole, the base

of which was sunk into a 30 kg concrete block. Receivers were fixed to the pole and

oriented vertically upwards with the hydrophone extending 10 cm above the top of the

metal pole in order to minimise interference between the mooring structure and

hydrophone reception (Clements et al. 2005). The shallow crest receivers were

therefore about 0.5 m below chart datum. Receivers along the reef base contour were

attached to a simple rope mooring which was anchored to the sea floor at a depth of

approximately 5 m. Receivers were fixed to the rope at least 1 m below a sub-surface

float, which held the receiver vertical in the water column at a depth of about 3 m.

While the receivers were deployed, climactic conditions remained consistent, with

moderate winds (< 15 kn) and swell (< 60 cm), overcast skies and < 1 mm of rain.

16

Two coded transmitters (Vemco, V9-1L, 69 kHz, random delay interval 190-290

s, power output 146 dB re 1 µPa at 1m) were moored at opposite ends of each receiver

line, one adjacent to receiver PB1 (1 m from receiver; transmitter 1) and the other

adjacent to receiver PB6 (transmitter 2) (Fig. 2.1b, c). The transmitters were held 0.5 m

from the substratum, simulating the depth at which most medium to large (20-70 cm

TL) benthic reef fish would be active while foraging or swimming. As a result of the

long random delay interval of the transmitters used in the long-term range testing

experiment, the number of code transmissions produced cannot be calculated with the

required precision over short time periods (hours) in the same manner as a transmitter

with a fixed delay transmission interval. Therefore, the number of detections recorded

by PB1 and PB6 for transmitters 1 and 2, respectively, were used for analysis as the

number of transmissions made by each transmitter during the study period. The

transmitters were left in place for a 7 d period, after which time they were removed

from the study site and the detection data files downloaded from each VR2W receiver.

Immediately after the 7-day data collection period, the transmitters used for the long-

term deployment were assessed to determine if they were representative of typical V9

transmitters. To do this, both transmitters used in the study and an identical third

transmitter (Vemco, V9-1L, 69 kHz, random delay interval 190-290 s, power output

146 dB re 1 µPa at 1m) were moved to a mooring 50 m from a receiver, which was left

in place for a 12 h period. Following this, the receiver was collected and data was

downloaded to compare the average number of detections from each transmitter during

five randomly selected 30 min time periods.

17

Data analysis

Overall detection probabilities and effective detection range

The average number of detections from the transmitters deployed on the array, and a

third transmitter, were compared using a one-way ANOVA. The assumption of

normality was inspected using residual plots, and homogeneity of variances was

checked using Levene’s test for homogeneity of variances. No transformations were

required to meet the assumptions of ANOVA.

For each of the two test transmitters, detections recorded at individual receivers

over the 7-day test period were grouped into 6-h bins and classified as either ‘‘day’’

(0601-1800 hours) or ‘‘night’’ (1801-0600 hours). Individual detection probabilities for

each 6-h period at each receiver were calculated based on the total number of recorded

detections expressed as a percentage of the known number of transmissions (derived

from the number of detections from the receiver adjacent to the transmitter). Missed

transmissions due to signal overlap from occasional visits of tagged taxa to the study

site were factored into the analysis. Individual detection probabilities for each receiver

were then plotted against the distance from the receiver to the transmitter for diurnal

and nocturnal sampling periods. Detections were modeled using linear regressions and

logistic regressions. For the reef base, a linear regression analysis was the best model

for the data (distance to transmitter as independent variable). For the reef crest, the

relationship between number of detections (number of signals per day present vs. absent

across the array) and the distance from the transmitter was best modeled by a logistic

regression.

18

Temporal (diel) variation in detection

Temporal variation in detection probabilities were examined by calculating the average

number of detections for each of the 12-h diurnal and nocturnal sampling periods

(average values per 12-h bin were treated as individual data points for analysis).

Differences in the proportion of signals detected by each receiver in diurnal and

nocturnal sampling periods were then compared using a repeated measures analysis of

variance (RMANOVA).

To evaluate the effect of interference, which may occur on a regular diel basis

(such as reef noise), diel detection densities (hourly detection frequencies) across the

array as a whole were also examined. For each day during which the array was in place,

detections from the two test transmitters were grouped into hourly bins to give a total

number of detections hour-1 by the array. Hourly values were then averaged across the 7

days of the study to give a mean hourly detection frequency in each of the 24 hourly

bins, and these hourly detection frequencies were compared using a Chi-squared

goodness of fit test. To detect any fine-scale cyclical patterns in diel detection

frequency, a Fast Fourier Transformation (FFT) (with Hamming window smoothing)

was also applied to the data. Following Payne et al. (2010) the magnitude of variation

of each hourly bin (the standardized detection frequency or SDF) around the overall

mean daily detection frequency was then calculated as: SDFb = Bb/µ, where B is the

mean detection frequency in each of the hourly bins and l is the overall mean detection

frequency. Therefore, should acoustic interference be high at certain periods of the day,

we would expect low SDF values for the hourly bins during that time period as the

receiver would be detecting fewer than average detections. This provides an indication

of the extent to which transmitter detections may have been under-represented during

particular parts of the diel cycle due to environmental factors.

19

Acoustic performance

Parameters recorded in the metadata file downloaded from each VR2W receiver were

used to provide a quantitative metrics of the overall performance of the array. Metrics

were based around four specific parameters relating to the 8-pulse train emitted by the

coded transmitters used in this study: (1) the total number of pulses recorded each day

by a receiver (P); (2) the number of recorded detections (D); (3) the number of valid

synchs (where a synch is the interval between the first two pulses of the 8-pulse train

that identifies the incoming code as belonging to a transmitter) (S) and; (4) the number

of codes rejected due to invalid checksum periods between the final two pulses of the

train (C). From these parameters the daily code detection efficiency (D!S-1), daily

rejection coefficient (C!S-1) and daily noise quotient (P-S!# of pulses required to make a

valid code) were calculated for each receiver (see Simpfendorfer et al. 2008 for further

description of individual parameters and metrics). It is worth noting that the VR2W can

also count non-synch periods (periods generated by transmission overlap and noise

interpreted by the receiver as pings) as syncs, however, there was very little evidence of

this factor herein. The effect of the receiver’s distance from each of the moored

transmitters on the aforementioned performance metrics was evaluated using Pearson’s

correlation analysis.

2.3 Results

Maximum detection range

The preliminary tests of maximum detection range revealed a rapid decline in detection

probability for a 9 mm transmitter over short distances within the reef environment. At

50 m from the receiver only 62% of transmissions from a fixed delay range-testing

20

transmitter were detected, decreasing to a probability of just 4% at a distance of 150 m.

At a distance of 125 m from the receiver, 22% of transmissions were detected, beyond

this distance, detection values fell to below 5% and therefore, 125 m was taken to be the

maximum workable detection range within the study reef environment. This means that,

in the absence of other competing transmitters, a lone individual tagged with an

acoustic transmitter must be resident, on average, for at least 1090 s

([190 + 290]!0.22-1) to be detected at a distance of 125m.

Overall detection probabilities and detection range

For each transmitter a significant negative relationship existed between both diurnal and

nocturnal detection probabilities and distance from receiver (Fig. 2.2). The slopes and

intercepts for the regression equations for diurnal and nocturnal periods were similar on

both the reef crest (y = e4.91-0.08(x)/(1 + e4.91-0.08(x) and y = e4.75-0.07(x)/(1 + e4.91-0.08(x),

respectively) and on the base (y = 94.56-0.52x and y = 90.92-0.49x, respectively). For

the 9-mm transmitter (random delay interval transmitter) moored on the reef base (next

to the deep receiver line), detection probabilities decreased gradually at increasing

distance from the receiver (Fig. 2.2a). For practical purposes, a cut-off of 50% detection

efficiency was deemed acceptable for biological interpretation (Payne et al. 2010),

meaning that the effective working detection range for this deep transmitter was 90 m.

However, an average 30% of detections were still being recorded at a distance of 125 m

from the transmitter. For the 9-mm transmitter moored on the reef crest (next to the

shallow receiver line), detections dropped off much more steeply, driven for the most

part by the small probability of detection by receivers moored along the reef base (Fig.

2.2b). In this case, the working (50%) detection range was just 60 m (Fig. 2.2b),

although this increased to approximately 90 m when considering only detections by the

21

shallow line of receivers. In contrast to the results for the deep transmitter, virtually no

detections were being recorded at a distance of 125 m from the shallow transmitter,

even by the shallow line of receivers (Fig. 2.2b).

Differences in the number of detections from the transmitter deployed on the reef

base and the one on the reef crest cannot be attributed to differences in transmitter

performance. Post hoc tests revealed no significant difference between the numbers of

transmissions made by either of the transmitters used over the 7-day trial period or a

third transmitter used to compare transmitter performance (F2,12 = 1.27, P > 0.05).

22

Fig. 2.2 a) Relationship between the probability of detection and distance from the

receiver for a transmitter moored on the reef base during diurnal hours (grey line, linear

regression, slope = -0.52, constant = 94.56, P < 0.001, r2 = 0.52) and nocturnal hours

(black line, linear regression, slope = -0.49, constant = 90.92, P < 0.001, r2 = 0.48) and

b) relationship between the number of successful versus unsuccessful detections and

distance from the receiver for a transmitter moored on the reef crest during diurnal

hours (grey line, logistic regression, slope = -0.084, constant = 2.35, P < 0.001,

Nagelkerke r2 = 0.71) and nocturnal hours (black line, logistic regression, slope = -

0.067, constant = 4.08, P < 0.001, Nagelkerke r2 = 0.64). Detection probabilities are

shown for each 6-h period of the 7-day test and are classified as diurnal (0601-1800 h)

(grey circles) or nocturnal (1801-0600 h) (black circles). Nocturnal data points have

been shifted slightly left on the y-axis to eliminated significant overlap with diurnal

data points.

0 20 40 60 80 100 120 1400

20

40

60

80

100 (b) Reef crest transmitter

Distance to transmitter (m)

PB1S

PB2SPB3S

PB4S

PB5SPB7D

PB8D

PB9DPB10D

0 20 40 60 80 100 120 1400

20

40

60

80

100

Day Night

(a) Reef base transmitter

Prob

abilit

y of

det

ectio

n (%

)

PB1S

PB2SPB3S

PB4SPB5SPB6D

PB7D

PB8DPB9D

PB10D

23

Temporal (diel) variation in detection

The comparison of average detection probabilities for 12-h diurnal and nocturnal

periods revealed no significant diel difference in signal detection probability for the

deep receiver line (F1,8 = 0.17, P = 0.69) or the shallow receiver line (F1,8 = 0.02, P =

0.88). On an hour-by-hour basis there were some differences in detection frequencies

over the course of the day (χ222 = 34.62, P = 0.042). However, the overall diel pattern of

detection densities did not reveal any distinct trend in over- or under-representation of

detections during nocturnal or diurnal hours (Fig. 2.3). FFT analysis likewise revealed

no prominent diel cycles of detection in the observed power spectrum (please see

Appendix A for FFT output). Instead, several major peaks were found and those with

the greatest spectral density occurred at 40, 10 and 16.7 hour cycles (see Appendix A).

Standardisation of detection frequencies to remove any artefacts of environment and

varying distance to receiver on detection frequency confirmed that there was little diel

variation in detection density, with the only discernable pattern being an under-

representation of detections in the period around dawn (0500-0600 h) (Fig. 2.3).

Otherwise, both positive and negative variation around the mean daily detection

frequency was observed in both diurnal and nocturnal periods (Fig. 2.3).

24

Fig. 2.3 Diel detection frequency (mean detections per hourly bin over the 7-day test

period ± SE) across the entire array for the two test transmitters. Shading indicates

nocturnal hours (1801-0600 h).

Receiver Performance

The daily code detection efficiency of the receivers used in this study ranged from 0.27

to 0.82 detections synch-1, with an overall average of 0.52 detections synch-1 (± 0.01

SE). This meant that just over half the codes transmitted by the two transmitters were

successfully recorded by the receiver array. The mean rate of code rejection was just

0.022 (± 0.001), suggesting that, on average, only 2% of codes were rejected due to

invalid checksum periods. The value of the noise quotient recorded by each receiver

was almost universally negative in value and averaged -1,067.8 (± 87.5). There was no

relationship between the distance of receivers to transmitters and code detection

12 1860

80

100

120

140

160

180

23 6 0

Hourly bin

Mea

n de

tect

ion

frequ

ency

(det

ectio

ns h

-1 ±

SE)

25

efficiency (r = -0.20, P > 0.05), code rejection rate (r = 0.23, P > 0.05) or the noise

quotient (r = -0.16, P > 0.05).

2.4 Discussion

Our results suggest that the working detection range for 9-mm transmitters (Vemco,

V9-1L, 69 kHz, power output 146 dB re 1 µPa at 1m), the size most suited for the

majority of benthic reef fishes on coral reefs, may be as low as 60 m. While transmitters

with higher power outputs may be detectable at a slightly greater range, this value is a

fraction of the ranges previously reported in the literature for this size of transmitter

within aquatic habitats. For example, a 450-m range was reported for 9-mm transmitters

in the Caloosahatchee River (Simpfendorfer et al. 2008), and a 200-m detection range

was reported for V9-2L transmitters (with a similar power output to those used herein)

in temperate reef habitats of South Australia (Payne et al. 2010). Instead, the overall

detection range found herein is most comparable to the minimum detection range of 60

m reported by Meyer et al. (2010) on Hawaiian reefs. Our results suggest that the

detection performance of acoustic receivers may be significantly impacted by the

unique nature of the reef environment and demonstrates the importance of testing the

range of acoustic arrays across individual habitats and study sites.

In the case of Pioneer Bay, the receiver performance metrics may provide

potential explanations for the reduced detection ranges reported. The low code rejection

coefficients exhibited by receivers indicates that codes were not being rejected because

of invalid checksum values (values that check the integrity of the code transmission

used by the receiver to validate the code and confirm it is a recognisable transmitter).

The reduced detection efficiencies recorded in this study, therefore, were driven by the

receiver unit not receiving the full sequence of pulses emitted by the transmitter. For the

coral reef environment, there are several possible explanations for the reception of

26

incomplete code sequences by the receiver. These include (1) distortion of the acoustic

pulse train (e.g. dampening of amplitude) via interference from environmental noise

(acoustic waves) (both physical and biological sources and periodic or chronic); (2) the

distortion of the code sequence via reflection off topographically complex substrata; (3)

the distortion of the code sequence via absorption by particles in the water; (4) collision

with pulses from other transmitters within the detection range of the receiver; (5)

blockage of the transmission by a tagged individual moving behind an obstacle. In the

case of the current study, the latter two explanations can be eliminated by virtue of the

fact that detection performance was based on stationary transmitters operating in an

environment with minimal transmitters present. This leaves background noise,

suspended sediment and topography as likely explanations for the fact that transmitter

code sequences attenuated over shorter than expected distances in the reef environment.

In terms of background noise, it has been suggested previously that the capacity

of an acoustic receiver to detect a signal emitted by a transmitter is hindered in the

presence of large amounts of background interference, such as the noise generated by

snapping shrimp and other marine taxa (e.g. Voegeli and Pincock 1996; Clements et al.

2005; Simpfendorfer et al. 2008). Intermittent noise recorded as a ping during an actual

transmitter’s transmission can cause the receiver to reject the transmission, resulting in

the receiver ignoring the actual transmitter’s acoustic signal. Continuous noise can raise

the threshold required to detect a transmission from a transmitter resulting in a lower

detection range (with fewer pings likely to be detected). Reefs are notoriously noisy

environments and, undeniably, there is a range of noises on coral reefs, mostly

biological in origin, occurring over an extremely broad acoustic spectrum. Reef noise

has been documented to reach frequencies as high as 200 kHz, in the case of the noise

produced by snapping shrimp (Au and Banks 1998). The evidence from the negative

noise quotient values in the present study suggests that, in the reef environment, the

27

receivers are not hearing intermittent noise, which would contribute to a high noise

quotient value, but are perhaps hearing continuous noise. Continuous background noise

would cause the receivers to adjust their signal detection sensitivity to ignore consistent

background noise, which may result in the occasional signal from the transmitter being

ignored, thus contributing to a lower detection range than has been reported in other

aquatic environments.

A further manner by which ambient noise may reduce the detection capacities of

the receiver is by modifying the acoustic signal of the transmitter itself. The further the

acoustic signal from a transmitter must travel, the more likely it becomes that the signal

will collide with other noise and thus, be modified. In this sense, reef noise may cause

an incomplete pulse train to reach the receiver. Ambient noise may therefore have both

an indirect (interference with the transmitter) and direct (interference with the receiver)

effect on acoustic signal detection.

Surprisingly, the current study did not detect a significant difference between the

diurnal and nocturnal performance of acoustic receivers within the reef habitat,

something which has been reported in other environments where testing of passive

acoustic arrays has been undertaken (Payne et al. 2010). In temperate, shallow, marine

environments and estuaries, the temporal variation in activity of invertebrates such as

snapping shrimp have been suggested as the cause of these patterns in the detection

range of acoustic receivers (Heupel et al. 2004, 2006). While the source of biological

noise on reefs is highly variable, and possibly more intense at night (Bardyshev 2007),

the acoustic characteristics of the noises produced are actually quite similar in diurnal

and nocturnal periods (Leis et al. 2002). Choruses from fish schools (McCauley and

Cato 2000) and invertebrates can be heard in both diurnal and nocturnal time periods

(Radford et al. 2008). Therefore, should noise be capable of having a significant impact

on the signal transmitted from a transmitter, it is likely to be having a similar impact in

28

both nocturnal and diurnal sampling periods. Small, yet significant, declines in the

number of detects were, however, recorded at dawn and dusk. These trends may arise as

a result of an increased instance of reef noise documented to occur during these time

periods on tropical reefs from fish choruses and invertebrates (Fish 1964; Cato 1978;

Radford et al. 2008). However, the absence of a distinct peak in the spectral density of

the FFT analysis herein suggests that these patterns are non-cyclic, and may be random

noise. This is most apparent when our results are compared to the strong spectral peaks

at 24 h, and secondary peaks at 6 and 12 h, described by Payne et al. (2010) using

stationary control transmitters. Although we did not see the same degree of diel

variation in the mean detection frequency of transmitters reported from previous studies

(Payne et al. 2010), our results do suggest that, to at least some extent, background

noise is contributing to lower detection ranges and small detection probabilities.

Within the reef environment at Pioneer Bay, several physical factors are also

likely to have contributed to interference in signal detection by physically blocking the

acoustic signal. High levels of suspended matter that are characteristic of turbid inshore

reefs, such as Orpheus Island, may cause reflection of acoustic signals, interrupting

acoustic pulse trains (Voegeli and Pincock 1996 cited in Simpfendorfer et al. 2008).

Moreover, the natural topographic complexity of reefs mean that a clear line of sight

between receiver and transmitter is likely to be more frequently breached than in a

sandy or muddy-bottomed lagoonal or estuarine habitat. Even in the current study

where receivers were detecting stationary transmitters, high topographic complexity

may have an impact on detection ability. Receiver PB7D, which consistently performed

below the level expected given its distance to the two transmitters, was in close

proximity to significant benthic complexity, which is likely to have effectively and

consistently blocked the acoustic signal. This result, even on a stationary transmitter,

stresses the importance of both optimal receiver placement and assessment of the

29

detection performance of individual receivers to the design of an effective remote

monitoring array.

However, the precise causes of the strong signal attenuation are probably complex

and may have several contributing factors. Intra-environmental variability in receiver

detection capacities, both holistically and in terms of diel variation, as seen in temperate

reefs (e.g. Payne et al. 2010), highlight the need to perform detailed range tests when

utilizing acoustic telemetry to monitor movement biology. Moreover, the unique

performance of acoustic telemetry in a variety of environments emphasizes the dangers

of simply inferring detection ranges from previous studies. It is strongly recommended

that simple range tests, such as those conducted herein, be undertaken to assess

maximum detection ranges in arrays, to help avoid misinterpretation of results.

Knowledge of the study environment and careful selection of individual receiver

placement is imperative to inferring the detection range not only of individual receivers,

but also the area covered by the detection array. Similar to the reduced detection

capacity of receiver PB7D, those receivers moored on the deep line detected a lower

than expected proportion of the acoustic signals emitted from the shallow transmitter.

This is likely to be due to the fact that pulses emitted from the transmitter would need to

pass the reef slope, at which point they may reflect off the reef matrix and attenuate

before reaching the receivers. Therefore the deep line of receivers is likely to be more

useful for the detection of off-reef movements and may not be effective for detecting

within-reef movement of focal organisms. Other aquatic habitats such as rivers,

estuaries and the open ocean are not likely to contain such pronounced drop-offs and

receivers are therefore likely to exhibit a more uniform performance in all directions.

For coral reefs, however, receivers are likely to have a more biased elliptical detection

range, extending further into less complex areas. The use of multiple lines of receivers

when designing arrays for the reef environment is therefore recommended for capturing

30

the movement patterns of animals over different reef zones. In the current study, a

shallow line was found to be most effective for the detection of organisms moving over

the reef crest and flat, with the likely benefits of decreased acoustic shadow-zones out-

weighing the disadvantages of potential exposure during low tide. By virtue of the

complexity to reefs, benthic organisms’ movements through structurally complex areas

may be under-represented in the data. It appears that specific care needs to be taken

during receiver deployment to minimize the number of acoustic shadow zones in areas

of high utilization by focal tagged individuals.

The results of the present study suggest that, for reef environments, maximum

detection ranges and defined diel variability in detection range cannot be assumed.

Moreover, they highlight the importance of receiver placement for passive monitoring

studies on coral reefs. In this study both environmental and acoustic attributes of coral

reefs which are likely to cause a lower detection range of acoustic transmitters were

found to be more or less constant throughout the diel period and thus, it would not be

necessary to correct for detection variability to infer activity levels across the diurnal-

nocturnal cycle. Given the size of reef fishes, 9-mm transmitters are suitable for the

majority of larger species on coral reefs. However, we suggest that studies aiming for

complete coverage of a site inhabited by individuals tagged with 9-mm transmitters (or

any transmitter with a similar power output) will require receivers in close (less than

100 m) proximity. Moreover, gated or curtain arrays may require double lines or some

other form of redundancy in the array in order to confirm the movement of an

individual past a particular point. The farther the acoustic signal must travel over the

reef, reflecting off various substrates and colliding with any number of propagating

acoustic signals, the more likely it is that the pulse will significantly attenuate before it

reaches the receiver and not be detected. A combination of particulate matter, extreme

topographic complexity and high ambient noise levels may therefore act in concert to

31

create a reduced capacity for acoustic signals to propagate in reef habitats, compared to

other aquatic environments. By their very nature, reefs create a challenge for working

with acoustic technology, the result of which appears to be a reduction in the effective

working range of 9 mm transmitters and receivers. Overall, estimates of animal

movement in the coral reef environment as determined by passive acoustic monitoring

must be interpreted with caution. In these systems, the old maxim that the absence of

evidence does not represent evidence of absence is particularly important.

32

Chapter 3: How far do schools of roving herbivores rove? A

case study using Scarus rivulatus Published in Coral Reefs 2012 31: 991-1003

3.1 Introduction

Coral reefs worldwide are showing evidence of declining health as increased instances

of coral bleaching, disease and overharvesting are taking their toll (e.g. Scheffer et al.

2001; Gardner et al. 2003; Bellwood et al. 2004; Wilson et al. 2006, 2008). In response

to the declining health of coral reefs, ecological research is beginning to focus on coral

reef resilience to better understand the extent to which coral reefs can absorb chronic

natural and anthropogenic disturbances without degrading to alternate states, such as

macroalgal dominated reefs (Folke et al. 2004; Hughes et al. 2005; Nyström et al. 2008;

Cheal et al. 2010). Numerous studies have identified key functional groups on coral

reefs, which, through their feeding activities or other interactions with their

environment, contribute significantly to coral reef resilience (e.g. Bellwood et al. 2004;

Ferreira and Gonçalves 2006; Graham et al. 2006; Nyström et al. 2008).

Herbivory, one of the key ecological processes responsible for supporting reef

resilience, and the taxa responsible for this process have been evaluated in detail on the

GBR (e.g. Fox and Bellwood 2007; Hughes et al. 2007; Bonaldo and Bellwood 2010)

and in other tropical reefs worldwide (Hay 1981; Paddack et al. 2006; Burkepile and

Hay 2008, 2010; Alwany et al. 2009). However, two underlying trends tend to emerge

when describing herbivory on coral reefs, and the GBR in particular: 1) functional

processes are often dominated by a small number of species (albeit often different

species between geographic locations), and 2) a great deal of spatial variability exists in

the magnitude of these processes both within and among reefs (e.g. Bruggemann et al.

33

1995; Bellwood et al. 2003; Mumby 2006; Hoey and Bellwood 2008, 2010; Vergés et

al. 2011). This raises the question: at what spatial scale do these key species or critical

functional groups exert their influence, and how may this shape larger scale variation in

ecosystem function? To address this question, we focus on one key group, the

parrotfishes.

Parrotfishes stand out as important members of reef fish assemblages, regardless

of the geographical location (Bruggemann et al. 1994a; Floeter et al. 2004; Fox and

Bellwood 2007; Hoey and Bellwood 2008; Burkepile and Hay 2010). Their large size,

unique jaw morphologies and numerical abundance are all factors which result in these

taxa dominating almost every functional process with which they are involved (Hoey

and Bellwood 2008; Alwany et al. 2009; Bellwood et al. 2012). However, these species

often have highly complex social behaviours, which have been suggested to govern

their movement patterns, and potentially restrict the reef area over which they feed and

thus, the area over which they contribute to the reefs’ ecological processes (van Rooij et

al 1996; Mumby and Wabnitz 2002; Bonaldo et al. 2006).

Several parrotfish taxa have been described as being highly mobile, often moving

across large areas of reef in large schools (e.g. Robertson et al. 1976; Choat and

Bellwood 1985; Clifton 1989; Myers 1989; Lukoschek and McCormick 2000; Fox and

Bellwood 2007). These schools are likely to be socially facilitated by a single, highly

abundant school-forming species, referred to as the nuclear species, and do not operate

from the perspective of the individual (Sazima et al. 2007). They benefit members by

reducing predation risk and allow access to resources, which would otherwise be

unavailable (Robertson et al. 1976; Lukoshek and McCormick 2000). The members of

these schools are often regarded as roving herbivores and the spatial range covered by

these schools is potentially extremely large. Given that the ecosystem impact of

functionally important individuals will inevitably be constrained by their spatial range,

34

an understanding of the spatial range over which schools of herbivorous fish feed is

paramount. Yet, despite the importance of spatial range in evaluating the functional

roles of individuals, the foraging ranges of both parrotfish schools and its component

members, have yet to be evaluated.

Despite the potential for parrotfishes to exhibit large-scale movement patterns,

recent evidence suggests that the home range of parrotfish species may be quite limited

(Mumby and Wabnitz 2002; Bonaldo et al. 2006; Afonso et al. 2008; Welsh and

Bellwood 2012a). For example, Chlorurus microrhinos exhibits restricted home ranges

(< 8,000 m2), and there exists the potential for small-scale reef partitioning between

individual harems. Within an individual’s home range boundaries, the feeding patterns

of C. microrhinos have been found to be heterogeneous, with the majority of their

feeding activity concentrated in only 22% of their home range. Such site-attached

behaviour may contribute greatly to the observed variation in the spatial application of

functional processes by C. microrhinos (Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). Restricted

movement patterns by individuals from key taxa raises the possibility that their

contribution to functional processes may be confined to a small area of reef. Moreover,

these findings call into question the extent to which the term “roving herbivore” can be

applied to some herbivorous reef fish species. Although there is a widespread belief that

schooling species cover large areas of reefs (e.g. Myers 1989; Randall et al. 1997;

Nyström and Folke 2001), what constitutes ‘large’ or ‘extensive’ and how different the

spatial range of schooling species are from their haremic counterparts remains to be

determined. The general assumption, however, is that the ranges of schools will be

several times larger than haremic or territorial species. To address this issue, we

selected one of the more mobile, schooling parrotfish species to examine the extent to

which a schooling roving herbivore roves.

35

The parrotfish species Scarus rivulatus is an ecologically important herbivorous

species, especially on inshore reefs of the GBR (Fox and Bellwood 2007; Hoey and

Bellwood 2008; Cheal et al. 2012). By virtue of their numerical abundance and high

feeding rates, S. rivulatus are the dominant grazers on inshore reefs (Fox and Bellwood

2007). S. rivulatus are commonly observed forming large schools (Randall et al. 1997;

Allen et al. 2003) and are widely regarded as roving herbivores, assumed to cover large

areas of reef as they forage. However, the extent to which these “roving herbivores”

rove is almost entirely unknown, as is the nature of schooling in S. rivulatus.

The aim of the present study, therefore, is to evaluate the movement patterns of

individual S. rivulatus and their foraging schools. More specifically, we aim to: 1)

quantify the foraging range of S. rivulatus and the spatial extent and temporal duration

of foraging range overlap of school members, 2) assess the implications of schooling

behaviour on the application of their functional roles on reefs, and finally, 3) to examine

the extent to which the term “roving herbivore” can be applied to this species.

