reciprocity in marketing relationships a dissertation

208
RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School University of Missouri In Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy by DONALD J. LUND Dr. Lisa Scheer, Dissertation Supervisor DECEMBER 2010

Upload: others

Post on 10-Jan-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS

A Dissertation presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School

University of Missouri

In Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirements for the Degree

Doctor of Philosophy

by DONALD J. LUND

Dr. Lisa Scheer, Dissertation Supervisor

DECEMBER 2010

Page 2: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

This dissertation is dedicated to my family. To my children, I’m sorry that I’ve missed out on much during the last few years. Thank you for understanding. Very few people know the sacrifices you have made. To my mom, I wish you could be here in person for me to thank for all of your support, love and caring throughout my life. I know you have always been with me, and I will never forget you. To my father, I hope that you have stopped trying to make up for whatever it was you were supposed to do while I was in college – because I am honestly done with school now! Your encouragement, support, and guidance have been invaluable. You have helped make me the man I am. And how could I forget: Doctor, Dentist or Lawyer!

Page 3: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to thank my dissertation committee, Detelina Marinova, Chris

Groening and Peter Klein for all of their help, guidance and time during the creation,

revision and completion of this dissertation. Each of you offered a unique perspective

throughout the entire process that helped make this a better project. I will be forever

grateful for your time and interest in helping make this process a success. Additionally I

would like to thank Thomas Rose, Karen Nazario and Laurel Youmans for their help in

navigating the dissertation process at the University of Missouri.

I would especially like to thank my dissertation chair, Lisa Scheer, for her the

extensive amount of time she put in to improving my experience during my four years at

the University of Missouri. Lisa helped me by focusing on professional development,

academic rigor, career planning, and managing my personal life. I could not have asked

for a better mentor for this process. I look forward to being able to earn the peer status

you have already begun to treat me with.

I would also like to thank a few organizations that made my dissertation

financially possible; the Robert J. Trulaske, Sr. College of Business provided financial

support for a number of conference presentations, statistical software, and for my PhD

program in general. Additional funding that helped pay for survey printing and postage

was provided by Professor Lisa Scheer’s Emma S. Hibbs Professorship. Finally, Boone

County National Bank provided financial support for printing and postage of a survey for

roset
Typewritten Text
ii
Page 4: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

this research. Without the financial support acknowledged above, this research would

not have been able to be completed.

I feel lucky and blessed to have chosen the University of Missouri for my doctoral

program. The faculty, staff, administration and students all combined to build an

environment that has allowed me to develop and grow. I have no doubt that my

training is far superior to my expectations when considering different programs for my

PhD.

roset
Typewritten Text
roset
Typewritten Text
iii
Page 5: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

iv

Table of Contents ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .................................................................................................................... ii

LIST OF FIGURES ..............................................................................................................................vii

LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................................................................ ix

ABSTRACT ........................................................................................................................................xii

CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................ 1

1.1 Reciprocity in Marketing Theory ................................................................................................ 1

1.1.1 Reciprocity in Interpersonal Relationships ......................................................................... 1

1.1.2 Reciprocity in Interorganizational Relationships ................................................................ 5

1.1.3 Reciprocity in the Relationship Marketing Literature ......................................................... 7

1.2 Primary Motivating Research Questions ................................................................................. 11

1.3 Organization of the Dissertation .............................................................................................. 14

CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW .................................................................................................. 15

CHAPTER 3: A GENERAL THEORY OF EPISODIC RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS ... 21

3.1 Episodic Reciprocity Abstract ................................................................................................... 21

3.2 Episodic Reciprocity Introduction ............................................................................................ 21

3.3 Episodic Reciprocity Conceptual Development ....................................................................... 23

3.3.1 Reciprocal Action – Debt Response Cycles ....................................................................... 27

3.3.2 Factors that impact the formation of Reciprocal Debt ..................................................... 34

3.3.2.1 Nature of the Action .................................................................................................. 34

3.3.2.2 Responsibility for Action ............................................................................................ 36

3.3.2.3 Justifiability of Action ................................................................................................. 38

3.3.2.4 Nature of the Focal Party ........................................................................................... 39

3.3.2.5 Nature of the Relationship ......................................................................................... 42

CHAPTER 4: RESEARCH ON EPISODIC RECIPROCITY ...................................................................... 51

4.1 Survey Research Model ........................................................................................................... 51

4.1.2 Operationalizations ........................................................................................................... 53

4.1.2.1 Pre-Scenario Questionnaire ....................................................................................... 53

4.1.2.2 Experimental Scenarios .............................................................................................. 56

4.1.2.3 Post-Scenario Questionnaire ..................................................................................... 57

Page 6: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

v

4.1.3 Sample Selection ............................................................................................................... 60

4.1.4 Measurement Model and Scale Development ................................................................. 63

4.1.4.1 Directional Reciprocal Debt ....................................................................................... 63

4.1.4.2 Exploratory Factor Analyses....................................................................................... 63

4.1.4.3 Confirmatory Factor Analysis ..................................................................................... 71

4.1.4.4 Manipulation Checks ................................................................................................. 74

4.1.5 Hypothesis Testing ............................................................................................................ 77

4.1.5.1 Effects of Partner Actions on Reciprocal Debts ......................................................... 77

4.1.5.2 Moderating Impacts on the Creation of Reciprocal Debt .......................................... 81

4.1.6 Discussion ......................................................................................................................... 84

4.2 Laboratory Experiment ............................................................................................................ 88

4.2.1 Research Model ................................................................................................................ 88

4.2.2 Operationalizations ........................................................................................................... 93

4.2.3 Sample Selection ............................................................................................................... 96

4.2.4 Measurement Model and Scale Creation ......................................................................... 96

4.2.4.1 Exploratory Factor Analyses....................................................................................... 96

4.2.4.2 Confirmatory Factor Analyses .................................................................................... 98

4.2.4.3 Manipulation Checks ............................................................................................... 101

4.2.6 Hypothesis-Testing .......................................................................................................... 102

CHAPTER 5: RELATIONAL RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS: THE ROLE OF COMPLEX RECIPROCITY ................................................................................................................................ 125

5.1 Relational Reciprocity Abstract .............................................................................................. 125

5.2 Relational Reciprocity Introduction ....................................................................................... 126

5.3 Theoretical Model Development ........................................................................................... 128

5.3.1 Complex and Simple Reciprocity .................................................................................... 134

5.4 Hypotheses ............................................................................................................................ 137

5.4.1 Outcomes of Complex Reciprocity .................................................................................. 137

5.4.2 Antecedents of Relational and Discrete Reciprocity ...................................................... 140

5.4.2.1 Characteristics of the Buyer and Seller .................................................................... 141

5.4.2.2 Characteristics of the Exchange ............................................................................... 143

5.5 Survey Research on Complex Reciprocity .............................................................................. 146

5.5.1 Research Model .............................................................................................................. 146

Page 7: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

vi

5.5.2 Operationalizations ......................................................................................................... 147

5.5.3 Sample Selection ............................................................................................................. 155

5.5.4 Measurement Model and Scale Creation ....................................................................... 156

5.5.5 Hypothesis Testing .......................................................................................................... 165

5.5.7 Discussion ....................................................................................................................... 169

5.5.7.1 Episodic Reciprocity ................................................................................................. 169

5.5.7.2 Complex reciprocity ................................................................................................. 173

5.5.7.3 Summary .................................................................................................................. 174

CHAPTER 6 CONCLUSIONS ........................................................................................................... 176

6.1 Contributions to Theory ......................................................................................................... 176

6.2 Contributions to Practitioners ............................................................................................... 178

6.3 Future Research Directions .................................................................................................... 179

APPENDIX ..................................................................................................................................... 181

Exhibit #1 – University of Missouri Email Cover letter ................................................................ 181

Exhibit #2 UAB Email Cover Letter ............................................................................................... 182

Exhibit #3 – Customer Survey ...................................................................................................... 183

BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................................................................. 187

VITA .............................................................................................................................................. 195

Page 8: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

vii

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page

1. The Domain of Marketing Interactions ........................................................................ 12

2. Positive Reciprocal Debt Responses ............................................................................ 30

3. Negative Reciprocal Debt Responses .......................................................................... 33

4. Dependence Configurations ........................................................................................ 45

5. Process Model of Positive Reciprocity ......................................................................... 49

6. Process Model of Negative Reciprocity ....................................................................... 50

7. Process Model of Reciprocity ...................................................................................... 51

8. Random Assignment to Beneficial Conditions ............................................................. 52

9. Random Assignment to Detrimental Conditions ......................................................... 52

10. Plots of mean differences in OwnOut and RO based on the Valence manipulation ......................................................................................... 75

11. Plots of mean differences in automaker and other based on the attribution manipulation ............................................................................... 76

12. Scatterplot of RDLocus per OwnOut ............................................................................ 80

13. Estimated Marginal Means of RDLocus ....................................................................... 83

14. Experimental Research Model Tested ......................................................................... 88

15. Experimental Process Flowchart .................................................................................. 89

16. Random Assignment to Conditions ............................................................................. 90

17. Manipulations: Partner’s Initial Allocation Offer ........................................................ 92

18. Conceptual Framework for Beneficial Actions ............................................................ 103

Page 9: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

viii

19. Conceptual Framework for Detrimental Actions ......................................................... 103

20. Impact of Valence on Locus (ANOVA) .......................................................................... 104

21. Locus*Valence Crosstabulation ................................................................................... 106

22. Locus*Magnitude Crosstabulation .............................................................................. 106

23. ANOVA Results of PartnerOut at Different Locus ........................................................ 112

24. ANOVA Results for Positive Norm of Reciprocity’s Impact on RD and RDLocus ....................................................................................... 119

25. Plots of RD based on Perceived Impact and the Positive Norm of Reciprocity .................................................................................................. 119

26. ANOVA Plots of NR on RD and RDLocus ...................................................................... 120

27. Moderating Impact of Negative Norm of Reciprocity ................................................. 122

28. ANOVA Plot of the moderating impact of PR .............................................................. 124

29. Relational Reciprocity Conceptual Framework............................................................ 145

30. Final Structural Model – Significant Paths ................................................................... 167

Page 10: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

ix

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1. Reciprocity Definitions in Marketing Interpersonal Relationships .............................. 5

2. Reciprocity Definitions in Marketing B2B Relationships ............................................. 6

3. Approaches to Reciprocity taken in Seminal Works .................................................... 20

4. Experimental Scenario Components ........................................................................... 57

5. Recruitment Regions .................................................................................................... 61

6. Summary of Individual EFA’s for 4 Latent Constructs ................................................. 64

7. Separate EFA on RD and Just ....................................................................................... 64

8. Overall Means, Standard Deviations and Correlations for Study Variables ................ 65

9. Means, Standard Deviations and Correlations for Participants in the Beneficial Condition ............................................................................................ 67

10. Means, Standard Deviations and Correlations for Participants in the Detrimental Condition ........................................................................................ 67

11. CFA Results ................................................................................................................... 72

12. ANOVA results for OwnOut and RO Valence manipulation checks ............................. 74

13. 12a. ANOVA results for automaker and other – attribution manipulation checks ..... 76

14. Mean difference in Locus based on valence manipulation ......................................... 78

15. Coefficients from regression of Locus on manipulation factors .................................. 78

16. Coefficients from regression of RDLocus on OwnOut ................................................. 80

17. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects ............................................................................... 82

18. Planned Contrast Results (K Matrix) Justifiability ........................................................ 83

Page 11: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

x

19. Test Results for planned contrast ................................................................................ 83

20. Car Dealer Survey Hypothesis Summary ..................................................................... 84

21. Combined EFA on 4 Latent Constructs ........................................................................ 97

22. Summary of Individual EFA’s for 4 Latent Constructs ................................................. 98

23. CFA Results ................................................................................................................... 100

24. ANOVA’s with the Valence manipulation as the Independent variable ...................... 101

25. ANOVA’s with the Magnitude manipulation as the independent variable ................. 102

26. General Linear Model Test of Mean Differences ......................................................... 105

27. Parameter Estimates .................................................................................................... 107

28. Classification Results of Multinomial Regression for Locus ......................................... 107

29. Spline Regression Results ............................................................................................ 109

30. Spline Regression Results with no Intercept ............................................................... 110

31. Contrast Coefficients ................................................................................................... 112

32. Contrast Tests .............................................................................................................. 112

33. Spline Regression for RDLocus effect on PartnerOut .................................................. 113

34. Spline Regression for the moderating impact of relative outcomes ........................... 115

35. Regression of PR and OwnImpact on RDLocus ............................................................ 116

36. Regression of OwnImpact and PROwnImpact on RDLocus ......................................... 117

37. Norm of Positive Reciprocity’s effect on Reciprocal Debt (ANOVA) ........................... 117

38. Between-Subjects Factors ........................................................................................... 118

39. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects ............................................................................... 118

Page 12: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

xi

40. ANOVA results for Negative Norm of Reciprocity’s impact on RD and RDLocus ................................................................................................... 120

31. Between-Subjects Factors ........................................................................................... 121

32. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects ............................................................................... 121

33. Between-Subjects Factors ........................................................................................... 122

34. Tests of Between-Subjects Effects ............................................................................... 123

35. Experimental Research Hypothesis Summary ............................................................. 124

36. Dimensions of Complex and Simple Reciprocity ......................................................... 135

37. Antecedent Variables EFA ............................................................................................ 156

38. EFA for Relational Reciprocity ...................................................................................... 157

39. Outcome variable EFA Results ..................................................................................... 158

40. Overall CFA results for all items in the theoretical model ........................................... 162

41. Means, Standard Deviations and Correlations Between Study Variables ................... 163

42. Structural Model Results ............................................................................................. 165

43. Mediation Analyses for the Structural Path Model ..................................................... 168

Page 13: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

xii

RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS

Donald J. Lund

Dr. Lisa Scheer, Dissertation Supervisor

ABSTRACT

Marketing researchers have often cited reciprocity as an important aspect of relational

exchange; however the extant research has not conceptualized or measured reciprocity in

marketing relationships. This research is a first attempt to explicate the role of reciprocity in

these exchange relationships. Reciprocity is first conceptualized as a component of discrete

transactions. A theoretical model is built and tested in (a) a laboratory setting and (b) through a

survey of car dealership managers. The role of reciprocal debt is established and shown to

impact exchange relationships. Next, reciprocity is conceptualized as a multidimensional

element of ongoing exchange relationships. Through a survey of business customers of a bank,

the existence of relational reciprocity is established and shown to impact numerous indicators of

relationship success. Implications for both theory and practitioners are presented, and future

research directions are discussed.

Page 14: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

1

CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION

1.1 Reciprocity in Marketing Theory

“I regard reciprocity as an essential feature of self-regulation and the problem of coordinating mutual actions for parties in a marketing relationship.”(Bagozzi 1995) Marketing as a discipline has failed to adequately incorporate the concept of reciprocity

in any theory of customer relationships. The term reciprocity appears in much of the marketing

literature, especially those streams focusing on relationship marketing (both consumer and

B2B), however very few researchers have attempted to conceptualize or measure reciprocity at

all. Reciprocity research within marketing can be categorized as two distinct types. The first is

explicit-reciprocity research, where the researchers attempted to measure reciprocity and/or

offer a specific definition, and subsequently place it into their conceptual framework and/or

hypotheses. The second category is implicit-reciprocity research, where the term reciprocity is

used, but is not measured nor explicitly defined. The former section is minute compared to the

latter. The following is an overview of the explicit marketing reciprocity research. This research

can be segregated depending on the focus of the reciprocal interaction – interpersonal versus

interorganizational.

1.1.1 Reciprocity in Interpersonal Relationships

The earliest explicit use of reciprocity located in the marketing literature drew almost

exclusively from legal cases. Moyer (1970) discusses “Reciprocal Buying” and describes

reciprocity as doing business with one’s friends (see definition in table #1). “You scratch my

back, and I’ll scratch yours” (p. 47). The focus of this line of research was on coercive

reciprocity, where partner A threatens reduced purchasing if partner B didn’t increase or

maintain purchase levels from A. Relationships like that described above led to the

development of anti-trust laws to attempt to prevent “strong-arming”, and the potential harm it

Page 15: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

2

could have on consumers (not to mention the less powerful partner). Finney (1978) later did a

follow up intending to examine if this coercive type of reciprocity continued to exist in

businesses. Finney found that although it probably continued to occur in some relationships,

the prevalence had decreased substantially since Moyer’s research in 1970. This concept of

reciprocal buying points out an important fact about reciprocal behavior: it has the potential to

influence the behavior of a relational partner. This is reminiscent of Howard Becker’s

description of the role of reciprocity in the social learning process. Do marketing relationships

exhibit goal oriented (positive or negative) reciprocal behavior intended to mold the response of

their partner?

Houston and Gassenheimer (1987) extend the conceptual work of Bagozzi (1975) in

developing exchange theory as the key component of marketing. Houston and Gassenheimer

argue that an exchange relationship is created when “…one party is expected to defer receipt of

value into the future…” (p. 10). They go on to explain the role of reciprocity in exchange

relationships in great detail, arguing that reciprocity is present in every exchange relationship,

and that its implementation varies depending on the “social distance” of the parties involved.

While they do not define social distance, it appears that minimum social distance is implied by a

familial relationship, intermediate would involve friendships and business relationships, and

maximum social distance is presumably strangers. Interestingly, Houston and Gassenheimer

theorize that negative reciprocity is only engaged in with strangers, citing an example of bribery.

Additionally, they suggest that with a minimum social distance (e.g. parental relationships),

partners give willingly, “the value to A being in the giving of the product”, which is more in line

with altruistic behavior when no expectation of future return of benefits exists. More recent

work has interpreted the parent-child relationship differently, suggesting that the level of late-

adult healthcare provided by the child may depend on child perceptions of parental investments

Page 16: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

3

during the child’s youth, suggesting that altruism may not be the only explanation of this type of

giving. While this paper does not specifically measure reciprocity, the authors do offer a

definition and explicitly place reciprocity as a functional component of exchange relationships

(see Table #1).

Marketing researchers have approached reciprocity from both a consumer perspective,

as well as from an interorganizational perspective. Dawson (1988) found that reciprocal

motivations were one of four motivations for consumer charitable giving. His definition of

reciprocity (see Table #1) could potentially be applied to any plausible exchange being that it

only encompasses a future benefit expectation from a donor. The measures used to capture

reciprocity focused on past benefits received (by the donor or their family) and on the expected

benefits to be gained by the donor from the charitable organization in the future. Frenzen and

Davis (1990) find that existing “social debts” increased the likelihood of purchase in socially

embedded markets (e.g. Mary Kay sales parties among friends). After controlling for the

economic utility of purchases made, they found that attendees at sales parties were more likely

to purchase from a host who had previously purchased from them. Miller and Kean (1997) find

that when local business owners exhibit interpersonal reciprocity (defined as emphasizing

concern for others or strong attachment to others), rural consumers are more likely to behave

reciprocally by purchasing locally. As we can see by this approach, the focus of reciprocity is on

cooperation, a concept that is definitely related to, but not the same as, reciprocity. In a

consumer-retailer exchange, Dahl, Honea and Manchanda (2005) find that consumers may

experience guilt if they are not able to reciprocate the social interaction (through a purchase).

Subsequently, they are more likely to return and purchase from that salesperson to assuage the

feeling of guilt.

Page 17: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

4

Within the framework of interpersonal relations, there were two other articles

attributable to marketing academics, one focused on research response rates and the other on

teacher evaluations. Cialdini and Rhoads (2001) review a large quantity of past research which

investigates the role of reciprocity in the process of social influence. The authors suggest that

reciprocity is one of six psychological principles (also including scarcity, authority, consistency,

liking, and consensus) that impacts persuasion, citing research on the granting of concessions to

individuals in order to increase participation in research (e.g., reducing the expected survey

duration to 20 minutes from an hour, etc.). Here again, we see how reciprocity might be

implicated in altering people’s behavior. The last interpersonal marketing paper investigated

the role of reciprocity in student evaluations of teacher effectiveness (Clayson 2004). The

author found that past grades received did in fact impact future evaluations of teacher

effectiveness, however given that the student’s were evaluating classes from 1-2 semesters

before the research was conducted, it is not clear if the same relationship would hold if the

typical student evaluations (given during the semester) were explored.

Reviewing the definitions of reciprocity (see table #1 on the following page) offered in

the marketing research on interpersonal relationships suggests two important facts: (1) there is

little consensus on the definition of what reciprocity is in marketing relationships; and (2) there

is a lack of research investigating the role of negative reciprocity in marketing relationships.

These are the first of many gaps in the literature that need to be addressed.

Page 18: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

5

1.1.2 Reciprocity in Interorganizational Relationships

In the B2B relationship framework, Kumar, Scheer and Steenkamp (1998) investigated

the existence of reciprocal punitive actions in auto-dealers’ relationships with their suppliers.

While these authors did not define reciprocity explicitly, the implied definition was evident in

the existence of higher rates of punitive dealer actions as a response to higher levels of supplier

punitive actions. Johnson and Sohi (2001) measured and found support for their

conceptualization of reciprocity as returning good for good between relational partners. They

developed a 6-item scale that captured the “positive reciprocity” as an outcome of interfirm

connectedness. Palmer (2002) attempted to measure reciprocity as a predictor of effectiveness

in cooperative marketing associations, however his measure failed to predict effectiveness.

Expanding on the lack of significance, Palmer suggests that his measures (being drawn from the

power/dependence literature) failed to capture reciprocity. Lee et al. (2008) modeled

Moyer, 1970; Finney, 1978

Journal of Marketing

Reciprocal purchasing - both the use of purchasing power to obtain sales and the practice of preferring one's customers in purchasing

Houston and Gassenheimer,

1987

Journal of Marketing

Reciprocity - the process whereby a mutual exchange of acceptable terms is actualized; it is a social interaction in which the movement of one party evokes a compensating movement in some other party

Dawson, 1988J. Health

Care MKT.Reciprocity - a cultural norm whereby individuals enter into an exchange with the anticipation of receiving personal benefits

Frenzen and Davis, 1990

JCRNorm of reciprocity - use of a purchase occasion in the short term to repay outstanding social debtsReciprocity - the degree to which individuals expect cooperative actionInterpersonal Reciprocity - emphasizing concern for others or strong attachment to othersInstitutional Reciprocity - having a built-in system for calculating the costs versus the benefits involved in the exchange

Dahl, Honea and Manchanda, 2005

JCPNorm of reciprocity - an obligation for people to return in kind what they've received from others

Cialdini and Rhoads, 2001

Marketing Research

Reciprocity - an obligation for people to return in kind what they've received from others

Clayson, 2004 MERReciprocity - evidence that student written teacher evaluations are related to grades received

Table #1. Reciprocity Definitions in Marketing Interpersonal Relationships

Miller and Kean, 1997

Psychology and

Marketing

Page 19: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

6

reciprocity as the outcome of mutualistic benevolence and altruistic benevolence in a

framework that is questionable in relation to the existing understanding of relationship

marketing. Troubled not only by the definition (see Table #2), this research is also challenged

with an inadequate conceptualization. The measure of reciprocity was the combination of two

questions: (1) Has your company helped this foreign exporter on at least one occasion; and (2)

Has this foreign exporter returned the favor on at least one occasion? While this measure quite

possibly captures one instance of a reciprocal interaction, it does little to explicate the role of

reciprocity in an ongoing relationship. Are we to assume that one beneficial act reciprocated by

a partner implies a healthy social exchange over the duration of the relationship?

Some more recent work by Pervan and Johnson attempts to develop Gouldner’s

conception of reciprocity. In their first paper (Pervan and Johnson 2003), the authors develop a

marketing based definition of reciprocity from the literature and theory on reciprocity in social

relationships (see Table #2). Their second paper (Pervan et al. 2009), attempts to conceptualize

Frazier and Colleagues, 1986,

1989 and 1991JM and JMR

Reciprocity - (implied definition) the actions taken by one party in response to the actions taken by the other party in an exchange relationship

Kumar, Scheer and Steenkamp, 1998

JMRReciprocity - (implied definition) evidenced by the existence of higher levels of dealer punitive actions in response to higher levels of suplier punitive actions

Johnson and Sohi, 2001

IJRMReciprocity - partner response is contingent on actions; mutually contingent exchange of benefits; ensures long run gratification for partners

Palmer, 2002J. Strat.

Marketing

Reciprocity - a disposition to return good for good in proportion to what they receive; to resist evil, but to do no evil in return; and to make reparation for the harm we do

Lee, Jeong, Lee and Sung, 2008

IMMReciprocity - mutual exchange of helping behaviors between importers and exporters

Pervan and Johnson, 2003

JCRReciprocity in RM is an expectation that good is returned for good in a fitting and proportional manner, resist negative acts but not return negative acts, and make reparation for any harm we do.

Pervan, Bove and Johnson, 2009

IMM Same as above.

Table #2 Reciprocity Definitions in Marketing B2B Relationships

Page 20: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

7

and develop a scale to measure reciprocity in B2B relationships. In attempting to develop a

scale to capture four proposed dimensions (make reparations, non-retaliation, returning good

for good and resisting evil) they found a 2 dimension scale explained the responses the best.

These dimensions were called Exchanging good and Response to Harm. In reality most of the

components of the exchanging good dimension appear to be based on equality of outcomes,

which should be related to reciprocity, but definitely is not the same as reciprocity. Additionally,

the response to harm dimension appears to capture more about cooperation between partners

than it does reciprocal exchanges. Again, cooperation should be related to reciprocity; however

reciprocity being a time delayed response to an action by a partner does not necessarily imply

concurrent cooperation.

1.1.3 Reciprocity in the Relationship Marketing Literature

Relationship marketing (RM) entails a substantial stream of literature in the field of

marketing. In a compelling article on the relationship development process, Dwyer, Schurr and

Oh (1987) describe five stages that social actors engage in: (1) awareness, (2) exploration, (3)

expansion, (4) commitment, and (5) dissolution. Further, they explain that the exploration stage

is sub-divided into 5 processes including (1) attraction, (2) communication and bargaining, (3)

development and exercise of power, (4) norm development, and (5) expectation development.

While reciprocity is implicitly included in more stages than just the communication and

bargaining phase of the exploration stage, these authors specifically discuss the role of

reciprocity during this stage of the relationship. At that point in the relationship, both parties

have some interest in furthering the relationship, they have mutually decided to exchange over

some indeterminate period of time, and are in the process of exploring exactly how receptive

their counterpart will be to a mutually beneficial relationship. It is expected that parties will try

to reveal specific information about themselves, their needs, and or their resources. It is

Page 21: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

8

suggested that if the relationship is to endure, “intimate disclosure (of information) must be

reciprocated” (p. 16). Further it is suggested that later in the relationship, there may be a lesser

need for strict-reciprocal accounting in that the future holds ample opportunity for, and

expectations of, the balancing of outcomes over time.

Dwyer, Schurr and Oh (1987) also implicate reciprocity during the second phase of the

relationship building process, the exploration stage. While they acknowledge that one party

may have more power over (ability to mediate the rewards of) their partner, they detail that the

unjust use of power (for the selfish gain of the more powerful partner) will lead to quick

dissolution, whereas legitimate or just uses of power which benefit both parties can actually

strengthen the relationship. Further, during the norm and expectation development stages, it is

clear that norms and expectations which support mutual gains will develop a stronger

relationship than those which are self-serving. These authors suggest that trust begins to

develop during these stages, and is a critical component to relationship continuation. Trust

leads to higher levels of risk taking which encourage the norm of reciprocity (granting

concessions, proposing compromises, etc.).

The third relationship phase is the expansion phase. Here norms and behavioral

expectations have led to a trusting relationship. During the expansion phase, trusting partners

risk to expand the benefits provided to their partners (and themselves) which has the effect of

increasing the interdependence of the actors on one another. Increased interdependence of

both parties can lead to higher levels of trust, commitment and reduced conflict between the

parties (Kumar et al. 1995a). Finally, the last stage of continuing relationships is proposed to be

the commitment phase. Now existing relational norms, expectations of future behaviors, trust,

higher levels of risk taking and increased interdependence create an expectation and a desire

for both parties to maintain (or expand) the relationship as it provides beneficial outcomes to

Page 22: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

9

both partners in both an absolute sense, and in a relative sense (Thibaut and Kelley 1961).

While Dwyer, Schurr and Oh only briefly mention reciprocity in their theory, it is clear that it is

implied throughout the relationship development process. Each party makes investments

exhibiting increasing levels of risk, over time, to communicate their role as an effective partner

and build commitment and dependence from the other in a series of repeated interactions and

exchanges. Clearly this is the development of reciprocity between two social actors.

Shortly thereafter, Morgan and Hunt (1994) developed and tested their Commitment-

Trust theory of marketing relationships, also referred to as the Key-Mediating Variable model

(KMV), due to the key roles of commitment and trust as mediators in marketing relationships.

One of the most highly cited articles in marketing (Cited by 4488 according to Google Scholar),

Morgan and Hunt built on Dwyer, Schurr and Oh by suggesting that all relational investments

were mediated through commitment and trust which in turn impacted all relational outcomes.

Morgan and Hunt find support for the majority of their hypotheses. Some notable exceptions

were that relationship benefits did not tend to lead directly to commitment and that

opportunistic behavior exhibited a direct path to conflict (+) and uncertainty (+). They also note

the possibility that additional mediators may exist that they have failed to capture. Additionally,

their results suggest that firms acquiesce to their partner not necessarily for fear of power (prior

assumption) but because they are committed to their partner. Numerous extensions of the

KMV model have consistently found support for the general framework; however some more

recent work suggests that commitment and trust may not be the only mediators at play in

marketing relationships.

