the paradox of / ɫ/-vocalisation in romance: phonetics ... · the paradox of / ɫ/-vocalisation in...

1
The paradox of /ɫ/-vocalisation in Romance: phonetics, phonotactics, and frequency effects Daniela Müller Universitat de Tolosa 2 – Lo Miralh & Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg [email protected] 1. Background The phenomenon: /ɫ/ > ɰ, w, ʊ̯ , ... preconsonantally or word-finally frequently found in, e.g., among Indo-European languages, in Germanic, Slavic, Romance, etc. Example: Lat. “alterum” > Fr. “autre”, Occ. “autre”, Sp. “otro”, Port. “outro” Dark quality of the Latin syllable-final lateral: L triplicem, ut Plinio [1st c. CE] uidetur, sonum habet: […] plenum, quando finit nomina vel syllabas […], ut ‚sol’, ‚silva’, […]. (“According to Plinius, l has three kinds of quality: […] dark (‘full’) when it is in word-final or syllable-final position […], as in ‘sol’, ‘silva’, […].”) Priscianus (6th c. CE), Institutiones grammaticares, I, 38 (Keil 1961b: 29) First indirect attestation from Suetonius (1st c. CE), De Vita Caesarum, Book III: Tiberius 42: 1: wordplay where the emperor Tiberius had been given the nickname “Caldius” (‘mulled wine’) instead of his real first name “Claudius”. First direct attestation in an edict of the Roman emperor Diocletian from 301 CE: “καυκουλατορι”(Leumann 1977: 142, §148d,ε). The Romance /ɫ/-vocalisation was initiated in Late Latin/Proto- Romance and completed during the Middle Ages. 2. The Romance paradox Observations from ongoing /ɫ/-vocalisation and synchronic variation (e.g., from English, Brazilian Portuguese (Caffee 1940, Espiga 2002, among many others)) suggest that •/ɫ/ before labial and velar consonants vocalises first, • followed by vocalisation of word-final /ɫ/, •/ɫ/ before dental-alveolars vocalises last, if at all. A following labial or velar consonant contributes to further darkening of the lateral and confusion with velar/labio-velar approximants; Following silence masks tongue tip contact release; The homorganicity in /ɫ + alveolar/-clusters prevents tongue tip contact loss. But: Both historical and dialectological data suggest that /ɫ/- vocalisation in Romance • occurred first before alveolar consonants, often exclusively so; • sometimes only when the preceding vowel was /a/. Perceptual dissimilation in /aɫt, aɫd/ > /awt, awd/ (e.g., Recasens 1996)? Can frequency effects contribute to a better understanding of the Romance paradox? 3. Method, materials Token frequencies of /VɫC/ and /VwC/-sequences V = /a, e, i, o, u/ C = /p, b, t, d, k, g/ from the Perseus under PhiloLogic corpus of Latin texts (3,2 million words of poetry, theater and prose texts from 25 different authors, ranging from the 3rd c. BCE to the 4th c. CE) Type frequencies of the same /VɫC/ and /VwC/-sequences from the Lewis & Short Latin-English Dictionary (L&S, electronic version). (/iwC/-sequences could not be counted because the graphical representation “iuC” also refers to /juC/-sequences.) 4. Theoretical predictions References BYBEE, JOAN (2001), Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CAFFEE, NATHANIEL M. (1940), “Southern ‘L’ plus a Consonant”. In American Speech 15:3; 259-261. ESPIGA, JORGE (2002), « A lateral posvocálica na fronteira dos Campos Neutrais: estudo sociolingüístico da regra telescópica nos dialetos de Chuí e Santa Vitória do Palmar ». In Letras de Hoje 37: 1; 49-68. KEIL, HEINRICH (ed.) (1961b [1855], Grammatici Latini II. Prisciani grammatici caesariensis Institutionum Grammaticarum Libri XVIII (Martin Hertz, ed.). Hildesheim: Georg Olms. LEUMANN, MANU (1977 [1926-1928]), Lateinische Laut- und Formenlehre. München: Beck’sche Verlagsbuchhandlung. Purely phonetically motivated sound change takes place faster in sequences of high token frequency, analogical change is due to the application of one phonotactic schema ranging over more types than another, similar, one. (based on Bybee 2001) Can the Romance paradox be better understood when taking into account analogical sound change? 