3.2 Materials and Methods

Study site

This study was conducted between February and November 2011 in Pioneer Bay on the

leeward side of Orpheus Island (18°35’S, 146°20’E), an inner-shelf granitic island in

the central GBR, Australia. Pioneer Bay contains a well-developed fringing reef with an

extensive reef flat, defined reef crest, and a gradual transition to the reef base with

several interspersed coral bommies (please see Fox and Bellwood 2007 for details). The

physical water conditions of Pioneer Bay are relatively stable with little wave action,

consistent moderate turbidity, and marked seasonal temperature fluctuations (Lefèvre

and Bellwood 2010) but no thermocline.

36

Passive acoustic monitoring

The movement patterns of tagged S. rivulatus were monitored within Pioneer Bay using

a network of 25 acoustic VR2W receivers (Vemco, Amirix Pty. Ltd., NS, Canada),

deployed in the study site using a grid configuration (Fig. 3.1; cf. Heupel et al. 2006).

Receivers were moored in two parallel lines, roughly 50 m apart, one along the reef

crest and the second along the reef base. Receivers along the crest and base were spaced

approximately 100 m and 150 m apart, respectively. Receivers were moored following

Welsh et al. (2012a) with two coats of Micron CSC ablative paint prior to deployment

following Heupel et al. (2008). Evaluations of the receiver performance in Pioneer Bay

suggest that receivers have a working detection range (range at which 50% of acoustic

signals are detected) of 60 m on the reef crest. Range testing throughout the study

period found no significant temporal variability in detection range. Data were

downloaded from the receivers approximately every two months, at which time

receivers and mooring equipment was cleared of fouling organisms. Receivers were left

in place for the duration of the study period.

Fish were tagged in late April, when a school of seven individuals were tagged,

and early July of 2011, when two more schools of five and six individuals were tagged.

To capture entire schools of S. rivulatus, the school was followed until individuals

within the school began feeding, then two-monofilament barrier nets (50 x 2 m, 35 mm

square mesh) were deployed around the school with particular care taken to exclude any

fish which were not part of the target school. Once the school had been captured, fish

were transported to the Orpheus Island Research Station and placed in a 3,300 L flow-

through tank for holding prior to surgery. Fish were then anaesthetized in a tricane

methanesulfonate (MS-222) seawater solution (0.13 gL-1) for approximately 60

seconds. Following this, the total length (TL; cm) was recorded and an ultrasonic

37

transmitter (tag; V9-1L, random delay interval 190-290 s, power output 146 dB re 1

µPa at 1m, Vemco) was inserted into the peritoneal cavity (for a detailed description of

the surgical procedure, please see Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). Fish were in captivity

for a maximum of 24 h from the time of capture to the time of release, at their

respective capture sites. Fish were released from a boat by immersion of their holding

containers and were then allowed to swim away.

Fig. 3.1 Map of the placement of acoustic receivers (VR2W) within Pioneer Bay on

Orpheus Island, Great Barrier Reef. Dark grey boxes represent the placement of shallow

water receivers on the sub-tidal reef crest, and light grey boxes represent deep water

receivers moored on the reef base. The circles around each receiver represent the

receiver’s estimated detection range.

400m

10m

0.5m

2.5m

0m

5m

1.1m0.6m

1m

0.8m

0.8m

1.3m

0.8m

0.6m

0.6m

1.1m

N

1

2 3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

38

General observational methodology

The school size and feeding rates of individuals were assessed at two separate sites

within Pioneer Bay. All observations were made on snorkel and observers remained at a

distance of 3 m from the focal individual (unless the individual approached the

observer), and allowed a period of at least 1 min for the fish to acclimate to the presence

of an observer prior to sampling. If at any point during the observational periods

observer effects became evident (e.g. individual continuously moving away from the

observer in a directional manner), the observation was terminated and the data excluded

from analysis. Sampling was restricted to fish within a single reef zone (sub-tidal reef

crest) and which were visually estimated to be between 20 and 30 cm (TL) to

standardize observations. To avoid sampling the same focal individual twice, individual

markings were observed and individuals with similar marking or sizes were not

successively sampled. Observations were also spread over a wide area (covering an area

of reef approximately 8,000 m2, at each site) to minimize the chances of re-

encountering previously observed individuals.

Feeding rates

The feeding rates (measured in bites.min-1) of schooling and solitary individuals were

recorded for both S. rivulatus and the other scarids that are commonly observed to

school with S. rivulatus (including Scarus ghobban, S. schlegeli, S. niger, S.

flavipectoralis and Hipposcarus longiceps). The latter were combined as ‘other scarid

species’. Sampling occurred over four separate diurnal periods: morning (08:00 to

11:00 h), mid-day (11:01 to 14:00 h), afternoon (14:01 to 16:00 h) and late afternoon

(16:01 to 18:00 h). Focal individuals were followed for a 1-minute period, during which

39

time the number of bites each fish took was recorded. For each time period and each

site, a total of 10 schooling and 10 non-schooling individual S. rivulatus (and 10

schooling and 10 non-schooling other scarids) were sampled, resulting in a total of 160

observations.

School size evaluation

The school size dynamics of S. rivulatus were assessed over three discrete time periods:

morning (08:00 to 10:00 h), mid-day (12:00 to 14:00 h) and in late afternoon (15:00 to

17:00 h). During each sampling period, a single focal individual was located by the

observer and followed for 5 min. Every 30 seconds during the observation window the

number and identity of any fish species within any school with which the focal

individual became associated were counted (resulting in a total of 11 counts per

observation). A total of 10 fish per time period, per site were observed, resulting in a

total of 60 observations.

Data analysis

Individual’s site fidelity was assessed in two ways. First, a residency index (RI) was

calculated by dividing the number of days individuals were detected by the number of

possible detection days since the fish was released (which yields the percentage of days

during the monitoring period during which individuals were detected). Second, a more

detailed representation of site fidelity within Pioneer Bay was assessed by dividing the

number of diurnal detections (06:15 to 18:45 h) for each individual at each receiver by

the total number of diurnal detections for that individual. Data for each individual were

also inspected using the VUE software (Vemco) for consistent patterns of area

40

utilization during the monitoring period. The maximum potential foraging range size for

each individual was also calculated using a MATLAB algorithm (Welsh and Bellwood

2012b for MATLAB code), which calculated the planar area of reef incorporated in the

detection range of all receivers (set at 60 m following range testing and Welsh et al.

2012) with ≥ 5% of all individual’s detections. For the purposes of maximum potential

home range estimations, the detection range of the receivers on the reef slope was set at

60 m in all directions as the movements of S. rivulatus are known to occur primarily on

shallow reef areas and seldom extend past the reef base (Randall et al. 1997; Fox and

Bellwood 2007; Hoey and Bellwood 2008). Any receiver with less than 5% of

detections was excluded from the analysis, as these detections were more likely to occur

as a result of a few acoustic signals travelling further than expected across the reef or,

from unusual wanderings of individuals, which are not considered as part of their

habitual home range (Brown 1975).

The spatial patterns of fishes, as evaluated in telemetry studies, are most often

expressed as home range areas, calculated using kernel utilization distributions (KUDs)

(e.g. Afonso et al. 2009; Hutchinson and Rhodes 2010; Fox and Bellwood 2011). These

home range areas depict a high-resolution area of the marine environment occupied by

a focal individual. KUD analyses require high-resolution spatial data and often between

50 and 200 location fixes for analytical packages to produce accurate representations of

an individual’s home range (Kernohan et al. 2001). For ecological purposes, if the fixes

included in the KUD analysis are restricted to those which occurred during the temporal

periods when a focal individual is foraging (e.g. Welsh and Bellwood 2012a), then

these KUDs are likely to represent the foraging range of an individual’s spatial range

(excluding non-feeding movements such as nocturnal migration). Due to the low-

resolution nature of the passive data used to monitor S. rivulatus herein, and the

complexities of utilizing telemetry on reefs (Welsh et al. 2012), we have applied a much

41

more coarse estimate of spatial range. We calculated the maximum potential diurnal

foraging range by excluding nocturnal data, when fish were resting and not feeding, and

calculating the area of reef enclosed by the detection range of the acoustic receivers in

which tagged S. rivulatus were detected during the day (when they are feeding). Areas

of reef with overlapping detection ranges of two or more adjacent receivers are

excluded from the area calculation if all overlapping receivers did not detect at least 5%

of the signals from a transmitter (please see Fig. 3.2b, d, f for a representation of

maximum detection areas of each school). Therefore, this method allows for a coarse

estimation of the entire area of reef potentially occupied by an individual while foraging

(maximum foraging range) and will almost certainly be larger than the actual foraging

range.

A two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the average

school size of S. rivulatus during each individual’s 5 min observation time at different

periods throughout the day and to test for a site effect. Time of day and site were fixed

factors and the average number of individuals with which the focal individual became

associated with was the dependent variable. Normality and homogeneity of variances

were assessed using residual analyses. Data were log10 transformed to improve

homogeneity of variances. School size variability was also assessed using a coefficient

of variation (CV; σ/mean), calculated for each individual during the 5 min sampling

period. The frequency distribution of the average 5 min school size with which an

individual was associated with was also compared to a Poisson distribution to test

whether the average school size differed from a random expectation (Quinn and

Keough 2002). As Poisson distributions require discrete data, average school sizes were

rounded up to the nearest integer.

The feeding rate data of the ‘other scarids’ were compared using a one-way

ANOVA. Feeding rates at different times of the day (morning, early afternoon, late

42

afternoon and evening), in schooling versus solitary individuals, and at two different

sites for S. rivulatus and the pooled other scarids, were compared using two separate

three-way ANOVAs with time of the day, schooling status and site as fixed factors and

feeding rates as the response variable. Normality and homogeneity of variances were

examined using residual analyses. Bite rate data for S. rivulatus were rank transformed

and bite rate data for other scarids were log10 transformed to improve homogeneity of

variances prior to analysis.

3.3 Results

Movement patterns

A total of 18 S. rivulatus were captured from three separate schools with no tagging-

induced mortality of any individual. School 1 consisted of seven individuals while

schools 2 and 3 consisted of six and five individuals, respectively. The size range of

captured and tagged individuals varied from 18.0 to 32.0 cm (TL) with an average size

of 23.6 cm (Table 3.1). Tagged individuals were detected for the majority of the

monitoring period with an average RI of 83%. However, most individuals exhibited

extreme site fidelity with only seven individuals having an RI of less than 95% (Table

3.1).

All individuals had strong site fidelity to a single area of reef within Pioneer Bay,

with the majority of detections occurring within the detection range of one or two

receivers. The average maximum potential diurnal foraging range size of all tagged

individuals was 24,440 m2 and individual ranges were found to be between 4,290 and

43,030 m2 (Table 3.1). This equates to approximately 250 m of reef front. In

comparison, the maximum potential foraging range occupied by the three schools was

only between 19,484 and 23,819 m2, smaller than the range of most individuals which

43

made up each school and covers an average of 219 m of reef front. The majority of

detections occurred on the reef flat and crest with those receivers detecting individuals

more often than receivers on the reef base (Fig. 3.2a, c, e). No shifts in area occupancy

were detected from the site of capture to the habitual area of reef occupied by tagged

individuals with all individuals captured from each school remaining within the same

area of the reef for the duration of the study (Fig. 3.2a, c, e). Individuals captured from

within the same school did, however, exhibit slightly different detection patterns, albeit

within the same area of reef. Individuals captured from schools 1 and 2 had a great deal

of area utilization overlap, however, areas of primary detection remained distinct

between the two schools (Fig. 3.2).

44

Fig. 3.2 Plot representing the relative proportion of total diurnal detections for each

individual Scarus rivulatus at each receiver for individuals in a) school 1, c) school 2

and e) school 3. The diameter of each circle is in proportion to the number of detections

at a receiver. Circles provided in the legend box are examples only. Receiver numbers

in black represent reef crest receivers and those in bold represent receivers on the reef

base. The maximum area of reef occupied by b) school 1, d) school 2 and f) school 3

within Pioneer Bay are represented by grey areas. The solid black line represents the

reef crest and the site of capture and release of the school is represented by the black

dot. Only receivers with > 5% of individuals or schools detections are shown.

SR31

SR32

SR33

SR34

SR35

Indi

vidu

al (S

. riv

ulat

us ID

#)

SR11

SR12

SR13

SR14

SR15

SR16

23 6 22 7 21 208 9 195

SR17

Receiver number

SR21

SR22

SR23

SR24

SR25

SR26

400mN

a) b)

23 6 22 7 21 208 9 195

19 10 18 11 17 1612 13 15

School 3

School 2

School 1

release site

release site

release site

100% Detections

75% Detections

50% Detections

25% Detections

c) d)

e) f)

0 m

0 m

0 m

45

Table 3.1 Summary of detection data and characteristics from 18 tagged Scarus rivulatus from three separate schools captured in Pioneer Bay, Orpheus Island, Australia.

Fish ID Release date Number of diurnal detections

Date last detected

Total length (cm) School

Phase (Initial; IP or Terminal; TP)

Residency index*

Max possible home range (x103 m2)

SR11 22/4/11 34,644 17/11/11 25.5 1 IP 100 43.03 SR12 22/4/11 50,558 17/11/11 24.0 1 IP 100 40.64 SR13 22/4/11 26,095 17/11/11 25.1 1 IP 99.1 27.54 SR14 22/4/11 11,569 14/6/11 25.5 1 IP 26.1 26.61 SR15 22/4/11 20,890 14/8/11 20 1 IP 55.0 33.51 SR16 22/4/11 20,796 17/11/11 18 1 IP 98.6 22.07 SR17 22/4/11 37,191 17/11/11 23 1 IP 93.8 22.07 SR21 3/7/11 11,053 29/9/11 31.5 2 TP 67.7 21.74 SR22 3/7/11 1,313 24/7/11 27.0 2 IP 11.5 25.59 SR23 3/7/11 14,089 17/11/11 32.0 2 TP 99.3 21.74 SR24 3/7/11 14,154 17/11/11 23.0 2 IP 100 21.74 SR25 3/7/11 12,802 17/11/11 21.1 2 IP 100 21.74 SR26 3/7/11 8,603 17/11/11 20.0 2 IP 100 25.59 SR31 4/7/11 13,628 17/11/11 26.0 3 TP 100 4.29 SR32 4/7/11 10,953 17/11/11 18.0 3 IP 100 22.64 SR33 4/7/11 8,477 18/10/11 21.1 3 IP 78.1 20.04 SR34 4/7/11 7,818 17/11/11 22.4 3 IP 100 9.34 SR35 4/7/11 14,259 29/9/11 21.3 3 IP 64.2 30.05

* Residency index calculated by dividing the number of days individuals were detected in Pioneer Bay by the number of possible detection days since the fish was released.

46

School size evaluation

Schooling behaviour was extremely common for S. rivulatus. On average, individuals

were in schools for 64.8 ± 3.5% (mean ± SE, n = 60) of observations. On average, S.

rivulatus schools consisted of 5.7 ± 0.8 individuals, however, the average school size

frequency histogram was heavily skewed to the left, with the average being a great deal

larger than the mode (2.64) and median (3.54) school size (Fig. 3.3). However, average

school sizes with which an individual became associated differed significantly from

what would be expected following a Poisson distribution (χ 22 = 95.84, P < 0.001) and

thus exhibited a non-random distribution. The size of the school with which an

individual was associated was extremely variable with an average CV for each

individual over the 5 min observation period of 0.69 ± 0.04. Moreover, the frequently

observed fracturing of schools tends to suggest that little fidelity exists from the

perspective of an individual to a particular school. There was no significant difference

detected in the average school sizes of individuals throughout the day (F2,54 = 0.30, P =

0.74), nor was there any site effect (F1,54 = 3.89, P = 0.53), or interaction between site

and time period (F2,54 = 0.63, P > 0.56). Data were therefore pooled across the two sites

for graphical representation (Fig. 3.3).

47

Fig. 3.3 Frequency histogram of the average school size that individual Scarus rivulatus

were associated with over a 5-min observational period (n = 60). The dashed line

approximates the school size frequencies that are expected following a Poisson

distribution.

Feeding rates

There was no significant difference in the feeding rates of S. ghobban, S. schlegeli, S.

niger, S. flavipectoralis and Hipposcarus longiceps (F4,155 = 2.13, P = 0.08 ) and

therefore feeding data for these species were pooled as ‘other scarids’ for all analyses.

Feeding rates for both S. rivulatus and ‘other scarids’ differed significantly

throughout the day (F3,143 = 18.8, P < 0.001 and F3,145 = 9.8, P < 0.001, respectively),

with highest feeding rates detected between 14:01 and 16:00 h in both cases (Table 3.2;

Fig. 3.4). No site effects were detected for S. rivulatus or the other scarids and thus,

data were pooled across sites for presentation (Table 3.2). In both S. rivulatus and other

scarids, feeding rates were consistently and significantly higher in schooling versus

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 >19

Freq

uenc

y

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

School size (x per 5 min-1)

48

solitary individuals across all temporal sampling periods (F1,143 = 135.7, P < 0.001 and

F3,145 = 283.2, P < 0.001 respectively). Mean feeding rates for S. rivulatus in the peak

feeding time period were 36.3 ± 2.8 bites.min-1 when schooling, over double the

feeding rate for solitary individuals of 15.7 ± 3.7 bites.min-1. A similar, yet more

pronounced, trend existed for the combined other scarids, with peak feeding rates of

schooling individuals being three times higher than solitary individuals (27.4 ± 2.2

bites.min-1 and 4.5 ± 1.2 bites.min-1, respectively).

Fig. 3.4 Feeding rate (bites min-1; mean ± SE) of a) Scarus rivulatus and b) other

scarids (which include S. ghobban, S. Schlegeli, S. niger, S. flavipectoralis and

Hipposcarus longiceps) recorded for both schooling and solitary individuals at four

discrete periods of the day at Pioneer Bay, Orpheus Island, Great Barrier Reef.

0

10

20

30

40

50 SchoolingSolitary

Feed

ing

rate

(Bite

s.m

in-1)

0

10

20

30

40

50

08:00-

11:00

11:01-

14:00

14:01-

16:00

16:01-

18:00

Diurnal period (h)

a)

b)

S. rivulatus

Other parrotfishes

49

Table 3.2 Three-way ANOVA comparing the a) rank-transformed feeding rates for Scarus rivulatus, b) pooled log-transformed feeding rates for Scarus ghobban, Scarus schegeli, Scarus niger, Scarus flavipectoralis and Hipposcarus longiceps

Source of variation SS df MS F P

a)

Site (S) 2,295 1 2295 2.37 0.13

Schooling status (SC) 121,398 1 121,398 135.70 < 0.001

Time of day (T) 54,688 3 18,229 18.826 < 0.001

Interaction (S.SC) 387 1 387 0.40 0.53

Interaction (S.T) 3,004 3 1,001 1.03 0.38

Interaction (SC.T) 158 3 53 0.05 0.98

Interaction (S.SC.T) 1508 3 503 0.52 0.67

Error 138,472 143 968

b)

Site (S) 0.24 1 0.24 2.31 0.13

Schooling status (SC) 29.87 1 29.87 283.20 < 0.001

Time of day (T) 3.10 3 1.03 9.8 < 0.001

Interaction (S.SC) 0.06 1 0.06 0.54 0.47

Interaction (S.T) 0.45 3 0.15 1.42 0.24

Interaction (SC.T) 0.25 3 0.08 0.80 0.50

Interaction (S.SC.T) 0.36 3 0.12 1.15 0.33

Error 15.29 145 0.11

50

3.4 Discussion

Our results represent the first detailed long-term quantification of the spatial range of

school-forming individuals of a roving herbivore. The spatial range of S. rivulatus was

found to be limited, with the majority of detections for all individuals within a school

occurring within a small area, measuring on average just 24,440 m2. This suggests that

the spatial range of some nominally ‘roving’ herbivores, and the schools they form,

may actually be quite limited. The ranges are of a similar size or just 2-3 times larger

than those previously described for solitary or haremic herbivore species. Despite

general assumptions (e.g. Myers 1989), schooling did not appear to have a marked

effect of the home ranges in S. rivulatus. By combining acoustic monitoring with field

observations we found that foraging schools of S. rivulatus appear to be highly unstable

with little school fidelity. Though individuals spent most of their time within schools,

the frequent fractioning and coalescing of schools resulted in solitary individuals also

being common. The variability in school composition was supported over longer

timeframes by differing detection patterns for individuals, which were initially captured

from the same school. The overlap between the spatial ranges of individuals captured

from two distinct schools indicates that there is little exclusion of conspecifics from

within an individuals’ foraging range. The limited movement, despite schooling

behaviour, might have significant implications for the spatial application of ecosystem

functions by both the nuclear species (in this case S. rivulatus) and those species

associating with their foraging schools. Basically, this schooling, roving herbivore does

not rove very far.

51

Spatial biology of S. rivulatus

Despite the nature of the analysis used herein, which almost guarantees overestimation

of an individual’s foraging range, the maximum foraging range of S. rivulatus appears

to be limited to a relatively small area of the reef crest and outer flat covering on

average, just 24,440 m2. The whole school occupied and area of, on average, less than

21,856 m2, equivalent to approximately a 219 m stretch of reef. These areas were

occupied for several months. The overestimation of the foraging range of S. rivulatus is

a potential contributing factor explaining why areas calculated for S. rivulatus are

several times larger than the average foraging range size reported for C. microrhinos in

Pioneer Bay of 7,930 m2 (Welsh and Bellwood 2012a), and other home range estimates

of site-attached parrotfish species evaluated using acoustic telemetry (e.g. Afonso et al.

2008). Although, the schooling rather than haremic behavior of S. rivulatus may also be

an important factor. It remains quite likely that the actual home range and foraging

range of S. rivulatus are much smaller than the maximum foraging ranges reported

herein. Nevertheless, the actual home range of S. rivulatus is likely to be larger than its

non-schooling parrotfish counterparts. Despite the methodological differences, the

present study demonstrates a high degree of site fidelity in schooling individuals,

similar to that found for other more solitary parrotfish species (van Rooij et al. 1996;

Mumby and Wabnitz 2002; Afonso et al. 2008; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a).

Limited spatial ranges and site-attached behaviour are increasingly being reported

all over the world for herbivorous reef fish species. For example, Naso lituratus and N.

unicornis in Guam, with home ranges estimated at 68,400 m2 and 32,100 m2,

respectively (Marshell et al. 2011). These spatial ranges are broadly similar to the

maximum foraging range described in the present study for S. rivulatus. Larger-scale

crepuscular movements have been observed in N. unicornis, although these probably

52

represent migrations between nocturnal resting/refuge sites and foraging grounds

(Meyer and Holland 2005; Marshell et al. 2011). No such patterns were observed

herein, suggesting that nocturnal resting sites are near to, or within, individuals’ diurnal

spatial range, as in the parrotfish C. microrhinos in Pioneer Bay (Welsh and Bellwood

2012a). This further emphasises the site-attached nature of S. rivulatus, with individuals

consistently remaining within a restricted area of reef throughout the day and night.

In some parrotfishes, for example, Sparisoma viride (van Rooij et al. 1996) and S.

aurofrenatum (Muñoz and Motta 2000), limited ranges have been proposed to arise

from the partitioning of the reefs between harems. In this case, a dominant male will

usually exclude other individuals from within their territory, and maintain almost

exclusive utilisation of resources for the harem (Bruggemann et al. 1994b). In the

present study, two males from school 2 (SR21 and SR23) were tagged from within the

same school and appear to utilise the same area of reef, exhibiting similar detection

patterns. This, in conjunction with the frequent observation of several males often

foraging near each other (pers obs), suggests that little, if any, behavioural exclusion of

spatial ranges exists in S. rivulatus. Therefore, despite schooling behaviour, the

foraging range of S. rivulatus is not dramatically larger than other territorial parrotfish

species or other herbivores, suggesting that schooling appears not to be a means of

increasing the area of reef occupied by an individual. What then may be the cause of the

limited home range sizes of S. rivulatus?

Rather than territoriality, the small spatial range of S. rivulatus might occur as a

result of favourable environmental features within the home range. The activity patterns

of the parrotfish species C. microrhinos were centred on areas of complexity, which

potentially reduce an individual’s risk of predation due to a familiarity with the

structure of reef within areas of high utilisation (Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). S.

rivulatus are much smaller than C. microrhinos and are likely to be vulnerable to a

53

greater number of predators, making complexity and predator avoidance potentially

more important. Therefore, the area occupied by S. rivulatus may represent an area of

familiarity, with adequate cover and known areas of escape from predators.

The nature and benefits of schooling

Small spatial ranges of individual S. rivulatus are mirrored by the entire school from

which they were captured and might be responsible, in part, for the relatively small

average school sizes of only 5.7 ± 0.83 individuals. As schools may arise from chance

encounters (Robertson et al. 1976; Lukoschek and McCormick 2000), at any one time

the number of individuals available to join a school is finite. School formation is

dependent on social interactions, the frequency of which is in turn, dependent on the

number of overlapping home ranges. As such, we would expect to see a heavily skewed

school size-frequency distribution towards smaller schools, as observed for S. rivulatus.

However, the average number of individuals with which a focal individual became

associated with over a 5-min observation period was significantly different from that of

a Poisson distribution, and was thus not effectively random. This deviation from

random occurred due to the abundance of large schooling associations suggesting that

fish actively choose to be associated with schools. Therefore, within the constraints of a

restricted home rage, schooling appears to be an ecologically favourable strategy for S.

rivulatus.

When the abundances of competitive species are high enough, territoriality, or

haremic behaviour, is suggested to no longer be favourable and schooling behaviour

may become more favourable (Bonaldo et al. 2006). To maximise their fitness, territory

holders need to trade off territory defence with mating and foraging activities (Itzkowitz

1977). It may be this trade-off, which favours the schooling behaviour, with limited

54

school fidelity, in S. rivulatus. As they are the most highly abundant Scarus species in

Pioneer Bay (Fox and Bellwood 2007; Hoey and Bellwood 2008), schooling would be

favoured over territoriality as, in this social environment, the costs of territory defense

may outweigh the benefits (Bonaldo et al. 2006) so individuals likely maximize fitness

by devoting more time to feeding and reproducing in another (non-territorial) fashion

(see Clifton 1989; Kuwamura et al. 2009). An interesting feature of the schools,

however, is that all individuals initially captured from school 1 tend to have centres of

activity, distinct from the adjacent school 2. Area partitioning of this type is similar to

that expected for territorial species as seen in C. microrhinos (Welsh and Bellwood

2012a) and S. viride (van Rooij et al. 1996). This suggests that there is little blending of

the foraging range of individuals in adjacent areas, even without any territorial

aggression, highlighting the site-attached nature of S. rivulatus. Therefore, while

schools are socially facilitated and individuals can potentially join any school operating

within their spatial range, they will not expand their home range to match the home

range of all co-schooling individuals within a school with which they are associated.

This is also supported by the average area occupied by the school being less than that of

the individuals. This occurs as the range of the school (the area where the average

percent detections of all individuals within a school are greater than 5%) is only an area

of home range overlap; and all individuals within the school do not have identical

ranges (the cumulative area of individual’s > 5% detections would be somewhat larger,

but would not be the area used by the entire school). Foraging ranges appear to be fixed

with schooling being a desirable, but facultative association.

From the perspective of an individual fish, foraging schools have been suggested

to provide several advantages, such as reduced individual predation risk, access to

resources which would otherwise be inaccessible (e.g. damselfish territories) and

increased efficiency at locating optimal foraging areas (e.g. Alevizon 1976; Robertson

55

et al. 1976). These advantages are likely to contribute to the nearly two times higher

peak feeding rates of 36.3 bites.min-1 observed for S. rivulatus in foraging schools (cf.

15.7 bites.min-1 for solitary individuals) and the threefold increase in feeding rates

recorded in other scarid species associated with S. rivulatus schools (27.4 bites.min-1;

cf. 4.5 bites.min-1 for solitary individuals).

The benefits of schooling behaviour may not be restricted to the individual, but

may also have significant implications for the ecological processes occurring on the

reef. Schooling associations may benefit ecosystem resilience as the application of

important ecosystem processes, such as herbivory, are locally enhanced. In the

Caribbean, functionally important species, such as Scarus croicensis (Robertson et al.

1976), Acanthurus coeruleus (Foster 1985) and Scarus iserti (Clifton 1989) have been

shown to increase their individual feeding rates while in the safety of a foraging school.

In this way, nuclear, school forming herbivorous species may have an added role on

reefs, not only acting as important herbivores, but also by facilitating higher feeding

rates in other fishes, thus increasing the functional role of conspecifics and

heterospecifics which associate with the school.

Functional implications

The nature of S. rivulatus schooling is highly unstable, as indicated by a high CV of

0.69. This suggests that any encounter between individuals is a potential schooling

event and individuals with overlapping spatial ranges are quite likely to school together

within areas of home range overlap but they equally readily disassociate as the school

moves. The enhancement of feeding rates, and thus ecosystem function, of S. rivulatus

and those species which associate with their schools may be common in areas of reef

where fish population densities are high. While this might act to support resilience in

56

healthy reef systems, it could also increase the vulnerability of sustained rates of

herbivory to the loss of individual fish. As the local population of a site attached

schooling species becomes depleted, the rate of foraging might exhibit a non-linear

decline rather than a linear one (as would be expected following a steady reduction of

ecosystem function, with no effect of schooling, where the rates of individual functional

impacts are not modified by encounters with other individuals) (Fig. 3.5). Therefore,

the high foraging rates and heightened ecosystem function associated with schooling

behaviour can only be maintained if reefs have sufficient home range overlap of

functionally important schooling conspecifics, and a high chance of encounters between

schooling individuals (Fig. 3.5). In this respect, the reduction of local schooling fish

populations may be analogous to Allee effects on reproduction. Declining populations

would reduce the rate of encounters between individuals until they reach a critical

threshold, and schools become so uncommon that feeding is no longer socially

facilitated by the school. Indeed, this phenomenon may already be occurring on reefs

with low densities of schooling species. As such, schooling (the proportion of

individuals within schools) may be an indication of declining ecosystem functions.

