rules or connections in past tense inflections

22
Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections Psychology 209 February 4, 2013

Upload: sidone

Post on 23-Feb-2016

37 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections. Psychology 209 February 4, 2013. Is there a past tense rule?. Early on, children often produce exceptional past tenses correctly (went, took, etc). But at some point, they also produce ‘regularizations’. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Psychology 209February 4, 2013

Page 2: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Is there a past tense rule?• Early on, children often produce exceptional

past tenses correctly (went, took, etc).• But at some point, they also produce

‘regularizations’.• Also, children (and adults) produce ‘regular’

inflections for novel items when prompted, as in: this man is ricking… yesterday he ____.

• This was once taken as suggesting that young children discover ‘the past tense rule’.

• The fact that children learn exceptions was explained by ‘memorization’ or ‘lexical lookup’.

Page 3: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

An Alternative to Assuming that Children ‘Acquire’ the Past-Tense Rule

• Rumelhart and McClelland proposed that the past tense reflects regularities captured in the connections among units in a connectionist system that learns from examples.

• We demonstrated this by implementing and running a simple model of how people perform the task.

• The resulting debate has been fierce… and there are many who still think our approach is misguided.

• But as I’ll try to show the evidence appears to be consistent with our perspective.

• The work illustrates how models influence empirical research; how easy it is to get misleading results in experiments; and how important it is for researchers on each side of an issue to follow up on each other’s findings.

Page 4: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Overview• The RM model, introducing the connectionist

alternative• Early critiques and responses lead to…• The Pinker symbolic, dual mechanism account• PDP approach argues that a single-system

connectionist approach has advantages over the dual mechanism approach

• The exchange that was published in TiCS (2002) is available in the readings – your can read about the debate and judge for yourself.

• The issues have not been pursued as intensively since then.

Page 5: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections
Page 6: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections
Page 7: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections
Page 8: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Training and Testing Procedure• Training:

– Present WF pattern representing present tense of verb.– Compute WF pattern representing past tense of verb using

stochastic sigmoid activation function.– Compare computed past-tense pattern to correct past tense

pattern.– Adjust connections using Perceptron Convergence Procedure:

• Increase strength of connection from active input units to output units that should be active but are not.

• Decrease strength of connection from active input units to output units that should not be active but are.

• Testing:– Present WF pattern of present tense of verb.– Compute WF pattern.– Compare to various alternatives on various measures.– OR: Generate output using fixed decoding net.

Page 9: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Training Regime

• First ten epochs use 10 most frequent words only– Feel, have, make, get, give, take, come, go, look, need

• Remainder of training uses 10 most frequent plus 400 words of ‘middle frequency’

• Each word is presented once per epoch• An additional 84 lower-frequency words are

saved for generalization testing

Page 10: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Recapitulation of U-shaped learning

Page 11: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Varieties of Regular Past-Tenses

Page 12: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Types of IrregularVerbs

Page 13: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Responses to t/d verbs and other

verbs

Page 14: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Performance with Novel Irregulars

Page 15: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Quasi-Regularity

• The tendency for forms that are irregular to share features of regular form– do/did; say/said; make/made; have/had– keep/kept; kneal/knelt; learn/learnt…– hide/hid; lead/led; beat/beat; cut/cut; bid/bid… – 59% of irregular past tense forms end in d or t

• In the PDP approach, such items participate in the regular pattern just as regular forms do.

• In other approaches, they are often relegated to a separate system, thought to operate without reference to the rules, and therefore must be treated as though they are out-and-out exceptions.

Page 16: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Novel Regulars

48/72 only activatedcorrect responses; 6 activated no response;these are the remaining 18 items

Page 17: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Summary• The RM model can learn regulars and

exceptions.• It correctly inflects most unfamiliar regular

verbs, and makes over-regularization errors.• It also captures children’s tendency to produce

occasional ‘irregularization’ responses and other signs of sensitivity to sub-regularities.

• It produces a U-shaped developmental curve, like children.

• It does all this in a single system without explicit rules, rule-acquisition mechanisms, or a separate lexicon of exceptions.

Page 18: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Critique (Pinker and Prince, 1988)

• Training regime unrealistic– Child’s experience is relatively constant over time.

• Performance on regulars not good enough– Makes quite a few errors, some quite strange

• Model can’t produce different past tenses for homophones – ring the bell, ring the city, wring the clothes

• Wickelfeature representation has problems

Page 19: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Reply: Conceptualizations are not implementations (MacWhinney and Leinbach)

• Included semantic as well as phonological input– Allows different outputs for cases of same phonology but

different meanings• Used a different input representation that led

to better performance on regulars– Right-justified slot-based representation

• Model learned the regular pattern quite well– Concept of ‘Condensing the Regularities’

• Did not address U-shaped curve

Page 20: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Plunkett and Marchman• Trained with a corpus modeled after English:

– ‘Stationary’ frequency-weighted training environment– Exceptions:

Small number of hf ‘arbitraries’others come in clusters and share some features with the regular

pattern (most stem phonemes are preserved in the past tense)

• Found ‘micro-U’ shaped patterns during learning:– Performance on a given item can vacillate so that correct responses

precede incorrect responses.– This is consistent with the actual pattern seen in most children, where over-

regularizations are infrequent.

• Suggested that properties of networks actually offer an explanatory basis for understanding U-shaped development and the distribution of word forms in the language:– Micro-U reflects competing changes in weights– E.g. arbitraries very difficult to learn; must be hi freq and few in number– no change and vowel change items are much easier, need not be of high

frequency.

Page 21: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Pinker (1991, and elsewhere)• Noted that performance on exceptions does show some signs of

exhibiting features like those seen in the RM model. E.g., there is some similarity-based generalization, so that forms occasionally ‘join’ irregular clusters (e.g., kneel-knelt, gling-glang).

• Proposed a dual mechanism account in which there is one system that uses categorical, ‘algebraic’ rules insensitive to item properties, and another that uses an ‘associative memory mechanism’ much like the RM model.

• With Marcus, developed the notion that the rule is completely insensitive to semantic and phonological factors, depending only on the form-class of the stem.

• Has suggested in many places that the past tense rule is acquired ‘suddenly’ in a ‘Eureka Moment’

• Pinker (with Ulman) claimed that brain-damage can produce a specific deficit in use of the regular inflection.

• He also argued that a familial genetic defect can lead to a deficit in use of the regular inflection.

Page 22: Rules or Connections in Past Tense Inflections

Reply to Pinker (see McClelland and Patterson paper in TiCS exchange)

• Past tenses are acquired gradually, not suddenly. This is true of other inflectional forms as well and does not suggest the sudden acquisition of a rule in a Eureka moment.

• Initially children learn typical cases and gradually generalize to other cases.

• The tendency to regularize and the tendency to irregularize are affected by phonological and semantic similarity to known items of both types.– According to Pinker, this should happen for irregular items only.

• For both the effects of brain damage and the genetic anomaly, the evidence supports to view that the deficit is phonological, not a matter of rules.– The individuals with these disorders have difficulty with complex phonological forms,

and their difficulty with regular past tenses disappears if you control for phonological complexity.

• Pinker’s approach fails to address the quasi-regularity in exceptions, and therefore misses much of what is systematic in language.