the development and design of an … · traditional classroom schooling (baxter-magolda, 1998)....

87
THE DEVELOPMENT AND DESIGN OF AN INSTRUMENT TO MEASURE SELF-AUTHORSHIP IN OUTDOOR EDUCATION PROGRAMS BY Taras Ferencevych Bachelor of Arts, Rutgers College, 1996 THESIS Submitted to the University of New Hampshire in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science in Kinesiology September, 2004

Upload: phungkhanh

Post on 08-Sep-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

THE DEVELOPMENT AND DESIGN OF AN INSTRUMENT TO MEASURE SELF-AUTHORSHIP IN OUTDOOR EDUCATION PROGRAMS

BY

Taras Ferencevych Bachelor of Arts, Rutgers College, 1996

THESIS

Submitted to the University of New Hampshire in Partial Fulfillment of

the Requirements for the Degree of

Master of Science

in

Kinesiology

September, 2004

This thesis has been examined and approved.

________________________________ Thesis Director, Dr. Keith Russell, Assistant Professor of Kinesiology ________________________________ Dr. Michael A. Gass, Professor of Kinesiology ________________________________

Laurie Gullion, Clinical Professor of Kinesiology ________________________________

Date

iii

DEDICATION

This thesis is dedicated to my parents who made me believe I was

invincible.

iv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

DEDICATION ................................................................................................. III

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .............................................................................. IV

LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................... VII

LIST OF FIGURES ...................................................................................... VIII

ABSTRACT ................................................................................................... IX

I. INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................... 1

STUDY OBJECTIVES ........................................................................................ 3 DEFINITION OF TERMS .................................................................................... 4 RESEARCH QUESTIONS ................................................................................... 5 LIMITATIONS ................................................................................................... 5

II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ................................................................. 6

OUTDOOR EDUCATION (OE)............................................................................ 6 SELF-AUTHORSHIP.......................................................................................... 8 CONSTRUCTS RELATED TO SA ......................................................................... 9 REASONED LINKS BETWEEN SA AND OE ........................................................ 12 RESEARCH FINDINGS LINKING SA TO OE ........................................................ 15

Self-efficacy ............................................................................................ 15 Locus of Control ..................................................................................... 19

CONCLUSIONS.............................................................................................. 21

III. RESEARCH METHODOLOGY ................................................................ 23

PARTICIPANTS .............................................................................................. 23 CASE SELECTION ......................................................................................... 25 PROCEDURES .............................................................................................. 27

Focus-group Interview ............................................................................ 27 Instrument Development ........................................................................ 28 SAQ Administration ................................................................................ 28

DATA ANALYSIS ............................................................................................ 29 Phase 1 .................................................................................................. 29 Phase 2 .................................................................................................. 30

IV. RESULTS ................................................................................................. 31

PHASE 1 ...................................................................................................... 31 Judgement/Decision Making .................................................................. 32 Self-regulation ........................................................................................ 32 Self-confidence ....................................................................................... 34 Interpersonal Skills ................................................................................. 34 Empowerment ........................................................................................ 35 Creative Problem Solving ....................................................................... 35

vi

Leadership/Responsibility ....................................................................... 36 Knowledge Creation ............................................................................... 36

PHASE 2 ...................................................................................................... 38 Participants ............................................................................................. 38 Initial Item Reduction .............................................................................. 40

V. DISCUSSION ............................................................................................ 49

INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................. 49 SUMMARY .................................................................................................... 49

Phase 1 .................................................................................................. 49 Phase 2 .................................................................................................. 50

FINDINGS AND CONCLUSIONS ........................................................................ 50 Phase 1 .................................................................................................. 50 Phase 2 .................................................................................................. 52

IMPLICATIONS ............................................................................................... 53 Phase 1 .................................................................................................. 53 Phase 2 .................................................................................................. 54

LIMITATIONS ................................................................................................. 54 Phase 1 .................................................................................................. 54 Phase 2 .................................................................................................. 56

RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH ................................................ 57

LIST OF REFERENCES ............................................................................... 59

APPENDIX A: SELF AUTHORSHIP QUESTIONNAIRE V.1 ......................... 65

APPENDIX B: SELF AUTHORSHIP QUESTIONNAIRE V.2 ......................... 70

APPENDIX C: INFORMED CONSENT LETTER ........................................... 72

APPENDIX D: OFFICE OF SPONSORED RESEARCH APPROVAL ........... 75

vii

LIST OF TABLES

TABLE 3-1: FOURTEEN TENETS OF THE PLAST PROGRAM (TYSOVSKY, 1912). ...... 26

TABLE 4-1: FOCUS-GROUP STATEMENTS AND IDENTIFIED OPEN CODES PATTERN-

CODED INTO THEMES. ............................................................................... 33

TABLE 4-2: ORIGINAL ITEMS GENERATED FOR INCLUSION IN SAQ V.1 .................. 39

TABLE 4-3: MEAN, STANDARD DEVIATION, SKEWNESS, AND KURTOSIS STATISTICS.

.............................................................................................................. 41

TABLE 4-4: ROTATED FACTOR MATRIX FOR THE FOUR FACTOR PRINCIPAL FACTOR

ANALYSIS. ............................................................................................... 46

TABLE 4-6: ITEMS INCLUDED IN IDENTIFIED SUB-SCALES. ................................... 48

viii

LIST OF FIGURES

FIGURE 4-1:THIRTY-TWO ITEM SCALE SCREE PLOT. ........................................... 43

ix

ABSTRACT

THE DEVELOPMENT AND DESIGN OF AN INSTRUMENT TO MEASURE SELF-AUTHORSHIP IN OUTDOOR EDUCATION PROGRAMS

By

Taras Ferencevych

University of New Hampshire, September 2004

Self Authorship (SA) is a growing area of interest (Baxter-Magolda, 1998;

1999; 2001; Kegan, 1994) that helps delineate specific skills for helping

young people develop into well-adjusted adults capable of dealing with the

demands of modern life. The purpose of this study was to identify domains

associated with self-authorship and to develop an instrument that could

reliably measure the construct. The study was completed in two phases.

During the first phase, a case study of the Plast Ukrainian Scouting

Organization (Plast) was completed to identify how this organization defines

self-authorship. Plast relies heavily on Outdoor Education (OE) methods in

its programs and identifies self-authorship as one of its program goals. The

domains generated from the case-study were compared to existing self-

authorship literature to develop the Self-Authorship Questionnaire (SAQ v.1).

The SAQ v. 1 was then administered to 289 young adults between the ages

of 18 and 38 (M = 21.34). The data was analyzed using Principal Factors

Analysis (PFA) to determine if distinct factors were evident in the instrument.

Four distinct factors which were identified converted into four sub-scales: (a)

x

Situational Coping, (b) Interpersonal Leadership, (c) Self-Efficacy and (d)

Knowledge Creation. The items comprising the four identified sub-scales

were retained and included in the second version of the Self-Authorship

Questionnaire (SAQ v.2). Implications for the use of the SAQ v. 2 in OE

program evaluation are presented.

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

One of the challenges of modern society is to teach youth to become

independent, self-directing adults. It is a common belief that young people

develop these skills during their college years. However, Kegan (1994)

suggests that today’s American college students are not prepared for the

rigors of life upon graduation because universities and colleges do not

provide them with an opportunity to develop and practice the necessary skills

to self-author their lives upon graduation. The author reasons that American

college graduates rely heavily on someone else (e.g. professors, teachers,

parents) for knowledge. They have, with a few exceptions, been socialized

and taught to accept knowledge from authority figures (Baxter-Magolda,

1998), a direct result of the epistemological development created by

traditional classroom schooling (Baxter-Magolda, 1998).

Self-authorship (SA) is defined as peoples’ ability to simultaneously

construct knowledge within the context of their world, define an internal

identity for themselves separate from external influences, and engage in

relationships with others without losing their internal identity (Baxter-Magolda,

1999). More specifically, SA looks at the development of inter- and

intrapersonal skills and how they make meaning of one’s experience. The

2

approach proposed by higher education proponents of SA is called

constructive developmental pedagogy (Baxter-Magolda, 1999).

Both Kegan (1994) and Baxter-Magolda (1998) suggest that the

incorporation of a constructive-developmental approach to traditional subject

matter would provide a better training for students to self-author upon

graduation. Kegan (1994) identifies three necessary components of

constructive-developmental pedagogy as (a) acknowledging the student as

knower, (b) basing education on the student’s experience, and (c) inclusion of

the student in creation of the experience. Kegan and Baxter-Magolda claim

that the implementation of these three ideas into traditional American college

curricula will help students develop ownership of the subject matter and learn

critical analysis, problem solving, and interpersonal skills, thus greatly

enhancing their ability to author their own future. More recently, Baxter-

Magolda (1999) has suggested that this type of pedagogy can and should

also be applied to traditional K-12 education.

It is my contention that Outdoor Education (OE), which bases learning

in the natural environment with emphases on group process, collaboration,

leadership, judgment and reflection (Ewert & Haywood, 1991; Gass in Miles &

Priest 1990; Kraft in Miles & Priest, 1990; Walsh & Golins, 1976), has since

its inception provided participants with the necessary skills to self-author.

Research conducted on OE outcomes (Bertolami, 1981; Ewert, 1983; Hans,

2000; Hattie, Marsh, Neill & Richards, 1997; Koesler & Propost, 1998;

Paxton, 1998; Sibthorp, 2003) has demonstrated that participation in OE

3

programs can facilitate positive change in self-efficacy and a more

internalized locus of control (LOC), both of which are closely related to SA.

Increased or enhanced self-efficacy and more internalized LOC help young

people depend on themselves for knowledge creation and direction, both of

which are critical elements of SA.

Though many concepts comprising SA have been researched at length

in OE (i.e. self-efficacy and locus of control), only recently has OE research

started to make mention of the connection between OE and self-authorship

(Gass, Garvey & Sugerman, 2003). This study will attempt to make evident

the connections between OE and SA, suggesting that this may be an

important area of research in OE in the future. This study will also attempt to

create a sound instrument for measuring self-authorship, which may be used

to empirically test the idea that OE programming facilitates the development

of SA in participants.

Study Objectives

The specific objectives of this study were: 1) to review literature to

better understand the underlying themes that define self-authorship, 2) to

explore how outdoor education processes and outcomes relate to self-

authorship, and 3) to design and pilot test a valid and reliable measure of self-

authorship.