57

Fig. 3.5 Conceptual model of the degradation in the feeding rate of schooling

individuals as individuals’ density declines and areas of home range overlap decrease

relative to non-schooling individuals, based on a doubling of feeding rates in schools.

Home range overlap was created using the random allocation of five home ranges to a

fixed area.

As individual S. rivulatus feed throughout the day, it is likely that the small

diurnal area of reef occupied by individuals also represents their foraging range.

However, the limited foraging ranges of S. rivulatus might have significant implications

for the spatial scales over which they confer their functional roles. Despite feeding

likely occurring over individuals’ entire spatial ranges, Welsh and Bellwood (2012a)

found that within the diurnal home range of C. microrhinos, foraging was probably

restricted to a small, core area of their home range. The small area occupied by S.

Density (ind.m-2)

Relat

ive g

razin

g int

ensit

y

HighLow

Schooling individualSolitary individual

Home range overlap

HighLow

58

rivulatus suggests each fish’s contribution to herbivory, and thus ecosystem impact, on

the reef might likewise occur over a limited spatial range, and may be focused within an

even smaller core area of activity. In order to maintain localised functional processes at

sufficient rates to maintain reef resilience (Bellwood et al. 2004; Hughes et al 2005;

Graham et al. 2006; Ledlie et al. 2007), reefs require adequate home range overlap of

functionally important individuals (i.e. spatial redundancy). Traditionally, schools of

herbivores are assumed to provide spatial resilience, in that schools of fishes have been

assumed to move across large areas of reef and thus, act as mobile links. Mobile links

interconnect ecosystem processes over large spatial scales, and support resilience by

connecting degraded systems, where the vital processes for ecosystem recovery have

been compromised, to areas where the ecological processes are still operating (Nyström

and Folk 2001). However, our results suggest that the role of roving herbivore schools

as mobile links supporting functional processes may be weaker than previously

assumed.

Overall, our study demonstrates a clear, long-term pattern of site fidelity of not

only individuals, but also schools of a coral reef fish. While this study focused on

individuals from a single bay, recent evidence tends to suggest that these results may be

more broadly applicable. Nash et al (2012), found limited foraging range variability in

parrotfishes in response to variation in habitat condition. The nature of S. rivulatus

schooling is highly unstable, with little short-term school fidelity, but high long-term

fidelity to a shared area. Schooling behaviour appears to be the preferred social

condition, and has significant implications for the rate at which individuals contribute to

ecosystem functions to the reef. However, the limited spatial ranges described herein

suggest that these are only applied over a small area by an individual or even a school.

Schooling plays a significant role in facilitating elevated rates of key processes but

schooling behaviour is vulnerable to declines in local fish densities.

59

Chapter 4: The ontogeny of home ranges: evidence from

coral reef fishes Published in Proceedings of the Royal Society B 2013 76: 20132066

4.1 Introduction

Animal movement patterns are dynamic in space and time, and are a fundamental

component of a species’ ecology (Börger et al. 2008; Owen-Smith et al. 2010). With the

exception of migrations, most movements by animals are restricted to a home range.

However, this home range is rarely consistent over time and is likely to change as a

result of changing demands and abilities as the animal grows and matures (Johnson et

al. 2001; Jetz et al. 2004; Börger et al; 2008; Cagnacci et al. 2010). Throughout its life,

the size of any animal’s home range is driven by three key demands. Animals must

shape their home ranges to avoid predation, find adequate food and then, reproduce. As

animals grow, their body size changes their vulnerability to predators. In high-diversity

systems with a wide array of predators, smaller animals are more vulnerable and, as

such, must take steps to minimise predation risk. This can be accomplished by reducing

movement or avoiding risky locations, thus restricting an animal’s home range (Huey

1991; Broomhall et al. 2003; Imansyah et al. 2008). With growth, the range of potential

predators reduces, as does overall predation risk, which permits home range expansion

(Gagliano et al. 2007; Holmes et al. 2010). Similarly, as animals grow, the nutritional

demands of a larger body must be met, often demanding a greater area to procure

necessary food (White and Ralls 1993; Haskell et al. 2002; Carbone et al. 2011).

Finally, following maturation, an animal will need to find a mate and reproduce. Home

ranges, therefore, must be large enough to ensure adequate encounters with potential

mates (Trent and Rongstad 1974; Brown and Brooks 1993). While these drivers place

60

seemingly simple demands on an animal’s home range, the responses of animals are

complicated by concurrent ontogenetic shifts in physiological and social factors which

can also shape changes in home range size and patterns of utilisation. As such,

determining the relative importance of specific drivers in shaping changes in home

range sizes can be difficult.

Disentangling the various drivers and responses can be challenging. For example,

flight and associated high levels of parental care essentially remove the effects of

growth on the home ranges of fledged birds (Harrison and Roberts 2000). Similarly, in

mammals the effects of parental care and highly complex social systems often lead to

highly intertwined effects of home range drivers (Holekamp and Sherman 1989).

However, in lower vertebrates these drivers may be more clearly separated. For

example, varanid lizards (e.g. Komodo dragons, Varanus komodoensis) provide useful

models, as they undergo considerable growth following hatching and their home ranges

are less impacted by parental care than mammals or birds (Sumner 2006; Imansyeah et

al. 2008). Coral reef fish are another useful model to study the ontogeny of home

ranges. Following a brief period in the plankton, post-settlement fish grow by several

orders of magnitude on the reef, experiencing temporally separated events including

diet shifts, maturation and sex change. This temporal separation enables us to explore

the relative impact of predation, diet and reproduction on lower vertebrates using reef

fishes as a model.

In addition to understanding the factors affecting the ontogeny of home ranges, it

is crucial to understand the ecological consequences. Any shift in an animal’s home

range can profoundly impact its interactions with its environment (Owen-Smith et al.

2010; Bonaldo et al. 2012), as such, it is important to understand how these factors

change over the course of its life (Persson et al. 1998; Marshall et al. 2012). Coral reefs

in particular are highly reliant upon several functional groups of herbivorous reef fishes

61

to maintain their resistance and resilience to disturbances (Bellwood et al. 2004).

Parrotfishes (scarine Labridae), with their unique jaw morphology and numerical

abundance, dominate several of these functional groups (Bellwood et al. 2012). Recent

research, however, suggests that the ecosystem functions of parrotfishes depend on their

home ranging behaviour (Nash et al. 2012; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). While it is

well documented that the role of herbivores changes with ontogeny (Chen 2002;

Bonaldo and Bellwood 2008; Lokrantz et al. 2008), it is not yet known a) how the home

range of an individual scales with body size, and b) which factors are most important in

determining changes in home range size through time.

This study examines the ontogeny of home ranges of three parrotfish species from

early post-settlement (approx. 10 mm, 0.01 g) to adulthood (over 300 mm and 1 kg).

This represents a change in mass of over five orders of magnitude, and incorporates a

dietary switch, from carnivory to detritivory, maturation and protogynous sex change.

During this time individuals are also subjected to distinct changes in predation risk,

social and reproductive demands (Bellwood 1988; Gagliano et al. 2007), and nutritional

needs (Bellwood 1988) which may also drive home range shifts. As such, these fishes

may provide novel insights into the relative importance of predation, nutrition and

reproduction, alongside other social factors, in shaping ontogenetic scaling of home

ranges in lower vertebrates.

4.2 Materials and Methods

Data collection

Field observations were conducted on Lizard Island, on the mid-shelf of the Great

Barrier Reef (14o41’5”S, 145o26’55”E). The primary study site was North Reef, an

obliquely exposed fringing reef extending from a distinctive crest (1 m below chart

62

datum) down to the reef base at 8-9 m. Home ranges were estimated for three species of

parrotfish (scarine Labridae; Cowman and Bellwood 2011), Scarus frenatus, S. niger

and Chlorurus sordidus. Home ranges were estimated from direct observations, using

three slightly differing methods, due to the wide range of size classes observed. Firstly,

for juvenile parrotfish (20-150 mm total length; TL) a scale map was used. The reef

front (approx. 80 m across) was mapped by recording prominent features, which were

used as reference points. The distances between reference points were measured, and by

triangulation, a two-dimensional scale map of the whole area created. Areas were

estimated based on a planar two-dimensional plot (where necessary the plot conformed

to any larger 3D surface complexity; landmark features would thus follow the benthos,

as a small fish would). The locations of fishes were subsequently recorded in relation to

the reference points. Where ranges were very small, additional reference points were

used to provide more detailed local maps.

Once a fish was located on the reef, it was identified to species, its size estimated

and any distinguishing features noted to allow individual identification (colour

markings and parasite scars were particularly useful). Each focal fish was followed for

a series of 30-min observation periods, marking its position every 15 s on a localised

scale map. These data points were then transferred onto overlays of the main scale map

and the area of the home range measured (as a convex polygon) using a digital graphic

pad. Individuals were followed for a minimum of four 30-min periods or until the

cumulative home range area (based on minimum convex polygons; MCP) reached an

asymptote (i.e. area occupied did not increase by more than 2%). This usually required

6-9 periods. No more than 2 observations were made per day, separated by at least three

hours. Ranges were estimated over a period of no more than 10 days, as they rapidly

expanded with fish growth. The cumulative area occupied over the entire observational

period was taken as the estimated home range. Due to low population densities of C.

63

sordidus, data on this species were supplemented by a few (6) individuals from the

Lizard Island lagoon using identical methods.

Extremely small or recently settled fishes (< 25 mm) were observed once, for 30-

60 min. The shorter observation times for these smaller size classes were necessary

because of difficulties in relocating and re-identifying individuals. Once located, small

individuals were observed constantly until they occupied no new areas within a 5-min

period. Maps of the substratum and key features were made immediately after

observations ceased and triangulated as described above. The observations on small

individuals were predominantly in the austral summer, November-February (coinciding

with peak recruitment). To allow for the short observation periods, the final estimated

home ranges of the smaller fish were scaled up based on a calibration equation derived

from cumulative area-observation time relationships obtained for the larger specimens

described above (Appendix B1).

Finally, for larger fishes (> 150 mm; > 143 g), an aerial photograph of the study

site was used to construct a scale map of the area. Once major features were identified

(e.g. gutters and outcrops) additional underwater features were added and their position

fixed by triangulation, as above. Again, individual fishes were identified based on size,

body patterns and (most reliably) abnormalities (scars, parasite deformities etc.). For S.

frenatus and S. niger most individuals in the area were identified and used; for C.

sordidus only individually recognisable fish were used. The majority of observations

were conducted during two 3-month periods when the site was visited most days.

Ranges were based on a minimum of 5 hours of cumulative observations, or as a result

of > 50 individual sightings. Locations were plotted on the map, and home range areas

were again estimated using minimum convex polygons.

64

Statistical analysis

For each individual, body mass was calculated using length-weight regressions (Gust et

al. 2001). The relationship between fish mass and home range area was initially

examined using raw data; the most appropriate model to describe the relationships

between body mass and home range being fitted to the entire data set. The relationship

was subsequently assessed separately for juveniles (< 150 mm) and adults (> 250 mm).

Several regression models (linear, logarithmic, power, growth and exponential) were

fitted to the data and the model with the highest r2 value was selected for the overall

relationship, then juvenile and adult individuals separately (Appendix B). Once the

model was selected, the inflection point (i.e. the point in a curve where the slope of the

tangent equals 1) and associated errors from the model were calculated using MATLAB.

To provide a dimensionally balanced view of fish size and habitat areas, body

mass data were cube root transformed to provide a shape-independent, one-

dimensional, metric of increasing body size. Estimated home range sizes were then

square root transformed to provide a one-dimensional measure of home range size. The

residual data from the model fitted to these transformed data were then used to test for

variation among species (as these data had lower variance than untransformed data).

Interspecific variability in the home range size to body mass relationship was assessed

using a one-way ANOVA. The analysis compared the residual data from the model for

each species to determine if there was a significant different in the model’s fit between

species. The home range size of IP (initial phase) and TP (terminal phase) individuals

were compared using a two-sample t-test.

65

4.3 Results

The home ranges of 75 fish were estimated: 42 S. frenatus (15-356 mm TL), 14 S. niger

(11-304 mm TL) and 19 C. sordidus (10-240 mm TL). A logarithmic model was found

to provide the best fit for the relationship between body mass and home range size for

these three species (home range = 24.40 ln[body mass] + 55.58; Fig. 4.1), and was

considered to be the most biologically relevant. This model was significant, with body

mass explaining 76% of the variability in the home range data (r2 = 0.76; F1,73 = 236.44,

P < 0.001). The inflection point of the curve occurred when body mass equalled 24.4 ±

1.6 g. This corresponds to a length of 106.9 ± 4.4, 106.9 ± 4.4 and 107.0 ± 4.5 mm (TL

± error associated with the model) for S. frenatus, S. niger and C. sordidus respectively.

After this point, the rate of increase in home range per unit body mass was significantly

reduced. A power curve also provided a good statistical fit, but only for small

individuals; it explained little variation in larger specimens (Appendix B).

66

Fig. 4.1 Relationship between body mass (g) and home range size (m2) for Scarus

frenatus, S. niger and Chlorurus sordidus. Triangles represent juvenile individuals and

squares and circles represent initial phase and terminal phase individuals respectively.

Grey triangles indicate juveniles, which are predominantly omnivores/carnivores

(following Bellwood 1988). The dotted vertical line indicates the maximum body mass

achieved before S. frenatus and S. niger undergo juvenile to adult colour changes, and

C. sordidus shifts from solitary to schooling behaviour.

The relationship between the cube root of body mass and the square root of the

home range size, a scale independent relationship between mass and home range area,

was also best modelled with a logarithmic regression (home range1/2 = 4.39 ln[body

mass1/3] + 5.52; Fig. 4.2). The model was significant and explained 87% of the variation

in the data (r2 = 0.87; F1,73 = 484.43, P < 0.001). All three species exhibited similar

patterns of home range growth with no significant differences in the residuals among

species (Appendix B). Despite the logarithmic relationship being significant for the

Mass (g)0 200 400 600 800 1000

0

1

2

3H

ome

rang

e (1

00 m

2 )

67

overall pattern, two distinct components within the relationship were evident (Appendix

B). Juveniles (< 150mm), displayed a rapid increase in home range size with growth,

with home range expansion with body mass best described by a power curve regression

(Home range = 13.40 × [mass]0.71; r2 = 0.79; F1,53 = 201.25, P < 0.001; Appendix B).

Above a length of 100-150 mm (106.9-107.0 mm based on the calculated inflection

points) there was a breakdown in the size-area relationship, with adults displaying no

significant relationship between body mass and home range size, and with no

significant difference in home range size after changing sex (t-test; t = 0.57, P = 0.58).

Fig. 4.2 Scale-independent relationship between body mass and home range size. To

remove the effects of scale, body mass was cube-root transformed and home range

areas were square-root transformed. Data are presented for all species; Scarus frenatus,

S. niger and Chlorurus sordidus, with the dotted line representing the size at maturity.

The fitted line is a logarithmic regression, see text for details.

Body Mass (g1/3)

Hom

e ra

nge

(m)

02468

101214161820

0 2 4 6 8 10 12

68

4.4 Discussion

We quantified the home range size of parrotfish species at every post-settlement stage

of their development, covering an increase in mass of over five orders of magnitude.

This increase in size leads to considerable shifts in predation risk, nutritional demands,

maturation and, in some individuals, a protogynous sex change (Choat and Robertson

1975; Booth and Beretta 2004; Depczynski and Bellwood 2005; Almany and Webster

2006; Gagliano et al. 2007), all of which can potentially drive changes in home range

size. Where ontogenetic effects on home ranges have been studied, the causes of the

changes have generally proved difficult to disentangle. Examples from birds and

mammals are hampered by relatively limited growth and a high degree of parental care

(Georgii and Schröder 1983; Jouventin and Weimerskirch 1990; Evans 2008).

Nevertheless, predation, nutritional needs and reproduction have been repeatedly

identified as key potential drivers (Jetz et al. 2004; Imansyah et al. 2008; Owen-Smith

et al. 2010; Avgar et al. 2013). In reef fishes, the clear separation of growth and

associated changes in trophic and sexual status may permit a better understanding of the

factors driving ontogeny of home ranges. As a result, we can begin to identify the roles

of potential drivers of home range size versus body mass relationships in one group of

lower vertebrates, the parrotfishes. In particular, three factors; predation, trophic and

reproductive demands, can be addressed separately.

Does predation restrict home ranges?

For small reef fishes, as with all small organisms, the risk of predation is extremely

high (Booth and Beretta 2004; Depczynski and Bellwood 2005; Almany and Webster

2006). In such a hazardous environment as a coral reef, fishes face a trade-off between

foraging and remaining in, or near, shelter (Booth and Beretta 2004; Leahy et al. 2011).

69

Predation pressure is therefore likely to limit movement in small fishes (Depczynski

and Bellwood 2004). As they grow, however, larger body size enables home range

expansion, probably as a result of increasing handling costs for predators seen in both

reef fishes (Holmes and McCormick 2010) and forest birds, such as the junco (Junco

phaenotus; Sullivan 1989). Therefore, if predation was the primary mechanism driving

the mass-area relationship in parrotfishes, we would expect initially slow expansion of

home ranges in juveniles, followed by a rapid increase in the area occupied as fish grew

and predation risk declined. This is not the case for juvenile parrotfish, as they rapidly

expand their home range from the smallest size observed. The breakdown in the

relationship observed in larger fishes (> 100-150 mm) may result from a size threshold

above which a new suite of predators becomes important. However, this seems unlikely

as reef predators are capable of handling a wide size range of prey, with the vast

majority targeting small fishes (Almany and Webster 2006; Holmes and McCormick

2010). Overall, home ranges do not expand in a manner consistent with decreasing

predation. This suggests that other factors such as food availability may be more

important.

Trophic constraints on home ranges

Rapid expansions of home range with body size, comparable to those seen in juvenile

scarids, are occasionally observed in species seeking food resources when food is

limiting. This has been seen in many reptile species including Komodo dragons

(Imansyah et al. 2008), and some mammalian species, including ground squirrels

(Holekamp and Sherman 1989; Haskell et al. 2002; Börger et al. 2008), all of which

forage for widely dispersed, cryptic food resources. Small parrotfishes inhabit the

epilithic algal matrix (EAM) following settlement (Bellwood and Choat 1989), preying

70

on small cryptobenthic crustaceans and other invertebrates (Bellwood 1988). While

these invertebrates are highly abundant on coral reefs (Kramer et al. 2012), the rate at

which small parrotfish expand their home ranges suggests that not all invertebrates are

available to the fish and that larger ranges are needed to access adequate food resources.

The complex 3-dimensional structure of the EAM might provide shelter for the prey.

This, in conjunction with the limited gapes and jaw strength of juvenile parrotfishes

(Bonaldo and Bellwood 2008) suggests that they are only able to detect and acquire

prey close to the surface of the EAM. Thus, a larger home range would be required to

increase prey encounter rates. This type of demand has previously been documented in

cheetahs (Broomhall et al. 2003), and is especially prevalent in taxa with insectivorous

life stages, such as tropical lizards (Rocha 1999; Imansyah et al. 2008). Despite insects

being highly abundant on land, their availability is patchy and consequently, terrestrial

insectivores require large home ranges to encompass sufficient resources. Parrotfish

follow this pattern throughout their invertivorous stage, suggesting that the distribution

of available invertebrate prey might be comparably sparse or patchy.

Larger juveniles continue to exhibit rapid home range expansions with body

mass, despite a marked dietary shift from crustaceans to detritus and algae (based on

specimens collected from this study location; Bellwood 1988). Juvenile scarids possess

small beaks and as such can only make relatively shallow bites (Bonaldo and Bellwood

2008). With their limited bite size, juvenile parrotfish may therefore have to make a

disproportionately large number of bites over a large area to ingest sufficient nutrients

or to seek out patches of high quality EAM, with low sediment loads and a high detrital

component (Wilson et al. 2003). Again, large home ranges would increase the chances

of encountering sufficient high-quality food.

Overall, it would appear that the relationship between somatic size and the home

ranges of juvenile parrotfish is influenced primarily by the need to acquire high quality

71

resources rather than directly avoiding predation risk. However, predation pressure may

be indirectly involved. By accessing a large quantity of resources of the highest

nutritional quality, juveniles are able to grow rapidly, minimising the time they must

spend in life stages that are subject to the highest rates of predation.

Following maturation, home ranges cease to expand at the same rate. The reason

for this may, in part, be due to parrotfish functional morphology. The strength and size

of adult parrotfish beaks is significantly greater than their juvenile counterparts

(Bellwood and Choat 1990). This allows them to bite deeper into the EAM, removing

greater volumes of algae, invertebrates and particulate organic matter (Bellwood 1988;

Choat et al. 2004; Kramer et al. 2013). By adding a new dimension (depth) to their

feeding, it appears that adult parrotfish can access more resources per area than

juveniles which can only scrape the surface (Bonaldo and Bellwood 2008). Similar

changes in function are observed in terrestrial and marine retiles, with potential impacts

on their home ranges (Herrel et al. 2006; Imansyah et al. 2008; Marshall et al. 2012).

While changing functional capabilities may provide an explanation of why home range

expansion can cease, it does not explain the relatively rapid transition. This appears to

be driven by social interactions.

Reproductive constraints on home ranges

Between 100 and 150 mm, the relationship between somatic size and home range

changes and parrotfishes exhibit a marked ontogenetic shift in their home ranging

behaviour. Adult fishes had home ranges between 160 and 300 m2, which are broadly

comparable to other Pacific and Atlantic parrotfishes (van Rooij et al. 1996; Mumby

and Wabnitz 2002). If home range expansion continued to follow the pattern seen in the

juveniles, an individual would occupy approximately 16,000 m2 by the time it reached

72

300 mm (TL). This is over 50 times larger than any home range found in adults. Why

home ranges cease to expand with body size is particularly interesting and raises several

questions: how are adults able to acquire sufficient resources to meet metabolic

demands, especially given that juveniles respond to their environment as if they are

resource limited, and what mechanism drives the marked change in the relationship?

The social status of parrotfishes directly influences their ecology (van Rooij et al.

1996; Muñoz and Motta 2000; Bonaldo and Krajewski 2008), and may be responsible

for limiting home range expansions in adult fishes (Afonso et al. 2008; Welsh and

Bellwood 2012a). The breakdown in the area-mass relationship corresponds closely

with reported colour changes in two of the three focal species. These colour changes are

associated with the transition from juvenile to initial phase adults, and occur at

approximately 120 mm for both S. frenatus and S. niger (Sullivan 1989). Following

colour changes, the new initial phase adults of these species join small social groups, or

harems, with a single aggressive male defending a fixed territory. This social transition

corresponds closely with the changing area-mass relationship observed in our study,

where the home ranges of relatively small individuals rapidly increase until they match

the size of the harem leader’s territory. Thereafter, home ranges no longer increase with

an individual’s somatic size. This pattern of territorial range delineation has been

recorded in a number of other social taxa such as cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus;

Broomhall et al. 2003), red deer (Cervus elaphus; Georgii and Schöder 1983) and

coyotes (Canis latrans; Messier and Barrette 1982), in which young individuals occupy

the territory of their parents or social group (Marler et al. 1995; Mumby and Wabnitz

2002; Hinsch 2013). This social control of home range expansion after maturity might

be the reason we see no evidence of an effect of the change in sex (and colour)

associated with a transition from initial to terminal phase in adult parrotfishes.

73

C. sordidus does not exhibit drastic colour changes as they transition from the

juvenile to initial phase, and do not form aggressively defended harems. However, at

around 90 mm long, juveniles of this species shift from being solitary occupants of

sheltered back-reefs or deeper areas, to schooling, mature adults, most frequently

occupying shallow, exposed fore-reefs (Sullivan 1989). In parallel with this behavioural

shift, the area-mass relationship again changes abruptly, as seen in the haremic species.

While schooling species have significantly fewer aggressive interactions, they too

remain site attached as adults, apparently occupying areas of familiarity to facilitate

escape from predators or aggression from other species (Choat and Bellwood 1985;

Welsh and Bellwood 2012b).

Many studies have highlighted the predictability of the mean home range size of a

species, given its average body size; a standard interspecific scaling relationship (Rocha

1999; Haskell et al. 2002; West and West 2012). Within species, however, marked

changes in the rate of home range expansion are often associated with ontogenetic

changes in diet. Reptiles are among the best examples of this phenomenon, with some

species exhibiting wholesale shifts in diet, habitat and home ranging behaviour as they

increase their body mass (Herrel et al. 2006; Imansyah et al. 2008). In the parrotfishes,

the cessation of home range expansion, despite continued somatic growth, is markedly

different. In parrotfishes, the effect of changing diet or predation risk seems to have

little influence on spatial utilization patterns. Instead, for adult parrotfishes, complex

social systems appear to drive their home range-size relationships; a pattern with strong

parallels to those relationships seen in social birds and mammals. In birds, mammals

and other taxa living in social groups, the size of a social group’s territory is often

described as a ‘minimum economically defensible area’ where all members of a social

group have access to the group’s entire territory (Johnson et al. 2001; Clutton-Brock et

74

al. 2008). In these examples, as in parrotfishes, body size does not play a significant

role in determining the home range size of an individual within the group.

The present study highlights the need to assess the spatial behaviour of organisms

at all stages in their growth and development in order to understand the nature of home

ranging behaviour. With growth, juvenile parrotfishes displayed rapid increases in

home range size which appears to be driven by increased nutritional demands. A

distinct change in the rate of home range expansion mirrors changes in colour patterns

and appears to be shaped primarily by social factors associated with sexual maturity

(Sullivan 1989), while changes in body size, diet and sex appear to have a limited

impact on the overarching area-size relationship. Overall, juvenile parrotfishes operate

like forest dwelling lizards, while adults operate like social mammals. Our observations

suggest that for fishes, inter- and intra-specific size-area relationships may be shaped by

markedly different drivers.

75

Chapter 5: Herbivorous fishes, ecosystem function and

mobile links on coral reefs Published in Coral Reefs 2014 33: 303-311

5.1 Introduction

Many studies have emphasised the need to increase resilience to help limit or prevent

ecosystem decline (Vitousek et al. 1997; Scheffer et al. 2001). The resilience of an

ecosystem refers to the capacity of the system to respond to, and recover, after a

disturbance event (Folke et al. 2004; Hughes et al. 2003, 2007). To be resilient, a

system must have the ecological capacity to maintain critical ecosystem processes

(Hughes et al. 2003; Carpenter et al. 2006). One key component of resilience is

connectivity via mobile links (Lundberg and Moberg 2003; Rico et al. 2012). Mobile

links are described as those taxa which have the capacity to move between systems,

particularly where they supplement functional processes by moving from a relatively

intact system to one degraded by exploitation or natural disturbances (Lundberg and

Moberg 2003). Mobile links may be passive (e.g. marine invertebrate larvae) or active

(e.g. bats, birds and large terrestrial herbivores), depending on their capacity for

movement (Lundberg and Moberg 2003; Couvreur et al. 2004; Kremen 2005;

Sekercioglu 2006).

In the marine environment, the passive mobile links of invertebrates and the more

active larval fishes have been widely studied, and their ability to interconnect reefs is

well established (e.g. Jones et al. 1999; Patterson and Swearer 2007; Almany et al.

2009). These larval connections are usually over large spatial and temporal scales

(regional to biogeographic scales, over weeks to years) (Patterson and Swearer 2007;

Shanks 2009). In contrast, the reef-scale, short-term movement or connectivity of adults

76

has received less attention, and the extent and nature of active mobile links in the

marine environment is poorly understood. This is especially troubling on coral reefs

given the potential importance of ecological interactions conferred by one of the key

active mobile links, roving herbivorous reef fishes (e.g. Nyström and Folke 2001;

Ceccarelli et al. 2011).

Reef fishes have a number of functional roles, which support the resilience of

coral reef ecosystems (Bellwood et al. 2004; Folke et al. 2004). One key process is

herbivory, where the feeding activities of herbivorous fish mediate coral-algae

interactions and help maintain a coral dominated state (McCook 1997; Bellwood et al.

2004; Burkepile and Hay 2010). Within the herbivore guild, separate functional groups

have been identified, each important in maintaining different components of reef

resilience. Scrapers (e.g. Scarus spp.), excavators (e.g. Chlorurus spp.) and croppers

(e.g. Acanthurus spp.) are responsible for grazing the reef’s epilithic algal matrix and

keeping algal growth in check (Fox and Bellwood 2007). Browsers (e.g. Kyphosus spp.)

are unusual in that they feed directly on adult leathery macroalgae, and play an

important role in the removal of macroalgae (Bellwood et al. 2004; Burkepile and Hay

2008; Hoey and Bellwood 2009; Rasher et al. 2013). Given its importance in

regenerating degraded ecosystems, a great deal of research has recently been focused on

quantifying the browsing functional role (e.g. Burkepile and Hay 2010; Michael et al.

