self-focus, gender, and habitual self- handicapping…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf ·...

14
SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY. 2005, 33(1). 43-56 © Society for Personality Research (Inc.) SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING: DO THEY MAKE A DIFFERENCE IN BEHAVIORAL SELF-HANDICAPPING? CHARLES E. KIMBLE University of Dayton, OH, USA EDWARD R. HIRT Indiana University, IN, USA This experiment examined the effects of public self-focus on individuals' behavioral self- handicapping tendencies. When faced with a threatening evaluation, a person may choose to self-handicap behaviorally. Men. more than women, and trait self-handicappers have been shown to self-handicap behaviorally. How do situational factors such as self-focus interface with these personal characteristics to affect such actions? Self-focus of attention was expected to make the self-evaluation implications of an upcoming performance more salient and to cause the self-focused performer to self-handicap behaviorally. Persons who were low or high in habitual self-handicapping were presented with an important intellectual evaluation and were allowed to practice for the upcoming test. Results showed that men self-handicap more by practicing less when they are self-focused, but women do not self-handicap under self- focus and self-handicapping instruction conditions. The implications of these findings for understanding the antecedent conditions of self-handicapping are discussed in the context of other recent work. Practically everyone is motivated to maintain a positive self-evaluation (Baumeister, Tice, & Hutton, 1989; Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1986; Charles E. Kimble. Department of Psychology. University of Dayton. OH. USA; Edward R. Hirt. Department of Psychology. Indiana University - Bloomington, Bloomington. IN. USA. The authors would like to thank Nicole Bickham. Brian Cooper, and Brenda Hartman-Bowers for their assistance in conducting this research. Appreciation is due to reviewers including: Andrew Martin. PhD, University of Western Sydney. SELF Research Centre. Penrith South, NSW 1797, Australia, EmaU: <[email protected]>; Ted Thompson, PhD, School of Psychology. University of Tasmania. Hobart, Tasmania. Australia. Email: <T.Thompson@ utas.edu.au> Please address correspondence and reprint requests to: Charles E. Kimble, Department of Psychology. University of Dayton, Dayton, OH 45469-1430. Phone: (937) 229 2167; Fax: (937) 229 3900; Email: <[email protected]> or Edward R. Hirt. Department of Psychology. Indiana University. - Bloomington. Bloomington. IN 47405; Email: <[email protected]> 43

Upload: ngohuong

Post on 06-Feb-2018

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

SOCIAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY. 2005, 33(1). 43-56© Society for Personality Research (Inc.)

SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF-HANDICAPPING: DO THEY MAKE A DIFFERENCE IN

BEHAVIORAL SELF-HANDICAPPING?

CHARLES E . KIMBLE

University of Dayton, OH, USAEDWARD R. HIRT

Indiana University, IN, USA

This experiment examined the effects of public self-focus on individuals' behavioral self-handicapping tendencies. When faced with a threatening evaluation, a person may choose toself-handicap behaviorally. Men. more than women, and trait self-handicappers have beenshown to self-handicap behaviorally. How do situational factors such as self-focus interfacewith these personal characteristics to affect such actions? Self-focus of attention was expectedto make the self-evaluation implications of an upcoming performance more salient and tocause the self-focused performer to self-handicap behaviorally. Persons who were low or highin habitual self-handicapping were presented with an important intellectual evaluation andwere allowed to practice for the upcoming test. Results showed that men self-handicap moreby practicing less when they are self-focused, but women do not self-handicap under self-focus and self-handicapping instruction conditions. The implications of these findings forunderstanding the antecedent conditions of self-handicapping are discussed in the context ofother recent work.

Practically everyone is motivated to maintain a positive self-evaluation(Baumeister, Tice, & Hutton, 1989; Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1986;

Charles E. Kimble. Department of Psychology. University of Dayton. OH. USA; Edward R. Hirt.Department of Psychology. Indiana University - Bloomington, Bloomington. IN. USA.The authors would like to thank Nicole Bickham. Brian Cooper, and Brenda Hartman-Bowers fortheir assistance in conducting this research.Appreciation is due to reviewers including: Andrew Martin. PhD, University of Western Sydney.SELF Research Centre. Penrith South, NSW 1797, Australia, EmaU: <[email protected]>; TedThompson, PhD, School of Psychology. University of Tasmania. Hobart, Tasmania. Australia. Email:<T.Thompson@ utas.edu.au>Please address correspondence and reprint requests to: Charles E. Kimble, Department of Psychology.University of Dayton, Dayton, OH 45469-1430. Phone: (937) 229 2167; Fax: (937) 229 3900; Email:<[email protected]> or Edward R. Hirt. Department of Psychology. Indiana University. -Bloomington. Bloomington. IN 47405; Email: <[email protected]>

