sensorimotor integration during rubber hand …...would like to thank kaviraj udupa and behzad elahi...

140
Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand Illusion in Normal Subjects and Subjects with Parkinson’s Disease by Reina Isayama A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Institute of Medical Science University of Toronto © Copyright by Reina Isayama 2018

Upload: others

Post on 16-Feb-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand Illusion in Normal Subjects and Subjects with Parkinson’s Disease

by

Reina Isayama

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Science

Institute of Medical Science University of Toronto

© Copyright by Reina Isayama 2018

Page 2: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

II

Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand Illusion in Normal

Subjects and Subjects with Parkinson’s Disease

Reina Isayama

Master of Science

Institute of Medical Science

University of Toronto

2018

Abstract

Rubber hand illusion (RHI) measures visuo-tactile multisensory integration. Non-motor

symptoms are well recognized in Parkinson’s disease (PD) and PD patients have impaired

sensation and sensorimotor integration. We hypothesized that 1) RHI decreases sensorimotor

integration in healthy subjects 2) PD patients show reduced RHI and less modulation of

sensorimotor integration by RHI. We used transcranial magnetic stimulation to examine short-

and long-latency afferent inhibition (SAI and LAI), which measures the influence of sensory

input on the primary motor cortex (M1), and the posterior parietal cortex (PPC)-M1 interactions

during RHI. In healthy subjects, we found reduced SAI and LAI during RHI, and stronger RHI

correlated with greater inhibitory PPC-M1 interactions. PD patients showed normal RHI, but had

no correlation between RHI strength and PPC-M1 interactions. Our results show that RHI

modulates both early and late stages of sensorimotor integration in healthy subjects and this

effect of RHI is altered in PD.

Page 3: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

III

Acknowledgments

I am very thankful to my supervisor, Dr. Robert Chen, for his continuous supports

throughout my graduate studies. Dr. Chen is the best mentor that I have ever had. He

always provided constructive feedback with encouragements and was very patient when

I am trying to acquire novel techniques and concepts. He was very good at responding

to my questions and always to the point. His attitude was very critical to my academic

development in general as well as the completion of my thesis work. He went beyond

his duties when I needed opinions and supports for my future carrier. He always listened

to me carefully and gave me his suggestions which opened my career paths; I was able

to study neurophysiology in movement disorders with him as an observer, a volunteer

researcher, a graduate student, and a clinical fellow. The years that I spend with him

have made a huge impact on my carrier.

I would also like to extend my gratitude to my thesis committee members, Dr.Tarek Rajji

and Prof. Matthias Niemeier. Scheduling the meetings was quite challenging due to

conferences and clinical works, however, their willingness to accommodate their

schedule always helped me organizing ones. Through the meeting, they kindly

supported me and gave me great suggestions. Every program advisory committee

meeting greatly improved my analysis and interpretation skills of the project.

I am also appreciative of our collaborators, Lucilla Cardinali and Professor Alessandro

Farne. Lucilla came to our lab all the way from France to teach us the method of rubber

hand illusion and helped us establish the basis of the project. Alessandro Farne always

provided me with his wonderful insights into the interpretations of the data and with

great deals of encouragements and excitements.

I was very fortunate to work in Dr. Chen’s laboratory. Here, I met many amazing

colleagues. Michael Vesia, who is also my mentor, always took time to improve the

project design, reviewed data with me, and offered me useful suggestions to improve

my thesis. His mentorship was not only limited to my thesis work but also extended to

other studies, including his projects. Working with him enabled me to expand my

knowledge of transcranial magnetic stimulation, motor controls, and academic writings.

Page 4: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

IV

I also greatly appreciate the enduring supports from Gaayathiri Jegatheeswaran and

Carolyn Gunraj. I am extremely thankful for their tireless and continuous efforts in data

collection and supports throughout the years. Sometimes the data collection went long

and they must have been very exhausted. But they were always willing to stay with me

until I finish the collection. This thesis could not be completed without them. I also

would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data

collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping with patient recruitment. I thank

Anuj Rastogi for his willingness to providing me his help throughout the program. I am

so glad that we could turn the consciousness course we took together into the module in

order to carry on the legacy of Jose Luis Perez-Velazquez. Without his

encouragements, I would not even think about giving a lecture about rubber hand

illusion in the module. All the friendship that I gained while working with Dr. Chen’s lab

members is one of the most precious gifts that I have.

I would also like to acknowledge Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) -

Dystonia Medical Research Foundation (DMRF) Canada, and Parkinson Canada for

funding this work. I thank all the participants in the study for their willingness to

contribute their time for science. Their supports enabled me to conduct the studies and

were the source of my motivation.

Last, but not the least, I would like to thank my family; my parents and my in-law parents

for physical and mental supports, encouragements and their trusts in me to achieve the

goal even though I needed to be far away from them. I am grateful to my husband,

Tetsuya. He was the reason I came to Canada, and I am glad that we did because both

of us have spent such wonderful years in Toronto. We were successfully obtained

graduate degrees from Canada; Master of science for myself, and Philosophy of doctor

for him. I am also grateful for our baby, Yume, who recently joined our family, for

bringing more joys to our lives. Without their love and supports, my achievement would

not be successful.

Page 5: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

V

Contributions

Project Conception: Reina Isayama, Robert Chen, Alessandro Farne, Michael Vesia,

Behzad Elahi, Lucilla Cardinali

Research Ethics and Subject Recruitment: Reina Isayama, Carolyn Gunraj, Tasnuva

Hoque

Data Collection and TMS Administration: Reina Isayama, Gaayathiri

Jegatheeswaran, Carolyn Gunraj

Data Analysis: Reina Isayama

Manuscript Writing: Reina Isayama

Fundings: Canadian Institutes of Health Research - Dystonia Medical Research

Foundation Canada Fellowship and Parkinson Canada Clinical Research Fellowship

Page 6: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

VI

Table of Contents

Acknowledgments ........................................................................................................... iii

Contributions .................................................................................................................... v

Table of Contents ............................................................................................................ vi

List of Abbreviations ....................................................................................................... xii

List of Figures ................................................................................................................ xv

List of Tables................................................................................................................. xvi

1 LITERATURE REVIEW ............................................................................................... 1

1.1 Parkinson’s Disease (PD) ..................................................................................... 1

1.1.1 Parkinson’s disease and its motor symptoms ............................................. 1

1.1.2 Sensory Symptoms and Sensory Impairments in Parkinson’s disease ...... 2

1.1.3 Deficits in Sensorimotor Integration in Parkinson’s Disease ....................... 4

1.2 Physiology and Pathophysiology of Sensorimotor Integration ............................... 7

1.2.1 Neurophysiology of Sensorimotor Integration in Healthy Populations ........ 7

1.2.2 The Use of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) to Assess Sensorimotor Integration ............................................................................ 8

1.2.2.1 Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) ...................................... 8

1.2.2.2 Short-latency Afferent Inhibition (SAI) and Long-latency Afferent Inhibition (LAI) in Healthy Populations ............................. 9

1.2.2.3 Short-latency Afferent Inhibition (SAI) and Long-latency Afferent Inhibition (LAI) in PD Patients ........................................ 12

1.3 Multisensory Integration ...................................................................................... 14

1.3.1 Multisensory Sensory Integration and Sensorimotor Integration in Healthy Populations .................................................................................. 14

1.3.2 Neural Correlates of Multisensory Integration .......................................... 16

1.3.3 The Role of Posterior Parietal Cortex in Multisensory Integration and Sensorimotor Integration .......................................................................... 17

1.3.4 Functional Interactions between the Left Posterior Parietal Cortex (PPC) and Ipsilateral Primary Motor Cortex (M1) Examined by Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) ................................................. 21

1.3.5 Rubber Hand Illusion as a Tool to Investigate Multisensory Integration ... 24

Page 7: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

VII

1.3.5.1 Behavioral Studies of Rubber Hand Illusion ................................ 24

1.3.5.2 The Role of Cognitive Process during RHI .................................. 27

1.3.5.3 RHI influences on Motor Responses ........................................... 28

1.3.5.4 Application of RHI in Neuropsychiatric Disorders ........................ 28

1.3.5.5 Brain Regions Involving in Multisensory Integration for Own Body and Rubber Hand Illusion ................................................... 29

1.3.5.6 Posterior Parietal Cortex and Rubber Hand Illusion .................... 30

1.3.6 Multisensory Integration in PD patients .................................................... 31

1.3.6.1 Multisensory Integration and its Involvement in Sensorimotor Integration in PD patients at the Behavioral Level ....................... 32

1.3.6.2 Posterior Parietal Dysfunction in PD patients .............................. 33

2 RESEARCH AIMS and HYPOTHESIS ...................................................................... 35

2.1 Objectives of the Present Thesis ......................................................................... 35

2.1.1 Study One: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by Rubber Hand Illusion in Healthy Subjects ....................................................................... 35

2.1.2 Study Two: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by Rubber Hand Illusion in Parkinson’s Disease Patients ................................................... 35

2.2 Hypotheses of the Present Thesis ...................................................................... 36

2.2.1 Study One: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by the Rubber Hand Illusion in Healthy Subjects ............................................................. 36

2.2.2 Study Two: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by the Rubber Hand Illusion in Parkinson’s Disease Patients .......................................... 36

3 STUDY ONE: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by Rubber Hand Illusion in Healthy Subjects ........................................................................................................ 37

3.1 Abstract ............................................................................................................... 37

3.2 Introduction ......................................................................................................... 38

3.3 Methods .............................................................................................................. 39

3.3.1 Subjects .................................................................................................... 40

3.3.2 Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI) and behavioural assessments ...................... 40

3.3.3 Digital nerve stimulation (DNS)................................................................. 41

3.3.4 Surface electromyography (EMG) recording ............................................ 41

Page 8: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

VIII

3.3.5 Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) ................................................. 42

3.3.5.1 Experiment 1: Influence of the RHI on SAI and LAI ..................... 42

3.3.5.2 Experiment 2: The influence of the RHI on the interaction between the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the primary motor cortex (M1) ........................................................................ 42

3.3.6 Experimental Designs ............................................................................... 44

3.4 Data Analysis ...................................................................................................... 48

3.5 Results ................................................................................................................ 49

3.5.1 Questionnaire on the Rubber Hand Illusion Experiences ......................... 49

3.5.2 Proprioceptive judgment (PJ) ................................................................... 50

3.5.3 Experiment 1: Modulation of Short- and Long-latency Afferent Inhibition by RHI ...................................................................................................... 53

3.5.4 Experiment 2: Modulation of the PPC-M1 interaction by the RHI ............. 56

3.6 Discussion ........................................................................................................... 59

3.6.1 Behavioural results confirmed the synchronous condition induces RHI ... 59

3.6.2 The RHI Reduces Short-latency Sensorimotor Integration ....................... 59

3.6.3 The RHI Reduces Long-latency Sensorimotor Integration Reduction of LAI with ..................................................................................................... 60

3.6.4 Inhibitory PPC-M1 interaction is associated with the strength of RHI ....... 62

3.6.5 Clinical Implications of the findings ........................................................... 63

3.7 Conclusions ......................................................................................................... 64

3.8 Acknowledgements ............................................................................................. 64

4 STUDY TWO: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by Rubber Hand Illusion in Parkinson’s Disease .................................................................................................. 65

4.1 Abstract ............................................................................................................... 65

4.2 Introduction ......................................................................................................... 66

4.3 Methods .............................................................................................................. 67

4.3.1 Subjects .................................................................................................... 67

4.3.2 Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI) and behavioral assessments ........................ 68

4.3.3 Digital nerve stimulation (DNS)................................................................. 69

4.3.4 Surface electromyography (EMG) recording ............................................ 69

Page 9: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

IX

4.3.5 Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) ................................................. 70

4.3.5.1 Experiment 1: Measurement of short-latency afferent inhibition (SAI) and long-latency afferent inhibition (LAI) ............................ 70

4.3.5.2 Experiment 2: Measurement of the interaction between the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the primary motor cortex (M1) ............................................................................................. 70

4.3.6 Experimental Designs ............................................................................... 71

4.4 Data Analysis ...................................................................................................... 72

4.4.1 Clinical Assessments ................................................................................ 72

4.4.2 RHI Behavioural Assessments ................................................................. 72

4.4.2.1 Evaluation of the Questionnaire ................................................... 72

4.4.2.2 Evaluation of the Proprioceptive judgment (PJ) task ................... 73

4.4.3 Assessment of Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) Measurements .......................................................................................... 73

4.4.3.1 Experiment1: The modulation of Short-latency afferent inhibition (SAI) and Long-latency afferent inhibition (LAI) by RHI .............................................................................................. 73

4.4.3.2 Experiment2: The modulation of Posterior parietal cortex (PPC)-Primary motor cortex (M1) interaction by RHI ................... 74

4.5 Results ................................................................................................................ 75

4.5.1 Clinical and demographic data ................................................................. 75

4.5.2 Behavioural assessments of Rubber Hand Illusion .................................. 77

4.5.2.1 Questionnaire on the rubber hand illusion experiences ............... 77

4.5.2.2 Proprioceptive judgment (PJ) ...................................................... 77

4.5.3 Modulation of short-latency afferent Inhibition (SAI) by Rubber Hand Illusion ...................................................................................................... 80

4.5.3.1 Comparison between Parkinson’s Disease Patients and Healthy Controls .......................................................................... 80

4.5.3.2 Dopaminergic Influence on the SAI modulation in Parkinson’s Disease Patients .......................................................................... 80

4.5.4 Modulation of Long-latency afferent Inhibition (LAI) by Rubber Hand Illusion ...................................................................................................... 82

Page 10: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

X

4.5.4.1 Comparison between Parkinson’s Disease Patients and Healthy Controls .......................................................................... 82

4.5.4.2 Parkinson’s Disease Patients and the Effect of Dopaminergic Medications ................................................................................. 82

4.5.5 Modulation of PPC-M1 interaction by RHI ................................................ 86

4.5.5.1 Comparison between Parkinson’s Disease Patients and Healthy Controls .......................................................................... 86

4.5.5.2 The Effect of Dopaminergic Medications in Parkinson’s Disease Patients .......................................................................... 87

4.5.5.3 Correlation between the Strength of RHI and PPC-M1 Interaction .................................................................................... 90

4.6 DISCUSSION ...................................................................................................... 93

4.6.1 Preserved RHI at the Behavioural Level in PD patients ........................... 93

4.6.2 Overall Reduction in SAI in PD-OFF Compared to HCs ........................... 95

4.6.3 Reduced modulation in LAI in PD- OFF patients and Tactile-motor Integration Improvement by Medication .................................................... 96

4.6.4 Disrupted PPC-M1 interaction in PD patients ........................................... 97

4.6.5 Limitation of the Study .............................................................................. 98

4.7 Conclusions ......................................................................................................... 99

5 GENERAL DISCUSSION ........................................................................................ 100

5.1 Summary of Results .......................................................................................... 100

5.2 Normal Perception of Rubber Hand Illusion in PD Patients at the Behavioural Level.................................................................................................................. 100

5.3 The effect of RHI on Short-latency Afferent Inhibition in Healthy individuals and PD patients ................................................................................................. 102

5.4 Rubber Hand Illusion Effect on Long-latency Afferent Inhibition in PD Patients and HCs ............................................................................................................ 102

5.5 PPC-M1 Interactions in PD patients and HCs ................................................... 104

5.6 Converging Evidence for Disrupted Sensorimotor and Multisensory Integration in Parkinson’s disease ..................................................................... 107

5.7 Future Directions ............................................................................................... 108

5.7.1 Multisensory Integration in PD patients .................................................. 108

Page 11: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

XI

5.7.2 Disrupted and Compensatory Sensorimotor Integration Pathways in PD patients ............................................................................................. 108

5.7.3 The Influence of Dopaminergic Medications ........................................... 109

5.7.4 Possible Intervention Therapy for PD patients ....................................... 110

5.8 Conclusions ....................................................................................................... 110

References................................................................................................................... 112

Page 12: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

XII

List of Abbreviations

aIPS Anterior part of Intraparietal Sulcus

ANOVA Analysis of Variance

cIPS Caudal part of Intra Parietal Sulcus

CS Conditioning Stimulus

DBS Deep Brain Stimulation

DNS Digital Nerve stimulation

DNS23-TS Digital Nerve Stimulation followed by Test Stimulus at Inter-stimulus

Interval of 23ms

DNS203-TS Digital Nerve Stimulation followed by Test Stimulus at Inter-stimulus

Interval of 203ms

EEG Electroencephalogram

EMG Electromyographic

FDG Fluorodeoxy glucose

FDI First Dorsal Interosseous

Fig Figure

f-MRI Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

HCs Healthy Controls

IPS Intraparietal Sulcus

ISIs Inter-stimulus Intervals

LAI Long-Latency Afferent Inhibition

LOC Lateral Occipital Cortex

MEP Motor Evoked Potentials

M1 Primary Motor Cortex

PA Posterior-Anterior

PD Parkinson’s Disease

Page 13: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

XIII

PD-OFF Patients with Parkinson’s Disease without Dopaminergic Medication State

PD-ON Patients with Parkinson’s Disease with Dopaminergic Medication State

PET Positron Emission Tomography

PJ Proprioceptive Judgment

PMv Ventral Premotor Cortex

PPC Posterior Parietal Cortex

Pre-SMA Pre-Supplementary Motor Area

PT Physiotherapy

RHI Rubber Hand Illusion

RLS Restless Leg Syndrome

rTMS repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

SAI Short-Latency Afferent Inhibition

SD Standard Deviation

SEPs Somatosensory Evoked Potentials

SEM Standard Error of the Mean

SLF Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus

SMA Supplementary Motor Area

SPL Superior Parietal lobe

ST Sensory Threshold

STDT Somatosensory Temporal Discrimination Threshold

STN-DBS Subthalamic Nucleus Deep Brain Stimulation

STS Superior Temporal Sulcus

S1 Primary Sensory Cortex

S2 Secondary Sensory Cortex

TMS Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

TPJ Temporoparietal Junction

Page 14: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

XIV

TS Test Stimulus

UPDRS Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale

Page 15: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

XV

List of Figures

Figure 1 Sensorimotor Integration and Multisensory Integration .................................... 6

Figure 2 Motor Evoked Potentials and Short- and Long- Latency Afferent Inhibition .... 11

Figure 3 Subregions in the Posterior Parietal Cortex .................................................... 20

Figure 4 TMS studies with Paired-pulse approach with Two Coils ................................ 23

Figure 5 Rubber Hand Illusion ...................................................................................... 26

Figure 6 Experimental Set-up and Design .................................................................... 45

Figure 7 Stimulation of PPC with TMS .......................................................................... 47

Figure 8 Behavioural Results ........................................................................................ 51

Figure 8 Modulations of Short- and Long-Latency Afferent Inhibitions by RHI .............. 54

Figure 9 RHI and PPC-M1 Interactions ......................................................................... 57

Figure 11 Behavioral Results in PD patients and HCs .................................................. 79

Figure 12 Modulation of Short-latency Afferent Inhibition in PD patients and HCs ....... 81

Figure 13 Modulation of Long-latency Afferent Inhibition in PD patients and HCs ........ 84

Figure 14 Modulation of PPC-M1 interactions by RHI ................................................... 88

Figure 15 Correlations between RHI Strength and PPC-M1 interactions ...................... 91

Page 16: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

XVI

List of Tables

Table 1 Patients demographic Characteristics .............................................................. 76

Page 17: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

1

1 LITERATURE REVIEW

1.1 Parkinson’s Disease (PD)

1.1.1 Parkinson’s disease and its motor symptoms

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is the second most common neurodegenerative disorder after

Alzheimer disease (S. Fahn, Przedborski, Serge, 2009). The cardinal sign of PD is

motor dysfunction. Symptomology includes slowness of movements (bradykinesia),

increased muscle tone (rigidity), tremor, loss of postural reflexes, flexed posture, and

freezing of gait. These motor dysfunctions often lead to difficulties in executing goal-

directed actions such as locomotion and fine motor movements with hands. For

instance, patients with PD can have a slowness of movement (bradykinesia), reduction

in the size of movements (hypokinesia), and difficulties in movement initiation (i.e.,

freezing). As the disease progresses, patients with PD have neurological deficits that

impair cognition, affect, and motor function, and are often left with disabilities making

them dependent on others for activities of daily living.

The pathological hallmark of PD is degeneration of the nigrostriatal dopaminergic

neurons. This results in striatal dopamine depletion, and abnormal protein inclusions

known as Lewy bodies are found in many of the surviving neurons. By the time the

motor symptoms appear, about 60% of the dopaminergic neurons in the substantia

nigra are lost and a decrease of about 80% of the dopaminergic content in the striatum

occurs (S. Fahn, Przedborski, Serge, 2009). Although the pathophysiology of PD is not

fully understood, it is widely accepted that it primarily involves disruption of the basal

ganglia-motor cortex circuits (Wichmann & DeLong, 2013).

The current treatments of PD are mainly aimed at controlling the motor symptoms.

Pharmacotherapy with dopamine precursors or dopamine agonists is the main basis of

Page 18: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

2

the treatment. When the disease advances, surgical treatments, usually deep brain

stimulation (DBS), may be considered. Although, these pharmacological and surgical

therapies are the main strategies for PD treatment, physiotherapy (PT) and physical

exercise are also important for managing the motor symptoms in PD.

1.1.2 Sensory Symptoms and Sensory Impairments in Parkinson’s

disease

Although the motor symptoms are often considered as the predominant symptoms of

PD and used to diagnose the disease, many PD patients experience non-motor

symptoms such as sensory impairments (S. Fahn, Jankovic, & Hallett, 2011).

Decreased sense of smell, hyposmia, can be detected in more than half of PD patients

and often precedes the onset of the motor symptoms (S. Fahn et al., 2011). Pain in the

initially affected limb may also be one of the first PD symptoms and is often relieved by

dopaminergic medications (S. Fahn et al., 2011). Specific sensory symptoms such as

pain, numbness, tingling or burning sensations are also reported in about 63% of PD

patients (Shulman, Taback, Bean, & Weiner, 2001). These abnormal sensations can

result from PD, but in some patients, are induced by dopaminergic therapy (S. Fahn et

al., 2011).

Routine clinical examinations of the sensory systems in PD patients are usually normal.

However, several experimental studies have revealed abnormal somatosensory

perceptions in PD patients.

Tactile perception is compromised in PD patients. Deficits in somatosensory tactile

perceptions in the larynx and airway are seen and can contribute to speech or

swallowing problems in PD patients (Conte, Khan, Defazio, Rothwell, & Berardelli,

2013). Higher levels of tactile sensory perceptions such as spatial and temporal

discriminations are also impaired in PD patients (Fiorio et al., 2007; Sathian,

Zangaladze, Green, Vitek, & DeLong, 1997; Zia, Cody, & O'Boyle, 2003). For example,

tactile spatial acuity (discrimination) of grating orientation decreases in PD patients

Page 19: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

3

(Sathian et al., 1997; Shin, Kang, & Sohn, 2005). Dopaminergic medications improve

this impairment (Shin et al., 2005). Many studies in PD patients have demonstrated

increased somatosensory temporal discrimination threshold (STDT), which can be

normalized or decreased by dopaminergic medications (Artieda, Pastor, Lacruz, &

Obeso, 1992; Conte et al., 2010; Fiorio et al., 2007; Lee, Kim, & Lyoo, 2005; Rocchi et

al., 2013). Proprioception, which refers to the ability to perceive movements or

orientations of own limbs/body parts, is another type of somatosensory perception that

can be abnormal in PD. Abnormalities in proprioception in PD patients are usually

presented as abnormal kinesthesia, and defined more specifically as conscious

awareness of position, motion and orientation of own limb or body part in space evoked

by movements (Conte et al., 2013). Several studies show that proprioception without

the visual feedback was deteriorated more in PD patients than healthy controls (Flash,

Inzelberg, Schechtman, & Korczyn, 1992; Jobst, Melnick, Byl, Dowling, & Aminoff,

1997; Konczak et al., 2009; Maschke, Gomez, Tuite, & Konczak, 2003; Zia, Cody, &

O'Boyle, 2000). Specifically, patients with PD showed significantly larger errors

compared to healthy controls (HC) when verbally reporting the sensed arm position

without vision for passive and active arm movements (Zia, Cody, & O'Boyle, 2002).

Other studies using joint-angle matching tests showed that PD patients had decreased

sensitivity to sense limb position either passively or actively when vision is occluded

(Maschke et al., 2003; O'Suilleabhain, Bullard, & Dewey, 2001; Zia et al., 2000). Several

studies reported that the sense of own arm motion was abnormalin PD patients

(Konczak, Krawczewski, Tuite, & Maschke, 2007; Maschke et al., 2003; Maschke, Tuite,

Pickett, Wächter, & Konczak, 2005). Dopaminergic medications are thought to worsen

proprioceptive deficits. For instance, PD patients in the OFF-medication state show a

worsening of their elbow proprioception after taking dopaminergic medications

(O'Suilleabhain et al., 2001). Similarly, PD patients on dopaminergic medications

showed greater errors in placing their own hand in a remembered position in space

compared to age-matched HC or PD patients in OFF-medication state (Wagle Shukla et

al., 2013). Several studies examined the effect of DBS on proprioceptive deficits in PD

patients. Subthalamic nucleus DBS (STN-DBS) reversed the proprioceptive deficits in

Page 20: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

4

PD patients (Maschke et al., 2005; Wagle Shukla et al., 2013), suggesting that the basal

ganglia are involved in proprioceptive processing.

Although the dopaminergic neurons in the basal ganglia seem to be involved in

abnormal sensory processing in PD patients, why these deficits in tactile, or

proprioceptive perceptions occur in PD remains unclear. To understand the

somatosensory deficits in PD patients, further knowledge of how sensory inputs are

utilized to guide accurate motor controls is required. The deficits in sensory system in

PD not only lead to the sensory symptoms but could also be related to the motor

symptoms. Indeed, the proprioceptive and kinaesthetic impairments seem to be

involved in the motor dysfunction as evidenced by significant a correlation between the

severity of the motor symptoms and the proprioceptive impairments (Maschke et al.,

2003).The following sections will review the abnormal interaction between the sensory

and the motor systems in PD.

1.1.3 Deficits in Sensorimotor Integration in Parkinson’s Disease

The motor system in the brain closely links with the sensory system because planning

and executing accurate voluntary movements depend crucially on inputs from the

sensory system. The sensory system transforms the sensory inputs into motor outputs

(Figure1). This process is known as sensorimotor integration and plays an important

role in motor planning and executions(Kandel, Schwartz, Jessell, Siegelbaum, &

Hudspeth, 2013; Stone & Gonzalez, 2015). Various types of sensory information such

as vision, tactile, auditory perceptions or proprioception can converge with motor

outputs through visuo-motor integration, tactile-motor integration, audio-motor

integration or proprioceptive-motor integration, respectively.

Research in sensorimotor integration in PD has led to the suggestion that sensory

impairments and defective sensorimotor integration can result in motor symptoms and

decreased motor performances. Behavioural or kinematic studies that examined

sensorimotor integration in PD patients have demonstrated failures of integration of

Page 21: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

5

sensory inputs with motor program at the behavioural level (Adamovich, Berkinblit,

Hening, Sage, & Poizner, 2001; Demirci, Grill, McShane, & Hallett, 1997; Flash et al.,

1992; Jobst et al., 1997; Rickards & Cody, 1997).

Proprioceptive-motor integration deficits are found in PD (Flash et al., 1992; Jobst et al.,

1997; Rickards & Cody, 1997). For instance, PD patients are less susceptible to muscle

vibration-induced movement errors that are normally observed in HC (Rickards & Cody,

1997). In fact, the defective proprioceptive-motor integration in PD patients leads to an

over-reliance on visual inputs (Flash et al., 1992; Jobst et al., 1997).

Tactile-motor integration in PD patients may also be affected. A study reported impaired

tactile-motor integration in PD patients during an object-shape-recognition test with

active exploratory movements (B. J. Weder et al., 1999). However, other studies have

suggested that tactile stimuli are equally beneficial on motor performance in PD patients

compared to HC. PD patients show normal accuracy when using tactile information to

reach-to-point at a target on the skin with the contralateral arm (Jobst et al., 1997). In a

tactile precision grip task, in which participants are required to use tactile feedback to

control grip force, both PD patients and HC successfully increased the grip force when

the objects were slippery (Ingvarsson, Gordon, & Forssberg, 1997). These results

indicate normal tactile-motor integration in PD.