2013), especially on the GBR (McCook 1997; Hughes et al. 2007; Hoey and Bellwood

2009). This research has found that the process is overwhelmingly dominated by a

limited number of species (predominantly Naso unicornis and Kyphosus vaigiensis),

and that it is highly variable both spatially and temporally (Cvitanovic and Bellwood

2009; Bennett and Bellwood 2011; Lefèvre and Bellwood 2011). Understanding the

nature of this variability is key to understanding how browsing herbivores interact with

their environment, support its resilience and offer a capacity to regenerate or recover

77

from degradation. However, the factors that underpin this variability remain to be

determined and are likely to be strongly influenced by the spatial scales over which

browsing herbivores feed.

In the terrestrial environment, large bodied herbivorous mammals will travel

several hundred kilometres in search of food. They act as mobile links, delivering both

their grazing activities and ingested plant seeds to distant, spatially separated locations,

and thus interconnect metapopulations (Couvreur et al. 2004; Owen-Smith et al. 2010).

On coral reefs, there is no evidence of similar widespread feeding behaviour and thus,

little evidence that fishes act as mobile links in this manner (but see Vermeij et al.

2013). Indeed, most reef fishes, including roving herbivores, appear to have extremely

limited movements (Eristhee and Oxenford 2001; Meyer and Holland 2005; Fox and

Bellwood 2011; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a, b). The basic question in understanding

functionally important mobile links on coral reefs is: at what point is an individual’s

home range large enough for them to act as a significant vector for functional

connectivity (i.e. a mobile link)?

Evidence of a relationship between body length and a fish’s home range length

has been used to identify the size range of fishes which are expected to be protected by

a MPA of a given size (Kramer and Chapman 1999). This relationship highlights the

limited movement of many smaller species, a finding that has been strongly supported

by recent studies, which have employed acoustic telemetry to assess the movement

patterns of individual herbivorous fish species. These studies have invariably found that

the movements of reef species are constrained to relatively small areas of reef,

regardless of the fish’s social characteristics (e.g. haremic or schooling) and thus, these

species may contribute little to functional connectivity (Afonso et al. 2008; Hardman et

al. 2010; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a, b). Indeed, the traditional distinction between

small solitary and large roving herbivores appears to be largely a reflection of size and

78

even the largest ‘roving’ herbivores do not move very far, even when foraging in

schools (Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). Therefore, most fishes on coral reefs may not be

mobile links at all, a sobering notion given that large-scale movements by herbivores

have been described as being among the most important mobile links on coral reefs

(Nyström and Folke 2001).

Our goal, therefore, was to: a) determine if all major nominal reef herbivore

groups show comparably small range sizes and b) to contextualize the movements of a

functionally important herbivorous species, K. vaigiensis, with those reported for other

coral reef species in the primary literature. To evaluate the first objective, we assess

home range data on a largely overlooked family of coral reef herbivores, the

Kyphosidae. We then compare these home range data to published home range sizes of

representative species from every major herbivore family and to a broad range of non-

herbivore species. With this information, we provide an overview of herbivorous

mobile links on coral reefs in order to better understand potential connectivity of

functional processes on an inter- and intra-reef scale, and discuss the implications of

how this may influence the predicted local-scale ecosystem benefits of MPAs.

5.2 Materials and Methods

Home range of Kyphosus vaigiensis

This study took place between September 2011 and January 2012 on the fringing reefs

surrounding Orpheus Island (18°350’S, 146°200’E), an inshore island on the GBR. The

majority of the spatial sampling effort occurred within Pioneer Bay, on the leeward side

of Orpheus Island, with an extensive reef flat and a moderately complex reef structure

(detailed descriptions of Pioneer Bay are given in Welsh et al. 2012).

79

Acoustic monitoring

The movements of K. vaigiensis around Orpheus Island were quantified using acoustic

monitoring. This involved the construction of an array of 46 acoustic receivers (VR2W;

Vemco, Halifax, Canada), deployed around Orpheus Island (Fig. 5.1a). The majority of

the receivers (25 × VR2Ws) were placed within Pioneer Bay to provide a high-

resolution indication of tagged individual’s activities within the bay (Fig. 5.1b). The

remaining receivers (15 × VR2Ws) were moored at key monitoring positions around

the island, i.e. the points and centres of nearly every bay (Fig. 5.1a). Six additional

receivers on adjacent islands (three on Pelorus and three on Phantom) had no

detections. The effective detection range (where 50% of acoustic signals are detected)

of the receivers was assessed and found to be approximately 55 m throughout the

duration of the study (Welsh et al. 2012).

Prior to tagging, the population size of K. vaigiensis was estimated. To ensure an

accurate estimation of the proportion of the population of K. vaigiensis being sampled,

five 1.4 km x 15 m snorkel transects (spanning the entire length of Pioneer Bay) were

conducted along the reef crest. A 15 m width was selected to ensure that widths could

be maintained regardless of visibility over the census days. Each census was undertaken

on non-consecutive days.

Individual K. vaigiensis were captured by divers on SCUBA from four separate

sites over a 1.4 km stretch of reef within Pioneer Bay using barrier nets in September

2011 (Fig. 5.1b). Four capture sites were used to maximize the chances of sampling fish

from separate schools. Once captured, fish were transported to the Orpheus Island

Research Station where they were held in 3300 L flow-through tanks prior to surgical

tagging. To tag the fish, individuals were first anaesthetised in a tricane

methanesulfonate (MS-222) seawater solution (0.13 gL-1). Once the fish was sedated,

80

the fork length (FL) was recorded and a small incision was made in the body wall. An

ultrasonic transmitter (tag; V9-1L, random delay interval 190-290 s, power output 146

dB re 1 µPa at 1m, Vemco) was then inserted into the peritoneal cavity of the

individual, and the incision was sutured closed and treated with antiseptic. Following

surgery, individuals were held in captivity for 12-24 h to recover before being released

back at their site of capture.

Fig. 5.1 Orpheus Island receiver (VR2W, Vemco) deployment sites. Black circles mark

the location of each receiver. Filled in circles represent receivers with > 5% of at least

one individual Kyphosus vaigiensis’ detections; open circles had no significant

detections. a) Map of Orpheus Island with large-scale receiver placements. b) Array in

Pioneer Bay showing depth contours and numbered stars representing capture and

release sites of individuals (capture site 1: K40, K41, K42, K43, K44; capture site 2:

K31, K32, K35; capture site 3: K36, K37, K38, K39; capture site 4: K33, K34). Dotted

lines represent reef area.

800 m

Orpheus Island

400m

0.5m

0m

5m

1.1m0.6m

1m

0.8m

0.8m

1.3m

0.8m

0.6m

0.6m

1.1m

N

4

32

1

3

b Pioneer Bay

a

81

Before the data were analysed, each individual's detection plots were inspected

using the VUE software package (Vemco) to check for signs of mortality. Mortality

was identified by an obvious change in an individual’s movement patterns to long

periods of inactivity. If mortality was suspected, all subsequent detections were

excluded from the analyses (one individual was excluded on this basis; Appendix C).

For analysis, the first 24 hours of data from each individual following release was

excluded to remove any unusual behaviour, which may arise from tagging. Each

individual’s detection data were then separated into diurnal and nocturnal sampling

periods. Diurnal periods were set from 05:30 to 19:30 h to incorporate crepuscular

movements and nocturnal periods were defined as 19:31 to 05:29 h. The frequency and

variability of large-scale movements were analysed using the adehabitat LT package for

R (Calenge 2006). The home range length of each individual was calculated for diurnal

and nocturnal periods using two metrics: the minimum linear dispersal (MLD) and the

median distance travelled (MDT). Both metrics were calculated using adehabitatLT.

The MLD is defined as the shortest possible distance between the two most distant

receivers where an individual has been detected (Murchie et al. 2010). For the purposes

of this study, this metric will be used to represent the minimum home range length. The

MDT provides a metric of the median dispersal of an individual from its principal area

of residence. Individual’s MDT values were quantified by first determining the receiver

on which an individual is most frequently detected, and then calculating the median

distance between that receiver and all other receivers where the fish was detected

(Murchie et al. 2010). The proportion of detections at an individual’s principal

detection location was also calculated for each individual in diurnal and nocturnal

periods to compare how stationary or site-attached individuals are at night versus the

day. To meet the assumptions of the parametric t-test, MLD data were log-transformed

and a logit-transformation was applied to the proportion data (Warton and Hui 2011).

82

To further explore individual movement patterns, we calculate the average number of

detections per day for each individual at each of the VR2W acoustic receivers. These

detection frequencies were calculated for diurnal and nocturnal periods. The

distribution of detections (expressed as a coefficient of variation [CV = standard

deviation / mean] for each individual) provides a representation of the spread of the

movements of each individual K. vaigiensis throughout their home range. High CV

values indicate a heterogeneous distribution of an individual’s detections throughout the

acoustic array, while low CV values suggest that the distribution is more homogeneous.

All the above metrics were calculated using the data set for the entire study period.

However, for each individual, the average MLD was also calculated over 5 separate

randomly selected days to quantify the average size of an individual’s daily movements.

A t-test for matched pairs was used to compare the diurnal and nocturnal

sampling periods for the overall MLD, the daily MLD, and the proportion of detections

at individual principal detection locations. The CV values for each individual were also

compared between diurnal and nocturnal samples using a t-test for matched pairs.

Data for general home range relationship

To evaluate the potential of herbivorous reef fishes to act as mobile links, a dataset of

published coral reef fish home range length data was assembled by searching the ISI

Web of Science and Google Scholar for primary research articles using the following

keywords: fish, coral reef, home range, movement, spatial. Studies were limited to

those conducted on adult coral reef taxa that provided an estimation of the linear

movements of focal taxa. Studies on adult reproductive migrations were excluded. The

selection criteria avoided confounding factors associated with ontogenetic home range

expansion and reproductive behaviour. Furthermore, data were excluded if individuals

83

did not survive for > 24 h following release after tagging and/or if based on homing

studies, as these cases would most likely represent unusual behaviour. From each study,

the maximum distanced moved by any individual of each species was recorded along

with the individual’s corresponding body size (measured as fork length) following

Freiwald (2012). The functional group was also noted and classified as either carnivore

or herbivore. If several studies were available for the same species, the study that

reported the largest movement, meeting the aforementioned criteria, was used.

Body size data exhibited a non-normal (positively skewed) distribution. Data

were therefore square root transformed to normalize the data. The relationship between

body size and home range length was analysed using GLMs in (lme4 package in R;

Bates & Maechler 2009; R Development Core Team 2011). Initially, an overall model

was constructed irrespective of functional group. Separate models were then

constructed for each functional group. To test for deviations from each model, an

analysis of each species’ Cook’s Distance was used (Quinn and Keough 2002).

5.3 Results

Home range of Kyphosus vaigiensis

In total, 14 individual K. vaigiensis (average length 29.4 ± 0.7 cm SE; range 23.4-32.6

cm) were tagged. This represented approximately 10% of the total within-bay

population size, which was estimated to be 148.4 ± 8.8 individuals. Over the 5-month

study, individual K. vaigiensis exhibited consistently large home ranges regardless of

capture site. Individuals travelled an average minimum linear distance (MLD) of

2,521.4 ± 713.7 m during the day and 2,625.9 ± 880.1 m at night (and ranging from 784

to 13,352 m; Fig. 5.1; Table 5.1; Appendix C). These values were extensive and, on

average covered over 10% of the available coastline of Orpheus Island for both diurnal

84

and nocturnal periods (and ranged from 2-53%). No significant difference was detected

between the diurnal and nocturnal MLDs over the whole study (t11 = 0.62, P = 0.55).

Assuming a constant home range width of 50 m (the approximate width of the reef from

base to outer flat on Orpheus Island), a conservative estimate of the potential home

range area of K. vaigiensis was 126,070 ± 35,683 m2 in the day and 131,294 ± 44,000

m2 at night.

The patterns for the MLD were mirrored by the median distance travelled (MDT),

with almost every fish moving large distances away from their principal area of

detection. Individuals had an average MDT of 442.5 ± 49.4 m during diurnal periods

and 333.6 ± 26.7 m during nocturnal periods. Despite individuals being detected over

the majority of the array, individuals had significantly higher proportions of their total

detections at their site of principal detection during nocturnal periods, when compared

to diurnal periods (30.2% and 15.8% respectively) (t11 = -5.0, P < 0.001). The values

reported for the MLD and MDT are likely to be conservative, with a much larger actual

movement range of K. vaigiensis as the residency index was on average 76 ± 0.8%,

Table 5.1 Average metrics of Kyphosus vaigiensis movement data separated by diurnal and nocturnal sampling periods. For individual data, please see Appendix C.

Diel period

Minimum linear distance (m)

Home range estimate (m2)

Median distance traveled (m)

Residency index Total study

period Five day average

(with percent of total)

Diurnal 2,521.4 ± 713.7

1,091.6 ± 103.7 (59% ± 5)

126,070 ± 35,683

442 ± 49.4

0.76 ± 0.1 Nocturnal 2,625.9 ±

880.0 875.7 ± 65.4

(57% ± 7) 131,293.9 ± 44,000

333.6 ± 26.7

85

indicating that individuals were beyond the detection regions of the array 24% of the

time on the days analysed (Table 5.1).

The distributions of an individual’s average daily detections were highly variable,

with detections over 24 h spread across the majority of the acoustic array. However,

when evaluated in diurnal versus nocturnal subsections, it was clear that species were

generally more mobile in diurnal sampling periods (Appendix C). Diurnal samples were

characterized by records at a number of receivers. Nocturnal samples usually had a

major location and relatively few records at other receivers. This is supported by CV

values, which were significantly lower over diurnal samples (mean = 1.76) compared to

nocturnal samples (mean = 2.50; t13 = -3.22, P < 0.01).

Daily movement patterns

When only a single 24 h period was considered (with 5 random days used for

replication), individual’s MLD values were quite large, with an average of 1,091.6 ±

103.7 m and 875.7 ± 65.4 m, during diurnal and nocturnal periods respectively.

Individual’s 24 h subsample movements values were, on average, 59% of an

individual’s total diurnal MLD and 57% of an individual’s total nocturnal MLD (Table

5.1; Appendix C). The average 24 h MLD values of each individual were significantly

different when comparing diurnal and nocturnal movements (t11 = 3.02, P = 0.01), with

most individual’s diurnal movements being larger (Appendix C).

86

General home range relationship

The overall model of body length versus home range length for both herbivores and

carnivores was significant and positive (F1,38 = 28.77, P < 0.001; r2 = 0.76). There was

also a significant positive relationship between body size and home range length (F1,15 =

5.47, P < 0.05; r2 = 0.51) for all herbivores, excluding K. vaigiensis. However, when K.

vaigiensis was included, there was no longer a significant relationship between body

size and home range length for herbivores (F1,16 = 1.48, P > 0.05). Of all the herbivores,

only K. vaigiensis had a significant Cook’s Distance value of > 0.5, indicating that the

inclusion of this outlying data point had a significant effect on the modelled relationship

(Fig. 5.2). There was also positive, significant relationship between body size and home

range for carnivorous fish taxa (F1,20 = 26.40, P < 0.001; r2 = 0.74). Based on the

Cook’s Distance analysis, two species, Sphyraena barracuda and Albula vulpes, were

exceptional and significantly influenced the carnivore model, with Cook’s Distance

values > 0.5 (Fig. 5.2).

87

Fig. 5.2 Relationship between home range length (m) and fish body length (√fork

length; mm) for carnivorous and herbivorous taxa with 95% confidence intervals for

each trendline. Species with a Cook’s distance value of > 0.5 are labelled and have solid

circles.

Hom

e ra

nge

leng

th (k

m)

10

5

15

20

0

10 15 20 25 30 35Body size1/2 (mm)

K. vaigiensis

A. vulpes

S. barracuda

Carnivore

Herbivore

88

5.4 Discussion

Despite our rapidly increasing knowledge of the spatial ecology of marine taxa, no

herbivorous species on coral reefs have been quantitatively identified as mobile links as

adults. Most reef fishes remain close to the structure provided by the reef and have been

shown to be less willing than their terrestrial counterparts to cross gaps in shelter of

more than 20 m (Meyer et al. 2010; Turgeon et al. 2010). Among herbivores, K.

vaigiensis appears to be an exception to this rule, being sufficiently vagile to travel

large distances, not only across the reef within a single bay, but also between fringing

reefs up to 11 km apart. It is, to our knowledge, the only documented coral reef

herbivore to-date capable of providing short-term reef-scale connectivity of functional

processes.

The association between body size and home range length highlights the site-

attached nature of reef fishes, especially the herbivores, the presumed mobile links.

Even one of the largest herbivores, Naso unicornis, is predicted by the herbivore model

to occupy a range of just 1,024 (± 381.84) m (linear home range). A finding strongly

supported by several studies of the species (estimating its range to be between 240 and

940 m long) and highlighting its site-attached behaviour (e.g. Meyer and Holland 2005;

Hardman et al. 2010). Most herbivores, therefore, appear to contribute little as mobile

links. However, K. vaigiensis, a functionally important herbivorous reef fish

(Cvitanovic and Bellwood 2009; Hoey and Bellwood 2011), appears to depart

significantly from the expected body size - home range length relationship. It is a

significant outlier for a herbivorous species, with a range over three times larger than

expected based on its size. The distances moved by this species are more similar to

those of the pelagic carnivorous species, Sphyraena barracuda (O’Toole et al. 2011)

and Albula vulpes (Murchie et al. 2013), which were likewise the only other taxa that

deviated significantly from the predicted relationship for non-herbivore fishes. It is by

89

virtue of these unusual, large-scale (i.e. > 2 km) daily movements that K. vaigiensis

may be important for the functional connectivity within reefs, serving as an active

mobile link and thereby able to contribute significantly to reef resilience across a range

of spatial scales.

The importance of the unique, large-scale movement of K. vaigiensis is most

apparent when placed in context of its functional role. The role of browsing herbivores

is one in which functional redundancy (i.e. niche overlap of several taxa performing the

same functional role) is extremely limited (Hoey and Bellwood 2009). On the GBR, the

removal of adult macroalgae is restricted to between two and six species (Bellwood et

al. 2006; Mantyka and Bellwood 2007), although often only one two species are

predominant in a single area (Cvitanovic and Bellwood 2009; Hoey and Bellwood

2010). Among these few species, K. vaigiensis stands out consistently as a significant

predator of macroalgae (Cvitanovic and Bellwood 2009; Lefèvre and Bellwood 2011).

Indeed on several occasions, this species and its congenerics have been noted to play a

vital role in coral reef resilience through intense predation of macroalgae (Downie et al.

2013; Michael et al. 2013). The exceptional mobility of a single species within this

vitally important ecological role for reef health suggests that short-term mobile links in

adult herbivores may be particularly rare on coral reefs.

Home range size in reef fishes

Herbivorous fishes have previously been recognized as important mobile links for

functional processes (Nyström and Folke 2001; Lundberg and Moberg 2003). However,

this may not be the case for most species. Only K. vaigiensis exhibited attributes that

would enable it to act as a mobile link, providing large-scale ecosystem connectivity by

interconnecting ecological processes on reefs up to 11 km apart. Observations of the

90

behaviour of K. vaigiensis at this site found that it invariably travels in large schools

(>20 individuals). Therefore the large home ranges exhibited by the tagged individuals

are likely to be representative of a significant proportion of the population.

On average, K. vaigiensis occupies a stretch of reef 2,521 m long. This is

approximately 2 to 100 times larger than other roving herbivorous species. In

comparison, the home ranges of several similarly-sized territorial or haremic

herbivorous species, such as Chlorurus microrhinos from the GBR (Welsh and

Bellwood 2012a), Sparisoma cretense in the mid-Atlantic (Afonso et al. 2008) and S.

viride from the Caribbean (van Rooij et al. 1996), have been found to be relatively

small, with home range lengths of just 266, 460, and 25 m respectively. These restricted

ranges likely occur as a result of the trade-off between the energetic expenditure of

territorial defence and the benefits of exclusive access to a territory’s resources. These

benefits become unfavourable when too large an area must be defended (Bonaldo et al.

2006; Laguë et al. 2012). We would therefore expect smaller, limited home ranges in

territorial or haremic species. However, even schooling, non-territorial species also

seem to have quite limited home range sizes. The schooling parrotfish Scarus rivulatus

in the GBR (Welsh and Bellwood 2012b) and Sparisoma chrysopterum from the

Caribbean (Muñoz and Motta 2000) both appear to have restricted home range sizes

(albeit slightly larger than those of territorial species). Small home ranges in schooling

species have been attributed to a need for familiarity with shelter sites from predation

(Wittenberger 1981; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a, b). Even herbivore species with

similar diets and behaviour to K. vaigiensis, such as Naso unicornis, N. lituratus, and K.

sectatrix, are reported to have restricted spatial ranges, regardless of the geographic

locations (e.g. Eristhee and Oxenford 2001; Meyer and Holland 2005; Hardman et al.

2010). The question remains as to why K. vaigiensis appears to be unique among reef

herbivores in regards to its movement patterns.

91

The explanation for the relatively large home range in K. vaigiensis may lie in

their feeding behaviour. Ecological theory suggests that there is a trade-off between the

risks (i.e. increased exposure to predation while moving across habitats and energetic

expenditure on swimming) and the benefits of having a large home range (i.e. access to

higher quality food resources) (Lindstedt et al. 1986; Kramer and Chapman 1999;

Owen-Smith et al. 2010). The available evidence for K. vaigiensis suggests that, when

encountered, it preferentially targets brown leathery macroalgae (Cvitanovic and

Bellwood 2009; Lefèvre and Bellwood 2011). Indeed, it is one of the few species to

ingest adult macroalgae (Green and Bellwood 2009). Previous studies on the diet and

gut physiology of K. vaigiensis found a dominance of brown macroalgae in their guts,

even when collected in areas with very low abundances of the algae (McCook 1997;

Choat et al. 2004). Hoey and Bellwood (2010) suggest that this species may be highly

effective at locating cryptic or isolated strands of macroalgae. If so, large home ranges

in this species would increase encounter rates with macroalgae and facilitate a selective

diet dominated by macroalgae. Such larger-scale movements for the purpose of food

acquisition have been noted for other kyphosid species on Ningaloo Reef. K.

sydneyanus, for example, was found to travel significantly further from patch reefs to

prey on macroalgae than other browsing species (Downie et al. 2013). This movement

is attributed to large body size and schooling behaviour in this species which may

reduce an individual’s predation risk while feeding far from the shelter of the reef

(Downie et al. 2013). It may a similar behaviour in K. vaigiensis that facilitates the

large-scale movement patterns recorded herein.

The large-scale movements of K. vaigiensis appear to occur over both diurnal and

nocturnal periods. However, diurnal movements are largest with longer times spent

away from the main receiver, which may reflect foraging activities. This foraging and

the associated searching for macroalgae, appears to occur mainly during the day. This is

92

supported by gut content analysis, which suggests that the majority of gut filling in K.

vaigiensis occurs during the day (Choat et al. 2004). Nocturnal movements however,

(excluding potential crepuscular movements) were also found to be quite extensive and

cannot be ignored. For the browsing herbivore species N. unicornis, nocturnal forays

have been reported away from shelter sites and it was suggested that these movements

most likely represent foraging forays (Meyer and Holland 2005). This may also be the

case for K. vaigiensis with higher peak nocturnal detections at a single receiver, likely

representing a resting site, but with significant movements away from that location at

night (this may be to feed as in the grazer Siganus lineatus; Fox and Bellwood 2011).

Overall, K. vaigiensis appears to be a diurnal browser with the possibility of some

nocturnal feeding activity.

Significance of mobile links in reef systems

The large (> 2 km) home ranges and selective feeding behaviour of K. vaigiensis holds

promise for the connectivity of ecological processes between areas of a reef, and for the

capacity of fish taxa to act as mobile links. Localized macroalgal outbreaks do occur on

coral reefs (Burgess 2006). In the event of an outbreak as a result of localized

disturbance or sporadic outbreaks, K. vaigiensis from neighbouring parts of the reef

have the capacity to travel to the affected areas and consume macroalgae within hours

or days. This is especially important as a reliance on larval connectivity may be

insufficient to respond to a pulse disturbance, especially given the stochastic nature of

larval recruitment (Siegel et al. 2008). Even if larvae recruit to an area immediately

following an algal outbreak, the ability of these fishes to consume algae is limited as the

ecological impact of fishes is size dependent (Bonaldo and Bellwood 2008). Given that

an experimentally-induced phase-shift exhibited dramatic increases in algal cover after

93

only 6 months (Hughes et al. 2007), the growth rate of fishes from larvae may not be

sufficient for functionally important taxa to reach a large enough size to reduce algae

after it has taken hold. Following an initial increase in algal colonization, negative

feedbacks described by Hoey and Bellwood (2011) may further reduce the capacity of

herbivores to remove macroalgae, thus increasing the likelihood of a large-scale phase-

shift. K. vaigiensis may, therefore, provide a vital first response linking large spatial

scales, ultimately supporting system-wide resilience on coral reefs.

It must be noted that mobile links are always scale dependent. K. vaigiensis is

demonstrably the most mobile herbivorous fish species recorded to date and the only

one capable of regular movement over 2 km. However, it still moved around a single

island, and all reefs and bays were connected by hard ground, not open sand or deep

water. Movement between widely spaced reefs may require mobile links operating on

even larger scales. Although such movement is possible (Goatley et al. 2012),

functional connectivity and mobile links by adult fishes, at present, appear to be

predominantly a within-reef phenomenon.

While the large spatial ranges of K. vaigiensis suggest that they are likely to serve

as important vectors for the transfer of functional processes, it may also leave this

species vulnerable to exploitation. The size of MPAs throughout the tropics is highly

variable, but the majority are less than 1 km2 (Wood et al. 2008). Many are likely to be

a great deal smaller than the average movement range of K. vaigiensis. The intraspecific

variability in individual’s mobility detected for this species, suggests that localized

fishing pressure is likely to impact each individual differently, depending on its

mobility. Individuals that exhibited the greatest large-scale movement ranges may be

the first to be lost from a population under heavy fishing pressure as they disperse out

of a marine reserve. Therefore, in areas of the world with high fishing pressure, it is

94

likely that highly mobile individuals have already been lost from the local population

and thus, a major source of functional connectivity may be missing from these regions.

The fact that K. vaigiensis is only one of a handful of reef fish species known to

eat adult brown macroalgae, and that it is the only species to have movements that may

be considered capable of offering functional connectivity, at a > 2 km scale, highlights

the vulnerability of reefs to the exploitation of reef fish taxa. Indeed, functional

redundancy on coral reefs (i.e. overlap in the functional roles of different species) has

been shown, on several occasions, to be much lower than expected (e.g. Bellwood et al.

2003; Hoey and Bellwood 2009). It appears that large-scale sources of spatial

redundancy (i.e. the spatial overlap of individuals contributing critical functional

processes (Goatley et al. 2012; Welsh and Bellwood 2012b) are also quite limited.

Ultimately, this suggests that, until the recruitment of new fishes has occurred

following a disturbance event, a degraded system will be heavily reliant on residual

local species and a limited number of taxa to maintain ecosystem functions, with one

possible exception. At present, K. vaigiensis appears to be unique among reef

herbivores and is the first fish to be identified as a potential mobile link, able to support

the large-scale application of herbivory on adult macroalgae on coral reefs. As a

specialist feeding on leathery brown macroalgae, this species may be the key to

avoiding phase-shifts on coral reefs. However, it is also a species that can gain little

protection from current small-size MPAs. For this critical species, gear restrictions or

species-specific protection may be a more appropriate management option (Graham et

al. 2013).

95

Chapter 6: Local degradation triggers a large-scale

response on coral reefs

6.1 Introduction

Ecosystem degradation is a common problem faced throughout the world, with changes

compromising the complexity and productivity of ecosystems (e.g. Ishii et al. 2004;

Hoegh-Guldberg et al. 2007; Pardini et al. 2010; Long and Shekar 2013). In many

systems, the recovery of ecosystem communities and processes relies strongly on

‘mobile links’ (Couvreur et al. 2004; Olds et al. 2012). Mobile links are taxa with large-

scale movements that act as vectors transferring essential elements of recovery from

relatively healthy systems to more degraded ones (Lundberg and Moberg 2003). In

tropical rainforests, fruit bats and bird taxa are good examples of such mobile links.

Through their large-scale movements, seeds originating from healthy fruit trees are

dispersed in faecal matter over a wide area (Cox et al. 1991; Duncan and Chapman

1999). This is especially important for ecosystem recovery, as the seeds transported by

the bats and birds may be deposited in degraded areas where the mature forest canopy

has been removed and the seed bank depleted (Lundberg and Moberg 2003; Henry et al.

2007). Bats and birds are airborne and thus the complexity of the forest, or lack thereof,

has only a limited influence on their dispersive movements (Nathan 2006; Muscarella

and Fleming 2007). In contrast, monkeys and other mammals, another key group

responsible for seed dispersal, exhibit a limited contribution to recovery processes. This

is because they are unwilling to enter areas of lowered complexity, where seeds are

required, due to the elevated risk of predation in open habitats (Wunderle 1997). It

appears that the scale of animal movements, and the factors that shape their spatial

movement patterns, are key elements underpinning the recovery of degraded systems.