43

Page 2: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

44 SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE

Greenwald & Breckler, 1985; Steele, 1988, 1990; Tesser, 1986,1988; Whitehead& Smith, 1986). Many people try to attain and maintain a positive self-evaluationby performing the best they can. But when individuals are faced with the prospectof being unable to succeed, many people engage in behavioral self-handicappingto salvage their positive self-evaluation (Hirt, McCrea, & Kimble, 2000; Jones &Berglas, 1978).

Self-handicapping involves the purposeful creation of a disadvantage foroneself prior to task performance. The hindrance to good performance createdallows the performer to escape the attribution that he or she does not have theability to perform well (Kelley, 1972). If the performer fails in the subsequentevaluation situation, he or she could say, "If I hadn't had this disadvantage, Iwould have done well." If the performer succeeds, he or she could say, "I mustreally be capable if I did that well despite the handicap." Self-handicapping helpsindividuals to maintain positive self-evaluations and to avoid disconfirmation ofdesired self-conceptions.

SELF-FOCUS AND SELF-EVALUATION

The primary situational variable we examined in this study was self-focus. Self-focus was originally described in psychological literature as objective self-awareness (Duval & Wicklund, 1972). Objective self-awareness or self-focusoccurs whenever one's attention is directed toward oneself rather than toward theenvironment. Subjective self-awareness or other-focus occurs whenever one'sattention is directed away from oneself toward other people, objects, events, oractivities in one's environment. If the individual is self-focused, he or she shouldmore readily perceive the self-evaluation implications of the performanceevaluation (Carver, 1979; Duval & Wicklund, 1972; Gibbons, 1990). Gibbonsstates that when one becomes self-focused by environmental conditions, self-schema are activated and self-evaluation is likely. Both Duval and Wicklund andGibbons indicate that self-focused individuals typically act in a mannerconsistent with their most established or habitual standards. If the performer ismade more self-aware by being in front of a real or implied audience (Duval &Wicklund), by being in front of a mirror (Carver & Scheier, 1978) or by writingshort stories about him/herself (Fenigstein & Levine, 1984), he or she should bemore attuned to the self-evaluation threat and should self-handicap more. If thislogic is correct, a means of enhancing the self-evaluative implications of one'sperformance would be to place participants in conditions of self-focus.

Before anyone manipulated self-focus and examined its effects on self-handicapping, one study (Shepperd & Arkin, 1989) looked at the effects ofindividual differences in self-consciousness on self-handicapping behavior.Individuals who score high in public self-consciousness are chronically attentiveto themselves (i.e., they are self-focused much ofthe time), especially on matters

Page 3: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45

related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975). Buss(1980) pointed out that the trait, self-consciousness, has many of the same effectsas the temporary state, self-focus or self-awareness. Based on the Kolditz andArkin (1982) argument that self-handicapping is primarily in the service of self-presentational concems, Shepperd and Arkin argued that people who are high inpublic self-consciousness should self-handicap more than do people low inpublic self-consciousness. Indeed, their study showed that high public self-conscious people were most likely to self-handicap (by choosing more interferingmusic before a test of academic success), but only when the test was described asimportant. Because they are more attuned to self-presentational concems, webelieve that an important part of high public self-conscious individuals' self-evaluation is how others evaluate them. We expected that induced public self-focus (using a one-way mirror with an observer's silhouette behind it) wouldaffect self-handicapping in the same way that public self-consciousness does.Several studies and reviews over the last 25 years have indicated that self-focusor self-awareness increases self-evaluation with salient standards (Carver, 1975,2003; Carver & Scheier, 1998; Duval & Silvia, 2002; Scheier & Carver, 1983;Silvia, 2002a, 2002b). Increased self-evaluation was expected to accentuateindividuals' self-handicapping.