In a meta-analysis investigating the effectiveness of relationship marketing strategies,

Palmatier et al. (2006a) explore the Morgan and Hunt framework by compiling and analyzing

empirical results from 94 different published and unpublished manuscripts. In broad terms their

Page 23: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

10

framework was motivated by both Dwyer Schurr and Oh (1987) and Morgan and Hunt (1994) in

that they tested a model of RM strategies → relational mediators → outcomes. The meta-

analysis broadly finds support for the framework; however some interesting findings suggest

that there may be some missing mediators in this framework. Examples include a finding that

beyond the path through the relational mediators, dependence and relationship investments

had a direct effect on seller performance. It is difficult to suggest that this direct effect is not

mediated by some relational variable, as ceteris paribus financial investments in the relationship

should not lead directly to positive outcomes for the investor. One plausible path these

investments might take leading to outcomes for the investor is if their partner reciprocates the

investment through increased purchase, spreading positive word of mouth, or reductions in

opportunistic behavior. Similarly one would not expect that higher levels of dependence on a

seller would necessarily lead directly to better performance for the seller. Much research has

suggested that greater levels of asymmetric dependence (favoring a seller in this case) actually

lead to higher levels of conflict and possibly relationship dissolution without some compensating

mechanism to increase the dependence of the other partner (seller on the buyer). These

findings lead the authors to suggest that “… the extant relational-mediated framework is not

comprehensive and that additional mediators (e.g., reciprocity) must be investigated to explain

the impact of RM on performance fully” (p. 150). Additional support for this extension is

provided by Morgan and Hunt (1994) who find that opportunistic behavior displayed the largest

effects on outcome variables (both direct and indirect). While Morgan and Hunt argue that it

might be beneficial to allow for direct effects of opportunistic behavior, another interpretation

is that opportunistic behavior is not only mediated through commitment and trust, but possibly

another relational construct like reciprocity. Do relational partners punish when they feel their

partner has appropriated too much of the benefits of the relationship?

Page 24: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

11

Extending this work, Palmatier et al. (2009) find support for the role of gratitude and

gratitude based reciprocal behavior as mediators in the relationship marketing framework.

While this finding does have implications for an overall role of reciprocity, it is important to note

that they measure felt gratitude (described as short-lived) and resulting reciprocity based

behaviors. This research is positioned within RM episodes or cycles whereby the seller can see

the gratitude based response of the buyer over a series of individual exchanges. The authors

find support for the role of gratitude based reciprocal actions as a mediator between RM

investments and firm outcomes. Two important questions are brought up by the

conceptualization in this research: (1) is gratitude a necessary component for reciprocal

exchange; and (2) what might impact the gratitude based reciprocal response within a

relationship?

1.2 Primary Motivating Research Questions

An additional concern that is raised by extant marketing research is that the response to

negative or detrimental actions is not often considered in this research. It is unclear whether

the appropriate response would be some sort of punishment or retaliation, or whether

marketers should always “turn the other cheek” when faced with a detrimental situation. What

about the evidence for the role of negative reciprocity? Are we to assume that in all marketing

relationships, the best (only?) response to a partner’s self interested behavior is exit from the

relationship? Doesn’t reciprocity include the response to harmful actions as well as to beneficial

actions?

Figure #1 charts the domain of reciprocity research in marketing. The common

assumption in marketing (either explicitly, or implicitly) is that relationships are either

cooperatively built, based on positive reciprocity (Pervan et al. 2009), or that they will end up

dissolving. There are exceptions to this general trend; Kumar, Scheer and Steenkamp (1998)

Page 25: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

12

investigate the reciprocation of punitive actions in supplier-dealer relationships. Frazier and

Rody (1991), and Frazier and Summers (1986) find evidence of reciprocity by significant

correlations between supplier and dealer actions (both coercive and non-coercive strategies),

however Frazier, Gill and Kale (1989) find no evidence of reciprocal coercive actions from

dealers who have very few alternative suppliers. Interestingly, these findings might provide

some preliminary support for the idea that negative reciprocity (or returning a punitive act in

response to one) will result in the degradation of marketing relationships. We see that higher

levels of supplier punitive or coercive actions lead to similar responses from dealers. It appears

from these studies that responding to an “evil” with an “evil” may lead to a spiral of increased

retaliation and revenge. But does this finding always hold? What boundary conditions might

make retaliation less likely when one party commits a “punitive act”?

Figure #1. The Domain of Marketing Interactions

"Recipient" Response

Positive Negative

Perceived "Donor" Action

Positive Most

Marketing Research

Negative

Kumar et al. '98,

Frazier et al. *

Shaded Region - Reciprocity according to Becker * 1986, 1989, 1991

We all know from experience that any relationship has challenges and problems. Figure

#1 shows that the marketing reciprocity research has only studied reciprocity in a very small

Page 26: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

13

area of the possible interactions between exchange partners. The interesting interactions have

been left to behavioral economists in sterile laboratory settings. What happens when one

partner suspects he is being taken advantage of? Does a negative response to an unjustified

negative action have beneficial outcomes for the relationship or does it lead to the degeneration

of the social interaction? When is it viable to strategically avoid responding to a partner’s

actions? There are numerous questions which need to be addressed with respect to the role of

reciprocity in marketing relationships.

While there are numerous directions that would help build a better understanding of

reciprocity in marketing relationships, this dissertation focuses on two distinct research

perspectives. The first investigates the role of episodic reciprocity between relational partners.

This perspective approaches reciprocity as part of a single interaction between actors in a

marketing interaction. The specific research questions addressed are:

1) Is reciprocal debt created in marketing relationships?

2) What characteristics of an exchange will impact the reciprocal response to a given action?

3) How do business partners respond to detrimental partner-actions, and what factors

will mitigate any potential retaliation? The second perspective positions reciprocity as an ongoing relational process. This

approach proposes that reciprocal behavior in ongoing relationships is a multi-dimensional

process that can enhance relational outcomes. While it is important to investigate a response to

a given action, relationships happen over time and over the course of numerous interactions.

This research investigates the role of reciprocation over time within ongoing relationships and

asks some very important questions:

1) What is the role of reciprocity in the development of ongoing marketing relationships?

2) Which characteristics of a relationship and the business transactions impact reciprocity over time?

Page 27: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

14

1.3 Organization of the Dissertation

The remainder of this dissertation is organized into six chapters. The next chapter

provides a literature review of reciprocity theory and research from other disciplines. Chapter

three develops a general theory of episodic reciprocity. Chapter four describes the survey and

experimental research completed to investigate episodic reciprocity. Chapter five develops a

general theory of ongoing or relational reciprocity in marketing relationships, and provides

results from a survey designed to test the research hypotheses. Finally, chapter six discusses

the conclusions and implications of the dissertation research. References and appendices

including survey materials and cover letters are attached at the end of the document.

Page 28: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

15

CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW

“… we ought to be disposed, as a matter of moral character, to make reciprocity a moral obligation.” - Lawrence C. Becker

If 20 people were asked to define reciprocity, like a lot of abstract constructs, there

would be some general agreement between the definitions; however none of them would

adequately define the entire conceptual domain of the term. This problem is highlighted when

the academic literature on reciprocity is reviewed. Not only is there a lack of consensus of a

definition of reciprocity, but there is very little agreement on the way reciprocity is

conceptualized as well. Reciprocity has been a cultural phenomenon throughout recorded

history in nearly every society, as can be seen in ancient writings from a number of cultures, for

example:

“Never impose on others what you would not choose for yourself” - Analects of Confucius (circa 500 B.C., as cited in Wikipedia 2009)

Also, the Golden Rule – Do unto others as you would have done unto you - was adapted

from the Bible verse: “Thou shalt not avenge, nor bear any grudge against the children of thy

people, but thou shalt love thy neighbor as thyself…(Leviticus 19:18)”. In fact nearly every

religion espouses some version of this edict. Buddhism, Hinduism, Islam, Judaism and Taoism all

promote some derivative of the Golden Rule. While the Golden Rule itself is not the same as

reciprocity, the fundamental concept, that we can expect behavior similar to our own from

those we interact with, definitely relates to a general understanding of reciprocal behavior.

One of the most highly cited articles on reciprocity is Gouldner’s (1960) “The Norm of

Reciprocity”. Gouldner distinguishes between three distinct conceptual domains of reciprocity,

(1) reciprocal behavior, (2) existential folk beliefs in reciprocity, and (3) a moral norm of

reciprocity. Gouldner argues that the moral norm of reciprocity is defined by two important

Page 29: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

16

edicts: (a) people should help those who have helped them; and (b) people should not injure

those who have helped them. His argument is based on the principal that reciprocity promotes

stable and enduring relationships. The fact that a recipient of help should in turn help those

who contributed in the first place ensures some enduring debt that people rely on in the future

of the relationship. Further, the idea that the indebted partner should not harm the debtor, at

least until the debt is repaid, helps to create peace and order within society. This brings up an

interesting question, what role does punishment or retaliation (negative reciprocity) have in

reciprocal relationships? Gouldner argues that since social debts are never paid entirely in full,

there is always a reciprocal debt remaining and thus harm of the partner should be constrained.

While there is value in this position, we all know that in reality people do take revenge and often

to those closest to them (e.g., “Hell hath no fury like a woman scorned”).

In 1956, Howard Becker collected his introduction to sociology lectures from the

University of Wisconsin and printed them in a book titled “Man in Reciprocity” (Becker 1956). In

this lecture series, Becker relates the development of all human social structures to reciprocity.

Coining the term Homo Reciprocus, Becker relates the mores of ancient civilizations, the

development of the division of labor, teaching a child to be obedient, and everything in between

to reciprocity between social actors and or social institutions. Becker argues that all social

interactions can be interpreted based on the concept of reciprocity, although he intentionally

fails to offer a definition of reciprocity, leaving it to the reader to formulate their own. Becker

viewed reciprocity as responsible for teaching children socially acceptable behavior through

parental responses to both proper and improper actions. Warm smiles or a slap on the wrist

were responses to behavior that condition the child to repeat actions which were rewarded, and

avoid actions that resulted in punishment. Thus Becker acknowledges that reciprocation to

“negative” actions can help reinforce socially acceptable behavior.

Page 30: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

17

Another Becker, this one a philosopher with a first name of Lawrence (LB) disagrees

with Howard Becker, not on the prominence of reciprocity in social relationships, but rather on

the role of negative reciprocity in social relationships. LB’s book Reciprocity (1986) argues that

reciprocity is a moral norm. To gain a solid understanding of this philosophical definition of

reciprocity, I want to reprint the maxims from LB’s thesis which illustrate this normative

position, and the disagreement with the sociologist:

1. Good received should be returned with good. 2. Evil received should not be returned with evil. 3. Evil received should be resisted. 4. Evil done should be made good. 5. Returns and restitution should be made by the ones who have received the good or

done the evil, respectively. 6. Returns and restitution should be fitting and proportional. 7. Returns should be made for the good received – not merely for good accepted or

requested. 8. Reciprocation, as defined by 1-7, should be made a moral obligation.

LB’s position is argued based on the idea that anything that promotes good (rather than evil) is a

moral norm of which social actors should abide. LB argues that if evil is returned for evil

received, the result is an escalation of revenge and punishment leading to the eventual

degeneration of society as a whole. We can see in maxim #5 that LB proposes an alternative

reaction to “evil received” in that the evil-doer should make up for the evil deed by providing

“fitting and proportional” restitution to the injured party. Would that it was so, our entire legal

system might be out of work. Importantly to our building discussion on the definition of

reciprocity, we see that this normative definition suggests that negative reciprocity should not

be a part of healthy social relationships. How do we reconcile these different perspectives?

Perceptions of the recipient of a punishment must be an important consideration in

considering “evil” actions. If LB’s maxims were completely correct, society would have

degenerated by now. Instead, parents continue to punish children (even if not by spanking),

marital partners punish through withholding affection or attention, business partners can

Page 31: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

18

spread negative word of mouth, and if the perception is that the punishment is justified for prior

actions, we expect to see these relationships persist. LB hints at the value of recipient

perceptions in maxim #6 by stating that returns and restitution should be “fitting and

proportional”. While this is not a statement about negative reciprocity, it is a hint that

perceptions about the appropriateness of reciprocal reaction are important.

Gouldner supports this view that recipient perceptions are important in the valuation of

reciprocal responses to positive actions. He describes the valuation process of the help (and

hence the debt) received, and argues that it is affected by:

1. The intensity of the recipient’s need at the time the benefit is bestowed 2. The resources of the donor 3. The motives imputed to the donor by the recipient 4. Nature of constraints perceived to exist (costs to the donor)

Later empirical work has offered support for much of Gouldner’s theory. Motives were found to

reduce reciprocal responses when they were perceived as inappropriate (Lerner and Lichtman

1968; Schopler and Thompson 1968). Additionally, subjects who were intentionally under-

rewarded tended to reciprocate less, and evaluated their exchange partners more poorly, than

subjects who were under-rewarded “by chance” (Leventhal et al. 1969). Importantly, this

suggests that reciprocal responses to actions will vary depending on the recipient’s perceptions.

Another perspective on reciprocity is that some people exhibit a resistance to take on a

reciprocal debt. Brehm and Cole (1966) termed this concept psychological reactance which is

defined as the resistance to a felt loss of freedom, resulting in hostility and acting out.

Berkowitz (1973) found that reactance is increased when a request for help is perceived as

“improper”. A vast amount of literature in psychology focuses on the impact of recipient

perceptions on reciprocal (positive and negative) responses and ratings of the donor.

To review, there is much agreement that reciprocity includes the return of some

benefits for benefits received. There is disagreement about the role of negative reciprocity, or

Page 32: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

19

retaliation. Some theorists suggest that returning evil for evil received should be avoided

(Becker 1986; Gouldner 1960), others suggest that punishing can actually be beneficial to the

development of acceptable social norms (Becker 1956; Michaels 1983). There is much

agreement that reciprocal responses involve perceptual valuation of the action (positive or

negative) received (Bolton et al. 1998; Eisenberger et al. 1987), and the motives at play by the

donor (Leventhal et al. 1969; Michaels 1983; Rabin 1993). There are definite implications that

reciprocity occurs over time, and many authors suggest that the waiting time until reciprocal

debts are repaid actually supports a positive social interaction as both parties have an interest in

not harming the other at least until the debt has been repaid. Evidence also suggests that

reciprocal responses can vary depending on the situation, and that some people (or situations)

will exhibit a resistance to accepting a reciprocal debt. However, very few articles offer any

explicit definition of the term, and the few that do only capture some subset of the concepts

discussed to this point (see Table #3 below).

Page 33: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Au

tho

rD

efi

nit

ion

Dis

cip

lin

eR

ole

of

reci

pro

city

Pro

acti

veR

eac

tive

Ho

war

d

Be

cke

r 19

56n

on

eSo

cio

logy

Man

is b

orn

inh

um

an, o

nly

th

rou

gh t

he

p

roce

ss o

f le

arn

ing

reci

pro

city

(i

nte

rpe

rso

nal

, in

ter-

inst

itu

tio

nal

, in

ter-

gro

up

, etc

.) d

o h

om

o s

apie

ns

be

com

e

hu

man

(h

om

o-r

ecip

roco

us

)

Ind

ivid

ual

s ad

he

re

to n

orm

s to

avo

id

ne

gati

ve r

esp

on

ses

asso

ciat

ed

wit

h

no

rm v

iola

tio

n

Pe

op

le in

po

siti

on

s o

f au

tho

rity

pu

nis

h

for

no

rm v

iola

tio

ns

to r

ein

forc

e

"pro

pe

r" b

eh

avio

r

Go

uld

ne

r 19

60

A p

atte

rn o

f m

utu

ally

co

nti

nge

nt

exc

han

ge

grat

ific

atio

ns

Soci

olo

gy

The

re

cip

ien

t o

f "h

elp

" h

as a

mo

ral

ob

liga

tio

n t

o r

etu

rn t

he

he

lp t

o t

he

d

on

or.

Th

e d

on

or

of

"he

lp"

has

a m

ora

l ri

ght

to r

ece

ive

he

lp f

rom

th

e r

eci

pie

nt

in t

he

fu

ture

.

Re

cip

roci

ty a

cts

as a

"s

tart

ing

me

chan

ism

" to

e

nab

le in

itia

l e

xch

ange

The

ob

liga

tio

n t

o

no

t h

arm

th

ose

wh

o

hav

e h

elp

ed

yo

u

cre

ate

s so

cial

so

lid

arit

y

Mac

ne

il

1980

, 198

6

Ge

ttin

g so

me

thin

g b

ack

for

som

eth

ing

give

n -

ge

ne

rali

zed

, n

on

-sp

eci

aliz

ed

, an

d

spe

cial

ize

d

Law

an

d

Eth

ics

All

ow

s m

an t

o b

alan

ce h

is c

on

flic

tin

g co

nce

rns

in li

fe f

or

self

-in

tere

st a

nd

fo

r so

cial

so

lid

arit

y. W

ith

ou

t re

cip

roci

ty,

man

wo

uld

no

t b

e a

ble

to

re

solv

e

inte

rnal

co

nfl

ict

ove

r th

ese

tw

o

con

cern

s.

Re

cip

roci

ty e

nab

les

hu

man

s to

be

ab

le

to "

give

" to

oth

ers

, w

ith

th

e

exp

ect

atio

n o

f re

turn

Law

ren

ce

Be

cke

r 19

86

8 m

axim

s -

see

pag

e

12 in

th

e t

ext

Ph

ilo

sop

hy

A s

oci

al m

ech

anis

m t

hat

en

able

s th

e

de

velo

pm

en

t o

f h

eal

thy

soci

al r

ela

tio

ns,

an

d f

urt

he

r d

isco

ura

ges

de

stru

ctiv

e

soci

al in

tera

ctio

ns

A r

esp

on

se b

y b

oth

th

e s

en

de

r an

d

rece

ive

r o

f go

od

an

d e

vil a

ctio

ns

Feh

r an

d

Gac

hte

r 19

98

Po

siti

ve -

be

kin

d t

o

tho

se w

ho

hav

e b

ee

n

kin

d t

o u

s. N

ega

tive

-

'an

eye

fo

r an

eye

'

Be

hav

iora

l Ec

on

om

ics

A m

ech

anis

m t

hat

en

cou

rage

s e

qu

ity

(of

som

e s

ort

) in

exc

han

ges,

evi

de

nce

d b

y p

eo

ple

s' w

illi

ngn

ess

to

incu

r p

ers

on

al

cost

s to

re

war

d o

r p

un

ish

oth

ers

A r

esp

on

se b

y a

reci

pie

nt

of

eit

he

r a

po

siti

ve o

r n

ega

tive

ac

t

Tab

le #

3. A

pp

roac

he

s to

Re

cip

roci

ty t

ake

n in

Se

min

al W

ork

s

roset
Typewritten Text
20
Page 34: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

21

CHAPTER 3: A GENERAL THEORY OF EPISODIC RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS

3.1 Episodic Reciprocity Abstract

Relationship marketing research relies on the implicit inclusion of reciprocation. One

relational member (typically the seller) performs some action that is intended to elicit a

response from their customer. The assumption is that investments in relationship marketing

programs will be reciprocated back to the seller through increased purchases, increased loyalty,

or improved relationship quality. In reviewing the extant marketing literature, it is obvious that

reciprocity is neither clearly defined, nor accurately measured within marketing relationships.

Furthermore, the role of reciprocity in the process of relational exchange has been overlooked in

the marketing literature. This research will address these issues. Building on social exchange,

reciprocity and relationship marketing theories, a process model of reciprocal exchange is

proposed. Factors which should influence reciprocal responses to both beneficial, and harmful,

actions are proposed. The role of the norm of reciprocity, and its influence on reciprocal

responses will be tested. Further, both positive and negative reciprocal debts are

conceptualized and measured as outcome variables in marketing relationships. This research

will be empirically tested through both a survey in a B2B setting, and a lab based experiment to

test the proposed process model of episodic reciprocal exchange.

3.2 Episodic Reciprocity Introduction

In proposing the Commitment-Trust theory of relationship marketing (RM), Morgan and

Hunt (1994) developed a framework that described how relational characteristics between

partners lead to trust and commitment, and how these key relational mediators impact dyadic

outcomes. This approach focused less on specific behaviors and more on the overall dynamics

Page 35: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

22

that define the marketing relationship. These are more enduring characteristics of the

relationship including the dependence structure, shared values, communication and

opportunistic behavior measured at a global relationship level. While it is clear that

commitment and trust are highly correlated with beneficial relational outcomes, the

commitment-trust theory did little to suggest specific actions business partners could utilize to

improve the relationship outcomes.

More recently, RM researchers have approached relationship marketing as a strategy

enacted by relational partners to build and strengthen long term business relationships (Bagozzi

1995; Cannon and Perreault Jr 1999; Gronroos 1999; Jap et al. 1999; Palmatier 2008; Palmatier

et al. 2007a). These researchers attempt to categorize investments in RM and to quantify

differential impacts of RM investments on financial outcomes (usually for the “selling” firm) of

the dyad. RM categories have been defined as financial, structural and social (Berry 1995;

Palmatier et al. 2008; Palmatier et al. 2007a), and research investigating the financial impact of

these different categories of RM suggest that they have unique effects on financial outcomes

(Palmatier et al. 2006b). For instance, social RM efforts had the largest direct effect on

customer specific returns (a proxy for the contribution margin a rep firm earns from a specific

customer), structural RM investments had the largest impact for firms that had frequent

interactions, and financial RM investments did not exhibit any direct effects on customer

specific returns.

While both perspectives are valuable for understanding business relationships, both the

Commitment Trust and the Relationship Marketing approaches fail to capture the process which

transforms one member’s actions (investments, behavior, etc.) into financial outcomes for

either themselves, or their partner. RM assumes that financial, social or structural investments

will result in financial benefits not only for the investor, but also for their partner. This research

Page 36: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

23

argues that it is important to understand the process which translates these investments into

objective performance outcomes for either partner. Even if trust and commitment are directly

impacted by RM investments, what process extracts financial returns from trust and

commitment? In order for any business to increase their outcomes based on relationship

investments, their partner must in some way repay that effort. RM research implicitly relies on

reciprocal exchange of investment and return from marketing relationships (e.g., Doney and

Cannon 1997; Li and Dant 1997; Morgan and Hunt 1994); however that process is rarely defined

nor explicitly measured in the extant literature. In fact, authors of a recent meta-analysis

suggest that “integrating reciprocity into the relational-mediating framework may also explain

the large, direct effect of relationship investment on performance, such that people’s inherent

desire to repay “debts” generated by sellers’ investments may lead to performance-enhancing

behaviors, independent of trust or commitment” (Palmatier et al. 2006a, pg. 152).

This research posits that the process of reciprocity is a critical component to marketing

exchange. While reciprocity is mentioned often in the marketing literature, the failure to

accurately conceptualize, define or measure the role of reciprocation has resulted in a lack of

understanding of the process of reciprocation in the development of marketing relationships.

Building off of reciprocity, relationship marketing and social exchange theories, this research will

investigate the role of reciprocal behavior, norms of reciprocity, and relational characteristics

proposed to affect reciprocal exchange.

3.3 Episodic Reciprocity Conceptual Development

Relationships do not happen at one point in time; they are dynamic, evolving and

occurring over time. Reciprocal exchange occurs over several interactions within a given

relationship (Bagozzi 1995; Macneil 1980). Given this understanding, it would be appropriate to

consider all actions within a relationship as reciprocal actions. In fact Gouldner (1960) suggests

Page 37: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

24

that the norm of reciprocity encourages the first trusting act by one party to a potential

relationship by reducing the risk that their efforts will be taken advantage of; every action after

this first effort is considered a reciprocal response to prior actions.

What types of actions are being referred to? Given that relationships are dynamic and

occurring over time, expectations are developed for future interactions based on prior exchange

occasions. The status quo becomes the expected range of actions over time as the relationship

develops and partners become more familiar and comfortable with each other in the exchange

process (Bagozzi 1975; Dwyer et al. 1987; Morgan and Hunt 1994). Actions can easily be

categorized any number of ways. This research distinguishes between beneficial actions and

detrimental actions. Beneficial actions are defined as any action which is perceived to have a

more positive effect than expected given the status quo of the relationship (e.g., an unexpected

price discount offered to a customer, supplier investments in an integrated distribution system

that increases customer profitability, etc.). Similarly, a detrimental action is defined as any

action which is perceived to have a more negative effect than expected given the status quo of

the relationship (e.g., unexpected reductions in product quality due to cost increases, reduction

in sales force which causes service quality deterioration, etc.). Obviously these actions do not

cover the normal interactions involved with making repeated purchases, paying accounts

receivable, or making a regular periodic sales call on a customer.

Relationship marketing research has focused on the beneficial actions as investments

(of financial or non-financial resources) that a seller makes to build a stronger relationship with

its customers (Anderson and Weitz 1992; Bowman and Narayandas 2004; Palmatier et al.

2006b). Other research has examined how detrimental actions can affect relational outcomes

(Frazier and Rody 1991; Hibbard et al. 2001; Kumar et al. 1998). While much of this research

suggests that beneficial actions lead to more positive exchange outcomes, and detrimental

Page 38: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

25

actions lead to more negative outcomes, the extant marketing literature fails to capture the

process which enables specific actions to affect firm outcomes, or what relationship specific

characteristics might impact the response to beneficial or detrimental actions by one’s business

partner.

Business relationships involve prior social history, and expectations of future exchange

(Jap et al. 1999; Weitz and Jap 1995), however much (if not all) of the empirical work dedicated

to measuring the role of reciprocity in social exchange has examined interactions between

strangers with no social history, and no expectation of future interactions. The primary context

for the existing reciprocity research is a sterile lab environment, where researchers often

intentionally eliminate any chance of prior social interactions. Research set in the context of

existing social relationships would provide a fertile ground to test theory about the role of

reciprocity in social interactions. Building off social exchange theory, and incorporating

empirical results from reciprocal dyadic exchanges (Fehr and Gachter 2000; Fehr and Gachter

1998; Loch and Wu 2008) into the theoretical RM framework of social relationships, this

research will investigate factors which impact reciprocal responses to a partner’s action within

the framework of established marketing relationships.

Substantial empirical evidence exists for various factors which affect reciprocal

exchange, however very little investigates reciprocity within a social relationship. That is, most

empirical research involves interactions between total strangers. Acknowledging that a social

history may impact reciprocal behavior, Loch and Wu (2008) used a manipulation in a lab setting

to simulate a social history. In the social condition, they allowed participants a brief

introduction to get acquainted and then asked each participant to read the following paragraph:

“You have already met the person with whom you will play the game. Now the person is no longer a stranger to you. You can imagine that the other player is a good friend. You have a good relationship and like each other” (p. 1837).

Page 39: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

26

In the control condition, players interacted anonymously, separated throughout the study. In a

sequential move game, even with this weak manipulation, they find higher levels of trust and

positive reciprocation in the social history condition than the control condition. On the other

hand, Fehr and Gachter (2000) find that (lab manipulated) “partners” actually punish more

severely for norm violations than do strangers, and Chen, Chen and Portnoy (2009) find that

friends (existing student friendships) respond less positively to unfavorable, inequitable offers

than do complete strangers. All of these examples illustrate that a history of social interactions

(even if those interactions are imagined) will impact future reciprocation between “partners”.

Does this suggest that the existence of a social relationship simply magnifies the level of

reciprocal reactions to partner efforts? Without understanding perceptions of the actions, and

the underlying relationship dynamics, it is difficult to determine with certainty how these

findings will apply to marketing relationships.

Additionally, we know very little about the existence or role of negative reciprocity in

marketing relationships. The relationship marketing literature advocates the philosophical,

normative positive role of reciprocity, suggesting that evil not be responded to with evil (Bagozzi

1995; Pervan et al. 2009), but without testing responses to negative actions within functional

relationships it is difficult to assume that avoiding punishment always provides the best

solution. In fact, some analytical work from the behavioral economics literature suggests that

“unconditional cooperation” can result in overall deterioration of relational networks as those

parties who tend to reciprocate positively will eventually tire of being taken advantage of (Sethi

and Somanathan 2003). These inconsistencies suggest that the true range and role of reciprocal

responses in marketing relationships is not fully understood. Does negative reciprocity impact

marketing relationships? How will partners react to detrimental actions by their counterpart?

What relational dynamics will affect these reciprocal responses? The majority of evidence for

Page 40: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

27

influences on reciprocal responses lies within the field of economics, where unfortunately social

histories and expectations of future interactions have been designed away (Eisenberger et al.

1987; Falk et al. 1999).

3.3.1 Reciprocal Action – Debt Response Cycles

Reciprocity has been defined a variety of different ways in the marketing literature.

Numerous authors have defined reciprocity in line with Becker’s (1986) conceptualization of

returning good for good received, and not harming a relational counterpart, even if you have

been harmed by them (Bagozzi 1995; Palmer 2002; Pervan et al. 2009; Pervan and Johnson

2003). Others have defined reciprocity either explicitly or implicitly (through their

measurements) as the existence of similar tactics used by members of a marketing relationship

(Clayson 2004; Kumar et al. 1998; Lee et al. 2008). Interestingly, in a stream of research by

Frazier and colleagues (Frazier et al. 1989; Frazier and Rody 1991; Frazier and Summers 1986),

the authors define reciprocity as actions taken by one party in response to the actions taken by

the other party in an exchange relationship, however their empirical evidence for the existence

of reciprocity is a correlation between buyers and sellers’ use of coercive tactics. While they

define reciprocity as being any action which is induced by the action of a partner, they measure

it by only “in-kind” returns of potentially destructive behavior.