96

Mobile links may also be important determinants ecosystem recovery in coral reef

ecosystems (Hoey and Bellwood 2011; Chong-Seng et al. 2012). Herbivorous fish

species are the main predators of macroalgae on coral reefs and are agents of system

restoration in the early phases of macroalgal proliferation (Bellwood et al. 2004;

Burkepile and Hay 2010; Ceccarelli et al. 2011). Indeed, acute marcroalgal blooms (i.e.

outbreaks) are becoming increasingly common on even seemingly healthy reefs

(Burgess 2006) and thus, the capacity of fish to mitigate these occurrences is critical.

However, recent evidence suggests that when macroalgae replace corals as the

dominant structure-forming benthic organisms, mobile herbivores are less willing to

forage in these areas (Bellwood et al. 2006; Hoey and Bellwood 2011, Chong-Seng et

al 2012). It has been hypothesised that fishes avoid areas of high algal density because

the complexity provided by the algae may conceal predators and thus, present an area of

elevated predation risk (Hoey and Bellwood 2011). This phenomenon may explain why

high algal cover is correlated with a marked decline in fish biomass and diversity

(Friedlander et al. 2007; Chong-Seng et al. 2012).

Given the potential avoidance of degraded areas by mobile links, and the resultant

lack of their respective ecosystem functions, it is important to understand how coral

reef herbivores change their spatial patterns in response to outbreaks. It is known that

the movement patterns of fish are strongly influenced by the presence of complexity

and predation pressure (e.g. Afonso et al. 2009; Fox and Bellwood 2011; Welsh and

Bellwood 2012a), and the availability of food sources (Bruggemann et al. 1994a; Meyer

and Holland 2005). However, it is not yet known if key fish taxa exhibit plasticity in

their home range utilisation patterns, and are willing to shift their centres of activity to

access temporally variable resources or, conversely, to reduce activity in areas of their

habitat that become unfavourable.

97

The notion that the spatial tendencies of taxa can have a marked impact on the

recovery potential of ecosystems is troubling given the available evidence for coral

reefs. The scales over which herbivorous fish taxa operate appear, with only rare

exceptions, to be small, with site-attached behaviour being the most common (e.g.

Eristhee and Oxenford 2001; Meyer and Holland 2005; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a;

Brandl and Bellwood 2013). Moreover, our understanding of the response of herbivores

to the presence of algae on reefs is entirely limited to small-scale simulated outbreaks

(Hay 1981; Lewis 1985; Hoey and Bellwood 2011). Indeed, assessments of herbivore

predation of macroalgae on healthy reefs are overwhelmingly based on assays

comprised of a single thallus, or bunch of algae, and rarely exceed 1 m2 (e.g. Hoey and

Bellwood 2009; Rasher et al. 2013, but see Hughes et al. 2007; Burkepile and Hay

2010). These evaluations have identified several groups of herbivores with important

functions (croppers, scrapers and excavators which graze algae, and browsers which

consume adult macroalgae; Bellwood et al. 2004). Yet we know little of the behaviour

and spatial scales over which these key taxa operate, especially in response to

macroalgal outbreaks. Clearly larger-scale experimental manipulations are required to

understand how the spatial tendencies of local herbivores may change in response to

local algal outbreaks and to determine how reliant areas of reef are on the movement of

fishes from afar.

The aim of this study, therefore, is to evaluate changes in the spatial tendencies of

coral reef herbivores when exposed to an acute, large-scale simulated algal outbreak.

Specifically, we aim to determine if, and to what extent, the response of key fish taxa to

algal outbreaks is a localized one (i.e., the response of herbivorous taxa will be limited

to those taxa whose home range encompasses the outbreak) or a broader, community

level response (i.e., the response occurs over the entire reef and key species move in to

prey on macroalgae).

98

6.2 Materials and Methods

Study locations

The study was conducted between April and November 2013, on reefs surrounding

Lizard Island, a mid-shelf reef of the GBR (14o40’S 145o28’E). Two locations were

selected to conduct the experiment, Mermaid Cove and Turtle Beach (Appendix D).

Both locations are similar, on the leeward side of Lizard Island, with a distinct reef flat,

crest, slope and base on sand at 6-8 m. Data for video analysis occurred exclusively on

the reef crest (1-3 m) while algal deployment extended from the reef flat (0 m) to the

reef slope (5 m) at both locations (Fig. 6.1). At both study locations, macroalgae

naturally occurs in very low biomass (Wismer et al. 2009) and therefore, the algae had

to be collected from off-site.

Fig. 6.1 Visual representation of the simulated macroalgal outbreak depicting initial

macroalgal deployment density with acoustic receiver placement.

Algal treatment area

Receiver0 m

8 m

99

Simulated degradation

A degraded macroalgal-dominated reef was simulated by transferring Sargassum

sp. (cf. S. swartzii) from the Turtle Island group, an inshore group of reefs 27 km

southwest of Lizard Island (14°43’S, 145°12’E) to Lizard Island. Sargassum was

chosen for the simulation as it has been shown to be the dominant successional

macroalgal genus on the GBR (Hughes et al. 2007). Sargassum outbreaks have also

been reported from the Indian Ocean (Graham et al. 2006). Sargassum thalli of

relatively uniform height (~50 cm) were removed from the benthos by gently prying the

holdfast from the substratum. The algae were then transported to the Lizard Island

Research Station (LIRS) where they were held in flow through tanks before being

deployed. Algae were never held for more than a week and no algae were deployed

which showed signs of degradation. Prior to being deployed, algal thalli were spun,

weighted and attached together using twist ties to ensure that each deployed unit

weighted approximately 0.5 kg. To fix the algae to the reef, 6 m long chains were

placed in a grid configuration within a 50 m2 treatment area (algal plot), two days prior

to algal-treatment video data collection. Following the pre-deployment recording period

(see below for details) algae were attached to the chains using cable ties, which were

attached to the holdfast of the thallus.

Between October and November 2013, algae were fixed to the reef in a treatment

site within each location, measuring approximately 50 m2 (Fig. 6.1). The algal plot

extended 5 m along the reef and 10 m down the reef gradient, encompassing the reef

flat, crest and base. Initially, 200 thalli were deployed haphazardly within the algal plot

at each treatment site, resulting in an initial density of 4 thalli m-2 (approximately 2 kg

m-2). However, supplemental algae were added to each site every second day to

maintain densities of between 150-220 thalli per plot (density range; 3-4.4 thalli per m2;

100

1.5-2.2 kg m-2). At even the lowest algal density, sufficient Sargassum was present to

ensure that the macroalgal composition of the benthos was numerically dominant to

coral colony abundance. Algal plots were maintained on the reef for 14 days before

being removed.

Community response

To quantify the effect of an algal outbreak on the herbivore community, fishes were

monitored using cameras and acoustic telemetry. For video recordings, four monitoring

sites were chosen at each location. The monitoring sites roughly corresponded the

placement of VR2W acoustic receivers (Vemco, Halifax) moorings deployed along the

reef crest (details below). Cameras were placed to monitor a small-scale and large-scale

response. The small-scale response was assessed with two recording sites, one within

the algal treatment site, and the other (used to quantify changes in the herbivore

community in the immediate vicinity) was just outside the plot, 40 m away. Larger-

scale effects on the herbivore community were assessed using two supplementary

monitoring sites, roughly situated at acoustic receiver moorings 80 m along the reef on

either side of the central monitoring sites (Appendix D).

In total, 30 days of videos were captured over 34 days. Monitoring was divided

into three periods: ten pre-algal treatment days in October prior to algal deployment, ten

days when the ‘algal-treatment’ was present and finally, ten post-disturbance days

following algal removal. Video monitoring was suspended for two days prior to the

algal treatment and two days following the algal treatment to allow the community to

acclimate to the placement and removal (respectively) of the chains used to fix the

algae to the reef. Within each of these monitoring periods, five days were randomly

chosen for analysis. Video recordings were made using GoPro cameras, which were

haphazardly deployed onto the reef crest within 10 m of each monitoring site (4

101

cameras per location), between 11:00 h and 16:00 h. After each camera had been

deployed, a 1 m2 quadrat with markings at 5 cm intervals on all four sides was placed

50 cm from the lens of the camera for 30 s. This ensured that the video sampling area

was standardised and that size of fish that entered the sampling area could be estimated.

Each camera was then left for a minimum of 3.5 h. Videos were examined on a

computer with the sampling area marked on a plastic overlay on the screen. On the

overlay, the 5 cm increments placed on the sampling quadrate were also marked down

to aid in the accurate size estimation of the fish observed. Observer size estimations

were validated by holding a model of a size unknown to the observer within the

quadrate area. This was replicated 30 times. The estimated size of the model was then

compared to its actual size. The absolute discrepancy between actual size and observer

estimations was small (4.5 ± 0.49 cm; mean ± SE) and the relationship between

estimated and actual size of the model yielded an r2 value of 0.92.

In each video the identity, size and number of herbivorous fish species passing

through the sampling area in the videos were recorded for the second 15 min period in

each hour of recording, resulting in a total of 45 min per replicate. Fish abundance is

based on the number of individuals recorded over the 45 min period. As individuals

could not be reliably identified, the total number of appearances in the video is recorded

as an indication of herbivore presence.

Response of residents

The response of focal individuals to the localized algal outbreak was also quantified

using passive acoustic telemetry. In April 2013, two arrays of 10 VR2W acoustic

receivers were constructed at each location (total of 20 receivers). Prior to receiver

placement, extensive range testing was conducted at both study locations and the

102

working detection range (range at which 50% of known signal transmissions from a

transmitter are detected; Welsh et al. 2012a) was found to be 40 m for the V7-4L

acoustic transmitters (Vemco, Halifax) used in this study. Therefore, receivers were

placed at increments of 40 m along the reef crest at each location. At each location,

most of the reef crest was thus incorporated into the detection range of at least one

receiver. Receivers were moored following the shallow water moorings described in

Welsh et al. (2012) and care was taken to ensure that the hydrophone of each VR2W

unit was well above the algal canopy.

Once the array was in place, representative herbivore fish taxa were captured and

tagged between April and September 2012. Prior to tagging, visual censuses at each

location were used to estimate the average abundance of major herbivore species per

location. Visual censuses were conducted 5 times over five-months, prior to the study,

to ensure patterns of fish abundance were stable through time. At each location, a fish

census consisted of nine 2 x 20 m transects along the reef crest. The start-point of

transects were haphazardly chosen within 20 m of each receiver mooring. Based on

these data, representative individuals from herbivore functional groups that were likely

to respond to algal presence (browsers, scrapers and croppers) were selected for

tagging; these include Naso unicornis (browser), Scarus schlegeli (scraper), Siganus

vulpinus and S. corallinus (croppers) (Cheal et al. 2012).

Fish for acoustic tagging were captured using barrier nets. Once captured, fish

where transported to LIRS where they were held in flow-though tanks prior to surgery.

To implant transmitters, fish were anaesthetized in 70 L bath containing an MS-222

seawater solution (0.13 gL-1). Fish were considered anaesthetised when their righting

reflex failed and gilling rate became reduced, which took approximately 5 mins in the

anesthetic bath. The surgery was preformed in a relatively sterile environment, and the

fish were placed on a moist foam block, out of water, for the procedure. A small, 2 cm

103

incision was then made mid-way between the upper margin of the pectoral fin insertion

and the anus and a V7-4L acoustic transmitter inserted into the peritoneal cavity, below

the swim bladder. The wound was closed with two dissolvable BIOSYN 3/0 sutures,

tied using a Surgeon’s Knot. The surgical procedure took on average 180 (± 40)

seconds to complete once the fish had been removed from the anesthetic bath. Fish

were released following a 12-24 h recovery period to the capture location. Based on

visual observations of the tagged individuals, fish were fully recovered, showing no

signs of the incision, one week after being released.

Data analysis

Herbivore community response

The taxa observed in video data recordings were classified into functional groups

(grazers, scrapers, excavators and browsers; see Appendix D for species classification),

to examine the response of functional groups to the presence of macroalgae.

The abundance data for functional groups were compared using a three-way

MANOVA, in which Site, Location and Treatment were used as independent fixed

factors and Location was nested within Site. For this analysis, the numbers of

individuals from each functional group were used as dependant variables. Within the

MANOVA analysis, one-way ANOVA analyses were used to test the between-subject

effects, identifying which functional groups differed significantly between sites,

locations and algal treatments. Least significant differences analyses (LSD; based on

mean comparisons; t-test) were then used to detect homogeneous subsets of functional

group abundance in the independent factors. To satisfy the assumptions of the

MANOVA, abundance data were square-root transformed. Assessments of the square-

104

root transformed data revealed that the MANOVA was a suitable analysis based on a

non-significant Box’s test for equality of covariance matrices and a non-significant

Leven’s test for homogeneity. Residual plots were also inspected to verify the

suitability of the test.

To investigate species-level response of coral reef herbivores to macroalgae, non-

metric multidimensional scaling analyses (nMDS) were examined using video-based

based abundance data for each herbivore species (all herbivore species and non-

browsing species alone). In both cases, the significance of groupings identified in the

nMDS were analysed using a one-way analysis of similarity (ANOSIM) preformed on

an average distance matrix. Prior to nMDS and ANOSIM analyses, data were square-

root transformed to improve normality in the data set and reduce the influence of highly

abundant species on the analyses.

Resident response

To assess the response of tagged fish to the macroalgal disturbance, the detection data

from 14 days prior to, during and following the algal deployment were used (excluding

the two days during which algae was being deployed). For each fish an individual’s

core receiver (with the majority of an individual’s detections during the 14 days prior

the algal deployment) was identified. The change in occupancy at the core receiver was

calculated for each tagged individual by subtracting the average number of detection

per day prior to algal deployment from the average number of daily detections at the

same receiver while the algae were in place. The resulting delta values were then

compared to 0 (representing no change in occupancy) using four one-sample t-test (one

separate analysis per species). Furthermore, the activity of tagged fish within the

detection range of the receivers in the centre of each simulated algal outbreak

105

(represented by number of detections per day) was calculated for each of the three

temporal periods and compared using two RMANOVAs. Where necessary, the alpha

values for the statistical tests were adjusted using Bonferroni correction (please see

supplemental materials).

6.3 Results

Herbivore community response

Video footage revealed no significant difference in the abundance of individuals in the

four herbivore functional groups between the Mermaid Cove and Turtle Beach (Pillai’s

trace4,99 = 1.99, P > 0.05) nor was there an interaction effect between location and

treatment (Pillai’s trace8,200 = 0.71, P > 0.05). During the macroalgal treatment, the

mean abundance (± SE) of herbivorous fishes at the treatment site increased from 38.8

± 3.8 to 103.5 ± 14.3 individuals.day-1, a near 3 fold increase (Fig. 6.2; Appendix D).

Following the removal of the macroalgae, the abundance of fish at the treatment

locations decreased to 43.1 ± 1.5 individuals.day-1, similar to initial abundance

estimates (Fig. 6.2). These patterns were not seen in any other monitoring location

outside the algal treatment area, with fish abundances remaining relatively constant

throughout the study (Fig. 6.2). The pronounced effect of algae on the abundance of

herbivores was supported by the MANOVA, which revealed a significant effect of algal

treatment (before, during and after; Pillai’s trace8,200 = 10.54, P < 0.001), site (inside

and outside treatment areas; Pillai’s trace12,303 = 3.02, P < 0.01) and an interaction

effect (Pillai’s trace24,408 = 4.41, P < 0.001) (Appendix D), as sites were only

significantly different from each other when algae were present (Appendix D).

However, the effect was not even across the four functional groups. Univariate

ANOVAs indicated that, of the four herbivore functional groups analysed, only browser

106

densities changed significantly with algal treatments (F2,120 = 53.97, P < 0.001) and the

interaction effect (F6,120 = 25.37, P < 0.001) (i.e., as increases in browser abundance

only occurred at sites where and when algae were present)( Appendix D).

Algal removal during the simulated algal outbreak was significant. During the

period over which the algae were deployed, approximately 50 new thalli were added to

each site every second day to replace lost algal thalli. When removed, the holdfast of

the algal thalli were still present, and attached to the benthos, however, the standing

stipe and blades were all but removed. This suggested that thalli were not being lost as a

result of dislodgement but instead were being fed upon by herbivores in the

environment.

107

Fig. 6.2 The average abundance of a) total herbivore community and b) browsing

herbivore community before (white), during (black) and after (grey) a simulated phase

shift to macroalgae.

The size structure of the browser population also changed dramatically when

algae were placed on the reef. After deployment, there was more than a 10-fold increase

in the number of large N. unicornis, with an average of 33.2 ± 4.0 individuals recorded

per day over 30 cm when algae were present (Fig. 6.3). This pattern was even more

pronounced for K. vaigiensis and S. doliatus, in which the total number of individuals

increased in abundance at algal treatment sites by 4,900% (to 24.8 ± 3.1 individuals

recorded per treatment day) and 5,600% (to 26.0 ± 9.7 individuals recorded per

treatment day), respectively (Fig. 6.3). A smaller, yet still notable increase in abundance

by 470% was also found for K. cinerascens at sites of algal deployment (Fig. 6.3).

Brow

ser a

bund

ance

(mea

n ±

SE)

(a)

0

20

40

60

80

100

120

0

20

40

60

80

100

120(b)

0m 20m 80mDistance from phase shift

Her

bivo

re a

bund

ance

(mea

n ±

SE)

108

Fig. 6.3 Size frequency distribution of key browsing taxa; Kyphosus vaigiensis, Naso

unicornis, K. cinerascens and Siganus doliatus before (white), during (black) and

following (grey) a simulated phase shift to macroalgae.

K. vaigiensis

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

18

S. doliatus

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

18K. cinerascens

<10 10-20 20-30 >300

1

2

3

4

5

N. unicornis

Size class (cm)

Ave

rage

num

ber o

f ind

ivid

uals

(± S

E)

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

40

45

<10 10-20 20-30 >30 <10 10-20 20-30 >30

<10 10-20 20-30 >30

Pre algal deploymentAlgal deploymentPost algal deployment

109

The results from the species-level MDS analysis support the above observations

with a strong separation of sites during algal deployment (Fig. 6.4). ANOSIM detects a

significant difference between samples in which algae were present and all others

(global R = 0.937, significance level 0.1%; Fig. 6.4). As expected, the separation of

these sites was largely driven by the presence of browsing species, namely Naso

unicornis, Kyphosus vaigiensis, Siganus doliatus, and to a lesser extent, K. cinerascens

(Fig. 6.4). When all browsers were removed from the data set, no significant groupings

were detected (global R = -0.125, significance level 89.2%)(Appendix D).

Resident response

A total of 34 fish tagged with acoustic transmitters were monitored for the duration of

the study. The number of each species tagged (S. vulpinus n = 6, S. corallinus n = 6,

Scarus schlegeli n = 17 and N. unicornis n = 3) corresponded to the relative abundance

of the taxa at each site and represented at least 40% of the estimated local population

size (based on counts made during fish censuses; Appendix D). Despite the marked

changes in herbivore detections observed by videos, of the tagged fishes, none

significantly changed their patterns of occupancy during algal deployment, regardless

of whether their core area of detection encompassed the simulated algal outbreak or not

(Fig. 6.5; Appendix D).

110

Fig. 6.4 Non-metric multidimensional scaling (nMDS) analysis showing the

relationship between 8 sites at three different treatment levels (pre, during and post

algal treatment) based on the abundance of herbivorous fish taxa. During algal

deployment, sites are designated as either control or treatment. Ellipses represent

significant groupings identified by ANOSIM. Vector lengths indicate the relative

contribution of each species to the observed pattern. Grey triangles ( ) represent control

sites while algae were present, inverted black triangles ( ) represent treatment sites

while algae were present, unfilled triangles ( ) represent all sites prior to algal

treatment and black triangles ( ) represent all sites post-algal treatment. 2D stress: 0.18.

K. cinerascensK. vaigiensis

N. unicornis

S. doliatus

Z. scopas

A. nigrofuscus

111

Fig. 6.5 Change in individual detections rates (# detections.day-1 during algal

deployment - detections.day-1 prior to deployment) for all Signaus vulpinus (n = 6), S.

corallinus (n = 6), Scarus schlegeli (n = 17) and Naso unicornis (n = 3). Dark grey

boxes represent fishes with core areas of detection was at the algal deployment site (n =

4, 6, 9 and 3 respectively). Box represents mean ± SE and whiskers represent minimum

and maximum values. None of the species’ change in detection rates following algal

deployment were significantly different from zero.

0-20-40 20 40

N. unicornis

Sc. schlegeli

S. corallinus

S. vulpinus

Decreased occupancy Increased occupancy

Δ detections at core receiver (mean ± SE)

112

6.4 Discussion

Following a simulated outbreak of macroalgae the herbivorous fish community

exhibited a rapid, positive, response. This activity was highly focused, with no other

areas on the same reef, even within 20 m, exhibiting similar increases in herbivore

abundance. The observed increase in fish abundance, however, did not arise from a

response in resident taxa. Instead, it occurred as large mobile non-resident herbivores

moved in to the site of algal deployment to feed on the available macroalgae. It appears

that large mobile browsing herbivores can alter their behaviour to occupy areas

exhibiting a localized algal outbreak. These observations suggest that coral reefs may

rely on the activities of mobile links, i.e. large, mobile, non-resident individuals, to

mitigate the effects of localized algal outbreaks on coral reefs.

A community response to algal outbreaks

The response of the fish community to the simulated macroalgal outbreak was

marked. On average, the fish abundance at the site of algal deployment increased by

267%. This response was surprising, as several studies have evaluated the effect of

macroalgal growth on coral reefs and have overwhelmingly found a decline in fish

abundance and/or diversity (McClanahan et al. 1999; Wilson et al. 2008; Chong-Seng

et al. 2012). The cause for these declines has largely been attributed to a decline in reef

complexity or suitable feeding surfaces following a phase-shift over long temporal

scales (Graham et al. 2006; Chong-Seng et al. 2012), or the proliferation of undesirable

complexity provided by macro-algae (Hoey and Bellwood 2011). However, we found

that for localized, acute increases in algae where the reef structure remains intact, the

overall herbivore community shows no aversion to macroalgae; instead, large browsers

exhibit a strong attraction to its location.

113

There are two possible explanations for these differences among studies

evaluating acute algal deployments on coral reefs: 1) they may be a result of differences

in algal identity, density, quality and/or extent, or 2) variation among studies in the

composition of the herbivore assemblages. Previous work that has shown an aversion to

large stands of macroalgae (Bellwood et al. 2006) may in part be due to algal densities

that were higher than those in the present study. This suggests that there is a threshold

above which algal densities elicit a negative herbivore response and algal outbreak

removal becomes difficult (Bellwood et al. 2006; Hoey and Bellwood 2011).

Alternatively algae may differ in palatability where larger, older thalli are less desirable

to herbivores (Bellwood et al. 2006; Hoey 2010; Lefèvre and Bellwood 2010), reducing

the likelihood of herbivores responding to algal presence on reefs. Fish assemblage

structure may also be important. The present study is the first on a mid-shelf reef, where

the higher fish diversity provides a greater potential a range of species to respond to

algal deployments. Furthermore, macroalgae are rare on mid-shelf reefs relative to

inner-shelf reefs (Wismer et al. 2009). Therefore, browsing herbivores may be more

willing to feed on high density macroalgae on mid-shelf reefs, as the benefits of

accessing the rare resource outweigh the elevated predation risks that may be associated

with dense algal fields (Hoey and Bellwood 2011).

The observed increase in total fish abundance was driven almost entirely by large

macroalgal browsers. When algae were present we recorded abundances of the

browsing species, N. unicornis, up to 10 times higher than in either pre- or post-

monitoring periods and a similar, yet less marked increase in the abundance of K.

vaigiensis was recorded. These species are know to feed primarily on macroalgae

(Choat et al. 2002) and have been found to be uniquely capable of removing large

quantities of macroalgae on the great barrier reef (Cvitanovic and Bellwood 2009; Hoey

and Bellwood 2009) and elsewhere in Australia (Vergés et al. 2011). This demonstrated

114

a localized accumulation of functionally important taxa with the ability to reverse local

algal outbreaks. Studies from the Seychelles (Chong-Seng et al. 2012) and the Red Sea

(Khalil et al. 2013) found a similar trend, in which degraded reefs with algal

proliferation supported a higher abundance of browsing herbivorous taxa, as individuals

presumably move in to feed on outbreaks of algae. While movement of taxa for the

purposes of resource exploitation is not a rare phenomenon in terrestrial or pelagic

ecosystems (e.g. Polovina 1996; Pomilla and Rosenbaum 2005; McKinney et al. 2012),

where large-scale movements are common, movements of this kind for the purpose of

resource exploitation have not yet been recorded coral reefs. It appears that mobile fish

taxa are capable of concentrating their residency and foraging activities at the site of

macroalgal outbreaks in the same way terrestrial taxa change their behaviour to exploit

specific resources. However, this raises the question of where did the individuals

responsible for the increase in browser abundance come from?

The origin of responders

One of the most remarkable observations was that the increase in browsers was

not a result of the movement of local residents. Of the three resident N. unicornis and

29 other herbivores tagged, none moved. The lack of response from the resident

community is also supported by the distinct shift in the size-frequency distribution of

the browsing taxa. We recorded a clear peak in the number of large individuals (> 30

cm), which were, in the case of N. unicornis, not recorded on the reef prior to the algal

deployment. A similar pattern was found for K. vaigiensis, K. cinerascens and S.

doliatus in which the number of large individuals was greater than the combined daily

counts for these taxa across all sites prior to algae deployment. Indeed, K. vaigiensis

was only recorded twice on the reef prior to the algal deployment in data from both

115

video recordings and visual census. The dramatic increase in browser abundance, and

subsequent decline following algal removal, was recorded at both sites and arose from

an influx of non-resident individuals that exhibited home ranges of a sufficient

magnitude to allow them to encounter the algae.

Large-scale movements have been documented previously for K. vaigiensis on

inshore reefs. Welsh and Bellwood (2014) recorded movements in K. vaigiensis that

were unprecedented for a coral reef herbivore and speculated that these movements

may facilitate the locating of food sources with a patchy distribution. This study

supports this hypothesis and demonstrates the ability of these highly vagile taxa to

change their movements in response to food availability. In contrast, the lack of

movement in the smaller local fish is consistent with number recent studies, which

report limited home ranges (Eristhee and Oxenford 2010; Meyer and Holland 2005;

Fox and Bellwood 2011; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). This site-attached behaviour in

reef fish highlights the importance of those species that do exhibit a spatial response.

It appears that upon prey detection, large browsing herbivores can quickly alter

their spatial range to exploit areas of high food availability. Previous research has

suggested that N. unicornis is a highly site attached species with a restricted home range

(Meyer and Holland 2005; Marshell et al. 2011). However, studies on this species’

movements are often restricted to small individuals although ontogenetic home range

expansions have been documented (Marshell et al. 2011). The present observations are,

again, consistent with these previous studies where smaller, resident N. unicornis do not

move far, but larger individuals exhibit far greater mobility.

Once the macroalgae were removed, the abundance of browsing taxa returned to

abundances almost indistinguishable from those before the simulated phase shift. This

suggests that the browsers did not shift their home range to a new location in favour of

a new resource but instead, temporarily changed their core-areas of utilization within a

116

much larger spatial range. This is significant, as it would suggest that the plasticity of

movements within an individual’s home range might be key to preventing algal

establishment and proliferation on coral reefs over large spatial scales, and may

supplement the small-scale functional processes on reefs conferred by resident taxa.

The importance of mobile links on coral reefs

The response of the mobile browsing herbivore community to the presence of

macroalgae may be an important line of defence against reef phase- or regime shifts. In

this study, I present evidence that the large-scale movements of roving browsing

herbivores may be significant in preventing long-term macroalgal outbreaks on coral

reefs. Resident taxa alone may be unable to control algal outbreaks. It has been

suggested that the growth rate of Sargassum may be too great for the algae to be

removed entirely by resident taxa, and once established, fish may be unwilling to prey

on the macroalgae (Hoey and Bellwood 2011). Moreover, even on a small scale, the

site-attached nature of numerous reef taxa is such that, even if an outbreak occurs on

their reef, they may never encounter it (Nash et al. 2013). Large, mobile herbivores may

act as key mobile links, safeguarding against algal outbreaks, providing a broad-scale

application of functional processes, with sufficient plasticity in their movements to

focus their ecosystem services where needed.

This ecosystem response, however, is reliant on a healthy herbivore community,

with large browsing individuals remaining in the system. In particular it is not just

species or adult individuals that are needed but large, mobile, individuals. This is

especially concerning given that these large, mobile taxa are highly coveted by

fishermen and among the first to be removed from a system when fishing intensity

increases (Hoey and Bellwood 2009; Moffitt et al. 2009; Nash et al. 2013; Bejarano et

al. 2013). Furthermore, most MPAs are small, especially in tropical developing nations,

117

where reef fish are highly targeted as a primary food source (Wood et al. 2008).

Therefore, by virtue of their mobility, these highly important individuals and species

are more likely to diffuse out of MPA boundaries and be removed first from the system.

Fish that act as mobile links, therefore, appear to be some of the most important yet

vulnerable organisms on coral reefs.