Hirt et al. (2000) studied the effect of self-focus of attention by havingparticipants complete sentences starting with "I am" and to rate themselves on apersonality questionnaire with a camera recording them in a small room. In theother-focus condition, there was no camera pointed at participants as they readabout another person to form an impression and rated him/her on the samepersonality questionnaire as was used in the self-focus condition. Both self-focusand other-focus participants then practiced for an upcoming culture-fairintelligence test under Practice Matters (self-handicapping) conditions orPractice Doesn't Matter (no self-handicapping) conditions. Only males in theself-focus condition self-handicapped by practicing less. Habitual or trait self-handicapping, as measured by the Self-handicapping Scale (SHS; Jones &Rhodewalt, 1982), was unrelated to self-handicapping behavior by men orwomen. Regression analyses showed that the self-handicapping behavior by menwas mediated by a Concem with Failure factor, self-report items such as "Iexpect to do poorly on this test" completed right before practice and the test. Thisresult that men in the self-focus condition who were concemed with failing weremost likely to self-handicap behaviorally supports the idea that self-focus fostersbehavioral self-handicapping by increasing the self-evaluation threat in such testor evaluation situations.

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AND SELF-HANDICAPPING

A benefit of using a situational manipulation of self-focus (rather than

Page 4: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

46 SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE

individual differences in self-consciousness) is that it allows us to examine therole of other individual difference variables known to affect self-handicapping.Indeed, there appear to be some people who are much more inclined to use self-handicapping than are others. Interestingly, the self-handicapping literature hasbeen successful in identifying several individual difference variables thatinfiuence the use of self-handicapping. For instance, the SHS developed by Jonesand Rhodewalt (1982) is an individual difference measure designed to identifythose who are habitual self-handicappers. The SHS has proven successful atpredicting which individuals are most likely to self-handicap (Hirt, Deppe, &Gordon, 1991; Rhodewalt, Morf, Hazlett, & Fairfield, 1991: Rhodewalt,Saltzman & Wittmer, 1984; Strube, 1986; Strube & Roemmele, 1985). However,in the Hirt, McCrea, and Kimble (2000) study men, not just high SHS men, self-handicapped under self-focus conditions. Thus, in the present study, weexamined the effect of situationally induced self-focus on high and low trait self-handicappers.

A second interesting individual difference factor has been gender. Numerousstudies (Berglas & Jones, 1978; Harris & Snyder, 1986; Hirt et al., 1991; Kimble,Kimble, & Croy, 1998; Rhodewalt & Davison, 1986; Shepperd & Arkin, 1989)have demonstrated that males are more likely to self-handicap than are women,although these gender differences appear to emerge only with regard tobehavioral self-handicapping. It should be noted that gender differences inbehavioral self-handicapping in the Hirt et al. (1991) study were limited to highself-handicappers; only male high self-handicappers self-handicapped more thanothers did. Many explanations have been offered for this gender difference inbehavioral self-handicapping (see Hirt et al., 1991, for a more detailed discussionof this issue). However, for purposes of the present discussion, we will focus onShepperd and Arkin's explanation that, compared with women, men appear to actas if performance evaluations on the intellectual tasks commonly used are morerelevant to their self-evaluations.

Given this logic, we used the behavioral self-handicap of effort withdrawal inthe present experiment. Borrowing a paradigm used by Smith, Snyder, andHandelsman (1982) and Hirt et al. (1991), participants were assigned to either apractice matters or practice doesn't matter condition. In the practice matterscondition, participants were told that their test scores would be affected by theamount of practice they had (i.e., practice improves test scores); in this condition,low amounts of practice are indicative of self-handicapping. In the practicedoesn't matter condition, participants were told that amount of practice would notaffect their test scores; this condition serves as a baseline for comparison with thepractice matters conditions.

Within each of the practice conditions, we manipulated self-focus. Weexpected that participants in the self-focus condition would be more likely to

Page 5: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 47

self-handicap than would participants in the other-focus condition. In addition,we examined the effects of sex and level of trait self-handicapping (SHS) to seeif these participant characteristics interacted with the manipulated variables. Forinstance, it might be the case that the effects of induced self-focus might work foreveryone, for high self-handicappers only, for males only, or for high self-handicapping males only. Altematively, the effects of self-focus might be morebroad and might override any of these individual difference variables. Thus, thepresent study should provide insight into the interaction of situational andindividual difference variables on behavioral self-handicapping.