This research will adopt the definition of reciprocity offered by Frazier and colleagues

as: actions taken by one party in response to the actions taken by their counterpart in an

exchange relationship. This definition of reciprocal behavior is also supported by Houston and

Gassenheimer (1987), who define reciprocity as “a social interaction in which the movement of

one party evokes a compensating movement in some other party” (pg 11). Importantly this

definition of reciprocal behavior does not require that the response to any action is necessarily

an action “in-kind.” Note that is also does not imply that reciprocation necessarily takes place

Page 41: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

28

immediately. In fact, the time lapse between the acceptance and repayment of a reciprocal

debt may be an important component of reciprocity (Becker 1956; Becker 1986).

Reciprocity theory argues that actions by one member of a relationship create in their

counterpart a reciprocal debt that must be repaid in the future in order for the relationship to

endure (Cialdini and Rhoads 2001; Gouldner 1960; Macneil 1980). When this reciprocal debt

involves the repayment of some beneficial action, it is suggested that the existence of a debt can

actually help to build social solidarity, or the degree of integration in society. This is so because

during the elapsed time between the incurrence and the repayment of the debt, it is expected

that both parties will avoid harming the other (Macneil 1980). Gouldner (1960) claims that

during this time period, when debts have yet to be repaid, the person owing the debt is “morally

constrained to manifest their gratitude toward, or at least to maintain peace with, their

benefactors” (pg 174). When the reciprocal debt involves the return of a benefit or favor, it is

contrary to the self interest of both parties to harm the other: the debtor avoids harming the

creditor for fear of reduced chances of gaining a benefit in future interactions; and the creditor

because it may reduce the chances of the existing debt being fulfilled. It is exactly this process

which Macneil (1986) argues enables reciprocity to satisfy the dual, conflicting interest of

“schizophrenic” man: self interest and social solidarity. Cialdini (2009) claims that the social

debt, or future obligation, created by the rule of reciprocity is directly responsible for the unique

social advantages of the human race.

Evidence suggests that these social debts are actually consciously recognized by

relational partners, and have predictive power over behavior in future exchange (Frenzen and

Davis 1990). Frenzen and Davis (1990) find that individuals who claim to owe a social debt to a

sales host are more likely to purchase in a home selling context than are those who claim no

existing debt. In this study, the debts referred to were prior home selling events during which

Page 42: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

29

the current host either purchased from the individual (causing a social debt needing to be repaid

in the future), or did not purchase (and hence created no reciprocal debt). Further evidence for

the existence of reciprocal debts is shown through an experimental selling context, during which

customers actually reported feeling guilty if they did not make a purchase from a salesperson

when social connectedness exists, and often returned to make a future purchase from that

salesperson to satisfy their reciprocal debt (Dahl et al. 2005). This feeling of guilt was also found

in a consumer relationship management context (Palmatier et al. 2009) when consumers were

unable to repay reciprocal debts. Eisenberger, Cotterell and Marvel (1987) find that recipients

of help or favors experience a feeling of obligation and indebtedness. Cialdini (2009) argues that

any benefits received from a relationship create debts which require reciprocation to be

fulfilled.

Based on reciprocity theory, positive reciprocal debt is defined as: the conscious

recognition that a benefit is owed to a relational partner in repayment of a benefit received. It

is important to note that there are response options available to someone after they receive

some beneficial treatment from there partner that would fulfill the reciprocal debt that is

created. The assumption of course is that in a given relationship, if person A does something

that benefits person B, then at some point in the future B will do something to benefit A. This

would be a mirrored response strategy. Alternatively, it is possible that A could eliminate B’s

debt through some counter-valenced compensatory action. For example maybe A takes more

than their fair share in a future interaction with B. Extant research has focused on the mirrored

response strategies, but a counter-valenced strategy is also a possibility. These possible

responses are illustrated below in figure #2:

Page 43: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

30

While it is acknowledged that reciprocity can work both upstream and downstream in a

marketing relationship, consistent with the relationship marketing literature this research will

focus on a selling firm’s actions and their customer’s response. Based on the prior discussion,

the following hypothesis is offered:

H1: Beneficial actions by a selling firm will lead to a positive reciprocal debt felt by their customer. So what are the expected outcomes to these positive reciprocal debts? Theorists agree

that the existence of positive reciprocal debts helps to ensure the health of the relationship

(Becker 1986; Gouldner 1960; Macneil 1980). Positive reciprocal debts serve at least two

beneficial purposes in a relationship, the first is that at some point the debt will be repaid,

helping enforce the norm of reciprocity and a healthy exchange relationship (Macneil 1986;

Macneil 1980). Additionally, it is argued that the existence of positive reciprocal debts create a

barrier to harmful actions in that both parties are motivated to maintain the peace, at least until

the debt has been repaid (Gouldner 1960). It has also been suggested that in most

relationships, the balance of existing reciprocal debts is never exactly paid so that there always

remains a reciprocal debt to help ensure the health of the relationship (Becker 1986).

Relationship marketing theory relies on customer reciprocation of seller relationship

investments (Morgan and Hunt 1994). Selling firms invest in financial, social or structural

relationship marketing programs relying on the idea that their customer will reciprocate those

A’s Action Benefits B

B’s Positive Reciprocal Debt

B’s Action Benefits A

A’s Action Benefits A

or

Figure #2 Positive Reciprocal Debt Responses

Page 44: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

31

efforts through increased loyalty, order frequency and quantity, and possibly through positive

word of mouth (Palmatier et al. 2006a). While the role of reciprocal debts has not been

explicitly investigated in marketing research, evidence suggests that relationship marketing

investments do provide returns to selling firms through increased sales, share of wallet, and

reduced customer switching behavior. If seller initiated beneficial actions do translate into

improved seller performance, the existence and subsequent satisfaction of reciprocal debts

must explain some of the gains for selling firms. Therefore, I expect that:

H2: Customer positive reciprocal debt will lead to increased selling firm outcomes.

But what should be expected of detrimental actions? We have established that

reciprocity does not only refer to the return of benefits gained for benefits received. Based on

the definition of reciprocal behavior as actions in response to those of a relational partner, we

must also consider how a customer will respond to detrimental actions in a relationship. Becker

(1986) argues that evil actions should not be responded to with evil, however empirical

evidence suggests otherwise. Numerous marketing researchers have found that potentially

harmful actions by one partner are often correlated with similar harmful actions by their

counterpart, and interestingly, these relationships have survived despite the apparent negative

reciprocation. Kumar, Scheer and Steenkamp (1998) find that the use of punitive capabilities by

a channel member is positively correlated with similar use by its channel partner. Similarly,

Frazier and colleagues (Frazier and Summers 1984; Frazier and Summers 1986) find that dealers

and manufacturers’ use of coercive tactics are highly correlated. In an interpersonal setting,

Clayson (2004) finds that students who received grades lower than they expected in class

tended to evaluate their professors more severely than those whose expectations were met,

suggesting a type of punishment for the perceived mistreatment.

Page 45: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

32

Experimental evidence also suggests that people will punish for perceived detrimental

actions. For example, Greenglass (1969) found that participants whose task performance was

hindered were likely to exert effort to hinder “similar-others” in the future. In another

experiment, when the experimenter divulged that participants were to be intentionally

underpaid for their participation, these individuals punished the experimenter by under-

performing compared to those who were “unintentionally” underpaid (Michaels 1983).

Evidence suggests that this tendency to punish for detrimental actions persists even when the

absolute outcomes favor the individual, for example Keysar, Converse, Jiunwen and Epley

(2008) find that when the same outcomes are framed as a partner taking some of their

counterpart’s endowment, those counterparts tend to punish in future interactions, however if

the exchange is framed as the partner giving some of their own endowment, future exchange

shows evidence of positive reciprocity. This suggests that in response to a perceived

detrimental action, and given the opportunity to do so, people may retaliate or punish their

partner. While it is possible that this response happens immediately, it should not always be the

case. The opportunity when a fitting and proportional punishment is available may take time to

develop.

Negative reciprocal debt is therefore defined as: the intent to respond to a detrimental

action through either (a) punishing the responsible party, or (b) by acquiring extra benefits from

the responsible party at some point in the future. It should be noted that the two options

offered above provide two distinct routes to the satisfaction of the reciprocal debt. The first is a

mirrored response strategy whereby the harmed party punishes their partner in the future.

Alternatively, they would expect their partner to make up for the detrimental treatment by

providing some compensation to them in the future. In both cases, the focal partner has been

slighted and expects something to occur to make up for the mistreatment. However it is

Page 46: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

33

acknowledged that the route to the satisfaction of that reciprocal debt may depend on

characteristics of the person, and of the situation. These response strategies are illustrated

below in Figure #3:

Based on the prior discussion, this research proposes the following hypothesis regarding

negative reciprocal debts:

H3: Detrimental actions by a selling firm will lead to a negative reciprocal debt felt by their customer. While customer positive reciprocal debt is expected to lead to improved selling firm

outcomes, the opposite is true for negative reciprocal debts. Becker (1986) argues that negative

reciprocity will actually lead to the deterioration of social exchange. While I acknowledge that

negative reciprocity could lead to exit strategies, the reality is that all relationships experience

detrimental actions at one time or another. People also have some expectation that negative

reciprocation is a possible outcome if they harm their partner in a relationship (Becker 1956;

Perugini et al. 2003). Importantly if a customer’s negative reciprocal debt is resolved or

satisfied, the process of resolution will cause costs to the selling firm. These costs may or may

not be financial in nature, but over the course of the relationship, increased levels of negative

reciprocal debts should result in decreased relational outcomes for the selling firm.

H4: Customer negative reciprocal debt will lead to decreased selling firm outcomes.

A’s Action Harms B

B’s Negative Reciprocal Debt

B’s Action Harms A

A’s Action Benefits B

or

Figure #3 Negative Reciprocal Debt Responses

Page 47: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

34

But these first two hypotheses are really uninteresting aren’t they? Isn’t this the

intuitive understanding of reciprocity? With the exception of introducing a measurable,

conscious debt created by a partner firm, this is exactly what we would expect in relationships:

people return benefits received with a similar magnitude of benefits, and they also punish

“misbehavior” in social relationships. The more interesting implications of reciprocity remain:

Why would responses differ in magnitude from the action they are meant to reciprocate? What

aspects of the action and relationship will enhance, or suppress one party’s intent to repay a

positive or a negative debt? When is it likely that a specific reciprocal debt is not recognized?

These are the interesting questions that will be dealt with in the remainder of this research.

3.3.2 Factors that impact the formation of Reciprocal Debt

In line with past marketing research (Anderson and Weitz 1992; Jap and Ganesan 2000) I

view perceptions to be a key determinant of behavior in marketing relationships. In discussing

the valuation of reciprocal responses, Becker (1986) argues that creditor perceptions of the

value of a reciprocal response are important to satisfying the existing reciprocal debt. Gouldner

(1960) views perceptions from the opposite side of the transaction, the debtor’s, but would

agree, stating that obligations to repay beneficial acts are contingent on the perceived value of

the benefit received.

3.3.2.1 Nature of the Action

It is important to highlight that perceptions of any action will be affected by the

absolute value (or cost in the case of detrimental actions), but could also be impacted by some

relative valuation. Gouldner argues that the recipient’s perception of the value of a benefit

impacts the magnitude of debt she feels towards the creditor. He elaborates by suggesting that

perceptions of the value of the benefit will be impacted by the resources available to the

creditor, the motives imputed to the creditor, and the nature of constraints perceived to exist or

Page 48: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

35

be absent. This suggests that there could be multiple relative valuation processes taking place.

For example, even a small gift from a child may be priceless to a parent, whereas an expensive

wedding gift from a rich relative may not be valued as highly by the recipient. Experimental

evidence supports this contention as well: Eisenberger, Cotterell and Marvel (1987) find that

experimental participants responded more favorably when the same amount given represented

a larger proportion of their partner’s available resources.

In a business relationship, it is possible that the relative outcomes for the recipient

when compared with the outcomes to the benefactor of a gift may be the most important

perception affecting the valuation of any action. In this context, beneficial relational

investments may often directly or indirectly benefit both partners. An example may be the

investment of an integrated distribution system by a selling firm. From the customer’s

perspective, the value of this investment may significantly depend on their perceptions of how

much it will benefit their own firm relative to the selling firm. When they perceive the selling

firm as benefitting as much (or more) than themselves, it is likely that the perceived value of this

relational investment will be reduced. Experimental research has shown that individuals do not

respond positively to gifts which they perceive as a direct attempt to benefit their partner

through the creation of a reciprocal debt (Rynning 1989). Additionally, Bolton and Ockenfels

(2000) find that a common strategy in games is to strive for equity in the relative payoff division.

In the context of games, this suggests that if one party benefits more from the transaction than

the other, equity considerations reduce the likelihood of positive reciprocation. Therefore the

magnitude of any positive reciprocal debt may depend not only on the absolute magnitude of

the benefit received, but also on the relative benefit to the customer. As the customer

perceives the selling firm to benefit more from a given action than the customer, it is likely that

their perceived reciprocal debt will be diminished, suggesting that:

Page 49: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

36

H5a: As the customer’s perceived relative benefits increase, the link between a beneficial action and a positive reciprocal debt will be enhanced. When faced with a detrimental action instigated by the selling firm, it seems likely that

the perceived relative costs of the action will also be important factors. Hibbard, Kumar and

Stern (2001) defined a destructive act as “an action that is perceived by the aggrieved channel

member as having a significant negative impact on the viability or functioning of the affected

firm” (pg. 46). This implies that some additional costs have been incurred by the affected firm;

examples could include increased financial costs, decreased convenience, or any change that is

viewed as detrimental to the firm. It is likely that the selling firm may experience similar

increased costs due to necessary policy changes. An example may be increased costs of supply

that must be carried down the channel to the customer. If the customer perceives that the

selling firm has also experienced a decrease in profit due to higher supply prices, they may

object less to the increased prices they face. On the other hand, if the customer perceives that

the selling firm has encountered minimal additional costs (financial or otherwise) relative to

their own, they may view this new policy as a destructive act and their negative reciprocal debt

may increase. As with beneficial acts, it is expected that the response to detrimental acts will be

affected not only by the absolute costs incurred by the act, but also by the relative costs the

customer faces compared to the selling firm:

H5b: As the customer’s perceived relative costs increase, the link between a detrimental action and a negative reciprocal debt will be enhanced. 3.3.2.2 Responsibility for Action

Both beneficial and detrimental actions can occur for a number of reasons. Partners to

a relationship may see a positive outcome due to intentional efforts by their counterpart, or

environmental factors which naturally affect both members. More importantly, customers may

perceive that the control of these relational outcomes can be attributed to the selling firm, or

Page 50: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

37

possibly some external entity. How would we expect reciprocity to interplay with these

perceptions? In a buyer-seller relationship, selling firm-attributed actions may include

relationship specific investments that benefit both parties (positive) or an unexplained price

increase (negative). Alternatively, a customer may attribute the control of a particular action

externally, for example the elimination of minimum pricing policies could be attributed to

governmental regulation instead of the seller. Obviously it should be expected that these

attributions of responsibility for inter-relational actions would impact reciprocal responses.

Becker’s (1986) 5th maxim states returns and restitution should be made by the ones who

have received the good or done the evil, respectively. This suggests that positive and negative

reciprocal debts should be directed at a specific target: that entity deemed responsible for the

beneficial or detrimental act. In marketing, perceived salesperson control has been found to

increase the positive effect of RM programs on the customer’s relationship quality with the

salesperson, and similarly, RM programs attributed to the firm tend to increase relationship

quality with the firm (Palmatier et al. 2007a). Cialdini (2009) suggests that reciprocal debts will

be repaid to the perceived provider of the benefit. It is clear that attributions about who is

responsible for relationship building activities will impact relational outcomes. This implies that

obligations to repay (either good or evil) will be directed towards the perceived responsible

party. However, it is possible that both beneficial and detrimental actions could be partially

attributed to external entities. Even if a beneficial action is mediated by the selling firm (e.g. the

partial pass through of a price reduction by the selling firm’s supplier), it is likely that the

positive reciprocal debt felt towards the selling firm will be reduced. With respect to

detrimental actions, Hibbard, Kumar and Stern (2001) find that actions attributed to external

forces are more likely to be responded to by passive acceptance and less likely to be responded

to with detrimental behavior (defined here as disengagement or venting), suggesting that

Page 51: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

38

external attributions can reduce the negative reciprocal debt directed towards the selling firm.

Therefore the following hypotheses are offered:

H6a: As customer external attribution of the control of beneficial acts increases, the link between the beneficial action and a positive reciprocal debt will be suppressed. H6b: As customer external attribution of the control of detrimental acts increases, the link between the detrimental action and a negative reciprocal debt will be suppressed. 3.3.2.3 Justifiability of Action

“A conspicuous slight without justification produces anger and an impulse toward revenge, whose fulfillment produces pleasure” (as quoted in Eisenberger et al. 2004, pp. 787) The justifiability of a destructive act is also an important perception which will impact

reciprocal response. Recent reciprocity research (Pervan et al. 2009) defined whether an action

is evil based on the perceptions of how justifiable the action is. In an organizational context, it is

easy to imagine that a price increase could be perceived as a harmful action, however if the

customer understands that the seller’s price increase was necessary to continue offering the

same quality of service, they may view that “evil” action as justified. Support for the importance

of this perception has also been shown empirically when investigating civilian opinions about

prison abuse during wartime (Eder et al. 2006). In this study, Eder and colleagues found that

when civilians believed that prisoner abuse was justified during wartime, they believed that

punishing the American soldiers responsible for the abuse was inappropriate. Conversely, those

who viewed the prison abuse as unjustified felt that punishing those responsible was an

appropriate response. Detrimental actions could be perceived as justifiable if they are a

response to a failure to act within the norms of a given relationship.

Becker (1956) highlights this fact when discussing the role of punishments in

conditioning children to behave within acceptable bounds. Additionally, detrimental acts could

be perceived as justifiable if external factors are to blame. Clearly if an action, even a

detrimental action, is perceived as justified, the expected or appropriate response will be

Page 52: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

39

different than for one that is perceived as unjustified. It would not be surprising to find that a

measure of justifiability may be highly correlated with a measure of “external control” when

considering a detrimental action. In fact, some researchers have defined justifiable, detrimental

actions as such. Importantly this research will not rule out the possibility that other factors (e.g.,

expectations based on past behavior) might influence a perception that a detrimental action is

justified. Also note that beneficial actions could be perceived as inappropriate (as in a bribe),

however it is not expected that this concept will be as prevalent in a business relationship, and

particularly not with the research design as planned, therefore justifiability is only considered

for destructive acts.

H7: As the customer’s perception of the justifiability of a detrimental act increases, the link between detrimental actions and negative reciprocal debt will be suppressed. 3.3.2.4 Nature of the Focal Party

Macneil (p. 38, 1980) refers to norms as “A principle of right action binding upon the

members of a group and serving to guide, control, or regulate proper and acceptable behavior.”

One implication is that norms provide guidelines to influence the behavior of members of a

relationship. It is important to realize that norms do not define actual behavior; rather they

portray what behavior ought to be in a particular context, and influence individuals to either act

appropriately, or to inhibit their actions through self-regulatory mechanisms (Bagozzi 1992).

This is an important distinction, members of social relationships can and do choose to behave in

ways inconsistent with accepted norms of behavior, including the norm of reciprocity; however

we should expect that strongly held social norms will ultimately influence social behavior.

Becker’s (1986) norm of reciprocity (as discussed previously) focuses on responding to

beneficial actions, and avoiding evil, and therefore is referred to here as the positive norm of

reciprocity. It focuses on the use of beneficial or positive reactions in a relationship to build the

strength of the relationship over time. Based on the understanding that norms guide behavior,

Page 53: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

40

it should be expected that as an individual’s belief in the positive norm of reciprocity increases,

they will be more likely to acknowledge a positive reciprocal debt. Thus, an individual with a

strong belief in the norm of reciprocity should be more likely to consciously recognize the need

to reciprocate beneficial actions to their partner.

H8a: Customer belief in the positive norm of reciprocity will enhance the link between beneficial actions and a positive reciprocal debt. Although Bagozzi (1995) calls for future relationship marketing research to utilize

Becker’s (1986) positive norm of reciprocity, other researchers have extended the definition of

reciprocity beyond this utopian conception of consistent beneficial reactions. Eisenberger and

colleagues (2004) find support for a negative norm of reciprocity as an individual difference

variable which helps explain why some people tend to punish more severely than others. They

define the negative norm of reciprocity as “a unitary set of beliefs favoring retribution as the

correct and proper way to respond to unfavorable treatment” (p. 788). Gouldner (1960) also

incorporates retaliation in his explication of the norm of reciprocity, noting that reciprocity can

refer to the return of harms instead of benefits.

In fact, just as we see evidence of the Golden Rule in both the Bible and the writings of

Confucius, Eisenberger and colleagues (2004) discuss the origins of the concept of retribution,

not only from the Bible, “And if any mischief follow, then thou shalt give life for life, eye for eye,

tooth for tooth…” (Exodus 21:23-25, King James Version), but also in the Hammarabian code,

which is replete with examples of punishments for various crimes which serve no purpose save

to discourage evil acts by sentencing severe punishments on the perpetrators of evil. Cialdini

(2009) states that “… the rule for reciprocation assures that, whether the fruit of our action is

sweet or bitter, we reap what we sow” (p. 21), and goes on to quote the poetry of W.H. Auden:

Page 54: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

41

I and the world know What every schoolboy learns, Those to whom evil is done Do evil in return. (1939)

Evidence for retribution as a moral imperative remains in our penal system, which while some

argue is designed to rehabilitate offenders, others maintain that its primary purpose is to punish

criminal offenders through a socially sanctioned process so that victims of crime gain some

feeling of catharsis through retributive justice (Carlsmith et al. 2002; Logan and Gaes 1993).

These proponents of the penal system as a punitive mechanism do not consider rehabilitation a

goal of incarceration.

This research will utilize a slightly less restrictive conceptualization of the norm of

reciprocity developed by Perugini, Gallucci, Presaghi and Ercolani (2003). Recognizing that the

norm of reciprocity includes not only positive reciprocation, but also can include retributive

punishment, or negative reciprocity as a moral imperative, these authors conceptualized the

norm of reciprocity as a two part internalized social norm. The “Personal Norm of Reciprocity”

scale measures individual adherence to a norm of both positive reciprocity and negative

reciprocity. The authors find evidence that individuals with higher positive or negative

reciprocity norms behave differently in interactions with others depending on the perceived

valence of the others’ past behavior. In other words, individuals exhibit different levels of

adherence to the norm of positive and negative reciprocity, and these norms influence their

behavior differentially in relationships.

In development of their scale, Perugini et al. (2003) measured the two distinct norms of

reciprocity; along with a third scale which measured a general belief in reciprocal behavior. The

normative scales measured opinions about what the respondent should do in response to a

particular action (e.g., positive – If someone is helpful with me at work, I am pleased to help

him/her; and negative – I am willing to invest time and effort to reciprocate an unfair action),

Page 55: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

42

while the “Beliefs in reciprocity” scale captured general beliefs about what others might do in

response to the respondent’s action (e.g., positive – If I work hard, I expect it will be repaid; and

negative – I avoid being impolite because I do not want others being impolite with me). One

might argue that the positive and negative norms of reciprocity are at opposite ends of the

same spectrum, as they deal with actions of an opposing valence. If this were true, we might

expect to see a negative correlation between these norms in individuals, where (as Becker

argues) people should always try to avoid returning evil for evil, however empirical evidence

suggests this is not the case. In an Italian sample, no relationship was found between the two,

and the two norms were positively associated in an English sample (Perugini et al. 2003).

Further, in both samples the correlation between the general belief in reciprocity, and both the

positive and negative norm of reciprocity scales was positive. This suggests that people can and

do exhibit unique agreement to both a positive norm (returning good for good) and a negative

norm (an eye for an eye…) of reciprocity. In fact, Eisenberger et al. (2004) find that people with

a stronger belief in negative reciprocity are more responsive to mistreatment than those with a

lesser belief in the negative norm of reciprocity. Further, it was found that the belief in a

negative norm of reciprocity enhanced the impact of a mistreatment on negative outcomes

including anger, disagreement and ridicule directed at the experimenter. This suggests that in

exchange situations involving a detrimental action, the negative norm of reciprocity will be more

salient in affecting the response behavior.

H8b: Customer belief in the negative norm of reciprocity will enhance the link between detrimental actions and a negative reciprocal debt. 3.3.2.5 Nature of the Relationship

Defined as the need to maintain a relationship with a partner in order to achieve one’s

goals, dependence has a long tradition in the marketing literature. Early dependence research

positioned dependence as a key variable for the quality of relationships (Emerson 1962).

Page 56: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

43

Research found that more dependent parties were subjected to greater coercion from their

partner (El-Ansary and Stern 1972; Stern 1967). Later work investigated the role of

interdependence and dependence asymmetry as predictors of relational outcomes (Kumar et al.

1995a; Kumar et al. 1998). Recently the role of dependence as a predictor of performance in

marketing relationships has been questioned. In a meta-analysis on the effectiveness of various

relationship factors, dependence was found to be one of the least influential constructs in the

relationship marketing framework (Palmatier et al. 2006a). It seems unlikely that the degree a

firm’s success relies on their partner is not an important aspect of marketing relationships.

The extant marketing research has measured dependence in a number of ways, without

respect to how these various measurements might impact the empirical results. For instance,

dependence has been conceptualized as a firm’s relative dependence compared to their partner

(Anderson and Narus 1990; Stern and Reve 1980); interdependence, or the aggregate

dependence among both parties (Gundlach and Cadotte 1994; Kumar et al. 1995a; Kumar et al.

1998); dependence asymmetry or the difference (and direction of that difference) between the

focal firm and their partner (Anderson and Weitz 1989; Stern and Reve 1980); or the absolute

level of dependence of the focal party (Frazier et al. 1989). While there has been research that

finds significant impacts using these measures of dependence, researchers have recently

ascertained that dependence is not a one-dimensional construct.

Recent research suggests that dependence is actually a two-dimensional construct

comprised of cost based and benefit based dependence, which may have unique impacts on

relationships (Scheer et al. 2008b). For example, decomposing dependence resulted in a direct

positive impact of benefit-based dependence on customer relational loyalty, however while

cost-based dependence positively influenced insensitivity to competitive offerings, it showed no

effect on relational loyalty (Scheer et al. 2008a). In a meta-analysis investigating the empirical

Page 57: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

44

evidence for dependence in the marketing literature, Scheer, Palmatier and Miao (2008) find

support for breaking dependence into two dimensions, cost-based dependence (CBD) and

benefit-based dependence (BBD).

CBD is defined as the need to maintain the relationship because of the unrealized costs

that would be incurred if the relationship ended (termination costs). BBD is the need to

maintain the relationship because of the irreplaceable, unique net value that would be forfeited

if the relationship ended (relationship benefits received). The findings of the meta-analysis with

respect to the roles of CBD and BBD are highlighted when the authors state “Benefit-based,

cost-based, and overall dependence display strikingly different patterns of relationships with the

other relational constructs in our framework.” The authors find that while both CBD and BBD

have a positive relationship with commitment, CBD has a more positive correlation than does

BBD. This suggests that high anticipated termination costs can act as stabilizing mechanisms in a

relationship. The results also show that as a customer’s CBD increases, the supplier is able to

exercise greater influence over the customer; however the same cannot be said of BBD.

So how should cost-based dependence and benefit-based dependence affect reciprocal

behavior? It is important to examine how cost based and benefit based dependence may differ

within relationships. While it is likely that the two will be positively correlated, it is not

necessary that the correlation be strong. BBD deals with the benefits mediated through the

partner firm, while CBD deals with the latent termination costs if the relationship were to end.

It is possible that in some situations, BBD is very high; however multiple suppliers (or customers

from the seller’s perspective) are available who would be equally able to provide similar benefits

should this relationship end, suggesting that CBD is very low. Similarly, a firm could have a high

level of CBD given a lack of available alternatives for a partner firm that provides very small

amounts of benefits through the relationship.

Page 58: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

45

Reciprocity can be viewed as a behavioral norm for independent interactions that

transpire between any two individuals in a one-shot episodic transaction (Macneil 1980), and

also as a mechanism that enables the development of enduring social relationships that deepen

in value over time (Bagozzi 1995; Gouldner 1960; Macneil 1986). As Gouldner (1960) points out,

the valuation of an individual exchange is dependent on the perceptions of the actors involved.

Ongoing exchange creates expectations for future exchange. Given that actions have been

defined as significant deviations from the status quo in a relationship, we might expect that the

same action could have a unique impact in two separate relationships depending on the history

of the relationship, and hence the expectations for future interactions within each relationship.

Figure #4 illustrates different configurations of cost-based and benefit-based dependence which

should be expected to affect the reference point in a given relationship, and hence the response

to any beneficial action within that relationship.

Figure #4. Dependence Configurations

Benefit Based Dependence

Low High

Cost Based Dependence

Low

Quadrant A Quadrant C

Low Benefits, Low Latent

Costs

High Benefits, Low Latent

Costs

High

Quadrant B Quadrant D

Low Benefits, High Latent

Costs

High Benefits, High Latent

Costs

It seems that in a relationship described by low benefit-based dependence (quadrants A and B in

Figure #4) a given beneficial action would be perceived as more positive when compared to a

relationship described by a high level of BBD. In the first case (low BBD) the action may be

perceived as a significant positive deviation from the expected outcomes. In the latter case

(high BBD), the same action may not make much of an impact as the recipient is already

Page 59: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

46

accustomed to greater benefits mediated through their partner. This suggests the following

hypothesis:

H9a: Customer benefit-based dependence will suppress the impact of beneficial actions on positive reciprocal debts. Cost-based dependence is expected to impact the response to detrimental actions in a

similar way. CBD is directly related to the need to maintain a relationship because of the latent

costs if the relationship were to end. It does not necessarily depend on the benefits accrued

directly from the relationship, but rather on how costly it would be to replace this relational

partner. Frazier and colleagues (Frazier and Rody 1991; Frazier and Summers 1984) find that

coercive manufacturer tactics are reciprocated by dealers in the U.S., however they find that in a

developing country, coercive manufacturer tactics are less likely to be reciprocated by their

dealers (Frazier et al. 1989). They argue that this is the case because in developing markets,

firms have fewer alternatives for suppliers, and hence a higher level of dependence on their

supplier than similar firms in a developed country. In the U.S., that is not the case. Dealers in

the U.S. have multiple alternatives, and hence their CBD is relatively lower than those in

developing countries.