The inconsistency of the sensorimotor integration in PD patients at the behavioural level

may arise from the complex physiology of sensorimotor integration process. To design

appropriate experiments to address whether sensorimotor integration is defective in PD

patients, it is necessary to understand the pathophysiology of sensorimotor integration.

Page 22: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

6

Figure 1 Sensorimotor Integration and Multisensory Integration

Figure 1

Sensorimotor integration involves the conversion of sensory inputs into motor outputs

Multisensory integration, which involves combining sensory inputs from different

modalities such as tactile, proprioceptive and visual inputs, can modulate motor

responses through sensorimotor integration. As each sensory input is encoded in a

different reference frame, multisensory integration areas such as premotor or posterior

parietal cortices transform the sensory inputs into a common reference frame, which are

then combined in the motor system. M1: primary motor cortex, S1: primary

somatosensory cortex, V1: primary visual cortex.

Page 23: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

7

1.2 Physiology and Pathophysiology of Sensorimotor

Integration

1.2.1 Neurophysiology of Sensorimotor Integration in Healthy

Populations

Sensorimotor integration has been observed in vast areas in the brain including basal

ganglia (Hall, 2008; McFarland & Haber, 2000; Reig & Silberberg, 2014), thalamus

(Hirashima & Yokota, 1997; McFarland & Haber, 2000), cerebral cortices (Chen,

Corwell, & Hallett, 1999; Pfurtscheller, Stancák, & Neuper, 1996; Salenius, Schnitzler,

Salmelin, Jousmäki, & Hari, 1997; Salmelin & Hari, 1994), cerebellum (Proville et al.,

2014) and the superior colliculus in the brainstem (Doubell, Skaliora, Baron, & King,

2003). The basal ganglia receive inputs from cortical areas including sensory areas and

project back to a wide range of cortical areas including motor areas via the thalamus

(Hall, 2008). Thus, the basal ganglia as well as the thalamus allow sensorimotor

integration using the basal ganglia-motor cortical circuits. Synaptic convergence of

inputs from the primary motor cortex with inputs from sensory cortex has been found in

striatum of rats, suggesting an anatomical substrate of sensorimotor integration in the

basal ganglia.

Cortico-spinal pathway also contributes to sensorimotor integration. Electrophysiological

studies with healthy human subjects have shown that electrical stimuli to peripheral

nerves modulate the excitability of the corticospinal motor neurons(Chen et al., 1999;

Hirashima & Yokota, 1997). Depending on the timing of the nerve stimulation, the

cortico-spinal motor neurons may be facilitated or inhibited. Magnetoencephalographic

(MEG) and electroencephalographic (EEG) studies suggest that sensorimotor

integration occurs at cortical level. They showed hat peripheral nerve stimulation

induced immediate suppression of rhythmic oscillation followed by increased activity in

Page 24: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

8

the primary motor cortex (Chen et al., 1999; Pfurtscheller et al., 1996; Salenius et al.,

1997; Salmelin & Hari, 1994).

1.2.2 The Use of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) to Assess

Sensorimotor Integration

1.2.2.1 Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS)

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is a technique to focally stimulate the brain

non-invasively and painlessly from the scalp (Rothwell, 1997). The stimulator consists of

a large capacitor and a stimulation coil. The stimulator delivers electrical currents, which

produce transient magnetic fields lasting approximately 200 µs. The magnetic field

penetrates the scalp and the skull, and reaches the cerebral cortex usually 1-2 cm

below the scalp. The intensity of magnetic field decreases rapidly with distance from the

coil, and it is likely that the magnetic field activates neural elements at the surface of the

cerebral cortices (Wassermann et al., 2011). TMS likely activates axons rather than the

cell bodies or the initial segments of neurons (Barker, Garnham, & Freeston, 1991;

Rothwell, 2011). As different neuronal populations have different activation thresholds,

the intensity of the stimuli determines which neuronal populations will be activated

(Rothwell, 2011).

The magnetic stimulations of the primary motor cortex (M1) can evoke

electromyographic (EMG) responses in the contralateral muscles, termed motor evoked

potentials (MEPs) (Figure2). Comparing the responses from different coil orientations,

the largest MEPs are produced when the coil is held to induce currents in the posterior-

anterior (PA) direction, and approximately perpendicular to the central sulcus (Hallett,

2007). MEP recordings are widely used to assess motor cortical excitabilities in healthy

subjects and in patients with impairment in the central nervous system (Rosler &

Magistris, 2011). The modulation of the MEP amplitude size after a single-pulse TMS to

M1 reflects the excitability of the both cortical and spinal motor neurons (Abbruzzese &

Trompetto, 2002). The modulation before and after a task can be used to assess the

Page 25: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

9

change in the cortical excitability by the task if the excitability of the spinal motor

neurons is unchanged.

1.2.2.2 Short-latency Afferent Inhibition (SAI) and Long-latency

Afferent Inhibition (LAI) in Healthy Populations

The modulation of cortical excitability in M1 by peripheral nerve stimulation can be

measured by pairing the peripheral nerve stimulation with TMS to M1 (Figure 2). TMS to

M1 after median nerve stimulation (MNS) at the wrist or digital nerve stimulation (DNS)

decreases MEP amplitudes, suggesting an inhibitory effect of electrical nerve

stimulation (Chen et al., 1999; Ni et al., 2011; Sailer et al., 2003; Tokimura et al., 2000).

The inhibitory effect depends on the inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs) between the median

or digital nerve stimulation and TMS to M1 (Chen et al., 1999; Devanne et al., 2009; Ni

et al., 2011; Tokimura et al., 2000). The inhibition is greatest when nerve stimulation is

three times the individual’s sensory threshold (ST). Interestingly, inhibition does not

increase with higher nerve stimulation intensities once the intensity reaches three times

of ST (Cash, Isayama, Gunraj, Ni, & Chen, 2015; Chen et al., 1999; Ni et al., 2011).

M1 inhibition that occurs approximately 20ms after MNS is known as short-latency

afferent inhibition (SAI). SAI likely occurs at the cortical level because direct recordings

from patients with implanted cervical epidural electrodes show that the later

corticospinal waves (Indirect or I waves) were suppressed by the MNS (Tokimura et al.,

2000). Considering that approximately 20ms is required for peripheral stimuli to arrive at

the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) as indicated by the N20 wave of somatosensory

evoked potentials (SEPs) from MNS, SAI likely reflects the direct interaction between

S1and M1 (Sailer et al., 2003). Indeed, it has been shown that M1 receives mono-

synaptic inputs from S1 in rodents (Rocco-Donovan, Ramos, Giraldo, & Brumberg,

2011), suggesting that SAI may reflect the mono-synaptic inputs (Cash et al., 2015).

Long-latency afferent inhibition (LAI) occurs when the MNS is delivered about 200ms

before TMS to M1 (Chen et al., 1999; Sailer et al., 2003). The mechanism of LAI

Page 26: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

10

remains unclear. However, the finding that LAI was not influenced by dopaminergic

medications suggests that LAI may involve sensorimotor integration in cortical

association areas (Sailer et al., 2003).

SAI and LAI are well-established parameters to measure tactile sensorimotor integration

in healthy subjects (Lapole & Tindel, 2015; Wood, Gallese, & Cattaneo, 2010) as well

as patients with neurological disorders such as PD (Sailer et al., 2007; Sailer et al.,

2003; Wagle Shukla et al., 2013), dystonia (Abbruzzese, Marchese, Buccolieri,

Gasparetto, & Trompetto, 2001; Kessler, Ruge, Ilić, & Ziemann, 2005; Pirio Richardson,

Bliem, Voller, Dang, & Hallett, 2009; Richardson et al., 2008), restless leg syndrome

(RLS) (Rizzo et al., 2010) and Tourette syndrome (Orth & Rothwell, 2009).

Page 27: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

11

Figure 2Motor Evoked Potentials and Short- and Long- Latency Afferent Inhibition

Figure 2

Pairing peripheral nerve stimulation with transcranial stimulation (TMS) to the primary

motor cortex (M1) and comparing the amplitude of motor evoked potentials (MEPs)

enable us to measure how the peripheral nerve stimulation modulates the cortical

excitability of M1. If the ratio of the conditioned (with peripheral/digital nerve stimulation)

to the unconditioned (test TMS alone to M1) MEP amplitudes exceeds 1, it indicates

facilitatory influence of the peripheral nerve stimulation on M1 excitability. In contrast,

the ratio below 1 indicates inhibitory influence of the nerve stimulation.

Page 28: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

12

1.2.2.3 Short-latency Afferent Inhibition (SAI) and Long-latency

Afferent Inhibition (LAI) in PD Patients

Several studies tested SAI and LAI in PD patients to investigate sensorimotor

integration. A study showed reduced LAI in PD patients in the OFF medication state

while SAI was normal (Sailer et al., 2003). LAI was also reduced in PD patients ON

medications, indicating that dopaminergic medications did not restore the abnormal LAI

(Sailer et al., 2003). Interestingly, SAI was reduced with dopaminergic medications but

the reduced SAI was restored by STN-DBS (Sailer et al., 2007). LAI was also restored

by STN-DBS (Wagle Shukla et al., 2013). The level of SAI in PD patients was found to

be correlated with the severity of the motor symptoms, especially bradykinesia,

suggesting a possible contribution of the altered sensorimotor integration to the motor

symptoms (Tamburin et al., 2003).

Considering the pathological changes occurring in the basal ganglia in PD and the

ability of the basal ganglia to integrate sensory inputs with motor outputs, the basal

ganglia may mediate the abnormal sensorimotor integration in PD patients. PD patients

showed an abnormal reduction of the first inhibitory component of EMG activities in a

cutaneous reflex study and the reduced inhibition was restored by dopaminergic

medications (Fuhr, Zeffiro, & Hallett, 1992). The normalization of the reduced cutaneous

reflex response by dopaminergic medications and the restoration of SAI and LAI by

STN-DBS (Sailer et al., 2007; Wagle Shukla et al., 2013) may suggest involvement of

the basal ganglia circuits in sensorimotor integration. However, because SAI was

normal in PD OFF medication and reduced LAI was not restored by dopaminergic

medications, the altered sensorimotor integration can also be mediated by non-

dopaminergic pathways such as cortical areas.

Because SAI likely reflects direct interaction between S1 and M1, normal SAI in PD

OFF medication and reduced SAI in PD ON medication likely represents negative

dopaminergic influence over sensory cortical areas, especially S1. The reduced SAI

was seen in the more affected side but not in the less affected side, suggesting that the

deficits in SAI in PD patients ON medication occur in more advanced PD (Sailer et al.,

Page 29: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

13

2003). Given that dopaminergic medication influences proprioception (O'Suilleabhain et

al., 2001), it is possible that the processing of cutaneous or muscle afferent information

can be also influenced. Intracortical circuits within S1 show that perception of sensory

stimuli induced by paired-pulse TMS over S1was modulated in healthy subjects and that

this modulation was abnormal in PD patients with dopaminergic medications compared

to HC.PD patients in the OFF medication state did not differ from HC (Palomar et al.,

2011). The result suggests dopaminergic influence in S1 in PD patients, consistent with

functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) data showing decreased activity in

somatosensory and motor regions in PD patients during passive tactile and movement

tasks (Zhao et al., 2014).

Reduced LAI in PD patients regardless of dopaminergic medication states is most likely

related to structures involved in sensory processing other than S1 because SAI in PD

OFF medication state was not different from HC. Although the pathway involving LAI

remains unclear, the LAI deficit in PD patients may involve cortical sensory processing

areas rather than basal ganglia pathways because it was not normalized by

dopaminergic medications (Sailer et al., 2003).Previous studies have demonstrated

depressed SEPs and proprioception-related potentials in PD (Abbruzzese & Berardelli,

2003; Seiss, Praamstra, Hesse, & Rickards, 2003). SEPs in PD patients were reduced

in the frontal and parietal areas (Bostantjopoulou et al., 2000; Pierantozzi et al., 1999;

Rossini et al., 1993). These findings support the involvement of the frontal (Abbruzzese

& Berardelli, 2003) and parietal (S1 or secondary sensory cortex:S2) (Seiss et al., 2003)

areas in the abnormal sensory processing in PD patients. Reduced LAI may involve

abnormal cortical activations in the frontal-parietal areas. Several neuroimaging studies

have also shown abnormal cortical activation related to somatosensory processing in

PD. A positron emission tomography (PET) study showed an abnormal pattern of

somatosensory processing at the cortical level (Boecker et al., 1999). Sensory

stimulation results in increased activation of ipsilateral associative sensory areas (S2

and insular cortex), and reduced activation in contralateral sensorimotor and promoter

cortices, S2and posterior cingulate cortex in PD patients compared to HC (Boecker et

al., 1999).Over-activated bilateral prefrontal regions in PD patients are found with fMRI

Page 30: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

14

during a tactile-motor integration, possibly reflecting a compensatory mechanism (Zhao

et al., 2014).Importantly, these studies indicate the involvement of multiple cortical

areas in the abnormal sensorimotor integration.

1.3 Multisensory Integration

1.3.1 Multisensory Sensory Integration and Sensorimotor Integration

in Healthy Populations

Healthy individuals often rely on multiple sensory inputs to perceive the environment

more accurately rather than depending on unimodal sensory information (Amedi,

Malach, Hendler, Peled, & Zohary, 2001; Buelte et al., 2008; Kayser & Logothetis,

2007). This interaction between sensory inputs from different modalities is termed

multisensory integration (Ernst & Bülthoff, 2004). When multisensory information from

different sensory modalities is in different units, or coordinates, it has to be transformed

into the same units and coordinates and then, integrated into a coherent multisensory

percept (Ernst & Bülthoff, 2004). Previous studies suggest that multisensory integration

can modulate perception. Depending on the context, multisensory integration increases

or decreases sensitivity to subtle stimuli in one sensory modality, resulting in perceptual

changes (Kayser & Logothetis, 2007; Mahoney, Wang, Dumas, & Holtzer, 2014;

Pavani, Spence, & Driver, 2000). Multisensory integration can also influence motor

responses. Planning and executing an action requires an integration of vision,

proprioception, audition, and tactile perception to provide a perceptual representation of

the external world (Figure 1). When audio cue is combined with visual cue to a motor

response, the motor response is facilitated in healthy individuals due to modulation of

the motor response by multisensory integration (visuo-auditory facilitation) (Fearon,

Butler, Newman, Lynch, & Reilly, 2015). Whether the motor response is facilitated or

inhibited also depends on the context (Fearon et al., 2015; Hershenson, 1962;

Kanayama, Sato, & Ohira, 2007; Kayser & Logothetis, 2007). Multisensory integration

plays an important role in sensorimotor integration. Because sensory information is

Page 31: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

15

initially encoded relative to the characteristics of the individual sensory receptor system,

it needs to be integrated and transformed into a common reference frame (Giummarra,

Gibson, Georgiou-Karistianis, & Bradshaw, 2008) to construct a coherent perceptual

representation in the brain. This representation is then transformed into motor outputs

for appropriate action.

How multisensory integration modifies perception or motor response in healthy

individuals remains unclear because there are few neurophysiological investigations on

the influence of multisensory integration on the sensory and motor systems. The

modification of perceptions or motor responses by multisensory integration may be

related to the efficiency of sensory processing of each modality during multisensory

integration. Recent computational research proposes that there is flexible weighting of

different sensory modalities during multisensory integration. Sensory signals are

weighted in each context and this weighting is flexible in order to minimize errors

resulting from noisy signals and to improve sensorimotor performance (Sober & Sabes,

2005). Further studies are required to test this computational model.

Page 32: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

16

1.3.2 Neural Correlates of Multisensory Integration

Historically, physiological studies of multisensory integration focused primarily on

identifying structures that show responses to more than one sensory modality. Parietal,

temporal and frontal cortical regions of primates have been reported to respond to

multisensory inputs (Rizzolatti, Cattaneo, Fabbri-Destro, & Rozzi, 2014). A human fMRI

study revealed areas in the premotor and the posterior parietal cortices that respond to

both visual and tactile stimuli to specific body parts (Bremmer et al., 2001).These areas

are distributed across wide areas of association cortices which include posterior parietal

cortex (PPC), ventral premotor cortex (PMv), lateral occipital cortex (LOC) in occipito-

temporal region and superior temporal sulcus (STS). Electrophysiological studies in

monkeys have shown that neurons in the premotor area and PPC, particularly areas

within the inferior parietal lobe and the ventral intraparietal sulcus (IPS) respond to both

visual and tactile stimuli (Graziano, 2001; Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Gallese, 1997).

IPS in the PPC and the LOC become active during visual-haptic object shape

recognition task (Amedi et al., 2001; Lacey & Sathian, 2012), suggesting that these

areas mediate visuo-tactile integration. Neurons in PMv and STS respond to

somatosensory, auditory and visual signals (Cappe, Rouiller, & Barone, 2012).

In addition to these areas within the association cortices, few studies have reported that

low-level sensory cortical areas such as primary somatosensory cortex (S1), thalamus,

putamen, and superior colliculus in the brain stem may also contribute to multisensory

integration(Cappe et al., 2012; C. Cappe, E. M. Rouiller, & P. Barone, 2012; Graziano,

2001; Rizzolatti, Fadiga, et al., 1997). The thalamus interconnects with cortical areas

including PPC and premotor cortical areas and may be involved in multisensory

integration. S1 is originally thought to be a purely somatosensory area. However,

monkey studies reveal a direct connection between S1 and visual cortical areas (Cappe

et al., 2012) and visual responsiveness in parts of S1 (Iwamura, 1998; Lacey & Sathian,

2012), suggesting that S1 plays a role in multisensory integration. Interestingly, some of

the connections were restricted to the representations of certain body parts (Cappe &

Page 33: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

17

Barone, 2005), suggesting the importance of multisensory integration for perception of

body parts. This topic will be covered in a later section (section 1.3.5).

1.3.3 The Role of Posterior Parietal Cortex in Multisensory Integration

and Sensorimotor Integration

Fronto-parietal association areas such as the premotor cortex and posterior PPC play

an important role in the planning and control of goal-directed movements (Koch,

Fernandez Del Olmo, et al., 2008; Petrides & Pandya, 1984; Vesia & Crawford, 2012).

These areas have been shown to respond to multisensory stimuli as well as being a

critical hub sub-serving specific motor functions (Brozzoli, Makin, Cardinali, Holmes, &

Farne, 2012). This network is thought to provide an interface between perception and

action. Goal-directed movements are mediated by PPC neurons, which receive inputs

from the early visual or somatosensory areas and are interconnected with the premotor

cortex(Koch et al., 2007; Petrides & Pandya, 1984; Rizzolatti, Fogassi, & Gallese, 1997;

Vesia & Crawford, 2012). The premotor cortex directly projects to M1 with access to the

control of upper limb movements (Davare, Montague, Olivier, Rothwell, & Lemon, 2009;

Graziano & Gross, 1998; Koch, Fernandez Del Olmo, et al., 2008).Monkey studies

show that PPC is active during the preparation of specific movements involving the

hand(Andersen, 1997; Graziano & Gross, 1998).

The PPC can be subdivided into several functionally specialized subregions (Figure

3).Area 5 in the superior parietal lobe (SPL) predominantly receives tactile and

proprioceptive inputs from S1 and S2 (Sakata, Takaoka, Kawarasaki, & Shibutani,

1973). An electrophysiological study showed that some neurons in the area also

responded to certain visual stimuli (Sakata et al., 1973). These multisensory inputs

converge with neurons in Brodmann area 5 in SPL (Sakata et al., 1973), which codes

the location or the posture of arm relative to the other body parts (Andersen, 1997;

Gentile, Petkova, & Ehrsson, 2011; Lewis et al., 2011) and creates a holistic concept of

the body image by transforming the sensory inputs into a common coordinate system

Page 34: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

18

(Franklin & Wolpert, 2011; Sakata et al., 1973). SPL is also known to send intra-

hemispheric inputs to the arm and hand representations in the M1 in human (Andersen,

1995; Andersen & Buneo, 2002; Premji, Rai, & Nelson, 2011; Ziluk, Premji, & Nelson,

2010)and non-human primates (Petrides & Pandya, 1984).Fibers from SPL also project

to other frontal areas such as dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) and supplementary motor

area (SMA) (Makris et al., 2005; Petrides & Pandya, 1984). A lesion in SPL causes both

motor and sensory deficits due to inability to maintain internal representations of objects

and own body parts(Wolpert, Goodbody, & Husain, 1998).Thus, SPL is critical in

sensory-motor transformations and coding of spatial location of objects with the arms

(Crammond & Kalaska, 1989; Granek, Pisella, Blangero, Rossetti, & Sergio, 2012;

Hawkins, Sayegh, Yan, Crawford, & Sergio, 2013; Kalaska, 1996; Kalaska, Caminiti, &

Georgopoulos, 1983; Reed, Klatzky, & Halgren, 2005; Sakata et al., 1973).

The inferior parietal lobe (IPL) is another subregion of the PPC which has multiple

functions such as processing information for perceptual body judgement (Kammers,

Verhagen, et al., 2009) and recognising an object with the hand (Reed et al., 2005). IPL

receives indirect influences of somatosensory inputs from other areas in PPC such as

SPL and visual regions to facilitate multisensory integration.

Areas within the intra-parietal sulcus (IPS), the sulcus between SPL and IPL, dominate

visuo-motor integration. Several monkey studies have revealed that a part of the IPS is

active during preparation of reaching, grasping, and eye movements (Andersen, 1997;

Graziano & Gross, 1998). The area transforms stimuli in peripersonal space, the space

within an arm’s reach, into a hand-centered representation, therefore informing the

spatial position of target objects with respect to the hand position (Graziano & Gross,

1998; Vesia & Crawford, 2012). The anterior part of the IPS (aIPS) is specialized for

grasping (Begliomini, Wall, Smith, & Castiello, 2007),while the medial part of the IPS

(mIPS) is involved in reaching (Vesia & Crawford, 2012). Virtual lesions induced by

TMS to the aIPS disrupts ongoing movements (Franklin & Wolpert, 2011; Tunik, Frey, &

Grafton, 2005; Wolpert et al., 1998).

Page 35: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

19

The superior parieto-occipital cortex (SPOC) is located in medial posterior part of PPC.

This area selectively encodes reaching and pointing actions to peripheral locations

(Vesia & Crawford, 2012). It is also strongly activated by viewing tools in personal space

(Vesia & Crawford, 2012). Repetitive TMS to SPOC resulted in deviation of reaching,

suggesting that sensorimotor integration relating to arm involves SPOC (Vesia, Prime,

Yan, Sergio, & Crawford, 2010).

Thus, PPC allows functional interactions between the sensory and the motor system

and plays an important role in multisensory integration.

Page 36: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

20

Figure 3 Subregions in the Posterior Parietal Cortex

Figure 3: The posterior parietal cortex (PPC)can be subdivided into several functionally

specialized subregions. SPL:the superior parietal lobe, IPL:the inferior parietal lobe,

SPOC: the superior parieto-occipital cortex, aIPS:the anterior part of the intra-parietal

sulcus (IPS), mIPS:the medial part of the IPS, and cIPS: the caudal part of the IPS. CS:

the central sulcus.

Page 37: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

21

1.3.4 Functional Interactions between the Left Posterior Parietal

Cortex (PPC) and Ipsilateral Primary Motor Cortex (M1)

Examined by Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS)

Functional imaging studies have shown the involvement of fronto-parietal network

including the premotor and the posterior parietal cortices in goal-directed action

(Culham & Valyear, 2006). However, functional interactions between two different areas

and the time course of the interaction can be better probed by studies with paired-pulse

TMS paradigm with two coils, which detects inputs from other cortical areas to M1

(Koch & Rothwell, 2009; Rothwell, 2011). The paired-pulse TMS paradigm involves a

conditioning TMS pulse (conditioning stimulus, CS) to a cortical region of interest

followed by TMS to M1 (test stimulus, TS) with a few ms interval. The CS activates a

putative pathway from the site of interest to M1. The method can probe changes inM1

excitability induced by the CS. If the CS increases the amplitude of the MEP evoked by

test stimulus (TS) to M1, it is considered facilitatory, whereas a decrease is thought to

have an inhibitory influence over the M1 (Figure 4).

With this approach, interactions between PPC and ipsilateral M1 have been probed

(Karabanov, Christensen, Ritterband-Rosenbaum, Siebner, & Nielsen, 2014; Koch et

al., 2007; Koch, Ribolsi, et al., 2008; Vesia, Bolton, Mochizuki, & Staines, 2013).

Whether PPC has facilitatory or inhibitory influence over M1 depends on several factors

including the CS intensity, the ISIs between CS and TS and the specific areas targeted

within PPC (Karabanov et al., 2014; Koch et al., 2007; Koch, Ribolsi, et al., 2008; Vesia

et al., 2013; Ziluk et al., 2010).

SPL is essential to representing maintained postures and receives proprioceptive inputs

from primary and secondary somatosensory areas. A TMS study reported that SPL-M1

interactions were neither inhibitory or facilitatory at rest while SPL facilitated M1

excitability during application of vibratory tactile inputs from a digit (Ziluk et al., 2010).

Other TMS studies with paired-pulse TMS approach targeted IPL (Koch, Ribolsi, et al.,

2008). At rest, the CS facilitated M1 excitability to the caudal intra parietal sulcus (cIPS)

Page 38: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

22

at 90% of resting motor threshold. There were two phases of facilitation with peaks at 4

ms and 15 ms after cIPS stimulation. Stimulation of the aIPS at the same intensity

inhibited the M1 excitability. Stimulating mIPS did not influence M1 excitability. Several

TMS studies found that aIPS-M1 interaction was facilitated at rest at 4 and 6 ms ISIs

(Karabanov et al., 2014; Koch et al., 2007) as well as during motor planning at 4 ms ISI

(Vesia et al., 2013). The same study also demonstrated that the interaction between

SPOC and M1 at rest was different from during motor planning. At rest, SPOC-M1

interactions did not show either facilitation or inhibition. Interestingly, interaction

between SPOC and M1 is facilitated at 4 ms ISI during motor planning. Thus, PPC-M1

interactions are strongly influenced by the subdivisions of PPC targeted and the

cognitive state. Further research using the paired-pulse TMS approach is still required

to determine functional interactions between other PPC regions and M1 for other tasks.

Page 39: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

23

Figure 4 TMS studies with Paired-pulse approach with Two Coils

Figure 4. With paired-pulse TMS approach with two coils, condition stimulus (CS) is

applied to a brain region, with the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) illustrated in this figure,

followed by test stimulus (TS) to the primary motor cortex (M1). The ratio of the

conditioned (CS+TS) to the unconditioned (TS alone) less than 1 indicates inhibitory,

and more than 1 indicates facilitatory influences of PPC over M1.

Page 40: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

24

1.3.5 Rubber Hand Illusion as a Tool to Investigate Multisensory

Integration

1.3.5.1 Behavioral Studies of Rubber Hand Illusion

As mentioned in the previous section (1.3.2), multisensory integration is strongly

associated with perception of one’s own body parts. Goal-directed movements require

the identification of one’s own body parts, which involves informing the brain what parts

belong to the body (ownership of a body part) and where those parts are currently

located (localization of a body part) (Berlucchi & Aglioti, 2010) to construct the

representation of one’s own body in the brain. The representation of one’s own body

involves encoding and integrating a wide range of multisensory inputs, primarily visual,

tactile, and proprioceptive inputs (Aspell, Lenggenhager, & Blanke, 2012; Aspell,

Palluel, & Blanke, 2012). The update of mental representations of our body is necessary

for determining what motor commands are needed to achieve a desired movement by

calculating motor outputs from sensory inputs. It is also important for anticipating future

sensory inputs as the result of the motor command currently being held in the brain

(Franklin & Wolpert, 2011; T. Heed, Roder, Brigitte, 2012; Kandel et al., 2013; Wolpert,

Ghahramani, & Jordan, 1995). Because body postures constantly change with actions,

the brain needs to align tactile coordinates precisely to proprioceptive information, both

of which should also be matched with visual inputs. The spatial locations constituted of

the different sensory modalities are transformed into a common reference system

through multisensory integration to create a coherent and consistent body

representation (J. E. Aspell et al., 2012). In other words, creating a mental

representation of our own body requires multisensory integration (Rizzolatti et al., 2014;

Stone & Gonzalez, 2015).