METHOD

PARTICIPANTS

Ninety-one (42 male, 49 female) introductory psychology students at theUniversity of Dayton participated individually in the study as partial fulfillmentof a course requirement. Participants' level of self-handicapping was determinedon the basis of the short-form Self-Handicapping Scale (Strube, 1986)administered during a mass testing at the beginning of the semester. Strubedeveloped a 10-item scale, which had higher intemal consistency (a = .70) thandid the original scale, fi^om Jones and Rhodewalt's (1982) 25-item measure. All10 items (with minor rewording) are among the 25 items in the longer measureand are answered on a 1 to 6 (strongly disagree to strongly agree) scale. Strubereported the following statistics for women (M = 33.15, SD = 7.50,A = 82) and for men (M = 32.99, SD = 6.93, N = 86) on the short SHS. Only oneitem, "I always try to do my best, no matter what," was recoded and the othernine (e.g., "I tend to make excuses when I do something wrong") were addeddirectly so that higher scores represent more self-handicapping. Participants wereidentified as either low or high self-handicappers on the basis of a median split.Participants were recruited from the large mass testing pool and were randomlyassigned to the focus conditions and the practice conditions. Data from allparticipants who participated in the individual sessions were included in theanalyses.

PROCEDURE

Upon arrival, each participant was seated in a small cubicle. Self-focusparticipants were facing a large one-way mirror through which they could see theexperimenter's silhouette as the experimenter observed them before and duringactivities. Self-focus participants were told that the mirror was a one-way mirrorand that the experimenter would be observing them during the study. Other-focusparticipants did an extemal focus activity - writing a story about Abraham

Page 6: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

48 SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE

Lincoln from a list of words - while the mirror was completely covered (cf.Fenigstein & Levine, 1984).

From this point, the experiment followed the procedure used by Hirt et al.(1991) except that there were no self-reported handicapping conditions.Participants were introduced to the Altemations Test, a test designed to measureindividual differences in Integrative Orientation (cf. Trope & Brickman, 1975).Integrative Orientation (10) is described as a new psychological variable that hasbeen shown to be a consistent predictor of future success in academics and in theworkplace. These instructions were designed to ensure that the relevance of theperformance to self-evaluation (task importance) would be high for allparticipants.

The Altemations Test consisted of a series of arithmetic problems that requireda unique problem-solving procedure. The Altemations Test problems use aformat consisting of two lines with 3 numbers and operator signs of plus, minus,and parentheses; for example, (9-5)-i-4 with (7+4)-6 on the second line.Participants were told that to complete these problems, they needed to first solvethe top line of the two (using the appropriate operations of addition andsubtraction). Then they were to solve the bottom line of the pair. The catch wasthat they were not to write down either answer, but were to remember bothanswers. If the top solution was larger than the bottom solution, they were tosubtract the bottom from the top and write down the final solution. If the topsolution of a particular equation pair was smaller than the bottom one, they wereto add the two solutions and write down the final solution.

Participants were also informed that they would be allowed to practicealtemations problems as much as they liked before being asked to complete theactual Altemations Test. At this point, participants were given differentinstmctions about the effects of practice on test performance. Half of theparticipants were told that amount of practice mattered in affecting the test'sassessment of their true ability (self-handicapping condition); specifically, theinstmctions stated that people who have not had much practice with these typesof problems when they take this test tend to get a score that is significantly belowtheir tme level of ability. The other half of the participants were told that practicedid not matter (no self-handicapping control condition); in this condition, theinstmctions stated that the Altemations Test is designed in such a way thatregardless of the amount of practice a person has had, his or her score on the testis an accurate measure of the person's level of integrative orientation.Participants in both groups were then given the opportunity to practice as muchas they would like before actually taking the test.

The two primary dependent measures were the number of problems practicedand the time spent practicing. By practicing less, participants would be creatinga motivated disadvantage in the self-handicapping condition. A postpractice

Page 7: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 4g

instructions, prepractice item was included that assessed how well theparticipants expected to do on the upcoming test. All participants were thendebriefed, thanked, given participation credit, and excused.

DESIGN

The design was a 2 (level of habitual self-handicapping — low vs. high) X 2(sex - male vs. female) X 2 (self-focus condition - self-focus vs. other-focus) X2 (instmctions about practice — practice matters vs. practice doesn't matter)between-subjects factorial design.