118

Chapter 7: Concluding Discussion

The movements of animals are of particular importance for our understanding of

ecosystems, as the spatial ecology of organisms delineates their functional impact (Fox

and Bellwood 2011; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a). Thus, there has been a recent

emphasis on the study of spatial aspects of a species’ biology. Although this field is still

in its infancy, the last decade has seen a rapid expansion in the number of studies that

quantify animal movements, specifically those of marine taxa (Welsh and Bellwood

2014; Nash et al. in review). The majority of studies into the spatial utilization patterns

of marine taxa, especially taxa on coral reefs, have focused on quantifying the

effectiveness of MPAs and the survivorship of individuals (Meyer et al. 2010; Claisse

et al. 2011; Maypa et al. 2012; Calò et al. 2013). This thesis represents a contrasting

view, to explore the ecological implications of movement, and to examine the functions

of coral reef fishes in a spatial context.

Acoustic telemetry; an evolving frontier

Methods for quantifying the movements of marine taxa have traditionally lagged

behind those of terrestrial species (Lacroix and Voegeli 2000; Simpfendorfer et al.

2008; Mathies et al. 2014). This is largely due to the logistical difficulties of tracking

movements in the marine environment (Heupel et al. 2006, 2008; Welsh et al. 2012). In

the past two decades, however, the refinement of acoustic technology has facilitated a

rapid expansion in the number, kind and quality of spatial studies in the marine

environment (Kerwath et al. 2005; Holland et al. 2009; Biesinger et al. 2013). Yet,

movement studies on adult coral reef organisms have received comparably less

119

attention than those inhabiting temperate or pelagic systems. This is, yet again,

attributable to the difficulties of using acoustic telemetry on structurally complex reefs

(Biesinger et al. 2013; Mathies et al. 2014). Given the threats facing coral reefs and the

need for new questions to be answered regarding their functioning and how best

conserve them, a growing body of empirical studies and new analysis programs are

overcoming the difficulties of utilizing acoustic telemetry on reefs (Cagua et al. 2013;

Mathies et al. 2014). Chapter 2 of this thesis is one such study and provided among the

first empirical data and framework to facilitate the expansion of acoustic studies on

coral reefs. These have now opened up this environment to be studied in greater detail

than was previously possible, and there are now fewer logistical constraints on

ecological studies involving animal movements on coral reefs. With a growing

understanding of the complications associated with the use of telemetry in coral reef

environments, such as those outlined in chapter 2, future studies may account for the

unique complexities and ultimately, conduct more detailed evaluations of reef fish

movements.

Spatial ecology

In conjunction with advancements in spatial data analyses, available data on the

movements of marine taxa has dramatically expanded. Acoustics telemetry has leaded

this expansion, with refinements to the technology increasing the number of species that

can be studied using internal transmitter implantation and subsequent tracking (Fig.

1.1). As a result, there is now a growing body of robust, empirical studies that present

data on the range size of coral reef fishes (e.g. Meyer and Holland 2005; Hutchinson

and Rhodes 2010; Fox and Bellwood 2011, Welsh and Bellwood 2012a, b; Welsh and

Bellwood 2014).

120

Overexploitation is among the greatest threats to reef fish populations, and

indeed, their declines have been recorded from around the globe (Wood et al. 2008). As

a result, conservation initiatives are being informed by numerous studies, which analyse

movement patterns of reef fishes (Bennett et al. 2012). These data are being used with

the goal of quantifying the effectiveness of MPAs, to conserve marine populations.

Separately, the interaction between marine taxa and their environment are among the

topics in the forefront of ecological research, as ecosystem degradation is another factor

that continues to threaten coral reefs (Bellwood et al. 2006; Hughes et al. 2007). Thus,

several studies have endeavoured to quantify the impact of key taxa on their

environment. By combining these types of research, and incorporating movement

studies into the evaluation of ecological processes, it is possible to better understand the

ecology of coral reef ecosystems and gain spatially explicit view of coral reef functional

processes. This thesis stands as an example of this. In using acoustic telemetry, spatial

data and ecological data in concert, key questions ecological questions that have never

before been able to be addressed have been answered. This has broadened our

understanding of the underlying variability in functional processes, and highlighted the

importance of considering the spatial resilience of ecosystems. With this knowledge,

future studies may consider the incorporation of key species’ spatial ecology as a key

element in determining taxa’s importance for ecological process.

Among the challenges associated with quantifying movements in the marine

environment is to understand which factors shape and/or limit the home range of an

individual. At present, limitations to an individual’s home range size have largely been

attributed to both physical barriers, such as habitat discontinuity (e.g. Bakker and van

Vuren 2004; Afonso et al. 2009; Downie et al. 2013; José et al. 2013) and unfavourable

substratum (Hoey and Bellwood 2011), as well as social restrictions, i.e. the boundaries

of a social group’s territory (Bruggemann et al. 1994b; Mumby and Wabnitz 2002;

121

Welsh and Bellwood 2012a; Welsh et al. 2013). Despite a growing body of research on

the topic, no consensus has yet to produced and indeed, the mechanisms may vary

heavily between taxa, or even within taxa depending on ontogenetic development.

Despite this, the evidence presented herein strongly suggests that factors limiting the

movements of fishes play an important role in the spatial applications of functional

processes. Further research on the topic may provide invaluable information for our

understanding of the variability associated with quantifying the rate of ecosystem

functions, as well the effectiveness of marine reserves.

Regardless of the factors that limit home range boundaries in fishes, it is a

sobering notion that the home range areas of functionally important fishes are often

limited to a small area within a reef (Chapter 5; Welsh and Bellwood 2014). However,

the exact scale of movement associated with ‘roving’ behaviour appears to be

ambiguous. Indeed, evidence of roving behaviour in reef fishes has been limited, with

largely invariable reports of site-attached home ranging behaviour for herbivorous

species (e.g. Meyer and Holland 2005; Fox and Bellwood 2011; Marshell et al. 2011).

Concluding remarks

Previous evaluations of the ecosystem processes that occur on coral reefs, and other

ecosystems, have been largely processes-centric (e.g. Fox and Bellwood 2007;

Burkepile and Hay 2010; Vergés et al. 2011). As a result, the rates at which ecosystem

processes are applied are well documented and a detailed understanding of the

processes themselves has been established. However, estimates of the rate at which a

functional process is applied are often subject to a great deal of variability, in both

space and time (e.g. Bennett and Bellwood 2011; Lefèvre and Bellwood 2011). By

incorporating an understanding of the movements and spatial habits of the taxa that

122

drive ecological processes, it may be possible to better understand dynamics in these

processes. In doing this, we can theoretically track seasonal shifts in home range

patterns of a key herbivorous species to predict where algae will be removed from a

reef, or seeds may be dispersed through an ecosystem by a bird or bat. Moreover,

utilization frequency data for a species in a given area can be used to predict the

intensity at which a process may occur in that environment (Welsh and Bellwood

2012a). Put simply, it is perhaps by understanding the spatial biology of some of the

smallest units of functional processes, the individuals that drive them, that we may gain

a broader understanding of processes occurring in today’s ecosystems.

123

References

Afonso P, Fontes J, Holland KN, Santos RS (2008) Social status determines behaviour

and habitat usage in a temperate parrotfish: implications for marine reserve

design. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 359:215-227

Afonso P, Fontes J, Holland KN, Santos RS (2009) Multi-scale patterns of habitat use

in a highly mobile reef fish, the white trevally Pseudocaranx dentex, and their

implications for marine reserve design. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 381:273-286

Alevizon WS (1976) Mixed schooling and its possible significance in a tropical western

Atlantic parrotfish and surgeonfish. Copeia 1976:796-798

Allen GR, Steene RC, Humann P, Deloach N (2003) Reef fish identification - tropical

pacific. New World Publications, Jacksonville, FL

Almany GR, Webster MS (2006) The predation gauntlet: early post-settlement

mortality in reef fishes. Coral Reefs 25:19-22

Almany GR, Connolly SR, Heath DD, Hogan JD, Jones GP, McCook LJ, Mills M,

Pressey RL, Williamson DH (2009) Connectivity, biodiversity conservation and

the design of marine reserve networks for coral reefs. Coral Reefs 28:339-351

Alwany MA, Thaler E, Stachowitsch M (2009) Parrotfish bioerosion on Egyptian Red

Sea reefs. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 371:170-176

Au WW, Banks K (1998) The acoustics of the snapping shrimp Synalpheus

parneomeris in Kaneohe Bay. J Acoust Soc Am 103:41-47

Avgar T, Mosser A, Brown GS, Fryxell JM (2013) Environmental and individual

drivers of animal movement patterns across a wide geographical gradient. J

Anim Ecol 82:96-106

Bakker VJ, van Vuren DH (2004) Gap-crossing decisions by the Red Squirrel, a forest-

dependent small mammal. Conserv Biol 18:689-697

124

Bardyshev VI (2007) Underwater ambient noise in shallow-water areas of the Indian

Ocean within the tropical zone. Acoust Phys 53:167-171

Bates, D.M. & Maechler, M. (2009) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4

classes.

Bellwood DR (1988) Ontogenetic changes in the diet of early post-settlement Scarus

species (Pisces: Scaridae). J Fish Biol 33:213-219

Bellwood DR, Choat JH (1989) A description of the juvenile phase colour patterns of

24 parrotfish species (family Scaridae) from the Great Barrier Reef, Australia.

Aust Mus 41:1-41

Bellwood DR, Choat JH (1990) A functional analysis of grazing in parrotfishes (family

Scaridae): the ecological implications. Environ Biol Fish 28:189-214

Bellwood DR, Hoey AS, Choat JH (2003) Limited functional redundancy in high

diversity systems: resilience and ecosystem function on coral. Ecol Lett 6:281-

285

Bellwood DR, Hughes TP, Hoey AS (2006) Sleeping functional group drives coral-reef

recovery. Curr Biol 16:2434-2439

Bellwood DR, Hoey AS, Hughes TP (2012) Human activity selectively impacts the

ecosystem roles of parrotfishes on coral reefs. Proc R Soc B 279:1621-1629

Bellwood DR, Hughes TP, Folke C, Nyström M (2004) Confronting the coral reef

crisis. Nature 429:827-833

Bejarano S, Golbuu Y, Sapolu T, Mumby PJ (2013) Ecological risk and the exploitation

of herbivorous reef fish across Micronesia. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 482:197-215

Bennett S, Bellwood DR (2011) Latitudinal variation in macroalgal consumption by

fishes on the Great Barrier Reef. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 426:241-252

Bennett RH, Cowley PD, Childs A-R, Whitfield AK (2012) Area-use patterns and diel

movements of white steenbras Lithognathus lithognathus in a temporarily

125

open/closed South African estuary, inferred from acoustic telemetry and long-

term seine-netting data. Afr J Mar Sci 34:81-91

Berger J (2004) The last mile: How to sustain long-distance migration in mammals.

Conserv Biol 18:320-331

Biesinger Z, Bolker BM, Marcinek D, Grothues TM, Dobarro JA, Lindberg WJ (2013)

Testing an autonomous acoustic telemetry positioning system for fine-scale

space use in marine animals. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 448:46-56

Bolden SK (2001) Using ultrasonic telemetry to determine home range of a coral-reef

fish. In: Sibert JR, Nielsen JL (eds) Electronic tagging and tracking in marine

fisheries. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Netherlands,

Bonaldo RM, Bellwood DR (2008) Size-dependent variation in the functional role of

the parrotfish Scarus rivulatus on the Great Barrier Reef, Australia. Mar Ecol

Prog Ser 360:237-244

Bonaldo RM, Krajewski JP (2008) Differences in interspecific associations of initial

and terminal phase parrotfish in north-eastern Brazil. Mar Biodivers Rec 1:e87

Bonaldo RM, Bellwood DR (2010) Spatial variation in the effects of grazing on

epilithic algal turfs on the Great Barrier Reef, Australia. Coral Reefs 30:381-390

Bonaldo RM, Welsh JQ, Bellwood DR (2012) Spatial and temporal variation in coral

predation by parrotfishes on the GBR: evidence from an inshore reef. Coral

Reefs 31:263-272

Bonaldo RM, Krajewski JP, Sazima C, Sazima I (2006) Foraging activity and resource

use by three parrotfish species at Fernando de Noronha Archipelago, tropical

West Atlantic. Mar Biol 149:423-433

Booth DJ, Beretta GA (2004) Influence of recruit condition on food competition and

predation risk in a coral reef fish. Oecologia 140:289-294

126

Börger L, Dalziel BD, Fryxell JM (2008) Are there general mechanisms of animal

home range behaviour? A review and prospects for future research. Ecol Lett

11:637-650

Brandl SJ, Bellwood DR (2013) Pair formation in the herbivorous rabbitfish Siganus

doliatus. J Fish Biol 82:2031-2044

Broomhall LS, Mills MGL, du Toit JT (2003) Home range and habitat use by cheetahs

(Acinonyx jubatus) in the Kruger National Park. J Zool 261:119-128

Brown JL (1975) The evolution of behaviour. Norton, New York

Brown GP, Brooks RJ (1993) Sexual and seasonal differences in activity in a northern

population of snapping turtles, Chelydra serpentine. Herpetologica 49:311-318

Bruggemann HJ, Kuyper MWM, Breeman AM (1994a) Comparative analysis of

foraging and habitat use by the sympatric Caribbean parrotfish Scarus vetula

and Sparisoma viride. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 112:51-66

Bruggemann HJ, van Oppen MJH, Breeman AM (1994b) Foraging by the stoplight

parrotfish Sparisoma viride. I. Food selection in different, socially determined

habitats. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 106:41-55

Bruggemann JH, van Rooij MJ, Videler JJ, Breeman AM (1995) Dynamic and

limitations of herbivore populations on a Caribbean coral reef. Ph.D. thesis,

University of Groningen, pp 153-190

Bryars S, Rogers P, Huveneers C, Payne NL, Smith I, McDonald B (2012) Small home

range in southern Australia’s largest resident reef fish, the western blue groper

(Achoerodus gouldii): implications for adequacy of no-take marine protected

areas. Mar Freshw Res 63:552-563

Burgess SC (2006) Algal blooms on coral reefs with low anthropogenic impact in the

Great Barrier Reef. Coral Reefs 25:390-390

127

Burkepile DE, Hay ME (2008) Herbivore species richness and feeding complementarity

affect community structure and function on a coral reef. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S

A 105:16201-16206

Burkepile DE, Hay ME (2010) Impact of herbivore identity on algal succession and

coral growth on a Caribbean reef. PLoS ONE 5:e8963

Cagnacci F, Boitani L, Powell RA, Boyce MS (2010) Animal ecology meets GPS-

based radiotelemetry: a perfect storm of opportunities and challenges. Phil Trans

R Soc B 365:2157-2162

Cagua EF, Berumen ML, Tyler E (2013) Topography and biological noise determine

acoustic detectability on coral reefs. Coral reefs 32:1123-1134

Calenge C (2006) The package adehabitat for the R software: a tool for the analysis of

space and habitat use by animals. Ecol Model 197:516-519

Calò A, Félix-Hackradt FC, Garcia J, Hackradt CW, Rocklin D, Otón JT, Charton JAG

(2013) A review of methods to assess connectivity and dispersal between fish

populations in the Mediterranean Sea. Adv Oceanogr Limnol 4:150-175

Carbone C, Pettorelli N, Stephens PA (2011) The bigger they come, they harder they

fall: body size and prey abundance influence predator-prey ratios. Biol Lett

7:312-315

Carpenter R, Bennett EM, Peterson GD (2006) Scenarios for ecosystem services: an

overview. Ecol Soc 11:29-43

Cato DH (1978) Marine biological choruses observed in tropical waters near Australia.

J Acoust Soc Am 64:736-743

Ceccarelli DM, Jones GP, McCook LJ (2011) Interactions between herbivorous fish

guilds and their influence on algal succession on a coastal coral reef. J Exp Mar

Biol Ecol 399:60-67

128

Chapman MR, Kramer DL (2000) Movements of fishes within and among fringing

coral reefs in Barbados. Environ Biol Fish 57:11-24

Cheal A, Emslie M, Miller I, Sweatman H (2012) The distribution of herbivorous fishes

on the Great Barrier Reef. Mar Biol 159:1143-1154

Cheal AJ, Aaron MacNeil M, Cripps E, Emslie MJ, Jonker M, Schaffelke B, Sweatman

HPA (2010) Coral-macroalgal phase shifts or reef resilience: links with diversity

and functional roles of herbivorous fishes on the Great Barrier Reef. Coral Reefs

29:1005-1015

Chen LS (2002) Post-settlement diet shift of Chlorurus sordidus and Scarus schlegeli

(Pisces: Scaridae). Zool Stud 41:47-58

Choat J, Robertson D (1975) Protogynous hermaphroditism in fishes of the family

Scaridae Intersexuality in the animal kingdom. Springer, pp263-283

Choat JH, Bellwood DR (1985) Interactions amongst herbivorous fishes on a coral reef:

influence of spatial variation. Mar Biol 89:221-234

Choat JH, Clements KD, Robbins WD (2002) The trophic status of herbivorous fishes

on coral reefs. Mar Biol 140: 613-623.

Choat JH, Robbins WD, Clements KD (2004) The trophic status of herbivorous fishes

on coral reefs. Mar Biol 145:445-454

Chong-Seng KM, Mannering TD, Pratchett MS, Bellwood DR, Graham NAJ (2012)

The influence of coral reef benthic condition on associated fish assemblages.

PLoS ONE 7:e42167

Cinner JE, Graham NAJ, Hutchery C, MacNeil M (2012) Global effects of local human

population density and distance to markets on the condition of coral reef

fisheries. Conserv Biol 27:453-458

Claisse J, Clark T, Schumacher B, McTee S, Bushnell M, Callan C, Laidley C, Parrish J

(2011) Conventional tagging and acoustic telemetry of a small surgeonfish,

129

Zebrasoma flavescens, in a structurally complex coral reef environment.

Environ Biol Fish 91:185-201

Clements S, Jepsen D, Karnowski M (2005) Optimization of an acoustic telemetry

array for detecting transmitter-implanted fish. N Am J Fish Manag 25:429-436

Clifton KE (1989) Territory sharing by the Caribbean striped parrotfish, Scarus iserti:

patterns of resource abundance, group size and behaviour. Anim Behav 37:90-

103

Clutton-Brock TH, Hodge SJ, Flower TP (2008) Group size and the suppression of

subordinate reproduction in Kalahari meerkats. Anim Behav 76:689-700

Connell J (1997) Disturbance and recovery of coral assemblages. Coral Reefs 16:S101-

S113

Cooke SJ, Hinch SG, Wikelski M, Andrews RD, Kuchel LJ, Wolcott TG, Butler PJ

(2004) Biotelemetry: a mechanistic approach to ecology. Trends Ecol Evol

19:334-343

Couvreur M, Christiaen B, Verheyen K, Hermy M (2004) Large herbivores as mobile

links between isolated nature reserves through adhesive seed dispersal. Appl

Veg Sci 7:229-236

Cowman PF, Bellwood DR (2011) Coral reefs as cryptic extinction events, and the

development of biodiversity hotspots. J Evol Biol 24:2543-2562

Cox PA, Elmqvist T, Pierson ED, Rainey WE (1991) Flying foxes as strong interactors

in South Pacific island ecosystems: A conservation hypothesis. Conserv Biol

5:448-454

Cvitanovic C, Bellwood DR (2009) Local variation in herbivore feeding activity on an

inshore reef of the Great Barrier Reef. Coral Reefs 28:127-133

Depczynski M, Bellwood DR (2003) The role of cryptobenthic reef fishes in coral reef

trophodynamics. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 256:183-191

130

Depczynski M, Bellwood DR (2004) Microhabitat utilisation patterns in cryptobenthic

coral reef fish communities. Mar Biol 145:455-463

Depczynski M, Bellwood DR (2005) Shortest recorded vertebrate lifespan found in a

coral reef fish. Curr Biol 15:R288-R289

Downie RA, Babcock RC, Thomson DP, Vanderklift MA (2013) Density of

herbivorous fish and intensity of herbivory are influenced by proximity to coral

reefs. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 482:217-225

Dudgeon SR, Aronson RB, Bruno JF, Precht WF (2010) Phase shifts and stable states

on coral reefs. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 413:201-216

Duncan RS, Chapman CA (1999) Seed dispersal and potential forest succession in

abandoned agriculture in tropical Africa. Ecol Appl 9:998-1008

Egli DP, Babcock RC (2004) Ultrasonic tracking reveals multiple behavioural modes of

snapper (Pagrus auratus) in a temperate no-take marine reserve. ICES J Mar Sci

61:1137-1143

Elmqvist T, Maltby E. 2010. Biodiversity, ecosystems and ecosystem services. In:

Kumar P (ed) The economics of ecosystems and biodiversity: ecological and

economic foundations. Earthscan, Oxford, UK, pp 41–111.

Eloff PFC (1959) Observations on the migration and habits of the Antelopes of the

Kalahari Gemsbok Park - Part II

Eristhee N, Oxenford HA (2001) Home range size and use of space by Bermuda chub

Kyphosus sectatrix (L.) in two marine reserves in the Soufrière Marine

Management Area, St Lucia, West Indies. J Fish Biol 59:129-151

Evans MC (2008) Home range, burrow-use and activity patterns in common wombats

(Vombatus ursinus). Wildl Res 35:455-462

131

Ferreira CEL, Gonçalves JEA (2006) Community structure and diet of roving

herbivorous reef fishes in the Abrolhos Archipelago, southwestern Atlantic. J

Fish Biol 69:1533-1551

Fish MP (1964) Biological sources of sustained ambient sea noise. In: Tavolga WN

(ed) Marine bio-acoustics. Pergamon Press, New York, pp 175-194

Floeter SR, Ferreira CEL, Dominici-Arosemena A, Zalmon IR (2004) Latitudinal

gradients in Atlantic reef fish communities: trophic structure and spatial use

patterns. J Fish Biol 64:1680-1699

Folke C, Carpenter S, Walker B, Scheffer M, Elmqvist T, Gunderson L, Holling CS

(2004) Regime shifts, resilience, and biodiversity in ecosystem management.

Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics 35:557-581

Foster SA (1985) Group foraging by a coral reef fish: a mechanism for gaining access

to defended resources. Anim Behav 33:782-792

Fox RJ, Bellwood DR (2007) Quantifying herbivory across a coral reef depth gradient.

Mar Ecol Prog Ser 339:49-59

Fox RJ, Bellwood DR (2011) Unconstrained by the clock? Plasticity of diel activity

rhythm in a tropical reef fish, Siganus lineatus (Pisces: Siganidae). Funct Ecol

25:1096-1105

Fox RJ, Bellwood DR (2013) Niche partitioning of feeding microhabitats produces a

unique function for herbivorous rabbitfishes (Perciformes, Siganidae) on coral

reefs. Coral Reefs 32:13-23

Freiwald J (2012) Movement of adult temperate reef fishes off the west coast of North

America. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 69:1362-1374

Friedlander M, Parrish JD, DeFelice RC (2002) Ecology of the introduced snapper

Lutjanus kasmiva (Forsskal) in the reef fish assemblage of a Hawaiian bay. J

Fish Biol 60:28-48

132

Friedlander AM, Brown E, Monaco ME (2007) Defining reef fish habitat utilization

patterns in Hawaii: comparisons between marine protected areas and areas open

to fishing. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 351:221-233

Gagliano M, McCormick MI, Meekan MG (2007) Survival against the odds:

ontogenetic changes in selective pressure mediate growth-mortality trade-offs in

a marine fish. Proc R Soc B 274:1575-1582

Gardner TA, Cote IM, Gill JA, Grant A, Watkinson AR (2003) Long-term region-wide

declines in Caribbean corals. Science 301:958-960

Georgii B, Schröder W (1983) Home range and activity patterns of male red deer

(Cervus elaphus L.) in the Alps. Oecologia 58:238-248

Goatley CHR, Hoey AS, Bellwood DR (2012) The role of turtles as coral reef

macroherbivores. PLoS ONE 7:e39979

Graham NAJ, Wilson SK, Jennings S, Polunin NVC, Bijoux JP, Robinson J (2006)

Dynamic fragility of oceanic coral reef ecosystems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A

103:8425-8429

Graham NA, Bellwood DR, Cinner JE, Hughes TP, Norström AV, Nyström M (2013)

Managing resilience to reverse phase shifts in coral reefs. Front Ecol Environ

11:541-548

Green AL, Bellwood DR (2009) Monitoring functional groups of herbivorous reef

fishes as indicators of coral reef resilience IUCN resilience science group

working paper series. IUCN, Gland

Gust N, Choat HJ, McCormick MI (2001) Spatial variability in reef fish distribution,

abundance, size and biomass: a multi-scale analysis. Mar Ecol Prog Ser

214:237-251

Hardman E, Green JM, Desire SM, Perrine S (2010) Movement of sonically tagged

bluespine unicornfish, Naso unicornis, in relation to marine reserve boundaries

133

in Rodrigues, Western Indian Ocean. Aquat Conserv: Mar Freshw Ecosyst

20:357-361

Harris S, Cresswell WJ, Forde PG, Trewhella WJ, Woollard T, Wray S (1990) Home-

range analysis using radio-tracking data- a review of problems and techniques

particularly as applied to the study of mammals. Mammal Rev 20:97-123

Harrison JF, Roberts SP (2000) Flight respiration and energetics. Annu Rev Physiol 62:

179-205

Hartill BW, Morrison MA, Smith MD, Boubée J, Parsons DM (2003) Diurnal and tidal

movements of snapper (Pagrus auratus, Sparidae) in an estuarine environment.

Mar Freshw Res 54:931-940

Haskell JP, Ritchie ME, Olff H (2002) Fractal geometry predicts varying body size

scaling relationships from mammal and bird home ranges. Nature 418:527-530

Hay ME (1981) Herbivory, algal distribution and the maintenance of between habitat

diversity on a tropical fringing reef. Am Nat 118:520-540

Hedger RD, Martin Fo, Hatin D, Caron Fo, Whoriskey FG, Dodson JJ (2008) Active

migration of wild Atlantic salmon Salmo salar smolt through a coastal

embayment. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 355:235-246

Henry M, Pons JM, Cosson JF (2007) Foraging behaviour of a frugivorous bat helps

bridge landscape connectivity and ecological processes in a fragmented

rainforest. J Anim Ecol 76:801-813

Herrel A, Joachim R, Vanhooydonck B, Irschick DJ (2006) Ecological consequences of

ontogenetic changes in head shape and bite performance in the Jamaican lizard

Anolis lineatopus. Biol J Linn Soc 89:443-454

Heupel MR, Simpfendorfer CA, Heuter RE (2004) Estimation of shark home ranges

using passive monitoring techniques. Environ Biol Fish 71:135-142

134

Heupel MR, Semmens JM, Hobday AJ (2006) Automated acoustic tracking of aquatic

animals: scales, design and deployment of listening station arrays. Mar Freshw

Res 57:1-13

Heupel MR, Reiss KL, Yeiser BG, Simpfendorfer CA (2008) Effects of biofouling on

performance of moored data logging acoustic receivers. Limnol Oceanogr

Methods 6:327-335

Hinsch M, Pen I, Komdeur J (2013) Evolution of defense against depletion of local

food resources in a mechanistic foraging model. Behav Ecol 24:245-252

Hoegh-Guldberg O, Mumby PJ, Hooten AJ, Steneck RS, Greenfield P, Gomez E,

Harvell CD, Sale PF, Edwards AJ, Caldeira K, Knowlton N, Eakin CM,

Iglesias-Prieto R, Muthiga N, Bradbury RH, Dubi A, Hatziolos ME (2007)

Coral reefs under rapid climate change and ocean acidification. Science

318:1737-1742

Hoey AS (2010) Size matters: macroalgal height influences the feeding response of

coral reef herbivores. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 411:299-302

Hoey AS, Bellwood DR (2008) Cross-shelf variation in the role of parrotfishes on the

Great Barrier Reef. Coral Reefs 27:37-47

Hoey AS, Bellwood DR (2009) Limited functional redundancy in a high diversity

system: single species dominates key ecological process on coral reefs.