4

Definition of Terms

Outdoor Education – Education based on the experiential philosophy of

learning by doing in the outdoors. Outdoor Education emphasizes

relationships between people and natural resources as its major modality for

learning. Outdoor education is divided down into adventure education and

environmental education with focus on four types or relationships:

interpersonal, intrapersonal, ecosystemic, and ekistic (Priest & Gass, 1997).

Self-authorship – Individuals’ ability to simultaneously construct knowledge

within the context of their world, define an internal identity for themselves

separate from external influences, and engage in relationships with others

without losing their internal identity (Baxter-Magolda, 1999).

Self-efficacy – The belief in one’s capabilities to organize and execute the

sources of action required to manage prospective situations (Bandura, 1986).

Locus of control – An individual’s belief about the relationship between his/her

actions and the outcomes of those actions (Rotter, 1966). Generally refers to

a person’s belief that he/she controls his/her own destiny or that it is

controlled by others or fate.

Plast – A 91-year-old Ukrainian scouting organization founded in Ukraine

which has existed in the United States and Canada since the late 1940’s.

5

The organization uses scouting as its main programmatic delivery method

with a heavy reliance on outdoor education programming (e.g. summer

camps, hiking, camping, canoeing, etc.).

Research Questions

1. How is self-authorship defined in the literature?

2. Do proven OE outcomes lead to self-authorship?

3. What constructs should be included in a measure of self-authorship?

4. Can a reliable measure of SA be developed?

Limitations

The researcher recognizes several limitations to this study. They are:

1) The analysis of case study data was not peer reviewed.

2) It is assumed that participants responded to the SAQ truthfully.

3) The sample number N=289 was on the lower end of the acceptable

sample size for Principal Factor Analysis (PFA).

4) The sample was not randomly selected.

5) The researcher has limited experience.

6) The results of the study are not generalizable beyond the sample.

6

CHAPTER II

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

In this chapter an overview of OE and its relation to other psychological

theories and the underlying principles of experiential and outdoor education

are explored to provide a context for the research. SA is examined primarily

by introducing the concept of constructive-development pedagogy. A review

of outdoor education research literature is provided, focusing on evidence

about outcomes related to self-authorship, particularly self-efficacy and locus

of control. Finally, in light of self-authorship theory and outdoor education

evidence pertaining to related constructs, suggestions for future research

about the connections of OE and SA are made.

Outdoor Education (OE)

The use of the outdoors for educational purposes is by no means a

novel concept. Plato promoted the use of outdoor experiences for education

and the development of healthy bodies (Hattie, Marsh, Neill & Richards,

1997). On its way to its current form outdoor education has borrowed and

added ideas from the related field of education, most notably the ideas of

John Dewey (Dewey, 1938/1997). Dewey (1938/1997) introduces the use of

7

experience as a necessary element of education. Dewey talks about the

importance of cooperation both between teacher and student and among

students (Dewey, 1938/1997). We later see this idea incorporated into major

OE theories that focus on team work and facilitator competencies (Hahn,

1957; Walsh & Golins, 1976).

Miles and Priest (1990) claim that any history of OE must start with

Kurt Hahn. Hahn believed that the use of intense adventure experiences in a

natural setting and based on increasingly difficult challenges would build an

individual’s sense of self-worth and concern for others (Hahn, 1970). Hahn

used these ideas to found several schools, most importantly Outward Bound

(Miles & Priest, 1990). Outward Bound in turn has given rise to several “spin-

offs and adaptive programs” which have influenced traditional education all

over the world, most notably in the United States (Miner in Miles & Priest,

1990). Ewert (1989) reported that in 1975 U.S. universities were offering over

200 wilderness-related adventure programs, a number that rose to more than

640 by 1984. Hattie et. al. (1997) report that in 1994 alone more than 40, 000

students participated in Outward Bound. Since the origins of Outward Bound

in the 1940’s, adventure-based programs have been used for educational,

recreational, developmental and therapeutic programming. The populations

most often associated with these programs include, but are not limited to:

adolescents, adjudicated youth, corporate, people with disabilities, children,

dysfunctional families, and psychiatric patients (Hattie, et. al., 1997; Ewert,

1989; Priest & Gass, 1997).

8

Self-authorship

Baxter-Magolda defines SA as “the capacity to author, or invent, one's

own beliefs, values, sense of self, and relationship with others" (Baxter-

Magolda, 2001, p.14). Baxter-Magolda (1999) goes on to write that adulthood

in contemporary America requires the ability for lifelong learning in order to

keep up with the rapidly changing worlds of technology, science, economics,

and culture. Kegan (1994) suggests that today’s American college students

do not possess these skills upon graduating college because colleges do not

provide students with an opportunity to develop them. American students

graduate college being heavily reliant on others (e.g. professors, teachers,

parents) for knowledge. They have, with a few exceptions, been socialized

and taught to accept knowledge from authority figures (Baxter-Magolda,

1998). This trend is a result of the epistemological development created by

traditional classroom schooling (Baxter-Magolda, 1998).

Both Kegan (1994) and Baxter-Magolda (1998) suggest that the

incorporation of a constructive-developmental approach to traditional subject

matter would better train students to be self-authoring individuals upon

graduation. Kegan (1994) goes on to identify three necessary components of

constructive-developmental pedagogy: (a) acknowledging the student as

knower, (b) basing education on students’ experience and, (c) inclusion of the

student in creation of the experience. The inclusion of these three ideas into

traditional American college instruction will lead to ownership of the subject

9

matter and the development of critical analysis, problem solving, and

interpersonal skills enhancing students’ ability to self-author.

The most recent and comprehensive study (Baxter-Magolda, 2001) of

the use of a constructive-developmental pedagogy in higher education

involved tracking 101 incoming college freshman from college through their

thirties. Baxter-Magolda conducted more than 300 interviews with the

participants. The results of the 18-year longitudinal study focused on 39

remaining participants and identified several constructs which define SA. The

validation of a student’s capacity to learn through participation in collaborative

work, leadership roles, and membership in diverse communities creates the

backbone of SA. Young adults who participate in these types of activities are

better equipped to make meaning of their own experiences, ultimately

allowing them to self-author. The most interesting result of the research is the

relationship between SA and time. The research (Baxter-Magolda, 2001)

shows that young people with longer exposure to constructive-developmental

pedagogies and techniques have a greater ability to self-author their lives.

Constructs related to SA

SA incorporates various ideas (Baxter-Magolda, 1999, 2001) much like

OE (Walsh & Golins, 1976; Ewert & Haywood, 1991; Gass in Miles & Priest,

1990; Kraft in Miles & Priest, 1990). However, the most relevant connection

between SA and OE research can be seen in the realms of self-efficacy and

10

locus of control (LOC). A closer look at the research makes this connection

evident.

Kegan (1994) and Baxter-Magolda’s (1998;1999; 2001) ideas about

self-authorship and constructive-developmental pedagogy are related to the

theory of self-efficacy proposed by Bandura (1977). Self-efficacy refers to an

individual’s judgment about his/her capability to act in the face of novel,

uncomfortable, and stressful situations (Bandura, 1977). Bandura claimed

that an individual gains greater self-efficacy through successfully overcoming

challenges. Furthermore, Bandura proposed that self-efficacy beliefs strongly

determine an individual’s behavior. Thus, if self-efficacy beliefs can be made

more positive, then enhanced behavior will follow.

In order to boost self-efficacy beliefs Bandura recommended that

individuals have experiences of successfully overcoming relevant challenges.

Bandura goes on to say that an individual’s self-efficacy increases

incrementally after each successful encounter, meaning they believe they can

overcome more and more difficult situations. Bandura (1986; 1997) also

states that self-efficacy is not just about knowing how to do something, but it

is also combining cognitive, behavioral and social skills to develop a holistic

course of action. According to Bandura’s theory, it is important to have

success early so that self-efficacy can increase and not be affected by future

failures (Bandura, 1977). This idea is definitely reflected in the theories of

both Kegan and Baxter-Magolda. They both advocate the value of providing

students with the opportunity to learn how to succeed in order to increase

11

self-efficacy. This in turn allows them to tackle more difficult challenges

(interpersonal relationships, critical thinking, etc.) that eventually lead to SA

(Kegan, 1994, Baxter-Magolda, 1998, 1999).

Another worthy theory when thinking about self-authorship is Rotter’s

(1966) theory of locus-of-control. LOC relates to an individual’s beliefs

around the relationship between his/her actions and the outcomes of those

actions (Rotter, 1966). Lefcourt (1982) expands on Rotter and explains the

dichotomy inherent in LOC. The dichotomy of internal-external LOC

describes the way a person evaluates life events and subsequently assigns

meaning to those events. Individuals with internal LOC orientations view

events as resulting from one’s own actions, implying the potential for personal

control (Lefcourt, 1982). On the other hand individuals with an external LOC

orientation perceive events as being unrelated to personal actions and

thereby out of their control (Lefcourt, 1982). Individuals with internal LOC

attribute success and failure to skill and hard work, whereas individuals with

external LOC attribute them to luck, chance or fate (Zwart, 1988).

According to Zwart (1988) research suggests that an internal LOC

orientation is the more “psychologically healthy” of the two. Individuals with

an internal LOC orientation strive to achieve some measure of control over

personal development and environment. This idea of control of personal

development and environment also relates to Kegan and Baxter-Magolda’s

ideas of SA. Both Kegan (1994) and Baxter-Magolda (1999) talk about

students taking control of their own learning and being responsible for

12

creating their own knowledge and beliefs. More recently Baxter-Magolda

(2001) reported that young people experiencing a constructive-developmental

approach learn to take responsibility for their actions because they have been

given opportunities to make decisions with real consequences.

Both LOC and self-efficacy are complex concepts which explain young

people’s development and closely relate to SA. OE research has shown that

internal LOC and improved self-efficacy are outcomes of participation in OE

activities (Bertolami, 1981; Ewert, 1983; Hans, 2000; Hattie, Marsh, Neill &

Richards, 1997; Koesler & Propost, 1998; Paxton, 1998). Before delving into

the results of specific research studies, an exploration of the inherent

presence of SA in OE is warranted.

Reasoned Links between SA and OE

Since John Dewey formally introduced the idea of experiential

education in 1938, the essential components of SA have been present. The

connection can be made between OE’s emphasis on cooperation and

teamwork and SA’s need for creating relationships which aid in the creation of

knowledge. OE theory also has a deep history of problem solving (Walsh &

Golins, 1976) and provides a level of discomfort or dissonance (Dewey, 1997;

Walsh & Golins, 1976) which heightens student awareness of challenges and

impels them to create solutions both individually and within a group.