RESULTS

EXPECTATIONS ABOUT TASK PERFORMANCE

On the expectation measure right before practice, we obtained a significantmain effect for sex, F(l,80)=5.77, p<.02. Males indicated that they expected todo better on the upcoming test than did females. This result indicates that maleswere more confident about their performance on the task. No other significanteffects were obtained on this measure.

BEHAVIORAL SELF-HANDICAPPING

Following the Hirt et al. (1991) procedure, we combined the time spentpracticing and the number of problems practiced measures because they were sohighly correlated, r(91)=+.9O. The mean and standard deviation for the timemeasure in seconds were 524.24 and 409.96 and the mean and standard deviationfor the number of problems measure were 31.32 and 28.74. This combinedmeasure was obtained by adding the z-scores for time and the z-scores fornumber of problems practiced. The 91 participants were distributed nearlyequally over the eight cells with three conditions containing observations from 10participants, five conditions having 12 participants, and one condition having 13.

A 2 (level of self-handicapping) X 2 (sex) X 2 (self-focus) X 2 (instmctionsabout practice) ANOVA was performed on the combined practice measure. Thisanalysis revealed a significant Self-Focus main effect, F(l, 75)=6.02, p<.02.Overall, participants in the Self-Focus condition practiced longer on moreproblems than did participants in the Other-Focus condition. In addition, thisanalysis revealed a significant Sex by Self-Focus by Practice interaction,F(l,75) = 4.19,p<.05.

In order to interpret this three-way interaction, separate simple effects analyseswere conducted in the Self-Focus conditions and Other-Focus conditions. Therewere no significant sex, practice, or sex by practice effects within the Other-Focus conditions (all Fs < 1, ns). In the Self-Focus conditions, the simple effectsanalysis indicated a significant Sex by Practice interaction, F(l,42)=7.12, p<.02.

Page 8: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

50 SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE

Overall, the results show that only the men self-handicapped in the PracticeMatters (Self-Handicapping) condition. In fact, women worked harder in thePractice Matters (Self-Handicapping) conditions than in the Practice Doesn'tMatter (No Self-Handicapping) conditions, whereas men did the opposite. Thus,again in this study, it is only men who show any evidence of behavioral self-handicapping; however, men self-handicapped only under conditions of self-focus. In contrast to their male counterparts, women under conditions of self-focus reacted more adaptively by practicing more when practice made adifference. These data are illustrated in Table 1.

TABLE 1MEANS, STANDARD DEVIATIONS, AND NS ON THE COMBINED EFFORT MEASURE AS A FUNCTION OF

Focus, PRACTICE INSTRUCTIONS, AND SEX OF PARTICIPANTS

Self-Focus

Other-Focus

Practice Matters (S-H condition)

Practice Doesn't Matter (No S-H condition)

Practice Matters (S-H condition)

Practice Doesn't Matter (no S-H condition)

FemaleMaleFemaleMaleFemaleMaleFemaleMale

N

1210121013101212

M

1.38-.30-.491.11-.33-.65-.35-.59

SD

2.050.331.702.820.781.082.171.90

The astute reader may note that we did not obtain any significant effects oflevel of habitual self-handicapping. Indeed, this fmding appears in stark contrastto the results of Hirt et al. (1991), who obtained a Self-Handicapping X Sex XPractice interaction on their combined practice measure. However, one mightargue what effects the self-focus manipulation would have on individualdifferences in habitual self-handicapping. Perhaps the self-focus manipulationmay override any individual differences that might exist between males,nullifying any effects of the self-handicapping scale variable. Indeed, our resultsprovide suggestive evidence for this argument. A marginally significant four-wayinteraction (Self-Handicapping Scale X Sex X Self-Focus X Practice) wasobtained, F(l,75)=3.45, p=.O67. To explore the nature of this interaction and toclarify interpretation, we calculated difference scores between Practice Matters(Self-Handicapping) conditions and Practice Doesn't Matter (No Self-Handicapping) conditions. Table 2 displays these data.

The pattern of results indicates that in the Other-Focus conditions, high self-handicapping males behaviorally self-handicapped and low self-handicappingmales did not in the Practice Matters (Self-Handicapping) condition, replicatingHirt et al. (1991). However, in the Self-Focus conditions, all men, not just highself-handicapping men, behaviorally self-handicapped. Thus, it appears that the

Page 9: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 51

self-focus manipulation was able to override the individual differences betweenhigh and low self-handicapping males (evidenced in the Other-Focus conditions),leading all males in the Self-Focus condition to behaviorally self-handicap (bywithdrawing practice effort).