5. Results a)/aɫt, aɫd/ a) /Vɫ, Vw + alveolar/ a_t e_t i_t o_t u_t l w 0 2000 4000 6000 8000 numbe r of occurrences per million words vowel context type of liquid Frequency of occurrence of Vlt and Vwt-sequences l w a_d e_d i_d o_d u_d l w 0 1000 2000 3000 4000 number of occurrences per million words vowel context type of liquid Frequency of occurrence of Vld and Vwd-sequences l w b) /Vɫ, Vw + labial/ a_p e_p i_p o_p u_p l 0 100 200 300 400 number of occurrences per million words vowel context type of liquid Frequency of occurrence of Vlp and Vwp-sequences l w a_b e_b i_b o_b u_b l w 0 100 200 300 400 500 number of occurrences per million words vowel context type of liquids Frequency of occurrence of Vlb and Vwb-sequences l w c) /Vɫ, Vw + velar/ a_k e_k i_k o_k u_k l w 0 500 1000 1500 number of occurrences per million words vowel context type of liquid Frequency of occurrence of Vlk and Vwk-sequences l w a_g e_g i_g o_g u_g l w 0 100 200 300 400 500 number of occurrences per million words vowel context type of liquid Frequency of occurrence of Vlg and Vwg-sequences l w 542 2043,768 /Vw + velar/ 441 1739,634 /Vɫ + velar/ Type frequency (words in L&S) Token frequency (per million words) 345 20742,434 /Vw + alveolar/ 510 7184,314 /Vɫ + alveolar/ Type frequency (words in L&S) Token frequency (per million words) 55 465,183 /Vw + velar/ 247 1018,632 /Vɫ + labial/ Type frequency (words in L&S) Token frequency (per million words) 6. Discussion 330 10260,341 /aw + alveolar/ 141 2117,479 /aɫ + alveolar/ Type frequency (words in L&S) Token frequency (per million words) Strong enhancement of /ɫ/-vocalisation by phonotactic generalisations. The absence of vocalisation in /uɫt/-sequences (“multum” > “mucho”, „muito“) in Spanish and Portuguese may be due to this schema being better represented than any other /Vɫ+alveolar/ schema without having a competing /uw+alveolar/ schema, in addition to tongue tip contact preservation by articulatory homorganicity. b) /uɫt, uɫd/ 0 0 /uw + alveolar/ 330 4889,372 /uɫ + alveolar/ Type frequency (words in L&S) Token frequency (per million words) The fact that the schema /uɫ+velar/ has a similar strength as /uɫ+alveolar/ suggests that it, too, would constitute an exception to /ɫ/-vocalisation – this not being the case, it seems that in favourable phonetic contexts, regular phonetic sound change may override phonotactics. 0 0 /uw + velar/ 1334 not known /u + velar/ 243 1199,502 /uɫ + velar/ Type frequency (words in L&S) Token frequency (per million words) c) /uɫk, uɫg/ With the /Vɫ+labial/-sequences, the existing /Vw+labial/ schema seems too weak to allow for a strong phonotactic influence; /ɫ/-vocalisation seems to be grounded in articulation and perception only. d) /Vɫ, Vw + labial/ 7. Upshot The study of frequency effects, together with our knowledge of the historical developments and the articulatory and acoustic- perceptual underpinnings of the sound change can enlarge our understanding of /ɫ/-vocalisation in the Romance languages. Acknowledgments: This poster was made possible by a PhD scholarship (Landesgraduiertenförderung) of the State of Baden-Württemberg, Germany. I would like to thank Ana Paula Huback who was involved in early stages of this research. Of course, all errors are mine. LEWIS, CHARLTON T. & CHARLES SHORT (1879), A Latin dictionary, founded on Andrew’s edition of Freund’s Latin dictionary, revised, enlarged, and in great part rewritten. Oxford: Clarendon Press. http://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/resolveform?redirect=true&lang=Latin . (accessed 15/02/2011) Perseus under PhiloLogic corpus of Latin texts, under the supervision of HELMA DIK: http://perseus.uchicago.edu/. (accessed 15/02/2011) RECASENS, DANIEL (1996), “An articulatory-perceptual account of vocalization and elision of dark /l/ in the Romance languages”. In Language and Speech 39:1; 63-89. Phonetics & Phonology in Iberia 2011 – Tarragona, 21-22 June 2011