OE programs, particularly in North America, are grouped into four

categories by Priest & Gass (1997): recreational, educational, developmental

13

and therapeutic. A brief explanation of the four areas is warranted to better

understand which of the four categories best aid in helping individuals to self-

author. Recreation in OE is focused on having fun, learning new things, and

becoming reenergized (Priest & Gass, 1997). The learning of new skills

traditionally occurs through problem-solving tasks of increasing difficulty

which encourage creative thinking and collaboration. Through teaching new

skills recreation does provide some opportunity to transfer learning to future

events; traditionally, however, those events are the same or similar to the

recreational experience.

Educational OE programs are “aimed at understanding concepts,

enriching the knowledge of old concepts, or generating an awareness of

previously unknown needs through adventure” (Priest & Gass, 1997, p. 23).

These educational programs provide students a fresh perspective and

encourage them to create new attitudes regarding the way they conceptualize

daily life (Priest & Gass, 1997). In these programs students are challenged to

create connections between the adventure experience and their personal

realities. The development of these transfer skills is crucial to self-authorship

because it provides students with a mechanism for synthesizing information

and experience and making it relative to their own reality.

Developmental programming attempts to change people’s behaviors

by teaching alternative behaviors using adventure (Priest & Gass, 1997).

These programs focus a great deal on interaction with others (i.e. teamwork,

communication, cooperation) and personal development. Developmental

14

programs attempt to provide individuals with a better ability to function within

a group and develop a strong sense of self within a group. The development

of interpersonal skills through collaborative work is a very important part of SA

(Baxter-Magolda, 2001). Clearly developmental OE programs have a strong

connection to self-authorship.

Lastly, therapeutic programs strive to change dysfunctional behavior

patterns using adventure experiences (Priest & Gass, 1997). Therapeutic OE

programs focus particularly on changing dysfunction and fighting destructive

behaviors which often aids individuals in being able to self-author their future

lives. Additionally, therapeutic adventure programs help participants develop

appropriate and adaptive social skills (Russell, 2003) which are both

important in becoming self-authoring adults.

All four categories of OE programs have the potential to increase an

individual’s ability to self-author. The critical pedagogical components of OE

are: experiential learning, dissonance, critical reflection, leadership,

empowerment, group process, and collaboration (Gass in Miles & Priest,

1990; Kraft in Miles & Priest, 1990; Walsh & Golins, 1976). These central

tenets of OE provide students with opportunities to develop and practice self-

authorship skills. Through participation in OE activities students learn

interpersonal and problem-solving skills which aid them in relying on

themselves as sources of knowledge, rather than depending on someone

else to provide that knowledge. Additionally, students gain the self-efficacy

15

and internalize their locus of control, which helps them to stand by the

knowledge they have derived for themselves.

Research Findings linking SA to OE

Self-efficacy

Research findings reporting positive self-efficacy outcomes as a result

of participation in OE activities are abundant. The research shows that OE’s

emphasis on problem solving, feedback, and group process (Hattie, et.al.,

1997; Koesler & Propst, 1998; Paxton & McAvoy, 1998, Newhouse, 2002)

provides an excellent context for experiencing incremental successes which

aid in the development of self-efficacy.

Hattie et.al. (1997) conducted a meta-analysis of outcomes influenced

by adventure programs. They analyzed 96 unique studies in which they

identified 1,728 effect sizes based on 151 unique samples. The study

reported an overall immediate effect size of .34. Additionally, they reported a

follow-up effect of .17 which, when added to the immediate effect, results in a

total effect of .51. The study reported the greatest immediate effects on

leadership, academics, independence, assertiveness, emotional stability,

social comparison, time managemen,t and flexibility. The researchers went

on to create six encompassing dimensions (academic, leadership, self-

concept, personality, interpersonal, and adventuresome) across which effects

of adventure programming were systematically high (Hattie et.al., 1997).

Hattie et. al. (1997) then state that there are marked increases in

16

interpersonal dimensions as a result of adventure programming. It is

Important to note that in the conclusions reported by Hattie et.al. (1997) is the

claim that “adventure programs increase the amount of quality feedback that

is vital to the experiential learning process” (p.75). The results reported by

Hattie et.al. (1997) regarding the positive effects of adventure programming

on several dimensions necessary for self-authorship provide clear evidence of

the inherent connection between OE and SA. The analysis conducted by

Hattie et.al. is far reaching and comprehensive, providing good insight into

various OE outcomes which influence SA. Hattie et.al. provide a good

overview of self-efficacy as one outcome of participation in OE. A review of

research which focuses specifically on OE and self-efficacy will further the

understanding of the connection between SA and OE.

Self-efficacy has been measured using the self-efficacy scale and

shown to be increased as a result of participation in outdoor leadership

trainings at the National Outdoor Leadership School (Koesler & Propst, 1998).

An experiential group (N = 194) was administered a pre-test and two post-

tests (immediately after the course and one year later), and these results

were compared to a baseline group The study also showed that self-efficacy

decreased from post-course levels but remained significantly higher than

base levels a year after participation. Additionally, the study reported that

feedback and mentoring had a positive effect on self-efficacy (Koesler &

Propst, 1998).

17

In a study conducted by Paxton and McAvoy (1998), the researchers

focused on 68 participants aged 18 to 29 years who participated in a series of

21-day Outward Bound courses. Researchers used a pre-test post-test

design with an added post-test six months after completion of the course.

The study investigated the effect of adventure programs on participants’ self-

efficacy and the transference of self-efficacy outcomes of adventure programs

to everyday life. Respondents indicated a greater feeling of control of their

own lives. Additionally, respondents indicated greater feeling of self-reliance,

evidenced by statements such as “I have learned to do things on my own

without depending on someone else to do it for me” (Paxton & McAvoy, 1998,

p. 7) Such reported by-products of increased self-efficacy suggest that OE

programs help students to create their own identities and not rely on others

for knowledge.

Paxton and McAvoy (1998, p.2) adopt Bandura’s notion that “a resilient

sense of efficacy is needed to sustain a continual effort, which is needed for

success in any situation” and attempt to measure various elements of self-

efficacy: general efficacy, socio-political efficacy and interpersonal efficacy.

Their study showed an increase in all three elements pre- to post-test, and six

months later (Paxton & McAvoy, 1998). Showing increases in interpersonal

efficacy, they were able to show that adventure program participants are

better able to defend one’s point of view during group decisions, develop

working relationships, and develop personal relationships. (Paxton & McAvoy,

1998).

18

Another important finding of the study shows that participants were able to

transfer positive gains from participation in adventure programs to their

everyday lives (Paxton & McAvoy, 1998). All of the results reported by

Paxton & McAvoy support the contention that OE aids in SA.

Kolb (1993, p.8) suggests that “in a rapidly changing workplace,

employees need the ability to handle new and ambiguous situations”. In an

attempt to show that participation in outdoor experiential training increases

self-efficacy, Kolb examined two experimental and control groups of

adolescents over a four-month period (Kolb, 1993). Kolb’s study focused on

128 ninth graders participating in a five-week program consisting of: team

building activities, rock climbing, rappelling, wilderness living skills,

spelunking, challenge course events and problem-solving activities. The

participants were measured weekly for self-efficacy. Although Kolb

discovered numerous limitations in his study that may have altered results, he

reported that self-efficacy changes initially, but over time self-efficacy is

comprised of past successes and failures and will not automatically change in

a short period of time (Kolb, 1993).

Newhouse (2002) conducted a study of the use of a climbing wall

adventure activity on children’s perceived self-efficacy. The study included

over 100 participants from fourth- and fifth-grade classes in Albuquerque

public schools. The participants were given a pre-test one week prior to the

intervention and a post-test immediately after the intervention using Cowen’s

Perceived Self-Efficacy scale. Newhouse (2002) concluded that adventure

19

education increases self-efficacy of participants to deal with problems with

people. Additionally, Newhouse (2002) underscored the importance of

debriefing in adventure education as a necessary element to produce

increased self-efficacy. It was reported that children need help in making

connections and transferring learning to everyday lives.

Locus of Control

OE programs provide an opportunity for participants to see immediate

results of their actions, making the amount of control they exercise over their

circumstance very apparent. This element of OE programs aids students in

accepting responsibility for their actions and enhances a feeling of personal

control over their lives. When young people internalize a feeling of control

over their lives, they become better prepared to begin self-authoring.

Hans’(2000) meta-analysis of the effects of outdoor programming on

LOC found an effect size of .38 (Cohen’s d), substantiating previous findings

(Cason, 1993; Cason & Gillis, 1994; Hattie et.al.,1997) of increased internality

of participants’ LOC. The meta-analysis included 24 studies with 1,632

subjects and 30 effect sizes. Hans (2000) concluded that the individual

studies regarding LOC and participation in outdoor/adventure programming

need to focus on specific program elements and their relationship to changes

in LOC. Hans (2000) suggested that by using better measurement tools and

developing further clarity around program characteristics as moderators of

change, would strengthen research connecting increases in internal LOC to

adventure programming.

20

Another study connecting LOC and adventure programming reports

that students have an increased sense of their ability to take control of their

lives (Bertolami, 1981). The study used a quasi-experimental control group

design to collect quantitative data from young adults participating in a

standard 26-day Outward Bound course. Bertolami’s (1981) study collected

descriptive data in the form of journal entries and written self-evaluations.

The data supported the quantitative evidence which concluded that

wilderness programs provide an important medium for enhancing the

personal development of young adults (Bertolami, 1981). Additionally, the

qualitative data supported the quantitative findings that wilderness programs

increase internal LOC orientations in males and decrease female perceptions

that events were controlled by chance or others (Bertolami, 1981). The study

reported that students felt more able to control their lives in light of realistic

personal limits (Bertolami, 1981). Lastly, Bertolami (1981) attributes changes

in “personal control” to accomplishment of challenging activities, supportive

group environment, intense personal interactions, wilderness environment,

and increased self-awareness (Bertolami, 1981). The language used by

Bertolami, suggesting internalization of LOC, again resembles tenets of SA

concerned with development of personal identity.

A study conducted to examine the effects of participation in an

Outward Bound program on multidimensional self-concepts (Marsh, Richard

& Barnes, 1986a, 1986b) reported a significant shift toward an internal LOC

orientation during the program. The study included 361 participants from 16

21

to 31-year-olds who completed one of ten Outward Bound courses over the

course of seven months in the early 1980’s (Marsh, Richard & Barnes, 1986a,

1986b). The researchers used the Rotter I-E scale (Rotter, 1966) at the start

and finish of the Outward Bound course and again 18 months later.