TABLE 2MEAN DIFFERENCE SCORES BETWEEN PRACTICE CoNDmoNS AS A FUNCTION OF GENDER,

SELF-FOCUS, AND TRAIT SELF-HANDICAPPING LEVEL (SHS)

Males

Females

Self-focusOther-focus

Self-focusOther-focus

Low Self-Handicappers

-1.8371.281

2.617-.789

High Self-Handicappers

-.965-.893

.705

.460

Note. Negative scores indicate less practice in Practice Matters conditions; that is. self-handicappingbehavior.

It could be that we had too few participants to have enough power to analyzethe data in a 4-way ANOVA and that is why we did not fmd the usual interactionbetween gender and low and high SHS scores. The results just cited indicate thatin the Other-Focus conditions, the usual interaction between gender and SHSscores does occur; but in the Self-focus conditions, the SHS variable has noeffect. We do not believe that inadequate power accounts for the differencebetween self-focus and other-focus conditions.

DISCUSSION

Our results indicate that males' tendency to self-handicap behaviorally isaccentuated by self-focus. That is, under self-focus all males, not just the habitualself-handicappers, practiced less in order to create a self-handicap. Thus, thepresent results suggest that if any male (regardless of his level of trait self-handicapping) is threatened and made self-attentive, he will be likely to self-handicap. Women practiced more under self-focus and practice mattersinstructions than under practice doesn't matter instructions. Self-focusaccentuated men's tendency to self-handicap behaviorally and women's tendencynot to self-handicap. Self-focus had an overriding effect over self-reported,habitual self-handicapping, as indicated by short SHS scores, among men.

Recall that the Hirt et al. (1991) results showed that only high self-handicapping males practiced less when practice mattered. When we examinedthe other-focus conditions in the present study, the same pattern of resultsoccurred. That is, only habitual self-handicapping males self-handicapped in the

Page 10: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

52 SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE

Other-focus conditions. But when self-attentiveness was increased in the self-focus conditions, all males practiced less when practice mattered. Therefore,when self-evaluation implications are made more salient by self-focus, men whodo not ordinarily use behavioral self-handicapping strategies did so.

In addition, our results add to the growing literature demonstrating that womendo not engage in behavioral self-handicapping. In the present study, women didnot behaviorally self-handicap under either self-focus or other-focus conditions.Thus, even when the self-evaluative implications were made salient to women,they (unlike the men) did not respond by self-handicapping. As we remarked inthe introduction, although several explanations for this sex difference have beenoffered (see Hirt et al., 1991; Hirt et al., 2000; Kimble & Hirt, 1993), the reasonsunderlying this effect remain unclear. Among the suggested reasons for thegender difference is that men are more threatened by possible failure than womenare (Hirt et al., 2000), that men are more sensitive to competitive situations thanwomen are (Burke, 1989; Kimble & Bryant, 2002), that men are more attuned tothe implications of ability and effort for performance and women are more awareof the performance outcomes (Hirt, McCrea, & Boris, 2003; Luginbuhl &Palmer, 1991), and men believe that ability is fixed and women believe thatability can be enhanced (Rhodewalt, 1994). This result may suggest thatperforming well may be more important to women than is protecting a positiveself-evaluation.

The present results (using a manipulation of state self-focus) are quiteconsistent with Shepperd and Arkin's (1989) fmdings using a measure of traitself-focus, public self- consciousness. They found that high public self-consciousparticipants self-handicapped before an important evaluation. Although they didnot find an interaction with sex of subject, they did obtain a main effect for sex,indicating that men self-handicapped more than women did, regardless ofevaluation importance. Hence, high public self-conscious men were most likelyto self-handicap in their study, just as self-focused men were most likely to self-handicap in our study. Thus, it appears that heightened self-focus, as a result ofindividual differences or experimental induction, increases the salience of self-evaluation and promotes the use of behavioral self-handicapping.