Ecosystems 12:1316-1328

Hoey AS, Bellwood DR (2010) Cross-shelf variation in browsing intensity on the Great

Barrier Reef. Coral Reefs 29:499-508

Hoey AS, Bellwood DR (2011) Suppression of herbivory by macroalgal density: a

critical feedback on coral reefs? Ecol Lett 14:267-273

Holekamp KE, Sherman PW (1989) Why male ground squirrels disperse: a multilevel

analysis explains why only males leave home. Am Sci 77:232-239

135

Holland G, Bruyère CL (2013) Recent intense hurricane response to global climate

change. Clim Dyn 42:617-627

Holland KN, Peterson JD, Lowe CG, Wetherbee BM (1993) Movements, distribution

and growth rates of the white goatfish Mulloides flavolineatus in a fisheries

conservation zone. Bull Mar Sci 52:982-992

Holland KN, Lowe CG, Wetherbee BM (1996) Movements and dispersal patterns of

blue trevally (Caranx melampygus) in a fisheries conservation zone. Fish Res

25:279-292

Holland KN, Meyer CG, Dagorn LC (2009) Inter-animal telemetry: results from

deployment of acoustic 'business card' tags. Endanger Species Res 10:287-293

Holmes TH, McCormick MI (2010) Size-selectivity of predatory reef fish on juvenile

prey. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 399:273-283

Huey RB (1991) Physiological consequences of habitat selection. Am Nat 137:S91-

S115

Hughes TP (1994) Catastrophes, phase shifts, and large-scale degradation of a

Caribbean coral reef. Science 265:1547-1551

Hughes TP, Baird AH, Bellwood DR, Card M, Connolly SR, Folke C, Grosberg R,

Hoegh-Guldberg O, Jackson JBC, Kleypas J, Lough J, Marshall P, Nyström M,

Palumbi SR, Pandolfi JM, Rosen B, Roughgarden J (2003) Climate change,

human impacts, and the resilience of coral reefs. Science 301:929-933

Hughes TP, Bellwood DR, Folke C, Steneck RS, Wilson SK (2005) New paradigms for

supporting the resilience of marine ecosystems. Trends Ecol Evol 20:380-386

Hughes TP, Rodrigues MJ, Bellwood DR, Ceccarelli D, Hoegh-Guldberg O, McCook

L, Moltschaniwskyj N, Pratchett MS, Steneck RS, Willis B (2007) Phase shifts,

herbivory, and the resilience of coral reefs to climate change. Curr Biol 17:360-

365

136

Hughes TP, Graham NAJ, Jackson JBC, Mumby PJ, Steneck RS (2010) Rising to the

challenge of sustaining coral reef resilience. Trends Ecol Evol 25:633-642

Hutchinson N, Rhodes KL (2010) Home range estimates for squaretail coralgrouper,

Plectropomus areolatus (Rüppell 1830). Coral Reefs 21:511-519

Imansyah MJ, Jessop TS, Ciofi C, Akbar Z (2008) Ontogenetic differences in the

spatial ecology of immature Komodo dragons. J Zool 274:107-115

Ishii HT, Tanabe S, Hiura H (2004) Exploring the relationships among canopy

structure, stand productivity, and biodiversity of temperate forest ecosystems.

For Sci 50:342-355

Itzkowitz M (1977) Social dynamics of mixed-species groups of Jamaican reef fishes.

Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:361-384

Jackson JBC, Kirby MX, Berger WH, Bjorndal KA, Botsford LW, Bourque BJ,

Bradbury RH, Cooke R, Erlandson J, Estes JA, Hughes TP, Kidwell S, Lange

CB, Lenihan HS, Pandolfi JM, Peterson CH, Steneck RS, Tegner MJ, Warner

RR (2001) Historical overfishing and the recent collapse of coastal ecosystems.

Science 293:629-637

Jetz W, Carbone C, Fulford J, Brown JH (2004) The scaling of animal space use.

Science 306:266-268

Johansson C, van de Leemput I, Depczynski M, Hoey A, Bellwood D (2013) Key

herbivores reveal limited functional redundancy on inshore coral reefs. Coral

Reefs 32:963-972

Johnson DDP, Macdonald DW, Newman C, Morecroft MD (2001) Group size versus

territory size in group-living badgers: a large-sample field test of the resource

dispersion hypothesis. Oikos 95:265-274

Jones GP, Milicich MJ, Emslie MJ, Lunow C (1999) Self-recruitment in a coral reef

fish population. Nature 402:802-804

137

José de Anchieta C, Sampaio CL, Barros F (2013) How wave exposure, group size and

habitat complexity influence foraging and population densities in fishes of the

genus Halichoeres (Perciformes: Labridae) on tropical rocky shores. Marine

Biology 160:2383-2394

Jouventin P, Weimerskirch H (1990) Satellite tracking of wandering albatrosses. Nature

343:746-748

Jud ZR, Layman CA (2012) Site fidelity and movement patterns of invasive lionfish,

Pterois spp. in a Florida estuary. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 414-415:69-74

Kernohan BJ, Gitzen RA, Millspaugh JJ (2001) Analysis of animal space use and

movements. In: Millspaugh JJ, Marzluff JM (eds) Radio tracking and animal

populations. Academic Press, New York, pp 126-164

Kerwath S, Götz A, Cowley PD, Sauer W, Attwood C (2005) A telemetry experiment

on spotted grunter Pomadasys commersonnii in an African estuary. Afr J Mar

Sci 27:389-394

Khalil MT, Cochran JEM, Berumen ML (2013) The abundance of herbivorous fish on

an inshore Red Sea reef following a mass coral bleaching event. Environ Biol

Fish 96:1065-1072

Klimley AP, Voegeli F, Beavers SC, Le Boeuf BJ (1998) Automated listening stations

for tagged marine fish. J Mar Technol Soc 32:94-101

Knight TM, McCoy MW, Chase JM, McCoy KA, Holt RD (2005) Trophic cascades

across ecosystems. Nature 437:880-883

Knowlton N (2001) The future of coral reefs. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:5419-5425

Kramer DL, Chapman MR (1999) Implications of fish home range size and relocation

for marine reserve function. Environ Biol Fish 55:65-79

Kramer MJ, Bellwood DR, Bellwood O (2012) Cryptofauna of the epilithic algal matrix

on an inshore coral reef, Great Barrier Reef. Coral Reefs 31:1007-1015

138

Kramer MJ, Bellwood O, Bellwood DR (2013) The trophic importance of algal turfs for

coral reef fishes: the crustacean link. Coral Reefs 32:575-583

Kremen C (2005) Managing ecosystem services: what do we need to know about their

ecology? Ecol Lett 8:468-479

Kuwamura T, Sagawa T, Suzuki S (2009) Interspecific variation in spawning time and

male mating tactics of the parrotfishes on a fringing coral reef at Iriomote

Island, Okinawa. Ichthyol Res 56:354-362

Lacroix GL, Voegeli FA, (2000) Development of Automated Monitoring Systems for

Ultrasonic Transmitters. In: Moore A, Russell I (eds) Fish telemetry:

Proceedings of the 3rd Conference on Fish Telemetry in Europe. CEFAS:

Lowestoft, UK, pp 37-50

Laguë M, Tania N, Heath J, Edelstein-Keshet L (2012) The effects of facilitation and

competition on group foraging in patches. J Theor Biol 310:88-96

Laver PN, Kelly MJ (2008) A critical review of home range studies. J Wildl Manag

72:290-298

Leahy SM, McCormick MI, Mitchell MD, Ferrari MCO (2011) To fear or to feed: the

effects of turbidity on perception of risk by a marine fish. Biol Lett 7:811-813

Ledlie MH, Graham NAJ, Bythell JC, Wilson SK, Jennings S, Polunin NVC,

Hardcastle J (2007) Phase shifts and the role of herbivory in the resilience of

coral reefs. Coral Reefs 26:641-653

Lee R (2011) The outlook for population growth. Science 333:569-573

Lefèvre CD, Bellwood DR (2010) Seasonality and dynamics in coral reef macroalgae:

variation in condition and susceptibility to herbivory. Mar Biol 157:955-965

Lefèvre CD, Bellwood DR (2011) Temporal variation in coral reef ecosystem

processes: herbivory of macroalgae by fishes. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 422:239-251

139

Leis JM, Carson-Ewart BM, Cato DH (2002) Sound detection in situ by the larvae of a

coral-reef damselfish (Pomacentridae). Mar Ecol Prog Ser 232:259-268

Lewis MS, Wainwright PC (1985) Herbivore abundance and grazing intensity on a

Caribbean coral reef. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 87:215-228

Lindholm J, Knight A, Kaufman L, Miller S (2006) Site fidelity and movement of the

parrotfishes Scarus coeruleus and Scarus taeniopterus at Conch Reef (northern

Florida keys). Caribb J Sci 42:138-144

Lindstedt SL, Miller BJ, Buskirk SW (1986) Home range, time, and body size in

mammals. Ecology 67:413-418

Lokrantz J, Nyström M, Thyresson M, Johansson C (2008) The non-linear relationship

between body size and function in parrotfishes. Coral Reefs 27:967-974

Lokrantz J, Nyström M, Norstrom AV, Folke C, Cinner JE (2010) Impacts of artisanal

fishing on key functional groups and the potential vulnerability of coral reefs.

Environ Conserv 36:327-337

Long SP, Shekar R (2013) 2013 reviews of global change biology. Global Change

Biol19:1-2

Luckhurst B, Luckhurst K (1978) Analysis of the influence of substrate variables on

coral reef fish communities. Mar Biol 49:317-323

Lukoschek V, McCormick MI (2000) A review of multi-species foraging associations

in fishes and their ecological significance. Proc 9th Int Coral Reef Symp 23-27

Lundberg J, Moberg F (2003) Mobile link organisms and ecosystem functioning:

implications for ecosystem resilience and management. Ecosystems 6:87-98

Mantyka CS, Bellwood DR (2007) Direct evaluation of macroalgal removal by

herbivorous coral reef fishes. Coral Reefs 26:435-442

140

Mathies NH, Ogburn MB, McFall G, Fangman S (2014) Environmental interference

factors affecting detection range in acoustic telemetry studies using fixed

receiver arrays. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 495:27-38

March D, Palmer M, Alós J, Grau A, Cardona F (2010) Short-term residence, home

range size and diel patterns of the painted comber Serranus scriba in a

temperate marine reserve. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 400:195-206

Marler C, Walsberg G, White M, Moore M, Marler CA (1995) Increased energy

expenditure due to increased territorial defense in male lizards after phenotypic

manipulation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 37:225-231

Marshall CD, Guzman A, Narazaki T, Sato K, Kane EA, Sterba-Boatwright BD (2012)

The ontogenetic scaling of bite force and head size in loggerhead sea turtles

(Caretta caretta): implications for durophagy in neritic, benthic habitats. J Exp

Biol 215:4166-4174

Marshell A, Mills J, Rhodes K, McIlwain J (2011) Passive acoustic telemetry reveals

highly variable home range and movement patterns among unicornfish within a

marine reserve. Coral Reefs 30:631-642

Maypa AP, Green A, Almany GR, Abesamis R, White A (2012) Movement patterns

and life history strategies of key coral reef and coastal pelagic fisheries species

for consideration in MPA network design in the Coral Triangle. Report Prepared

for the Coral Triangle Support Partnership

McCauley RD, Cato DH (2000) Patterns of fish calling in a nearshore environment in

the Great Barrier Reef. Philos Trans R Soc B 355:1289-1293

McClanahan TR, Hendrick V, Rodrigues MJ, Polunin NVC (1999) Varying responses

of herbivorous and invertebrate-feeding fishes to macroalgal reduction on a

coral reef. Coral Reefs 18:195-203

141

McCook LJ (1997) Effects of herbivory on zonation of Sargassum spp. within fringing

reefs of the central Great Barrier Reef. Mar Biol 129:713-722

McKinney JA, Hoffmayer ER, Wu W, Fulford. R, Hendon J (2012) Feeding habitat of

the whale shark Rhincodon typus in the northern Gulf of Mexico determined

using species distribution modeling. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 458:199-211

Messier F, Barrette C (1982) The social system of the coyote (Canis latrans) in a

forested habitat. Can J Zool 60:1743-1753

Meyer CG, Holland KN (2005) Movement patterns, home range size and habitat

utilization of the bluespine unicornfish, Naso unicornis (Acanthuridae) in a

Hawaiian marine reserve. Environ Biol Fish 73:201-210

Meyer CG, Papastamatiou YP, Clark TB (2010) Differential movement patterns and

site fidelity among trophic groups of reef fishes in a Hawaiian marine protected

area. Mar Biol 157:1499-1511

Moffitt EA, Botsford LW, Kaplan DM, O'Farrell MR (2009) Marine reserve networks

for species that move within a home range. Ecol Appl 19:1835-1847

Mouillot D, Bellwood DR, Baraloto C, Chave J, Galzin R, Harmelin-Vivien M,

Kulbicki M, Lavergne S, Lavorel S, Mouquet N (2013) Rare species support

vulnerable functions in high-diversity ecosystems. PLoS Biol 11:e1001569

Michael PJ, Hyndes GA, Vanderklift MA, Vergés A (2013) Identity and behaviour of

herbivorous fish influence large-scale spatial patterns of macroalgal herbivory in

a coral reef. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 482:227-240

Mumby PJ (2006) The impact of exploiting grazers (Scaridae) on the dynamics of

Caribbean coral reefs. Ecol Appl 16:747-769

Mumby PJ, Wabnitz CCC (2002) Spatial patterns of aggression, territory size, and

harem size in five sympatric Caribbean parrotfish species. Environ Biol Fish

63:265-279

142

Muñoz RC, Motta PJ (2000) Interspecific agression between two parrofishes

(Sparisoma, Scaridae) in the Florida Keys. Copeia 3:674-683

Murchie KJ, Schwager E, Cooke SJ, Danylchuk A, Danylchuk S, Goldberg T, Suski C,

Philipp D (2010) Spatial ecology of juvenile lemon sharks (Negaprion

brevirostris) in tidal creeks and coastal waters of Eleuthera, The Bahamas.

Environ Biol Fish 89:95-104

Murchie KJ, Cooke SJ, Danylchuk AJ, Danylchuk SE, Goldberg TL, Suski CD, Philipp

DP (2013) Movement patterns of bonefish (Albula vulpes) in tidal creeks and

coastal waters of Eleuthera, The Bahamas. Fish Res 147:404-412

Muscarella R, Fleming TH (2007) The role of frugivorous bats in tropical forest

succession. Biol Rev 82:573-590

Myers RF (1989) Micronesian reef fishes: a practical guide to the identification of the

coral reef fishes of the tropical central and western Pacific. Coral Graphics,

Guam

Nash KL, Graham NAJ, Januchowski-Hartley FA, Bellwood DR (2012) Influence of

habitat condition and competition on foraging behaviour of parrotfish. Mar Ecol

Prog Ser 457:113-124

Nathan R (2006) Long-distance dispersal of plants. Science 313:786-788

Nyström M, Folke C (2001) Spatial resilience of coral reefs. Ecosystems 4:406-417

Nyström M, Graham NAJ, Lokrantz J, Norström AV (2008) Capturing the cornerstones

of coral reef resilience linking theory to practice. Coral Reefs 27:795-809

O'Toole AC, Danylchuk AJ, Goldberg TL, Suski CD, Philipp DP, Brooks E, Cooke SJ

(2011) Spatial ecology and residency patterns of adult great barracuda

(Sphyraena barracuda) in coastal waters of the Bahamas. Mar Biol 158:2227-

2237

143

Olds AD, Pitt KA, Maxwell PS, Connolly RM (2012) Synergistic effects of reserves

and connectivity on ecological resilience. J Appl Ecol 49:1195-1203

Owen-Smith N, Fryxell JM, Merrill EH (2010) Foraging theory upscaled: the

behavioural ecology of herbivore movement. Phil Trans R Soc B 365:2267-2278

Payne NL, Gillanders BM, Webber DM, Semmens JM (2010) Interpreting diel activity

patterns from acoustic telemetry: the need for controls. Mar Ecol Prog Ser

419:295-301

Paddack MJ, Cowen RK, Sponaugle S (2006) Grazing pressure of herbivorous coral

reef fishes on low coral-cover reefs. Coral Reefs 25:461-472

Pandolfi JM, Bradbury RH, Sala E, Hughes TP, Bjorndal KA, Cooke RG, McArdle D,

McClenachan L, Newman MJH, Paredes G, Warner RR, Jackson JBC (2003)

Global trajectories of the long-term decline of coral reef ecosystems. Science

301:955-958

Pardini R, Bueno AdA, Gardner TA, Prado PIc, Metzger JP (2010) Beyond the

fragmentation threshold hypothesis: regime shifts in biodiversity across

fragmented landscapes. PLoS ONE 5:e13666

Patterson, B.D., Willig, M.R. & Stevens, R.D. (2003). Trophic strategies, niche

partitioning, and patterns of ecological organization. In: Kunz TH, Fenton MB

(eds) Bat ecology. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp 536–579

Patterson HM, Swearer SE (2007) Long-distance dispersal and local retention of larvae

as mechanisms of recruitment in an island population of a coral reef fish.

Austral Ecol 32:122-130

Persson L, Leonardsson K, de Roos AM, Gyllenberg M, Christensen B (1998)

Ontogenetic scaling of foraging rates and the dynamics of a size-structured

consumer-resource model. Theor Popul Biol 54:270-293

144

Polovina JJ (1996) Decadal variation in the trans-Pacific migration of northern bluefin

tuna (Thunnus thynnus) coherent with climate-induced change in prey

abundance. Fish Oceanogr 5:114-119

Pomilla C, Rosenbaum HC (2005) Against the current: an inter-oceanic whale

migration event. Biol Lett 1:476-479

Quinn GP, Keough MJ (2002) Experimental design and data analysis for biologists.

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

Radford CA, Jeffs AG, Tindle CT, Montgomery JC (2008) Temporal patterns in

ambient noise of biological origin from a shallow water temperate reef.

Oecologia 156:921-929

Randall JE, Allen GR, Steene RC (1997) Fishes of the Great Barrier Reef and Coral

Sea. University of Hawai'i Press, Honolulu, HI

Rasher DB, Hoey AS, Hay ME (2013) Consumer diversity interacts with prey defenses

to drive ecosystem function. Ecology 94:1347-1358

Reyer CPO, Leuzinger S, Rammig A, Wolf A, Bartholomeus RP, Bonfante A, de

Lorenzi F, Dury M, Gloning P, Abou Jaoudé R, Klein T, Kuster TM, Martins M,

Niedrist G, Riccardi M, Wohlfahrt G, de Angelis P, de Dato G, François L,

Menzel A, Pereira M (2013) A plant's perspective of extremes: terrestrial plant

responses to changing climatic variability. Global Change Biol 19:75-89

Rico Y, Boehmer H, Wagner H (2012) Determinants of actual functional connectivity

for calcareous grassland communities linked by rotational sheep grazing. Landsc

Ecol 27:199-209

Robertson DR, Sweatman HPA, Fletcher EA, Cleland MG (1976) Schooling as a

mechanism for circumventing the territoriality of competitors. Ecology 57:1208-

1220

145

Rocha CFD (1999) Home range of the tropidurid lizard Liolaemus lutzae: sexual and

body size differences. Rev Bras Biol 59:125-130

Sazima C, Krajewski JP, Bonaldo RM, Sazima I (2007) Nuclear-follower foraging

associations of reef fishes and other animals at an oceanic archipelago. Environ

Biol Fish 80:351-361

Scheffer M, Carpenter S, Foley JA, Folke C, Walker BH (2001) Catastrophic shifts in

ecosystems. Nature 413:591-596

Sekercioglu CH (2006) Increasing awareness of avian ecological function. Trends Ecol

Evol 21:464-471

Semmens JM, Buxton CD, Forbes E, Phelan MJ (2010) Spatial and temporal use of

spawning aggregation sites by the tropical sciaenid Protonibea diacanthus. Mar

Ecol Prog Ser 403:193-203

Shanks AL (2009) Pelagic larval duration and dispersal distance revisited. Biol Bull

216:373-385

Siegel DA, Mitarai S, Costello CJ, Gaines SD, Kendall BE, Warner RR, Winters KB

(2008) The stochastic nature of larval connectivity among nearshore marine

populations. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:8974-8979

Simpson SD, Meekan MG, Jeffs A, Montgomery JC, McCauley RD (2008a)

Settlement-stage coral reef fish prefer the higher-frequency invertebrate-

generated audible component of reef noise. Anim Behav 75: 1861-1868

Simpson SD, Jeffs A, Montgomery JC, McCauley RD, Meekan MG (2008b) Nocturnal

relocation of adult and juvenile coral reef fishes in response to reef noise. Coral

Reefs 27: 97-104

Simpfendorfer CA, Heupel MR, Collins AB (2008) Variation in the performance of

acoustic receivers and its implications for positioning algorithms in a riverine

setting. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 65:482-492

146

Simpfendorfer CA, Yeiser BG, Wiley TR, Poulakis GR, Stevens PW, Heupel MR

(2011) Environmental influences on the spatial ecology of juvenile smalltooth

sawfish (Pristis pectinata): results from acoustic monitoring. PLoS ONE

6:e16918

Smith VH, Schindler DW (2009) Eutrophication science: where do we go from here?

Trends Ecol Evol 24:201-207

Stephenson W, Searles R (1960) Experimental studies on the ecology of intertidal

environments at Heron Island. I. exclusion of fish from beach rock. Mar Freshw

Res 11:241-268

Sullivan KA (1989) Predation and starvation: age-specific mortality in juvenile juncos

(Junco phaenotus). J Anim Ecol 58:275-286

Sundstrom SM, Allen CR, Barichievy C (2012) Species, functional groups, and

thresholds in ecological resilience. Conserv Biol 26:305-314

Sumner J (2006) Higher relatedness within groups due to variable subadult dispersal in

a rainforest skink, Gnypetoscincus queenslandiae. Austral Ecol 31:441-448

Terborgh J, Lopez L, Nunez P, Rao M, Shahabuddin G, Orihuela G, Riveros M,

Ascanio R, Adler GH, Lambert TD (2001) Ecological meltdown in predator-free

forest fragments. Science 294:1923-1926

Trent TT, Rongstad OJ (1974) Home range and survival of cottontail rabbits in

southwestern Wisconsin. J Wildl Manag 38: 459-472

Turgeon K, Robillard A, Jacinthe G, Duclos V, Kramer DL (2010) Functional

connectivity from a reef fish perspective: behavioral tactics for moving in a

fragmented landscape. Ecology 91:3332-3342

van Rooij MJ, Kroon FJ, Videler JJ (1996) The social and mating system of the

herbivorous reef fish Sparisoma viride: one-male versus multi-male groups.

Environ Biol Fish 47:353-378

147

Vergés A, Vanderklift MA, Doropoulos C, Hyndes GA (2011) Spatial patterns in

herbivory on a coral reef are influenced by structural complexity but not by algal

traits. PLoS ONE 6:e17115

Vermeij MA, Heijden R, Olthuis J, Marhaver K, Smith J, Visser P (2013) Survival and

dispersal of turf algae and macroalgae consumed by herbivorous coral reef

fishes. Oecologia 171:417-425

Vitousek PM, Mooney HA, Lubchenco J, Melillo JM (1997) Human domination of

earth's ecosystems. Science 277:494-499

Voegeli FA, Pincock DG, (1996) Overview of underwater acoustics as it applies to

telemetry. In: Baras E, Philippart, JC (eds) Underwater biotelemetry. University

of Liege, Liege, pp. 23-30

Voegeli FA, Smale MJ, Webber DM, Andrade Y, O'Dor RK (2001) Ultrasonic

telemetry, tracking and automated monitoring technology for sharks. Environ

Biol Fish 60:267-281

Warton DI, Hui FK (2011) The arcsine is asinine: the analysis of proportions in

ecology. Ecology 92:3-10

Welsh JQ, Bellwood DR (2012a) Spatial ecology of the steephead parrotfish

(Chlorurus microrhinos): an evaluation using acoustic telemetry. Coral Reefs

31:55-65

Welsh JQ, Bellwood DR (2012b) How far do schools of roving herbivores rove? A case

study using Scarus rivulatus. Coral Reefs 31:991-1003

Welsh JQ, Bellwood DR (2014) Herbivorous fishes, ecosystem function and mobile

links on coral reefs. Coral Reefs 33:303-311

Welsh JQ, Goatley CHR, Bellwood DR (2013) The ontogeny of home ranges: evidence

from coral reef fishes. Proc R Soc B 280:20132066

148

Welsh JQ, Fox RJ, Webber DM, Bellwood DR (2012) Performance of remote acoustic

receivers within a coral reef habitat: implications for array design. Coral Reefs

31:693-702

West D, West BJ. (2012) On allometry relations. Int J Mod Phy B 26:1230010.

White PJ, Ralls K (1993) Reproduction and spacing patterns of kit foxes relative to

changing prey availability. J Wildl Manag 57:861-867

Wilson SK, Bellwood DR, Choat JH, Furnas MJ (2003) Detritus in the epilithic algal

matrix and its use by coral reef fishes. Oceanogr Mar Biol 41:279-309

Wilson SK, Graham NAJ, Pratchett MS, Jones GP, Polunin NVC (2006) Multiple

disturbances and the global degradation of coral reefs: are reef fishes at risk or

resilient? Global Change Biol 12:2220-2234

Wilson SK, Fisher R, Pratchett MS, Graham NAJ, Dulvy NK, Turner RA, Cakacaka A,

Polunin NVC, Rushton SP (2008) Exploitation and habitat degradation as agents

of change within coral reef fish communities. Global Change Biol 14:2796-2809

Winter HV, Jansen HM, Bruijs MCM (2006) Assessing the impact of hydropower and

fisheries on downstream migrating silver eel, Anguilla anguilla, by telemetry in

the River Meuse. Ecol Freshw Fish 15:221-228

Wismer S, Hoey AS, Bellwood DR (2009) Cross-shelf benthic community structure on

the Great Barrier Reef: relationships between macroalgal cover and herbivore

biomass. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 376:45-54

Wittenberger JF (1981) Animal social behavior. Duxbury Press, Boston

Wood LJ, Fish L, Laughren J, Pauly D (2008) Assessing progress towards global

marine protection targets: shortfalls in information and action. Oryx 42:340-351

Wunderle JM (1997) The role of animal seed dispersal in accelerating native forest

regeneration on degraded tropical lands. For Ecol Mang 99:223-235

149

Zeller DC (1997) Home range and activity patterns of the coral trout Plectropomus

leopardus (Serranidae). Mar Ecol Prog Ser 154:65-77

150

Appendix A: Supplementary Materials for Chapter 2

Fig. A1 Spectral analysis using a Fast Fourier Transformation for hourly detection

frequencies. Each value on the graph represents the number of hours in a potential cycle

of detections.

0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15 0.20 0.25 0.30 0.35 0.40 0.45 0.50Frequency

0

1000

2000

3000

4000

5000

Spec

tral D

ensi

ty

40

16.7

10

6.1

4.84.1

3.3 2.7

151

Appendix B: Supplementary Materials for Chapter 4

Appendix B1: Individuals’ home ranges were standardised due to varying observation

periods between the size classes. To accomplish this, an equation was generated using

data from larger individuals, which reached a home range asymptote, by plotting the

percentage of their total home range an individual had reached at each observational

interval:

(home range correction factor) = -0.0027*(monitoring time)2 + 0.9657*(monitoring

time) + 14.396)

With this equation, it was possible to generate a correction factor, which represents the

portion of the home range that was occupied during the observation period of the

individual. A corrected estimated of home range size for individuals that did not reach

the asymptote was calculated using:

(corrected home range) = (home range estimate) / (home range correction factor)

152

Table B1 Regression model comparison for a) cube-root body mass (g) versus square-root

home range area (m2), b) body mass (g) versus home range size (m2) in juveniles (< 150

mm), c) body mass (g) versus home range size (m2) in adults (> 150 mm) and d) body mass

(g) versus home range size (m2) in all individuals. Model in bold indicates the equation of

best-fit.

Equation Summary Parameter Estimates

r2 F df1 df2 P Constant b1 a) Linear .775 251.875 1 73 .000 2.906 1.518 Logarithmic .869 484.425 1 73 .000 5.516 4.393 Power .829 354.973 1 73 .000 3.324 .818 Growth .545 87.587 1 73 .000 .840 .243 Exponential .545 87.587 1 73 .000 2.316 .243 b) Linear .584 74.433 1 53 .000 10.496 5.376 Logarithmic .438 41.355 1 53 .000 43.116 14.677 Power .792 201.248 1 53 .000 13.396 .706 Growth .427 39.479 1 53 .000 1.517 .164 Exponential .427 39.479 1 53 .000 4.561 .164 c) Linear .068 1.307 1 18 .268 212.023 .037 Logarithmic .028 .513 1 18 .483 158.977 12.100 Power .031 .572 1 18 .459 159.419 .059 Growth .075 1.453 1 18 .244 5.330 .000 Exponential .075 1.453 1 18 .244 206.435 .000 d) Linear .611 114.614 1 73 .000 48.016 .251 Logarithmic .764 236.438 1 73 .000 55.578 24.399 Power .829 354.973 1 73 .000 11.048 .545 Growth .313 33.318 1 73 .000 2.533 .004 Exponential .313 33.318 1 73 .000 12.597 .004

153

Table B2 a) Levene’s test for homogeneity of variances in interspecific residual

data from the logarithmic regression model. b) One-way ANOVA comparing

the residual data from the logarithmic regression model between Scarus

frenatus, S. niger and Chlorurus sordidus to test for interspecific variability in

the model’s predictive capacity.

a) F df1 df2 P

0.377 2 72 0.687 b)

Source of variation

SS df MS F P

Corrected model

20.042 2 10.021 3.082 0.052

Intercept 8.034 1 8.034 2.471 0.120 Species 20.042 2 10.021 3.082 0.052 Error 234.097 72 3.251 Total 255.219 75 Corrected total 254.139 74

154

Fig. B1 Relationship between body mass (g) and home range size (m2) for Scarus

frenatus, S. niger and Chlorurus sordidus in a) all individuals below 150 mm (TL). Diet

for individuals from different size classes are shown as; filled circles for detritivores

and grey triangles for omnivores/carnivores following Fig. 3.1; b) the largest size class

(> 150 mm; total length; TL).