The Hattie et.al (1997) meta-analysis reported an effect of .30 on

internal LOC. The researchers concluded that the increase in internal LOC in

conjunction with increases in independence (.47), self-efficacy (.31), self-

understanding (.34), assertiveness (.42), and decision making (.47) resulted

in the greatest effects on self-control (Hattie et. al, 1997). The outcome

related to a reported sense of control or regulation of self, responsibility or an

assurance of self (Hattie et.al., 1997). The above clearly show that

participants gain a level of self-regulation which is critical to self-authorship by

participating in adventure programs (Hattie et.al., 1997).

By experiencing the feeling of control over their lives, participants are

empowered to move toward SA. Increased independence and greater ability

to self-regulate action enhances an individual’s ability to self-author.

Conclusions

An attempt has been made to present relevant research which makes

clear the connection between OE and self-authorship. SA and OE share

common pedagogical approaches which incorporate critical thinking, problem

solving, interpersonal skill development, empowerment, and making meaning

of one’s experience. SA is a relatively new concept being explored in the

22

realm of higher education; however, OE methodologies appear to inherently

posses mechanisms which enhance SA. Recently connections between SA

and OE have appeared in OE research (Gass, et.al., 2003).

OE research demonstrates that participation in OE programs has

positive effects on personal development including transference to everyday

life over time. The research demonstrates that OE increases self-efficacy and

internalizes LOC, both concepts related to SA.

23

CHAPTER III

RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

A case study design is best suited for this study. The case study

design allows for multiple sources of data, which complement and reinforce

each other (Yin, 1989). This particular case study called for the inclusion of

several components to meet the stated purpose of the research. The first

component involved a review of SA and OE literature. The second

component was a focus group used to add clarity to the relationship between

SA and OE and inform the generation of themes and items to include in a

measure of SA. Lastly, data were collected using the Self-authorship

questionnaire (SAQ) created using themes and items identified during

analysis of focus-group data.

Participants

The participants in this study are divided into two phases: (a) focus-

group interview participants and (b) SAQ subjects. Phase 1 participants were

all members of the Plast organization. Phase 2 subjects were young adult

members (18 – 38 years old) of Plast and students at the University of New

Hampshire (UNH) in Outdoor Education and Recreation Management/Policy

24

classes. The University of New Hampshire is a traditional state university

located in Durham, New Hampshire with approximately 13,000 students.

Phase 1 participants were seven adult members of Plast who were

asked to participate by the author. The actual interview was conducted

during Plast’s bi-annual national convention held at a resort in the Catskill

Mountains of New York State. All the participants gave their consent to

participate in the interview. Demographic information was obtained from all

interview participants.

Phase 2 Plast subjects (49.1%) were from the United States and

Canada. The participants were identified using the database of Plast’s

national headquarters and through personal contacts of the author. In total

233 e-mail addresses were identified and used to invite participants to

complete an on-line version of the SAQ. Participants provided consent

electronically via the internet. Phase 2 UNH subjects were graduate and

undergraduate students in Outdoor Education and Recreation Management &

Policy classes. Students in six classes were administered paper versions of

the SAQ during class times scheduled with instructors: KIN 550 Philosophy

and Methods of Outdoor Education, KIN 682 Outdoor Leadership, RMP 557

Recreation Services Program Design, RMP 668 Youth Culture and Programs,

RMP 667 Recreation Resource Planning, RMP 501 Recreation Services for

Individuals with Disabilities and RMP 772 Law and Public Policy in Leisure

Services. Prior to administration of the survey participants provided their

25

consent (see Informed Consent Form in Appendix C). Demographic

information was collected about all SAQ test subjects.

Case Selection

Plast was founded in 1911 by a group of progressive educators led by

Dr. Olexsander Tysovsky. The organization was created only a few years

after scouting was founded in England by Lord Baden-Powell. The founders

of the Plast organization sought to provide young men with an opportunity to

become well-rounded “citizens of the world” (Starosolsky, 1948). Since its

founding, Plast has broadened it scope to include females. During the mid-

20th century Plast was forced to exist in exile in the Diaspora because it was

outlawed in Ukraine by the Soviet government.

Plast membership traditionally begins at age six, but the major

educational programming occurs between the ages of 11 and 18. At age 11

Plast members pledge to strive to adhere to the 14 core tenets (see Table 3-

1) set forth by Plast’s charter (Yurchuk, 2003).

George Starosolsky, the head scout and author of the seminal book

explaining Plast’s philosophy and methods, states that Plast does not

demand unconditional adherence to its tenets, but throughout many years it

provides a scout with a certain directive to strive to abide by its principles,

eventually making it a way of life (Starosolsky, 1948). The ultimate goal of

Plast closely resembles SA (Kegan, 1994; Baxter-Magolda, 1999). The key

phrase which Plast uses directly translates to “self-rearing” and is defined by

26

Starosolsky (1948) as having the courage to function independently and

relying on one’s own strengths to be in control of the direction one’s life takes.

Table 3-1

Fourteen tenets of the Plast program (Tysovsky, 1912).

Keep your word.

Be conscientious.

Be punctual, exact.

Be thrifty.

Be fair.

Be polite.

Be brotherly, good-natured.

Be even-tempered.

Be useful.

Be obedient.

Be diligent.

Care for your health.

Love beauty and nature, and care for it.

Be optimistic

Plast relies on a year-long program to achieve its goals. From the age

of 11 participants progress through three major levels which are summarized

into: (a) knowing, (b) ability to demonstrate, and (c) the ability to teach. Older

Plast members act as guides and models along the way, incrementally

rendering themselves obsolete and allowing for the older adolescents to

direct their own program. The year-long program culminates every summer at

various camps where participants take part in experiential programs focused

on group development, leadership development, task/skill mastery, problem

solving and further integration of the 14 tenets into their way of life.

Ultimately, Plast’s program is an intentional pairing of youth development and

outdoor education in the hopes of achieving SA.

27

Procedures

Focus-group Interview

After receiving approval from the IRB (Appendix D) the researcher

traveled to the bi-annual convention of Plast in the Catskill Mountains in New

York State. The researcher identified potential participants, based on their

years of leadership, position within the organization, and personal relationship

and shared experience with the researcher. In total eight participants agreed

to participate in the focus-group interview. All the participants were given

informed consent letters (Appendix C) to review and were asked to bring

them to the arranged time and place of the interview. Of the eight consenting

participants, only seven actually participated in the interview.

The interview was conducted using a general interview guide format

(Gall, Gall & Borg, 2003). The researcher began the interview with a short

introduction and then asked the participants to describe self-authorship as

they understood it in the context of Plast. Based on cues taken from the

participants the researcher directed the interview exploring the philosophy,

methods, programs and outcomes the participants believed to be critical to

achieving SA. Particular attention was paid to outdoor and camp

programming. The entire interview was video recorded for future re-visiting

and data analysis.

28

Instrument Development

The SAQ was created to measure eight domains which were identified

as a result of reviewing SA literature and analyzing qualitative data collected

during Phase 1. The eight identified domains of self-authorship are: (a)

judgment/decision making, (b) self-regulation, (c) self-confidence, (d)

interpersonal skills, (e) empowerment, (f) creative problem solving, (g)

leadership/responsibility, and (h) knowledge creation.

SAQ Administration

The researcher utilized an internet-based survey for the Plast group

and an in-person paper survey for UNH group. Participants in the Plast group

were asked to participate via an e-mail distribution list. In an attempt to

increase the return rate, a $100 gift certificate to Amazon.com was offered as

incentive. In total 233 emails were collected. The participants were directed

to a website where the purpose, requirements, benefits and commitment of

the study were explained. Participants indicated their consent by choosing

the “I agree to participate” button, which redirected them to the actual SAQ.

Upon completion of the survey the participants clicked the “submit” button,

which sent an email with their responses to the researcher. The 233

collected e-mail addresses returned 142 completed surveys, yielding a 61%

response rate. The remaining 39% resulted from expired or erroneous e-mail

addresses and non-responses.

29

Participants in the UNH group were surveyed during class at a time

pre-arranged with the class instructor. The researcher explained the purpose

of the study and distributed informed consent forms (Appendix C) to the

participants. They were given a paper version of the SAQ to fill out.

Confidentiality was maintained throughout the data collection process.

Data Analysis

Data were analyzed in two phases. The first phase consisted of

analyzing qualitative data collected during the focus-group interview. The

second phase involved analyzing quantitative data collected using the SAQ.

Phase 1

The focus-group interview material was analyzed using a pattern

coding approach (Miles & Huberman, 1994) within the context of a case study

(Gall, Gall & Borg, 2003). Transcripts were viewed several times to identify

common themes. During each viewing general patterns were noticed by the

researcher and were noted. These patterns emerged from interview

participants repeatedly mentioning similar ideas and beliefs about SA. Initially

the researcher viewed the video transcript and on each occasion started

without notes from previous viewing. After several viewings, notes were

analyzed and themes were identified from the patterns. The researcher

compared the identified themes from several sessions of pattern analysis with

published research and literature on the topic of SA to determine if the

interview data was analogous to published research.

30

Phase 2

Data were analyzed to determine internal reliability and Cronbach’s

Alpha for each item as well as a total measure alpha. Alpha scores between

.65 and .70 are considered minimally acceptable; between .70 and .80

respectable; between .80 and .90 very good (DeVellis, 2003). The researcher

focused on inter-scale correlations making sure that none of the inter-item

correlations approached one with a desired range between .3 and .9.

Descriptive analysis was conducted to compute a mean, standard

deviation, skewness, and kurtosis for each variable. This procedure checked

for the normality of data and determined which items were appropriate for

inclusion in subsequent analysis. As outlined by Tabachnik and Fidell (2001)

Principal Factor Analysis (PFA) was used to determine if independent factors

exist and are distinguishable among the identified items. Lastly, identified

factors were converted into sub-scales for inclusion in subsequent versions of

the SAQ.

31

CHAPTER IV

RESULTS

The objectives of this study were (a) to review literature for a better

understanding of the underlying themes that define self-authorship, (b) to

explore how outdoor education processes and outcomes relate to self-

authorship, and (c) to design and pilot test a valid and reliable measure of

self-authorship. The first two objectives were met by the review of literature.

The third objective was accomplished in two phases. Phase 1 involved

qualitative review of data for the purpose of generating a pool of items to be

included in a preliminary version of a Self-authorship questionnaire (SAQ v.1).

In addition to analysis of qualitative data, self-authorship literature was

reviewed to inform the generation of items. Phase 2 involved the analysis of

quantitative data collected using SAQ v.1 for the purpose of refining the SAQ

v.1 into a more psychometrically sound instrument.