It seems logical that all else equal, higher switching costs (Quadrants B and D in Figure

#4) would encourage channel members to tolerate more negative treatment from their

counterparts, at least to some extent. In fact, channels researchers generally agree that firms

lacking alternatives, and hence higher cost-based dependence, are likely to focus less on equity

in their relationship and also more likely to endure detrimental actions than those in low

dependence relationships (Bucklin 1973; Frazier 1983). Additionally, Scheer, Palmatier and

Miao (2008b) find that cost-based dependence has a very strong positive relationship with

commitment, relationship continuity and cooperation. It seems that the anticipation of high

costs if a relationship is terminated can have a stabilizing effect, and possibly reduce the

Page 60: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

47

tendency to actively respond to negative actions. Overall it is expected that cost based

dependence will exhibit the following impact on negative reciprocal debts:

H9b: Customer cost-based dependence will suppress the impact of detrimental actions on negative reciprocal debts. Kumar, Scheer and Steenkamp (1998) cite bilateral deterrence theory (Lawler and

Bacharach 1987) in their prediction that increasing total dependence in a relationship will lead

to the avoidance of punitive tactics as the parties are motivated by loss avoidance. While this

research will focus on the customer’s dependence on its selling firm, I believe the impact of

bilateral deterrence theory will obtain. While this theory is focused on loss avoidance, it applies

to both BBD and CBD in with respect to benefit based dependence a party may be motivated to

refrain from doing anything that would cause a reduction in benefits through that relationship.

On the other hand, avoiding loss with respect to cost-based dependence suggests avoidance of

realizing the latent costs that would be incurred should the relationship terminate. Bilateral

deterrence theory examines interactions at a broad level, including the dependence from both

parties, and a general trend of interactions over time. For example, Kumar, Scheer and

Steenkamp (1998) examine the overall use of punitive actions in channel relationships, and find

that as relationship interdependence increases the use of punitive action decreases. The reason

that high levels of overall dependence may have had this suppressing effect on punitive action

may be due to high levels of both cost-based and benefit-based dependence, suggesting that

when both are high we might expect to see the lowest levels of punitive or detrimental actions.

Unlike Kumar, Scheer and Steenkamp, this research will focus on one particular exchange

between a customer and their supplier firm. If we examine one single response to an action

through the scope of bilateral deterrence theory, understanding that we are not capturing the

entire relationship, we would expect that as one party’s total dependence increases (Quadrant

Page 61: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

48

D in Figure #4) they will be less likely to acknowledge a negative reciprocal debt when faced

with a detrimental action by their partner, and hence expect that:

H10: The customer’s negative reciprocal debt will be lowest when the customer CBD and BBD are both high.

Figure #5 (below) presents a process model of the reciprocation of beneficial actions,

while figure #6 presents a model of negative reciprocation. In brief, seller actions lead to

positive or negative reciprocal debts. It is proposed that the path from the seller action to the

customer reciprocal debt is moderated by: (1) the customer’s perception of relative outcomes;

(2) the customer’s attributions of control of the seller action; (3) the customer’s norms of

reciprocity; (4) the customer’s perception of the dependence structure of the relationship; (5)

and the customer’s judgment of the justifiability of seller detrimental actions.

Page 62: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Selle

r Ini

tiate

d B

enef

icia

l A

ctio

n (B

A)

Posit

ive

Rec

ipro

cal

Deb

t

Posit

ive

Nor

m

of R

ecip

roci

ty

Perc

eive

d R

elat

ive

Ben

efit

for t

he

Cus

tom

er

Cus

tom

er

Exte

rnal

A

ttrib

utio

n of

C

ontro

l

Cus

tom

er

Ben

efit-

Bas

ed

Dep

ende

nce

H1

+

H5a

+

H6a

-

H8a

+

H9a

-

Figu

re #

5Pr

oces

s Mod

el o

f Pos

itiv

e Re

cipr

ocity

Partn

er

Out

com

esH

2 +*

* -t

este

d in

exp

erim

enta

l res

earc

h on

ly

roset
Typewritten Text
49
Page 63: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Cus

tom

er

Ben

efit-

Bas

ed

Dep

ende

nce

Perc

eive

d R

elat

ive

Cos

t fo

r the

C

usto

mer

Cus

tom

er

Exte

rnal

A

ttrib

utio

n of

C

ontro

l

Neg

ativ

e N

orm

of

Rec

ipro

city

Selle

r Ini

tiate

d D

etrim

enta

l A

ctio

n (B

A)

Neg

ativ

e R

ecip

roca

l D

ebt

Cus

tom

er

Cos

t-Bas

ed

Dep

ende

nce

Cus

tom

er

Perc

eive

d Ju

stifi

abili

ty

H3

+ H5b

+H6b

-

H8b

+H9b

-H

10 + H

7 +

Figu

re #

6Pr

oces

s Mod

el o

f Neg

ativ

e Re

cipr

ocity

Partn

er

Out

com

esH

4 -*

* -t

este

d in

exp

erim

enta

l res

earc

h on

ly

roset
Typewritten Text
50
Page 64: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

51

CHAPTER 4: RESEARCH ON EPISODIC RECIPROCITY

4.1 Survey Research Model

A survey was administered to address the front end of the theoretical framework (up

until the creation of reciprocal debt) developed to address episodic reciprocity (see figure #7

below).

The survey was organized such that respondents (customers) first answered questions about

their relationship with the seller during the pre-scenario questionnaire. After completing these

relationship scales, participants were exposed to an experimental scenario which placed them in

a hypothetical situation during which their selling firm enacted some strategic action that would

presumably have an impact on the customer firm. After reading the hypothetical scenario,

participants responded to a number of questions gauging their perceptions (both research

variables and manipulation checks) of the strategic action, and questions that measured the

Perceived Relative

Outcomes

Norms of Reciprocity

Customer Attribution of Control

Seller Action

Reciprocal Debt

Customer Dependence

Customer Perceived

Justifiability

Figure #7 – Process Model of Reciprocity

Page 65: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

52

dependent variable for this portion of the research – reciprocal debt. Finally, participants were

asked to complete demographic information about themselves and about their firm.

The survey was administered using Qualtrics online survey software. Participants were

randomly assigned to one of 8 conditions in a 2 [valence: detrimental vs. beneficial] x 2

[magnitude: low vs. high] x 2 [attribution of control: seller vs. external] full factorial

experimental design. Figures #8 and 9 below summarize the random assignment.

Figure #8

Random Assignment to Beneficial Conditions (N = 81)

Magnitude

High n = 24

(females = 4) (males = 20)

n = 20 (females = 2) (males = 18)

Low n = 18

(females = 3) (males = 15)

n = 19 (females = 2) (males = 17)

Internal External

Attribution of Control

Figure #9

Random Assignment to Detrimental Conditions (N = 81)

Magnitude

High n = 19

(females = 0) (males = 18)

n = 18 (females = 0) (males = 18)

Low n = 19

(females = 0) (males = 19)

n = 25 (females = 3) (males = 22)

Internal External

Attribution of Control

Page 66: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

53

The 8 conditions were created by putting together various components into a hypothetical

scenario that involved a car manufacturer (selling firm) announcing a pricing change that either

increased (beneficial) or decreased (detrimental) the car dealer markup. The dealer markup is

the portion of retail vehicle sales that is adds to the dealership’s bottom line – making it an

appropriate marketing variable to manipulate for our study. The actual scenario components

will be discussed in detail in the measures section of this document.

4.1.2 Operationalizations

Measures were captured by Qualtrics and included attitudinal and perceptual scales

which can be separated into two distinct questionnaires: pre-scenario questionnaire; and post-

scenario questionnaire. Although scales will be discussed that measured participants’

perceptions about the scenario in the post-scenario questionnaire, the experimental scenarios

will be discussed in detail in this section as well.

4.1.2.1 Pre-Scenario Questionnaire

A battery of questions capturing the participants’ adherence to the positive norm of

reciprocity, their adherence to the negative norm of reciprocity and their dealership’s

dependence on their primary automaker was asked prior to exposure to the experimental

scenarios. When possible, scale items were adapted from existing scales.

Norms of Reciprocity

The norms of reciprocity were conceptualized as multi-item reflective latent constructs.

Items were adapted from Perugini et al (2003) to capture both the positive and negative norms

of reciprocity. 6 items were used to capture the participant's adherence to the positive norm of

reciprocity (pr1 - pr6) and to the negative norm of reciprocity (nr1 - nr6) using 7-point Likert

scales ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor

Agree” labeling the midpoint.

Page 67: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

54

Positive Norm of Reciprocity

1. When I profit from the actions of someone else, I will do something that benefits him/her in return.

2. When someone does me a favor, I feel committed to repay the favor. 3. If someone does a favor for me, I will return the favor. 4. I am ready to incur personal costs to help someone who has helped me. 5. I go out of my way to help a person who has been kind to me. 6. I'm willing to do things I don't enjoy to return someone's assistance.

Negative Norm of Reciprocity

1. If someone purposely injures me, I will try to injure him/her as well. 2. If somebody takes action that damages me, I will go to great lengths to inflict damage

on him/her. 3. If somebody is impolite to me, I become impolite in return. 4. I am willing to invest time and effort to reciprocate a harmful action. 5. If someone puts me in a difficult position, I will do the same to him/her. 6. I will be unfair to someone who has been unfair to me.

Primary Automaker – Control Variable

One question was asked of participants to focus their attention on one automaker for

the scenario and other relationship questions:

“Please select the automaker that represents the largest share of your dealership’s sales”

Acura Ford Lincoln Porsche Audi Honda Mazda Scion BMW Hyundai Mercedes Suzuki Buick Infiniti Mercury Toyota Cadillac Jeep Mini Volkswagen Chevrolet Kia Mitsubishi Volvo Chrysler Lexus Nissan Subaru Dodge

Dependence

Dependence was conceptualized as a two-dimensional construct comprised of (1) cost

based dependence and (2) benefit based dependence. Cost based dependence is defined as the

need to maintain the relationship because of the unrealized costs that would be incurred if the

relationship ended (Scheer et al. 2009). Benefit based dependence is defined as the need to

Page 68: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

55

maintain the relationship because of the irreplaceable, unique net value that would be forfeited

if the relationship ended. Both cost based and benefit based dependence were conceptualized

as multi-item reflective latent constructs. Additionally, a “global” dependence single item was

asked of participants, along with a single relative dependence item so that these measures could

be included as controls in the analysis. Finally, share of wallet was measured with one item.

The items for all of these measures are included below, with “Porsche” being used where text

was piped into questions by Qualtrics based on the primary automaker answer.

Benefit Based Dependence – captured with 3 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the

midpoint.

1. If we were no longer a “Porsche” dealer, it would be difficult to maintain our current sales volume.

2. My dealership receives benefits as a “Porsche” dealer that couldn’t be fully duplicated by another automaker.

3. If my dealership stopped selling “Porsche”, our other options would not be as profitable.

Cost Based Dependence– captured with 3 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the

midpoint.

1. My dealership would incur many costs to end its business relationship with “Porsche” and switch to another automaker.

2. It would be costly for my dealership to search for and locate an alternative automaker to replace “Porsche”.

3. It would be costly for my dealership to end its relationship with “Porsche”.

Global Dependence

1. How dependent is your dealership on “Porsche”?

Captured with a 7 point semantic differential scale anchored by (1) Not at all dependent

and (7) Extremely dependent.

Page 69: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

56

Relative Dependence

1. In your market, is your dealership more dependent on “Porsche” or is “Porsche” more

dependent on your dealership?

Captured with a 7 point semantic differential scale anchored by (1) They are more

dependent on us and (7) We are more dependent on them.

Share of Wallet – Control Variable

1. Consider all your dealership’s sales from all makes you carry. What share of your dealership’s total sales (# of vehicles) are “Porsche” vehicles?

Please enter the % of total number of vehicles sold that Porsche represents _________

4.1.2.2 Experimental Scenarios

In order to manipulate the valence, attribution of control and relative outcomes of the

seller action, participants were randomly assigned to view one of 8 scenarios in a 2 [valence:

detrimental vs. beneficial] x 2 [magnitude: low vs. high] x 2 [attribution of control: seller vs.

external] full factorial experimental design. The scenarios were presented as “What If’s” and

participants were asked to think about how they would react if their primary automaker actually

made the strategic decision described in the scenario. The three factors were manipulated

within the scenarios using standard components embedded within the text of the hypothetical

situations. As with many of the preceding survey questions, the name of the primary automaker

was piped in by Qualtrics such that each participant viewed a personalized scenario. Porsche

again will be used as the example automaker for the scenario components. Table #4 (on the

following page) contains the components of the scenarios that were designed to manipulate the

various factors. Figures 8 and 9 (section 4.1) showed the number of participants assigned to

each condition.

Page 70: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

57

Two sample scenarios are recreated below for illustrative purposes.

Scenario #1 – Valence = Beneficial; Magnitude = Low; Attribution = Porsche

Porsche announced that because of decreased costs of production, 3rd quarter 2010 dealer prices for all passenger cars will be decreased. Due to the current economic environment, MSRP’s will remain the same. The price change will increase your effective dealer markup by 1%.

Scenario #2 – Valence = Detrimental; Magnitude = High; Attribution = External

Porsche announced that because of changes in governmental regulations, 3rd quarter 2010 dealer prices for all passenger cars will be increased. Due to the current economic environment, MSRP’s will remain the same. The price change will decrease your effective dealer markup by 2%.

4.1.2.3 Post-Scenario Questionnaire

After reading the hypothetical scenario, participants answered a number of questions

about their perceptions of the strategic decision presented to them during the scenario.

Partner Actions

Each participant assessed the impact of the automaker’s decision in the scenario on

their dealership’s outcome and the automaker’s outcomes by completing two questions using 7-

point semantic differential scales.

Table #4 - Experimental Scenario Components

Common Statements Valence Magnitude Attribution of Control

Beneficial Detrimental High Low Porsche External

Porsche announced that because of

decreased increased

costs of production,

changes in governmental regulations,

3rd quarter 2010 dealer prices for all passenger cars will

be decreased.

be increased.

Due to the current economic environment, MSRP's will remain the same. The price change will

increase decrease your effective dealer markup

by 2%.

your effective dealer markup

by 1%.

Page 71: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

58

• Own Impact (dealerimpact): How will the decision made by Porsche impact your dealership? [1 = “Very negative effect on my dealership;” 7 = “Very positive effect on my dealership”]

• Partner Impact (makerimpact): How do you think the decision by Porsche will impact Porsche? [1 = “Very negatively;” 7 = “Very positively”]

• Relative Outcomes (ROut) were computed by subtracting makerimpact from dealerimpact. As the two items were assessed consecutively on consistent scales, I believe that this calculated variable inherently reflects the participant's perception of the extent to which his/her outcomes were relatively greater or lower than the partner's.

Reciprocal Debts

Reciprocal debt was conceptualized as a multi dimensional reflective latent construct.

Extant reciprocity literature was referenced to help develop items to assess the participant's

perceptions regarding reciprocity debts for this research. A large pool of possible scale items

was developed to capture the perceived reciprocal debt. After further consideration and a

pretest to examine measurement properties, a smaller set of measures was selected to assess

these latent constructs. Three items capturing reciprocal debt were measured using 7-point

Likert scales [1 = "Strongly Disagree;" 4 = "Neither Disagree nor Agree;" 7 = "Strongly Agree"]

and one item assessed the locus of the debt using forced choice.

• Existence of Reciprocal Debt (RD): Indicate your agreement or disagreement with the

following statements.

1. An obligation has been created that should be repaid. 2. Something should be done to make up for this situation. 3. Porsche’s decision has created an imbalance in our relationship that needs to be

addressed.

• Locus of Reciprocal Debt (Locus): Given the outcome of the game you just played, which

statement below best captures your opinion of what should happen?

1. Porsche should do something in response to this situation. 2. My dealership should do something in response to this situation. 3. Either Porsche or my dealership or both should do something in response to this

situation, but I don’t have a strong opinion about which should act.

Page 72: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

59

4. Neither Porsche nor my dealership should do anything in response to this situation. Justifiability

Justifiability was conceptualized as a multi-item reflective latent construct. Participants

were asked to rate their agreement with four statements to capture their perceptions of the

justifiability of Porsche’s action.

“Overall, I believe that Porsche’s pricing decision is…”

1. … completely justified. 2. … appropriate under current circumstances. 3. … a viable strategic option by Porsche. 4. … a good business decision.

Responses to these 4 statements were captured using 7-point Likert scales [1 = "Strongly

Disagree;" 4 = "Neither Disagree nor Agree;" 7 = "Strongly Agree"].

Attribution of Control

Participants were asked to respond to a single question about their perceptions of

responsibility for (a) the automobile manufacturer, (b) external forces and (c) their own

dealership. Using these 3 questions, both relative and absolute attribution of control measures

can be calculated.

“How much responsibility do the following parties have for Porsche’s action?”

• Porsche • External forces (competition, government, etc.) • My dealership

Answers were recorded for each of the three choices on a 5-point Likert scale using a dropdown

list to select the appropriate answer:

1. No responsibility 2. Slight responsibility 3. Moderate responsibility 4. Primary responsibility 5. Full responsibility

Page 73: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

60

4.1.3 Sample Selection

We decided to survey car dealership managers about their relationships with car

manufacturers. This is a fertile context to investigate the response to selling-firm actions given

the recent turmoil in the automobile industry (dealership closings, government interventions,

safety recalls, etc.). A database was purchased from www.autodealerlist.com that included

approximately 23,000 contacts (owners, CEO’s, parts managers, sales managers, etc.) from

12,000 new car dealers across the United States. The database included the contact’s name,

phone number, email address and job title. It also included some information about the

dealership, including automaker(s) represented, annual sales volume ($) and number of

employees.

Because the database included numerous contacts per dealership, ranging from

assistant managers to owners, decisions had to be made about who would be best qualified as

both an informant and respondent for this survey. The ideal participants would be those who

(1) have a direct interest in the financial outcomes of the dealership’s relationship with an

automaker; (2) have a reasonable amount of direct interactions with an automaker – to be

qualified to respond about those relationship dynamics; and (3) have decision making authority

at the dealership. Owners and executive officers were eliminated because of the uncertainty of

their day-to-day operational experience at the dealership. Parts and service managers were

eliminated because of the uncertainty of their meeting any of the above criteria. We decided to

only approach general managers and sales managers at the car dealerships because of the

likelihood that they would meet all three criteria we specified for our participants. The final

sample we selected included 17,081 sales and general managers at 9,437 unique car dealerships

throughout the United States.

Page 74: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

61

Given the online data collection procedure, cost savings concerns, and the availability of

the email contact list from the purchased database, potential participants were recruited

through email. After unsuccessful negotiations with the host research university, a multi-

method recruitment process ensued. In order to bolster response rates as much as possible, the

email cover letters were customized with letterhead from the two Universities involved with this

research: the University of Missouri, and the University of Alabama – Birmingham (UAB); see

exhibit 2 and 3 in the appendix. The total sample was split geographically into 3 regions: (1)

University of Missouri Region; (2) UAB Region; and (3) the rest of the United States. Details for

each region are included in Table #5 below.

Table#5 - Recruitment Regions

Region States Included

Email Contacts

Bouncebacks Email

Delivery System

Responses Response

Rate

MU

Arkansas, Colorado, Iowa, Illinois, Kansas,

Nebraska, Oklahoma, Texas

3102 737 *

Researcher's Personal

Email Account

119 3.76%

UAB

Alabama, Florida, Georgia,

Louisiana, Mississippi,

South Carolina, Tennessee

2869 681 ** Qualtrics 58 2.65%

USA All Others 11,110 2,640 ** Qualtrics 157 1.85%

Total 17,081 4058 ** 334 2.56%

* - actual tracked bounced emails ** - estimated # of bounced emails based on MU Region

(23.76%)

Page 75: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

62

The researchers endeavored to email potential participants using their own email

accounts to minimize the number of emails that would end up in junk email boxes (every test

email – no matter the designation of a return email address – sent from Qualtrics ended up in

the researcher’s junk email folder), however found that it was not possible to do so with the

entire sample. Because of the proximity of the MU region to the university, and the likelihood

that response rates would be higher in that area, the MU region was selected as the one region

that would receive emails directly from the researchers instead of any mass email system.

However, limitations in email capacities required that recruitment emails be sent out in batches

of < 250 per day. So the MU region received emails at a rate of ~250 per day, followed by a

reminder email after the entire MU region sample had been emailed their first recruitment

letter. The UAB and USA regions were mass emailed from Qualtrics with a follow up reminder

email sent out approximately 10 days after the first email.

Participants were offered a summary report of the research findings, and entry into a

drawing for one of 10 $100 gift cards as an incentive for their participation. A link to the survey

was included in the email cover letter so that participants could click the link in the email and be

taken directly to the survey hosted by Qualtrics.

Although it was impossible to estimate or track the number of emails that ended up in

junk email folders, using a personal email account to send out the emails to the MU region

allowed tracking of bounced emails that were returned due to spam tracking at the receivers

end, or bad email addresses (or any other reason). As table #5 shows the bounce back rate for

emails in that sample was 23.76%. This bounce back rate was assumed for the other two

regions, which should be a very conservative estimate given that emails sent from a mass email

system like Qualtrics have a much higher probability of being flagged as spam by the recipient’s

email server than those sent from an individual’s account. Assuming the same bounce back

Page 76: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

63

rate, the response rates were realistic considering the medium used to solicit participation: MU

region – 3.76%; UAB region – 2.65%; USA region – 1.85%; for a total response rate of 2.56%.1

4.1.4 Measurement Model and Scale Development

4.1.4.1 Directional Reciprocal Debt

RDLocus was calculated by multiplying the multi-item latent reciprocal debt measure by

the locus measure. The locus measure was The RDLocus variable was coded negatively when the

participant felt the automaker (Porsche in our examples) was responsible, when they thought

that their own dealership was responsible, the RDLocus variable was positive. If they said

“neither” party should do anything, the RDLocus = 0 – which makes sense since they obviously

feel that there is no reciprocal debt. The “either” option was a bit more complicated, but

ultimately we decided on dropping them from the analysis. While they may have thought

something should be done (based on the RD measure), it is unclear how to analyze these

responses without making assumptions about the locus. RDLocus therefore represents the

intensity and directionality of the perceived reciprocal debt, ranging from strong participant

reciprocal debt (at high positive values of this variable) to strong partner reciprocal debt (at high

negative values of this variable).

4.1.4.2 Exploratory Factor Analyses

Exploratory factor analyses were conducted on each latent construct separately, and the

results are summarized in Table 6 (Pre-Scenario items) and Table 7 (Post-Scenario items). Each

separate EFA resulted in all items loading onto one factor. These exploratory factor analyses

results provide preliminary evidence for the validity of the latent constructs.

1 Plans exist to use “snail-mail” to recruit car dealership managers from the state of Missouri to participate in our online survey which should serve 2 purposes: (1) increase our total sample for this survey; and (2) allow comparisons of response rates from email versus mail based recruitment to solicit participation for an online survey. However those results will not be included at this point in the analysis.

Page 77: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

64

Table#6 – Summary of Individual EFA’s for 4 Latent Constructs

Eigenvalue 2.985 3.909 1.628 1.683 % Var. Extracted 49.74% 65.15% 54.25% 56.10% # of factors with Eigenvalue > 1

1 1 1 1

PR1 .711 PR2 .816 PR3 .620 PR4 .704 PR5 .728 PR6 .634 NR1 .769 NR2 .704 NR3 .825 NR4 .874 NR5 .876 NR6 .781

BBD1 .750 BBD2 .681 BBD3 .775 CBD1 .754 CBD2 .845 CBD3 .634

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

Table#7 – Separate EFA on RD and Just

Eigenvalue 2.436 3.510 % Var. Extracted 81.21% 87.75% # of factors with Eigenvalue > 1

1 1

RD1 .874 RD2 .933 RD3 .896 Just1 .934 Just2 .923 Just3 .943 Just4 .948

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

Page 78: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Ta

ble

#8 -

Ove

rall

Mea

ns, S

tand

ard

Dev

iati

ons

and

Corr

elat

ions

for

Stud

y V

aria

bles

1 2

3 4

5 6

7 8

9 10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

(1) V

alen

ce

(man

ip)

μ =

.500

σ

= .5

01

r

p

N

(2) M

agni

tude

(m

anip

) μ

= .5

00

σ =

.501

r .0

86

p

.274

N

162

(3) A

ttri

buti

on

(man

ip)

μ =

.494

σ

= .5

01

r .0

49

.074

p

.533

.3

49

N

162

162

(4) O

wnO

ut

μ =

4.05

σ

= 2.

16

r .6

29

.097

.0

80

p

.000

.2

19

.310

N

162

162

162

(5) O

ther

Out

μ

= 4.

16

σ =

1.85

r .3

68

.067

-.0

06

.484

p

.000

.3

98

.943

.0

00

N

162

162

162

162

(6) R

elat

iveO

ut

μ= -.

111

σ

= 2.

06

r .3

31

.042

.0

90

.617

-.3

90

p

.000

.5

95

.257

.0

00

.000

N

162

162

162

162

162

(7) P

R μ=

23.5

1

σ =

2.40

r .0

01

.002

.0

78

.014

-.0

16

.029

p

.990

.9

76

.326

.8

61

.837

.7

11

N

162

162

162

162

162

162

(8) N

R μ=

11.6

6

σ =

5.59

r .0

54

.031

.0

26

-.048

-.0

99

.039

-.0

24

p

.497

.6

92

.743

.5

47

.211

.6

25

.760

N

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

(9) B

BD

μ =

8.83

σ

= 2.

23

r -.1

44

.154

.0

03

-.085

-.0

49

-.046

.0

09

-.110

p

.068

.0

51

.973

.2

81

.537

.5

63

.912

.1

62

N

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

(10)

CBD

μ=

10.4

2

σ =

2.56

r -.1

14

-.055

-.0

93

-.015

-.0

62

.040

.0

61

-.111

.5

34

p

.149

.4

86

.240

.8

49

.432

.6

13

.442

.1

61

.000

N

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

roset
Typewritten Text
65
Page 79: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

(11)

Dep

ende

nce

μ=19

.25

σ

= 4.

20

r -.1

46

.048

-.0

55

-.054

-.0

64

.000

.0

42

-.126

.8

57

.894

p

.064

.5

44

.486

.4

91

.419

.9

99

.598

.1

10

.000

.0

00

N

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

(12)

Rec

. Deb

t μ=

11.4

3

σ =

4.45

r -.4

46

-.064

-.0

99

-.503

-.2

50

-.305

.0

51

.224

.0

67

.112

.1

04

p

.000

.4

21

.211

.0

00

.001

.0

00

.516

.0

04

.396

.1

55

.188

N

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

(13)

RD

Locu

s μ=

-5.9

0

σ=10

.02

r .5

65

.184

.1

88

.488

.3

10

.230

.0

73

.001

.0

40

-.043

-.0

04

-.536

p

.000

.0

39

.035

.0

00

.000

.0

10

.418

.9

90

.653

.6

31

.964

.0

00

N

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

(14)

Loc

us

μ= -.

325

σ

= .8

37

r .5

98

.174

.2

07

.526

.3

35

.247

.1

57

.009

.0

38

-.063

-.0

17

-.544

.9

44

p

.000

.0

51

.020

.0

00

.000

.0

05

.079

.9

18

.670

.4

87

.852

.0

00

.000

N

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

126

(15)

Just

μ=

15.4

6

σ =

6.67

r .4

90

.063

.1

94

.637

.4

69

.248

.1

36

-.228

-.0

47

-.080

-.0

74

-.539

.5

26

.531

p

.000

.4

22

.013

.0

00

.000

.0

01

.085

.0

04

.549

.3

10

.349

.0

00

.000

.0

00

N

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

126

126

(16)

aut

omak

er

resp

onsi

bilit

y μ

= 3.

98

σ =

1.05

r -.0

71

.024

.0

47

-.063

.0

02

-.068

.1

19

.033

-.1

15

-.098

-.1

21

.032

-.0

56

-.015

-.2

00

p

.369

.7

65

.552

.4

29

.979

.3

92

.132

.6

75

.146

.2

13

.126

.6

87

.533

.8

72

.011

N

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

126

126

162

(17)

oth

er

resp

onsi

bilit

y μ

= 2.

65

σ =

1.12

r .0

17

-.006

-.1

98

-.003

-.0

39

.032

.0

72

-.148

.0

33

.049

.0

47

-.160

.0

71

.046

.1

51

-.380

p

.834

.9

44

.012

.9

69

.626

.6

91

.364

.0

60

.678

.5

34

.549

.0

43

.427

.6

06

.056

.0

00

N

16

2 16

2 16

2 16

2 16

2 16

2 16

2 16

2 16

2 16

2 16

2 16

2 12

6 12

6 16

2 16

2

(18)

dea

ler

resp

onsi

bilit

y μ

= 1.