The body representation in the brain formed by the process of multisensory integration

can be examined and modulated by an experimental procedure known as the “rubber

hand illusion (RHI).” Since its first report by Botvinick and Cohen (Botvinick & Cohen,

1998), the RHI has been widely used to evoke and assess visuo-tactile-proprioceptive

Page 41: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

25

multisensory integration at the behavioural level (Armel & Ramachandran, 2003;

Botvinick, Nystrom, Fissell, Carter, & Cohen, 1999; Ehrsson, Holmes, & Passingham,

2005; Tsakiris & Haggard, 2005). In RHI experiment, participants view an artificial

rubber hand being stroked with a brush while his or her real hand is visually occluded

and is simultaneously stroked with another brush (Figure 5). When the timing of the

brush strokes is synchronized, the participants perceives the touch of the brush stroking

the rubber hand but not of their own hidden hand, as if the rubber hand is a part of one’s

own body (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998). In addition to the ownership feeling of the rubber

hand, the participants also demonstrate shifts in the perceived position of their own

hand and arm towards the rubber hand, suggesting a shift of proprioception. The

ownership feeling of the rubber hand and the shift in the perceived hand position is

reduced when the two brushes asynchronously stroke the hands (Botvinick & Cohen,

1998; Makin, Holmes, & Ehrsson, 2008). To evoke RHI, there must be a congruency

among the different sensory modalities. Visual capture of body postures can be used to

calibrate the spatial relationship among the different modes of sensory inputs, and the

brain weighs the different sensory inputs depending on their reliabilities (J. E. Aspell et

al., 2012). The relative weighting function of RHI will be discussed in the next section.

Page 42: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

26

Figure 5 Rubber Hand Illusion

Figure 5 shows the rubber hand illusion (RHI) set-up. A participant sat at a table putting

his or her own hand on it. Medial to the own hand, an artificial rubber hand was placed

in a way that it appears to be coming out of his or her body. A partition was placed

between the subject’s own hand and the rubber hand, so that the own hand was

occluded from his or her vision. The two hands were simultaneously being stroked by

two separate paintbrushes while the participant was attending to the rubber hand.

Page 43: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

27

1.3.5.2 The Role of Cognitive Process during RHI

Although multisensory integration is essential to establish RHI, a cognitive component is

also required for this process. During the cognitive process of RHI, the hand

representation is matched with pre-stored information about the own corresponding

body part (internal representation). Some RHI experiments have shown that RHI does

not occur if the rubber hand is placed in anatomically incongruent position or substituted

by a non-corporeal object (Longo, Schüür, Kammers, Tsakiris, & Haggard, 2008;

Tsakiris, 2010). Therefore, the brain distinguishes what can and what cannot be

incorporated into one’s own body representations (Tsakiris & Haggard, 2005). If the

incoming visual inputs are sufficiently matched with the pre-existing body image, then

multisensory inputs will be matched for synchronicity and the integration occurs (Kilteni,

Maselli, Kording, & Slater, 2015; Tsakiris, 2010).

Cognitive processes also influence weighting of different sensory modalities during the

multisensory integration. Unlike the natural circumstance of self-body perception, the

RHI experiment creates conflicts between vision, tactile and proprioception. It is known

that when sensory inputs derived from different modalities are experimentally placed in

a conflict, for example by mismatching vision of the mouth and audition of a person

speaking, the percept is skewed to something intermediate between the percept of each

modality alone (McGurk & MacDonald, 1976).Similarly, during the RHI, the

proprioception is compromised to match with visual input, which is supported by

synchronized tactile inputs to resolve the conflict (Makin et al., 2008; Tsakiris &

Haggard, 2005). Due to this cognitive process, the gain of the incongruent sensory

inputs such as proprioception aredecreased and the gain of reliable inputs such as

vision are increased (Dempsey-Jones & Kritikos, 2014; Makin et al., 2008), resulting in

on increased relative weighting on visual inputs than somatosensory inputs during

multisensory integration. Thus, the RHI experiences arise from the interaction between

the cognitive process and the multisensory integration during the process of

constructing a coherent body representation in the brain (Makin et al., 2008).

Page 44: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

28

Some behavioral studies suggested that the modulation of sensory gains might result in

tactile perception changes as well as proprioceptive modulation. Somatosensory tactile

inputs from one’s own hand are suppressed after RHI( Folegatti, de Vignemont, Pavani,

Rossetti, & Farnè, 2009; G. L. Moseley et al., 2008; Zopf, Harris, & Williams, 2011).

Participants show slower responses to tactile stimuli to their own hand after the RHI

(Folegatti et al., 2009). However, the neurophysiological basis of this result has not

been investigated.

1.3.5.3 RHI influences on Motor Responses

Considering that multisensory integration is closely linked to sensorimotor integration, it

is possible that RHI influences motor responses. RHI induces multisensory integration

and modulatesmotor performance (T. Heed et al., 2011). Likewise, cortical excitability in

M1 decreases after the RHI (Della Gatta et al., 2016)and when participants experienced

ownership over a moving hand compared to when they did not feel ownership (Schutz-

Bosbach, Avenanti, Aglioti, & Haggard, 2009). However, RHI does not influence

kinematic parameters for a ballistic movement (Kammers, de Vignemont, Verhagen, &

Dijkerman, 2009; Kammers, van der Ham, & Dijkerman, 2006). Therefore, further

investigations and neurophysiological evidence supporting modulation of the motor

system by RHI are needed to resolve these discrepancies.

1.3.5.4 Application of RHI in Neuropsychiatric Disorders

RHI studies have revealed how multisensory integration is involved in self-body

perception in healthy individuals and how this process is impaired in neurological and

psychiatric disorders. RHI is abnormal in patients with focal hand dystonia,

schizophrenia and autism (Fiorio et al., 2011; Peled, Pressman, Geva, & Modai, 2003;

Thakkar, Nichols, McIntosh, & Park, 2011).Patients with schizophrenia show a greater

degree of the RHI, evidenced by a stronger rubber hand ownership and greater

Page 45: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

29

proprioceptive arm shift than HCs (Peled et al., 2003; Thakkar et al., 2011).Patients with

focal hand dystonia, a movement disorder characterized by sustained or intermittent

muscles contraction causing abnormal posture or repetitive movements, show reduced

arm shift toward the rubber hand (Fiorio et al., 2011). Given that sensorimotor

integration is altered in both focal hand dystonia (Avanzino & Fiorio, 2014) and

schizophrenia at the behavioural level (Shergill, Samson, Bays, Frith, & Wolpert, 2005;

Velasques et al., 2011), it is likely that RHI may interfere with sensorimotor integration.

By corollary, the RHI paradigm can be used to examine diseases where multisensory

integration and sensorimotor integration are disrupted.

1.3.5.5 Brain Regions Involving in Multisensory Integration for Own

Body and Rubber Hand Illusion

Lesions studies show that multiple brain regions are involved in multisensory integration

for self-body perception. Denial of the ownership of own limb using the RHI has been

reported in patients with lesions in the premotor area and the parietal lobe (Vallar &

Ronchi, 2009; Zeller, Gross, Bartsch, Johansen-Berg, & Classen, 2011). The perception

of limb movements likely depends on the primary sensory (S1), motor, and premotor

cortices. In contrast, the current body posture is likely represented differently from limb

movement perception. Proprioceptive inputs are processed initially in S1 and then S2.

Maintenance of a still posture is represented in posterior parietal areas, such as the

SPL (J. E. Aspell, B. Lenggenhager, & O. Blanke, 2012; Harris & Wolpert, 1998;

Pellijeff, Bonilha, Morgan, McKenzie, & Jackson, 2006).

It is still controversial whether a body representation in motion is different from a still

body representation (Gallagher, 1986; Kammers et al., 2006; Preston & Newport, 2011).

The body representation in motion is mostly based on proprioception and information

from a motor command while the still body representation is derived primarily from

visual inputs of own body and a body image constructed from stored memories

(Berlucchi & Aglioti, 2010; Schwoebel & Coslett, 2005). However, both types of mental

Page 46: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

30

body representations share multisensory integration networks (T. Heed, Roder, Brigitte,

2012; Rizzolatti, Fogassi, et al., 1997; Serino et al., 2013; Vesia & Crawford, 2012).

Therefore, it is likely that these processes interact with each other. Brain regions

activated during RHI are similar to regions involved in multisensory integration of the

self-body representation (Bremmer et al., 2001; Graziano & Gross, 1998),suggesting

multisensory integration during RHI. Evoked potential studies show RHI-related

potentials over frontocentral-parietal areas (J. E. Aspell et al., 2012; T. Heed, Roder,

Brigitte, 2012; Zeller, Litvak, Friston, & Classen, 2015),supporting the involvement of

both somatosensory and multisensory areas of the fronto-parietal cortices in RHI. Many

fMRI studies have consistently shown that RHI is associated with the activation of PPC

and premotor cortex. The strength of RHI is correlated with the activity in PMv (Ehrsson

et al., 2005; Ehrsson, Spence, & Passingham, 2004; Limanowski & Blankenburg, 2015).

RHI increases activity in the IPL, particularly in the medial wall of IPS (Ehrsson et al.,

2004). Because IPL sends information to PMv (Petrides & Pandya, 1984), the IPL is

likely involved in the correlation between PMv activity and the subjective RHI

experience. A lesion study in stroke showed that the white matter connecting the

parietal and premotor cortices was necessary to induce RHI (Giummarra et al., 2008).

In addition, perceptual shift in one’s own hand after RHI was attenuated by inhibiting the

IPL with repetitive TMS (Kammers, Verhagen, et al., 2009). However, the subjective

RHI experience including ownership feeling was not disrupted by the TMS of

IPL(Kammers, Verhagen, et al., 2009). Another study reported that the participants

experienced exaggerated ownership even for non-corporeal objects when the right

temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) was suppressed by TMS (Serino et al., 2013; Tsakiris,

Costantini, & Haggard, 2008). These results indicate that the parietal-premotor

multisensory integration areas are involved in the RHI.

1.3.5.6 Posterior Parietal Cortex and Rubber Hand Illusion

Given that evidence suggests that the processing of multisensory stimuli in peripersonal

space, sensorimotor integration and RHI takes place in the parietal-frontal cortical

Page 47: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

31

network, we will focus on PPC in this section. Peripersonal space is the immediate

space surrounding our body(Holmes & Spence, 2004).In this space, visual information

of external world is integrated with tactile information arising from own body, combining

the mental representations of own body with stimuli from the external world (Brozzoli et

al., 2012). Furthermore, the network involving PPC-premotor/motor areasis disrupted in

patients with focal hand dystonia (Delnooz, Helmich, Toni, & van de Warrenburg, 2012;

Pirio Richardson, Beck, Bliem, & Hallett, 2014; Vesia & Chen, 2014), supporting PPC-

premotor involvement in RHI deficits.

Human fMRI studies have identified IPS, PMv, and the superior parietal occipital

junction as a part of peripersonal space networks (Bremmer et al., 2001; Quinlan &

Culham, 2007). The network codes visual stimuli presented in a space near a body part

that receives tactile stimuli in a body-centered reference frame, allowing the integration

of the external visual information around the body and the tactile information of a

specific body part (Brozzoli et al., 2012). The bimodal neurons in the network

responding to visual and tactile stimuli are modulated by the distance between the

visual object and the tactile receptive field, with greater modulation in peripersonal

space than in far space (Brozzoli et al., 2012). The close relationship between motor

and sensory stimuli in peripersonal space implies that the peripersonal space also

serves as a mediator for interactions between the sensory and motor systems.

In summary, several brain areas within the premotor-parietal network are involved in

RHI. IPL is involved in proprioceptive shift but not in subjective RHI experience and

body ownership. Because IPL is more related to visuo-motor integration rather than

somatosensory-motor integration, other subdivisions in PPC such as SPL, which

predominantly receives somatosensory inputs, may also be relevant to the RHI

experience.

1.3.6 Multisensory Integration in PD patients

Page 48: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

32

1.3.6.1 Multisensory Integration and its Involvement in Sensorimotor

Integration in PD patients at the Behavioral Level

Multisensory integration and sensorimotor integration in PD patients have been tested

at the behavioural level in several studies using different tasks. Visuo-auditory

integration at the perceptual level was tested by a task in which facilitation of a motor

response is normally found in healthy individuals. PD patients with freezing of gait show

less of the visuo-auditory facilitation compared to age-matched HCs (Fearon et al.,

2015), suggesting a deficiency of multisensory integration in PD patients. Haptic

perception is another type of multisensory integration during which participants explore

an object shape with the hands without vision, integrating tactile perception and

proprioception. The haptic sensitivity is compromised in PD patients compared to similar

age-matched HCs (Konczak, Li, Tuite, & Poizner, 2008; Konczak et al., 2012; Li,

Pickett, Nestrasil, Tuite, & Konczak, 2010), suggesting impairment of tactile-

proprioceptive integration. When the exploring movement was performed passively, it

only involved tactile-proprioceptive multisensory integration without involving the motor

system. However, when it was performed actively, sensorimotor integration occurred

involving the motor system. Active haptic threshold increases in PD patients compared

to HCs, suggesting the impairment of multisensory integration and sensorimotor

integration (Konczak et al., 2008; Konczak et al., 2012).

It is not known whether the altered sensorimotor integration in PD patients was

attributed to the stage where multisensory inputs are integrated or a later stage of

sensorimotor integration. Elevated haptic thresholds in both active and passive

explorations implies that PD affects an early stage of multisensory integration that

eventually influences sensorimotor integration (Konczak et al., 2012). However, even in

the passive exploration paradigm, separation of multisensory integration from kinetic

feedback from moving arm is unavoidable, resulting in inability to distinguish a deficit in

the process of multisensory integration from a deficit in sensorimotor integration. In this

view, examining the extent of RHI may be useful to assess multisensory integration.

Multisensory integration for body representation and peripersonal space has not been

Page 49: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

33

investigated in PD patients. When compared to HCs, PD patients also have a tendency

to rely more on external sensory inputs such as vestibular or visual information than

internal representation of body during an orientation determination task (Barnett-Cowan

et al., 2010). This suggests abnormal interaction between the external sensory inputs

and the mental representations of own body. Erroneous mental rotation of hand (Amick,

Schendan, Ganis, & Cronin-Golomb, 2006; Dominey, Decety, Broussolle, Chazot, &

Jeannerod, 1995) and slow motor imagery (Helmich, de Lange, Bloem, & Toni, 2007)

reported in PD patients may indicate an altered process of multisensory integration for

body representation.

Determining how sensorimotor integration is modulated in PD patients will provide a

better understanding of the underlying mechanisms in multisensory integration.

1.3.6.2 Posterior Parietal Dysfunction in PD patients

As described above, PPC plays a crucial role in multisensory integration and

sensorimotor integration. The loss of dopaminergic projections from basal ganglia leads

to abnormal cortical activation patterns in M1 and other cortical areas including PPC -

probably due to aberrant control of the ascending thalamocortical system (Marsden &

Obeso, 1994). Therefore, PPC likely contributes to the altered multisensory and

sensorimotor integration leading to deficits at the behavioural level in PD patients.

The altered activation in PPC in PD has been reported in several neuroimaging studies.

Fluorodeoxy glucose (FDG) PET studies have repeatedly shown reduced metabolic

activities in parietal association cortex together with premotor and prefrontal cortex at

rest (Lozza et al., 2004; Ma, Tang, Spetsieris, Dhawan, & Eidelberg, 2007; Moeller et

al., 1999; Samuel et al., 1997). These studies utilized spatial covariant analysis and

revealed that the reduced activities in these areas were associated with PD-related

motor scores (Eidelberg, 2009; Ma et al., 2007). Resting state fMRI also reported

reduced activity in PPC and prefrontal cortex in PD patients with freezing of gait

(Tessitore et al., 2012). fMRI also shows that increased activity in superior parietal

Page 50: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

34

cortex at rest in PD patients (Wu et al., 2015).During movements, inferior parietal cortex

together with lateral premotor cortex is over activated in PD patients compared to HCs

(Samuel et al., 1997). With motor tasks, the activation in PPC is abnormally increased

in PD patients (Catalan, Ishii, Honda, Samii, & Hallett, 1999; Samuel et al., 1997; Tessa

et al., 2010). Abnormal activation in PPC is also observed during motor imagery

(Helmich et al., 2007).These imaging studies imply altered PPC activation in PD

patients leading to abnormal sensorimotor integration.

A decreased functional connectivity between PPC and pre-supplementary motor area

(pre-SMA), a motor related area, has been found at rest (Wu et al., 2011).Functional

connection between PPC and M1 can also be studied using paired-pulse TMS

paradigm with two coils. So far, only one TMS study investigated PPC-M1 interactions

at rest in PD patients. In the study, PPC was defined as IPL. It revealed alack of

facilitatory interaction between PPC and M1 in PD patients (Palomar et al., 2013). The

study supported the hypothesis that PPC may be involved in the altered sensorimotor

integration in PD patients, suggesting a possible key role in multisensory integration

deficits in PD patients.

However, PPC-M1 interactions in response to multisensory integration in PD patients

has not been investigated.

Page 51: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

35

2 RESEARCH AIMS and HYPOTHESIS

2.1 Objectives of the Present Thesis

2.1.1 Study One: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by Rubber

Hand Illusion in Healthy Subjects

The primary purpose of this study is to establish the validity of a paradigm to assess

how RHI-induced multisensory integration impacts on sensorimotor integration and the

interactions between PPC and M1. The RHI was used to evoke and assess visuo-

tactile-proprioceptive integration. In this study, healthy subjects were tested.

Behavioral studies have suggested that RHI altered somatosensory processing

(Folegatti et al., 2009; G. Lorimer Moseley et al., 2008) and interfered with motor

performance. However, the neurophysiological underpinnings for the modulation of

somatosensory processing by the RHI remain unclear. The first specific aim of the

Study One is to examine neurophysiology of RHI modulation of sensorimotor integration

by measuring the extent of SAI and LAI (i.e. the MEP amplitude ratio) during RHI with

TMS. The second specific purpose of the study is to investigate the modulation of the

interaction between PPC and M1 during RHI using the paired-pulse TMS approach with

two coils.

2.1.2 Study Two: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by Rubber

Hand Illusion in Parkinson’s Disease Patients

The primary purpose of Study Two is to assess abnormalities in multisensory integration

and its influence on sensorimotor integration in patients with PD by applying the

methods established in Study One. The results from PD patients were compared with

those from age-matched HCs (HC). In addition, the influence of dopaminergic

Page 52: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

36

medications was investigated by comparing the results in the on and off medication

states.

The firstly, whether PD patients have deficits in visuo-tactile-proprioceptive integration

induced by RHI was evaluated at the behavioral level. The secondly, the modulations of

SAI and LAI during RHI was measured. Thirdly, the influence of RHI on the PPC-M1

interaction was investigated.

2.2 Hypotheses of the Present Thesis

2.2.1 Study One: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by the

Rubber Hand Illusion in Healthy Subjects

We hypothesize that the extent of SAI and LAI at baseline will decrease during RHI due

to suppression of somatosensory processing. We also hypothesize that PPC-M1

interaction will be reduced during the RHI.

2.2.2 Study Two: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by the

Rubber Hand Illusion in Parkinson’s Disease Patients

We hypothesize that PD patients will experience less RHI (i.e. weaker subjective RHI

feeling and smaller proprioceptive arm shift) than age-matched HC at the behavioral

level. The modulation of SAI and LAI by RHI will be reduced in PD patients compared to

HC. The dopaminergic medications will normalize the modulation of SAI and LAI by

RHI. The PPC-M1 interaction in PD patients will be less influenced by the RHI than HC.

The modulation of the PPC-M1 interaction by the RHI will be normalized by the

dopaminergic medications.

Page 53: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

37

3 STUDY ONE: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by

Rubber Hand Illusion in Healthy Subjects

3.1 Abstract

The rubber hand illusion (RHI) paradigm experimentally manipulates multisensory

integration involving visual, tactile and proprioceptive inputs by simultaneously stroking

a rubber hand in view and a participant’s visually occluded hand, and produces an

illusion of rubber hand ownership and arm shift. The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is

an important area for multisensory integration. Although motor control requires

multisensory inputs to be transformed into motor outputs, whether the RHI modifies the

interactions between sensory and motor systems remains unclear. We examined the

functional connections from the primary sensory and association cortices, and from the

PPC to the primary motor cortex (M1) during the RHI. In Experiment 1, short-latency

afferent inhibition (SAI) and long-latency afferent inhibition (LAI) were measured using

transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) before and immediately after a synchronous

(RHI) or an asynchronous (control) condition. In Experiment 2, PPC-M1 interaction was

measured using paired-pulse TMS with two coils. We found that SAI and LAI were

reduced in the synchronous condition compared to baseline, suggesting that the RHI

decreased the functional connections from the primary sensory and the association

cortices to M1. We also found that greater inhibitory PPC-M1 interaction was associated

with stronger RHI assessed by questionnaire. Our findings suggest that the RHI

modulates sensorimotor integration at the early and late stages by reducing the gain of

tactile processing to resolve conflicts among multisensory inputs.

Keywords: Rubber hand illusion, transcranial magnetic stimulation, short- and long-

latency afferent inhibition, multisensory integration, posterior parietal cortex

Page 54: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

38

3.2 Introduction

The status of our body is perceived and mapped in broad brain networks.

Somatosensory information such as tactile, proprioceptive, and visual inputs regarding

one's own body parts first arrives at the primary sensory area and are then integrated in

the association areas including the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), the ventral premotor

cortex (PMv) and the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) (G. L. Moseley et al., 2008; Serino

et al., 2013). Because lesions of these areas can lead to decreased awareness of the

ownership or location of one's own body part (Giummarra, Gibson, Georgiou-Karistianis,

& Bradshaw, 2008), integration of multisensory inputs seems be essential for perceiving

our body ownership and location.

Multisensory integration for body perception can be experimentally manipulated by a

paradigm known as “rubber hand illusion (RHI)” (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998). A participant

observes a rubber hand in view being stroked with a paintbrush while his or her visually

occluded own real hand is being stroked simultaneously with another paintbrush. When

the touches of the two brushes are synchronized, the participant often experiences an

ownership feeling over the rubber hand and perceives a shift of own hand's position

towards the rubber hand. These illusory experiences are thought to occur as a result of

an integration of synchronized visual capture of the rubber hand being touched and

tactile inputs from the occluded own hand (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998; Makin et al., 2008).

Unlike natural circumstances, multisensory integration evoked by the RHI paradigm

creates a conflict between vision and tactile inputs. The conflict is thought to be resolved

by modulating the sensory inputs gain: increasing the gain of reliable visual inputs, and

decreasing the gain of incongruent somatosensory inputs (Dempsey-Jones & Kritikos,

2014; Makin et al., 2008). Reduction of somatosensory inputs from own hand, especially

tactile processing, has been suggested by behavioural studies (Folegatti et al., 2009; G.

L. Moseley et al., 2008; Zopf et al., 2011).

Skilled motor behaviours rely on the integration of motor commands with sensory

information from various modalities, a process termed sensorimotor integration.

Therefore, changing the gain of multisensory integration may influence the motor

Page 55: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

39

system. However, the influence of the RHI on the motor system has shown inconsistent

results in behavioural studies (T. Heed et al., 2011; Kammers, de Vignemont, et al.,

2009; Kammers, Verhagen, et al., 2009). Decreased motor performance with “affected”

own hand after the RHI was demonstrated in a study (T. Heed et al., 2011) while other

studies showed no impact of the RHI (Kammers, de Vignemont, et al., 2009; Kammers,

Verhagen, et al., 2009). Recent work shows a decrease in corticospinal excitability

associated with own hand disembodiment during the RHI (Della Gatta et al., 2016).

However, the neurophysiological basis of altered tactile somatosensory processing

during RHI and its influence on sensorimotor integration has not been investigated.

The PPC, a multimodal sensory integration area, receives abundant inputs from sensory

cortices and has strong projections to M1 and other motor areas (Petrides & Pandya,

1984). It is a critical node for transforming multimodal sensory inputs into motor

commands. In addition, a study showed a modulation of tactile sensitivity through the

PPC when there was a conflict between visual and tactile inputs (Ro, Wallace,

Hagedorn, Farnè, & Pienkos, 2004). Therefore, the PPC may play a role in modulations

of sensory gain during the RHI.

In the present study, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to assess how

the RHI modulates the influence of tactile somatosensory input on M1 excitability. We

examined short-latency afferent inhibition (SAI), which reflects direct impact of the

primary somatosensory cortex (S1) on M1, and long-latency afferent inhibition (LAI),

which likely represents the influence of secondary sensory and association areas

including multisensory areas such as the PPC or the premotor cortex on M1 (Sailer et

al., 2003). We also investigated functional connectivity from PPC to M1 immediately

after the RHI. We hypothesized that SAI and LAI would be reduced immediately after

the RHI due to reduced somatosensory influences from gating mechanisms and that

PPC-M1 interaction would be weakened as a result of reduced reliance on

somatosensory inputs after the RHI.

3.3 Methods

Page 56: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

40

3.3.1 Subjects

Thirty-one healthy volunteers (15 women, age43.3 ±18.9 years, mean ± SD; range,21-

77 years) participated in the study. All but one were assessed as right handed by the

Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971). All participants gave written informed

consent. University Health Network Research Ethics Board approved the protocol. We

tested twenty-seven subjects (12 women; age 44.7±18.9 years) in Experiment 1 and

twenty-six subjects (13 women; age 45.2±19.0 years; 22 subjects from Experiment 1) in

Experiment 2. All the participants gave written informed consents and the protocol was

approved by University Health Network Research Ethics Board.

3.3.2 Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI) and behavioural assessments

We used the RHI task originally reported by Botvinick and Cohen (Botvinick & Cohen,

1998)in the current study. Our experimental set-up is illustrated in Figure 6A. A life-sized

rubber right hand was presented in an anatomically congruent way on a table, and the

participant’s own real right hand was placed lateral to the rubber hand (15cm from the

real hand). Prior to the induction of the RHI, a movable black board covered both hands

with a measurement tape. Participants estimated the position of their right hidden index

finger by naming the corresponding number on the measurement tape (Proprioceptive

Judgment: PJ). The judgment was repeated six times with the onset and offset numbers

on the tape being changed for each judgment to avoid biases. The distance between

the actual and the estimated finger positions was averaged and used as estimated hand

position at baseline. Then, the movable black board was replaced by a black partition

set between the rubber hand and the participants’ hand to occlude only the own hand’s

view for the RHI induction. The dorsal surface of index fingers of both the rubber and

the own hands were touched by an experimenter using two paintbrushes with brisk

strokes at approximately 1Hz for three minutes (about 180 brush strokes in total) while

participants attended to the rubber hand. We tested two conditions, synchronous and

asynchronous conditions. In synchronous condition, the two brushes were moved

synchronously in terms of timing while in asynchronous condition, the timing between

the two brushes was delayed with approximately 500 ms . The PJ was repeated

Page 57: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

41

immediately after the RHI induction. At the end of each condition, a questionnaire

consisted of 9 statements (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998) was administered to quantify the

subjective feeling of embodiment of the rubber hand. The first three statements (Q1-3)

concern the RHI experiences (#1. I felt as if the rubber hand were my own hand, #2. It

seemed as if I were feeling the touch of the paintbrush in the location where I saw the

rubber hand touched, #3. The touching of the rubber hand felt just like an actual touch)

and the other 6 statements were controls (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998). Participants rated

their agreement to each statement by marking on a 14 cm-long visual-analog scale

(0=completely disagree, 14=completely agree).

3.3.3 Digital nerve stimulation (DNS)

Electrical stimulation was applied to the palm side of the right index finger over the distal

and middle phalanx through small pad electrodes (size, 28mm x 20 mm) with cathode

positioned proximally using a DS7A constant-current stimulator (pulse width 0.2ms;

Digitimer, Welwyn Garden City, UK). Sensory threshold (ST) was determined as the

lowest intensity felt in three consecutive trials by the participants and the stimulus

intensity was set at 3ST for SAI and LAI (Chen, Corwell, & Hallett, 1999; Sailer et al.,

2003).