Phase 1

The seven focus group participants were four males ranging in age

form 27 to 49 and three females ranging in age from 24 to 58. All participants

were lifelong members of Plast hailing from: Toronto, New Jersey, New York,

Boston and Washington DC. On average the seven participants had

32

attended 5 Plast summer camps as participants and had been counselors or

leaders within the organization for an average of more than 20 years.

The focus-group interview explored the participants’ perceptions of SA

in the context of the Plast program as they have experienced it and tried to

facilitate it with the youth in their care. Open pattern coding methods

suggested by Miles & Huberman (1994) resulted in the identification of eight

major themes (see Table 4-1).

Judgement/Decision making

Participants reported that the Plast organization provided adolescents

the opportunity to practice decision-making skills over the course of at least

five years which resulted in the development of good judgment. Participants

noted that the process was based on a progression of decision-making with

an increasing level of consequences. “…you were responsible for planning it,

carrying it out, making sure the logistics were there. And if the group didn’t

pull it together, then you had a miserable two weeks.” The importance of

actual consequences was identified as being more effective at developing

judgment and decision-making than hypothetical consequences.

Self-regulation

Focus-group participants consistently mentioned that Plast required

adolescents to take responsibility for their actions, especially when tasks

needed to be accomplished. The Plast program is designed in such a way

that as the adolescent progresses his/her program becomes increasingly

Table 4-1 Focus-group statements and identified open codes pattern-coded into themes.

Themes Sample open codes Sample interview statements

Judgment/Decision Making self-confident decision-making “safe” failure progression of consequences discomfort within a controlled “familiar”

environment

You still need the ability to say “I’m going to take that risk, and these are the consequences. I’m willing to accept them or I’m not willing to accept them.”

Self-regulation self-regulating community learn not to fear failure

…Basically you are taking on the responsibility of checking up on your own behavior and attitude and outlook.

Self-confidence confidence self-confident decision-making success breeds success

And it really helps to have self-confidence when they are able to get through these challenges.

Interpersonal Skills feedback dependant tied to group consequences tied to group standards come from within the group support inherent duration of group

…This group of 6-10 really regulating each other. They are held to a similar standard by a similar community…

Empowerment transition from told to telling empowerment leadership opportunities rites of passage

I think the key word you hit on was empowerment. Begin to empower that child to make decisions…

Creative Problem Solving creativity calculated risks solution oriented non-judgmental

We want a child, a young person to develop their own ability to grasp the situation and understand the complexity of it and then figure out a way of dealing with that complexity.

Leadership/Responsibility alternating position of responsibility within group

leadership opportunities peer leadership

…Every child has the opportunity to see what they are capable of and then subsequently to develop, you know, strengthen their weak areas in these leadership roles…

Knowledge Creation moment of internalization of 14 tenets solution oriented targeted method: knows, can show, can teach “greater context”

…You start to have your own feelings about it and your own interpretations or understanding about it.

more self-directed. Plast participants are expected to create and execute a

program which results in specifically outlined outcomes. Plast

leaders/counselors employ a theory of planned obsolescence, whereby they

gradually remove themselves from a leadership role and retreat to limited

oversight: “…it is the road from being led by the guide to being a self-

motivator…”

Self-confidence

Self-confidence was the theme that surfaced most often throughout the

interview and data analysis. One of the interview participants stated that the

main purpose of Plast’s program was to get adolescents to a certain point:

“So when a young adult arrives at a point of having to make decisions for

themselves, that person is self-confident enough to live in a society as a

useful person, rather than depend on society to provide for them.”

Participants continually noted that self-confidence was the key element that

allowed all the other gains in knowledge and maturity made by an adolescent

to be incorporated into everyday life. Another participant commented that “It

really helps to have that self-confidence when they are able to get through

those challenges” when referring to a person’s ability to deal with

uncomfortable and novel situations.

Interpersonal Skills

The importance of group continually surfaced throughout the interview.

Participants identified that adolescents benefited from going through the Plast

program with the same core group “…our patrol groups are very beneficial to

35

that development, because these children grow up within that same group…”

In addition to the core group, participants talked about the constant interaction

of adolescents at various camps, outings, jamborees, and competitions. The

idea of a progression was again mentioned when talking about the

development of interpersonal skills. At first counselors/leaders set and model

very specific guidelines of interaction which eventually result in the

adolescents establishing and enforcing their own group norms of behavior.

Empowerment

Second only to self-confidence, the theme of empowering youth

surfaced throughout the interview. Participants mentioned that Plast

empowered adolescents by providing group and individual challenges,

leadership opportunities, and a well-defined reward system. Empowerment

was the theme which was mentioned most often in connection with

transference to life outside of Plast. One participant recalled the feeling of

empowerment he felt “I was ready to, you know, whether it be the outdoor

world or going to Tokyo into a new environment… well, I may not be entirely

comfortable with this new environment, but I had the background to say – I’m

ready to take on this challenge and deal with it.”

Creative Problem Solving

The following sentiment was reported by the majority of the

participants: “We want a child, a young person to develop [his/her] own ability

to grasp the situation and understand the complexity of it and then figure out

a way of dealing with that complexity”. The interviewees reported that Plast

36

continually tries to challenge its adolescent participants to enhance their

creativity in the face of challenges. The participants all agreed that creative

problem solving was a key ingredient to self-authorship. The interview

revealed that the summer camp program and annual competitions where the

most effective at encouraging and facilitating creativity.

Leadership/Responsibility

Participants continually mentioned that providing adolescents with

leadership opportunities with real consequences was vital to creating a self-

authoring individual. One of the participants stated “…you continue to up the

ante, if you will, so that you learn to be responsible, you learn something from

failures…how to do better, taking on larger responsibilities, to the point where

you understand that there may be some real risk involved.” Participants

agreed that in order for leadership and responsibility to have the desired

effect on self-authorship it had to be carefully facilitated and guided by a

counselor/leader.

Knowledge Creation

The final theme that was identified from the pattern coding was

knowledge creation. Participants reported that the final stage of the Plast

program turns almost all leadership and decision-making responsibility to the

adolescents. This shift forces participants to decide on and create a plan of

action which is scrutinized by peers and senior counselors. Interview

participants reported that all these factors required the adolescents to form

their own opinions and beliefs and to have the ability to defend them. Most

37

important, in the opinion of the participants, was the internalization and

interpretation of what they had learned to that point. One participant said: “So

it’s this internalization and using it creatively and interpreting for himself the

tenets and progressing forward.” The independent interpretation of Plast

tenets resulted in participants realizing that they are better prepared to collect

and interpret information when making decisions and forming a life plan.

The themes identified from the data analysis indicate the elements of

the Plast program which participants believe lead to self-authorship. The

themes identified were compared to the self-authorship literature (Baxter-

Magolda, 1998, 1999; Kegan, 1994), and it was decided that the two were

very similar. Therefore they provided the researcher with valuable insights

which would aid in the generation of a preliminary item pool for inclusion in a

self-authorship questionnaire.

An initial search for existing instruments measuring the identified

elements of SA was conducted. The following instruments were identified: (a)

the Review of Personal Effectiveness And Locus of Control (ROPELOC)

(Richards, Ellis, & Neill, 2002) and (b) the Empowerment Scale (Rogers,

Chamberlin, Ellison & Crean, 1997). The ROPELOC reports an average

internal reliability of .85 and an overall alpha of .96 (Richards, Ellis, & Neill,

2002). The Empowerment Scale reports an alpha of .86 (Rogers, Chamberlin,

Ellison & Crean, 1997). Where appropriate, intact items were used directly or

slightly adapted from the two measures; this was true for seven of the eight

identified elements of self-authorship. For the seven elements between

38

seven to eight items were selected from the existing measures and later

reduced to five per element resulting in 35 items. In the case of knowledge

creation the author created eight items and ultimately settled on five resulting

in the final instrument (see Appendix A) containing 40 items (see Table 4-2).

The 40 items were randomly ordered and measured on a five-point Likert

scale ranging from 1 False (not like me) to 5 True (very much like me),

providing respondents the opportunity for strong assertions (DeVellis, 2003).

Phase 2

Participants

Basic demographic information was collected to describe the sample.

In total SAQ v.1 data was collected from 289 subjects. Of the 289 subjects

176 (60.9%) were female and 113 (39.1%) were male. One hundred and

forty-two (49.1%) subjects were members of Plast, and 147 (50.9%) were not.

All of the 147 non-members of Plast were students at the University of New

Hampshire (UNH). The average age of the subjects was 21.34 (SD = 2.79),

and ages ranged from 18 to 38. The majority (79.3%) of subjects were

between the ages of 19 and 23.

39

Table 4-2

Original items generated for inclusion in SAQ v.1

Theme Items

Judgment/Decision Making I am good at deciding whether a risk is worth taking. I think about the consequences of my actions. I make the right decision a majority of the time. I show good judgment in most situations. I make decisions in an efficient manner.

Self-regulation I am calm in stressful situations. I am capable of regulating my own actions. I don’t need to be told how to behave. My actions are determined by my peer group. I am efficient and do not waste time.

Self-confidence I am able to do things as well as most other people. No matter what the situation I can handle it. I am confident that I have the ability to succeed in anything I want to do. No matter what happens I can handle it. I know I have the ability to do anything I want to do.

Interpersonal Skills I like cooperating in a team. I am effective in social situations. I cooperate well when working in a team. I communicate effectively with other people. I feel comfortable speaking in front of a group.

Empowerment I see myself as a capable person. People are only limited by what they think is possible. I am given opportunities to make a difference. I am given real responsibility in my life. I am able to handle positions of authority.

Creative Problem Solving Very often a problem can be solved by taking action. I cope well with changing situations. Whatever situation arises I can come up with a solution. I enjoy coming up with solutions to my problems. Problem-solving is stressful.

Leadership/Responsibility I am capable of accepting responsibility. I am willing to accept the consequences of my leadership decisions. I am a good leader when things need to get done. I am a capable leader. I am willing to make difficult decisions.

Knowledge Creation I believe experts are in the best position to decide what people should learn or do. I can research a topic and form my own opinion effectively. I never question the opinion of my superiors. Before making decisions I consult experts. I always look to my teacher/boss for direction.

40

Initial Item Reduction

All data was initially screened for illegal scores which were defined as

beyond the extremes of the 5 point Likert scale. Several illegal scores were

identified and corrected by referring to original data sheets. All illegal scores

were the result of data entry errors.