Mass (g)

0 10 20 30 40

Hom

e ra

nge

(100

m2 )

0

1.0

2.0

3.0 a)

100 300 500 700 900

b)

1.5

2.0

2.5

3.0

155

Appendix C: Supplementary Materials for Chapter 5

Table C1 Summary of acoustic monitoring data for 14 tagged Kyphosus vaigiensis.

Individuals highlighted in grey and suspected to have died during the course of the

study.

Individual Number of detections

Number of days detected

Date released Date last detection

K34 20,036 124 19/09/11 21/01/12

K33 25,574 124 19/09/11 22/01/12

K44 14,835 33 20/09/11 23/10/11

K43 12,111 109 20/09/11 17/11/11

K40 22,713 91 20/09/11 05/01/12

K41 11,777 43 20/09/11 19/01/12

K42 13,351 78 20/09/11 08/12/11

K31 39,041 90 20/09/11 18/12/11

K32 37,452 125 20/09/11 22/01/12

K35 54,995 123 20/09/11 22/01/12

K39 647 17 20/09/11 09/10/11

K38 15,580 124 20/09/11 22/01/12

K36 1,241 123 20/09/11 1/10/11

K37 30,440 123 20/09/11 22/01/12

156

Table C2 Movement data of 14 Kyphosus vaigiensis tagged with V9-1L transmitters.

Individual Length (FL; cm)

Diurnal MLD*

Nocturnal MLD*

Diurnal home range estimate (m2) †

Nocturnal home range estimate (m2) †

Daily diurnal MLD*

Daily nocturnal MLD*

Diurnal daily proportion of overall MLD*

Nocturnal daily proportion of overall MLD*

Diurnal MDT‡

Nocturnal MDT‡

Residency index

K34 27 783.6 939.1 39,180 46,955 734.2 623.8 0.94 0.66 215.5 249 0.98

K33 28 993.7 544.4 49,685 27,220 588.4 532 0.59 0.98 254.5 174 0.98

K44 31.8 1,577.1 952.5 78,855 47,625 1,009.8

836.8 0.64 0.88 345 259 0.26

K43 32.8 11,000.2 13,352 550,010 667,600 1,305.4

1,401.2 0.12 0.10 402.5 448.5 0.34

K40 29.3 2,131.5 4,643.4 106,575 232,170 1,192.4

785 0.56 0.17 457 385 0.87

K41 23.4 5,342.9 2,245.9 267,145 112,295 2,124.8

1320.5 0.40 0.59 537.5 424 0.73

K42 29 2,111.9 3,756.8 105,595 187,840 1,332.4

876 0.63 0.23 644 333 0.62

K31 30.2 1,575.1 1,575.1 78,755 78,755 1,204.8

771.4 0.76 0.49 334 326 0.72

K32 28.4 990.6 943.4 49,530 47,170 648.4 726 0.65 0.77 279.5 241 0.98

157

Table C2 Movement data of 14 Kyphosus vaigiensis tagged with V9-1L transmitters.

Individual Length (FL; cm)

Diurnal MLD*

Nocturnal MLD*

Diurnal home range estimate (m2) †

Nocturnal home range estimate (m2) †

Daily diurnal MLD*

Daily nocturnal MLD*

Diurnal daily proportion of overall MLD*

Nocturnal daily proportion of overall MLD*

Diurnal MDT‡

Nocturnal MDT‡

Residency index

K35 32.6 1,677.2 1,677.2 83,860 83,860 990.8 922.8 0.59 0.55 319 319 1

K39 28.4 1,677.2 1,119.9 83,860 55,995 1,048.2

713.2 0.62 0.64 505 310 0.98

K38 31.3 1,677.2 1,677.2 83,860 83,860 1,043.6

998 0.62 0.60 478 319 0.99

K36 28.8

K37 30.4 1,677.2 1,127.3 83,860 56,365 967 838 0.58 0.74 498 309.5 0.98

Average (± SE)

29.4 ± 0.66

2,521.4 ± 713.67

2,625.9 ± 880.01

126,070 ± 35,683

131,293.9 ± 44,000

1,091.6 ±

103.74

875.7 ± 65.36

0.59 ± 0.05

0.57 ± 0.07 442 ± 49.36

333.6 ± 26.70

0.76 ± 0.08

* Minimum linear distance (m) † Home ranges calculated assuming a constant width of 50 m ‡ Median distance traveled (m) Grey highlights indicate fish that may have died. Where possible, data were analyzed up to the point of mortality.

158

Table C3 List if species used in the meta-analysis and the values for fork length and maximum home range length added to the model.

Species Body length (mm)

Range length (m)

Estimation method*

Source

Acanthuridae A. coeruleus 205 171 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000

A. chirurgus 160 215 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Zebrasoma flavescens 171 370 Passive Claisse et al. 2011 Acanthurus bahianus 180 538 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Naso unicornis 480 940 Active Hardman et al. 2010 Albulidae

Albula vulpes 515 15,498 Passive Murchie et al. 2013 Carangidae

Caranx ruber 220 39 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Caranx melampygus 507 6,242 Active Holland et al. 1996 Chaetodontidae

Chaetondon striatus 155 127 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Cirrhitidae

Amblycirrhitus pinos 65 1.8 Visual Luckhurst & Luckhurst 1978 Haemulidae

Haemulon flavolineatum 175 62 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Holocentridae

Myripristis jacobus 170 22 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Kyphosidae

Kyphosus sectatrix 270 1,259 Active Eristhee and Oxenford 2001 K. vaigiensis 328 11,000.20 Passive Welsh and Bellwood 2014 Labridae

Scarus vetula 260 57 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Sparisoma aurofrenatum 215 97 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 S. rubripine 225 161 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 S. viride 280 127 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Chlorurus microrhinos 480 351 Active Welsh and Bellwood 2012a Scarus taeniopterus 170 897 Passive Lindholm et al. 2006 S. coeruleus 350 1,097 Passive Lindholm et al. 2006 Lutjanidae

Lutjanus kasmira 150 900 MR Friedlander et al. 2002 Monacanthidae

Cantherhines pullus 200 80 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Mullidae

Mulloidichthys sp. 235 150 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Mulloides flavolineatus 318 600 Passive Holland et al. 1993 Pomacanthidae,

Centropyge argi 45 1.2 Visual Luckhurst & Luckhurst 1978 Pomacanthus paru 180 41 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Holocentrus rufus 210 62 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Holocanthus tricolor 240 64 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Pomacentridae

159

E. planifrons 100 1.8 Visual Luckhurst & Luckhurst 1978 Eupomacentrus diencaeus 110 2 Visual Luckhurst & Luckhurst 1978 Eupomacentrus partitus 80 2.4 Visual Luckhurst & Luckhurst 1978 Microspathodon chrysurus 160 43 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Scorpaenidae

Pterois spp. 89 423 MR Jud & Layman 2012 Serranidae

E. fulvus 320 27 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Epinephelus cruentatus 330 32 Visual Chapman and Kramer 2000 Plectropomus leopardus 490 223 Active Zeller 1997 Sphyraenidae

Sphyraena barracuda 1,250 15,950 Passive O'Toole et al. 2011 Syngnathidae

Micrognathus ensendae 90 2.6 Visual Luckhurst & Luckhurst 1978 Tripterygiidae

Enneanectes atrorus 30 4 Visual Luckhurst & Luckhurst 1978 * Visual = visual estimation, Acitve = active acoustic, Passive = Passive acoustic tracking and MR = Mark recapture

160

Fig. C1 Continued on next page

161

Fig. C1 Continued on next page

1244

1231

1232

1237

1235

1238

VR

2W-109081

VR

2W-101865

VR

2W-109071

VR

2W-109073

VR

2W-109082

VR

2W-109074

VR

2W-109066

VR

2W-109072

VR

2W-112617

VR

2W-112610

VR

2W-112611

VR

2W-105219

VR

2W-105221

VR

2W-112624

VR

2W-105222

VR

2W-112618

VR

2W-105217

VR

2W-112622

VR

2W-112621

VR

2W-112616

VR

2W-105215

VR

2W-109079

VR

2W-112612

VR

2W-105220

VR

2W-105214

VR

2W-105216

VR

2W-112623

VR

2W-112614

VR

2W-112613

VR

2W-109026

VR

2W-105213

VR

2W-112615

VR

2W-112620

VR

2W-105218

VR

2W-112619

VR

2W-109069

VR

2W-109070

VR

2W-109077

VR

2W-109080

VR

2W-109068

VR

2W-109081

VR

2W-101865

VR

2W-109071

VR

2W-109073

VR

2W-109082

VR

2W-109074

VR

2W-109066

VR

2W-109072

VR

2W-112617

VR

2W-112610

VR

2W-112611

VR

2W-105219

VR

2W-105221

VR

2W-112624

VR

2W-105222

VR

2W-112618

VR

2W-105217

VR

2W-112622

VR

2W-112621

VR

2W-112616

VR

2W-105215

VR

2W-109079

VR

2W-112612

VR

2W-105220

VR

2W-105214

VR

2W-105216

VR

2W-112623

VR

2W-112614

VR

2W-112613

VR

2W-109026

VR

2W-105213

VR

2W-112615

VR

2W-112620

VR

2W-105218

VR

2W-112619

VR

2W-109069

VR

2W-109070

VR

2W-109077

VR

2W-109080

VR

2W-109068

120

100

80

60

40

20

0

60

50

40

30

20

10

0

60

50

40

30

20

10

0

100

80

60

40

20

0

30

25

20

15

10

5

0

20

18

16

14

12

10

8

6

4

2

0

Ave

rage

det

ectio

ns.d

ay-1

Diurnal Nocturnal

Diurnal COV: 1.31Nocturnal COV: 2.88

Diurnal COV: 1.09Nocturnal COV: 2.20

Diurnal COV: 0.84Nocturnal COV: 1.82

Diurnal COV: 1.21Nocturnal COV: 1.57

Diurnal COV: 1.58Nocturnal COV: 2.77

Diurnal COV: 1.55Nocturnal COV: 1.55

162

Fig. C1 Average number of detections per day for each tagged Kyphosus vaigiensis (±

SE) plotted across the acoustic array (plotted from the Northern to the South point of

Orpheus Island). Diurnal and nocturnal detections are plotted separately.

1239

VR

2W-109081

VR

2W-101865

VR

2W-109071

VR

2W-109073

VR

2W-109082

VR

2W-109074

VR

2W-109066

VR

2W-109072

VR

2W-112617

VR

2W-112610

VR

2W-112611

VR

2W-105219

VR

2W-105221

VR

2W-112624

VR

2W-105222

VR

2W-112618

VR

2W-105217

VR

2W-112622

VR

2W-112621

VR

2W-112616

VR

2W-105215

VR

2W-109079

VR

2W-112612

VR

2W-105220

VR

2W-105214

VR

2W-105216

VR

2W-112623

VR

2W-112614

VR

2W-112613

VR

2W-109026

VR

2W-105213

VR

2W-112615

VR

2W-112620

VR

2W-105218

VR

2W-112619

VR

2W-109069

VR

2W-109070

VR

2W-109077

VR

2W-109080

VR

2W-109068

30

25

20

15

10

5

0

Ave

rage

det

ectio

ns.d

ay-1

Diurnal Nocturnal

Diurnal COV: 1.21Nocturnal COV: 1.57

163

Appendix D: Supplementary Materials for Chapter 6

Table D1 Average abundance of taxa at algal deployment sites during pre, during and post

algal treatments. Data were pooled across sites.

Species Average abundance

Pre Algae Post

Grazers

Acanthurus pyroferus 1 0 0

A. aurenticavus 0 0 0

A. blochii 11 2.3 1

A. grammoptilus 0 0 0

A. lineatus 0 0 0

A. nigricauda 0 1.5 0

A. nigrofuscus 5.7 5.5 4.8

A. olivaceus 2 1 1.5

A. thompsoni 0 0 0

Centropyge bicolor 4 0 0

C. vroliki 2 0 0

Ctenochaetus striatus 8.5 7.9 8.3

Pomacanthus semicirculatus 0 0 0

P. sextriatus 1 1 1.5

Siganus argenteus 0 0 1

S. corallinus 3.2 2.8 3.4

S. lineatus 0 0 0

S. puellus 1 1 1.5

S. punctatus 1 1.6 1.5

S. punctatissimus 1 0 1.3

S. vulpinus 0 0 1.6

Zebrasoma scopas 1.8 2.3 1.8

Z. veliferum 1.5 1 1

Scrapers

Hipposcarus longiceps 1 0 1

Scarus spp. 2.5 0 0

S. quoyi 1 0 0

S. chameleon 2 1 1.2

S. dimidiatus 1 1 1.5

S. flavipectoralis 0 0 1

164

S. frenatus 1.1 1 1

S. ghobban 1 2 1

S. globiceps 1 0 0

S. niger 1.6 1.8 2

S. oviceps 1.5 1.6 1.4

S. psittacus 1.4 2.2 1.3

S. rivulatus 2.9 3.5 2.8

S. rubroviolaceus 1 0 1

S. schlegeli 2.7 2 2.3

S. spinus 1 0 0

Excavators

Cetoscarus bicolor 4 0 0

Chlorurus bleekeri 3 0 1

C. microrhinos 1.3 1.5 2.5

C. sordidus 3.9 2.1 3.8

Browsers

Calotomus carolinus* 0 1.6 1

Kyphosus cinerascens* 1.4 4.2 1.5

K. vaigiensis 1.25 20 3

Naso lituratus 3 2 2.3

N. unicornis 3.8 29.8 2.1

S. doliatus* 2 16 3 *Despite these taxa often being regarded as grazers, we noted them feeding on, and removing

significant quantities of macroalgae and were thus, for our purposes, counted as browsers.

165

Table D2 Results from three-way MANOVA comparing the abundance of herbivore

functional groups across study locations, sites and treatments.

Source of

variation

Test statistic

(Pillai’s trace)

F Hypothesis df Error df P

Location (L) 0.74 1.99 4 99 0.102

Site(Location) (S) 0.320 3.018 12 303 0.001

Treatment (T) 0.593 10.537 8 200 <0.001

Interaction

(S.T)

0.824 4.412 24 408 <0.001

Interaction

(L.T)

0.55 0.705 8 2000 0.687

Table D3 Multiple comparisons of MANOVA results using Least Significant Difference

analysis, using t-tests, to identify significant differences between treatment subsets using

square-root transformed abundance data from each herbivore functional group. Bold values

are significant.

Dependent Variable

(I) Treatment (J) Treatment Mean Difference (I-J)

Std. Error P

Grazers

Algal treatment Post algal treatment -0.0714 0.19293 0.712

Pre algal treatment -0.2547 0.19293 0.190 Post algal treatment

Algal treatment 0.0714 0.19293 0.712 Pre algal treatment -0.1833 0.19293 0.345

Pre algal treatment

Algal treatment 0.2547 0.19293 0.190 Post algal treatment 0.1833 0.19293 0.345

Scrapers

Algal treatment Post algal treatment 0.0797 0.21140 0.707

Pre algal treatment -0.2810 0.21140 0.187 Post algal treatment

Algal treatment -0.0797 0.21140 0.707 Pre algal treatment -0.3607 0.21140 0.091

Pre algal treatment

Algal treatment 0.2810 0.21140 0.187 Post algal treatment 0.3607 0.21140 0.091

Excavators Algal treatment

Post algal treatment -0.0782 0.21832 0.721

Pre algal treatment -0.2564 0.21832 0.243 Post algal treatment

Algal treatment 0.0782 0.21832 0.721 Pre algal treatment -0.1783 0.21832 0.416

166

Pre algal treatment

Algal treatment 0.2564 0.21832 0.243 Post algal treatment 0.1783 0.21832 0.416

Browsers

Algal treatment

Post algal treatment 1.8772* 0.21428 0.000

Pre algal treatment 2.0287* 0.21428 0.000

Post algal treatment

Algal treatment -1.8772* 0.21428 0.000 Pre algal treatment 0.1516 0.21428 0.481

Pre algal treatment

Algal treatment -2.0287* 0.21428 0.000 Post algal treatment -0.1516 0.21428 0.481

167

Table D4 Results of one-way ANOVAs to identify the herbivore functional groups that

significantly differ within the factors included in the three-way MANOVA. Bold values are

significant.

Source Dependent Variable

Type III Sum of Squares

df MS F P

Location (L)

Grazers 1.066 1 1.066 1.423 .236 Scrapers .276 1 .276 .283 .596 Excavators .657 1 .657 .686 .410 Browsers 2.351 1 2.351 2.483 .118

Treatment (T)

Grazers 1.381 2 .690 .922 .401 Scrapers 2.873 2 1.436 1.475 .234 Excavators 1.382 2 .691 .721 .489 Browsers 102.167 2 51.084 53.966 .000

Site(L)

Grazers 5.614 3 1.871 2.498 .064 Scrapers 5.285 3 1.762 1.810 .150 Excavators 6.676 3 2.225 2.322 .080 Browsers 9.698 3 3.233 3.415 .020

L * T

Grazers 1.596 2 .798 1.066 .348 Scrapers .977 2 .488 .502 .607 Excavators 1.080 2 .540 .563 .571 Browsers 3.279 2 1.639 1.732 .182

Site(L) * Treatment

Grazers 6.697 6 1.116 1.490 .189 Scrapers 10.947 6 1.824 1.874 .092 Excavators 6.659 6 1.110 1.158 .335 Browsers 144.092 6 24.015 25.370 .000

Error

Grazers 76.401 102 .749

Scrapers 99.299 102 .974

Excavators 97.770 102 .959

Browsers 96.552 102 .947

Total

Grazers 1690.000 120

Scrapers 1118.000 120

Excavators 404.000 120

Browsers 1704.000 120

168

Table D5 Number of each herbivorous taxa captured and successfully monitored and their

estimated abundance at each study site.

Species Site Number tagged

(average size; range

[cm])

Mean abundance ±

SE (max) based on

data from visual

census

S. vulpinus Mermaid 4 (22; 21.5 – 22.5) 1 ± 1.0 (4)

S. vulpinus Turtle 2 (22.8; 22.5 – 23) 0.25 ± 0.25 (2)

S. corallinus Mermaid 4 (20.3; 18.5 – 22) 5 ± 2.6 (10)

S. corallinus Turtle 2 (22.3; 22 – 22.5) 3.75 ± 2.5 (4)

Sc. schlegeli Mermaid 8 (24.4; 17.5 – 29.5) 8.8 ± 1.8 (13)

Sc. schlegeli Turtle 9 (25.9; 19.5 –31) 15 ± 1.7 (20)

N. unicornis Mermaid 2 (24.8; 20 – 28) 3 ± 0.9 (5)

N. unicornis Turtle 1 (22.9; no range) 1.5 ± 0.3 (2)

Table D6 Average fish abundance in Mermaid Cove and Turtle Bay

derived from underwater visual transects.

Average fish abundance per 40 m2

± SE

Species Mermaid Turtle

Acanthurus blochii 1 ± 1 1 ± 1

A. lineatus 19.25 ± 4.71 5 ± 2.68

A. nigrocauda 2 ± 0.91 0.25 ± 0.25

A. nigrofuscus 17 ± 8.37 0.25 ± 0.25

A. olivaceus 1 ± 0.58 1.25 ± 0.48

A. pyroferus 0.25 ± 0.25 18.25 ± 5.12

Calotomus carolinus 0.25 ± 0.25 3.75 ± 1.89

Cetoscarus bicolor 1.25 ± 0.63 1 ± 0.41

Chlorurus bleekeri 0.25 ± 0.25 1.5 ± 0.29

C. microrhinos 1 ± 0 9.75 ± 3.40

C. sordidus 10.5 ± 2.22 24.25 ± 3.09

Ctenochaetus binotatus 0.25 ± 0.25 0.25 ± 0.25

C. striatus 61.75 ± 8.21 3 ± 1.08

Kyphosus vaigiensis 5.75 ± 2.02 4.5 ± 1.32

Naso brachycentron 0.25 ± 0.25 4 ± 4

169

N. brevirostris 3.5 ± 2.36 5.25 ± 4.03

N. lituratus 12 ± 7.31 1.5 ± 0.29

N. unicornis 3 ± 0.91 1.5 ± 1.5

Pomacanthus sextriatus 2 ± 0.91 2.5 ± 1.19

Scarus dimidiatus 2.25 ± 0.94 1 ± 0.41

S. flavipectoralis 1 ± 0.41 0.75 ± 0.48

S. frenatus 4 ± 1.77 0.75 ± 0.48

S. ghobban 0.25 ± 0.25 1.25 ± 0.48

S. globiceps 0.75 ± 0.48 1.5 ± 0.96

S. niger 4.75 ± 1.25 0.25 ± 0.25

S. oviceps 3 ± 0.82 3.75 ± 1.38

S. quoyi 0.25 ± 0.25 1 ± 0.71

S. rivulatus 6.25 ± 2.50 0.5 ± 0.29

S. rubroviolaceus 1.25 ± 0.48 24 ± 4.60

S. schlegeli 8.75 ± 1.75 15 ± 1.73

Siganus argenteus 0.75 ± 0.75 0.25 ± 0.25

S. corallinus 5 ± 2.61 3.75 ± 2.5

S. doliatus 8.5 ± 1.5 3.75 ± 0.25

S. puellus 1.5 ± 0.96 25.5 ± 3.97

S. punctatus 0.5 ± 0.29 0.25 ± 0.25

S. vulpinus 1 ± 1 0.25 ± 0.25

Zebrasoma scopas 7.75 ± 2.50 4.75 ± 2.32

Z. veliferum 1 ± 0.41 0.5 ± 0.5

Table D7 One sample t-test comparing the mean change in detections at individual’s core

receiver after algae had been deployed to 0. Data were separated into individuals which were

(a) residents at the site of the phase shift and (b) residents at other areas of the reef.

Species t df P

(a)

S. vulpinus 1.528 3 0.224

S. corallinus -1.014 5 0.357

Scarus schlegeli -1.564 8 0.156

N. unicornis 0.989 2 0.427

170

(b)

S. schlegeli -1.314 10 0.218

Fig. D1 Map of the study locations used in Mermaid Cove and Turtle Bay at Lizard

Island with algal deployment sites highlighted. Sold dots along the reef crest represents

a receiver deployment location and white dots represent a combined video and receiver

deployment location.

150 m

Acoustic receiver with video monitoringSimulated phase shift site

N

Acoustic receiver

Mermaid cove

Turtle beach

171

Fig. D2 Non-metric multidimensional scaling (nMDS) analysis of showing the

relationship between 8 locations at three different treatment levels (pre, during and post

algal deployment) based on the abundance of herbivorous fish taxa excluding browsers.

Ellipses represent significant groupings identified by ANOSIM. Biplots indicate the

relative contribution of each species to the observed pattern. Vector lengths represent

correlation of the abundance of each taxa to the fist two nMDS dimensions and are

proportional to the squared multiple correlation coefficient (R2). Grey triangles ( )

represent control sites while algae were present, inverted black triangles ( ) represent

treatment sites while algae were present, unfilled triangles ( ) represent all sites prior to

algal treatment and black triangles ( ) represent all sites post-algal treatment. 2D stress:

0.21.

A. blochii

A. nigricauda

A. olivaceus

P. sextriatus

Sc. oviceps

Sc. psittacus

Sc. schlegeli

S. corallinus

S. puellus

172

Appendix E: Assessment of the impact of acoustic tagging

on fish mortality and behaviour Several studies have quantified the effects of tagging fish with acoustic transmitters,

with the goal of estimating behavioural modifications or enhanced mortality as a result

of the tagging process (Zeller 1999; Lower et al. 2005; Bellquist et al. 2008). These

works have overwhelmingly found that the internal tagging of fishes has little to no

effect on fish, with few exceptions. This is especially true for more recent studies that

employ refined surgical techniques and use smaller transmitters (Hutchinson and

Rhodes 2010; Marshell et al. 2011; Welsh and Bellwood 2012a, b). Indeed, the studies

enclosed in the present thesis corroborate this finding. Of the fish tagged herein, very

few were recorded to have died immediately following transmitter implantation (Table

E1). This suggests that the acute effects of internal tagging on the survival of reef fishes

appear to be minimal. However, behavioural modifications as a result of tagging have

been suggested to undermine the results of studies that utilize acoustic tagging.

The combination of movement data into ecological studies is among the novel

aspects of the present studies, and also affords us the ability to directly assess the

changes in a fish’s behavior following tagging. By monitoring a fish’s activity

following tagging, in conjunction with conspecifics, it is possible to identify underlying

subtle behavioural changes that may arise as a result of tagging. This is reported in

Welsh and Bellwood (2012a) in which the home range of Chlorurus microrhinos was

monitored using intensive active acoustic tracking. The study also monitored the

movements and behaviours of the tagged individual’s social groups to identify any

irregularities from the tagged specimen. Despite being tagged with an internal

transmitter, within hours of being released onto the reef, tagged fishes exhibited no

irregular behavior and moved with other harem members. Furthermore, these

173

individuals exhibited no change in social status or sex during the period of the study. In

the present studies, the behaviour of another parrotfish Scarus rivulatus was assessed in

a similar manner. Both tagged and non-tagged individuals were simultaneously

monitored for several weeks after tagging with their feeding behaviours, schooling

proclivities and social status recorded. Despite the intensive monitoring of tagged

individuals, no irregularities were detected and the movements and feeding of tagged

fishes remained indistinguishable from their conspecifics.

Unusual bahviours have however been reported following tagging however, the

explanation for these irregularities may not be directly attributed to the implanted

transmitter. Chateau and Wantiez (2009) report an individual Chlorurus microrhinos

moving 6 km, between reefs, following release. This finding is in direct contrast to the

findings of Welsh and Bellwood (2012a) for the same species, which only report highly

site-attached behaviours with restricted movements. This distinction is likely to arise as

a result of the tagging procedures used in the two studies. In the former study,

individuals are held in captivity for long periods of time (4.3 + 2.7 days) while the latter

kept individuals captive for a maximum of 24 h. Such excessive recovery periods in

captivity may have cause the C. microrhinos to be displaced from their social group or

territory resulting in a shift from site-attached, normal movements, to dispersive

movements as they search for a new territory or social group to join (Ogden and

Buckman 1973; van Rooij et al. 1996). Thus, while captive holding times of several

days are not unusual in the literature (e.g. Marshell et al. 2011), and have been highly

recommended by some works (Zeller 1999), it is important to consider the behavioural

implications of such removal times, and how these practices may fundamentally alter

the ‘normal’ movements of some taxa.

174

Table E1 Summary of the number of mortalities that may be attributed to tagging affects

from studies contained within the present thesis. For the purposes of this evaluation, mortality

was defined as unusual movements followed by signal loss.

Species Study site Total tagged Percent within one-

week post-tagging.

Scarus rivulatus Orpheus Island 18 0%

Kyphosus vaigiensis Orpheus Island 14 0%

Scarus schlegeli Lizard Island 17 17.6%

Naso unicornis Lizard Island 3 0%

Siganus vulpinus Lizard Island 6 0%

Siganus corallinus Lizard Island 6 0%

References

Bellquist LF, Lowe CG, Caselle JE (2008) Fine-scale movement patterns, site fidelity

and habitat selection of ocean whitefish (Caulolatilus princeps). Fish Res 91:325-

335

Chateau O, Wantiez L (2009) Movement patterns of four coral reef fish species in a

fragmented habitat in New Caledonia: implications for the design of marine

protected area networks. ICES J Mar Sci 66:50-55

Lower N, Moore A, Scott AP, Ellis T, James JD, Russell IC (2005) A non-invasive

method to assess the impact of electronic tag insertion on stress levels in fishes. J

Fish Biol 67:1202-1212

Ogden JC, Buckman NC (1973) Movements, foraging groups, and diurnal migrations of

the striped parrotfish Scarus croicensis Bloch (Scaridae). Ecology 54:589-596

Zeller DC (1999) Ultrasonic telemetry: its application to coral reef fisheries research.

Fish Bull 97:1058-1065

175

Appendix F: Publications arising from thesis

Publications derived from thesis chapters

Welsh JQ, Fox RJ, Webber DM, Bellwood DR (2012) Performance of remote acoustic

receivers within a coral reef habitat: implications for array design. Coral Reefs

31:693-702

Welsh JQ, Bellwood DR (2012) How far do schools of roving herbivores rove? A case

study using Scarus rivulatus. Coral Reefs 31: 991-1003

Welsh JQ, Goatley CHR Bellwood DR (2013) The ontogeny of home ranges: evidence

from coral reef fishes. Proc R Soc B 280: 20132066

Welsh JQ, Bellwood DR (2014) Herbivorous fishes, ecosystem function and mobile

links on coral reefs. Coral Reefs 33:303-311

Welsh JQ, Bellwood DR (in review) Local degradation triggers a large-scale response

on coral reefs. PLoS ONE.

Additional publications arising during PhD candidature

Welsh JQ, Bonaldo RM, Bellwood DR (2014) Clustered parrotfish feeding scars trigger

partial coral mortality of massive Porites colonies on the inshore Great Barrier

Reef. Coral Reefs in press

Welch MJ, Watson SA, Welsh JQ, McCormick MI, Munday PL (2014) Effects of

elevated CO2 on fish behaviour undiminished by transgenerational acclimation.

Nat Clim Chang in press

Bonaldo RM, Welsh JQ, Bellwood DR (2012) Spatial and temporal variation in coral

predation by parrotfishes on the GBR: evidence from an inshore reef. Coral

Reefs 31: 263-272