3.3.4 Surface electromyography (EMG) recording

Surface electromyography (EMG) was recorded from the relaxed right first dorsal

interosseous (FDI) muscle using Ag-AgCl electrodes with the active electrode on the

muscle belly and the reference electrode over the right metacarpophalangeal joint. The

EMG signals were amplified (1000 X), filtered (band pass 20 Hz-2.5kHz; Intronix

Technologies Corporation Model 2024F, Bolton, Ontario, Canada), digitized at 5kHz by

an analog-to-digital (A/D) interface (Micro 1401, Cambridge Electronics Design,

Page 58: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

42

Cambridge, UK) and stored in a laboratory computer for off-line analysis using SIGNAL

software (Cambridge Electronic Devices, Cambridge, UK).

3.3.5 Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was performed using a Magstim2002

stimulator (Magstim, Whitland, UK) and a custom figure-of-eight coil (diameter, 50 mm).

The area for eliciting the best motor response in the right FDI muscle was established

over the left M1 with posterior to anterior induced current direction, and the coil position

was marked on the scalp as the motor hot spot. The test stimulus (TS) intensity was

adjusted to evoke ~ 1 mV peak-to-peak motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitude from

the relaxed right FDI muscle. Peak-to-peak MEP amplitude for each trial was measured

and averaged for each stimulation conditions.

3.3.5.1 Experiment 1: Influence of the RHI on SAI and LAI

In Experiment 1, we measured SAI and LAI reflecting sensorimotor integration. The

maximum inhibitory effects of MNS at the wrist on M1 occur with inter-stimulus intervals

(ISIs) at around 20 ms (Tokimura et al., 2000) and 200 ms (Chen et al., 1999). To

account for the peripheral sensory conduction time from the digit to the wrist, 3 ms was

added for DNS (Chen et al., 1999; Sailer et al., 2003). Each run consisted of 3 different

conditions: test stimulus (TS) alone, DNS followed by TS at ISI of 23 ms for SAI (DNS23-

TS), and at ISI of 203 ms for LAI (DNS203-TS). These conditions were delivered in a

random order and repeated 11 times 5s apart.

3.3.5.2 Experiment 2: The influence of the RHI on the interaction

between the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the primary

motor cortex (M1)

Page 59: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

43

The left PPC-M1 interaction was assessed with a paired-pulse TMS paradigm with two

coils (Karabanov et al., 2014; Koch et al., 2007; Rothwell, 2011; Vesia et al., 2013). TS

intensity was adjusted to evoke ~ 1 mV MEP amplitudes. Each run consisted of 5

conditions with different ISIs (TS alone; CS followed by TS, CS-TS at 4, 6, 8, or 10 ms

of ISI) delivered in random order and repeated 9 times each. Inter-trial interval was 5s.

To identify the left PPC, we acquired T1-weightedmagnetic resonance images (MRI)

from 19 participants using a 3.0 T MR scanner (General Electric; 500µ in-plane

resolution; gradient-echo spiral EPI sequences) before the study visit. A frameless

stereotaxic neuronavigation system (Brainsight2 Rogue Research, Canada) was used.

We localized the left medial and anterior part of superior parietal lobe (SPL), a

subregion within the PPC, according to individually identified anatomical landmarks.

SPL was defined by a region medial to the intraparietal sulcus, posterior to the post

central sulcus, and anterior to the parieto-occipital sulcus (Figure 7). The mean

Talairach coordinates were x=19.9±5.5, y=-73.2±11.1, z=61.7±10.4.The mean

Euclidean distance between the two coils was 6.9 ± 1.5cm.For seven participants who

did not undergo the MRI due to various reasons (e.g. metals in the body, limited time

commitment or personal preference), CP1 in the International 10-20

electroencephalogram (EEG) electrode system was used to locate the left SPL(Koessler

et al., 2009). The mean Euclidean distances between the two coils were comparable

with 6.9 ± 1.8cm for the seven subjects and 6.7 ± 1.3cm for the other nineteen subjects.

A conditioning stimulus (CS) was delivered with a custom 40mm figure-of-eight coil

placed over the left PPC in a posterior-anterior current (Ziluk et al., 2010). The TS coil

was placed over the left M1 at the optimal position for the right FDI muscle. Figure 1D

illustrates the coil positions. The CS intensity was set at 90% of resting motor threshold

(RMT), which was determined from the motor hot spot as the lowest intensity that

evoked more than 50µV peak-to-peak amplitude MEPs in 5 out of 10 consecutive

trials(Rossini et al., 2015).

Page 60: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

44

3.3.6 Experimental Designs

We tested the influence of the RHI on SAI and LAI in Experiment 1, and the effects of

the RHI on PPC-M1 interaction in Experiment 2 on separate days. The study design is

illustrated in Figure 6B. Each experiment started with baseline TMS measurement

followed by proprioceptive judgment. After the RHI induction either with the

synchronous or asynchronous condition, the proprioceptive judgment and the TMS

measurements were repeated. The participants kept their right hand still until the

completion of the TMS measurements. After the questionnaire, the participants were

given a five-minute break, during which they moved the right hand to cancel the RHI

effect. Then, the other condition was tested with the same procedure. The order of the

synchronous and asynchronous conditions was randomized and counterbalanced

across subjects.

Page 61: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

45

Figure 6 Experimental Set-up and Design

Figure 6

A) Experimental set-up for rubber hand illusion (RHI) paradigm. The participants

were seated with their right arm rested at a table and their left arm rested on their

laps. A rubber right hand was placed in an anatomically congruent way on the table,

7 cm to the right of the midline of the participant. The participant’s actual right hand

was positioned 15cm lateral to the rubber hand. A black cloth was used to cover

the participants’ right shoulder, elbow and arm as well as the proximal parts of the

rubber hand. An experimenter simultaneously touched the participant’s hand and

the rubber hand with two paintbrushes with synchronous (RHI condition) or

asynchronous (control condition) strokes while the participant attended to the

rubber hand for three minutes.

B) Study design. TMS Baseline: The experiment started with baseline

measurements of TMS parameters, which were SAI and LAI in Experiment 1 and

Page 62: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

46

the PPC-M1 interaction in Experiment 2. Proprioceptive judgement (PJ) baseline:

PJ at baseline was assessed immediately before the induction of the RHI. RHI

sync: induction of RHI using synchronous brush strokes. RHI async: induction

period using asynchronous brush strokes. PJ post: PJ was assessed immediately

after the RHI induction in each condition. TMS post sync/async: the TMS

measurements were repeated immediately after the PJ post with the participant’s

hand stayed still. Questionnaire (Q): after completing the TMS post measurement

in each condition, the participants filled out the questionnaire. The order of the

synchronous and asynchronous conditions was randomized.

Page 63: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

47

Figure 7 Stimulation of PPC with TMS

Figure 7. Left: Location of the left PPC target in a representative participant.The

left SPL (superior parietal lobe) was identified in 3D reconstruction of the brain as the

medial and anterior part of the superior parietal lobe, medial to the intraparietal sulcus,

anterior to the parieto-occipital sulcus, and posterior to post-central gyrus. (Mean

Talairach coordinates ± SD:x -19.9±5.5, y 73.2±11.1, z 61.7±10.4). Right: Schematic

view of the paired-pulse TMS with two coils to examine the PPC (posterior parietal

cortex) -M1 (primary motor cortex) interaction. In order to probe the influence of

PPC on M1, conditioning stimuli (CS: 90% RMT) were applied to the PPC before test

stimulus (TS) were delivered to M1.

Page 64: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

48

3.4 Data Analysis

To avoid learning experience of the RHI at the behavioural level, the behavioural data

from the first visit was used in 22 participants who participated in both Experiments 1

and 2, except for a correlation analysis in Experiment 2. In Experiment 2, the RHI

scores obtained from the same visit was used to examine the relationship between the

behavioural result and the neurophysiological change (see correlation analysis below).

All the analyses were conducted with SPSS 17.0 and significance was set at P < 0.05

unless otherwise stated.

The questionnaire rating scores for the first three statements (Q1-3) concerning the RHI

experiences and the other six control statements were averaged separately for the

synchronous or asynchronous condition. Based on well-established results of the RHI

questionnaire in previous studies (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998; Longo, Schüür, Kammers,

Tsakiris, & Haggard, 2008; Tsakiris & Haggard, 2005), we predicted that the average

scores for Q1-3 would be larger than the control statements in the synchronous

condition. To compare the scores for Q1-3 with the control statements in the

synchronous condition, a paired one-tail t-test was used. Similarly, we also predicted

that the scores for Q1-3 in the synchronous condition would be larger than the scores

for the asynchronous condition. We used a separate paired one-tail t-test to compare

the average scores for Q1-3 in the synchronous and the asynchronous conditions.

The estimated hand positions in proprioceptive judgment were analyzed in the similar

way. As we predicted that the hand position in the synchronous condition would be

significantly larger than the asynchronous condition based on previously established

results (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998; Longo et al., 2008; Tsakiris & Haggard, 2005), we

used a paired one-tail t-test to compare the hand position in the synchronous with the

one in the asynchronous condition. Baseline hand position in these two conditions was

separately compared using another paired one-tail t-test to confirm no significant

differences at baseline.

Page 65: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

49

In Experiment 1, SAI and LAI were expressed as a ratio of the conditioned (DNS23-TS,

DNS203-TS) to the unconditioned (TS alone) MEP amplitudes. We defined that there is

no SAI or LAI at baseline if the MEP ratio exceeds 1.0 (Sailer et al., 2003). Participants

without SAI or LAI at baseline were excluded from the analysis because our aim was to

investigate the effects of the RHI on SAI and LAI. Both SAI and LAI were normally

distributed as determined by Kolmogorov-Smirov normality tests. The effects of the RHI

on SAI or LAI were assessed using repeated measures ANOVA with the factor

CONDITION (baseline, synchronous, asynchronous) as the repeated measure.

In Experiment 2, the MEP amplitudes at each ISI were expressed as a ratio of the

conditioned (CS-TS at ISI 4, 6, 8, and 10 ms of ISIs) to the unconditioned (TS alone)

MEP amplitudes. Ratios <1 indicate inhibitory and ratios>1 indicate facilitatory

influences of CS to M1. The effect of different ISIs on PPC-M1 interaction at baseline

was first compared using one-way repeated measures ANOVA with ISIs (4, 6, 8, and 10

ms) as the repeated measures. Then, the influence of the RHI on PPC-M1 interaction

was analyzed using two-way repeated measures ANOVA with CONDITION (baseline,

synchronous, asynchronous) and ISIs (4, 6, 8, and 10 ms) as repeated measures.

Significant effects were further analyzed by paired-t test with Bonferroni correction for

multiple comparisons. In addition, correlation between the strength of the RHI and

modulation of the PPC-M1 interaction was analyzed. The MEP ratios were averaged

across different ISIs in each individual because there were no significant differences

between ISIs. The correlation between the average Q1-3 scores in Experiment 2, and

the average MEP amplitude was analyzed using Pearson correlation for the

synchronous or asynchronous condition separately.

3.5 Results

3.5.1 Questionnaire on the Rubber Hand Illusion Experiences

Page 66: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

50

The scores of agreements to statements 1-3 (Q1-3: the RHI related experiences) and

the six control statements are shown in Figure 7A. We found higher agreement for Q1-3

in the synchronous condition compared to the asynchronous condition [paired t-test,

t(30)=8.87, p<0.001]. We also confirmed that the Q1-3 was scored higher than the

control statement in the synchronous condition [paired t-test, t(30)=9.55, p<0.001].

3.5.2 Proprioceptive judgment (PJ)

The estimated index finger positions at baseline and post RHI induction are shown in

Figure 7B. Participants perceived their hand drifted more towards the position of the

rubber hand after the synchronous compared to the asynchronous stimulation

[t(30)=1.81, p=0.04]. At baseline, no significant difference in their perceived hand

positions was detected between the synchronous and the asynchronous conditions

[t(30)=0.80, p=0.22].

Page 67: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

51

Figure 8 Behavioural Results

Figure 8A) Questionnaire results. The mean rating scores for the RHI experience

statements (Q1-3) and for the control statements (Control) in the synchronous (RHI) and

the asynchronous (control) conditions (maximum score = 14). The score for Q1-3 in the

synchronous condition was significantly higher than that in the asynchronous condition

(p<0.001, n=31). Q1-3 was scored significantly higher than the control statements. Error

bars denote the standard error of the mean (SEM). B) Proprioceptive judgments

before (baseline) and immediately after (post) the brush strokes in the

Page 68: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

52

synchronous and the asynchronous conditions. The Y-axis denotes the distance

from the position of own real hand toward the rubber hand with the own hand position at

0 cm. Participants perceived their hand shifted more towards the rubber hand in the

synchronous stimulation than the asynchronous stimulation[t(30)=1.81, p=0.04]. Error

bars indicate the standard error of the mean (SEM).

Page 69: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

53

3.5.3 Experiment 1: Modulation of Short- and Long-latency Afferent

Inhibition by RHI

Five participants did not show SAI at baseline (MEP ratio >1.0) and were excluded from

the analysis for SAI. Therefore, 22 participants (age 43.5±19.2 years) were included in

the analysis. The findings are shown in Figure 8A. SAI in the synchronous condition was

significantly reduced compared to the baseline (p=0.042). Repeated measures ANOVA

revealed a significant main effect of CONDITION [F(2,20)=4.31, p=0.028]. Post-hoc

comparison showed that SAI in the asynchronous condition was not significantly

different from that of the baseline or the synchronous condition.

Figure 8B shows the result for LAI. Four participants who did not show LAI at baseline

(MEP ratio >1.0) were excluded from the analysis. Therefore, 23participants (age

41.2±18.1 years) were included in the analysis. We found a significant reduction in LAI

after the synchronous condition compared to the baseline (p=0.047). Repeated

measures ANOVA showed a significant effect of CONDITION [F(2,21)=5.31, p=0.014]

for LAI. Post hoc analysis revealed that LAI after the asynchronous condition did not

differ significantly from the baseline or the synchronous condition.

Page 70: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

54

Figure 9Modulations of Short- and Long-Latency Afferent Inhibitions by RHI

A) The effect of RHI on Short-latency afferent inhibition (SAI). SAI at baseline(in

black), and after the synchronous (RHI, in light grey) and asynchronous (control,

in darker grey) conditions were shown in three bar graphs. Y-axis denotes the MEP

amplitudes, which are expressed as ratios to the mean MEP of the unconditioned

(TS alone) trials. Values below 1 indicate inhibition. SAI at baseline was

significantly reduced in the synchronous condition (p=0.042) but not in the

asynchronous condition (p=0.23) compared to the baseline. Error bars denote the

standard errors of the mean (SEM).

B) The effect of RHI on Long-latency afferent inhibition (LAI). LAI at baseline,

Page 71: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

55

synchronous (RHI), and asynchronous (control) conditions. LAI at baseline was

reduced in the synchronous condition compared to the baseline (p=0.049). The

LAI was not modified in the asynchronous condition (p=0.19). Error bars denote

SEM.

Page 72: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

56

3.5.4 Experiment 2: Modulation of the PPC-M1 interaction by the RHI

Figure 9A shows the result for the PPC-M1 interaction(n=26). At baseline, one-way

repeated measures ANOVA showed no effect of ISI, confirming the results of a previous

study (Ziluk et al., 2010). Modulation of the PPC-M1 interaction by the RHI was

assessed by two-way repeated measures ANOVA, which revealed no overall main

effect of CONDITION [F(2,24)=0.67, p=0.52 ], ISI [F(3,23)=1.17, p=0.34] or interaction

between CONDITION and ISI [F(6, 20)=1.13, p=0.38].

We investigated whether the PPC-M1 interaction after the RHI induction correlated with

the strength of the RHI. Figure 9B illustrates the relationship between the average

score for Q1-3 in the rubber hand ownership questionnaire and the averaged PPC-M1

interaction across different ISIs in the synchronous condition. Pearson correlation

revealed that stronger rubber hand ownership was correlated with greater inhibitory

PPC-M1 interaction in the synchronous condition (r2=0.212; p=0.018). The strength of

the rubber hand ownership did not correlate with the PPC-M1 interaction in the

asynchronous condition (Figure 9C).

Page 73: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

57

Figure 10 RHI and PPC-M1 Interactions

A) The functional connection from PPC to M1 at baseline, after synchronous

(RHI) or asynchronous (control) brush strokes. Group-averaged ±standard

error of the mean (SEM) (n=26) with MEP amplitudes expressed as ratios to the

Page 74: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

58

unconditioned MEP for each ISI are shown. There were no significant differences

across the three conditions.

B) and C) Relationship between the RHI strength and the PPC-M1

interaction in the synchronous (B) and asynchronous (C) conditions. The

RHI strength assessed by questionnaire (Q1-3) significantly correlated with the

greater inhibitory PPC-M1 interaction only in synchronous condition (r2=0.212;

p=0.018) but not in asynchronous condition (r2=0.002; p=0.812).

Page 75: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

59

3.6 Discussion

We found that the RHI reduced SAI and LAI, suggesting the involvement of S1 and the

sensory association areas, most likely the multisensory integration areas in the RHI

induction. We also displayed that the stronger ownership of the rubber hand was

associated with greater inhibitory influence of PPC over M1. These findings show that

modulating the sense of ownership over one’s hand is capable of altering both the early

and later processing of tactile afferent, as well as their integration at the level of their

motor outputs

3.6.1 Behavioural results confirmed the synchronous condition

induces RHI

In agreement with previous work (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998; Ehrsson, Spence, &

Passingham, 2004; Longo et al., 2008; Tsakiris & Haggard, 2005), our RHI behavioural

results confirmed that the synchronous but not the asynchronous condition induces

specific changes. The three statements on the RHI questionnaire (Q1-3) assess

cognitive changes in body ownership and body awareness regarding the RHI (Botvinick

& Cohen, 1998; Ehrsson et al., 2004; Longo et al., 2008; Tsakiris & Haggard, 2005). It

has been suggested that the ownership feeling is induced as a result of visuo-tactile-

proprioceptive multisensory integration (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998; Makin et al., 2008;

Tsakiris & Haggard, 2005). In turn, the established ownership feeling influences the

multisensory integration by gating sensory inputs coming from own hand, specifically

reducing the weight of incongruent inputs (Tsakiris, 2010).

3.6.2 The RHI Reduces Short-latency Sensorimotor Integration

SAI measures short-latency sensorimotor integration and likely reflects a direct

projection from S1 to M1 (Chen et al., 1999; Tokimura et al., 2000). We found that SAI

Page 76: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

60

was reduced in the presence of the RHI. The reduction of SAI in the synchronous

condition may thus be taken to indicate the reduced connection between S1 and M1

during the RHI. Interestingly, increased SAI was reported while observing brush stroking

of another person’s hand (Wood, Gallese, & Cattaneo, 2010). However, the gating

mechanism could occur at S1, thalamus or the spinal cord. Reduced SAI with the RHI

may also reflect a gain change in the tactile inputs into S1, resulting in reduced

processing of the tactile signals. Indeed, S1 involvement during the RHI has been

suggested in other neurophysiological studies (Aspell, Palluel, & Blanke, 2012; Shokur

et al., 2013). Body swap illusion which involves an illusory ownership and perceived

shift of a whole body instead of a hand corresponding to the RHI, lead to an enhanced

early component (P40) of tibial nerve somatosensory evoked potentials (SEP) which

presumably correspond to N20 of median nerve SEP (Aspell et al., 2012). The reduced

tactile somatosensory processing in S1 during the RHI may explain delayed tactile

perception after the RHI (Folegatti et al., 2009).

Noteworthy, baseline SAI level in our study was lower compared to previous studies

which used MNS at the wrist (Chen et al., 1999; Sailer et al., 2003; Tokimura et al.,

2000). The lower SAI level at baseline can be explained by the fact that we used DNS to

avoid contraction of thenar muscles which may disrupt the RHI. However, most of our

participants demonstrated the MEP ratio < 1.0 at baseline, which suggests an inhibitory

influence of DNS on M1 at short-latency in line with previous studies using DNS (Chen

et al., 1999; Sailer et al., 2003).

3.6.3 The RHI Reduces Long-latency Sensorimotor Integration

Reduction of LAI with

We found LAI at baseline in most participants, in line with previous literature

We found that the RHI also reduced LAI and think that the reduction of LAI may reflect

multisensory integration. Although anatomical correlates of LAI are not entirely

understood, LAI has been suggested to involve association areas such as the

Page 77: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

61

secondary sensory area, the PPC, the premotor cortex, and the basal ganglia (Chen et

al., 1999; Sailer et al., 2003), linking sensory systems with motor systems. Some of

these areas, the PPC and the premotor cortex, in particular, have shown to respond to

multisensory inputs (Andersen, 1997; Rizzolatti, Fogassi, & Gallese, 1997) serving as

multisensory integration areas. Several functional imaging studies have reported the

involvement of multisensory processing areas including the PPC, the premotor cortex,

the intra-parietal sulcus, the lateral occipito-temporal cortex, and the putamen in the RHI

(Ehrsson, Holmes, & Passingham, 2005; Ehrsson et al., 2004; Limanowski &

Blankenburg, 2015a, 2015b). In addition, the RHI led to attenuation of SEPs in the

frontal and parietal areas at latencies of ~50 ms, (Zeller, Litvak, Friston, & Classen,

2015) and enhancement of SEPs in the sensorimotor areas at longer latencies (140 -

460 ms) (Aspell et al., 2012; Peled, Pressman, Geva, & Modai, 2003; Press, Heyes,

Haggard, & Eimer, 2008). These studies indicate that multisensory integration cortices

in the fronto-parietal association areas are involved in the RHI. These results may not

be directly comparable to ours because most of them assessed the RHI changes during

the RHI induction with hand stroking while our assessments were performed

immediately after the RHI induction. However, we conducted TMS assessments while

the RHI was still present. Therefore, the reduced LAI reported here likely reflects the

modulation of the multisensory integration cortices by the RHI. It may also imply

increased errors in the motor performance of the affected hand after the RHI (Heed et

al., 2011) because motor planning requires proper multisensory integration in the PPC.

Of note is that baseline LAI in our study was lower than several previous studies that

used MNS, for the similar reason with SAI at baseline although most participants in our

study showed LAI at baseline in line with previous literature (Chen et al., 1999; Sailer et

al., 2003).

Reduced LAI with the RHI may be explained by reduced tactile gain in the multisensory

association cortices, similar to SAI reduction. The reduced gain of somatosensory

processing during the RHI could be due to a conflict between visual, tactile and

proprioceptive inputs. Ownership of a body part is thought to arise from the cognitive

processes of judging congruencies of the body part in view with body images pre-stored

Page 78: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

62

in the brain and built upon multisensory integration (Tsakiris, 2010). During the RHI

induction, the tactile perception of brush strokes in one’s hand is temporally congruent

with the visual capture of the rubber hand being touched, but is not spatially congruent

with the proprioceptive information from one’s own hand. The congruent visual and

tactile inputs are integrated and evoke an illusory ownership of the rubber hand, which

then reinforces the dominance of the visual information over the conflicting

proprioceptive signal and modifies the pre-stored body image (Makin et al., 2008; Ro et

al., 2004). As a result, the gain for proprioceptive and tactile inputs may be reduced in

S1 and association cortices for multisensory integration.

3.6.4 Inhibitory PPC-M1 interaction is associated with the strength of

RHI

We displayed that greater inhibitory PPC-M1 interaction was associated with the

strength of the RHI. Because the correlation between the greater inhibitory PPC-M1

interaction with the strength of the RHI was found only in the synchronous but not in the

asynchronous condition, it likely represents a RHI-specific effect. Functional MRI studies

have shown activation of the PPC during RHI (Ehrsson et al., 2005; Ehrsson et al.,

2004; Gentile, Petkova, & Ehrsson, 2011). However, the PPC-M1 functional connection

has not been examined during the RHI. A recent study demonstrated that M1 excitability

was decreased during the RHI (Della Gatta et al., 2016; Miller & Farnè, 2016). Our

finding supports this result; that is, the inhibitory influence from PPC to M1 may

contribute to the decreased corticospinal excitability during the RHI.

The PPC is a multisensory integration area with abundant projections to the motor areas

(Koch et al., 2007; Petrides & Pandya, 1984), and is involved in motor planning (Koch et

al., 2008; Vesia & Crawford, 2012). The SPL within the PPC was chosen as the target.

The SPL codes the location of the arm relative to other body parts (Andersen, 1997;

Gentile et al., 2011; Lewis et al., 2011) and transforms multisensory inputs into a

common coordinate system by visual, auditory and proprioceptive integration

Page 79: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

63

(Andersen, 1995; Lewis et al., 2011). This area is known to send intra-hemispheric

inputs to the arm and hand representations in the M1 in human (Andersen, 1995;

Andersen & Buneo, 2002; Premji, Rai, & Nelson, 2011; Ziluk et al., 2010). The SPL-M1

interaction was reported to be facilitatory at 6 ms ISI during vibratory tactile stimulation

to the index finger while there was no facilitation or inhibition without stimulation (Ziluk et

al., 2010).

In our study, the stronger the RHI was experienced, the more greatly reduced the

somatosensory tactile processing was, leading to less facilitatory or even inhibitory

PPC-M1 interactions. The PPC may play a role in reducing the somatosensory tactile

gain during the RHI. A study reported a modulation of somatosensory tactile sensitivity

under another type of visuo-somatosensory conflict. Transient suppression of the PPC

by repetitive TMS showed a lack of the tactile sensitivity modulation, suggesting that the

somatosensory gain may be altered through the PPC (Ro et al., 2004). The PPC also

may be involved in resolving the visuo-somatosensory conflict evoked by RHI. Future

studies may target other multisensory integration areas, such as the temporo-parietal

junction and the ventral premotor areas during the RHI to describe their involvement in

the maintenance of a stable sense of body ownership.

3.6.5 Clinical Implications of the findings

Abnormal RHI has been observed in some neurological and psychiatric disorders such

as dystonia, schizophrenia, and autism (Fiorio et al., 2011; Paton, Hohwy, & Enticott,

2012; Peled et al., 2003). Focal hand dystonia, a disorder characterized by sustained or

intermittent muscles contraction causing abnormal posture or repetitive movements,

showed disrupted proprioceptive arm shift in the RHI indicating a failure in visuo-tactile-

proprioceptive integration (Avanzino & Fiorio, 2014; Fiorio et al., 2011). Our findings

support the hypothesis that dystonia is associated with abnormalities in multisensory

integration areas (Delnooz, Helmich, Toni, & van de Warrenburg, 2012; Pirio

Page 80: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

64

Richardson, Beck, Bliem, & Hallett, 2014; Vesia & Chen, 2014) and sensorimotor

integration (Abbruzzese, Marchese, Buccolieri, Gasparetto, & Trompetto, 2001;

Avanzino, Tinazzi, Ionta, & Fiorio, 2015).

3.7 Conclusions

The RHI modulates sensorimotor integration by reducing the inhibitory influence of

somatosensory input to M1. Thus, the effects of the RHI are not limited to the sensory

areas but also encompass motor areas. The association between the greater inhibitory

PPC-M1 interaction and the strength of the RHI suggests that the RHI involves

modulation of the PPC-M1 interaction. Our findings imply that the RHI modifies

sensorimotor integration pathway at multiple levels.

3.8 Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR)and

Dystonia Medical Research Foundation (DMRF) of Canada, and Parkinson Canada.

Reina Isayama is supported by a CIHR/DMRF award and Parkinson Canada clinical

research award. Alessandro Farnè is supported by James S McDonnel Foundation

Scholar Award. We thank Dr. Matthias Niemeier and Dr. Tarek Rajji for their helpful

comments.

Page 81: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

65

4 STUDY TWO: Modulation of Sensorimotor Integration by

Rubber Hand Illusion in Parkinson’s Disease

4.1 Abstract

Although motor features are considered the classical symptoms of Parkinson’s disease

(PD), there are abnormalities in the sensory system. Sensorimotor integration, the

neural process of transforming sensory inputs to motor outputs, is known to be altered

in PD. However, the mechanism of the altered sensorimotor integration remains

unclear. As sensorimotor integration is a part of a process of combining sensory inputs

from different modalities known as multisensory integration, PD patients may have

abnormality in multisensory processing affecting sensorimotor integration.