The initial SAQ contained 40 items chosen to measure the eight

themes identified during Phase 1 of the study. The data for all 40 items was

screened. Of the 40 items, 19 were answered 289 subjects, 15 were

answered by 288 subjects, three were answered by 287 subjects, two were

answered by 286 subjects and one was answered by 284 subjects. In total,

260 subjects answered the SAQ completely. The 29 incomplete surveys

were not eliminated because the missing values were replaced by the mean

scores for that item for the purpose of analysis (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001).

Data screening was performed on all 40 items in an attempt to

eliminate poorly functioning items. Central tendency measures, skewness,

and kurtosis statistics were used to establish normality (see Table 4-3).

Although a high number of items returned mean scores toward the higher end

of the scale (M>3.5), it was decided not to eliminate items due to ceiling

effects. This decision was based on the fact that the subjects to whom the

SAQ was administered were not a random sample but instead were Plast

members or students in Recreation Management Policy and Outdoor

Education classes. Items #3 (I am willing to accept the consequences of my

41

Table 4-3

Mean, Standard Deviation, Skewness and Kurtosis Statistics.

Item Mean SD Skewness Kurtosis

4* (reverse) 2.56 .99 .16 -.67 13 (reverse) 2.96 .92 -.10 -.43 22* (reverse) 3.27 .97 -.16 -.69 38 3.33 1.07 -.25 -.66 39 (reverse) 3.39 .93 -.19 -.31 20 (reverse) 3.48 .98 -.28 -.49 5 3.63 1.16 -.49 -.73 31* (reverse) 3.64 .96 -.37 -.39 12 3.73 .80 -.28 -.13 26 3.75 .84 -.31 -.26 2 3.78 .92 -.69 .29 8 3.82 .99 -.69 -.10 15 3.83 .75 -.44 .11 33 3.84 1.03 -.71 -.08 19 3.96 .66 -.39 .48 25 4.01 .78 -.76 .79 36 4.02 .72 -.69 1.10 14 4.07 .78 -.60 .33 7 4.09 .83 -.61 -.14 21 4.10 .88 -1.01 .96 9 4.11 .80 -.89 .91 28 4.11 .85 -.76 -.06 32 4.11 .82 -.78 .39 24 4.14 .82 -1.01 1.21 1 4.15 .70 -.77 1.13 29 4.17 .65 -.57 1.32 37 4.22 .66 -.49 .16 16 4.24 .67 -.46 -.23 34 4.25 .75 -.88 .58 6* 4.25 .82 -1.17 1.74 23* 4.26 .88 -1.47 2.58 27* 4.27 .67 -.73 .78 17 4.27 .73 -.74 .10 18 4.27 .72 -.91 .85 11 4.31 .71 -.95 1.32 3* 4.37 .75 -1.32 2.13 10 4.42 .69 -1.04 .76 35 4.42 .65 -.85 .32 30 4.48 .58 -.73 .15 40* 4.54 .62 -1.55 3.95

*items were dropped

42

leadership decisions), #6 (I think about the consequences of my actions), #23

(I don’t need to be told how to behave) and #40 (I am capable of accepting

responsibility) were eliminated because of the returned skewness and

kurtosis levels beyond the desired range of +1 and -1.

Inter-item correlations were analyzed for the remaining 36 questions.

No correlations outside the desired range between .30 and .90 were noted.

Items #4 (Before making decisions I consult experts), #22 (Problem solving is

stressful), # 27 (I cooperate well when cooperating in a team) and #31 (My

actions are determined by my peer group) were eliminated because the

returned no inter-item correlations within the desired range of between .30

and .90. Prior to the removal of item #’s 3, 4, 6, 22, 23,27,31, and 40,

reliability analysis returned a Cronbach’s Alpha = .86. After removal of the

items reliability analysis returned a Cronbach’s Alpha = .87, indicating an

increase in reliability was achieved as a result of the elimination of the eight

items.

To determine if factor analytic techniques were appropriate for the

remaining 32 item scale, the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling

adequacy was used on the entire data set. The data set returned a KMO =

.842, indicating factorability (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001).

First PFA was run on the data set to reduce the remaining 32 items

into components. PFA was chosen because it is widely used and understood

(Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001). PFA was run using the eigenvalue rule (Kaiser,

1960), which asserts that factors with eigenvalues less than 1.0 should not be

43

retained. The PFA returned 9 components with eigenvalues > 1.0 explaining

45.1% of the variance in the data set (see Table 4-4). After the initial PFA the

Cattell (1966) scree test was used in conjunction with the variance results to

determine the number of factors to retain. Figure 4-1 shows a break between

Figure 4-1

Thirty-two Item Scale Scree Plot.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

Factor Number

0

2

4

6

8

Eig

en

va

lue

Scree Plot

the fourth and fifth eigenvalues and little difference between the fifth and sixth

eigenvalues. The remaining eigenvalues trail off with only slight differences,

indicating a four-factor solution.

The four-factor solution only extracts 34.5% of the variance, which

might indicate the extraction of additional factors would be beneficial.

44

However, PFA was used to develop scales, and thus the researcher was not

interested in a complete description of the variance. Based on DeVallis

(2003) a four-factor scale was decided on. While the PFA shown in Table 4-4

was used to determine the number of factors to retain, a second PFA with

orthogonal varimax rotation was used to improve the interpretability of the

four factor solution. Table 4-5 shows the results of the PFA with an

orthogonal varimax rotation. Since loadings below .30 account for less than

10% overlapping variance, they were left out of the output to assist the

reading and interpretation of the remaining factors.

45

Table 4-4

Total Variance Explained for the Initial Principal Factors Analysis.

Factor Initial eigenvalues Extraction Sums of Squared Loadings

Rotation Sums of Squared Loadings

Total % of Variance Cumulative % Total % of Variance Cumulative % Total

1 7.618 23.805 23.805 7.099 22.184 22.184 2.639

2 2.033 6.353 30.158 1.518 4.743 26.928 2.293

3 1.934 6.043 36.201 1.419 4.435 31.362 2.249

4 1.522 4.757 40.958 .997 3.115 34.477 1.484

5 1.402 4.381 45.339 .955 2.986 37.463 1.380

6 1.375 4.296 49.635 .857 2.677 40.140 1.235

7 1.167 3.648 53.283 .624 1.950 42.090 1.220

8 1.160 3.625 56.908 .572 1.786 43.876 1.053

9 1.005 3.141 60.050 .490 1.532 45.408 .977

10 .993 3.103 63.153

11 .908 2.837 65.990

12 .845 2.641 68.631

13 .824 2.574 71.205

14 .740 2.314 73.519

15 .714 2.230 75.749

16 .670 2.093 77.842

17 .652 2.036 79.878

18 .631 1.973 81.850

19 .610 1.907 83.757

20 .560 1.751 85.508

21 .530 1.657 87.165

22 .510 1.594 88.759

23 .493 1.541 90.300

24 .474 1.482 91.783

25 .439 1.371 93.154

26 .403 1.259 94.413

27 .377 1.179 95.592

28 .332 1.037 96.629

29 .304 .949 97.578

30 .287 .898 98.476

31 .252 .789 99.265

32 .235 .735 100.000

Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring.

46

Table 4-5

Rotated Factor Matrix for the Four Factor Principal Factor Analysis.

Factor

1 2 3 4

26 .712

12 .648

15 .566

8 .481

28 .460

25 .456

36 .407 .338

2 .392

32 .367

37 .355 .327

38 .311

16 .732

17 .634

10 .569

5 .565

14 .475

18 .388 .379

7 .387

35 .370

24 .341

29 .680

30 .604

19 .540

34 .478

9 .471

1 .351

11 .334

33

20 (reverse) .665

39 (reverse) .548

13 (reverse) .427

21

Extraction Method: Principal Axis Factoring. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.

47

In order to develop homogeneous sub-scales, only items that loaded on a

single factor were retained. Items 2, 8, 12, 15, 25, 26, 28, 32, and 38

comprised Factor 1. Items 7, 10, 14, 16, 17, 24, and 35 comprised Factor 2.

Items 1, 9, 11, 19, 29, 30, and 34 comprised Factor 3. Items 13, 20 and 39

comprised Factor 4. It was determined that each of the extracted factors

would comprise sub-scales for the next version of the Self-Authorship

Questionnaire (SAQ v.2). Graduate students and faculty in the KIN 885

“Psychological Factors of Adventure Programming” were enlisted to help

name the factors. Based on the items in the first sub-scale it was named the

Situational Coping sub-scale. Factor 2 was named the Interpersonal

Leadership sub-scale. Factor 3 was named the Self-efficacy sub-scale.

Lastly, Factor 4 was named the Knowledge Creation sub-scale.

SAQ v.2 (see Appendix B) retained 27 items from 40 items in the

original pool. The 27 remaining items were divided into four sub-scales:

Situational Coping, Interpersonal Leadership, Self-efficacy and Knowledge

Creation. Reliability analysis on the SAQ v.2 based on the original data

returned a Cronbach’s Alpha = .85 indicating high reliability.

48

Table 4-6

Items Included In Identified Sub-Scales.

Factor 1 Situational Coping

No matter what happens I can handle it.

No matter what the situation I can handle it.

Whatever situation arises I can come up with a solution.

I cope well with changing situations.

I enjoy coming up with solutions to my problems.

I am willing to make difficult decisions.

I am calm in stressful situations.

I can research a topic and form my own opinion effectively.

I am efficient and do not waste time. Factor 2 Interpersonal Leadership

I am a capable leader.

I am able to handle positions of authority.

I am a good leader when things need to get done.

I feel comfortable speaking in front of a group.

I communicate effectively with other people.

I am given opportunities to make a difference.

I am given real responsibility in my life.

I am effective in social situations.

Factor 3 Self-efficacy

I show good judgment in most situations.

I see myself as a capable person.

I make the right decision a majority of the time.

I know I have the ability to do anything I want to do.

I am able to do things as well as most other people.

I am good at deciding whether a risk is worth taking.

I am capable of regulating my own actions. Factor 4 Knowledge Creation

I never question the opinion of my superiors.

I believe experts are in the best position to decide what people should learn.

I always look to my teacher/boss for direction.

49

CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION

Introduction

This chapter provides a summary of the two phases of the study and

then presents findings, conclusions, implications, and limitations for each

phase. The last portion of the chapter will discuss recommendations for

future research.