66

σ =

.953

r .2

40

-.071

.0

28

.192

.0

45

.161

-.0

72

-.023

-.0

23

-.080

-.0

61

-.114

.1

98

.178

.2

31

-.083

.0

15

p

.002

.3

66

.723

.0

15

.568

.0

41

.363

.7

68

.770

.3

14

.442

.1

50

.026

.0

47

.003

.2

92

.845

N

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

162

126

126

162

162

162

roset
Typewritten Text
66
Page 80: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Ta

ble

#9 -

Mea

ns, S

tand

ard

Dev

iati

ons

and

Corr

elat

ions

for

Part

icip

ants

in t

he B

enef

icia

l Con

diti

on

1

2 3

4 5

6 7

8 9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

(1) V

alen

ce

(man

ip)

μ =

1.00

σ

= .0

00

r

p

N

(2) M

agni

tude

(m

anip

) μ

= .5

43

σ =

.501

r .a

p

.

N

81

(3) A

ttri

buti

on

(man

ip)

μ =

.518

σ

= .5

03

r .a

.0

59

p

. .6

02

N

81

81

(4) O

wnO

ut

μ =

5.41

σ

= 1.

92

r .a

-.0

12

.063

p

. .9

15

.575

N

81

81

81

(5) O

ther

Out

μ

= 4.

84

σ =

1.78

r .a

-.0

69

.038

.3

52

p

. .5

39

.734

.0

01

N

81

81

81

81

(6) R

elat

iveO

ut

μ =

.568

σ

= 2.

11

r .a

.0

47

.025

.6

14

-.522

p

. .6

74

.822

.0

00

.000

N

81

81

81

81

81

(7) P

R μ

= 23

.51

σ

= 2.

51

r .a

.0

11

.104

.0

58

.080

-.0

14

p

. .9

24

.354

.6

05

.477

.8

98

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

(8) N

R μ

= 11

.96

σ

= 5.

80

r .a

-.0

27

-.031

-.1

17

-.110

-.0

14

.002

p

. .8

08

.784

.3

00

.330

.9

02

.987

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

(9) B

BD

μ =

8.51

σ

= 2.

27

r .a

.1

46

.096

.0

81

.036

.0

44

-.123

-.1

96

p

. .1

94

.393

.4

70

.752

.6

95

.272

.0

79

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

(10)

CBD

μ

= 10

.13

σ

= 2.

77

r .a

-.0

42

-.156

.1

81

.165

.0

26

-.012

-.1

99

.482

p

. .7

08

.164

.1

05

.142

.8

14

.915

.0

75

.000

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

roset
Typewritten Text
67
Page 81: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

(11)

Dep

ende

nce

μ =

18.6

4 σ

=

4.35

r .a

.0

49

-.049

.1

58

.124

.0

40

-.072

-.2

29

.829

.8

89

p

. .6

63

.662

.1

58

.272

.7

23

.523

.0

39

.000

.0

00

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

(12)

Rec

. Deb

t μ

= 9.

44

σ =

4.39

r .a

-.0

76

-.004

-.3

48

-.178

-.1

67

.034

.3

31

.032

.1

13

.088

p

. .4

99

.969

.0

01

.111

.1

36

.765

.0

03

.779

.3

17

.433

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

(13)

RD

Locu

s μ

= .8

31

σ =

9.04

6

r .a

.1

90

.162

.2

88

.333

-.0

03

-.007

-.1

07

.250

.0

88

.193

-.3

57

p

. .1

78

.251

.0

38

.016

.9

83

.963

.4

49

.074

.5

37

.170

.0

09

N

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

(14)

Loc

us

μ =

.269

σ

= .8

19

r .a

.1

04

.212

.3

51

.309

.0

80

.111

-.1

28

.203

.0

42

.138

-.5

08

.919

p

. .4

63

.131

.0

11

.026

.5

73

.432

.3

65

.148

.7

69

.328

.0

00

.000

N

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

52

(15)

Just

μ

= 18

.72

σ

= 5.

43

r .a

.0

24

.197

.4

61

.428

.0

60

.217

-.2

91

.007

-.0

75

-.044

-.3

94

.421

.4

38

p

. .8

28

.078

.0

00

.000

.5

97

.052

.0

08

.948

.5

03

.695

.0

00

.002

.0

01

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

52

52

(16)

aut

omak

er

resp

onsi

bilit

y μ

= 3.

90

σ =

1.14

r .a

.1

39

.178

.2

31

.209

.0

34

.206

-.0

37

-.147

-.1

07

-.145

-.1

15

.107

.1

62

.134

p

. .2

15

.111

.0

38

.062

.7

62

.065

.7

41

.190

.3

40

.195

.3

07

.450

.2

51

.233

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

52

52

81

(17)

oth

er

resp

onsi

bilit

y μ

= 2.

67

σ =

1.17

r .a

-.0

28

-.360

-.2

05

-.062

-.1

35

.120

-.1

28

.030

-.0

17

.005

-.1

33

.068

.0

93

-.001

-.3

16

p

. .8

02

.001

.0

66

.583

.2

30

.286

.2

56

.793

.8

82

.966

.2

37

.634

.5

13

.991

.0

04

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

52

52

81

81

(18)

dea

ler

resp

onsi

bilit

y μ

= 1.

89

σ =

1.05

r .a

-.0

98

-.055

-.0

52

-.104

.0

40

-.188

-.0

72

.062

-.1

15

-.041

-.0

80

.089

.0

33

.098

.0

43

-.203

p

. .3

85

.624

.6

47

.358

.7

22

.092

.5

26

.583

.3

08

.718

.4

76

.532

.8

19

.382

.7

02

.069

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

52

52

81

81

81

roset
Typewritten Text
68
Page 82: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Ta

ble#

10 -

Mea

ns, S

tand

ard

Dev

iati

ons

and

Corr

elat

ions

for

Part

icip

ants

in t

he D

etri

men

tal C

ondi

tion

1 2

3 4

5 6

7 8

9 10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

(1) V

alen

ce

(man

ip)

μ =

.000

σ

= .0

00

r

p

N

(2) M

agni

tude

(m

anip

) μ

= .4

57

σ =

.501

r .a

p

.

N

81

(3) A

ttri

buti

on

(man

ip)

μ =

.469

σ

= .5

02

r .a

.0

82

p

. .4

69

N

81

81

(4) O

wnO

ut

μ =

2.69

σ

= 1.

42

r .a

.1

48

.065

p

. .1

88

.562

N

81

81

81

(5) O

ther

Out

μ

= 3.

48

σ =

1.67

r .a

.1

52

-.094

.3

53

p

. .1

76

.406

.0

01

N

81

81

81

81

(6) R

elat

iveO

ut

μ =

-.7

90

σ =

1.77

r .a

-.0

25

.141

.4

68

-.662

p

. .8

26

.210

.0

00

.000

N

81

81

81

81

81

(7) P

R μ=

23

.51

σ

= 2.

30

r .a

-.0

07

.049

-.0

43

-.131

.0

89

p

. .9

51

.665

.7

00

.243

.4

29

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

(8) N

R μ=

11

.36

σ

= 5.

38

r .a

.0

86

.082

-.0

89

-.148

.0

69

-.055

p

. .4

48

.467

.4

28

.186

.5

42

.627

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

(9) B

BD

μ =

9.15

σ

= 2.

16

r .a

.1

93

-.081

-.1

00

-.031

-.0

51

.161

.0

01

p

. .0

85

.473

.3

74

.785

.6

50

.152

.9

96

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

(10)

CBD

μ=

10

.71

σ

= 2.

31

r .a

-.0

50

-.007

-.1

02

-.259

.1

63

.157

.0

16

.586

p

. .6

55

.952

.3

65

.019

.1

45

.161

.8

90

.000

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

roset
Typewritten Text
69
Page 83: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

(1

1) D

epen

denc

e μ=

19

.86

σ

= 3.

98

r .a

.0

75

-.048

-.1

13

-.167

.0

67

.178

.0

09

.882

.8

99

p

. .5

05

.672

.3

13

.135

.5

51

.111

.9

34

.000

.0

00

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

(12)

Rec

. Deb

t μ=

13

.41

σ

= 3.

55

r .a

.0

31

-.188

-.2

74

-.004

-.2

16

.091

.2

09

-.033

.0

04

-.015

p

. .7

83

.093

.0

13

.971

.0

53

.421

.0

61

.770

.9

69

.892

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

(13)

RD

Locu

s μ=

-10

.64

σ

= 7.

74

r .a

.0

58

.170

.1

20

-.103

.1

86

.015

.0

60

.068

-.0

09

.031

-.4

49

p

. .6

23

.148

.3

08

.385

.1

13

.896

.6

14

.562

.9

40

.790

.0

00

N

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

(14)

Loc

us

μ =

-.7

43

σ =

.550

r .a

.1

00

.175

.0

83

-.086

.1

42

.109

.1

24

.116

-.0

07

.058

-.2

40

.928

p

. .3

98

.135

.4

84

.465

.2

27

.354

.2

93

.323

.9

51

.624

.0

39

.000

N

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

74

(15)

Just

μ=

12

.19

σ

= 6.

21

r .a

.0

24

.194

.5

38

.293

.1

54

.098

-.2

95

.045

.0

20

.036

-.4

41

.212

.1

58

p

. .8

30

.082

.0

00

.008

.1

69

.385

.0

08

.693

.8

56

.750

.0

00

.070

.1

79

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

74

74

(16)

aut

omak

er

resp

onsi

bilit

y μ

= 4.

05

σ =

.947

r .a

-.1

01

-.102

-.4

35

-.196

-.1

63

.007

.1

34

-.101

-.1

07

-.117

.1

71

-.071

-.0

33

-.533

p

. .3

71

.366

.0

00

.079

.1

47

.952

.2

35

.370

.3

40

.298

.1

26

.549

.7

79

.000

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

74

74

81

(17)

oth

er

resp

onsi

bilit

y μ

= 2.

63

σ =

1.07

r .a

.0

16

-.022

.2

62

-.032

.2

40

.014

-.1

74

.042

.1

42

.105

-.2

21

-.007

-.1

19

.324

-.4

64

p

. .8

84

.848

.0

18

.777

.0

31

.900

.1

20

.707

.2

08

.350

.0

47

.955

.3

13

.003

.0

00

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

74

74

81

81

(18)

dea

ler

resp

onsi

bilit

y μ

= 1.

43

σ =

.790

r .a

-.0

94

.113

.2

43

.030

.1

67

.089

.0

11

-.058

.0

42

-.007

.1

12

.008

-.0

16

.182

-.2

46

.326

p

. .4

02

.316

.0

29

.792

.1

37

.428

.9

23

.608

.7

11

.950

.3

18

.948

.8

93

.104

.0

27

.003

N

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

81

74

74

81

81

81

roset
Typewritten Text
70
Page 84: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

71

Table#8, 9 and 10 (above) summarize means, standard deviations and correlations between the

research variables for the entire sample, participants in the beneficial and the detrimental

conditions respectively.

4.1.4.3 Confirmatory Factor Analysis

An overall measurement model including all six latent constructs (positive norm of

reciprocity – PR, negative norm of reciprocity – NR, benefit based dependence – BBD, cost based

dependence – CBD, reciprocal debt – RD and justifiability – Just) was estimated using

confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) in EQS 6.1 for windows. The measurement model was

estimated using maximum likelihood estimation with elliptical reweighted generalized least

squares (ERLS) because ERLS estimates are equivalent to maximum likelihood for normally

distributed data, and superior to ML for non-normal data (Zou and Cavusgil 2002).

The measurement model fit the data very well. All items loaded on the a priori factors,

with positive and significant variances, and factor loadings of at least 0.465. The measurement

model had acceptable fit statistics: χ2 = 363.132 (d.f. = 260), p < .001; Bentler-Bonnett Normed

Fit Index = .892; Bentler-Bonnett Non-Normed Fit Index = .961; CFI = .966; Standardized RMR =

.059; RMSEA = .042. Factor weighted combinations of the final scale items were used in

subsequent analyses based on the factor loadings in table#11 (next page). The combination of

the EFA’s already performed and these CFA’s provide solid evidence for the discriminant validity

of the latent constructs in this study.

Page 85: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

T

able

#11

- CF

A R

esul

ts

Fact

ors

and

Item

Loa

ding

s

Item

It

em W

ordi

ng

Posi

tive

Re

cipr

ocit

y N

egat

ive

Reci

proc

ity

Bene

fit

Base

d D

epen

d.

Cost

Ba

sed

Dep

end.

Re

cipr

ocal

D

ebt

Just

ifiab

ility

pr1

Whe

n I p

rofit

from

the

actio

ns o

f som

eone

els

e, I

will

do

som

ethi

ng th

at b

enef

its h

im/h

er in

ret

urn.

0.

657

pr2

Whe

n so

meo

ne d

oes

me

a fa

vor,

I fe

el c

omm

itted

to

repa

y th

e fa

vor.

0.

830

pr3

If so

meo

ne d

oes

a fa

vor f

or m

e, I

will

ret

urn

the

favo

r.

0.49

7

pr4

I am

read

y to

incu

r pe

rson

al c

osts

to h

elp

som

eone

w

ho h

as h

elpe

d m

e.

0.62

0

pr5

I go

out o

f my

way

to h

elp

a pe

rson

who

has

bee

n ki

nd

to m

e.

0.64

2

pr6

I'm w

illin

g to

do

thin

gs I

don'

t enj

oy to

retu

rn

som

eone

's a

ssis

tanc

e.

0.56

1

nr1

If so

meo

ne p

urpo

sely

inju

res

me,

I w

ill tr

y to

inju

re

him

/her

as

wel

l.

0.57

6

nr2

If so

meb

ody

take

s ac

tion

that

dam

ages

me,

I w

ill g

o to

gr

eat l

engt

hs to

infli

ct d

amag

e on

him

/her

.

0.53

0

nr3

If so

meb

ody

take

s ac

tion

that

dam

ages

me,

I w

ill g

o to

gr

eat l

engt

hs to

infli

ct d

amag

e on

him

/her

.

0.68

1

nr4

I am

will

ing

to in

vest

tim

e an

d ef

fort

to r

ecip

roca

te a

ha

rmfu

l act

ion.

0.90

7

nr5

If so

meo

ne p

uts

me

in a

diff

icul

t pos

ition

, I w

ill d

o th

e sa

me

to h

im/h

er.

0.

923

nr6

I will

be

unfa

ir to

som

eone

who

has

bee

n un

fair

to m

e.

0.

728

BBD

1 If

we

wer

e no

long

er a

XXX

dea

ler,

it w

ould

be

diff

icul

t to

mai

ntai

n ou

r cu

rren

t sal

es v

olum

e.

0.56

8

roset
Typewritten Text
72
Page 86: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

BB

D2

My

deal

ersh

ip re

ceiv

es b

enef

its a

s a

XXX

deal

er th

at

coul

dn't

be fu

lly d

uplic

ated

by

anot

her

auto

mak

er.

0.46

5

BBD

3 If

my

deal

ersh

ip s

topp

ed s

ellin

g XX

X, o

ur o

ther

opt

ions

w

ould

not

be

as p

rofit

able

.

0.

652

CBD

1 M

y de

aler

ship

wou

ld in

cur m

any

cost

s to

end

its

busi

ness

rel

atio

nshi

p w

ith X

XX a

nd s

witc

h to

ano

ther

au

tom

aker

.

0.55

7

CBD

2 It

wou

ld b

e co

stly

for m

y de

aler

ship

to s

earc

h fo

r an

d lo

cate

an

alte

rnat

ive

auto

mak

er to

repl

ace

XXX.

0.76

4

CBD

3 It

wou

ld b

e co

stly

for m

y de

aler

ship

to e

nd it

s re

latio

nshi

p w

ith X

XX.

0.

501

RD1

An

oblig

atio

n ha

s be

en c

reat

ed th

at s

houl

d be

rep

aid.

0.

768

RD2

Som

ethi

ng s

houl

d be

don

e to

mak

e up

for

this

si

tuat

ion.

0.

954

RD3

XXX'

s de

cisi

on h

as c

reat

ed a

n im

bala

nce

in o

ur

rela

tions

hip

that

nee

ds to

be

addr

esse

d.

0.82

8

Just

1 O

vera

ll, I

belie

ve th

at X

XX's

pri

cing

dec

isio

n is

co

mpl

etel

y ju

stifi

ed.

0.

893

Just

2 O

vera

ll, I

belie

ve th

at X

XX's

pri

cing

dec

isio

n is

ap

prop

riat

e un

der c

urre

nt c

ircu

mst

ance

s.

0.

874

Just

3 O

vera

ll, I

belie

ve th

at X

XX's

pri

cing

dec

isio

n is

a v

iabl

e st

rate

gic

optio

n by

XXX

.

0.93

9

Just

4 O

vera

ll, I

belie

ve th

at X

XX's

pri

cing

dec

isio

n is

a g

ood

busi

ness

dec

isio

n.

0.

948

χ2 =

363.

132

(d.f.

= 2

60),

p <

.001

Be

ntle

r-Bo

nnet

t Nor

med

Fit

Inde

x =

.892

; Ben

tler-

Bonn

ett N

on-N

orm

ed F

it In

dex

= .9

61

CFI =

.966

; Sta

ndar

dize

d RM

R =

.059

; RM

SEA

= .0

42

roset
Typewritten Text
73
Page 87: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

74

4.1.4.4 Manipulation Checks

Valence

Valence was the first manipulation in the scenario. In this analysis, valence is coded as 1

for beneficial scenarios and 0 for detrimental scenarios. We should expect that participants

would associate the beneficial valence condition with greater own outcomes, and also greater

relative outcomes compared to their automaker. To test that this manipulation had the

hypothesized effect, 2 ANOVA’s were performed using PASW 18.0 with relative outcomes (RO)

and own outcomes (OwnOut) as the dependent variables. Table #12 below shows that both

ANOVA’s were significant (OwnOut – F = 104.637, d.f. = 161, p < .001; RO – F = 19.678, d.f. =

161, p < .001), and figure #10 below shows that the manipulation had the desired effect – when

presented with a beneficial scenario participants perceived greater own outcomes and greater

relative outcomes than when given the detrimental scenario. Therefore, it appears that the

valence of the action was adequately manipulated.

Table #12 – ANOVA results for OwnOut and RO Valence manipulation checks

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

OwnOut Between Groups 298.765 1 298.765 104.637 .000 Within Groups 456.840 160 2.855 Total 755.605 161

RO Between Groups 74.691 1 74.691 19.678 .000 Within Groups 607.309 160 3.796 Total 682.000 161

Page 88: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

75

Figure #10 – Plots of mean differences in OwnOut and RO based on the Valence manipulation

Magnitude

The magnitude manipulation may not be as straight forward to check as was the

valence. It is unclear that we would expect similar patterns of results from the high magnitude –

beneficial valence condition as we would from the high magnitude – detrimental valence

condition. Given this concern, it may be valuable to split the sample and investigate participants

with a beneficial manipulation separately from those with a detrimental manipulation. ANOVA’s

performed with the entire sample, and again with a split sample based on the valence of the

manipulation failed to produce any significant differences in own outcomes, relative outcomes

nor in the measure of reciprocal debt. This suggests that the magnitude manipulation did not

successfully manipulate participants’ perceptions of the magnitude of an action. Magnitude will

not be considered further in this analysis.

Attribution of Control

Three questions were asked of participants about the control of the locus of

responsibility for the strategic decision made in the scenario. They were asked to rate the level

of responsibility that the (1) automaker, (2) their own dealership, and (3) external forces had for

the pricing decision they saw in the scenario. If the attribution manipulation were successful,

we would expect to see higher levels of responsibility assigned to the automaker in the

Page 89: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

76

“Internal” (coded 1) condition than in the “external” (coded 0) condition. We also would expect

to see the reverse attributions with respect to (3) the responsibility of external forces. Table

#12a below shows that one of these ANOVA’s supports our expectations.

Table #12a – ANOVA results for automaker and other – attribution manipulation checks

Sum of Squares df

Mean Square F Sig.

automaker Between Groups .390 1 .390 .356 .552 Within Groups 175.511 160 1.097 Total 175.901 161

other Between Groups 7.870 1 7.870 6.522 .012 Within Groups 193.074 160 1.207 Total 200.944 161

When participants were in the internal-attribution condition, they assigned less responsibility to

external (other) sources than when they received the external-attribution condition (F=6.522,

d.f. = 161, p = .012). Figure #11 below shows that directional support was given by the

responsibility attributions for the automaker; however the differences were not significant.

Figure #11 – Plots of mean differences in automaker and other based on the attribution manipulation

While the evidence is somewhat inconclusive, the directional support from automaker

combined with the significant differences in attributions of responsibility to external sources

provides some support that the attribution of control was successfully manipulated.

Page 90: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

77

4.1.5 Hypothesis Testing

4.1.5.1 Effects of Partner Actions on Reciprocal Debts

The first hypotheses concern the impact of the automaker’s beneficial or detrimental

actions on the participant's perception of reciprocal debts. Specifically:

H1: Beneficial actions by a selling firm will lead to a positive (participant) reciprocal debt felt by their customer. H3: Detrimental actions by a selling firm will lead to a negative (automaker) reciprocal debt felt by their customer.

As has been previously discussed, the term “participant reciprocal debt” is used instead of

“positive reciprocal debt” because it is assumed that in response to a beneficial action, the

participant will perceive that they owe their partner something. Likewise, the “automaker

reciprocal debt” implies that the participant perceives their automaker to owe them something

in the future. I examine these hypotheses using various analyses, first focusing on the locus of

reciprocal debt and then examining the level or intensity of the reciprocal debt.

Locus of Reciprocal Debt

An ANOVA was performed to investigate the participant’s perceptions of locus based on

the valence of the condition they were exposed to. The ANOVA was significant (see table #13

below – F = 68.888, d.f. = 125, p < .001), and the difference in mean locus (Locus) was in the

expected direction. When participants received a detrimental scenario they had significantly

lower average locus score (-0.743) than when they received a beneficial scenario (0.269),

suggesting that car dealership managers were more likely to perceive that their dealership owed

the automaker when presented with a beneficial outcome for their dealership, and also that

they were more likely to expect that the automaker should make up the difference when they

had made a decision that negatively impacted the dealership.

Page 91: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

78

Table #13 – Mean difference in Locus based on valence manipulation

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

Between Groups 31.306 1 31.306 68.888 .000 Within Groups 56.352 124 .454 Total 87.659 125

While the ANOVA provides some support for the expected relationship between valence

and locus, regression analyses can be used to examine the specific impact of a given variable on

the participant’s perception of the locus of the debt. A regression was performed with Locus as

the dependent variable. Locus was regressed on the three dichotomous manipulation variables

– Valence, Magnitude and Attribution. While only the valence manipulation needs to be

included to test H1 and H3, the magnitude and attribution variables were included as controls to

examine if the impact of the valence manipulation was significant controlling for the other 2

manipulations. Support for H1 and H3 would be provided by a positive and significant impact of

Valence on Locus. The overall model is significant (F = 25.527, d.f. = 125, p < .001), explaining

approximately 37% of the variance in the Locus (adj R2 = .371). Table #14 shows the estimated

coefficients, importantly the coefficient for Valence (standardized β = .572) is positive and

significant (t = 7.935, p < .001) suggesting that those subjected to the beneficial manipulation

were more likely to assume that their own dealership owed the automaker in the future.

Table #14 – Coefficients from regression of Locus on manipulation factorsa Model

Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients

t Sig. B Std. Error Beta 1 (Constant) -.912 .108 -8.429 .000

Valence .970 .122 .572 7.935 .000 Magnitude .122 .120 .073 1.013 .313 Attribution .250 .119 .150 2.096 .038

a. Dependent Variable: Locus

Page 92: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

79

Interestingly, the attribution manipulation appears to impact the locus as well (standardized β =

.150, t = 2.096, p < .038) which means that when participants were given the internal attribution

condition, they were more likely to perceive the locus of the reciprocal debt with their own

dealership, rather than assign it to the automaker.

Thus, H1 and H3 receive support regarding the essential locus of the reciprocal debt.

Level or Intensity of Reciprocal Debt

It was expected that the manipulation of the magnitude of the automaker’s beneficial or

detrimental action would be associated with a greater level or intensity of reciprocal debt. But

greater intensity or level of a participant reciprocal debt is not equivalent to lower automaker

reciprocal debt. As it is possible that a participant reciprocal debt may not be conceptually and

functionally the converse of an automaker reciprocal debt, pooling the data across the two

valence conditions could mask differences or effects unique to each condition. That is, a simple

magnitude main effect would be unlikely, even if magnitude had been effectively manipulated.

Given that magnitude was not effectively manipulated, the participants’ perception of the

impact of the action on their own outcomes (OwnOut) is a theoretically sound proxy for the

level or intensity of the action in the scenario. Thus, I examine the impact of OwnOut on the

amount and locus (RDLocus) of reciprocal debt created. Figure #12 below shows a scatterplot of

the RDLocus values based on OwnOut perceptions.

Page 93: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

80

Figure #12 – Scatterplot of RDLocus per OwnOut

RDLocus was regressed on OwnOut using PASW 18.0. Support for H1 and H3 would be

found by a significant positive coefficient of OwnOut on RDLocus. The regression was significant

(F = 38.716, d.f. = 125, p < .001) and explained approximately 23% of the variance in RDLocus

(adj R2 = .232). Table #15 shows that the standardized coefficient for OwnOut was .488

(t=6.222, p < .001).

Table #15 – Coefficients from regression of RDLocus on OwnOuta Model

Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients

t Sig. B Std. Error Beta 1 (Constant) -14.404 1.575 -9.148 .000

OwnOut 2.333 .375 .488 6.222 .000 a. Dependent Variable: RDLocus

Overall, both ANOVA based analysis of the locus variable, and regression based analysis

of the RDLocus variable have provided support for H1 and H3. Thus we can conclude that when

a beneficial action is perceived, the participant believed they owed their automaker. Further

the greater benefits they perceived from the action translated into a greater debt they felt they

Page 94: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

81

owed the automaker. Conversely, detrimental actions resulted in the participant’s being more

likely to believe the automaker owed them in the future, and the more they felt slighted by the

automaker’s decision, the greater the intensity of reciprocal debt they perceived.

4.1.5.2 Moderating Impacts on the Creation of Reciprocal Debt

The hypotheses about moderating impacts of various variables were tested using

multiple procedures. Regressions were run with interaction terms for each of these moderating

variables proposed in the model. Unfortunately, none of the moderators were significant.

Interaction terms for each of these variables were multiplied by the OwnOut variable to test the

hypothesized moderation. The following regression equation was used to test these moderating

hypotheses:

RDLocus = α + β1OwnOut + β2Xi + β3XiOwnOut + e

Where xi = the proposed moderating variables

Feasible variations of this regression were also tested by dropping the xi term and only testing

the interaction. Additionally, the sample was split based on the valence manipulation to

investigate if there were differential effects of any of the variables for the detrimental

conditions than there were for the beneficial conditions. In no case were any of the interaction

terms significant. A stepwise regression was performed with all of the interaction terms and

main effects terms, only 2 variables were found to significantly impact the creation of RDLocus –

Valence and Justifiability.

Page 95: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

82

Table #16 – Tests of Between-Subjects Effects Dependent Variable:RDLocus Source Type III Sum

of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Corrected Model 2934.194a 3 978.065 12.394 .000 Intercept 1715.297 1 1715.297 21.736 .000 OwnOutHi 286.896 1 286.896 3.635 .059 JustHi 694.201 1 694.201 8.797 .004 OwnOutHi * JustHi 134.917 1 134.917 1.710 .193 Error 9627.749 122 78.916 Total 16953.669 126 Corrected Total 12561.943 125 a. R Squared = .234 (Adjusted R Squared = .215)

After completing the regression analyses described above, all of the proposed

moderators were tested with ANOVA’s after median splitting the moderator variables. In no

case were any of the moderators significant in predicting the mean difference of RDLocus.

Justifiability had a direct effect on RDLocus, table #16 (above) shows the results of a General

Linear Model explaining the variance in RDLocus based on OwnOutHi – a variable created by

performing a median split on the OwnOut measure, JustHi – median split on justifiability, and

the interaction of those 2 dichotomous variables. As can be seen, both variables have a

significant main effect (OwnOutHi F = 3.635, ρ = .059; JustHi F = 8.797, ρ = .004) however the

interaction term fails to reach significance (F = 1.71, ρ = .193). Planned contrasts showed that

when justifiability was high (JustHi = 1), the link between OwnOut and RDLocus was significantly

different (F = 8.797, ρ = .004), providing some support for the moderating impact of justifiability

(see tables #17 and 18 below), however as was already discussed, the interaction term in the

General Linear Model was not significant (see table #16 above).

Page 96: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

83

Table #17 – Planned Contrast Results (K Matrix) Justifiability JustHi Simple Contrasta Dependen

t Variable RDLocus

Level 1 vs. Level 2 Contrast Estimate -7.047 Hypothesized Value 0 Difference (Estimate - Hypothesized) -7.047 Std. Error 2.376 Sig. .004 95% Confidence Interval for Difference

Lower Bound -11.751 Upper Bound -2.344

a. Reference category = 2

Table #18 - Test Results for planned contrast Dependent Variable:RDLocus Source Sum of

Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Contrast 694.201 1 694.201 8.797 .004 Error 9627.749 122 78.916

Figure #13 below illustrates the difference in RDLocus given different values for the OwnOutHi

and JustHi variables.

Figure #13 – Estimated Marginal Means of RDLocus

Page 97: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

84

Therefore, none of the moderating hypotheses can be supported. Possibly a larger sample size

or more diverse group of respondents might help induce some variance which would help

detect the proposed relationships. A summary of the hypotheses tested in this section is

provided in table #19.

Hypothesis Hypothesis Wording Supported?