Upload: others

Post on 21-Oct-2019

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The paradox of / ɫ/-vocalisation in Romance: phonetics ... · The paradox of / ɫ/-vocalisation in Romance: phonetics, phonotactics, and frequency effects Daniela Müller Universitat

The paradox of /ɫ/-vocalisation in Romance: phonetics, phonotactics, and frequency effects

Daniela Müller

Universitat de Tolosa 2 – Lo Miralh & Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg

[email protected]

1. BackgroundThe phenomenon:

/ɫ/ > ɰ, w, ʊ̯, ... preconsonantally or word-finally

frequently found in, e.g., among Indo-European languages, in Germanic, Slavic, Romance, etc.

Example:

Lat. “alterum” > Fr. “autre”, Occ. “autre”, Sp. “otro”, Port. “outro”

Dark quality of the Latin syllable-final lateral:L triplicem, ut Plinio [1st c. CE] uidetur, sonum habet: […] plenum, quando finitnomina vel syllabas […], ut ‚sol’, ‚silva’, […].

(“According to Plinius, l has three kinds of quality: […] dark (‘full’) when it is in word-final or syllable-final position […], as in ‘sol’, ‘silva’, […].”) Priscianus (6th c. CE), Institutiones grammaticares, I, 38 (Keil1961b: 29)

First indirect attestation from Suetonius (1st c. CE), De Vita Caesarum, Book III: Tiberius 42: 1: wordplay where the emperor Tiberius had been given the nickname “Caldius” (‘mulled wine’) instead of his real first name “Claudius”.

First direct attestation in an edict of the Roman emperor Diocletianfrom 301 CE: “καυκουλατορι”(Leumann 1977: 142, §148d,ε).

The Romance /ɫ/-vocalisation was initiated in Late Latin/Proto-Romance and completed during the Middle Ages.

2. The Romance paradox

Observations from ongoing /ɫ/-vocalisation and synchronic variation (e.g., from English, Brazilian Portuguese (Caffee 1940, Espiga 2002, among many others)) suggest that

• /ɫ/ before labial and velar consonants vocalises first,

• followed by vocalisation of word-final /ɫ/,

• /ɫ/ before dental-alveolars vocalises last, if at all.

�A following labial or velar consonant contributes to further darkening of the lateral and confusion with velar/labio-velar approximants;

�Following silence masks tongue tip contact release;

� The homorganicity in /ɫ + alveolar/-clusters prevents tongue tip contact loss.

But: Both historical and dialectological data suggest that /ɫ/-

vocalisation in Romance

• occurred first before alveolar consonants, often exclusively so;

• sometimes only when the preceding vowel was /a/.

� Perceptual dissimilation in /aɫt, aɫd/ > /awt, awd/ (e.g., Recasens 1996)?

Can frequency effects contribute to a better understanding of the Romance paradox?

3. Method, materialsToken frequencies of /VɫC/ and /VwC/-sequences

V = /a, e, i, o, u/

C = /p, b, t, d, k, g/

from the Perseus under PhiloLogic corpus of Latin texts (3,2 million words of poetry, theater and prose texts from 25 different authors, ranging from the 3rd c. BCE to the 4th c. CE)

Type frequencies of the same /VɫC/ and /VwC/-sequencesfrom the Lewis & Short Latin-English Dictionary (L&S, electronic version).

(/iwC/-sequences could not be counted because the graphical representation “iuC” also refers to /juC/-sequences.)

4. Theoretical predictions

ReferencesBYBEE, JOAN (2001), Phonology and Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CAFFEE, NATHANIEL M. (1940), “Southern ‘L’ plus a Consonant”. In American Speech 15:3; 259-261.ESPIGA, JORGE (2002), « A lateral posvocálica na fronteira dos Campos Neutrais: estudo sociolingüístico da regra telescópica nos dialetos de Chuí e Santa Vitória do Palmar ». In Letras de Hoje 37: 1; 49-68.KEIL, HEINRICH (ed.) (1961b [1855], Grammatici Latini II. Prisciani grammatici caesariensis Institutionum Grammaticarum Libri XVIII (Martin Hertz, ed.). Hildesheim: Georg Olms.LEUMANN, MANU (1977 [1926-1928]), Lateinische Laut- und Formenlehre. München: Beck’sche Verlagsbuchhandlung.

Purely phonetically motivated sound change takes place faster in sequences of high token frequency,

analogical change is due to the application of one phonotactic schemaranging over more types than another, similar, one.(based on Bybee 2001)

� Can the Romance paradox be better understood when taking into account analogical sound change?