Summary

Phase 1

During the bi-annual national meeting of the Plast organization, seven

individuals were identified to participate in a focus-group interview in an

attempt to ascertain the underlying themes of self-authorship. The Plast

organization has existed for over 90 years and since its beginnings has

identified self-authorship as one of its program goals. The interview lasted

approximately one and a half hours and analysis of the resulting data

identified eight themes: Judgment/Decision making, Self-regulation, Self-

confidence, Interpersonal Skills, Empowerment, Creative Problem-solving,

Leadership/Responsibility and Knowledge Creation. These eight themes

were then compared with self-authorship research and used to generate a

50

pool of items for inclusion in a self-authorship measure (SAQ v.1). The SAQ

v.1 contained forty items each measured on a five-point Likert scale.

Phase 2

During the second phase of the study the SAQ v.1 was administered to

289 subjects. Using data screening, item analysis and principal factors

analysis, a number of questions from the SAQ v.1 were eliminated. The final

self-authorship questionnaire (SAQ v.2) was reduced from 40 to 27 questions

that measured self-authorship along four sub-scales: Situational Coping,

Interpersonal Leadership, Self-efficacy and Knowledge Creation.

Findings and Conclusions

Phase 1

The first phase of this study provided in-depth insight into a long

running program which has pursued the development of self-authorship in

young people for over 90 years. The case-study participants identified

several key areas of their program which they believe to be critical to self-

authorship. During the interview participants noted the importance of

developing a healthy sense of self-confidence in adolescents. This self-

confidence was carefully developed through a well-thought-out progression of

programming which included a heavy reliance on group/cooperative work with

gradually increasing responsibility and consequences. Additionally, young

51

people were provided with various opportunities for leadership during which

they practice decision-making skills. Another important key to the Plast

program was the constant presence, allowing for an intimate knowledge of

each participant over a substantial period of time (6 – 10 years), of counselors

and guides who provide feedback and supervision in an effort to aid

adolescents on their journey toward self-authorship. The focus-group

participants agreed that ultimately the goal of Plast was to empower young

people by providing them with a strong foundation for decision-making now

and in the future.

It is interesting to note that the majority of ideas and themes agreed

upon by the focus-group participants are congruent with the results of a 10-

year longitudinal study conducted by Baxter-Magolda (2001). In her book

Making Their Own Way (2001) Baxter-Magolda talks about validating

student’s ability to know as a critical element of self-authorship. Among the

things Baxter-Magolda identified were participation of collaborative work,

leadership roles, and membership in a community. Both Kegan (1994) and

Baxter-Magolda (1999, 2001) write about the importance of teachers acting

as guides for young people for an extended period of time. The extended

interaction allows for the teachers/guides to eventually remove themselves

from the relationship once the young person has the necessary foundation to

self-author their present and future.

52

Phase 2

The second phase of the study used various quantitative data analysis

techniques to develop an instrument to measure SA. Descriptive data

showed the majority of items scored toward the high end of the five point

Likert scale. Two primary reasons are given to explain this phenomenon.

First, the sample of subjects used in the study was not randomly selected

from the general population as is suggested for use in the development of

scales (DeVellis, 1991; Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001). Secondly, all of the

subjects that formed the data set were either Plast members or students in

Outdoor Education or Recreation Management & Policy classes. It is inferred

that these students have had some exposure to programming, which

seemingly enhances self-authorship thus explaining scores on the higher end

of the range.

Allowing for the not ideal distribution of data subsequent descriptive,

statistics were examined for the items, and based on unfavorable skewness

and kurtosis scores, four items were eliminated from the measure. Review of

inter-item correlations led to the elimination of four additional items. leaving a

total of 32 to be used in Principal Factors Analysis (PFA).

Principal Factors Analysis was selected because of its wide use and

ease of interpretation (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001), especially in the preliminary

stages of instrument design. The PFA ultimately returned four distinct factors

which were then converted into sub-scales. The four scales were labeled:

Situational Coping, Interpersonal Leadership, Self-efficacy and Knowledge

53

Creation. A closer look at the sub-scales reveals a great thematic similarity to

themes identified both by self-authorship literature (Baxter-Magolda, 1998,

1999, 2001; Kegan, 1994) and the case-study conducted during Phase 1.

For example, the Situational Coping sub-scale includes several items that

evoke self-regulation, decision-making, self-confidence and empowerment.

The Interpersonal Leadership sub-scale harkens back to SA’s reliance on

leadership opportunities and the development of interpersonal skills. In the

Self-efficacy sub-scale ideas of self-confidence, good judgment, and

empowerment are again evident. Lastly, the Knowledge Creation sub-scale

gets at the very heart of the antecedents to the inability to self-author (Baxter-

Magolda 1999, 2001; Kegan 1994) and the reliance on authority figures for

direction/knowledge. The results of the PFA suggest that the SAQ v.2 indeed

measures elements which are inherent to self-authorship.

Implications

Phase 1

By conducting a case study which examines a well-established youth

organization that relies on Outdoor Education methodology and defines SA as

a program goal, the author was able to gain valuable insights into SA. The

themes derived from the qualitative data allowed for comparison with

established SA research conducted in the realm of higher education.

Because of the congruence between the case-study results and SA literature

54

the use of the qualitative data in creating a quantitative measure of SA

enhances the SAQ’s theoretical base. Additionally, the comparison furthers

the author’s contention that Outdoor Education programming enhances SA in

young people.

Phase 2

By creating a statistically tested and valid measure of self-authorship

(SAQ v.2), future research can be conducted to investigate the relationship

between OE and the enhancement of SA. The SAQ v.2 can be

supplemented with additional demographic (i.e. age, gender, socio-economic

status, academic standing) and antecedent variables (i.e. program duration,

location) to provide more in-depth understanding of SA and its relationship to

OE. Additionally, the measure can be used by OE programs for evaluation

and program enhancement. The measure can be customized to relate SA to

any number variables. Lastly, the SAQ has the potential to be used as a

screening tool for admission into certain programs (i.e. graduate school, study

abroad) and by human resources personnel when hiring new employees.

Limitations

Phase 1

While the author attempted to use best practices of qualitative case-

study data collection and analysis, certain limitations were present. The

trustworthiness of the data collected and analyzed was primarily established

55

only by the researcher. Initially, the focus-group interview participants

member checked each other by coming to consensus, during the interview,

on what self-authorship meant to them and how Plast went about achieving it.

It would have strengthened the data if the interview participants were later

asked to review the analyzed data which yielded the eight identified themes

used to create the SAQ v.1. During the data analysis portion of Phase 1 the

author did not have the opportunity to involve others in analysis in an attempt

to increase trustworthiness using triangulation. The faculty chair of the thesis

committee did have oversight over the author and periodically reviewed the

author’s findings before allowing him to proceed to subsequent steps in the

research. This interaction acted as a form of peer debriefing; however the

inclusion of other faculty and graduate students would have been beneficial.

Lastly, it is important to mention that conducting several focus-group

interviews with different groups of Plast members would have strengthened

the study.

The generation of items using existing measures, though commonly

practiced (Neill, 2003; Sibthorp, 2000) has its limitations. The author chose

items based solely on his understanding of the case-study results and self-

authorship literature. Additionally, when the author could not find an existing

measure he created items again based solely on his understanding of the

pertinent data. In the future the involvement of experts for review of the items

prior to inclusion in an instrument is advisable.

56

Phase 2

The quantitative portion of the study had several limitations. The

sample used in collecting data for analysis was a volunteer convenience

sample and therefore does not have the same strength as would a completely

random sample. Since the sample in this study was predominantly in college

students or recent graduates with at least a minimum exposure to Outdoor

Education methods, the generalizability to other populations of young adults

is questionable. Additionally the total sample number (N = 289) was at the

lower end of suggested sample sizes for Principal Factors Analysis

(Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001).

While three or four items may constitute a reliable and valid scale,

Loewenthal (1996) suggests that between six and fifteen items should be

used to measure a single factor. Therefore, the inclusion of the Knowledge

Creation sub-scale into the SAQ v.2 could be questioned.

The administration of the SAQ v.1 at the University of New Hampshire

was done by permission of faculty and based on the consent of the

participants. However, the SAQ v.1 was usually administered at the end of

class, and the author observed several participants filling out the SAQ v.1

while seeming rushed or hurried, which likely had some effect on the integrity

of the data.

In an effort to distill the data into easily interpretable factors which

could be converted into sub-scales for inclusion in the SAQ v.2, only one type

of factor extraction method was used, namely Principal Factors Analysis with

57

orthogonal varimax rotation. For the purposes of this study this method was

the most appropriate because it weighted the factors independently of one

another, allowing for easy extraction of factors. Other rotational methods (i.e.

oblique) do not weigh the factors independently and allow co-variance into the

analysis, providing less distinguishable factors with greater explanatory value.

Recommendations for Future Research

While this study was a good first step toward creating a reliable and

valid measure of self-authorship, much more research has to be conducted

before a psychometrically and theoretically sound instrument can be

produced. Additional research is needed to identify more thoroughly the

underlying themes of self-authorship to better inform the creation of

subsequent SAQ’s. Based upon this study the following areas of study could

be explored:

similar studies involving a greater number of focus-groups;

similar studies involving other Outdoor/Experiential education

programs;

similar studies involving focus-groups made up of educational experts

interested in the field of SA;

similar studies involving experts in adolescent epistemological

development;

58

similar studies involving a larger randomly selected sample size for use

in Principal Factors Analysis;

similar studies involving a greater number factor analytic data analysis

techniques;

similar studies with several versions of the resulting measure

undergoing confirmatory factor analysis;

further investigation of the connection between particular program

elements (i.e. location, duration, facilitation style) and the development

of SA;

further investigation of the connection of antecedent variables (i.e.

gender, age, national origin, socio-economic status) to self-authorship.

Further research about SA will provide educators from every facet of

education a better understanding of their students needs. The continued

understanding of the connection between SA and OE will lead to the

strengthening of programs and facilitators. Additionally, by demonstrating a

strong connection between OE and the development of SA, the field will gain

greater standing as a viable or even preferable approach to education.

LIST OF REFERENCES

60

Auerbach, C. F., & Silverstein, L. B. (2003). Qualitative Data. New York, NY: New York University Press.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-Efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral

change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social

cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York: W. H.

Freeman and Company. Battegay, R. (1977). Group models, group dynamics, sociological and

psychological aspects of group formation and evaluation. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 55(5), 330-344.

Baxter Magolda, M.B. (1998). Developing self-authorship in young adult life.

Journal of College Student Development, 39, 143-156. Baxter Magolda, M.B. (1999). Creating Contexts for Learning and Self

Authorship: Constructive-Developmental Pedagogy. Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University.