In the current study, we used RHI paradigm to evoke and assess visuo-tactile-

proprioceptive integration. We examined and compared the level of RHI in PD patients

and age-matched HCs. We also measured SAI, LAI, and functional connectivity

between the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the primary motor cortex (M1) before

and immediately after the RHI in each group. We found that PD patients experienced

the RHI to the similar extent with the age-matched HCs, suggesting normal visuo-tactile-

proprioceptive integration in PD patients regardless of dopaminergic medication states.

Our result also revealed that, in PD-ON state, LAI during RHI was abnormally increased

by dopaminergic medications while it was decreased in HCs. The negative correlation

between the level of RHI and the inhibitory PPC-M1 interactions seen in HCs was lost in

PD patients. Our results suggest that sensorimotor integration pathway which normally

evoked by RHI is disrupted in PD patients and that sensorimotor integration is altered at

multiple levels in PD.

Page 82: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

66

4.2 Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder and is currently

diagnosed largely based on its motor features. Disruption in the basal ganglia-motor

cortex circuits is considered to account for the motor deficits. However, non-motor

symptoms, including sensory problems, are common in patients with PD(S. Fahn et al.,

2011) and it has been suggested that sensory abnormalities may contribute to motor

deficits.

Sensory features in PD encompass pain, tingling or numbness of the affected limbs,

and abnormal sensory processing such as decreased tactile processing and

proprioceptive deficits. Because previous studies have consistently reported deficits in

sensorimotor integration in PD patients, the abnormal sensory processing in PD patients

may influence their motor performance by affecting sensorimotor integration. Behavioral

studies demonstrated a failure of proprioceptive-motor integration in PD patients (Flash

et al., 1992; Jobst et al., 1997; Rickards & Cody, 1997). TMS studies in PD patients also

revealed abnormal sensorimotor integration. PD patients showed reduced LAI with off

medication which was normalized by STN-DBS but not by dopaminergic medications.

Although SAI was normal with off medication, dopaminergic medication reduced SAI

(Sailer et al., 2003). SAI is likely to represent direct interaction between S1 and M1,

while LAI possibly involves cortical association areas and the basal ganglia (Sailer et al.,

2003). Therefore, S1, M1 and cortical association areas are likely involved in the

altered sensorimotor integration in PD. However, the detailed cortical mechanisms

involved in the impaired sensorimotor integration in PD has not been studied.

Multisensory integration, a process of converging sensory inputs from different sensory

modalities, links closely with sensorimotor integration. Multisensory areas in the brain

include the PPC and ventral premotor cortex (PMv). These areas are also activated

during some motor responses, suggesting that they serve as the interference between

multisensory integration and sensorimotor integration. Neuroimaging studies have

Page 83: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

67

reported that these multisensory areas were hypoactive in PD patients (Eidelberg, 2009;

Lozza et al., 2004; Ma et al., 2007; Moeller et al., 1999; Tessitore et al., 2012). A TMS

study showed reduced PPC-M1 interaction in PD(Palomar et al., 2013). These results

indicate that multisensory integration may be altered in PD and that the alteration may

underlie the abnormal sensorimotor integration mechanisms. However, there are no

neurophysiological studies examining the interaction between sensory cortices and

motor cortices during multisensory integration in PD patients.

Multisensory integration can be evoked and evaluated by RHI, in which visual, tactile

and proprioceptive sensory information interacts with each other, leading to an illusory

feeling of ownership of a rubber hand in view and a shift in perceived position of

occluded own hand towards the rubber hand. In Study One, we showed that

sensorimotor integrations measured as SAI and LAI were reduced by RHI in healthy

individuals. We also demonstrated that stronger RHI correlated with less facilitatory

interaction between PPC and M1.

In the present study, we assessed RHI evoked by visuo-tactile-proprioceptive

integration in PD patients on and off dopaminergic medications and compared it with

that in age-matched HCs. We further investigated the modulations of SAI and LAI by

RHI in PD patients. Finally, we analyzed the correlation between the RHI strength and

the PPC-M1 interactions in PD patients. We hypothesize that: 1) RHI will be diminished

in PD patients OFF medications (PD-OFF) due to impaired visuo-tactile-proprioceptive

integration, 2) both SAI and LAI will be less affected by RHI in PD-OFF compared to

controls due to deficits in sensorimotor integration 3) the correlation between the PPC-

M1 interaction and the strength of RHI will be disrupted in PD-OFF reflecting PPC

dysfunction. 4) The above deficits will be restored by dopaminergic medications in PD-

ON state.

4.3 Methods

4.3.1 Subjects

Page 84: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

68

Twelve patients with PD (age 52-79, mean age ± SD 64.9±8.0 years old: 9 men and 3

women, 2 left handed and 10 right handed) and 12 age-matched HCs (age 51-77, mean

age ± SD 64.1 ± 7.8 years old: 6 men and 6 women, all right handed) were tested. The

data of 11HCs were included in Study One. PD patients who fulfilled the following

criteria were included; diagnosed with PD based on UK PD society Bank Criteria, aged

between 50 and 80 years old and stable dopaminergic medication dose over 4 weeks.

PD patients either with severe tremor, severe dyskinesia, cognitive impairment, any

other neurological or psychiatric diseases, medication of anti-depressants or sedatives,

or history of DBS surgery were excluded. A summary of the clinical features of the PD

patients are shown in Table 1. The disease duration was 7.6 ± 5.3 years. Unified

Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) part III was used to assess the severity of

the motor signs in “ON” or “OFF” dopaminergic medication states. The ON and OFF

medication states were evaluated on separate days. For the “on” medication visit, the

PD patients took their usual dopaminergic medications (PD-ON). For the “off”

medication visit, they were tested after overnight withdrawal of the dopaminergic

medications at least for 12 hours (PD-OFF). The presence of sensory symptoms was

assessed by asking whether the patient had uncomfortable feelings in his or her body

such as pain, aches, tingling or cramps over the past four weeks (adopted from question

1.9 in the UPDRS). All participants gave written informed consents and the protocol

was approved by University Health Network Ethics Board.

4.3.2 Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI) and behavioral assessments

The RHI task, originally reported by Botvinick and Cohen(Botvinick & Cohen, 1998) was

used in the same way with Study One. Briefly, a rubber right hand, which is in view, was

set in an anatomically congruent way on a table. The participant’s actual right hand was

placed lateral to the rubber hand and was occluded from the participant’s view (Figure

6A). Prior to the induction of RHI, proprioceptive judgement (PJ) reporting the right

index finger position was repeated 6 times and the values were averaged and

determined as the baseline position. The RHI was induced by stroking both the rubber

Page 85: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

69

hand index finger and the participants’ own index finger simultaneously for three

minutes. The strokes of the two paintbrushes were synchronized in terms of timing and

location in the synchronous condition (experimental condition), and they were out of

phase in the asynchronous condition (control condition). The order of the synchronous

and asynchronous conditions was randomized. PJ was repeated immediately after the

induction period in each condition. At the end of each block, questionnaires that

consisted of 9 statements were administered to assess the subjective experience of

RHI. For each statement, the participants rated their agreement by marking on a 14 cm-

long visual-analog scale (0=completely disagree, 14=completely agree).

4.3.3 Digital nerve stimulation (DNS)

Electrical stimulation was applied to the palm side of the right index finger over the distal

and middle phalanx through small pad electrodes (size, 28mm x 20 mm) with cathode

positioned proximally using a DS7A constant-current stimulator (pulse width 0.2ms;

Digitimer, Welwyn Garden City, UK). Sensory threshold (ST) was determined as the

lowest intensity felt by the participants.

4.3.4 Surface electromyography (EMG) recording

Surface electromyography (EMG) was recorded from the relaxed right first dorsal

interosseous (FDI) muscle using Ag-AgCl electrodes with the active electrode on the

muscle belly and the reference electrode over the right metacarpo-phalangeal joint. The

EMG signals were amplified (1000 X), filtered (band pass 20 Hz-2.5kHz; Intronix

Technologies Corporation Model 2024F, Bolton, Ontario, Canada), digitized at 5kHz by

an analog-to-digital (A/D) interface (Micro 1401, Cambridge Electronics Design,

Cambridge, UK) and stored in a laboratory computer for off-line analysis using SIGNAL

software (Cambridge Electronic Devices, Cambridge, UK).

Page 86: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

70

4.3.5 Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was performed using a Magstim 2002

stimulator (Magstim, Whitland, UK) and a custom-made figure-of-eight coil (diameter, 50

mm). The area for eliciting the best motor response in the right FDI muscle was

established over the left M1 with posterior to anterior induced current direction and the

coil position was marked on the scalp as the motor hot spot. The test stimulus (TS)

intensity was adjusted to evoke ~ 1 mV peak-to-peak amplitude motor evoked potentials

(MEPs) from the relaxed right FDI muscle. Peak-to-peak MEP amplitude for each trial

was measured and averaged for each stimulation condition.

4.3.5.1 Experiment 1: Measurement of short-latency afferent inhibition

(SAI) and long-latency afferent inhibition (LAI)

In Experiment 1, we assessed SAI and LAI in 12 PD patients and 12HCs. The data from

eleven healthy participants who participated in the study one were included. As a

conditioning stimulus, DNS was applied with the intensity of 3 times of sensory

threshold (Chen et al., 1999; Sailer et al., 2003). Each run consisted of 3 different

conditions: TS alone, DNS followed by TS at ISI of 23ms for SAI (DNS23-TS), and at ISI

of 203 ms for LAI (DNS203-TS). These conditions were delivered in a random order and

repeated 11 times with 5 seconds apart. Eleven trials for each condition (total 33 trials)

were recorded at baseline and as well as immediately after the induction of RHI.

4.3.5.2 Experiment 2: Measurement of the interaction between the

posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and the primary motor cortex

(M1)

Page 87: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

71

In Experiment 2, we assessed the PPC-M1 interaction with a paired-pulse TMS

paradigm using two coils. We studied 12 PD patients and 12 age-matched healthy

volunteers, 11 of whom were included in Study One. The left PPC was identified using

a frameless stereotaxic neuronavigation (Brainsight Rogue Research, Canada) system.

The participants’ T1-weighted magnetic resonance images (MRI) were obtained before

the study visit. We obtained the MRI images from 9 PD patients and 8 HCs. The left

medial superior parietal lobe (SPL) was identified based on the individual anatomical

landmarks (Talairach coordinates: group mean ± SD for PD patients: x=-24.8±14.1, y=-

78.1±12.18, z=60.6±16.4: Age matched HCs: x=-21.4±6.6, y=-68.2±10.6, z=55.4±10.1).

For those who did not have MRI images available, the CP1 in the International 10-20

electroencephalogram (EEG) system was used to locate the left SPL. Conditioning

stimuli (CS) was delivered to the left PPC with posterior-anterior (PA) current direction

through a 40mm figure-of-eight coil (Figure7). The TS coil was placed over the left M1 at

the optimal position for the right FDI muscle (the motor hot spot). TS intensity was

adjusted to evoke ~ 1 mV MEP amplitudes. The CS intensity was set at 90% of resting

motor threshold (RMT), which was determined from the motor hot spot as the lowest

intensity which evoked more than 50 µV peak-to-peak amplitude MEPs in 5 out of 10

consecutive trials. Each run consisted of 5 conditions with different inter-stimulus

intervals (ISIs: TS alone, CS followed by TS; CS-TS at 4, 6, 8, or 10 ms of ISI) and

these 5 conditions were delivered in random order with 5 seconds apart and repeated 9

times each.

4.3.6 Experimental Designs

We tested the modulation of SAI and LAI by RHI in Experiment 1, and that of PPC-M1

interaction in Experiment 2. These experiments were conducted either in a same day or

separate days. As shown in Fig 6B, each experiment started with baseline TMS

measurement followed by PJ. After RHI induction either with synchronous or

asynchronous condition and post-RHI PJ, the TMS measurements were repeated. The

participants kept their right hand still until the TMS measurement was completed. After

Page 88: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

72

filling out the questionnaire, the participants were given a five- minute break, during

which they moved their right hand to cancel the RHI effect. Then, the alternate condition

was tested with the same procedure. The order of the synchronous and asynchronous

conditions was randomized and counter-balanced.

4.4 Data Analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted with SPSS 17.0 and significance was set at P < 0.05

unless otherwise stated. Kolmogotov-Smirov normality tests were performed to test

normal distributions. If the values were not distributed normally, non-parametric tests

were used. Otherwise, parametric tests were performed.

4.4.1 Clinical Assessments

The severity of motor signs was assessed by UPDRS scores part III and the scores in

the “ON” medication state (PD-ON) and “OFF” medication state (PD-OFF) were

compared by paired-t test.

4.4.2 RHI Behavioural Assessments

The results from the first sets of synchronous and asynchronous conditions were used

to assess the strength of RHI at the behavioral level except for the correlation analysis

in the Experiment 2 in which the concurrent RHI experience was used to examine the

relationship between behavioural results and neurophysiological changes.

4.4.2.1 Evaluation of the Questionnaire

Page 89: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

73

The scores for the first three statements in the questionnaire (Q1-3) were added and

averaged in synchronous or asynchronous condition separately in each participant. The

RHI induction was examined by comparing the synchronous score with asynchronous

score using the paired t-test in each group. To evaluate the influence of dopaminergic

medications in PD patients, two-way repeated ANOVA was conducted with CONDITION

(Synchronous vs Asynchronous) and MEDICATION (ON vs OFF) as two separate

repeated measures. In order to compare the RHI strength in PD patients with age-

matched HCs, separate repeated measures ANOVA were performed with CONDITION

(Synchronous vs Asynchronous) as within subject factor and GROUP (PD “ON” patients

vs HCs, or PD “OFF” patients vs HCs) as between subject factor. Significant main

effects were further investigated by post-hoc paired t tests with Bonferroni correction.

4.4.2.2 Evaluation of the Proprioceptive judgment (PJ) task

The shift in the estimated position of own right index finger after the RHI induction (arm

shift) was computed by subtracting the baseline PJ value from the post induction PJ

value in each condition for individual participant. To compare PD patients with HCs, two-

separate ANOVAs were performed with GROUP (PD-ON vs HCs, or PD-OFF vs HCs)

as a between-subject factor and CONDITION (Synchronous vs Asynchronous) as

within-subject factor. The contribution of the dopaminergic medications was analyzed by

a separate two-way repeated measures ANOVA with MEDICATION (ON vs OFF) and

CONDITION (Synchronous vs Asynchronous) as two repeated measures.

4.4.3 Assessment of Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)

Measurements

4.4.3.1 Experiment1: The modulation of Short-latency afferent

inhibition (SAI) and Long-latency afferent inhibition (LAI) by

RHI

Page 90: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

74

As described in Study One, SAI and LAI were expressed as a ratio of the conditioned

(DNS23-TS, DNS203-TS) to the unconditioned (TS alone) MEP amplitudes. Ratios <1

indicate inhibitory and ratios > 1 indicate facilitatory influences of DNS to M1.

First, we examined the alteration of SAI at baseline by comparing the baseline SAI in

PD-ON or PD-OFF with that in HCs using two-tailed t-test. Secondly, the modulation of

SAI by RHI in HCs and PD patients were analyzed using two separate two-way

repeated ANOVAs with CONDITION (baseline, sync, async) as within-subject factor and

GROUP (HC, PD-ON/OFF) as between-subject factor. Thirdly, the effect of

dopaminergic medications on SAI in PD patients at baseline was assessed by

comparing PD-ON and PD-PFF with paired t-test. Then, the overall dopaminergic

medication influence was analyzed by another repeated measures ANOVA with

MEDICATION (ON and OFF) and CONDITION as two separate repeated measures.

The same analysis was performed for LAI. In addition, the baseline LAI ratio was

subtracted from the post induction LAI ratio (delta LAI) in each condition to further

investigate the LAI modulation by RHI. The delta LAI values for PD-ON or OFF group

was compared with that for HCs using repeated measures ANOVA with factor

CONDITION (synchronous, asynchronous) as within-group factor and GROUP (HCs vs

PD-ON/OFF) as a between-group factor. Significant main effects were followed up by

paired t tests with Bonferroni adjustment.

4.4.3.2 Experiment2: The modulation of Posterior parietal cortex

(PPC)-Primary motor cortex (M1) interaction by RHI

The MEP amplitudes at each ISI were expressed as a ratio of the conditioned (CS-TS at

ISI 4, 6, 8, and 10 ms of ISIs) to the unconditioned (TS alone) MEP amplitudes. Ratios

<1 indicates inhibitory and ratios > 1 indicates facilitatory influence of PPC to M1 at the

specific ISI. First, the baseline PPC-M1 interaction in the PD patients and the HCs were

compared using two separate repeated measures ANOVA with ISI (4, 6,8, and 10 ms)

being a within-subject factor and GROUP (PD-ON vs HCs or PD-OFF vs HC) being a

Page 91: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

75

between–subject factor. Secondly, the modulation of PPC-M1 interaction by RHI in

synchronous condition was analyzed by comparing PD patients with HCs using two

separate repeated measures ANOVA with ISI (4, 6,8, and 10 ms) being a within-subject

factor and GROUP (PD-ON vs HCs or PD-OFF vs HC) being a between–subject factor.

Thirdly, the same analysis was conducted for asynchronous condition. Any significant

main effects in these ANOVA tests were followed by post-hoc paired t-test with

Bonferroni correction.

In addition, the relationship between the strength of the RHI and the PPC-M1 interaction

was analyzed. The MEP ratios in synchronous condition were averaged across different

ISIs in each individual because there were no significant differences among ISIs in

Study One. The relationship between the averaged Q1-3 scores in Experiment 2 and

the averaged MEP amplitude in each group (PD-ON, PD-OFF, and HCs) was analyzed

separately using Pearson correlation.

4.5 Results

4.5.1 Clinical and demographic data

Table 1 shows the patients’ demographics, the scores for the UPDRS part III for ON or

OFF medication states as motor features and the scores for the sensation related

question as sensory symptoms. Three patients showed predominant motor symptoms

on the right side and 9 patients on the left side. The types of the predominant motor

symptoms were tremor in 5 patients (tremor type) and rigidity and bradykinesia in 7

patients(akinetic rigid type). Paired t-test showed that the UPDRS scores for the ON

visit was significantly lower than that for the OFF visit (p<0.001) confirming the clinical

benefits of dopaminergic medications on the motor symptoms.

Page 92: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

76

Table 1 Patients demographic Characteristics

Table 1

H&Y: Hoehn and Yahr scale, Gender F: female, M: male, Predominant side: the side of

the predominant motor symptoms, Predominant symp: types of the predominant motor

symptoms, T: tremor, AR: akinetic rigid type, UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s rating scale,

LED: levodopa equivalent dosage of dopaminergic medications, Sensory: score for

question 1.9 in UPDRS

Page 93: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

77

4.5.2 Behavioural assessments of Rubber Hand Illusion

4.5.2.1 Questionnaire on the rubber hand illusion experiences

The scores of agreements to statements 1-3 (Q1-3: the RHI related experiences) for the

synchronous and asynchronous conditions in each group are shown in Figure 11A. We

found that there was no difference in the level of RHI between PD patients and age-

matched HCs (HC) groups and that the dopaminergic medication did not influence

subjective feeling of RHI. In age-matched healthy population, the synchronous condition

scored significantly higher than the asynchronous condition (t(11)=3.68, p=0.004),

indicating successful induction of the RHI. PD patients in both ON and OFF medication

states also showed significant difference between the synchronous and asynchronous

scores (PD-OFF: t(11)=5.468, p=0.0002, PD-ON: t(11)=3.583, p=0.0043). Two-way

repeated ANOVA comparing PD-ON and OFF showed that there was an overall main

effect of Condition (F(1,11)= 22.7, p=0.001) with no main effect of Medication

(F(1,11)=0.04, p=0.85) or no interaction between Medication and Condition

(F(1,11)=3.91, p=0.07). Two-way ANOVA comparing PD-ON versus HC revealed an

overall main effect for Condition (F(1,22)=25.93, p<0.01), no significant main effect of

Group (F(1,22)<0.001, p=0.99) and no Condition by Group interactions (F(1.22)=0.295,

p=0.59). Similarly, comparison between PD-OFF versus HC also revealed an overall

main effect for Condition (F(1,22)=42.75,p<0.001), no significant main effect of Group

(F(1,22)=0.016, p<0.90) and no Condition by Group interactions (F(1.22)=2.82, p=0.11).

4.5.2.2 Proprioceptive judgment (PJ)

The shifts in perceived hand position in each group are shown in Figure 11B. We found

that no significant difference in the shift between PD group and HC. Dopaminergic

medication did not influence PJ in PD patients. Two-way ANOVA comparing PD-ON

and HC showed no main effect of Group (F(1,22)=0.63, p=0.44), no main effect of

Condition (F(1,22)=1.29, p=0.27), and no interaction between Condition and Group

Page 94: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

78

(F(1,22)=0.03, p=0.87). Comparison between PD-OFF and HC also showed no main

effect of Condition (F(1,22)=1.69, p=0.21), no main effect of Group (F(1,22)=0.63,

p=0.44) and no interaction between Condition and Group (F(1,22)=2.13, p=0.16). The

influence of dopaminergic medication was analyzed by two-way repeated ANOVA which

showed no main effect of Medication (F(1,11)=1.22, p=0.29), no main effect of Condition

(F(1,11)=2.08, p=0.18), or no significant interaction between Medication and Condition

(F(1,11)=0.23, p=0.64).

Page 95: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

79

Figure 11 Behavioral Results in PD patients and HCs

Figure 11 A) Questionnaire results showing subjective experience of RHI.X axis

denotes the average score for the first to the third statements in the questionnaire. The

average of the first three statements (Q1-3) in the synchronous condition were scored

higher than the asynchronous condition in HC, PD-OFF and PD-ON. All of them were

scored to the similar degrees. B) Proprioceptive judgment in each group. Shift in

hand position, shown in Y-axis, was calculated by subtracting the baseline hand position

from the post RHI position. There were no significant differences between HCs and PD-

OFF or PD-ON. The dopaminergic medication did not influence PJ. Error bars indicate

standard errors of means (SEM). S:synchronous, A:asynchronous, HC: age-matched

HCs, PD-OFF: PD patients with OFF medication, PD-ON: PD patients with ON

medication

Page 96: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

80

4.5.3 Modulation of short-latency afferent Inhibition (SAI) by Rubber

Hand Illusion

4.5.3.1 Comparison between Parkinson’s Disease Patients and

Healthy Controls

Figure 12 shows the modulation of SAI by RHI in each group. We found that PD-OFF

showed overall reduction in SAI compared to HC. T-test showed that SAI at baseline in

PD-OFF was not statistically different from HC (t(22)=-0.007, p=0.99). Two-way ANOVA

comparing PD-OFF and HC revealed a significant main effect of Group (F(1,22)=4.87,

p=0.038), but no main effect of Condition (F(2,44)= 1.91, p=0.16) or the interaction

between Condition and Group (F(2, 44)=1.08, p=0.35). The comparison between PD-

ON with HC by t-test showed that SAI at baseline in PD-ON was not different from HC

(t(22)=-0.985, p=0.34). Two-way ANOVA comparing PD-ON and HC showed no main

effect of Condition (F(2,44)=0.09, p=0.91), no main effect of Group (F(1,22)=0.75,

p=0.40) or no significant interaction between Condition and Group (F(2,44)=2.02,

p=0.14).

4.5.3.2 Dopaminergic Influence on the SAI modulation in Parkinson’s

Disease Patients

Paired t-test showed that SAI at baseline was not different between PD-ON and PD-

OFF (t(22)=0.90, p=0.39).Two-way repeated ANOVA comparing PD-ON and PD-OFF

showed no main effect of Medication (F(1,11)=1.18, p=0.30), or no main effect of

Condition (F(2,22)=1.30, p=0.29) or no interaction between Medication and Condition

(2,22)=1.22, p=0.32).

Page 97: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

81

Figure 12 Modulation of Short-latency Afferent Inhibition by RHI in PD patients

and HCs

Figure 12

Modulation of Short-latency afferent inhibition (SAI) in HCs, PD-OFF and PD-ON

patients. Y axis denotes the ratio of the conditioned to unconditioned MEP amplitude.

The ratio >1.0 indicates facilitatory and the ratio <1.0 indicates inhibitory influence of

DNS over M1 at short-latency (23ms). Baseline SAI in each group was not significantly

different each other. Overall SAI was significantly reduced in PD-OFF patients

compared to HCs. Error bars represents standard error of the mean. B: baseline,

S:synchronous, A:asynchronous, HC: age-matched HCs, PD-OFF: PD patients with

OFF medication, PD-ON: PD patients with ON medication.

Page 98: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

82

4.5.4 Modulation of Long-latency afferent Inhibition (LAI) by Rubber

Hand Illusion

4.5.4.1 Comparison between Parkinson’s Disease Patients and

Healthy Controls

A-priori t-test showed significant reduction of baseline LAI in PD-OFF patients compared

to HC (t(22)=-2.03, p=0.055 for PD-OFF). Baseline LAI in PD-ON was not significantly

different from HC (t(22)=-1.86, p=0.076).

The modulation of LAI by RHI in each group is shown in Figure13A. We found that LAI

in synchronous condition was increased in PD-ON while it was decreased in HC. The

comparison of PD-ON with HC using two-way ANOVA showed significant interaction

between Condition and Group (F(2,44)=5.21, p=0.009), with no main effect of Condition

(F(2,44)=1.64, p=0.21) or Group (F(1,22)=0.52, p=0.48). Post hoc analysis showed that

synchronous LAI in HC was significantly reduced compared to either baseline (p=0.038)

or the asynchronous condition (p=0.0089) in HC but not in PD-ON (sync vs baseline:

p=0.10, sync vs async p=0.94). In order to investigate the RHI modification of LAI

further, LAI change from the baseline in each condition in PD-ON patients was

compared to HC (Figure 13B). We confirmed the altered modulation of LAI in PD-ON.

Two-way ANOVA showed significant main effect of Group (F(1,22)=6.06, p=0.022) with

significant interaction between Group and Condition (F(1,22)=4.63, p=0.043). There was

no significant main effect of Condition (F(1,22)=2.21, p=0.15). We did not find any

difference between PD-OFF and HC (Figure 13A). Two-way ANOVA comparing PD-

OFF with HC revealed no significant effect of Group (F(1,22)=3.558, p=0.07),Condition

(F(2,44)=1.21, p=0.31) or Condition by Group interaction (F(2,44)=0.57,p=0.57).

4.5.4.2 Parkinson’s Disease Patients and the Effect of Dopaminergic

Medications

Page 99: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

83

As figure 13A shows, there was an overall reduction in LAI in PD-OFF compared to PD-

ON. LAI at baseline in PD-ON and OFF medication was compared by paired t-test

which showed no statistical difference (t(11)=-0.65, p=0.53).Two-way repeated ANOVA

comparing PD-ON and PD-OFF demonstrated a significant main effect of Medication

(F(1,11)=8.87,p=0.013), but no main effect of Condition (F(2,22)=0.20, p=0.82) or

Medication by Condition interaction (F(2,22)=0.94, p=0.41).

Page 100: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

84

Figure 13 Modulation of Long-latency Afferent Inhibition in PD patients and HCs

Figure 13A). Modulation of LAI.Y axis denotes ratio of the conditioned to

unconditioned MEP amplitude. Ratios>1.0 indicate facilitatory and ratios<1.0 indicate

inhibitory influence of DNS over M1 at long-latency (203 ms). Baseline LAI was

significantly reduced in PD-OFF compared to HCs. Overall LAI was also reduced in PD-

OFF compared to PD-ON. When PD-ON was compared to HC, there was a significant

interaction between Condition and Group suggesting that LAI was modulated differently

in PD-ON compared to HC. Error bars represents standard error of the mean (SEM). B:

baseline, S:synchronous, A:asynchronous, HC: age-matched HCs, PD-OFF: PD

patients with OFF medication, PD-ON: PD patients with ON medication. 13 B) Change

in LAI from the baseline. Baseline value for LAI was subtracted from post induction LAI

Page 101: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

85

are plotted. In synchronous condition, LAI was reduced from the baseline in HC while it

was increased in PD-ON confirming that RHI abnormal modulation of LAI in PD-ON.

Error bars indicates standard error of the mean (SEM).