H1 Beneficial Action (+) → Customer Reciprocal Debt H3 Detrimental Action (+) → Seller Reciprocal Debt

H5aParticipant Relative Benefits moderates (+) link between Beneficial Action and Customer Reciprocal Debt

H5bPartner Relative Benefits moderates (+) the link between Detrimental Action and Seller Reciprocal Debt

H6aExternal Attribution of Control moderates (-) the link between Beneficial Action and Customer Reciprocal Debt

H6bExternal Attribution of Control moderates (-) the link between Detrimental Action and Seller Reciprocal Debt

H7Perceived Justifiability moderates (-) the link between Detrimental Action and Seller Reciprocal Debt

H8aPositive Norm of Reciprocity moderates (+) the link between Beneficial Action and Customer Reciprocal Debt

H8bNegative Norm of Reciprocity moderates (+) the link between Detrimental Action and Seller Reciprocal Debt

H9aBenefit-Based Dependence moderates (-) the link between Beneficial Action and Customer Reciprocal Debt

H9bCost-Based Dependence moderates (-) the link between Detrimental Action and Seller Reciprocal Debt

H10Benefit-Based Dependence enhances the moderating impact of Cost-Based Dependence

Table #19 - Car Dealer Survey Hypothesis Summary

4.1.6 Discussion

With such disappointing results from the analyses of moderation hypotheses, it seems

appropriate to address some possibilities that may exist in the current data, or may be

possibilities for future research. While this discussion section addresses theory issues that were

not proposed in my dissertation, it seems valuable to at least try to make some sense of the

Page 98: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

85

non-findings. In that spirit, three possible future directions will be discussed that might help

explain the lack of findings of this research.

The first topic can be addressed with data collected in this survey. This research was

designed with the idea or plan of investigating variables that would enhance feelings of

reciprocal debt – debt owned by either the respondent, or their business partner. The

hypotheses were mostly in line with that thinking. It seems logical that if people have a greater

personal norm of positive reciprocity, they would be likely to respond more strongly when

someone does something that benefits them. This thinking assumes that the baseline response

for business partners is minimal, or non-existent. Only when the large group of moderating

variables line up to enhance the reciprocal debt will we see greater magnitude or strength of

response. Alternatively, it could be that the baseline, or norm, for these partners is a

reasonable reciprocal response. This alternative assumption suggests that the moderators

proposed in this research really don’t do much to enhance the reciprocal response because the

reciprocal response will already be substantial. If this is the case, it might be valuable to look at

variables that might minimize the need or desire for partners to reciprocate. For instance, when

the respondent has a great deal of trust in their automaker, maybe the detrimental action will

not be reciprocated. This suggests that some relational variables may actually diminish negative

reciprocation between business partners. Other variables that might exhibit similar diminishing

effects on reciprocation might include: commitment, loyalty, and the duration of the

relationship. All of these variables were measured in this survey. Analyses will be conducted in

the future to examine if these relational variables have any moderating impact on the creation

of reciprocal debt that might help better explain differences in reciprocal responses.

A second topic came up during the planning of this research that was not addressed in

these analyses. One challenge that was originally conceptualized with this research was the

Page 99: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

86

difficulty in finding specific reciprocal responses within an ongoing relationship. One reason this

is problematic is that it is possible that an existing reciprocal debt in the relationship might

impact future responses to impactful actions. Secondly, and more importantly, as a relationship

develops and becomes more relational in nature, it is expected that the relationship partners

will focus less and less on responding to specific actions but instead view the relationship in its

entirety. Actions that are within some normal operating range will be basically overlooked as

they are viewed as just a part of the relationship. In this case, it would take a major event that

might alter the dynamics of the relationship in order to capture substantial instances of

reciprocation. It is possible that our scenarios were not viewed as any major event by the car

dealers. Some evidence for this argument exists in the fact that the magnitude manipulation

appeared to be totally unsuccessful in affecting the participants. While a number of brief

interviews were conducted with car dealership managers during the planning and development

of this research, it is possible that a better – more impactful – scenario might be able to better

capture reciprocal responses in this context.

Finally, the car dealership – automaker context was probably not the best context for

this research. Originally we had planned on doing this survey in a B2B setting that involved large

investments of personal interactions by both parties – a marketing firm and their customers.

That context was probably a much better context to study the reciprocation of business partners

as more interpersonal relationships would be created as members of both firms had to work in

close proximity, and rely on members of the other firm to accomplish their own goals. It is

unclear in the automotive industry how prevalent interpersonal relationships are between car

dealer managers and automaker representatives. If these interpersonal relationships are not

perceived as being important for the entities involved, then the likelihood of finding much

variance in the response to an impactful action would be much lower. We knew from the

Page 100: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

87

beginning that contractual relationships between car dealerships and auto manufacturers would

not be the best context for this research; however the timing and costs of finding a more fitting

context (after our original company fell through) were prohibitive for this research. Ideally a

B2B services setting would be explored where interpersonal relationships were more common

and more important to the functioning of the relationship.

Page 101: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

88

4.2 Laboratory Experiment

4.2.1 Research Model

Figure #14 below illustrates the research model tested with the laboratory experiment.

The primary purpose of this research is to test the mediation of reciprocal debt leading to

changes in partner outcomes.

Figure #15 (next page) summarizes the stages of the laboratory experiment –

Preliminary Procedures, Game One – Dictator Game, and Game Two – Investment Game. Each

stage involved several steps and points of data capture, which are clearly illustrated in Figure

#15. Descriptions of each of these stages, and the data collected follow Figure #15.

Partner Action

Recipient Relative

Outcomes

Participant Reciprocal Debt

Partner Outcomes

Recipient Norm of Reciprocity

Figure #14 – Experimental Research Model Tested

Page 102: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Prel

imin

ary

Proc

edur

es

Pre-

expe

rim

enta

l Q

uest

ionn

aire

Gam

e 2

Dir

ectio

ns

Gam

e 2

Allo

catio

n D

ecis

ion

Acce

pt/R

ejec

t Al

loca

tion

Figu

re #

15:

Expe

rim

enta

l Pro

cess

Flo

wch

art

Gam

e 1

Allo

catio

nEx

peri

men

ter

Act

ion

Part

icip

ant

Act

ion

Post

Gam

e 1

Que

stio

nnai

re

Dat

a Co

llect

ed

•Nor

m o

f Pos

itive

Re

cipr

ocity

•Nor

m o

f Neg

ativ

e Re

cipr

ocity

•Acc

epta

nce

•Ow

n Im

pact

•Par

tner

Impa

ct•R

elat

ive

Out

com

es•R

ecip

roca

l Deb

t•L

ocus

of D

ebt

•Par

tner

At

trac

tiven

ess

•Par

tner

Out

com

es

roset
Typewritten Text
89
Page 103: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

90

Preliminary Procedures

Random Assignment to Conditions. Each participant was randomly assigned to one of 4

conditions in the 2 [valence: detrimental vs. beneficial] X 2 [magnitude: low vs. high] full

factorial experimentation design. A total of 175 undergraduate students participated in this

experiment for extra credit. Figure #16 below summarizes the random assignment.

Low (0) High (1)

n = 42 n = 45

(females = 20) (females = 20)

(males = 22) (males = 25)

n = 44 n = 44

(females = 29) (females = 17)(males = 15) (males = 27)

N = 175

Random Assignment to Conditions

Figure #16

Beneficial (+1)

MAGNITUDE

VALENCE

Detrimental (-1)

The participants entered the experimental lab and took their seats.

Preliminary Questionnaire. First, participants completed the pre-experiment questionnaire--a

battery of questions capturing demographic information, their adherence to the positive norm

of reciprocity and their adherence to the negative norm of reciprocity (same items as prior

description).

Preliminary Instructions. Participants were told:

• they would be engaging in a series of bargaining, negotiation, and decision-making games; • with one randomly assigned Game Partner among the other participants in the same room; • they would interact with that paired Game Partner via a computer-mediated exchange that would record their decisions;

Page 104: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

91

• although the procedures for each game would vary, each game would begin with one Game Partner being given $10 and that each round would involve an allocation offer splitting that $10 from one Game Partner and either acceptance or rejection of that offer by the paired Game Partner; • although the participants were asked to accept or reject the initial allocation, the only clear explanation of the ramifications of rejecting the allocation was that neither party would get any of the $10 during the first game; • the number of rounds within each game could vary from only one round up to several rounds and that neither Game Partner knew when each game would end; • the amount of money the participant had when each game ended would be banked; and • the primary objective was to maximize the amount banked across all games.

The interactions were actually generated by the experimental software (MediaLab v2008).

Participants engaged in two distinct games--first, the Dictator Game in which the mythical

counterpart was the dictator and second, the Investment Game in which the participant made

the opening allocation decision. Each game will be discussed below.

Game One - Dictator Game

Background Information. Each participant was told that:

• His/her counterpart had been given $10 with instructions to divide that sum between himself/herself and the paired Game Partner, i.e. the participant. The Game Partner would make the Initial Allocation Offer. • The participant would then make his or her Offer Response -- whether to accept or reject that offer. • An initial allocation offer and subsequent offer response constituted one round of the Dictator Game. Participants were told the game was over after 1 round.

Manipulations: Partner's Initial Allocation Offer. The Dictator Game began. After a delay of 20

seconds – during which the participant was told their Game Partner was making their allocation

decision -- each participant received an initial allocation offer based on the condition to which

they were randomly assigned, as indicated in Figure #17 below.

Page 105: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

92

Low (0) High (1)

Participant: $3 Participant: $1

Game Partner: $7 Game Partner: $9

Participant: $7 Participant: $9

Game Partner: $3 Game Partner: $1

Figure #17Manipulations: Partner's Initial Allocation Offer

MAGNITUDE

VALENCE

Detrimental (-1)

Beneficial (+1)

Thus, in the detrimental action conditions, it appeared that the Game Partner kept the majority

of the $10; the participant was offered less than the Game Partner would keep. In the beneficial

action conditions, it appeared that the Game Partner allocated the majority of $10 to the

participant. Two levels of the magnitude of the disparity were examined. Ultimately, each

participant received an initial allocation offer from the Game Partner of either:

$9 (Game Partner would keep $1) in the high/beneficial condition, $7 (Game Partner would keep $3) in the low/beneficial condition, $3 (Game Partner would keep $7) in the low/detrimental condition, or $1 (Game Partner would keep $9) in the high/detrimental condition.

Offer Response. After being presented with the proposed allocation, the participant was asked

to either accept or reject the offered split. The software captured the participant's offer

response -- acceptance or rejection.

Post-Game Questionnaire. Following the offer response, each participant completed a post-

game questionnaire assessing perceptions about the positive or negative impact of the

proposed division of $10 and about the reciprocity debt created by this interaction.

Page 106: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

93

The participant was then told that the first game was over and the participant and Game Partner

would bank the funds resulting from Game 1. Game 2 would begin fresh with a new $10

available for allocation.

Game Two -- Investment Game

As with the first game, Game 2 involved splitting $10 between the paired game

partners. Unlike in Game 1, where the Game Partner made the initial allocation, in Game 2, the

participant had control of the initial allocation. The participant could divide the $10 in any way

desired, knowing that the Game Partner would then be able to return a portion of their

allocation to the participant. Although Game 2 continued after this point, the final measure

taken was the participant's allocation decision. The amount the participant allocated to the

Game Partner is the focal dependent variable in this experiment -- partner outcomes.

4.2.2 Operationalizations

Measures were captured by the experimental software and included both attitude

scales and reports of decisions/actions made by the participant.

Preliminary Questionnaire

The preliminary questionnaire included a battery of questions capturing demographic

information, the participants’ adherence to the positive norm of reciprocity and their adherence

to the negative norm of reciprocity. Items were adapted from Perugini et al (2003) to capture

both the positive and negative norms of reciprocity. 6 items were used to capture the

participant's adherence to the positive norm of reciprocity (pr1 - pr6) and to the negative norm

of reciprocity (nr1 - nr6) using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7

(Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the midpoint.

Positive Norm of Reciprocity

1. When I profit from the actions of someone else, I will do something that benefits him/her in return.

Page 107: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

94

2. When someone does me a favor, I feel committed to repay the favor. 3. If someone does a favor for me, I will return the favor. 4. I am ready to incur personal costs to help someone who has helped me. 5. I go out of my way to help a person who has been kind to me. 6. I'm willing to do things I don't enjoy to return someone's assistance.

Negative Norm of Reciprocity

1. If someone purposely injures me, I will try to injure him/her as well. 2. If somebody takes action that damages me, I will go to great lengths to inflict damage on him/her. 3. If somebody is impolite to me, I become impolite in return. 4. I am willing to invest time and effort to reciprocate a harmful action. 5. If someone puts me in a difficult position, I will do the same to him/her. 6. I will be unfair to someone who has been unfair to me.

Allocation Response -- Offer Acceptance or Rejection

Each participant's response to the Game Partner's initial allocation in Game 1(Dictator Game)

was recorded. The participant's affirmative response (acceptance of the offer) was coded "1,"

while a negative response (rejection of the offer) was coded "0."

Post-Game Questionnaire

Immediately following the participant's acceptance response, the participant completed a

questionnaire assessing perceptions and reactions regarding the Game Partner's actions and

their own reactions.

Partner Actions

Each participant assessed the impact of the Game Partners’ actions during Game 1 on the

participant's outcome and the partner’s outcomes by completing two questions using 7-point

Likert scales.

• Own Impact (OwnImpact): How have the outcomes of this game affected you? [1 = “Very negative effect on me;” 7 = “Very positive effect on me”] • Partner Impact (PartnerImpact): How do you think the outcomes of this game affected your partner? [1 = “Very negative effect on my partner;” 7 = “Very positive effect on my patner”] • Relative Outcomes (ROut) were computed by subtracting PartnerImpact from OwnImpact. As the two items were assessed consecutively on consistent scales, I believe

Page 108: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

95

that this calculated variable inherently reflects the participant's perception of the extent to which his/her outcomes were relatively greater or lower than the partner's.

Reciprocal Debts

Extant reciprocity literature was referenced to help develop items to assess the participant's

perceptions regarding reciprocity debts for this research. A large pool of possible scale items

was developed to capture the perceived reciprocal debt. After further consideration and a

pretest to examine measurement properties, a smaller set of measures was selected to assess

these latent constructs. Three items capturing reciprocal debt were measured using 7-point

Likert scales [1 = "Strongly Disagree;" 4 = "Neither Disagree nor Agree;" 7 = "Strongly Agree"]

and one item assessed the locus of the debt using forced choice.

• Existence of Reciprocal Debt (RD): Indicate your agreement or disagreement with the following statements.

1. An obligation has been created that should be repaid. 2. Something should be done to make up for this situation. 3. This game has created an imbalance between my partner and myself that needs to be addressed.

• Locus of Reciprocal Debt (Locus): Given the outcome of the game you just played, which statement below best captures your opinion of what should happen?

1. My partner should do something to balance out the outcomes. [coded -1] 2. I should do something to balance out the outcomes. [coded +1] 3. Neither my partner nor I need to do anything to balance out the outcomes. [coded 0]

Directional Reciprocal Debt (RecipDebt) was calculated by multiplying the multi-item latent

reciprocal debt measure by the coded locus dummy variable. This variable represents the

intensity and directionality of the perceived reciprocal debt, ranging from strong participant

reciprocity debt (at high positive values of this variable) to strong partner reciprocity debt (at

low negative values of this variable).

Partner Attractiveness

Four items measured the perceived the attractiveness of the Game Partner. These items were

created for this research, and were designed to capture the degree to which a participant liked

Page 109: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

96

their partner. Participants rated their agreement or disagreement with all items on 7-point

Likert scales [1 = "Strongly Disagree;" 4 = "Neither Disagree nor Agree;" 7 = "Strongly Agree"].

1. I could see myself becoming good friends with this person. 2. I think this person and I are a lot alike. 3. I like my partner very much. 4. I think my partner is nice.

Participant's Allocation Decision in Game 2 -- Partner Outcomes

After reciprocal debt was measured, the participant began Game 2 with the same Game

Partner. If a reciprocal debt was created during Game 1, the participant's control of the initial

allocation decision in Game 2 provides an opportunity for the participant to resolve that

reciprocal debt. Therefore, the $ amount allocated to the partner is used to measure partner

outcomes (PartnerOut).

4.2.3 Sample Selection

Participants were undergraduate students at a Midwestern University. Participants

received extra credit in a marketing class for their participation in the study. Three separate

classes were recruited to participate in these experiments.

4.2.4 Measurement Model and Scale Creation

4.2.4.1 Exploratory Factor Analyses

Exploratory factor analyses using SPSS 17.0 was conducted with all 4 latent constructs

(positive norm of reciprocity, negative norm of reciprocity, reciprocity debt and partner

attractiveness). The analysis summarized in Table #20 revealed 5 factors with all items loading

on the a priori latent constructs except for the positive norm of reciprocity, which loaded onto 2

factors. Follow-up EFA on each separate construct summarized in Table #21 confirmed

unidimensionality for these 4 latent constructs. Again, the analysis of the items assessing the

positive norm of reciprocity revealed 2 factors with items 1-3 loading on one factor and items 4-

Page 110: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

97

6 loading on a separate factor. After reviewing the items for the positive norm of reciprocity, it

is not surprising that the factor structure exhibits some multi-dimensionality. Items 1-3 are

much more concrete in nature, specifying particular responses to particular actions by an

exchange partner, while items 4-6 are much more subjective in nature – e.g., “I’m willing to do

things I don’t enjoy to return someone’s assistance”. Although further analysis is needed to

determine the best factor structure for the positive norm of reciprocity, I conclude that the EFAs

provide strong evidence for the discriminant validity of the latent constructs, but confirmatory

factor analyses were conducted to further examine the measures.

1 2 3 4 5

Eigenvalue 3.842 3.551 2.873 1.412 1.022

nr1 .737nr2 .822nr3 .622nr4 .775nr5 .793nr6 .758pr1 .726pr2 .733pr3 .779pr4 .536 .501pr5 .850pr6 .829rd1 .646rd2 .755rd3 .871pa1 .865pa2 .785pa3 .910pa4 .881

a. Rotation converged in 5 iterations.

Table #20: Combined EFA on 4 Latent Constructs

Rotated Component Matrixa

Component

Extraction Method: Principal component Analysis.

Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.

Page 111: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

98

Eigenvalue 3.842 3.056 2.871 1.023 1.975% Var. Extracted 57.23% 76.41% 47.85% 17.06% 65.84%# of factors with Eigenvalue > 1

1 1 1 of 2a 2 of 2a 1

nr1 .724nr2 .812nr3 .637nr4 .766nr5 .807nr6 .779pr1 .732pr2 .759pr3 .783pr4 .582pr5 .876pr6 .839rd1 .811rd2 .838rd3 .784pa1 .893pa2 .786pa3 .918pa4 .893

Table #21: Summary of Individual EFA's for 4 Latent Constructs

Extraction Method: Principal component Analysis.

a. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.

4.2.4.2 Confirmatory Factor Analyses

An overall measurement model including all four latent constructs (positive norm of

reciprocity - PR, negative norm of reciprocity - NR, reciprocal debt - RD and partner

attractiveness - PA) was estimated using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) in EQS 6.1 for

windows. This measurement model was estimated using maximum likelihood estimation with

elliptical reweighted generalized least square (ERLS) because ERLS estimates are equivalent to

ML for normally distributed data, and superior to ML for non-normal data (Zou and Cavusgil

2002).

Page 112: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

99

Upon examination of the initial CFA, scales were purified to develop more internally

consistent and valid measures of the latent constructs. Four items were retained from the

original NR (nr1, nr2, nr4 and nr5) scale; nr3 and nr6 were eliminated due to factor loadings

below .5 or R2 of less than .3. Three items were retained from the PR scale (pr4, pr5 and pr6),

the three which loaded on the second factor in the exploratory factor analyses. While the

exploratory factor analyses appeared to suggest that pr1-pr3 might be the best items (explained

the most variance in the latent construct), pr1-pr3 were dropped due to R2 values of

approximately .3 and lower standardized factor loadings. While this was an iterative process,

the best fitting model used pr4-pr6. All three RD items were retained. Three of the four PA items

were retained; pa2 was dropped based on the modification indices. Further consideration of

the conceptual content of these items helped solidify the decision to drop pa2 as it asked a

substantively different question than the other three partner attractiveness items.

The final measurement model incorporating the final items for each construct had

acceptable fit statistics: χ2 = 120.82 (d.f. = 59), p < .001; Bentler-Bonnett Non-Normed Fit Index

= .903; CFI = .927; RMSEA = .078 (see table #22 below). All items loaded on the a priori factors,

with positive and significant variances, and factor loadings of at least .541. Factor weighted

combinations of the final scale items were used in subsequent analyses based on the factor

loadings in table #22.

Page 113: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Ta

ble

#22

– CF

A R

esul

ts

Fact

or L

oadi

ngs

Item

#

Item

Po

sitiv

e Re

cipr

ocity

N

egat

ive

Reci

proc

ity

Reci

proc

al

Deb

t Pa

rtic

ipan

t A

ttra

ctiv

enes

s pr

4 I a

m re

ady

to in

cur

pers

onal

cos

ts to

hel

p so

meo

ne w

ho h

as h

elpe

d m

e.

0.57

7

pr5

I go

out o

f my

way

to h

elp

a pe

rson

who

has

bee

n ki

nd to

me.

0.

785

pr

6 I'm

will

ing

to d

o th

ings

I do

n't e

njoy

to re

turn

som

eone

's a

ssis

tanc

e.

0.75

4

nr1

If so

meo

ne p

urpo

sely

inju

res

me,

I w

ill tr

y to

inju

re h

im/h

er a

s w

ell.

0.

754

nr2

If so

meb

ody

take

s ac

tion

that

dam

ages

me,

I w

ill g

o to

gre

at le

ngth

s to

in

flict

dam

age

on h

im/h

er.

0.

888

nr4

I am

will

ing

to in

vest

tim

e an

d ef

fort

to r

ecip

roca

te a

har

mfu

l act

ion.

0.67

2

nr

5 If

som

eone

put

s m

e in

a d

iffic

ult p

ositi

on, I

will

do

the

sam

e to

him

/her

.

0.62

7

rd

1 A

n ob

ligat

ion

has

been

cre

ated

that

sho

uld

be r

epai

d.

0.81

4

rd2

Som

ethi

ng s

houl

d be

don

e to

mak

e up

for

this

situ

atio

n.

0.69

8

rd3

This

gam

e ha

s cr

eate

d an

imba

lanc

e be

twee

n m

y pa

rtne

r an

d m

ysel

f th

at n

eeds

to b

e ad

dres

sed.

0.

541

pa1

I cou

ld s

ee m

ysel

f bec

omin

g go

od fr

iend

s w

ith th

is p

erso

n.

0.

834

pa3

I lik

e m

y pa

rtne

r ver

y m

uch.

0.90

8 pa

4 I t

hink

my

part

ner

is n

ice.

0.88

9

χ2

= 12

0.82

(d.f.

= 5

9), p

< .0

01

Be

ntle

r-Bo

nnet

t Non

-Nor

med

Fit

Inde

x =

.903

CFI =

.927

; RM

SEA

= .0

78

roset
Typewritten Text
100
Page 114: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

101

4.2.4.3 Manipulation Checks

An ANOVA was conducted using SPSS’s General Linear Model module to investigate if

the experimental conditions effectively manipulated the experimental variables of valence and

magnitude of the partner's actions. The participant's perceptions regarding the impact of the

partner's initial allocation in Game 1 was used as a manipulation check.

Valence Manipulation. With respect to all 3 measures (OwnImpact, PartnerImpact and

ROut), the valence manipulation had a significant main effect (see table #23). Thus, I

conclude that the valence of the partner's action was successfully manipulated.

Magnitude Manipulation. In contrast, magnitude had neither a direct nor indirect effect

on any of the three perceived outcome measures (see table #24). Thus, I conclude that

magnitude was not successfully manipulated.

Table #23 – ANOVA’s with the Valence manipulation as the independent variable.

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

RD Between Groups 213.198 1 213.198 31.870 .000

Within Groups 1157.317 173 6.690

Total 1370.514 174

RDlocus Between Groups 7958.746 1 7958.746 206.900 .000

Within Groups 6654.717 173 38.467

Total 14613.463 174

OwnImpact Between Groups 287.344 1 287.344 204.928 .000

Within Groups 242.576 173 1.402

Total 529.920 174

PartnerImpact Between Groups 113.324 1 113.324 30.914 .000

Within Groups 634.184 173 3.666

Total 747.509 174

ROut Between Groups 761.573 1 761.573 135.724 .000

Within Groups 970.736 173 5.611

Total 1732.309 174

Page 115: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

102

Table #24 – ANOVA’s with the Magnitude manipulation as the independent variable.

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

RD Between Groups .718 1 .718 .091 .764

Within Groups 1369.796 173 7.918

Total 1370.514 174

RDlocus Between Groups 54.093 1 54.093 .643 .424

Within Groups 14559.370 173 84.158

Total 14613.463 174

OwnImpact Between Groups .003 1 .003 .001 .975

Within Groups 529.917 173 3.063

Total 529.920 174

PartnerImpact Between Groups .239 1 .239 .055 .814

Within Groups 747.269 173 4.319

Total 747.509 174

ROut Between Groups .295 1 .295 .030 .864

Within Groups 1732.013 173 10.012

Total 1732.309 174

This has two implications for hypothesis-testing analyses. First, as it is possible that partner's

actions could impact reciprocal debt formation or the participant's subsequent actions even

without conscious awareness of partner initiating action, I will use ANOVA to examine the

impact of both valence and manipulation factors. However, it could be argued that it is the

participant's perception of the partner's action that is critical, for those perceptions will

determine subsequent reactions. Participants' perceptions of the impact of their partner’s

action (OwnImpact) vary within the sample (μ = 4.76, σ2 = 3.046). Therefore, I will also examine

the participants' perceptions of the impact of the partner's actions (OwnImpact) on other

variables using regression analysis.

4.2.6 Hypothesis-Testing

Figures #18 and 19 illustrate the hypotheses tested in this experimental portion of the

research.

Page 116: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

103

Effects of Partner Actions on Reciprocal Debts

The hypotheses were adapted to the experimental context, and hence the wording has

changed slightly, however they are substantively the same hypotheses as have been discussed

previously. The first hypotheses concern the impact of the partner's beneficial or detrimental

actions on the participant's perception of reciprocal debts. Specifically:

Beneficial Partner Action

Participant’s Positive Norm of

Reciprocity

Participant’s Reciprocal Debt

H1+ Partner Outcomes

H8a+

H2+

Participant’s Perceived Relative

Benefits

H5a+

Figure #18 – Conceptual Framework for Beneficial Actions

Detrimental Partner Action

Participant’s Negative Norm of Reciprocity

Partner’s Reciprocal Debt

Partner Outcomes

H3+

H8b+

H4-

Perceived Partner Relative

Benefits

H5b+

Figure #19 – Conceptual Framework for Detrimental Actions

Page 117: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

104

H1: Beneficial partner actions will lead to a participant reciprocal debt.

H3: Detrimental partner actions will lead to a partner reciprocal debt.

As has been previously discussed, the term “participant reciprocal debt” implies that the

participant will perceive that they owe their partner something. Likewise, the “partner

reciprocal debt” implies that the participant perceives their partner to owe them something in

the future. I examine these hypotheses using various analyses, first focusing on the locus of

reciprocal debt and then examining the level or intensity of the reciprocal debt.

Locus of Reciprocal Debt

First, basic ANOVA’s were conducted examining the impact of the experimental factors

(valence and magnitude of partner actions) on the locus of the reciprocal debt which ranges

from participant's debt (+1) to partner's debt (-1). As can be seen in Figure #20, participants in

the detrimental condition (valence = -1) were much more likely to assign a reciprocal debt to

their partner (locus = -1), while those in the beneficial condition (valence = 1) assumed that they

owed their partner a debt (locus = 1; F = 222.051, d.f. = 173, ρ < .001).

Figure #20 – Impact of Valence on Locus (ANOVA)

Page 118: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

105

This was not the case when investigating the impact of the magnitude manipulation. A

simple ANOVA revealed no impact of the magnitude manipulation (F = .468, d.f. = 173, ρ = .495).

However, since the magnitude manipulation was crossed with the valence manipulation, a

simple ANOVA would be meaningless if the relationship between the conditions and the

outcome (here locus) is nonlinear. In order to verify that the magnitude manipulation had no

effect on the locus of a debt, the Univariate General Linear model was used in SPSS 17.0 which

allows the comparison of mean levels of the dependent variable based on multiple independent

variables and their interactions (see table #25 below). The magnitude manipulation failed to

provide any direct effects (F = .591, d.f. = 174, p = .443), and further did not have an indirect

effect when the interaction of valence and magnitude was considered (F = .918, d.f. = 174, p =

.339).

Table #25 - General Linear Model Test of Mean Differences

Dependent Variable:Locus

Source Type III Sum of

Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

Corrected Model 82.711a 3 27.570 74.317 .000

Intercept 1.707 1 1.707 4.601 .033

Magnitude .219 1 .219 .591 .443

Valence 82.134 1 82.134 221.398 .000

Magnitude * Valence .341 1 .341 .918 .339

Error 63.438 171 .371

Total 148.000 175

Corrected Total 146.149 174

a. R Squared = .566 (Adjusted R Squared = .558)

Further examination of the data supports the hypothesized relationship between the

valence of an action and the locus of the reciprocal debt. Crosstabs showed that the debt was

more likely to be perceived as the participant's own debt when the partner's actions were

beneficial (+1) than when they were detrimental (-1). In the beneficial condition, 84%

Page 119: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

106

considered the participant responsible for resolving the debt. In the detrimental condition, only

10% considered the participant responsible, but 69% consider the partner responsible

(compared to 5.7% in the beneficial condition) for resolving the debt (see figure #21 below). No

such differences were found for the magnitude manipulation (see figure #22 below).