Baxter Magolda, M.B. (2001). Making Their Own Way: Narratives for

Transforming Higher Education to Promote Self-Development. Sterling, VA: Stylus.

Bertolami, C. (1981). Effects of a Wilderness Program on Self-esteem and

locus of control orientations of young adults. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 266 928)

Brower, M. (1998). Group development as constructed social reality: A social-

cognitive understanding of group formation. Social Work with Groups, 12(2), 23-41.

Cason, D. R. (1993). A meta-analysis of adventure programming with

adolescents. Unpublished master’s thesis, Georgia College, Milledgeville.

Cason, D. R., & Gillis, H. L. (1994). A meta-analysis of adventure

programming with adolescents. Journal of Experiential Education, 17, 40-47.

Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate

Behavioral Research, 1, 245-276.

61

DeVellis, R. F. (2003). Scale development: theory and applications (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Dewey, J (1938/1997). Experience and Education. New York: Simon &

Schuster. Ewert, A. (1983). Outdoor adventure and self-concept: A research analysis.

Eugene, OR: Center of Leisure Studies, University of Oregon. Ewert, A. (1989). Outdoor adventure pursuits: foundations, models, and

theories. Columbus, OH: Publishing Horizons. Ewert, A., & Haywood, J. (1991). Group development in the natural

environment: Expectations, outcomes and techniques. Journal of Environment and Behavior, 23(5), 592-615.

France, T.J. (1994) The impact of Project Adventure activities on self-

perception. Unpublished masters thesis, Springfield College, Springfield, OH.

Gall, M., Gall, J., & Borg, W. (2003) Educational Research, Pearson Allyn &

Bacon. Gass, M., Garvey, D. & Sugerman, D. (2003). The Long-Term Effects of First-

Year Student Wilderness Orientation Program, Journal of Experiential Education, 26(1), 34-41.

Hahn, K. (1957). Outward Bound. New York: World Books. Hahn, K. (1970). The education of thought. In Kurt Hahn and the development

of Outward Bound: A compilation of essays. London: Outward Bound. Hans, T. (2000) A meta-analysis of the effects of adventure programming on

locus of control. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy. 30, 33-60. Hattie, J., Marsh, H. W., Neill, J. T., & Richards, G. E. (1997). Adventure

education and Outward Bound: Out-of-class experiences that make a lasting difference. Review of Educational Research, 67, 43-87.

Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor

analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 141-151. Kegan, R. (1982). The evolving self: Problem and process in human

development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

62

Kegan, R. (1994). In over our heads: The mental demands of modern life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Koesler, R. & Propst, D. (1998). Bandura goes outdoors: Role of self-efficacy

in the outdoor leadership development process. Leisure Studies. 20, 319-344.

Kolb, D. C. (1993) General self-efficacy and outdoor experiential training.

(Doctoral dissertation, Georgia State University, 1993). Dissertation Abstracts International, 54, 1733.

Lefcourt, H.M. (1982) Locus of Control: Current trends in theory and research.

New York: Wiley. Loewenthal, K. (1996). An introduction to psychological tests and scales.

London: UCL Press. Marsh, H. W., Richards, G.E., & Barnes, J. (1986a) Multidimensional self-

concepts: The effect of participation in an Outward Bound Program. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 50, 195-204.

Marsh, H. W., Richards, G.E., & Barnes, J. (1986b) Multidimensional self-

concepts: A long-term follow-up of the effect of participation in an Outward Bound course. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 12, 475-492.

McFee, M., & Gass, M. (1993). A group development model for adventure

therapy programs. In M. Gass (Ed.), Adventure therapy: Therapeutic applications of adventure programming. Dubuque, IA: Kendall Hunt.

Miles, M.B., & Huberman, A.M. (1994). Qualitative Data Analysis: An

Expanded Sourcebook, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Miles, J.C. & Priest, S. (1990) Adventure Education. State College, PA:

Venture Publishing. Newhouse, G. C. (2002). Effects of adventure education utilizing a portable

climbing wall activity on children’s self-efficacy. (Doctoral dissertation, The University of New Mexico, 2002). Dissertation Abstracts International, 63 (02).

Outdoor Education Research and Evaluation (2003, September 10). A

Measurement Tool for Assessing the Effects of Adventure-based Programs on Outcomes for Youth-at-Risk Participants. Retrieved October 15, 2003 from http://www.wilderdom.com/tools/leq/ YouthDevelopmentLEQScalesPaper.htm

63

Parle, M. D. (1986). The role of self-efficacy in Outward Bound: An investigation of a high school course. Unpublished bachelor’s Honours dissertation, University of New England, Armidale, New South Wales, Australia.

Paxton, T. S. (1998) Self-efficacy and outdoor adventure programs: A

quantitative and qualitative analyses. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1998). Dissertation Abstract International, 59, 2717.

Paxton, T. & McAvoy, L. (1998) Self-efficacy and Adventure Programs:

Transferring Outcomes to Everyday Life. Coalition for Education in the Outdoors Research Symposium Proceedings.

Priest, S. & Gass, M. (1997). Effective leadership in adventure programming.

Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Richards, G. E., Ellis, L. & Neill, J. T., (2002). The ROPELOC: Review of

Personal Effectiveness and Locus of Control: A Comprehensive Instrument for Reviewing Life Effectiveness. . Self-Concept research: Driving International research Agendas. Proceedings of the 2nd International Biennial Conference. University of Western Sydney, Australia. Aug 6-8, 2002.

Rogers, S.E., Chamberlin, J., Langer Ellison, M., & Crean, T. (1997). A

Consumer-Constructed Scale to Measure Empowerment Among Users of Menatl Health Services, 48(8), 1042-1047.

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal and external

control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 609 (80), 1-28. Rubin, H. J., & Rubin, I. S. (1995). Qualitative Interviewing: The Art of Hearing

Data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Russell, K.C. (2003). An assessment of outcomes in outdoor behavioral

healthcare treatment. Child and Youth Care Forum, 32(6), 355-381. Russell, K.C., & Phillips-Miller, D. (2002). Perspectives on the wilderness

therapy process and its relation to outcome. Child and Youth Care Forum, 31(6), 415-437.

Shelter, Jay (1998). Adventure recreation and adolescent perceived self-

efficacy. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering Vol 59 (6-B) Dec. 1998 p. 3099.

64

Sibthorp, J. (2000). Instrument Validation and Multivariate Assessment of Life Skill Development in Adolescents through the Adventure Education Process. Unpublished dissertation. Indiana University, Bloomington.

Sibthorp, J. (2003). An Empirical Look at Walsh and Golins’ Adventure

Education Process Model: Relationships between Antecedent Factors, Perceptions of Characteristics, and Changes in Self-Efficacy. Journal of Leisure Research, 35(1), 80-106.

Starosolsky, Y. (1948). The Big Game, Munich: Plast Publishing. Stenger, T.L. (2001). Sequence of adventure-based resident outdoor

education programs and middle school students’ perceptions of life effectiveness. (Doctoral dissertation, Oklahoma State University, 2001). Dissertation Abstracts International, 62, 2881.

Tabachnik, T. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using Multivariate Statistics.

Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Tysovsky, O. (1912). Life In Plast, Lviv: Plast Publishing. Walsh, V., & Golins, G. (1976). The exploration of the Outward Bound

Process. Denver, CO: Colorado Outward Bound. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 144 754).

Yin, R.K. (1989). Case study research: design and methods. Newbury Park,

CA: Sage. Yin, R.K.(1993). Applications of Case Study Research. Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage. Yurchuk, D. (2003) The Exploration and Use of Tenets in Outdoor Education

Programs, Unpublished Qualifying Masters Packet, Prescott College. Zwart, T. J. (1988). The effects of wilderness/adventure program on the self-

concept, locus of control orientation, and interpersonal behavior of delinquent adolescents. Dissertation Abstracts International, 49 (07), 1709A. (UMI No. 8816827)

APPENDIX A

66

67

68

69

APPENDIX B

71

APPENDIX C

73

Dear Participant:

I am conducting a research project to find out the effects of participation in outdoor

education programs on self-authorship. I am writing to invite you to participate in

this project. I plan to work with approximately 10 participants in this study.

If you agree to participate in this study, you will be asked to participate in a video-

taped interview conducted by Taras Ferencevych which will take approximately 1 – 1

½ hours. Additionally, you will be asked to review transcripts and analyzed data to

ensure that your ideas are not being misrepresented.

While you will not receive any compensation to participate in this project, the

anticipated benefits are the personal fulfillment and satisfaction you will receive from

helping me in my pursuit of a Masters degree. Additionally, the information you

provide will potentially be used to better the field of Outdoor Education.

Participation is strictly voluntary; refusal to participate will involve no prejudice,

penalty, or loss of benefits to which you would otherwise be entitled. If you agree to

participate and then change your mind, you may withdraw at any time during the

study without penalty.

The investigator seeks to maintain the confidentiality of all data and records

associated with your participation in this research. You should understand, however,

there are rare instances when the investigator is required to share personally-

identifiable information (e.g., according to policy, contract, regulation). For example,

in response to a complaint about the research, officials at the University of New

Hampshire, designees of the sponsor(s), and/or regulatory and oversight government

agencies may access research data. Data will be kept on my computer; only myself

and my faculty advisor will have access to the data. If you would like to have access

to any data that you provided during the course of this study please ask and it will be

made available to you. At the end of the research study the videotapes will be erased

or returned to you, the transcripts will be kept on file.

The work will be conducted by me under the supervision of Dr. Michael Gass as part

of a Masters thesis in the department of Kinesiology at the University of New

Hampshire. Occasionally, I will ask fellow graduate students for help in analyzing

the data. In these instances your identity will be protected by the use of a pseudonym.

I am a graduate student in Outdoor Education in the department of Kinesiology at the

University of New Hampshire.

If you have any questions about this research project or would like more information

before, during, or after the study, you may contact Taras Ferencevych at Department

of Kinesiology, New Hampshire Hall, Durham, NH 03820, 603.781.0672,

74

[email protected]. If you have questions about your rights as a research subject, you

may contact Julie Simpson in the UNH Office of Sponsored Research at 603-862-

2003 to discuss them in confidence.

I have enclosed two copies of this letter. Please sign one indicating your choice and

return in the enclosed envelope. The other copy is for your records. Thank you for

your consideration.

Sincerely,

Taras Ferencevych

Masters Student – Department of Kinesiology

Yes, I, ________________________________ consent/agree to participate in this

research project.

No, I, ________________________________ refuse/ do not agree to participate in

this research project.

APPENDIX D

76

77