Page 102: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

86

4.5.5 Modulation of PPC-M1 interaction by RHI

4.5.5.1 Comparison between Parkinson’s Disease Patients and Healthy

Controls

Figure 14A shows the baseline PPC-M1 interaction in each group. There was no

significant difference among the three group. Two-way ANOVA comparing PD-OFF with

HCs showed that there was no significant main effect of Group (F(1,22)=3.52, p=0.07)

or ISI (F(3,66)=0.38, p=0.77),and no interaction between Group and ISI (F(3,66)=0.19,

p=0.90). Similarly, the comparison between HCs and PD-ON with two-way ANOVA

showed no significant main effect of Group (F(1,22)=0.97, p=0.34) or ISI (F(3, 66)=1.36,

p=0.26), nor Group by ISI interaction F(3,66)=0.93, p=0.43).

PPC-M1 interactions in the synchronous condition are shown in Figure 14B.Comparison

between PD-OFF and HC by two-way ANOVA showed no significant main effect of

Group (F(1,22)=1.12, p=0.30) or ISI (F(3,66)=1.55, p=0.21), and no significant

interaction between Group and ISI (F(3, 66)=0.38, p=0.77).Two-way ANOVA comparing

PD-ON with HCs showed that there was no significant main effect of Group

(F(1,22)<0.001, p>0.99) or ISI (F(3,66)=0.27, p=0.85), and no Group x ISI interaction

(F(3,66)=1.00, p=0.40).

In the asynchronous condition (Figure 14C), PD-OFF was compared with HC using two-

way ANOVA showed no significant main effect of Group(F(1,22)=0.35, p=0.56) or ISI

((F3,66)=0.85, p=0.47), nor Group x ISI interaction (F(3,66)=2.59, p=0.06). Comparison

between PD-ON and HC revealed a significant main effect of ISI (F(3,66)=3.34,

p=0.024). There was no significant main effect of Group (F(1,22)=0.005, p=0.95) nor ISI

x Group interaction (F(3,66)=0.88, p=0.46). Post hoc analysis showed that PPC-M1

interactions at 8 ms (p=0.041) and 10 ms (p=0.02) ISIs were more facilitated than 4 ms

ISI in HC and PD-ON patients(Figure 13C).

Page 103: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

87

4.5.5.2 The Effect of Dopaminergic Medications in Parkinson’s

Disease Patients

The influence of dopaminergic medications on PPC-M1 interaction at baseline (Figure

14A) was analyzed by two-way repeated ANOVA which showed no significant main

effect of Medication (F(1,11)=0.051, p=0.83) or ISI (F(3,33)=0.55, p=0.65). There was

no interaction between Medication and ISI (F(3,33)=1.22, p=0.32). In the synchronous

condition, there was no significant main effect of Medication (F(1,11)=1.02, p=0.34), ISI

(F(3,33)=0.45, p=0.72) or no interaction between Medication and ISI (F(3,33)=1.28,

p=0.30). In the asynchronous condition, two-way repeated ANOVA showed no

significant main effect of Medication (F(1,11)=0.31, p=0.59), ISI (F(3,33)=0.98, p=0.41).

and interaction between Medication and ISI (F(3,33)=1.56, p=0.22).

Page 104: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

88

Figure 14 Modulation of PPC-M1 interaction by RHI

Page 105: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

89

Figure 14 A) PPC-M1 interaction at baseline.PPC-M1 interaction is expressed as a

ratio of the conditioned to unconditioned MEP amplitudes and is shown in the Y-axis.

The X-axis denotes 4 different ISIs, 4, 6, 8 and 10 ms. The ratios<1 indicate inhibitory,

and the ratios>1 indicate facilitatory influence of PPC over M1. PPC-M1 interaction at

baseline was similar between PD and HC. B) PPC-M1 interaction in the synchronous

condition. Modulation of PPC-M1 interaction by RHI was similar in HC and PD patients.

The dopaminergic medications did not influence the way RHI modulated the PPC-M1

interaction. C)PPC-M1 interaction in the asynchronous condition. No difference was

found between PD and HC in the asynchronous condition. PPC-M1 interaction overall

was more facilitated at 8ms and 10 ms ISI compared to 4ms ISI. Error bars denotes

standard error of means (SEM).

Page 106: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

90

4.5.5.3 Correlation between the Strength of RHI and PPC-M1

Interaction

Figure 15 shows the relationship between the strength of RHI experience and the

modulation of the PPC-M1 interactions by RHI in controls, PD-OFF and PD-ON group.

We found that the negative correlation in HC was lost in PD patients suggesting deficits

in PPC-M1 interaction in PD patients regardless of medication status. In HC, the

average score for Q1-3 negatively correlated with the average MEP amplitude across 4-

10ms ISI in synchronous condition (r2=0.40, p=0.028). The result indicates that stronger

RHI experience is associated with more inhibitory PPC-M1 interaction. In PD-OFF, the

average Q1-3score did not correlate with the MEP amplitudes in the synchronous

condition (r2=0.118, p=0.28). The correlation between the average Q1-3 and the

averaged MEP amplitude was not restored in PD-ON (r2=0.085, p=0.78).

Page 107: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

91

Figure 15 Correlations between RHI Strength and PPC-M1 interactions

Figure 15. Correlation between the level of RHI experience and PPC-M1

interaction in the synchronous condition in HCs (HC:top), PD-OFF(middle) and

Page 108: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

92

PD-ON (bottom). The PPC-M1 interaction is expressed as an average MEP amplitudes

ratio across 4-10 ms ISIs in the synchronous condition. The Y-axis represents the

average ratio of the amplitude of the conditioned to unconditioned MEPs. The ratios>1

indicate facilitatory, and the ratios<1.0 indicate inhibitory influence of PPC over M1. X-

axis denotes the average score for the first three statements in the questionnaire in the

synchronous condition. In HCs, there was a negative correlation between the average

MEP ratio and average score for the first three statements, indicating that greater

inhibitory PPC-M1 interactions were associated with stronger RHI experience.

Page 109: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

93

4.6 DISCUSSION

In the current study, we investigated whether multisensory integration evoked by RHI

was different in PD patients compared to HC at the behavioural level. The behavioural

result revealed that the visuo-tactile integration was preserved in PD patients. In

Experiment 1, we examined modulation of SAI and LAI by RHI to assess how

sensorimotor integration was influenced by the RHI-induced visuo-tactile integration.

We found that SAI was reduced overall in PD patients in the OFF-medication state

compared to HC. LAI in PD patients was found to be reduced in the OFF compared to

ON medication state, suggesting that dopaminergic medications influence LAI. LAI was

differently modulated by RHI in PD-ON compared to HC. LAI in the synchronous

condition was increased in PD-ON while it was decreased in HC. In Experiment 2, the

association between the strength of RHI and the inhibitory PPC-M1 interactions

observed in HC was not observed in PD patients regardless of dopaminergic medication

states, suggesting that the functional connectivity between PPC and M1 is disrupted in

PD.

4.6.1 Preserved RHI at the Behavioural Level in PD patients

Similar with HC, PD patients exhibited stronger feeling of RHI experiences assessed by

questionnaire in the synchronous condition than the asynchronous condition. The result

showed that there was no significant difference in the extent of the subjective feeling of

RHI between PD patients and HC, indicating that PD patients are able to integrate

vision and tactile inputs relating to own hand. We also demonstrated that dopaminergic

medications did not modulate the strength of RHI in PD. In a previous study, patients

with dystonia showed normal pattern of subjective RHI experience (Fiorio et al., 2011).

Therefore, both PD and focal hand dystonia patients have the ability to integrate vision

with tactile inputs relating to RHI. Proprioceptive shift occurs as a result of multisensory

integration in which more reliable vision dominates over proprioception( Botvinick &

Cohen, 1998). Our results showed that proprioceptive judgment in PD patients was also

Page 110: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

94

comparable to that in HC regardless of dopaminergic medication states. Patients with

focal hand dystonia showed reduced proprioceptive shift after RHI while they still

experienced subjective RHI( Fiorio et al., 2011). The selective impairment of

proprioceptive arm shift without lack of subjective experience of RHI may indicate the

failure of reducing the weight of proprioceptive inputs during multisensory integration.

The reduced proprioceptive shift in focal hand dystonia was presumed to be associated

with proprioceptive deficits (Fiorio et al., 2011). Although previous studies also reported

proprioceptive deficits in PD (Maschke et al., 2003; O'Suilleabhain et al., 2001; Zia et

al., 2000; Zia et al., 2002), we did not find any difference in the proprioceptive arm shift

between PD patients and HC. The underlying mechanism of proprioceptive deficits in

PD is likely different from that in focal hand dystonia. In previous studies, proprioceptive

deficits in PD or dystonia have been examined in tasks that required arm movements

(Avanzino & Fiorio, 2014; Maschke et al., 2003; O'Suilleabhain et al., 2001; Zia et al.,

2000; Zia et al., 2002) and therefore the deficits may reflect proprioceptive-motor

integration rather than proprioception as perception. Proprioceptive judgment in our

study only involves the multisensory systems but not the motor system, and thus

enables us to separate proprioceptive-motor integration from visuo-tactile-proprioceptive

integration. One possibility is that proprioceptive deficiency in PD patients may arise

from alterations in the link between the sensory and motor systems (proprioceptive-

motor integration) while that in focal hand dystonia may resulted from altered

multisensory integration of vision, tactile and proprioceptive perceptions. Another

possibility is that visual dominance in PD patients might have compensated for the

proprioceptive deficits. During RHI in healthy participants, a mismatch between visuo-

tactile and proprioceptive inputs compromises the vague proprioceptive inputs from own

hand and enforces visual inputs. As a result, the weight of the proprioceptive inputs is

reduced and the weight of the visual inputs are increased, leading to proprioceptive arm

shift towards the rubber hand. Because PD patients have been reported to rely more on

vision than proprioception (Jobst et al., 1997; Konczak et al., 2009; Martens & Almeida,

2012), they already have visual over-reliance and compromised proprioception before

Page 111: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

95

the induction of RHI begins. Their visual over-reliance might have concealed the

possible deficits in RHI induced multisensory integration.

4.6.2 Overall Reduction in SAI in PD-OFF Compared to HCs

SAI at baseline in PD patients did not differ from HC regardless of dopaminergic

medication states. Although it was reported in previous study that SAI was reduced at

rest in PD-ON but not in PD-OFF when MNS was used (Sailer et al., 2003), the same

study reported that SAI elicited by DNS did not show this finding.

When PD-OFF patients were compared with HCs, SAI overall was significantly reduced.

As there was no significant main effect of Condition or Group x Condition interaction, the

SAI reduction was not likely specific to RHI. However, considering that there was no

difference in SAI at baseline between the two groups, the reduced SAI in PD-OFF

medication might have evoked by the sensory stimuli in the both synchronous and

asynchronous conditions, such as visual or tactile, or both inputs. In other words, the

reduced SAI in PD-OFF may be more pronounced when visual and tactile sensory

stimuli were applied compared to without these additional stimuli.

We did not see any significant differences in overall SAI between PD-ON patients and

HC. This may seem to conflict the previous result showing reduced SAI in PD-ON

medication but normal SAI in PD-OFF medication (Sailer et al., 2003; Wagle Shukla et

al., 2013). However, several technical differences might lead to the different results.

Firstly, the disease severity of the tested hand might have been different in our study

and the past studies. In the previous studies, SAI was tested on the more affected side

while our study tested SAI in the right hand regardless of the side of the more affected

hand. In fact, nine out of twelve of our patients were tested in the less affected hand.

We tested the right hand because previous studies which showed the involvement of

parietal cortex in RHI tested the left hemisphere (Ehrsson et al., 2004; Kammers,

Verhagen, et al., 2009)and there maybe hemispheric specialization of PPC functions(

Kessels, d'Alfonso, Postma, & de Haan, 2000; Vesia & Crawford, 2012).In order to

Page 112: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

96

examine the left PPC-M1 interactions, the right hand was chosen for the induction of

RHI. Secondly, the technique to evoke SAI was different. MNS was stimulated at the

wrist in the previous studies while our study used DNS, with which a previous study

reported no significant reduction of SAI in PD-ON patients (Sailer et al., 2003). SAI by

DNS is not as pronounced as SAI by MNS. In addition, SAI was measured at rest in

previous studies while we measured SAI not only at rest but also during RHI

As SAI is likely to represent a direct interaction between S1 and M1, the reduced SAI in

PD-OFF may represent the reduction in the connection between S1 and M1 relating to

sensory stimuli. A neuroimaging study in PD patients reported impaired S1 activation

during a coin rotation task, which was not affected by dopaminergic medications(Foki et

al., 2015). Reduced M1 and S1 activities were reported during tactile stimulation in a

fMRI study in PD-OFF patients(Cao, Xu, Zhao, Long, & Zhang, 2011; B. Weder et al.,

2000). The reduced SAI in our study may reflect reduced activation in S1 after tactile

stimulation.

4.6.3 Reduced modulation in LAI in PD- OFF patients and Tactile-

motor Integration Improvement by Medication

Baseline LAI in PD-Off was reduced compared to HC while that in PD-ON showed non-

significant trend of reduction. Our results were consistent with a previous study that

showed reduction in LAI in PD patients regardless of dopaminergic medication states

(Sailer et al., 2003).

There was an overall reduction in LAI in PD-OFF compared to PD-ON. Comparison

between PD-ON and HC showed a significant Group x Condition interaction in LAI

modulation by RHI. The result indicates that dopaminergic medications might have

influenced the way RHI modulated LAI. In HC, LAI was disinhibited only in the

synchronous condition but not in the asynchronous condition, suggesting that the

disinhibition of LAI was associated with RHI. In PD-ON, LAI was increased compared to

baseline both in the synchronous and asynchronous conditions. LAI change from

Page 113: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

97

baseline also showed the significant interaction between Group and Condition

suggesting that the LAI modulation by RHI in the synchronous condition was abnormally

increased in PD-ON patients. While the mechanisms of LAI and this abnormal

modulation of LAI by RHI in PD-ON are not known, we speculated that LAI may involve

both basal ganglia and cortical association areas. The reduced LAI in the synchronous

condition in HC may represent a mechanism to resolve the conflict between

somatosensory inputs and visual inputs by reducing the weight of the conflicting

somatosensory inputs. This conflict-resolving mechanism may occur at cortical

association areas. PD-ON showed increase in LAI rather than reduction during the

conflict-resolving process. The result may imply the altered function in cortical

association areas in PD-ON. In addition, increased LAI in PD-ON may also reflect

improved tactile processing at basal ganglia level. LAI was restored by STN-DBS,

indicating the involvement of the basal ganglia in LAI (Wagle Shukla et al., 2013). It has

been proposed that altered tactile processing in PD patients can be attributed to

abnormalities in the basal ganglia (Conte et al., 2013) and several studies showed that

dopaminergic medications improved tactile processing (Artieda et al., 1992; Conte et al.,

2010; Shin et al., 2005). We speculated that the better tactile processing may increase

LAI in PD--ON, however, the function in cortical association areas, which supposed to

detect the conflict and decrease LAI, may remain altered in PD-ON, leading to the

different LAI modulation by RHI. Despite the altered modulation of LAI by RHI in PD

patients, they experienced RHI to the similar extent as HC, suggesting that cortical

association areas not involved in LAI may compensate to solve the conflict and to evoke

the ownership feeling. Another possibility is that the areas involved in LAI are also

activated by RHI, but the altered modulation of LAI by RHI in PD-ON may not lead to

changes in RHI experience.

4.6.4 Disrupted PPC-M1 interaction in PD patients

PPC is one of the cortical association areas that integrates multisensory information.

We showed that greater inhibitory projection from PPC to M1 was associated with

Page 114: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

98

stronger RHI experience in HC. We also demonstrated the disruption of this association

in PD patients regardless of dopaminergic medications, suggesting the abnormality in

PPC-M1 interaction involved in RHI. Our findings support a previous TMS study which

showed impaired PPC-M1 functional connectivity at rest in PD patients using the paired-

pulse TMS paradigm (Palomar et al., 2013). In this report, less impaired PPC-M1

functional connectivity was associated with faster reaction time, suggesting that this

functional connectivity is related to motor function. We did not find significant difference

in PPC-M1 interaction at rest between HC and PD patients. This may be due to the

difference in the functions of different subdivisions in the PPC. We stimulated SPL while

IPL was targeted in the previous study (Palomar et al., 2013). Our study indicates that

the disruption in PPC-M1 interaction in PD patients is not limited to IPL but also includes

SPL. The disruption of PPC-M1 interactions in PD could be due to the altered PPC

function. As PD patients showed normal RHI, they may have activated cortical areas

other than PPC for multisensory integration for RHI to compensate the disrupted

parieto-motor circuits. Imaging studies showed increased activation of the cerebellum

and premotor area in PD (Sabatini et al., 2000; Tessitore et al., 2012; Wu & Hallett,

2005).These areas may compensate for the PPC dysfunction in PD patients to integrate

multisensory inputs. Alternatively, changes in the subcortical white matter connections

between PPC and M1 may lead to the disrupted PPC-M1 interaction.

4.6.5 Limitation of the Study

The study has several limitations.

First, the behavioural measurement of RHI might not be sensitive enough to observe the

abnormal multisensory integration in PD. While RHI is visuo-tactile integration, other

types of multisensory integration such as visuo-proprioceptive integration or tactile-

proprioceptive integration may be affected in PD.

Another limitation is related to the complicated mechanism of RHI. Multiple brain areas

across the cerebral cortices seem to be involved in the experience of RHI. We did not

Page 115: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

99

test cortical areas other than PPC which may be abnormal in PD. The majority of

projections from PPC go to M1 via the premotor areas, and the premotor area may be

disrupted in PD.

The pathway mediating LAI remains unclear and we could not determine the locations

where the sensorimotor integration process was disrupted in PD. Further studies

investigating cortical association areas and the basal ganglia using techniques such as

EEG/MEG source localization or functional MRI during RHI in PD patients may be

helpful.

Lastly, the RHI behavioural and neurophysiology results did not correlate with UPDRS

scores. Therefore, the clinical relevance of the deficits shown in the current studies

remains unclear. UPDRS may not be sensitive enough to detect correlations with our

findings, which may relate to non-motor rather than motor features of PD. In addition,

we tested only the right hand for RHI behavioral study and the left hemisphere for the

neurophysiological studies. The majority of the patients had the left side as the more

affected side. Future study can test both hands and both hemispheres.

4.7 Conclusions

The current study showed normal visuo-tactile- integration in PD patients at the

behavioural level. However, our neurophysiological studies revealed abnormal

sensorimotor integration while utilizing multisensory inputs in PD patients. The abnormal

sensorimotor integration pathways in PD patients may involve in cortical association

areas including PPC which functionally interacts with M1.

Page 116: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

100

5 GENERAL DISCUSSION

5.1 Summary of Results

Study One examined how RHI, a method to induce visuo-tactile integration, modulates

the integration between sensory and motor systems in HC. The main results of the

studies are: 1) RHI reduced sensorimotor integration measured by SAI and LAI in

healthy participants and 2) Stronger RHI experience was correlated with greater

inhibitory PPC-M1 interaction in healthy participants. The reduction in SAI by RHI may

indicate a diminished gain of somatosensory inputs to S1 due to the conflict between

vision and proprioceptive/tactile inputs. The reduction in LAI by RHI likely also reflects

modulation of somatosensory inputs in cortical association areas such as PPC, where

multisensory inputs are integrated. Using these methods established in Study One,

Study Two compared PD patients with age-matched HC. We demonstrated that 1) PD

patients showed RHI experience to a similar degree as age-matched HC at the

behavioural level, 2) PD-ON showed increase in LAI after RHI while LAI was decreased

in HCs, suggesting that RHI activated multisensory and sensorimotor integration

pathways differently compared to control participants and 3) PD patients lost the

association between the strength of RHI and the inhibitory PPC-M1 interactions

regardless of the medication status.

5.2 Normal Perception of Rubber Hand Illusion in PD Patients

at the Behavioural Level

We evaluated the level of RHI using a questionnaire. Q1 asked the ownership of the

rubber hand; #1. I felt as if the rubber hand were my own hand.Q2 and Q3 asked the

relocation of the touch; #2. It seemed as if I were feeling the touch of the paintbrush in

the location where I saw the rubber hand touched, #3. The touching of the rubber hand

Page 117: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

101

felt just like an actual touch. Experiencing ownership of a rubber hand requires both

multisensory integration (Botvinick & Cohen, 1998; Makin et al., 2008; Tsakiris &

Haggard, 2005) and higher-level of cognitive process of judgment in which the

appearance of the rubber hand is compared with participants’ own body image stored in

the brain (Kilteni et al., 2015; Tsakiris, 2010). When the rubber hand appearance is

realistic and there is simultaneous tactile stimulation from the own hand with visual

capture of the paintbrush stroke on the rubber hand, the rubber hand will be

incorporated into the subject's own body image, leading to ownership of the rubber

hand. Relocation of the touches of the paintbrush from own hand to rubber hand occurs

when the simultaneous visual and tactile stimuli reinforce the RHI, leading to

compromising incongruent proprioceptive inputs, which inform the origin of the tactile

inputs. Therefore, the average score for these three questions reflects dominated visuo-

tactile integration and reduced weight of proprioception. Because there was no

significant difference between HC and PD patients in these questionnaire scores, higher

level of sensory processing as well as visual-tactile integration accompanied by reduced

gain of proprioception appear to be intact in PD patients.

Another way of evaluating RHI was PJ, in which we measured how much the

participants perceived their own hand position towards the rubber hand. It examined

more specifically the reduced gain of proprioceptive inputs in the multisensory

integration due to the conflict. Because this process involves proprioception, the PJ

result in PD patients is of interest. Regardless of medication status, PJ in PD patients

did not differ from the results in age-matched HC. Proprioception or proprioceptive-

motor integration has been reported as defective in PD patients, especially without

vision (Jobst et al., 1997; Konczak et al., 2009; Martens & Almeida, 2012),suggesting

compensation of proprioception by visual inputs. We need to distinguish proprioceptive-

motor integration from proprioception. Previous experiments used kinematic movements

of arm or fingers to assess proprioception (Flash et al., 1992; Jobst et al., 1997;

Konczak et al., 2009; Maschke et al., 2003; O'Suilleabhain et al., 2001; Zia et al., 2000;

Zia et al., 2002), and therefore, they evaluated proprioceptive-motor integration. On the

other hand, PJ in our study likely represents perceptive aspect of proprioception, rather

Page 118: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

102

than proprioceptive-motor integration. Normal PJ in PD patients may support the

hypothesis that the proprioceptive deficit in PD patients reported in the previous studies

can be attributed to proprioceptive-motor integration deficits rather than perception in

proprioception.

5.3 The effect of RHI on Short-latency Afferent Inhibition in

Healthy individuals and PD patients

Study One with healthy individuals showed significant reduction of SAI in the

synchronous condition but not in the asynchronous condition compared to baseline. We

proposed that the reduction of SAI during RHI was due to the suppression of

somatosensory tactile processing in S1, leading to its reduced inhibitory influence over

M1. However, there was no significant reduction in SAI in the synchronous condition in

the age-matched HC group in Study Two. This may be because different analyses were

performed in Studies One and Two. The aim of Study One was to examine how the SAI

was affected by RHI while Study Two was to compare the modulation of SAI by RHI in

PD and controls. In Study One, participants who did not show SAI at baseline were

excluded from the analysis while in Study Two, all patients and controls were analyzed

regardless of the level of SAI at baseline.

No previous studies have examined the impact of vision or tactile stimulus on SAI. SAI

under the influence of sensory inputs may be different from SAI at rest. In Study Two,

we showed the overall reduction in SAI in PD-OFF. This suggests that SAI deficits in PD

may occur under the influence of the sensory inputs rather than at rest.

5.4 Rubber Hand Illusion Effect on Long-latency Afferent

Inhibition in PD Patients and HCs

Page 119: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

103

In Study One, we showed that the long-latency inhibitory influence of somatosensory

inputs was reduced after RHI. We speculated that the reduction of LAI reflects the

involvement of cortical association areas such as PPC because we also showed the

inhibitory PPC-M1 interaction was associated the strength of RHI. The cortical

association areas may serve as a gate keeper to judge which somatosensory inputs

should be weighted more when a conflict occurs among multisensory inputs. Study Two

examined LAI modulation by RHI in PD patients. Again, it should be noted that we

excluded the participants who did not showed LAI at baseline from the analysis in Study

One whereas all the participants were included in the analysis in Study Two regardless

of level of baseline LAI. Nevertheless, healthy participants in Studies One and Two

showed the similar results.

In Study Two, PD-OFF patients showed reduced LAI at baseline compared to HC, and

overall reduction in LAI compared to PD-ON. The results indicate impairment in tactile-

motor integration in PD-OFF. It has been proposed that the somatosensory

abnormalities in PD may be attributed to a change in the receptive fields of tactile inputs

in the basal ganglia as a result of dopaminergic denervation in the striatum in PD (Conte

et al., 2013; Suppa, Bologna, Conte, Berardelli, & Fabbrini, 2016). The tactile receptive

field was proposed to be enlarged in PD and the enlarged receptive field may bring

more “noise” into sensory perception resulting in increased tactile threshold.

Dopaminergic medications were hypothesized to reduce both the noise and the

threshold of tactile perception (Conte et al., 2013) and they can influence tactile

perception at the basal ganglia level. Taking the theory into account, in PD-OFF, tactile

stimulation from own hand during the RHI procedure might be contaminated by noises

due to the enlarged receptive fields. The noisy tactile signal from own hand may have

less influence on M1 in PD-OFF, resulting in reduced LAI.

On the other hand, in PD-ON, the noise level and the tactile threshold may be improved

by dopaminergic medications leading to better tactile-motor integration represented as

increased LAI. However, LAI was supposed to be decreased in the synchronous

condition as seen in HC while it was increased LAI in PD-ON. Because the modulation

of LAI only occurred in the synchronous condition, it is specific to the RHI experience.

Page 120: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

104

As mentioned above, we suggested that the reduction of LAI in the synchronous

condition in healthy individuals can be attributed to reduced somatosensory processing

in cortical association areas. It is possible that the different modulation of LAI by RHI in

HC and PD-ON is due to the altered activation pattern in cortical association areas,

which was shown in several imaging studies in PD(Lozza et al., 2004; Ma et al., 2007;

Moeller et al., 1999; Samuel et al., 1997).In PD, the weighting mechanism in cortical

association areas may be defective. Because the abnormal LAI modulation by RHI was

only seen in PD-ON but not in PD-OFF, dopaminergic medications may play a role in

the LAI modulation. Dopamine receptors were predominantly found in striatal regions in

the basal ganglia in monkey and human brain, followed by substantia nigra, thalamus

and amygdala(Christian et al., 2009; Missale, Nash, Robinson, Jaber, & Caron, 1998).

Dopamine receptors were also found in cerebral cortices such as frontal and temporal

areas with lower amount than the basal ganglia (Rani & Kanungo, 2006). Because the

amount of the dopaminergic receptors at the cortex was less than the basal ganglia,

dopaminergic medications may not fully restore the altered function in the cortical

association areas leading to no reduction in the influence of tactile inputs by the

weighting system. In other words, the tactile-motor integration reflected as LAI may be

increased in PD-ON because of the enlarged tactile receptor field and the improvement

in tactile processing by dopaminergic medications at the basal ganglia level (Conte et

al., 2013) and the PPC failed to suppress it during RHI.

5.5 PPC-M1 Interactions in PD patients and HCs

Both Studies One and Two showed that greater inhibitory PPC-M1 interaction was

associated with stronger subjective feelings of RHI in HC. Study Two revealed that this

association was not present in PD patients regardless of dopaminergic medication

states.

Functional MRI studies in healthy individuals showed that the strength of RHI correlated

with the level of activities in the PMv (Ehrsson et al., 2005; Ehrsson et al., 2004).