Figure #21 Locus * Valence Crosstabulation

Count

Valence

Total -1.00 1.00

Locus -1.00 60 5 65

.00 18 9 27

1.00 9 74 83

Total 87 88 175

Figure #22 Locus * Magnitude Crosstabulation

Count

Magnitude

Total .00 1.00

Locus -1.00 31 34 65

.00 11 16 27

1.00 44 39 83

Total 86 89 175

This suggests that in the beneficial condition, the participant perceived a reciprocal debt

that they owed their partner -- and that it is primarily the participant's responsibility to take

action to resolve that debt. The converse was true in the detrimental condition; the participant

was more likely to perceive a reciprocal debt owed by their partner -- and that the partner had

greater responsibility to resolve that debt.

Page 120: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

107

Although the ANOVA and crosstabs presented above provide substantial support for H1

and H3, regression based analysis was also conducted to provide additional support for the

hypotheses. First, because the locus variable was a trichotomous indicator (-1, 0 and 1), a

multinomial logistic regression was performed in SPSS 17.0 with locus as the dependent

variable, and the valence manipulation as the independent variable. This model with only one

explanatory variable provided a better fit than a null model (Δχ2 = 115.994, d.f. = 2, p < .001),

statistically significant coefficients for the impact of valence (see table #26 below), and

accurately predicted 76.6% of the locus classifications (see table #27 below).

Table #26 - Parameter Estimates

Locusa B Std.

Error Wald df Sig. Exp(B)

95% Confidence Interval for Exp(B)

Lower Bound

Upper Bound

-1.00 Intercept -2.695 .462 34.007 1 .000

[Bene=-1.00] 4.592 .584 61.778 1 .000 98.667 31.398 310.058

[Bene=1.00] 0b . . 0 . . . .

.00 Intercept -2.107 .353 35.617 1 .000

[Bene=-1.00] 2.800 .540 26.914 1 .000 16.444 5.710 47.361

[Bene=1.00] 0b . . 0 . . . .

a. The reference category is: 1.00.

b. This parameter is set to zero because it is redundant.

Table #27 - Classification Results of Multinomial Regression for Locus

Observed Locus

Predicted Locus

-1.00 .00 1.00 Percent Correct

-1.00 60 0 5 92.3%

.00 18 0 9 .0%

1.00 9 0 74 89.2%

Overall Percentage 49.7% .0% 50.3% 76.6%

Page 121: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

108

Thus, Hypotheses 1 and 3 receive strong support regarding the essential locus of the reciprocal

debt.

Level or Intensity of Reciprocal Debt

It was expected that the manipulation of the magnitude of the partner's beneficial or

detrimental partner action would be associated with greater level or intensity of reciprocal debt.

But greater intensity or level of a participant reciprocal debt is not equivalent to lower partner

reciprocal debt. As it is possible that a participant reciprocal debt may not be conceptually and

functionally the converse of a partner reciprocal debt, pooling the data across the two valence

conditions could mask differences or effects unique to each condition. That is, a simple

magnitude main effect would be unlikely, even if magnitude had been effectively manipulated.

Further, it is unclear how everyone would perceive the detrimental condition. For example,

when a partner splits up the $10 allocation and gives the participant $3, the planned

interpretation is that the participant got slighted by receiving a lesser amount. An equally valid

interpretation could be that the participant is lucky to receive anything because the $10 was

given to the partner and hence the $3 was a reasonable gesture at sharing the allotment. Thus,

I examine the participant's perceived impact of the partner's actions (OwnImpact) on the

amount of reciprocal debt created.

Because it is unclear the specific relationship we should expect to see across the entire

spectrum ranging from detrimental to beneficial actions and responses, an analysis technique

was chosen which would allow for the examination of beneficial and detrimental actions

possibly unique impacts on reciprocal debt. Spline regression allows for unique impacts of

separate “knots” of data without sacrificing the degrees of freedom that would be lost from

performing multiple distinct regressions on subsets of the data (Marsh and Cormier 2002). In

this analysis, the variable relative outcomes (Rout) was used as a break point to investigate the

Page 122: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

109

relationship between the participant’s perceptions of the impact of their partner’s actions on

their own outcomes (OwnImpact) and RDLocus – which should allow for the investigation of

how the magnitude of benefits received (or lost) will impact the participant’s (or partner’s)

reciprocal debt. Two knots were used from the overall data; the first was all participants who

perceived their relative outcomes to be less than 0 which signifies that their partner benefitted

more than they did – or the perception of a detrimental partner action. The second knot was all

those participants who perceived Rout to be positive – which signifies a beneficial partner

action. The following dummy variables were introduced to execute this analysis:

BenRout = 1 when Rout > 0; 0 otherwise

DetRout = 1 when Rout < 0; 0 otherwise

SplineBen = BenRout * OwnImpact

SplineDet = DetRout * OwnImpact

Support for H1 will be provided by SplineBen having a positive impact on RDLocus. Support for

H3 will be provided by a negative impact of SplineDet on RDLocus. The regression explained

approximately 34% of the variance in RDLocus (adj. R2 = .339), and the coefficient for SplineBen

was positive and significant (see table #28 below), however even though the coefficient for

SplineDet was negative as expected, it was not significant.

Table #28 - Spline Regression Resultsa

Model

Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients

t Sig. B Std. Error Beta

1 (Constant) -2.812 1.265 -2.223 .028

SplineBen 1.572 .241 .530 6.533 .000

SplineDet -.482 .456 -.086 -1.058 .292

a. Dependent Variable: RDlocus

Page 123: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

110

Further examination of these results highlights the fact that the constant in this

regression is largely negative at -2.812. RDLocus has a mean value of 1.817 (σ = 9.16). It is not

clear why the intercept should be negative for this regression, nor is it clear what the intercept

means if the spline knot is at zero – when both SplineBen and SplineDet are equal to zero, we

would expect a zero value for reciprocal debt. Because of this, a second spline regression was

run without an intercept in the model. The overall regression model is significant (F = 47.354,

d.f. = 175, p < .001) with an adjusted R2 = .346 (interpret this with caution as the lack of

intercept in the model creates a different meaning for this measure). The standardized

coefficient (see table #29 below) for SplineBen is positive and significant (β = .536, t = 8.77, p <

.001) and for SplineDet is negative and significant (β = -.257, t = -4.205, p < .001) providing

support for both H1 and H3 when the level or intensity of the reciprocal debt is considered.

Table #29 - Spline Regression Results with no Intercepta,b

Model

Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients

t Sig. B Std. Error Beta

1 SplineBen 1.116 .127 .536 8.777 .000

SplineDet -1.255 .298 -.257 -4.205 .000

a. Dependent Variable: RDlocus

b. Linear Regression through the Origin

Overall, both ANOVA based analysis of the locus variable, and regression based analysis

of the RDLocus variable have provided support for H1 and H3. Thus we can conclude that when

a beneficial action is perceived, the participant believed they owed their partner. Further the

greater benefits they perceived from the action translated into a greater debt they felt they

owed their partner. Conversely, detrimental actions resulted in the participant’s being more

Page 124: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

111

likely to believe their partner owed them in the future, and the more they felt slighted by their

partner, the greater the intensity of reciprocal debt they perceived.

Effect of Participant and Partner Reciprocal Debt on Partner Outcomes

The participant's allocation decision in Game 2 represents Partner Outcomes, that is, the

ramifications of the participant's response to the partner's actions in Game 1. Partner Outcomes

(PartnerOut) is measured as the dollars allocated by the participant to the partner in the first

round of Game 2, the partner's first opportunity to take action following the partner's beneficial

or detrimental action in Game 1. As with the prior analysis, H2 and H4 will be considered

together here. When considered together, these hypotheses suggest that when the participant

perceives that they owe their partner, PartnerOut will be larger than when they perceive that

their partner owes them. Both ANOVA’s and regression analyses will be performed to test these

hypotheses. The hypotheses are reprinted below:

H2: Participant reciprocal debt will lead to increased partner outcomes.

H4: Partner reciprocal debt will lead to decreased partner outcomes.

An ANOVA was performed comparing the level of partner outcomes based on the locus

of the reciprocal debt perceived by the participant. Planned contrasts were included as there

were three levels of the locus variable such that independent comparisons of the dependent

variable were analyzed at each of the three levels of locus. This ANOVA provides some support

for our hypotheses, as the mean of PartnerOut (4.546) when the participant perceive a partner

reciprocal debt (Locus = -1) was significantly lower than the mean of PartnerOut (5.831) when

they perceived that they owed their partner (Locus = 1). Neither partner nor participant locus

values were significantly different from the mean of PartnerOut when locus was set to 0. Figure

#23 below and tables #30 and 31 illustrate these findings.

Page 125: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

112

Figure #23 – ANOVA Results of PartnerOut at Different Locus

Table #30 - Contrast Coefficients

Contrast

Locus

-1.00 .00 1.00

1 -1 1 0

2 0 -1 1

3 -1 0 1

Table #31 - Contrast Tests

Contrast

Value of Contrast

Std. Error t df

Sig. (2-tailed)

PartnerOut

Assume equal variances

1 .528 .5328 .991 172 .323

2 .757 .5155 1.469 172 .144

3 1.285 .3854 3.335 172 .001

Does not assume equal variances

1 .528 .4972 1.062 53.002 .293

2 .757 .4844 1.563 49.746 .124

3 1.285 .3896 3.299 139.956 .001

The ANOVA results above provide ample support for H2 and H4; however it is possible

to consider that as the level or intensity of reciprocal debt increases we should expect an impact

Page 126: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

113

on partner outcomes. The preceding ANOVA simply tests that the mean value of partner

outcomes is greater when participants perceive their own reciprocal debt (locus = 1) rather than

their partner’s reciprocal debt (locus = -1). Regression analysis is required to examine if the

intensity of debt perceived has any impact on partner outcomes. Without theoretical reasoning

to assume a linear relationship between those who perceived their own reciprocal debt and

those who perceived a reciprocal debt from their partner, spline regression was again used.

New spline variables had to be created, as the spline knots now need to be separated based on

the locus of the debt rather than the relative outcomes used previously. The new variables that

were created are the following:

PosLocus = 1 when Locus > 0; 0 otherwise NegLocus = 1 when Locus < 0; 0 otherwise SplinePosRD = PosLocus * RDLocus SplineNegRD = NegLocus * RDLocus

PartnerOut was regressed on both SplinePosRD and SplineNegRD. Support for H2 would be

found by a significant positive coefficient for SplinePosRD. Support for H4 would be found by a

significant negative coefficient for SplineNegRD. The overall model was significant (F = 202.687,

d.f. = 175, p < .001), explaining approximately 70% of the variance in PartnerOut (adj R2 = .697).

The coefficient on SplinePosRD was positive and significant and the coefficient for SplineNegRD

was negative and significant (see table #32 below).

Table #32 - Spline Regression for RDLocus effect on PartnerOuta,b

Model

Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients

t Sig. B Std. Error Beta

1 SplinePosRD .539 .032 .702 16.882 .000

SplineNegRD -.475 .043 -.456 -10.971 .000

a. Dependent Variable: PartnerOut

b. Linear Regression through the Origin

Page 127: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

114

Taken together, these analyses provide substantial support for H2 and H4. When

participants viewed their own reciprocal debt, they allocated a larger amount to their partner

than when they viewed the reciprocal debt as being their partner’s. Additionally, as the level or

intensity of the participant’s reciprocal debt increased, so did the amount they allocated their

partner. The converse is also true: as the participant perceived a greater reciprocal debt from

their partner, they allocated less to their partner when given the chance.

The Moderating Impact of Relative Benefits

Spline regression was again used to test these hypotheses. Some new spline variables

had to be created so that the differing impacts could be examined when relative outcomes were

either positive or negative. The first spline variables were also used which are copied below:

BenRout = 1 when Rout > 0; 0 otherwise DetRout = 1 when Rout < 0; 0 otherwise SplineBen = BenRout * OwnImpact SplineDet = DetRout * OwnImpact

In addition, the following variables were created:

SBRout = Rout * BenRout SDRout = Rout * DetRout

To investigate the moderating impact, the following interaction variable was created:

RoutOwnImp = Rout * OwnImpact

And to investigate the differential impacts of the moderation when relative outcomes are

greater than zero versus when they are less than zero:

SBRoutOwn = RoutOwnImp *BenRout SDRoutOwn = RoutOwnImp * DetRout

H5a: As the participant’s perceived relative benefits increase, the link between a beneficial action and a participant reciprocal debt will be enhanced. H5b: As the participant’s perceived relative benefits decrease, the link between a detrimental action and a partner reciprocal debt will be enhanced.

Page 128: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

115

The regression was significant (F = 31.847, d.f. = 175, p < .001), explaining approximately 51% of

the variance around the origin (adj R2 = .514). As for the hypotheses, we would find support for

H5a if SBRoutOwn had a positive, significant coefficient. Support for H5b would be found with a

negative, significant coefficient for the SDRoutOwn variable. As can be seen in table #33

(below), the coefficient for SBRoutOwn is positive and significant, supporting H5a. While the

coefficient for SDRoutOwn is in the expected direction, it failed to reach significance, therefore

we cannot support H5b.

Table #33 - Spline Regression for the moderating impact of relative outcomesa,b

Model

Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients

t Sig. B Std. Error Beta

1 SplineBen 1.571 .322 .755 4.881 .000

SplineDet -.145 .803 -.030 -.180 .857

SBRout -9.815 1.494 -2.897 -6.570 .000

SDRout 2.039 .964 .393 2.115 .036

SBRoutOwn 1.398 .200 2.718 7.001 .000

SDRoutOwn -.170 .516 -.096 -.329 .743

a. Dependent Variable: RDlocus

b. Linear Regression through the Origin

The Moderating Impact of the Norms of Reciprocity

It was hypothesized that the norm of positive reciprocity would positively moderate the

impact of a beneficial action on the participant’s reciprocal debt:

H8a: Participant belief in the positive norm of reciprocity will enhance the link between beneficial actions and a participant reciprocal debt.

On the other hand, it is expected that the negative norm of reciprocity will moderate the link

between detrimental actions and the partner’s reciprocal debt:

H8b: Participant belief in the negative norm of reciprocity will enhance the link between detrimental actions and a partner reciprocal debt.

Page 129: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

116

While it is possible to test these hypotheses with spline regression, in reality they involve

different constructs – the positive norm of reciprocity in beneficial situations and the negative

norm of reciprocity in detrimental situations. Because they do not include the same constructs,

linear regressions were performed splitting the sample based on the relative outcomes measure

– those who considered the relative outcomes to be positive (beneficial, n = 97) versus those

who considered the relative outcomes to be negative (detrimental, n = 53). Those who

perceived a zero value (n = 25) on relative outcomes were removed. To test H8a, the following

regression was performed using only those participants who perceived a beneficial outcome:

RDLocus = α + PR + OwnImpact + PR(OwnImpact)

The results of this regression suggested that multicollinearity was a problem in the data

(VIF’s ranging from 29.8-75.9) and therefore alternate methods must be found for analyzing the

data. Because the interaction of PR and OwnImpact variable was highly correlated with both PR

and OwnImpact, a second regression was run with only PR and OwnImpact as explanatory

variables:

RDLocus = α + PR + OwnImpact

This regression was significant (F = 50.307, d.f. = 96, p < .001) and explained approximately 50%

of the variance (adj R2 = .507). PR was not a significant predictor of RDLocus in this regression;

however OwnImpact was a significant predictor of RDLocus (see table #34 below).

Table #34 Regression of PR and OwnImpact on RDLocusa,b

Model

Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients

t Sig. B Std. Error Beta

1 (Constant) -20.238 4.486 -4.511 .000

PR -.013 .295 -.003 -.045 .964

OwnImpact 4.423 .442 .719 10.013 .000

a. Dependent Variable: RDlocus

b. Selecting only cases for which ROut > .00

Page 130: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

117

A third regression was run with the variables OwnImpact and the interaction of PR and

OwnImpact as explanatory variables:

RDLocus = α + OwnImpact + PR(OwnImpact)

Once again, the regression was significant (F=50.334, d.f. = 96, p < .001) and explained

approximately 50% of the variance in RDLocus (adj R2 = .507). The interaction variable failed to

produce a significant coefficient (see table #35 below), thus we cannot support H8a.

Table #35 Regression of OwnImpact and PROwnImpact on RDLocusa,b

Model

Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients

t Sig. B Std. Error Beta

1 (Constant) -20.410 2.653 -7.693 .000

OwnImpact 4.332 .705 .704 6.147 .000

PROwnImpact .008 .048 .019 .168 .867

a. Dependent Variable: RDlocus

b. Selecting only cases for which ROut > .00

The preceding analysis was duplicated for the detrimental situations and similar results

were found. The negative norm of reciprocity did not moderate the response to detrimental

actions. Given these lack of findings, some ANOVA based analyses were conducted after

dichotomizing the variables of interest into high and low categories. We will first examine the

direct impacts of the positive norm of reciprocity on the creation of reciprocal debt (see Table

#36 below).

Table #36 - Norm of Positive Reciprocity’s effect on Reciprocal Debt (ANOVA)

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

RD Between Groups 24.737 1 24.737 4.232 .043

Within Groups 450.087 77 5.845

Total 474.824 78

RDlocus Between Groups 112.864 1 112.864 4.968 .029

Within Groups 1749.173 77 22.717

Total 1862.037 78

Page 131: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

118

As with relative outcomes, the positive norm of reciprocity has a direct impact on the creation

of reciprocal debt whether that debt is measured without respect to the locus of the debt (F =

4.232, ρ = .043) or when the reciprocal debt is adjusted for the locus (F = 4.968, ρ = .029). Next

we will examine the hypothesized moderating impact of the norm of positive reciprocity. Table

#37 shows the sample size for the median split of OwnImpact (HiLoImpact) and on PR (HiLoPR).

Table #38 shows the results of the General Linear Model investigating the impact of HiLoImpact,

HiLoPR and the interaction of those 2 variables on RD. As can be seen in table #38, HiLoImpact

and HiLoPR both have a significant main effect on RD (FSplitOwn = 11.533, ρ = .001; FsplitPR = 4.961, ρ

= .029), however the interaction is not significant (ρ > .05). Figures #24 and 25 plot these direct

effects on RD.

Table #37 - Between-Subjects Factors

N

HiLoImpact .00 33

1.00 46

HiLoPR .00 37

1.00 42

Table #38 - Tests of Between-Subjects Effects

Dependent Variable:RD

Source Type III Sum of

Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

Corrected Model 85.006a 3 28.335 5.452 .002

Intercept 8009.636 1 8009.636 1541.034 .000

HiLoImpact 59.941 1 59.941 11.533 .001

HiLoPR 25.786 1 25.786 4.961 .029

HiLoImpact *HiLoPR .000 1 .000 .000 .993

Error 389.818 75 5.198

Total 9051.609 79

Corrected Total 474.824 78

a. R Squared = .179 (Adjusted R Squared = .146)

Page 132: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

119

Figure #24 – ANOVA Results for Positive Norm of Reciprocity’s impact on RD and RDLocus

Figure #25 – Plots of RD based on Perceived Impact and the Positive Norm of Reciprocity

It doesn’t appear that the norm of positive reciprocity moderates this relationship at all. While

the plots of the marginal means suggest a possible direct effect, the slopes of the high and low

PR participants appear to be exactly the same. Therefore, we fail to find support for H8a.

Page 133: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

120

Again, we first examine the direct effects of the negative norm of reciprocity (NR) on the

creation of reciprocal debt. Table #39 shows that a simple ANOVA investigating the impact of

the negative norm of reciprocity on both RD and RDLocus fails to provide any direct impact (FRD

= .027, ρ = .869; FRDLocus = 1.785, ρ = .185). Figure #26 plots the average value of RD and RDLocus

given the median split measure of NR (HiLoNR).

Table #39 – ANOVA results for Negative Norm of Reciprocity’s Impact on RD and RDLocus

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

RD Between Groups .177 1 .177 .027 .869

Within Groups 549.875 85 6.469

Total 550.052 86

RDlocus Between Groups 70.123 1 70.123 1.785 .185

Within Groups 3339.108 85 39.284

Total 3409.231 86

Figure #26 – ANOVA Plots of NR on RD and RDLocus

Based on this analysis, it appears that the negative norm of reciprocity does not directly impact

reciprocal debt; however that doesn’t rule out an interactive impact. Table #40 shows the

crosstabs of participants after the data was median split on the participant’s rating of

OwnImpact (HiLoImpact) and their norm of negative reciprocity (HiLoNR). Table #41 shows the

Page 134: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

121

results of a General Linear Model investigating the impact of HiLoImpact, HiLoNR and their

interaction on RDLocus. As can be seen in table #41, none of the proposed variables is a

significant predictor of RDLocus.

Table #40 – Between-Subjects Factors

N

HiLoImpact .00 74

1.00 13

HiLoNR .00 44

1.00 43

Table #41 – Tests of Between-Subjects Effects

Dependent Variable:RDlocus

Source Type III Sum of

Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

Corrected Model 148.950a 3 49.650 1.264 .292

Intercept 743.819 1 743.819 18.936 .000

HiLoImpact 65.651 1 65.651 1.671 .200

HiLoNR 62.914 1 62.914 1.602 .209

HiLoImpact*HiLoNR 9.766 1 9.766 .249 .619

Error 3260.281 83 39.280

Total 5554.225 87

Corrected Total 3409.231 86

a - R Squared = .044 (Adjusted R Squared = .009)

Page 135: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

122

Figure #27 – Moderating Impact of Negative Norm of Reciprocity

Finally, figure #27 plots the marginal means of RDLocus given HiLoImpact and HiLoNR. We can

see that the plots of the marginal means suggest an interaction; however we fail to reach

statistical significance. Possibly with a larger sample size or a stronger manipulation of the

detrimental scenario we would see the hypothesized effects. Based on these plots, the

hypothesized relationship is directionally supported: those with a greater adherence to the

negative norm of reciprocity decreased their partners’ outcomes more than those with a low

negative reciprocity norm. We cannot support H8b however.

As an exploratory analysis, the positive norm of reciprocity was examined as a possible

antecedent to reciprocal debt in the detrimental situation.

Table #42 – Between-Subjects Factors

N

HiLoImpact .00 74

1.00 13

HiLoPR .00 43

1.00 44

Page 136: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

123

Table #43 – Tests of Between-Subjects Effects

Dependent Variable:RDlocus

Source Type III Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

Corrected Model 225.958a 3 75.319 1.964 .126

Intercept 689.896 1 689.896 17.988 .000

HiLoImpact 85.406 1 85.406 2.227 .139

HiLoPR 134.903 1 134.903 3.517 .064

HiLoImpact*HiLoPR 27.473 1 27.473 .716 .400

Error 3183.273 83 38.353

Total 5554.225 87

Corrected Total 3409.231 86

a. R Squared = .066 (Adjusted R Squared = .033)

Table #42 shows the crosstabs for these 2 variables. Table #43 explains the General Linear

Model that was run investigating the impact of these 2 variables on RDLocus. It seems that the

positive norm of reciprocity has an impact (F = 3.517, ρ = .064) on the reciprocal debt in the

detrimental manipulation. Those who are low in the positive norm of reciprocity tend to punish

their partners more severely for what they view as less beneficial treatment. Figure #28

illustrates this relationship as those high in the positive norm of reciprocity did not differ greatly

in their response to detrimental treatment.

Page 137: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

124

Figure #28 – ANOVA Plot of the moderating impact of PR

Table #44 summarizes the hypotheses tested from the experimental portion of this

dissertation.

Hypothesis Hypothesis Wording Supported?

H1 Beneficial Action (+) → Participant Reciprocal Debt H2 Participant Reciprocal Debt (+) → Partner Outcomes H3 Detrimental Action (+) → Partner Reciprocal Debt H4 Partner Reciprocal Debt (-) → Partner Outcomes H5a

Participant Relative Benefits moderates (+) link between Beneficial Action and Participant Reciprocal Debt

H5bPartner Relative Benefits moderates (+) the link between Detrimental Action and Partner Reciprocal Debt

H7aPositive Norm of Reciprocity moderates (+) the link between Beneficial Action and Participant Reciprocal Debt

H7bNegative Norm of Reciprocity moderates (+) the link between Detrimental Action and Partner Reciprocal Debt

Table #44 - Experimental Research Hypothesis Summary

Page 138: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

125

CHAPTER 5: RELATIONAL RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS:

THE ROLE OF COMPLEX RECIPROCITY

5.1 Relational Reciprocity Abstract

Marketing theorists and researchers have noted that transactional and relational

exchange differ on many dimensions. Social exchange and reciprocity theory suggest that the

process and nature of reciprocation differs as a relationship develops and deepens. Earlier

chapters of this dissertation focused on the reciprocation evident in a single exchange

transaction. The remainder of this dissertation focuses on the role of reciprocity in ongoing,

continuing relationships. The term Relational Reciprocity is adopted to distinguish this

reciprocity from the reciprocity evident in discrete transactions. While this portion focuses on

existing, continuing relationships, it is important to note that not all relationships are equal.

Many business relationships persist even as arms-length transactions which may appropriately

be viewed as a series of discrete transactions, while others may develop into more enmeshed,

relational exchange.

Relationships characterized as discrete should evidence a more simple reciprocity

through more immediate, direct and in kind reciprocal actions. On the other hand, relational

exchange should involve more complex reciprocity which is characterized by more comfort with

longer duration reciprocal debts, less exact accounting of reciprocal returns, and wider latitude

in the types of responses acceptable to qualify as reciprocation. This research seeks to examine

changes in relational reciprocation and how that may help explain the development of

relationships over time. A dyadic longitudinal survey, supplemented with secondary

performance data, is planned to test the theoretical model built based on this relational

reciprocity.

Page 139: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

126

5.2 Relational Reciprocity Introduction

Reciprocity theory suggests that the process of reciprocation is the necessary

component to the development of social ties within a society (Becker 1986). Reciprocity is

implicated as the driving force in the development of the human social system, enabling the

introduction of labor specialization, trust, informal social contracts and numerous other social

institutions we take for granted in everyday life (Macneil 1980). Becker (1956) goes so far as to

rename human beings as homo-reciprocus, suggesting that reciprocity is the key enabling

process in all social relationships. If we accept that reciprocity is the glue that binds together

social relationships, does this suggest that the process of reciprocation is the same in all

relationships? Or more likely, might there be differences in the process of reciprocation

depending on the strength of the social ties? While it is expected that reciprocity is a necessary

component of all relationships, it would be naïve to suggest that reciprocity functions the same

in a marriage or familial relationship as it does in a market transaction.

Bagozzi (1975) categorizes exchange relationships based on both the number of

participants to the exchange and, more importantly for this research, on the meaning of the

exchange process as either utilitarian, symbolic or mixed exchange. Utilitarian exchange focuses

on the anticipated use or tangible characteristics commonly associated with the objects of

exchange. Symbolic exchange is “the mutual exchange of psychological, social, or other

intangible entities between two or more parties” (p. 36). Bagozzi acknowledges that many

marketing interactions are based on the utilitarian meaning of the exchange. However, he also

argues the importance of the symbolic meaning of the exchange, and research has generally

shown support given that social aspects of the relationship like trust (Doney and Cannon 1997;

Fang et al. 2008), loyalty (Palmatier et al. 2007b), dependence (Kumar et al. 1995a), procedural

fairness (Kumar et al. 1995b), and power (Gaski 1984) all have important consequences for

Page 140: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

127

marketing relationships, not only impacting the quality of the relationship, but also more

objective outcomes like the financial returns to both parties. In fact, the premise behind the

relationship marketing field is that building stronger more enmeshed relationships will benefit

both firms through greater financial outcomes (Dwyer et al. 1987; Morgan and Hunt 1994;

Palmatier et al. 2006a).

But what determines whether a given marketing relationship relies more on the

utilitarian meaning of the exchange or on the symbolic meaning of the exchange? Dwyer,

Schurr and Oh (1987) argue that relationships develop over time and can transition from

discrete exchange into relational exchange (e.g., Macneil 1980). They posit that when at least

one of the parties has a high level of motivational investment (or expected net benefits from a

relationship), it may be managed as relational exchange. On the other hand, when the

motivational investment of both parties is low, the relationship is likely to remain as a series of

discrete exchanges over its duration. As numerous theorists have pointed out, the true

“discrete exchange” probably never happens, the common example being paying for fuel in cash

while traveling to a location the consumer is unlikely to return to, more likely most interactions

involve some degree of social interaction, relying on social norms and expectations of future

performance (Bagozzi 1975; Macneil 1980; Williamson 1996). This does not imply that all

relationships are and only relational in nature; however as is often pointed out (Bagozzi 1975;

Dwyer et al. 1987; Gouldner 1960; Macneil 1980), relationships can generally be categorized on

a continuum from discrete to relational exchange. As relationships favor either discrete or

relational exchange, theory suggests that the process of reciprocation should differ as well

(Cialdini 2009; Gouldner 1960; Macneil 1980).

Employing Macneil’s (1980) social exchange theory, this research will propose and

develop a scale that captures relational reciprocity behavior ranging from simple relational

Page 141: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

128

reciprocity (in more discrete exchange) to complex relational reciprocity (in relational exchange)

in nature. Theoretically, this research will help broaden the understanding of the role of

reciprocity in marketing relationships. Examining both simple and complex reciprocity will build

on existing reciprocity theory, and may help explain why recent attempts to explain reciprocity

in marketing relationships have failed to capture the entire domain of reciprocal exchange

(Pervan et al. 2009). From a managerial perspective this research should help to better

understand which relationship marketing strategies will be more effective in a given

relationship. Understanding of customer preference for complex or simple reciprocal

relationships will allow more judicious allocation of marketing resources to help build more

embedded relationships, or to help promote the convenience of exchange based more on

discrete transactions. Further, it is possible that certain trends may arise related to a customer’s

industry, purchase basket, or other signals which will help a selling firm delineate which

customers to approach with relational marketing tactics versus focusing on the discrete

exchange aspects the firm has to offer.