5. Results

a)/aɫt, aɫd/

a) /Vɫ, Vw + alveolar/

a_t e_t i_to_t u_t

lw

0

2000

4000

6000

8000

number of occurrences per

million words

vowel context

type of liquid

Frequency of occurrence of Vlt and Vwt-sequences

l

w

a_d e_d i_d o_d u_d

lw0

1000

2000

3000

4000

number of occurrences per

million words

vowel context

type of liquid

Frequency of occurrence of Vld and Vwd-sequences

l

w

b) /Vɫ, Vw + labial/

a_p e_p i_p o_p u_p

l0

100

200

300

400

number of occurrences per

million words

vowel context

type of liquid

Frequency of occurrence of Vlp and Vwp-sequences

l

w

a_b e_b i_b o_b u_b

lw0

100

200

300

400

500

number of occurrences per

million words

vowel context

type of liquids

Frequency of occurrence of Vlb and Vwb-sequences

l

w

c) /Vɫ, Vw + velar/

a_k e_k i_k o_k u_k

lw0

500

1000

1500

number of occurrences per

million words

vowel context

type of liquid

Frequency of occurrence of Vlk and Vwk-sequences

l

w

a_g e_g i_go_g u_g

lw

0

100

200

300

400

500

number of occurrences per

million words

vowel context

type of liquid

Frequency of occurrence of Vlg and Vwg-sequences

l

w

5422043,768 /Vw + velar/

4411739,634/Vɫ + velar/

Type frequency (words in L&S)

Token frequency

(per million words)

34520742,434/Vw + alveolar/

5107184,314/Vɫ + alveolar/

Type frequency (words in L&S)

Token frequency

(per million words)

55465,183/Vw + velar/

2471018,632/Vɫ + labial/

Type frequency (words in L&S)

Token frequency

(per million words)

6. Discussion

33010260,341/aw + alveolar/

1412117,479/aɫ + alveolar/

Type frequency (words in L&S)

Token frequency

(per million words)

� Strong enhancement of /ɫ/-vocalisation by phonotactic generalisations.

� The absence of vocalisation in /uɫt/-sequences (“multum” > “mucho”, „muito“) in Spanish and Portuguese may be due to thisschema being better represented than any other /Vɫ+alveolar/ schema without having a competing /uw+alveolar/ schema, in addition to tongue tip contact preservation by articulatoryhomorganicity.

b) /uɫt, uɫd/

00/uw + alveolar/

3304889,372/uɫ + alveolar/

Type frequency (words in L&S)

Token frequency

(per million words)

� The fact that the schema /uɫ+velar/ has a similar strength as /uɫ+alveolar/ suggests that it, too, would constitute an exception to /ɫ/-vocalisation – this not being the case, it seems that in favourable phonetic contexts, regular phonetic sound change may override phonotactics.

00/uw + velar/

1334not known/u + velar/

2431199,502/uɫ + velar/

Type frequency (words in L&S)

Token frequency

(per million words)c) /uɫk, uɫg/

�With the /Vɫ+labial/-sequences, the existing /Vw+labial/ schema seems too weak to allow for a strong phonotacticinfluence; /ɫ/-vocalisation seems to be grounded in articulationand perception only.

d) /Vɫ, Vw + labial/

7. Upshot

The study of frequency effects, together with our knowledge of the historical developments and the articulatory and acoustic-perceptual underpinnings of the sound change can enlarge our understanding of /ɫ/-vocalisation in the Romance languages.

Acknowledgments: This poster was made possible by a PhD scholarship(Landesgraduiertenförderung) of the State of Baden-Württemberg, Germany.

I would like to thank Ana Paula Huback who was involved in early stages of thisresearch. Of course, all errors are mine.

LEWIS, CHARLTON T. & CHARLES SHORT (1879), A Latin dictionary, founded on Andrew’s edition of Freund’s Latin dictionary, revised, enlarged, and in great part rewritten. Oxford: Clarendon Press. http://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/resolveform?redirect=true&lang=Latin. (accessed 15/02/2011)Perseus under PhiloLogic corpus of Latin texts, under the supervision of HELMA DIK: http://perseus.uchicago.edu/. (accessed 15/02/2011)RECASENS, DANIEL (1996), “An articulatory-perceptual account of vocalization and elision of dark /l/ in the Romance languages”. In Language and Speech 39:1; 63-89.

Phonetics & Phonology in Iberia 2011 – Tarragona, 21-22 June 2011