IMPLICATIONS FOR UNDERSTANDING THE ANTECEDENTS OF SELF-HANDICAPPING

These results are important for several reasons. First, this study adds to theliterature by identifying another situational factor that encourages the use of self-handicapping. The literature has focused primarily on two situational factors -noncontingent success feedback and task importance - that have been shown toincrease self-handicapping. Indeed, we believe that both of these factors increasethe evaluative threat, either by increasing uncertainty about one's ability tomaintain a high level of performance and/or by rendering the evaluation more

Page 11: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 53

important and critical to one's self-concept. Thus, we believe that any factor thatincreases the evaluative threat (or the salience of the evaluative threat) shouldtherefore increase the incidence of self-handicapping. Consistent with this logic,we found that self-focus, a manipulation shown to increase the salience of self-evaluation, led males to self-handicap more. Hirt et al. (2000), in fact, found thatconcem with failure on the upcoming performance was the mediating factor thatled to more self-handicapping by males under their self-focus conditions.Moreover, we suspect that other manipulations that increase the evaluative threat(e.g., competition, recent failure in some other self-relevant domain) shouldlikewise increase the incidence of self-handicapping behavior.

Second, results of our study attest to the need to consider the interaction ofsituational and individual difference factors in affecting self-handicappingbehavior. Several studies have investigated either situational or individualdifference factors, but few have examined possible interactions between thesetwo sets of factors. In the present study, we found that our situationalmanipulation of self-focus had effects on males but not on females. Moreover, itappears that (at least for males) situationally induced self-focus overridesindividual differences in habitual self-handicapping, suggesting that under highevaluative pressure low and high trait self-handicapping males are inclined toself-handicap in the service of self-protection. We suspect that there are manysituations in which a situational factor may overwhelm any individual differencesthat exist in self-handicapping proclivities, implying that the situational factorplays a central role in promoting self-handicapping behavior. Conversely, theremay be individual difference factors (e.g., individual differences in intuitivetheories about ability (Rhodewalt, 1994)) that encourage self-handicapping in theabsence of conducive situational factors. However, such knowledge shouldfacilitate the development of better models of self-handicapping behavior. It isour belief that taking an interactionist point of view toward understanding theantecedent conditions promoting self-handicapping behavior will be a fruitfulapproach. We hope that this paper will encourage additional research along theselines.

REFERENCES

Baumeister. R. K. Tice. D. M.. & Hutton, D. G. (1989). Self-presentational motivations andpersonality differences in self-esteem. Journal of Personality, 57, 547-579.

Berglas. S.. & Jones. E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-handicapping strategy in response tononcontingent success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36. 405-417.

Burke. P. J. (1989). Gender identity, sex, and school performance. Social Psychology Quarterly. 52.159-169.

Buss. A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety. San Francisco. CA: W. H. Freeman.Carver. C. S. (1975). Physical aggression as a function of objective self-awareness and attitudes

toward punishment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11. 510-519.

Page 12: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

54 SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE

Carver. C. S. (1979). A cybernetic model of self-attention processes. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, yi, 1251-1281.

Carver. C. S. (2003). Self-awareness. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self andidentity (pp. 179-196). New York: Guilford.

Carver. C. S., & Scheier. M. F. (1978). Self-focusing effects of dispositional self-consciousness.mirror presence, and audience presence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36. 324-332.

Carver. C. S.. & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: CambridgeUniversity Press.

Duval, T. S., & Silvia. P. J. (2002). Self-awareness, probability of improvement, and the self-servingbias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82. 49-61.

Duval. S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: AcademicPress.

Fenigstein, A.. & Levine. M. P. (1984). Self-attention, concept activation and the causal self. Journalof Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 231-245.

Fenigstein, A.. Scheier. M. F., & Buss. A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness:Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43. 522- 527.

Gibbons. F. X. (1990). Self-attention and behavior: A review and theoretical update. Advances inExperimental Social Psychology, 23. 249-303.

Greenberg. J.. Pyszczynski. T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The causes and consequences of a need forself-esteem: A terror management theory. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.). Public self and private self(pp. 181-212). New York. NY: Springer-Verlag.

Greenwald. A. G., & Breckler, S. J. (1985). To whom is the self presented? In B. R. Schlenker (Ed.),The self and social life (pp. 126-145). New York: McGraw Hill.

Harris. R. N.. & Snyder. C. R. (1986). The role of uncertain self-esteem in self-handicapping. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 51.451-458.

Hirt, E. R., Deppe, R. K.. & Gordon. L. J. (1991). Self-reported versus behavioral self-handicapping:Empirical evidence for a theoretical distinction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,61.981-991.

Hirt, E. R., McCrea. S. M., & Boris, H. I. (2003). "I know you self-handicapped last exam": Genderdifferences in reactions to self-handicapping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84.177-193.