Page 121: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

105

Increased activities in the IPL, the medial wall of intraparietal sulcus was reported during

RHI (Ehrsson et al., 2004). As IPL is connected to PMv (Petrides & Pandya, 1984), IPL

was assumed to be involved in the association between PMv activity and the subjective

feeling of RHI. However, inhibitory rTMS to IPL did not disrupted the ownership feeling

but instead, it led to attenuation of proprioceptive arm shift (Kammers, Verhagen, et al.,

2009). Considering these previous studies, other areas in PPC may also be associated

with RHI experiences. We focused on SPL in our study because this area involves static

representation of hand (Jane E Aspell et al., 2012; Harris & Wolpert, 1998; Pellijeff et

al., 2006) and the area is likely involved in multisensory integration during RHI.IPL

involves visuo-motor integration and IPL-M1 interactions were modulated during motor

tasks rather than when perceiving/ judging sensory stimuli. SPL receives greater tactile

and proprioceptive inputs from S1 than IPL (Sakata et al., 1973) and converges them to

create a body image relating to its location and posture relative to other body parts

(Andersen, 1997; Gentile et al., 2011; Lewis et al., 2011). A TMS study showed that

SPL-M1 interactions were facilitated compared to at rest while receiving vibratory stimuli

to the index finger, indicating that SPL-M1 interactions was modulated by

somatosensory inputs (Ziluk et al., 2010).All these studies suggest that SPL-M1

interaction may be modulated by RHI.

Area 5 in SPL was localized based on individual MRIs using anatomical landmarks in

most participants. However, anatomical MRIs were not available for seven out of

twenty-two participants in Study One, and four out of thirteen HC and three out of

thirteen PD patients in Study Two. When the MRI was not available, the CP1 in the

International 10-20 EEG system was used to locate SPL. This method may not be as

accurate as using individualized MRI. However, a previous study showed that CP1

corresponded to SPL (Koessler et al., 2009) and therefore, SPL was likely stimulated in

all the participants.

In Study One, SPL did not have significant influence over M1 at rest. Although the SPL-

M1 interactions were not significantly different between at rest, in the synchronous and

the asynchronous conditions, we found that the individual SPL-M1 interaction in the

synchronous condition were associated with the strength of RHI experiences.

Page 122: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

106

Participants who experienced stronger subjective feelings of RHI had greater inhibitory

SPL-M1 interactions. The result suggests that SPL-M1 interaction is involved in RHI.

In Study Two, the correlation between the RHI strength and the PPC-M1 interaction in

the synchronous condition was not observed in PD patients regardless of medication

states, suggesting altered PPC-M1 interaction in response to RHI in PD. Only one study

investigated PPC-M1 interactions in PD patient using the paired-pulse TMS technique

and IPL was the stimulation target (Palomar et al., 2013). The IPL-M1 interaction was

found to be facilitatory at 4 ms ISI at rest in HC while PD patients showed a lack of the

IPL-M1 interaction. In our study, PD patients as well as HC showed no inhibitory or

facilitatory influences of SPL on M1 at baseline. As SPL and IPL have different

functions, the modulation of M1 by SPL can be different from IPL modulation. Our study

showed that the SPL-M1 interaction at rest in PD patients did not differ from that in HC.

However, the lack of correlation between the strength of RHI experience and greater

inhibitory SPL-M1 interactions in PD during RHI may indicatethe altered SPL function in

PD. Neuroimaging studies in PD suggested abnormal activations in PPC, including SPL

(Lozza et al., 2004; Ma et al., 2007; Moeller et al., 1999; Samuel et al., 1997). In PD

patients, SPL may not be involved in the occurrence of RHI because they still felt RHI to

the similar extent with the HC in spite of the loss of correlation. PD patients may activate

other cortical or subcortical areas than SPL to experience RHI. Association cortical

areas, such as the premotor area, may compensate the SPL dysfunction to experience

RHI because several studies have shown enhanced activation of this area in PD

patients (Michely et al., 2015; Turner, Grafton, McIntosh, DeLong, & Hoffman, 2003).

Cerebellum may play a role in the compensation because the local metabolism in this

area is found to be increased in PD patients at rest (Ko, Lerner, & Eidelberg, 2015).

Alternative explanation of the lack of correlation in PD is abnormality in the white matter

tracts which connect PPC to M1. Microstructural abnormalities were found in the

superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF) in PD (Burton, McKeith, Burn, Williams, & O'Brien,

2004; Gattellaro et al., 2009). As PPC projects to M1 directly or indirectly to M1 through

SLF, the disrupted white matter could lead the aberrant PPC-M1 interactions in PD.

Page 123: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

107

5.6 Converging Evidence for Disrupted Sensorimotor and

Multisensory Integration in Parkinson’s disease

In summary, our study in PD patients showed the following results; 1) Normal RHI at the

behavioural level in PD patients with ON and OFF dopaminergic medication status

suggesting that visuo-tactile integration is preserved in PD, 2) Overall reduction in SAI

in PD-OFF compared to controls, which was restored by dopaminergic medications, 3)

Overall reduction in LAI in PD patients OFF medications compared to ON medications,

4) Increased LAI in PD patients with dopaminergic medications, with abnormal response

to multisensory stimuli, 5) Disrupted PPC-M1 connectivity in PD-ON and OFF

conditions.

These findings suggest that sensorimotor integration pathways in PD patients is

disrupted at multiple levels. The overall reduction in SAI in PD-OFF suggests the

disruption occurs at S1. The reduction of LAI in PD-OFF also suggests the disruption

along the basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical pathway, likely involving both basal ganglia and

cortical association areas. Functional interactions between PPC, one of the critical

cortical areas for multisensory integration and M1, was altered in PD patients during

multisensory integration suggesting deficits in the PPC-M1 pathway.

Functional MRI studies have shown that levodopa reduces the over-activated neural

activity at rest in PD-OFF medication state as well as during visuo-motor tasks

(Asanuma et al., 2006; Feigin et al., 2001; Ko et al., 2015; Ng, Palmer, Abugharbieh, &

McKeown, 2010), suggesting a a “network normalization” effect of levodopa (Ko et al.,

2015).” However, the effects of levodopa were prominent at rest rather than during

motor tasks, indicating that the effects of levodopa is influenced by a task involved. Our

studies employed a multisensory integration task which activated large areas of the

sensory system. Dopaminergic treatment might have influenced these areas differently

during our task from at rest or during other motor tasks. Our results suggest that the

network involved in multisensory integration in the PD-OFF state was not restored by

Page 124: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

108

dopaminergic treatment and that it was rather abnormally modulated. In the future, it will

be interesting to test whether the network involved in multisensory integration will be

restored by DBS therapy because cortical association areas receive inputs from basal

ganglia.

5.7 Future Directions

5.7.1 Multisensory Integration in PD patients

Impairments in visuo-auditory integration and tactile-proprioceptive integration in PD

patients have been reported in the past (Fearon et al., 2015; Konczak et al., 2008;

Konczak et al., 2012; Li et al., 2010). In our study, visuo-tactile integration was tested in

PD patients and was normal at the behavioural level. As vision and tactile perceptions

are more involved in RHI than proprioception, RHI may not fully detect impairment of

integration of proprioceptive inputs into other sensory inputs. In the future, a paradigm

that evokes vision, tactile, and proprioception to the same degree may be used to

further assess visuo-tactile-proprioceptive integration in PD.

Our behavioral result also suggests that visuo-tactile cue may be helpful for PD patients

in rehabilitation settings. It is well known that auditory or visual cues often help PD

patients with freezing of gait. As we showed that visuo-tactile integration is normal in PD

patients, tactile cues combined with visual cues may be more effective than visual or

auditory cue alone or visuo-tactile cue, which found to be less appreciated in the

patients compared to HC (Fearon et al., 2015). The development of a method to

facilitate visuo-tactile integration may have a potential role in rehabilitation in PD.

5.7.2 Disrupted and Compensatory Sensorimotor Integration

Pathways in PD patients

Page 125: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

109

Our neurophysiology studies found that sensorimotor integration pathways that normally

evoked by RHI in HC were disrupted in PD patients. The disruption includes multiple

brain areas such as S1 and PPC. Dopaminergic medications influence the pathways,

suggesting the involvement of the basal-ganglia-thalamo-cortical circuits for

sensorimotor integration. It will be intriguing to investigate how DBS modulates the

abnormal sensorimotor integration during multisensory integration because a previous

report showed that DBS restored LAI at rest(Wagle Shukla et al., 2013). Because visuo-

tactile integration at the behavioural level is normal in PD, it is possible that other areas

may be activated as a compensatory mechanism. In the future, it will be helpful to

identify the areas that are abnormally activated in PD patients during visuo-tactile

integration. Imaging techniques, such as functional MRI or magneto-encephalography

(MEG), may help to identify the areas that are more activated in PD than controls during

visuo-tactile integration. These neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies should be

done with multisensory integration tasks rather than at rest as our findings suggested

that the difference between PD and HC which found during the task performance may

not be apparent at rest.

It is also possible that the functional connection between PPC and M1 was disrupted in

PD rather than PPC dysfunction. In our study, we did not distinguish whether PPC itself

was malfunctioning or the interactions between PPC and M1 was disrupted. MRI

tractography of the SLF in PD maybe helpful to identify the nature of the disrupted PPC-

M1 interactions.

5.7.3 The Influence of Dopaminergic Medications

Although dopaminergic medications did not influence multisensory integration at the

behavioural level, they abnormally modulated the sensorimotor integration process

which may involve association cortices at neurophysiological level. We did not observe

any correlation between the neurophysiological results and the clinical scores, which

mainly assessed PD motor signs. However, it is possible that the disruption of

Page 126: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

110

sensorimotor integration by dopaminergic medications may be relevant to dyskinesias,

which are excessive, involuntary movements induced by dopaminergic medications in

advanced PD. Future study may examine the relationship between dyskinesias and the

neurophysiological sensorimotor integration parameters.

5.7.4 Possible Intervention Therapy for PD patients

Our study explored neurophysiological modulation of sensorimotor integration pathways

in response to multisensory stimuli. Multiple brain regions along the pathways seem to

be disrupted in PD leading to an abnormal connection between the sensory and the

motor systems in the brain. In the future, interventions restoring the connections

between the two systems may improve symptoms in PD patients. One of the possible

interventions would be inhibiting or facilitating cortical areas using non-invasive brain

stimulation techniques such as repetitive TMS or transcranial direct current stimulation.

PPC can be a target for the neuromodulation. However, whether it should be inhibited or

facilitated needs to be further studied because imaging studies have shown conflicting

results with reduced (Lozza et al., 2004; Ma et al., 2007; Moeller et al., 1999; Samuel et

al., 1997) or over-activated (Wu et al., 2015) PPC at rest and over activation during a

motor task (Catalan et al., 1999; Samuel et al., 1997; Tessa et al., 2010).In addition,

further paired-pulse TMS study examining other areas such as premotor cortex or

cerebellum should be performed to prove compensatory mechanisms in PD.

5.8 Conclusions

We conclude that multisensory integration evoked by RHI modulates both early and late

stages of sensorimotor integration pathway. We show that multisensory integration

occurs in PD patients at the behavioural level to the same extent as HC. Nevertheless,

our neurophysiology results revealed alterations in the sensorimotor integration pathway

mediating RHI, suggesting alteration of sensorimotor integration along the basal-

Page 127: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

111

ganglia-thalamo-cortical pathway including S1, cortical association areas and the basal

ganglia in PD.

Page 128: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

112

References

Abbruzzese, G., & Berardelli, A. (2003). Sensorimotor integration in movement disorders. Mov

Disord, 18(3), 231-240.

Abbruzzese, G., Marchese, R., Buccolieri, A., Gasparetto, B., & Trompetto, C. (2001).

Abnormalities of sensorimotor integration in focal dystonia: a transcranial magnetic

stimulation study. Brain, 124(Pt 3), 537-545.

Abbruzzese, G., & Trompetto, C. (2002). Clinical and research methods for evaluating cortical

excitability. J Clin Neurophysiol, 19(4), 307-321.

Adamovich, S. V., Berkinblit, M. B., Hening, W., Sage, J., & Poizner, H. (2001). The interaction

of visual and proprioceptive inputs in pointing to actual and remembered targets in

Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience, 104(4), 1027-1041.

Amedi, A., Malach, R., Hendler, T., Peled, S., & Zohary, E. (2001). Visuo-haptic object-related

activation in the ventral visual pathway. Nat Neurosci, 4(3), 324-330.

Amick, M. M., Schendan, H. E., Ganis, G., & Cronin-Golomb, A. (2006). Frontostriatal circuits

are necessary for visuomotor transformation: mental rotation in Parkinson's disease.

Neuropsychologia, 44(3), 339-349.

Andersen, R. A. (1995). Encoding of intention and spatial location in the posterior parietal

cortex. Cereb Cortex, 5(5), 457-469.

Andersen, R. A. (1997). Multimodal integration for the representation of space in the posterior

parietal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 352(1360), 1421-1428.

Andersen, R. A., & Buneo, C. A. (2002). Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. Annu Rev

Neurosci, 25, 189-220.

Armel, K. C., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2003). Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence

from skin conductance response. Proc Biol Sci, 270(1523), 1499-1506.

Artieda, J., Pastor, M. A., Lacruz, F., & Obeso, J. A. (1992). Temporal discrimination is

abnormal in Parkinson's disease. Brain, 115 Pt 1, 199-210.

Asanuma, K., Tang, C., Ma, Y., Dhawan, V., Mattis, P., Edwards, C., et al. (2006). Network

modulation in the treatment of Parkinson's disease. Brain, 129(Pt 10), 2667-2678.

Aspell, Lenggenhager, & Blanke. (2012). Multisensory Perception and Bodily Self-

Consciousness From OUt-of-Body to Inside -Body Experience. In The Neural Bases

of Multisensory Processes (pp. 467-481). Florida, US: CRC Press.

Aspell, J. E., Lenggenhager, B., & Blanke, O. (2012). Multisensory Perception and Bodily Self-

Consciousness From OUt-of-Body to Inside -Body Experience. In The Neural Bases

of Multisensory Processes (pp. 467-481). Florida, US: CRC Press.

Aspell, J. E., Palluel, E., & Blanke, O. (2012). Early and late activity in somatosensory cortex

reflects changes in bodily self-consciousness: an evoked potential study. Neuroscience,

216, 110-122.

Avanzino, L., & Fiorio, M. (2014). Proprioceptive dysfunction in focal dystonia: from

experimental evidence to rehabilitation strategies. Front Hum Neurosci, 8, 1000.

Barker, A. T., Garnham, C. W., & Freeston, I. L. (1991). Magnetic nerve stimulation: the effect

of waveform on efficiency, determination of neural membrane time constants and the

measurement of stimulator output. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl, 43, 227-

237.

Page 129: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

113

Barnett-Cowan, M., Dyde, R. T., Fox, S. H., Moro, E., Hutchison, W. D., & Harris, L. R. (2010).

Multisensory determinants of orientation perception in Parkinson's disease.

Neuroscience, 167(4), 1138-1150.

Begliomini, C., Wall, M. B., Smith, A. T., & Castiello, U. (2007). Differential cortical activity

for precision and whole-hand visually guided grasping in humans. Eur J Neurosci, 25(4),

1245-1252.

Berlucchi, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). The body in the brain revisited. Exp Brain Res, 200(1),

25-35.

Boecker, H., Ceballos-Baumann, A., Bartenstein, P., Weindl, A., Siebner, H. R., Fassbender, T.,

et al. (1999). Sensory processing in Parkinson's and Huntington's disease: investigations

with 3D H(2)(15)O-PET. Brain, 122 ( Pt 9), 1651-1665.

Bostantjopoulou, S., Katsarou, Z., Zafiriou, D., Gerasimou, G., Alevriadou, A., Georgiadis, G.,

et al. (2000). Abnormality of N30 somatosensory evoked potentials in Parkinson's

disease: a multidisciplinary approach. Neurophysiol Clin, 30(6), 368-376.

Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands 'feel' touch that eyes see. Nature, 391(6669),

756.

Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict

monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature, 402(6758),

179-181.

Bremmer, F., Schlack, A., Shah, N. J., Zafiris, O., Kubischik, M., Hoffmann, K., et al. (2001).

Polymodal motion processing in posterior parietal and premotor cortex: a human fMRI

study strongly implies equivalencies between humans and monkeys. Neuron, 29(1), 287-

296.

Brozzoli, C., Makin, T. R., Cardinali, L., Holmes, N. P., & Farne, A. (2012). Peripersonal Space

A Multisensory Interface for Body-Object Interactions. In M. M. Murray & M. T.

Wallace (Eds.), The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes (pp. 449-465). Florida, US:

CRC Press.

Buelte, D., Meister, I. G., Staedtgen, M., Dambeck, N., Sparing, R., Grefkes, C., et al. (2008).

The role of the anterior intraparietal sulcus in crossmodal processing of object features in

humans: an rTMS study. Brain Res, 1217, 110-118.

Burton, E. J., McKeith, I. G., Burn, D. J., Williams, E. D., & O'Brien, J. T. (2004). Cerebral

atrophy in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia: a comparison with Alzheimer's

disease, dementia with Lewy bodies and controls. Brain, 127(Pt 4), 791-800.

Cao, H., Xu, X., Zhao, Y., Long, D., & Zhang, M. (2011). Altered brain activation and

connectivity in early Parkinson disease tactile perception. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol,

32(10), 1969-1974.

Cappe, Rouiller, & Barone. (2012). Cortical and Thalamic Pathways for Multisensory and

Sensorimotor Interplay. In M. M. Murray & M. T. Walleace (Eds.), The Neural Bases of

Multisensory Processes (pp. 15-30). Florida, US: CRC Press.

Cappe, C., & Barone, P. (2005). Heteromodal connections supporting multisensory integration at

low levels of cortical processing in the monkey. Eur J Neurosci, 22(11), 2886-2902.

Cappe, C., Rouiller, E. M., & Barone, P. (2012). Cortical and Thalamic Pathways for

Multisensory and Sensorimotor Interplay. In M. M. Murray & M. T. Walleace (Eds.), The

Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes (pp. 15-30). Florida, US: CRC Press.

Page 130: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

114

Cash, R. F., Isayama, R., Gunraj, C. A., Ni, Z., & Chen, R. (2015). The influence of sensory

afferent input on local motor cortical excitatory circuitry in humans. J Physiol, 593(7),

1667-1684.

Catalan, M. J., Ishii, K., Honda, M., Samii, A., & Hallett, M. (1999). A PET study of sequential

finger movements of varying length in patients with Parkinson's disease. Brain, 122 ( Pt

3), 483-495.

Chen, R., Corwell, B., & Hallett, M. (1999). Modulation of motor cortex excitability by median

nerve and digit stimulation. Exp Brain Res, 129(1), 77-86.

Christian, B. T., Vandehey, N. T., Fox, A. S., Murali, D., Oakes, T. R., Converse, A. K., et al.

(2009). The distribution of D2/D3 receptor binding in the adolescent rhesus monkey

using small animal PET imaging. Neuroimage, 44(4), 1334-1344.

Conte, A., Khan, N., Defazio, G., Rothwell, J. C., & Berardelli, A. (2013). Pathophysiology of

somatosensory abnormalities in Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Neurol, 9(12), 687-697.

Conte, A., Modugno, N., Lena, F., Dispenza, S., Gandolfi, B., Iezzi, E., et al. (2010).

Subthalamic nucleus stimulation and somatosensory temporal discrimination in

Parkinson's disease. Brain, 133(9), 2656-2663.

Crammond, D. J., & Kalaska, J. F. (1989). Neuronal activity in primate parietal cortex area 5

varies with intended movement direction during an instructed-delay period. Exp Brain

Res, 76(2), 458-462.

Culham, J. C., & Valyear, K. F. (2006). Human parietal cortex in action. Curr Opin Neurobiol,

16(2), 205-212.

Davare, M., Montague, K., Olivier, E., Rothwell, J. C., & Lemon, R. N. (2009). Ventral

premotor to primary motor cortical interactions during object-driven grasp in humans.

Cortex, 45(9), 1050-1057.

Della Gatta, F., Garbarini, F., Puglisi, G., Leonetti, A., Berti, A., & Borroni, P. (2016).

Decreased motor cortex excitability mirrors own hand disembodiment during the rubber

hand illusion. Elife, 5.

Delnooz, C. C., Helmich, R. C., Toni, I., & van de Warrenburg, B. P. (2012). Reduced parietal

connectivity with a premotor writing area in writer's cramp. Mov Disord, 27(11), 1425-

1431.

Demirci, M., Grill, S., McShane, L., & Hallett, M. (1997). A mismatch between kinesthetic and

visual perception in Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol, 41(6), 781-788.

Dempsey-Jones, H., & Kritikos, A. (2014). Higher-order cognitive factors affect subjective but

not proprioceptive aspects of self-representation in the rubber hand illusion. Conscious

Cogn, 26, 74-89.

Devanne, H., Degardin, A., Tyvaert, L., Bocquillon, P., Houdayer, E., Manceaux, A., et al.

(2009). Afferent-induced facilitation of primary motor cortex excitability in the region

controlling hand muscles in humans. Eur J Neurosci, 30(3), 439-448.

Dominey, P., Decety, J., Broussolle, E., Chazot, G., & Jeannerod, M. (1995). Motor imagery of a

lateralized sequential task is asymmetrically slowed in hemi-Parkinson's patients.

Neuropsychologia, 33(6), 727-741.

Doubell, T. P., Skaliora, I., Baron, J., & King, A. J. (2003). Functional connectivity between the

superficial and deeper layers of the superior colliculus: an anatomical substrate for

sensorimotor integration. J Neurosci, 23(16), 6596-6607.

Page 131: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

115

Ehrsson, H. H., Holmes, N. P., & Passingham, R. E. (2005). Touching a rubber hand: feeling of

body ownership is associated with activity in multisensory brain areas. J Neurosci,

25(45), 10564-10573.

Ehrsson, H. H., Spence, C., & Passingham, R. E. (2004). That's my hand! Activity in premotor

cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb. Science, 305(5685), 875-877.

Eidelberg, D. (2009). Metabolic brain networks in neurodegenerative disorders: a functional

imaging approach. Trends Neurosci, 32(10), 548-557.

Ernst, M. O., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2004). Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends Cogn

Sci, 8(4), 162-169.

Fahn, S., Jankovic, J., & Hallett, M. (2011). Hypokinetic disorders. In S. Fahn, J. Jankovic & M.

Hallett (Eds.), Principles and Practice of Movement Disorders (2nd ed., pp. 66-196).

Philadelphia, USA: Elsevier Saunders.

Fahn, S., Przedborski, Serge. (2009). Parkinson Disease. In L. P. Rowland, Pedley, Timothy A

(Ed.), Merritt:s Neurology (12th Edition ed., pp. 751-769). Philadelphia, PA USA:

Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Fearon, C., Butler, J. S., Newman, L., Lynch, T., & Reilly, R. B. (2015). Audiovisual Processing

is Abnormal in Parkinson's Disease and Correlates with Freezing of Gait and Disease

Duration. J Parkinsons Dis, 5(4), 925-936.

Feigin, A., Fukuda, M., Dhawan, V., Przedborski, S., Jackson-Lewis, V., Mentis, M. J., et al.

(2001). Metabolic correlates of levodopa response in Parkinson's disease. Neurology,

57(11), 2083-2088.

Fiorio, M., Stanzani, C., Rothwell, J. C., Bhatia, K. P., Moretto, G., Fiaschi, A., et al. (2007).

Defective temporal discrimination of passive movements in Parkinson's disease. Neurosci

Lett, 417(3), 312-315.

Fiorio, M., Weise, D., Onal-Hartmann, C., Zeller, D., Tinazzi, M., & Classen, J. (2011).

Impairment of the rubber hand illusion in focal hand dystonia. In Brain (Vol. 134, pp.

1428-1437). England.

Flash, T., Inzelberg, R., Schechtman, E., & Korczyn, A. D. (1992). Kinematic analysis of upper

limb trajectories in Parkinson's disease. Exp Neurol, 118(2), 215-226.

Foki, T., Pirker, W., Geißler, A., Haubenberger, D., Hilbert, M., Hoellinger, I., et al. (2015).

Finger dexterity deficits in Parkinson's disease and somatosensory cortical dysfunction.

Parkinsonism Relat Disord, 21(3), 259-265.

Folegatti, A., de Vignemont, F., Pavani, F., Rossetti, Y., & Farnè, A. (2009). Losing one's hand:

visual-proprioceptive conflict affects touch perception. PLoS One, 4(9), e6920.

Franklin, D. W., & Wolpert, D. M. (2011). Computational Mechanisms of Sensorimotor Control.

Neuron, 72(3), 425-442.

Fuhr, P., Zeffiro, T., & Hallett, M. (1992). Cutaneous reflexes in Parkinson's disease. Muscle

Nerve, 15(6), 733-739.

Gallagher, S. (1986). Body Image and Body Schema: A Conceptural Clarification (Vol. 7, pp.

541-554). The Journal of Mind and Behavior: The Institute of Mind and Behavior, Inc.

Gattellaro, G., Minati, L., Grisoli, M., Mariani, C., Carella, F., Osio, M., et al. (2009). White

matter involvement in idiopathic Parkinson disease: a diffusion tensor imaging study.

AJNR Am J Neuroradiol, 30(6), 1222-1226.

Gentile, G., Petkova, V. I., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2011). Integration of visual and tactile signals

from the hand in the human brain: an FMRI study. J Neurophysiol, 105(2), 910-922.

Page 132: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

116

Giummarra, M. J., Gibson, S. J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2008).

Mechanisms underlying embodiment, disembodiment and loss of embodiment. In

Neurosci Biobehav Rev (Vol. 32, pp. 143-160). United States.

Granek, J. A., Pisella, L., Blangero, A., Rossetti, Y., & Sergio, L. E. (2012). The role of the

caudal superior parietal lobule in updating hand location in peripheral vision: further

evidence from optic ataxia. PLoS One, 7(10), e46619.

Graziano, M. S. (2001). A system of multimodal areas in the primate brain. Neuron, 29(1), 4-6.

Graziano, M. S., & Gross, C. G. (1998). Spatial maps for the control of movement. Curr Opin

Neurobiol, 8(2), 195-201.

Hall, W. c., White, L.E. (2008). Modulation of Movement by the Basal Ganglia. In D. Purves,

Augustine, G.J., Fitzpatrick, D., Hall, W.c., LaMantia, A. S., McNamara, J.O., White L.

E. (Ed.), Neuroscience (Fourth Edition ed., pp. 453-474). Massachusetts, U.S.A.: Sinauer

Associates, Inc.

Hallett, M. (2007). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: a primer. Neuron, 55(2), 187-199.

Harris, C. M., & Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning.

Nature, 394(6695), 780-784.

Hawkins, K. M., Sayegh, P., Yan, X., Crawford, J. D., & Sergio, L. E. (2013). Neural activity in

superior parietal cortex during rule-based visual-motor transformations. J Cogn Neurosci,

25(3), 436-454.

Heed, T., Grundler, M., Rinkleib, J., Rudzik, F. H., Collins, T., Cooke, E., et al. (2011). Visual

information and rubber hand embodiment differentially affect reach-to-grasp actions. In

Acta Psychol (Amst) (Vol. 138, pp. 263-271). Netherlands: 2011 Elsevier B.V.

Heed, T., Roder, Brigitte. (2012). The Body in a Multisensory World. In M. M. Murray, Wallace, Mark T.,

(Ed.), The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes (pp. 557-580). Boca Raton (FL): CPC

Press.

Helmich, R. C., de Lange, F. P., Bloem, B. R., & Toni, I. (2007). Cerebral compensation during

motor imagery in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2201-2215.

Hershenson, M. (1962). Reaction time as a measure of intersensory facilitation. J Exp Psychol,

63, 289-293.

Hirashima, F., & Yokota, T. (1997). Influence of peripheral nerve stimulation on human motor

cortical excitability in patients with ventrolateral thalamic lesion. Arch Neurol, 54(5),

619-624.

Holmes, N. P., & Spence, C. (2004). The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of

peripersonal space. Cogn Process, 5(2), 94-105.

Ingvarsson, P. E., Gordon, A. M., & Forssberg, H. (1997). Coordination of manipulative forces

in Parkinson's disease. Exp Neurol, 145(2 Pt 1), 489-501.

Iwamura, Y. (1998). Hierarchical somatosensory processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 8(4), 522-

528.

Jobst, E. E., Melnick, M. E., Byl, N. N., Dowling, G. A., & Aminoff, M. J. (1997). Sensory

perception in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol, 54(4), 450-454.

Kalaska, J. F. (1996). Parietal cortex area 5 and visuomotor behavior. Can J Physiol Pharmacol,

74(4), 483-498.