Hirt. E. R.. McCrea. S. M., & Kimble. C. E. (2000). Public self-focus and sex differences inbehavioral self-handicapping: Does increasing self-threat still make it 'just a man's game?'Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26. 1131-1141.

Jones. E. E.. & Berglas. S. (1978). Control of attributions about the self through self-handicappingstrategies: The appeal of alcohol and the role of underachievement. Personality and SocialPsychology Bulletin, 4. 200-206.

Jones. E. E.. & Rhodewalt, F. (1982). The Self-handicapping Scale. Available from F. Rhodewalt.Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT.

Kelley. H. H. (1972). Attribution in social interaction. Morristown. NJ: General Learning Press.Kimble, C. E., & Bryant, J. (2002). Gender and behavioral self-handicapping: Competitive men make

self-handicapping choices. University of Dayton, Dayton, OH, Unpublished manuscript.Kimble. C. E.. & Hirt, E. R. (1993). Why are there gender-related differences in self-handicapping?

Society of Experimental Social Psychology meeting. Santa Barbara.Kimble. C. E.. Kimble, E. A.. & Croy, N. A. (1998). Development of self-handicapping tendencies.

Journal of Social Psychology, 138, 524-534.Kolditz. T. A.. & Arkin. R. M. (1982). An impression management interpretation of self-

handicapping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43.492-502.

Page 13: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,

SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 55

Luginbuhl. J.. & Palmer. R. (1991). Impression management aspects of self-handicapping: Positiveand negative. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17. 655-662.

Rhodewalt, F. (1994). Conceptions of ability, achievement goals, and individual differences in self-handicapping behavior: On the application of implicit theories. Journal of Personality, 62, 67-85.

Rhodewalt. F . & Davison, J. Jr. (1986). Self-handicapping and subsequent performance: Role ofoutcome valence and attributional certainty. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 307- 322.

Rhodewalt. F., Morf, C . Hazlett, S.. & Fairfield. M. (1991). Self-handicapping: The role ofdiscounting and augmentation in the preservation of self-esteem. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 61. 122-131.

Rhodewalt, F., Saltzman. A. T., & Wittmer. J. (1984). Self-handicapping among competitive athletes:The role of practice in self-esteem protection. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 5, 197-210.

Scheier. M. F.. & Carver, C. S. (1983). Self-directed attention and the comparison of self withstandards. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 205-222.

Shepperd, J. A., & Arkin. R. M. (1989). Self-handicapping: The moderating role of public self-consciousness and task importance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15. 252-265.

Silvia, P. J. (2002a). Self-awareness and emotional intensity. Cognition and Emotion, 16. 195-216.Silvia, P. J. (2002b). Self-awareness and the regulation of emotional intensity. Self and Identity, 1, 3-

10.Smith, T. W.. Snyder, C. R.. & Handelsman, M. M. (1982). On the self-serving function of an

academic wooden leg: Test anxiety as a self-handicapping strategy. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 42. 314-321.

Steele. C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L.Berkowitz (Ed.). Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21. pp. 261-302). New York:Academic Press.

Steele, C. M. (1990). Self-affirmation: Applications to social psychological theory and minorityachievement. Address presented at the American Psychological Society Convention, Dallas, TX.

Stiiibe. M. J. (1986). An analysis of the self-handicapping scale. Basic and Applied SocialPsychology, 7. 211-224.

Strube. M. J., & Roemmele. L. A. (1985). Self-enhancement, self-assessment, and self-evaluative taskchoice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 981-993.

Tesser. A. (1986). Some effects of self-evaluation maintenance on cognition and action. In R. M.Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of socialbehavior (pp. 435-464). New York: Guilford Press.

Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation model of social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advancesin experimental social psychology (Vol. 21. pp. 181-227). New York: Academic Press.

Trope. Y. & Brickman, P. (1975). Difficulty and diagnosticity as determinants of choice among tasks.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 918-926.

Whitehead. G. I., IQ. & Smith. S. H. (1986). Competence and escuse-making as self-presentationalstrategies. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private self (pp. 161- 177). New York, NY:Springer-Verlag.

Page 14: SELF-FOCUS, GENDER, AND HABITUAL SELF- HANDICAPPING…guidotheshark.tripod.com/15967596.pdf · SELF-FOCUS AND PRACTICE 45 related to how others evaluate them (Fenigstein, Scheier,