Kalaska, J. F., Caminiti, R., & Georgopoulos, A. P. (1983). Cortical mechanisms related to the

direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison

with motor cortex. Exp Brain Res, 51(2), 247-260.

Page 133: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

117

Kammers, M. P., de Vignemont, F., Verhagen, L., & Dijkerman, H. C. (2009). The rubber hand

illusion in action. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 204-211.

Kammers, M. P., van der Ham, I. J., & Dijkerman, H. C. (2006). Dissociating body

representations in healthy individuals: differential effects of a kinaesthetic illusion on

perception and action. In Neuropsychologia (Vol. 44, pp. 2430-2436). England.

Kammers, M. P., Verhagen, L., Dijkerman, H. C., Hogendoorn, H., De Vignemont, F., &

Schutter, D. J. (2009). Is this hand for real? Attenuation of the rubber hand illusion by

transcranial magnetic stimulation over the inferior parietal lobule. J Cogn Neurosci,

21(7), 1311-1320.

Kanayama, N., Sato, A., & Ohira, H. (2007). Crossmodal effect with rubber hand illusion and

gamma-band activity. In Psychophysiology (Vol. 44, pp. 392-402). United States.

Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., Jessell, T. M., Siegelbaum, S. A., & Hudspeth, A. J. (2013).

Principles of Neural Science 5th Edition, Part VI/ Movement (5th Edition ed., pp. 744-

747): McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc.

Karabanov, A. N., Christensen, M. S., Ritterband-Rosenbaum, A., Siebner, H., & Nielsen, J. B.

(2014). TMS as a tool to investigate frontoparietal connectivity during the rubber hand

illusion. Paper presented at the Neuroscience 2014.

Kayser, C., & Logothetis, N. K. (2007). Do early sensory cortices integrate cross-modal

information? Brain Struct Funct, 212(2), 121-132.

Kessels, R. P., d'Alfonso, A. A., Postma, A., & de Haan, E. H. (2000). Spatial working memory

performance after high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the left

and right posterior parietal cortex in humans. Neurosci Lett, 287(1), 68-70.

Kessler, K. R., Ruge, D., Ilić, T. V., & Ziemann, U. (2005). Short latency afferent inhibition and

facilitation in patients with writer's cramp. Mov Disord, 20(2), 238-242.

Kilteni, K., Maselli, A., Kording, K. P., & Slater, M. (2015). Over my fake body: body

ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception. Front

Hum Neurosci, 9, 141.

Ko, J. H., Lerner, R. P., & Eidelberg, D. (2015). Effects of levodopa on regional cerebral

metabolism and blood flow. Mov Disord, 30(1), 54-63.

Koch, G., Fernandez Del Olmo, M., Cheeran, B., Ruge, D., Schippling, S., Caltagirone, C., et al.

(2007). Focal stimulation of the posterior parietal cortex increases the excitability of the

ipsilateral motor cortex. In J Neurosci (Vol. 27, pp. 6815-6822). United States.

Koch, G., Fernandez Del Olmo, M., Cheeran, B., Schippling, S., Caltagirone, C., Driver, J., et al.

(2008). Functional interplay between posterior parietal and ipsilateral motor cortex

revealed by twin-coil transcranial magnetic stimulation during reach planning toward

contralateral space. J Neurosci, 28(23), 5944-5953.

Koch, G., Ribolsi, M., Mori, F., Sacchetti, L., Codecà, C., Rubino, I. A., et al. (2008).

Connectivity between posterior parietal cortex and ipsilateral motor cortex is altered in

schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry, 64(9), 815-819.

Koch, G., & Rothwell, J. C. (2009). TMS investigations into the task-dependent functional

interplay between human posterior parietal and motor cortex. Behav Brain Res, 202(2),

147-152.

Koessler, L., Maillard, L., Benhadid, A., Vignal, J. P., Felblinger, J., Vespignani, H., et al.

(2009). Automated cortical projection of EEG sensors: anatomical correlation via the

international 10-10 system. Neuroimage, 46(1), 64-72.

Page 134: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

118

Konczak, J., Corcos, D. M., Horak, F., Poizner, H., Shapiro, M., Tuite, P., et al. (2009).

Proprioception and motor control in Parkinson's disease. J Mot Behav, 41(6), 543-552.

Konczak, J., Krawczewski, K., Tuite, P., & Maschke, M. (2007). The perception of passive

motion in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol, 254(5), 655-663.

Konczak, J., Li, K. Y., Tuite, P. J., & Poizner, H. (2008). Haptic perception of object curvature

in Parkinson's disease. PLoS One, 3(7), e2625.

Konczak, J., Sciutti, A., Avanzino, L., Squeri, V., Gori, M., Masia, L., et al. (2012). Parkinson's

disease accelerates age-related decline in haptic perception by altering somatosensory

integration. Brain, 135(Pt 11), 3371-3379.

Lacey, S., & Sathian, K. (2012). Representation of Object Form in Vision and Touch. In M. M.

Murray & M. T. Wallace (Eds.), The Neural Bases of Multi Sensory Processes (pp. 179-

189). Florida, US: CRC Press.

Lapole, T., & Tindel, J. (2015). Acute effects of muscle vibration on sensorimotor integration.

Neurosci Lett, 587, 46-50.

Lee, M. S., Kim, H. S., & Lyoo, C. H. (2005). "Off" gait freezing and temporal discrimination

threshold in patients with Parkinson disease. Neurology, 64(4), 670-674.

Lewis, J. W., Frum, C., Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Talkington, W. J., Walker, N. A., Rapuano, K.

M., et al. (2011). Cortical network differences in the sighted versus early blind for

recognition of human-produced action sounds. Hum Brain Mapp, 32(12), 2241-2255.

Li, K. Y., Pickett, K., Nestrasil, I., Tuite, P., & Konczak, J. (2010). The effect of dopamine

replacement therapy on haptic sensitivity in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol, 257(12), 1992-

1998.

Limanowski, J., & Blankenburg, F. (2015). Network activity underlying the illusory self-

attribution of a dummy arm. Hum Brain Mapp, 36(6), 2284-2304.

Longo, M. R., Schüür, F., Kammers, M. P., Tsakiris, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). What is

embodiment? A psychometric approach. Cognition, 107(3), 978-998.

Lozza, C., Baron, J. C., Eidelberg, D., Mentis, M. J., Carbon, M., & Marié, R. M. (2004).

Executive processes in Parkinson's disease: FDG-PET and network analysis. Hum Brain

Mapp, 22(3), 236-245.

Ma, Y., Tang, C., Spetsieris, P. G., Dhawan, V., & Eidelberg, D. (2007). Abnormal metabolic

network activity in Parkinson's disease: test-retest reproducibility. J Cereb Blood Flow

Metab, 27(3), 597-605.

Mahoney, J. R., Wang, C., Dumas, K., & Holtzer, R. (2014). Visual-somatosensory integration

in aging: does stimulus location really matter? Vis Neurosci, 31(3), 275-283.

Makin, T. R., Holmes, N. P., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2008). On the other hand: dummy hands and

peripersonal space. In Behav Brain Res (Vol. 191, pp. 1-10). Netherlands.

Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., McInerney, S., Sorensen, A. G., Wang, R., Caviness, V. S., et al.

(2005). Segmentation of subcomponents within the superior longitudinal fascicle in

humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study. Cereb Cortex, 15(6), 854-869.

Marsden, C. D., & Obeso, J. A. (1994). The functions of the basal ganglia and the paradox of

stereotaxic surgery in Parkinson's disease. Brain, 117 ( Pt 4), 877-897.

Martens, K. A., & Almeida, Q. J. (2012). Dissociating between sensory and perceptual deficits in

PD: more than simply a motor deficit. Mov Disord, 27(3), 387-392.

Maschke, M., Gomez, C. M., Tuite, P. J., & Konczak, J. (2003). Dysfunction of the basal

ganglia, but not the cerebellum, impairs kinaesthesia. Brain, 126(Pt 10), 2312-2322.

Page 135: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

119

Maschke, M., Tuite, P. J., Pickett, K., Wächter, T., & Konczak, J. (2005). The effect of

subthalamic nucleus stimulation on kinaesthesia in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol

Neurosurg Psychiatry, 76(4), 569-571.

McFarland, N. R., & Haber, S. N. (2000). Convergent inputs from thalamic motor nuclei and

frontal cortical areas to the dorsal striatum in the primate. J Neurosci, 20(10), 3798-3813.

McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264(5588), 746-

748.

Michely, J., Volz, L. J., Barbe, M. T., Hoffstaedter, F., Viswanathan, S., Timmermann, L., et al.

(2015). Dopaminergic modulation of motor network dynamics in Parkinson's disease.

Brain, 138(Pt 3), 664-678.

Missale, C., Nash, S. R., Robinson, S. W., Jaber, M., & Caron, M. G. (1998). Dopamine

receptors: from structure to function. Physiol Rev, 78(1), 189-225.

Moeller, J. R., Nakamura, T., Mentis, M. J., Dhawan, V., Spetsieres, P., Antonini, A., et al.

(1999). Reproducibility of regional metabolic covariance patterns: comparison of four

populations. J Nucl Med, 40(8), 1264-1269.

Moseley, G. L., Olthof, N., Venema, A., Don, S., Wijers, M., Gallace, A., et al. (2008).

Psychologically induced cooling of a specific body part caused by the illusory ownership

of an artificial counterpart. In Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (Vol. 105, pp. 13169-13173).

United States.

Moseley, G. L., Olthof, N., Venema, A., Don, S., Wijers, M., Gallace, A., et al. (2008).

Psychologically induced cooling of a specific body part caused by the illusory ownership

of an artificial counterpart. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the

United States of America, 105(35), 13169-13173.

Ng, B., Palmer, S., Abugharbieh, R., & McKeown, M. J. (2010). Focusing effects of L-dopa in

Parkinson's disease. Hum Brain Mapp, 31(1), 88-97.

Ni, Z., Charab, S., Gunraj, C., Nelson, A. J., Udupa, K., Yeh, I. J., et al. (2011). Transcranial

magnetic stimulation in different current directions activates separate cortical circuits. J

Neurophysiol, 105(2), 749-756.

O'Suilleabhain, P., Bullard, J., & Dewey, R. B. (2001). Proprioception in Parkinson's disease is

acutely depressed by dopaminergic medications. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 71(5),

607-610.

Orth, M., & Rothwell, J. C. (2009). Motor cortex excitability and comorbidity in Gilles de la

Tourette syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 80(1), 29-34.

Palomar, F. J., Conde, V., Carrillo, F., Fernández-del-Olmo, M., Koch, G., & Mir, P. (2013).

Parieto-motor functional connectivity is impaired in Parkinson's disease. Brain Stimul,

6(2), 147-154.

Palomar, F. J., Díaz-Corrales, F., Carrillo, F., Fernández-del-Olmo, M., Koch, G., & Mir, P.

(2011). Sensory perception changes induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation over

the primary somatosensory cortex in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord, 26(11), 2058-

2064.

Pavani, F., Spence, C., & Driver, J. (2000). Visual capture of touch: out-of-the-body experiences

with rubber gloves. Psychol Sci, 11(5), 353-359.

Peled, A., Pressman, A., Geva, A. B., & Modai, I. (2003). Somatosensory evoked potentials

during a rubber-hand illusion in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res, 64(2-3), 157-163.

Page 136: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

120

Pellijeff, A., Bonilha, L., Morgan, P. S., McKenzie, K., & Jackson, S. R. (2006). Parietal

updating of limb posture: an event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 44(13), 2685-

2690.

Petrides, M., & Pandya, D. N. (1984). Projections to the frontal cortex from the posterior parietal

region in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol, 228(1), 105-116.

Pfurtscheller, G., Stancák, A., & Neuper, C. (1996). Post-movement beta synchronization. A

correlate of an idling motor area? Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 98(4), 281-293.

Pierantozzi, M., Mazzone, P., Bassi, A., Rossini, P. M., Peppe, A., Altibrandi, M. G., et al.

(1999). The effect of deep brain stimulation on the frontal N30 component of

somatosensory evoked potentials in advanced Parkinson's disease patients. Clin

Neurophysiol, 110(10), 1700-1707.

Pirio Richardson, S., Beck, S., Bliem, B., & Hallett, M. (2014). Abnormal dorsal premotor-motor

inhibition in writer's cramp. Mov Disord, 29(6), 797-803.

Pirio Richardson, S., Bliem, B., Voller, B., Dang, N., & Hallett, M. (2009). Long-latency

afferent inhibition during phasic finger movement in focal hand dystonia. Exp Brain Res,

193(2), 173-179.

Premji, A., Rai, N., & Nelson, A. (2011). Area 5 influences excitability within the primary motor

cortex in humans. In PLoS One (Vol. 6, pp. e20023). United States.

Preston, C., & Newport, R. (2011). Evidence for dissociable representations for body image and

body schema from a patient with visual neglect. Neurocase, 17(6), 473-479.

Proville, R. D., Spolidoro, M., Guyon, N., Dugué, G. P., Selimi, F., Isope, P., et al. (2014).

Cerebellum involvement in cortical sensorimotor circuits for the control of voluntary

movements. Nat Neurosci, 17(9), 1233-1239.

Quinlan, D. J., & Culham, J. C. (2007). fMRI reveals a preference for near viewing in the human

parieto-occipital cortex. Neuroimage, 36(1), 167-187.

Rani, M., & Kanungo, M. S. (2006). Expression of D2 dopamine receptor in the mouse brain.

Biochem Biophys Res Commun, 344(3), 981-986.

Reed, C. L., Klatzky, R. L., & Halgren, E. (2005). What vs. where in touch: an fMRI study.

Neuroimage, 25(3), 718-726.

Reig, R., & Silberberg, G. (2014). Multisensory integration in the mouse striatum. Neuron,

83(5), 1200-1212.

Richardson, S. P., Bliem, B., Lomarev, M., Shamim, E., Dang, N., & Hallett, M. (2008).

Changes in short afferent inhibition during phasic movement in focal dystonia. Muscle

Nerve, 37(3), 358-363.

Rickards, C., & Cody, F. W. (1997). Proprioceptive control of wrist movements in Parkinson's

disease. Reduced muscle vibration-induced errors. Brain, 120 ( Pt 6), 977-990.

Rizzo, V., Aricò, I., Liotta, G., Ricciardi, L., Mastroeni, C., Morgante, F., et al. (2010).

Impairment of sensory-motor integration in patients affected by RLS. J Neurol, 257(12),

1979-1985.

Rizzolatti, G., Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., & Rozzi, S. (2014). Cortical mechanisms

underlying the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action

understanding. Physiol Rev, 94(2), 655-706.

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1997). The space around us. Science,

277(5323), 190-191.

Page 137: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

121

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1997). Parietal cortex: from sight to action. Curr Opin

Neurobiol, 7(4), 562-567.

Ro, T., Wallace, R., Hagedorn, J., Farnè, A., & Pienkos, E. (2004). Visual enhancing of tactile

perception in the posterior parietal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci, 16(1), 24-30.

Rocchi, L., Conte, A., Nardella, A., Li Voti, P., Di Biasio, F., Leodori, G., et al. (2013).

Somatosensory temporal discrimination threshold may help to differentiate patients with

multiple system atrophy from patients with Parkinson's disease. Eur J Neurol, 20(4), 714-

719.

Rocco-Donovan, M., Ramos, R. L., Giraldo, S., & Brumberg, J. C. (2011). Characteristics of

synaptic connections between rodent primary somatosensory and motor cortices.

Somatosens Mot Res, 28(3-4), 63-72.

Rosler, K. M., & Magistris, M. R. (2011). The size of motor-evoked potentials:influencing

parameters and qualification. In E. M. E. Wassermann, Charles M., U. Ziemann, V.

Walsh, T. Paus & S. H. Lisanby (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of Transcranial

Stimulation (pp. 78-89). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Rossini, P. M., Burke, D., Chen, R., Cohen, L. G., Daskalakis, Z., Di Iorio, R., et al. (2015).

Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and

peripheral nerves: Basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research

application. An updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clin Neurophysiol, 126(6),

1071-1107.

Rossini, P. M., Traversa, R., Boccasena, P., Martino, G., Passarelli, F., Pacifici, L., et al. (1993).

Parkinson's disease and somatosensory evoked potentials: apomorphine-induced transient

potentiation of frontal components. Neurology, 43(12), 2495-2500.

Rothwell, J. C. (1997). Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation of the

human motor cortex. J Neurosci Methods, 74(2), 113-122.

Rothwell, J. C. (2011). Using transcranial magnetic stimulation methods to probe connectivity

between motor areas of the brain. Hum Mov Sci, 30(5), 906-915.

Sabatini, U., Boulanouar, K., Fabre, N., Martin, F., Carel, C., Colonnese, C., et al. (2000).

Cortical motor reorganization in akinetic patients with Parkinson's disease: a functional

MRI study. Brain, 123 ( Pt 2), 394-403.

Sailer, A., Cunic, D. I., Paradiso, G. O., Gunraj, C. A., Wagle-Shukla, A., Moro, E., et al. (2007).

Subthalamic nucleus stimulation modulates afferent inhibition in Parkinson disease.

Neurology, 68(5), 356-363.

Sailer, A., Molnar, G. F., Paradiso, G., Gunraj, C. A., Lang, A. E., & Chen, R. (2003). Short and

long latency afferent inhibition in Parkinson's disease. Brain, 126(Pt 8), 1883-1894.

Sakata, H., Takaoka, Y., Kawarasaki, A., & Shibutani, H. (1973). Somatosensory properties of

neurons in the superior parietal cortex (area 5) of the rhesus monkey. Brain Res, 64, 85-

102.

Salenius, S., Schnitzler, A., Salmelin, R., Jousmäki, V., & Hari, R. (1997). Modulation of human

cortical rolandic rhythms during natural sensorimotor tasks. Neuroimage, 5(3), 221-228.

Salmelin, R., & Hari, R. (1994). Spatiotemporal characteristics of sensorimotor neuromagnetic

rhythms related to thumb movement. Neuroscience, 60(2), 537-550.

Samuel, M., Ceballos-Baumann, A. O., Blin, J., Uema, T., Boecker, H., Passingham, R. E., et al.

(1997). Evidence for lateral premotor and parietal overactivity in Parkinson's disease

during sequential and bimanual movements. A PET study. Brain, 120 ( Pt 6), 963-976.

Page 138: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

122

Sathian, K., Zangaladze, A., Green, J., Vitek, J. L., & DeLong, M. R. (1997). Tactile spatial

acuity and roughness discrimination: impairments due to aging and Parkinson's disease.

Neurology, 49(1), 168-177.

Schutz-Bosbach, S., Avenanti, A., Aglioti, S. M., & Haggard, P. (2009). Don't do it! Cortical

inhibition and self-attribution during action observation. J Cogn Neurosci, 21(6), 1215-

1227.

Schwoebel, J., & Coslett, H. B. (2005). Evidence for multiple, distinct representations of the

human body. J Cogn Neurosci, 17(4), 543-553.

Seiss, E., Praamstra, P., Hesse, C. W., & Rickards, H. (2003). Proprioceptive sensory function in

Parkinson's disease and Huntington's disease: evidence from proprioception-related EEG

potentials. Exp Brain Res, 148(3), 308-319.

Serino, A., Alsmith, A., Costantini, M., Mandrigin, A., Tajadura-Jimenez, A., & Lopez, C.

(2013). Bodily ownership and self-location: components of bodily self-consciousness.

Conscious Cogn, 22(4), 1239-1252.

Shergill, S. S., Samson, G., Bays, P. M., Frith, C. D., & Wolpert, D. M. (2005). Evidence for

sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry, 162(12), 2384-2386.

Shin, H. W., Kang, S. Y., & Sohn, Y. H. (2005). Dopaminergic influence on disturbed spatial

discrimination in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord, 20(12), 1640-1643.

Shulman, L. M., Taback, R. L., Bean, J., & Weiner, W. J. (2001). Comorbidity of the nonmotor

symptoms of Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord, 16(3), 507-510.

Sober, S. J., & Sabes, P. N. (2005). Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor

planning. Nat Neurosci, 8(4), 490-497.

Stone, K. D., & Gonzalez, C. L. (2015). The contributions of vision and haptics to reaching and

grasping. Front Psychol, 6, 1403.

Suppa, A., Bologna, M., Conte, A., Berardelli, A., & Fabbrini, G. (2016). The effect of L-dopa in

Parkinson's disease as revealed by neurophysiological studies of motor and sensory

functions. Expert Rev Neurother, 1-12.

Tamburin, S., Fiaschi, A., Idone, D., Lochner, P., Manganotti, P., & Zanette, G. (2003).

Abnormal sensorimotor integration is related to disease severity in Parkinson's disease: a

TMS study. Mov Disord, 18(11), 1316-1324.

Tessa, C., Lucetti, C., Diciotti, S., Baldacci, F., Paoli, L., Cecchi, P., et al. (2010). Decreased and

increased cortical activation coexist in de novo Parkinson's disease. Exp Neurol, 224(1),

299-306.

Tessitore, A., Amboni, M., Esposito, F., Russo, A., Picillo, M., Marcuccio, L., et al. (2012).

Resting-state brain connectivity in patients with Parkinson's disease and freezing of gait.

Parkinsonism Relat Disord, 18(6), 781-787.

Thakkar, K. N., Nichols, H. S., McIntosh, L. G., & Park, S. (2011). Disturbances in body

ownership in schizophrenia: evidence from the rubber hand illusion and case study of a

spontaneous out-of-body experience. PLoS One, 6(10), e27089.

Tokimura, H., Di Lazzaro, V., Tokimura, Y., Oliviero, A., Profice, P., Insola, A., et al. (2000).

Short latency inhibition of human hand motor cortex by somatosensory input from the

hand. J Physiol, 523 Pt 2, 503-513.

Tsakiris, M. (2010). My body in the brain: a neurocognitive model of body-ownership.

Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 703-712.

Page 139: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

123

Tsakiris, M., Costantini, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). The role of the right temporo-parietal

junction in maintaining a coherent sense of one's body. Neuropsychologia, 46(12), 3014-

3018.

Tsakiris, M., & Haggard, P. (2005). The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration

and self-attribution. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 31(1), 80-91.

Tunik, E., Frey, S. H., & Grafton, S. T. (2005). Virtual lesions of the anterior intraparietal area

disrupt goal-dependent on-line adjustments of grasp. Nat Neurosci, 8(4), 505-511.

Turner, R. S., Grafton, S. T., McIntosh, A. R., DeLong, M. R., & Hoffman, J. M. (2003). The

functional anatomy of parkinsonian bradykinesia. Neuroimage, 19(1), 163-179.

Vallar, G., & Ronchi, R. (2009). Somatoparaphrenia: a body delusion. A review of the

neuropsychological literature. Exp Brain Res, 192(3), 533-551.

Velasques, B., Machado, S., Paes, F., Cunha, M., Sanfim, A., Budde, H., et al. (2011).

Sensorimotor integration and psychopathology: motor control abnormalities related to

psychiatric disorders. World J Biol Psychiatry, 12(8), 560-573.

Vesia, M., Bolton, D. A., Mochizuki, G., & Staines, W. R. (2013). Human parietal and primary

motor cortical interactions are selectively modulated during the transport and grip

formation of goal-directed hand actions. Neuropsychologia, 51(3), 410-417.

Vesia, M., & Chen, R. (2014). Focal hand dystonia: using brain stimulation to probe network

interactions and brain plasticity. Mov Disord, 29(10), 1227-1229.

Vesia, M., & Crawford, J. D. (2012). Specialization of reach function in human posterior parietal

cortex. Exp Brain Res, 221(1), 1-18.

Vesia, M., Prime, S. L., Yan, X., Sergio, L. E., & Crawford, J. D. (2010). Specificity of human

parietal saccade and reach regions during transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurosci,

30(39), 13053-13065.

Wagle Shukla, A., Moro, E., Gunraj, C., Lozano, A., Hodaie, M., Lang, A., et al. (2013). Long-

term subthalamic nucleus stimulation improves sensorimotor integration and

proprioception. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 84(9), 1020-1028.

Wassermann, E. M., Epstein, C. M., Ziemann, U., Walsh, V., Paus, T., & Lisanby, S. H. (2011).

Physics and Biophysics of TMS. In C. M. Epstein (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of

Transcranial Stimulation (pp. 3-46). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Weder, B., Azari, N. P., Knorr, U., Seitz, R. J., Keel, A., Nienhusmeier, M., et al. (2000).

Disturbed functional brain interactions underlying deficient tactile object discrimination

in Parkinson's disease. Hum Brain Mapp, 11(3), 131-145.

Weder, B. J., Leenders, K. L., Vontobel, P., Nienhusmeier, M., Keel, A., Zaunbauer, W., et al.

(1999). Impaired somatosensory discrimination of shape in Parkinson's disease:

association with caudate nucleus dopaminergic function. Hum Brain Mapp, 8(1), 1-12.

Wichmann, T., & DeLong, M. R. (2013). The basal ganglia. In E. R. Kandel, J. H. Schwartz, T.

M. Jessell, S. A. Soegelbaum & A. J. Hudspeth (Eds.), Principals of Neural Science (5th

Edition ed., pp. 982-998). the United States of America: The McGraw-Hill Companies,

Inc.

Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor

integration. Science, 269(5232), 1880-1882.

Wolpert, D. M., Goodbody, S. J., & Husain, M. (1998). Maintaining internal representations: the

role of the human superior parietal lobe. Nat Neurosci, 1(6), 529-533.

Page 140: Sensorimotor Integration during Rubber Hand …...would like to thank Kaviraj Udupa and Behzad Elahi for their help during the data collection, Tasnuva Hoque and Puja Bhakta for helping

124

Wood, R., Gallese, V., & Cattaneo, L. (2010). Visuotactile empathy within the primary

somatosensory cortex revealed by short-latency afferent inhibition. In Neurosci Lett (Vol.

473, pp. 28-31). Ireland: 2010 Elsevier Ireland Ltd.

Wu, T., & Hallett, M. (2005). A functional MRI study of automatic movements in patients with

Parkinson's disease. Brain, 128(Pt 10), 2250-2259.

Wu, T., Long, X., Wang, L., Hallett, M., Zang, Y., Li, K., et al. (2011). Functional connectivity

of cortical motor areas in the resting state in Parkinson's disease. Hum Brain Mapp,

32(9), 1443-1457.

Wu, T., Ma, Y., Zheng, Z., Peng, S., Wu, X., Eidelberg, D., et al. (2015). Parkinson's disease-

related spatial covariance pattern identified with resting-state functional MRI. J Cereb

Blood Flow Metab, 35(11), 1764-1770.

Zeller, D., Gross, C., Bartsch, A., Johansen-Berg, H., & Classen, J. (2011). Ventral premotor

cortex may be required for dynamic changes in the feeling of limb ownership: a lesion

study. J Neurosci, 31(13), 4852-4857.

Zeller, D., Litvak, V., Friston, K. J., & Classen, J. (2015). Sensory processing and the rubber

hand illusion--an evoked potentials study. J Cogn Neurosci, 27(3), 573-582.

Zhao, Y., Zheng, X., Wang, Q., Xu, J., Xu, X., & Zhang, M. (2014). Altered activation in visual

cortex: unusual functional magnetic resonance imaging finding in early Parkinson's

disease. J Int Med Res, 42(2), 503-515.

Zia, S., Cody, F., & O'Boyle, D. (2000). Joint position sense is impaired by Parkinson's disease.

Ann Neurol, 47(2), 218-228.

Zia, S., Cody, F. W., & O'Boyle, D. J. (2002). Identification of unilateral elbow-joint position is

impaired by Parkinson's disease. Clin Anat, 15(1), 23-31.

Zia, S., Cody, F. W., & O'Boyle, D. J. (2003). Discrimination of bilateral differences in the loci

of tactile stimulation is impaired in subjects with Parkinson's disease. Clin Anat, 16(3),

241-247.

Ziluk, A., Premji, A., & Nelson, A. J. (2010). Functional connectivity from area 5 to primary

motor cortex via paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation. In Neurosci Lett (Vol.

484, pp. 81-85). Ireland: 2010 Elsevier Ireland Ltd.

Zopf, R., Harris, J. A., & Williams, M. A. (2011). The influence of body-ownership cues on

tactile sensitivity. Cogn Neurosci, 2(3-4), 147-154.