This section of the dissertation is organized as follows: the next section includes a brief

literature review and builds a theory of relational reciprocity. Hypotheses will then be

developed differentiating simple and complex relational reciprocity, and to suggest antecedents

that should impact a customer’s tendency towards either end of the spectrum. Additionally,

hypotheses will be offered to suggest outcomes of relational reciprocity. Finally, a research

project will be discussed that tests the theoretical model in real customer-selling firm

relationships.

5.3 Theoretical Model Development

Macneil (1980) differentiates exchange on a continuum ranging from discrete

transactions to relational exchange. While he admits that a true discrete transaction probably

Page 142: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

129

does not exist, he argues that the continuum is a useful categorization system to analyze how

factors of exchange will vary depending on which end of the continuum is favored. Macneil

argues that the human is basically a schizophrenic creature, always trying (and failing) to

balance self-interest with social solidarity. He relates discrete exchange and relational exchange

to these two opposing forces within humans, the predominantly discrete exchanges appealing

to the self-interest, while relational exchange functions to satisfy the human need for social

interaction. Importantly, he acknowledges that while exchange is typically more discrete or

more relational, in reality nearly every exchange has characteristics which could place it on both

the discrete and relational ends of the spectrum. Macneil defines discrete exchange as one in

which no relationship exists between the parties apart from the simple exchange of goods.

While he offers a definition of relational exchange as one involving relations other than a

discrete exchange, his definition does not adequately illustrate the difference between the two

concepts.

This research borrows from Macneil’s theory of relational exchange. Accepting that

marketing relationships can vary from more discrete to more relational in nature, this research

applies Macneil to build a theory of relational reciprocity and how it will differ depending on the

characteristics of the parties. Macneil examines 12 factors which should be substantially

different at opposing ends of the exchange continuum (also see Dwyer et al. 1987 for a concise

review of these factors). While it is possible relational reciprocity may exhibit differences based

on these 12 factors, it is noted that it may be possible that marketing relationships do not

require examination of all 12 of these dimensions (Dwyer et al. 1987). Eight of the 12 factors

which should be particularly relevant to the process of reciprocation are examined in this

research. The following is a discussion of these 8 dimensions of relational exchange.

Page 143: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

130

Personal Relations

The first of the factors of exchange is the level at which personal relations exist within

an exchange relationship. At the discrete end of the scale, there are no social relations.

Interactions between parties to an exchange are highly formalized, lacking informal or

noncritical communication. Discrete exchange partners have no need to interact on a social

level. The extent of their interaction is related to defining, monitoring and executing the terms

of the exchange. On the other hand, relational exchange involves social relationship whereby

individuals gain not only economic returns, but also psychological or social wellbeing (Dwyer et

al. 1987) through their interactions with their counterpart. Communications will include

extensive, informal as well as (or instead of) formal communications. Concern for the wellbeing

of the counterpart should be evidenced in relational exchange, whereas in discrete exchange,

self-interest is the primary motivator to exchange.

Measurement and Specificity

In a typical discrete exchange, the content exchanged will be some commodity with a

market value on the one hand and money on the other. Measurement is precise, and often

predetermined by market factors external to this specific exchange. The terms of the exchange

are very specific: one party delivers the agreed commodity while the other exchanges the

agreed upon price. In contrast, relational exchange is often much less clear on the terms of

performance. Measurement may be much less precise as the benefits of exchange cannot

always be determined by market forces. Additionally, the social exchange may allow for less

specificity in a given exchange because the parties to the exchange are committed to some level

of equity over the entire spectrum of the relationship, rather than with respect to one specific

interaction.

Page 144: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

131

Sources of Contractual Solidarity

Macneil (1980) defines contractual solidarity as “the social solidarity making exchange

work – that at a minimum holds the parties together enough so that they will not kill and steal in

preference to exchanging” (p. 14). In a discrete exchange, lacking any social relation, the only

sources of contractual solidarity must come from external enforcement: laws, rules, societal

norms, etc. The parties have no interest necessarily in continuing in the relationship, so

relationship-specific norms are nonexistent. Contractual solidarity must be enforced through

mechanisms external to this specific transaction. On the other hand, when considering

relational exchange, both parties have some interest in maintaining the relationship. Forces

from within the relationship may help promote contractual solidarity. Of course, external forces

including laws and industry regulations will also influence the interactions, but over the duration

of the relationship norms may develop to help promote healthy interaction (Heide and John

1992). Relational factors like trust, commitment and loyalty develop that also provide a source

of solidarity to both parties in the relationship.

Commencement, Duration and Termination

The discrete transaction has a clearly defined lifecycle. The transaction begins when

parties agree to the exchange, and terminates upon performance of the exchange. The duration

of the transaction will be relatively short, if not immediate upon consummation of the

transaction. Termination occurs when successful completion of the terms of the exchange has

been satisfied, which is a clear point in time because the measurement of performance has been

specifically delineated. Simply put, parties are both aware of the end of the interaction because

the terms clearly spell out the contractual obligations. Relational exchange tends to be of a

longer duration and have less well defined commencement and termination. Using the nuclear

family as an analogy, commencement begins at birth and termination may actually extend

Page 145: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

132

beyond death. Because of the vague measurement terms of the contract, parties may never be

100% sure that they have performed what is required, and in fact exchange may be managed

such that there is no clear termination of the exchange relationship.

Planning

In a discrete transaction, planning is complete, specific and binding. It is achieved by the

explicit consent of both parties to the terms of the exchange, and no planning is anticipated in

the future of the interaction. In relational exchange, planning may be an ongoing, long term

process with anticipation of amendments and adjustments in the future as the needs and

resources of both parties change. Recognizing that perfect information is nonexistent, relational

exchange allows for future changes to the plan and further may actually develop a less accurate

plan that accounts for externalities and contingencies in the relationship. Social norms (both

external and internal) may affect the planning, and often self-interest will be sacrificed in favor

of other goals (e.g., social solidarity).

Future Cooperation

Discrete transactions, by definition, do not require any future cooperation or

interaction. Parties to discrete transactions have no goals mediated by a desire for future

cooperation. Self-interest dictates, and if it weren’t for external sources of contractual solidarity

parties to exchange would appropriate as much of the outcomes as possible through this single

transaction. On the other hand, relational exchange involves an expectation of future

interaction and often requires joint efforts in the execution of the contract. Further, as the

planning allows for future adjustments and amendments as needed, future cooperation is

required in relational exchange.

Page 146: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

133

Sharing and Dividing of Benefits and Burdens

Macneil (1980) states that “in a discrete transaction benefits and burdens are sharply

divided; each party has his benefits, all his, and his burdens, also all his” (p. 16). The terms of

the transaction clearly specify which party is responsible for costs of the exchange, and how

much of the benefits are allocated to each party. There is no room for sharing of either. In

relational exchange, parties may plan on future negotiations to find equitable exchange terms

that are beneficial for both parties. In transaction cost economics, this process is referred to as

ex-post contracting and it is positioned as a necessary component to exchange due to the lack of

perfect information, managers’ bounded rationality and the consequent impossibility of

accurately planning for every contingency prior to a transaction (Williamson 2007).

Obligations – content, sources of obligations and specificity

Macneil (1980) argues that in discrete transactions the content of obligations originate

internally from the promises of parties to the transaction, whereas the obligations to perform

are external to the relationship. This causes the obligations to be clearly defined and very

specific such that external sources can verify, monitor and enforce the obligations where

necessary. In relational exchange, both the content and sources of obligations come from

within the relation itself. These could evolve out of relational norms over time, but importantly

they are likely to be less specific than those in a discrete transaction.

These 8 factors are argued to vary in the specified ways depending on whether an

exchange is more relational or more discrete. While reciprocity is specifically implicated by

Macneil (1980) when discussing these factors, given the expectation that reciprocity functions

differently in more embedded relationship than in a market based relationship, we should be

able to determine some important characteristics of relational reciprocity, and hence also

discrete reciprocity.

Page 147: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

134

5.3.1 Complex and Simple Reciprocity

The process of reciprocation is not the same in all relationships. As has been discussed

the exchange process itself differs, and it should not be surprising that as part of that process,

reciprocation will also differ depending on how discrete or relational the transaction is. In fact,

Cialdini (2009) goes so far as to suggest that “in its purest form reciprocity is unnecessary and

undesirable in certain long-term relationships such as families or established friendships” (p.

34). “Its purest form” refers to economic reciprocity where if you spend $20 to take me out to

lunch, it is understood that I owe you a $20 lunch in return. While Cialdini’s indictment of

reciprocity in long-term relationships appears ominous at first glance, he goes on to suggest that

instead of economically valued commodities being reciprocally exchanged in these relationships,

what is being reciprocated is the “willingness to provide what the other needs, when it is

needed” (p. 35). Importantly, his thesis suggests that reciprocity functions at multiple levels.

The traditionally understood form, where exchange is evaluated based on market rates,

reciprocal debts are repaid to the creditor of the debt, and at any given time both parties are

aware of who the debtor is, a market based valuation of how much they owe, and possibly

when the debt is expected to be repaid. On the other hand, in closer, longer-term exchange

relationships it is unlikely that either party keeps track of who owes who, how much is owed,

nor when it is expected to be repaid.

This view of relational reciprocal debts, as lacking in specificity, is shared by other

researchers as well. Bagozzi (1975) suggests that in symbolic exchange the motivations for

exchange are the “social and psychological significance of the experiences, feelings, and

meanings of the parties to the exchange” (p. 36). While he doesn’t suggest that all marketing

exchange occurs for symbolic reasons, he does posit that most exchange occurs for both

symbolic and utilitarian reasons. Given that this symbolic reason for exchange is impossible to

Page 148: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

135

value based on market factors, it seems likely that reciprocity with respect to these more

relational interactions will have less specificity in the measurement, definition and planning of

the actual terms of the exchange.

Dwyer, Schurr and Oh (1987) also discuss how reciprocal debts may change as a

relationship develops over time. One of the key factors that these researchers suggests impacts

this deepening of a relationship is the development of trust and commitment in the relationship.

They suggest that these relational qualities help give confidence to partners that their

investments in the relationship will be returned by their partner, and thus help build solidarity

between the partners. Additionally, they suggest that the process of reciprocation will change

as the relationship develops when they say “Later, when the parties are both projecting their

association into the future, there may be a lesser need for strict-reciprocity accounting in that

the future holds ample opportunity for and expectations of balancing” (p. 16). Clearly there are

implications for the specificity of any given exchange; more relational exchange should be

characterized by less specific accounting of reciprocal debts.

Reciprocity theorists also agree that while early in a relationship, reciprocity will exist at

a more discrete, simple level. These initial exchanges help build confidence and trust in the

partner so that in the future relational partners will relax the strict specificity and accounting of

the terms of exchange (Becker 1956; Becker 1986). Reciprocity becomes more complex and

flexible as exchange partners become more familiar with each other and develop more

confidence about the future of their relationship and the ability to even out benefits and

burdens of exchange over time (Gouldner 1960). This research proposes that relational

reciprocity is a multi-dimensional construct. Further, I propose five dimensions which should

distinguish between complex and simple relational reciprocity. These factors are summarized

in table #45 below.

Page 149: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

136

Table #45. Dimensions of Complex and Simple Reciprocity

Dimension Complex Simple

Comfort with own and other reciprocal debts

High Low

Presumption of Innocence

High Low

Valuation of reciprocal debts

Benefits provided Costs incurred

Repayment terms of reciprocal debts

Long term Short term

Accounting of reciprocal debts

Informal - nonexistant Formalized system

• Comfort with own and other’s reciprocal debt – complex reciprocity involves being more amenable to both accepting favors from, and doing favors for, their exchange partners, understanding that the relationship duration will allow them to repay favors in the future.

• Presumption of Innocence - Given greater levels of trust and commitment by both parties in relational exchange relationships, it seems unlikely that either party would jump to assigning blame or guilt to their counterpart when encountering a detrimental action. More likely they might presume innocence, at least until the offending action can be discussed and investigated.

• Valuation of reciprocal debts – is the valuation of relational inputs based on the benefits obtained, or the costs incurred to obtain them? Is the partner the focus of valuation, or is the self the focus? It should be expected that more complex reciprocity is characterized by partners being more concerned with their counterpart’s benefits than with their own costs in providing those benefits. This also agrees with Cialdini’s (2009) suggestion that in deeper relationships, the commodity reciprocated is the willingness to provide for the partner rather than the inputs exchanged themselves.

• Repayment terms of reciprocal debts – Macneil (1980) discusses the key distinction between discrete transactions and relational exchange as the expectation of repayment at sometime in the future. It seems that parties involved in more relational exchange should be willing to await repayment of reciprocal debts longer than those whose exchange is characterized as more discrete in nature.

• Accounting of reciprocal debts – Cialdini (2009) suggests that in longer-term, more committed relationships, “formal” reciprocity is unnecessary as parties are more concerned with the overall balance of benefits than with an exact accounting of who owes who what. It would seem that more discrete exchange would involve a strict, formalized accounting system for reciprocal debts, whereas a more relational exchange

Page 150: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

137

level might be characterized by only informal accounting, or even a lack of accounting of debits and credits to the relationship.

This research positions these dimensions as the differentiating dimensions for complex and

simple relational reciprocity.

5.4 Hypotheses

If reciprocity functions differently in relational exchange than it does in discrete

transactions, it should be expected that outcomes will differ depending on whether the

relationship is characterized by relational or discrete reciprocity. In fact, this understanding may

help explain the relationship marketing findings that financial relationship marketing programs

do not directly affect selling firm outcomes (Palmatier et al. 2006a). Based on the hypotheses

offered above, it seems that financial investments in relational exchange partners may be

valued less than other types of non-financial relational investments. It is possible that the type

of reciprocity functioning in a relationship has implications for which types of relationship

investments will have the greatest appeal to relational partners.

5.4.1 Outcomes of Complex Reciprocity

Marketing researchers have utilized the dependent variable of “share of wallet” as an

important outcome variable in marketing relationships (Reynolds and Beatty 1999; Siguaw et al.

1998). Unlike a measure of sales which can depend on the size of the customer, the industry

being investigated, or market factors like the general economy, share of wallet measures the

percentage of total purchases one customer obtains from a given supplier acknowledging that

customers often have multiple suppliers and hence alternative sources. When alternatives exist,

and a supplier offers multiple products or services that a customer could use, share of wallet

represents the customer’s choice to use this supplier for a greater proportion of their purchases.

In the relationship marketing literature, it is argued that increases in share of wallet should be

expected based on relationship building programs designed to develop a stronger, possibly

Page 151: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

138

more relational exchange relationship. This suggests that marketing relationships characterized

by relational reciprocity should be expected to have a higher share of wallet than those

characterized by discrete transactions. With discrete reciprocity, the driving force to exchange

is economic self-interest. Alternatively, relational reciprocity suggests that the focus also

includes the goals and success of the partner firm. Decisions will be made which benefit both

firms instead of seeking out the best economic alternative. Share of wallet might be witnessed

in a relationship characterized by discrete reciprocity; however the existence of discrete

reciprocity is not expected to be an antecedent to share of wallet. Specifically, it is expected

that:

H1: Relationships characterized by greater complex reciprocity will exhibit increased share of wallet over time. Discrete transactions are motivated by self-interest maximization (Macneil 1980). It is

expected that relationships characterized by discrete reciprocity will likely involve an ongoing

search for alternatives as the customer is constantly searching for the best economic outcomes.

On the other hand, when relational exchange is achieved the benefits being reciprocated in the

relationship are more complex than simple economic utility, including the social and

psychological benefits of the social interactions (Dwyer et al. 1987). The search for alternatives

is unlikely in relationships characterized by relational reciprocity, as trust, commitment and

interdependence have helped to bind the two parties together. These relational characteristics

essentially create exit barriers to the relationship. The only time we would see a search for

alternatives in this case is when the relationship has deteriorated and one party seeks to exit the

relationship. Otherwise, it is unlikely that either party would engage in any search for

alternatives, therefore:

H2: Relationships characterized by greater complex reciprocity will exhibit decreased search for alternatives.

Page 152: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

139

On the other hand, discrete reciprocity is motivated by self-interest. Terms of the

exchange are expected to be specified and agreed upon prior to execution. Trust and

commitment aren’t driving the solidarity of the relationship. When encountering a detrimental

act within the confines of the relationship, it is unlikely that the offended party will presume

innocence of their counterpart. Rather, they are more likely to assume that the negative

outcomes are caused by their partner attempting to increase their own positive outcomes at

their expense. Negative reciprocity may be the expected tactic in this situation. When your

partner injures you, and you expect that they do not share in the increased costs, you might

take action to even out the costs imposed by this new interaction in the relationship.

H3: Relationships characterized by less complex reciprocity will exhibit a higher level of negative reciprocal responses to detrimental actions. The preceding hypotheses suggest another outcome of relational reciprocity. If

relationships characterized by relational reciprocity are expected to exhibit an initial

presumption of innocence in the face of detrimental partner actions, while discrete reciprocal

relations are expected to respond with negative reciprocity, we would expect that constructive

conflict will differ between the two types of reciprocal relationships. Relational exchange

partners are not expected to presume innocence indefinitely in the face of detrimental partner

actions. Marriages end up in divorce if one member continually takes advantage of the other,

and I would expect the same for marketing relationships no matter how relational they are at a

given point in time. However, if there is an increased presumption of innocence, it should be

expected that higher levels of constructive conflict or the active response of voice will also exist

in relationships characterized by relational reciprocity. On the other hand, with discrete

reciprocity, the expected response lacks any communicative efforts to resolve the detrimental

action because the presumption is that the partner is attempting to appropriate more than their

agreed upon outcomes of the exchange. Therefore I expect that:

Page 153: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

140

H4: Relationships characterized by greater complex reciprocity will exhibit higher levels of constructive conflict in response to detrimental acts. The norm of reciprocity suggests that all reciprocal debts need to be repaid. It functions

as a social bonding norm which promotes social solidarity between anyone who receives a favor

or benefit from another person (Becker 1986). In fact, it has been shown that customers who

fail to reciprocate (purchase) a salesperson’s social interaction actually experience guilt (Dahl et

al. 2005; Palmatier et al. 2009). Based on the moral norm of reciprocity, we would expect that

some level of guilt will be felt whenever someone anticipates they will be unable to repay a

reciprocal debt. However, given that discrete reciprocity is governed by norms and rules

external to the relationship, while relational reciprocity is motivated by norms and expectations

developed internally, it should be expected that the anticipation of non-repayment of a

reciprocal debt will cause a greater feeling of guilt in relational partners than in discrete

transaction partners. Further, the expectation that the relationship will continue into the future

in relational exchange would also argue for an increased level of guilt; whereas in discrete

exchange a partner may anticipate a lack of future interaction and therefore while guilt may be

felt, they may not expect to have to face the creditor of that unpaid debt again in the future.

H5: Relationships characterized by greater complex reciprocity will exhibit a higher degree of guilt in the face of anticipated unpaid reciprocal debts. 5.4.2 Antecedents of Relational and Discrete Reciprocity

So what impacts the type of reciprocity engaged in by firms in a marketing relationship?

Can a firm enable the development of relational reciprocity with its channel partners? If

marketing relationships can be characterized as either more relational or more discrete in

nature, it is important to understand what determines the trajectory the relationship will take.

Acknowledging that both upstream and downstream parties in a relationship may make efforts

to develop relational exchange, consistent with prior marketing literature (De Wulf et al. 2001;

Page 154: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

141

Morgan and Hunt 1994; Palmatier et al. 2006b) this research will assume the role of the selling

firm as being motivated to develop stronger relational exchange with their customers. I argue

that characteristics of the parties of the relationship – the customer’s relationship orientation,

the selling firm’s relational investments, and the customer’s adherence to the positive and

norms of reciprocity, and characteristics of the exchange itself – degree of specialization

required, frequency of interactions, and contractual commitments may help determine whether

relational or discrete reciprocity will be favored.

5.4.2.1 Characteristics of the Buyer and Seller

It has been argued that some customers will prefer less relational exchange in their

marketing transactions (Bagozzi 1995), possibly going so far as to avoid or resist relational

efforts by their selling firms (Crosby et al. 1990). Alternatively, customers in some situations will

desire deeper relationships with their suppliers. Investigating the impact of this relationship

orientation, or desire to engage in a strong relationship with a current or potential partner to

conduct a specific exchange, Palmatier et al. (2008) found that a buyer’s relationship orientation

enhanced the impact of salesperson relationship marketing activities on buyer’s trust, which

lead to more favorable seller outcomes. This suggests that when a customer is predisposed to

building stronger relationships, relationship marketing efforts are more likely to be reciprocated

through increased purchase, higher share of wallet, and lower propensity to switch suppliers

(Palmatier et al. 2008). I expect that:

H6: Customer relationship orientation will lead to increased complex reciprocity. Relationship marketing is based on the idea that relational investments build stronger,

more trusting customer relationships (Morgan and Hunt 1994), which leads to improved selling

firm performance (De Wulf et al. 2001; Palmatier et al. 2006a). While relationship marketing

efforts do not always lead to the desired outcomes (Palmatier et al. 2008), it has been shown

Page 155: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

142

that relationship marketing efforts can improve the relationship quality (De Wulf et al. 2001; Jap

et al. 1999), relationship satisfaction (Caceres and Paparoidamis 2007; Ping 2003), and selling

firm outcomes (Jap 1999; Palmatier et al. 2006a). Applying these results to the study of

reciprocity in marketing relationships suggests that relational reciprocity may be enhanced by

the selling firm’s efforts at building a relationship. When a customer knows that the selling firm

is dedicated to building a stronger relationship, unless there are specific reasons to avoid

relational exchange, I would expect to see a higher degree of relational reciprocity resulting

from these efforts, therefore:

H7: The customer’s perception of selling firm relational investments will lead to increased complex reciprocity. Finally, the customer’s adherence to both positive and negative norms of reciprocity

may also be important in determining whether the relationship is characterized by relational

reciprocity or discrete reciprocity. Considering first the positive norm of reciprocity, individuals

who have a stronger belief in positive reciprocity are more likely to reciprocate all beneficial

actions by their partners (Becker 1986; Perugini et al. 2003). This includes not only economically

beneficial actions, but also more socially motivated actions. While they may feel some

reactance, or resistance to accepting certain reciprocal debts (Berkowitz 1973), if they have a

strong belief in the positive norm of reciprocity, they will feel compelled to reciprocate any

benefits obtained. This suggests that over time, as non-economic actions are reciprocated, the

relationship may tend more towards relational reciprocity, rather than discrete exchange.

H8: Customers scoring higher on the positive norm of reciprocity scale are more likely to exhibit greater complex reciprocity. What about the negative norm of reciprocity? Individuals who have a stronger belief in

negative reciprocity tend to react through negative reciprocity when encountering detrimental

actions. Retaliating or negatively reciprocating detrimental actions does not agree with the

Page 156: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

143

conceptualization of relational reciprocity. One of the components of relational reciprocity is a

presumption of innocence, and increased constructive conflict while detrimental actions are

investigated and appropriate responses determined. Negative reciprocation assumes the guilt

of the other party, and takes actions which can only be considered self-interested and possibly

not even economic self-interest, but rather with a goal of obtaining some catharsis through the

punishment of the offending party. Relational exchange assumes that externalities and

contingencies will affect the relationship in the future, and flexibility is allowed in response to

changing situations. Negative reciprocity is counterproductive to cooperation when dealing

with potentially detrimental actions. Instead of taking time to understand the counterpart’s

situation (e.g., increased costs, budget constraints, etc.), negative reciprocity advocates a fitting

punishment for a crime without consideration to the outcomes of the party as a whole.

Therefore, I expect that:

H9: Customers scoring higher on the negative norm of reciprocity scale are more likely to exhibit decreased levels of complex reciprocity. 5.4.2.2 Characteristics of the Exchange

It is also possible that certain characteristics of the exchange itself will push

relationships towards relational exchange. For example, a manufacturer purchasing

commodities that are commonly exchanged on the open market, with little need of

customization may not find relational exchange to be beneficial for his purchasing purposes. On

the other hand, it is argued that in services where the exchange is often co-produced

(Bendapudi and Leone 2003) a greater degree of integration, cooperation and flexibility are

required to consummate the exchange. Dwyer, Schurr and Oh (1987) suggest that duties and

performance are relatively more complex in relational exchange than in discrete transactions,

where all contingencies have been either eliminated or planned for in the transaction

Page 157: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

144

agreement. This suggests that as the content of exchange increases in complexity, we should

expect to see a higher prevalence of relational exchange.

H10: As the content of exchange increases in complexity, greater complex reciprocity will exist. Related to the complexity of the exchange content, the frequency of interaction is also

expected to impact the process of reciprocity within the relationship. It should be expected that

more complex exchange will likely involve more frequent interactions, however even more

simple exchanges may involve frequent interactions (e.g., buying gas for a commuter).

Increased frequency of interactions will promote relational exchange on multiple dimensions.

Interacting more often will increase partners’ confidence that reciprocity will happen sometime

in the future. It is likely that given a frequent exchange, parties may be more amenable to

waiting repayment of reciprocal debts longer as they anticipate frequent opportunities to collect

on those debts in the future. Additionally, more frequent interactions create an environment

that is conducive to relational reciprocity. On the other hand, if marketing partners interact

very infrequently, it may be difficult to develop the social ties outside of the formalized

communications directed at consummating the exchange. Therefore I expect that:

H11: As the interaction frequency increases, greater complex reciprocity will exist. These hypotheses are summarized in the conceptual model in figure #29 below:

Page 158: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Com

plex

Rec

ipro

city

•Com

fort

with

reci

proc

al d

ebt

•Pre

sum

ptio

n of

Inno

cenc

e•R

epay

men

t ter

ms

•Acc

ount

ing

of re

cipr

ocal

deb

ts•V

alua

tion

of re

cipr

ocal

deb

ts

Shar

e of

W

alle

t

Sear

ch fo

r A

lter

nati

ves

Cons

truc

tive

Co

nflic

t

Gui

lt

Cust

omer

Re

lati

onsh

ip

Ori

enta

tion

Cust

omer

Pe

rcep

tion

of S

elle

r Re

lati

onal

In

vest

men

ts

Cust

omer

Neg

ativ

e N

orm

of R

ecip

roci

ty

Cust

omer

Pos

itiv

e N

orm

of R

ecip

roci

ty

Exch

ange

Co

mpl

exit

y

Inte

ract

ion

Freq

uenc

y

Char

acte

ristic

s of

the

part

ies

Char

acte

ristic

s of

the

exch

ange

Neg

ativ

e Re

cipr

ocit

y

Figu

re #

29 –

Rela

tiona

l Rec

ipro

city

Con

cept

ual F

ram

ewor

k

Com

mit

men

t

roset
Typewritten Text
145
Page 159: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

146

5.5 Survey Research on Complex Reciprocity

This portion of the research was generously funded by Boone County National Bank

(BCNB). BCNB wanted to survey both their consumer and business customers, although only the

business customers are included in this report. Given the substantial investment in this

research, BCNB retained much input over issues that affected the costs of administering this

survey. In particular, we only sent out one full survey and one reminder postcard to the sample

because of both cost considerations, and BCNB’s concern about possibly upsetting their

customers by sending too much “junk mail.”

This survey was delivered on paper using postal mail services. BCNB customers received

two personalized cover letters – one from the VP of the bank, and one from the academic

researchers, the 2-page (front and back – see exhibit #4 in the appendix) survey, and a postage

paid return envelope in one package addressed to the account owner of record from the bank’s

database. Approximately 3 weeks after the surveys were mailed out, personalized reminder

postcards (see exhibit #5 in the appendix) were sent out to all non-respondents asking that they

fill out the survey that had already been sent. A summary report was offered as an incentive for

participation that compared the participant’s business with others responding on relational

characteristics and other variables of interest. The survey was split into four sections – section 1

dealt with the respondents’ personal norms of reciprocity; section 2 asked the respondent to act

as a informant about their firm’s relationship with BCNB; section 3 asked about the

respondent’s personal relationship with BCNB; and finally section 4 asked for some personal and

firm demographic information.

5.5.1 Research Model

This research will address the conceptual model illustrated in Figure #29 on the

preceding page.

Page 160: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

147

5.5.2 Operationalizations

Measures were captured through two distinct data capture techniques: the survey

items; and the bank’s database. Measures specific to each of these data collection methods will

be discussed separately. When possible, items were adapted from existing scales. Participants

were first asked about their norms of reciprocity in section I. Section II asked the participants to

be an informant reporting on their firm’s relationship with BCNB. Section III contained items to

capture aspects of the participant’s personal relationship with BCNB. And finally, section IV

captured some demographic and descriptive statistics about the participant and their firm.

Section I: Norms of Reciprocity

Items were adapted from Perugini et al (2003) to capture both the positive and negative

norms of reciprocity. 6 items were used to capture the participant's adherence to the positive

norm of reciprocity (pr1 - pr6) and to the negative norm of reciprocity (nr1 - nr6) using 7-point

Likert scales ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor

Agree” labeling the midpoint.

Positive Norm of Reciprocity

1. When I profit from the actions of someone else, I will do something that benefits him/her in return.

2. When someone does me a favor, I feel committed to repay the favor. 3. If someone does a favor for me, I will return the favor. 4. I am ready to incur personal costs to help someone who has helped me. 5. I go out of my way to help a person who has been kind to me. 6. I'm willing to do things I don't enjoy to return someone's assistance.

Negative Norm of Reciprocity

1. If someone purposely injures me, I will try to injure him/her as well. 2. If somebody takes action that damages me, I will go to great lengths to inflict

damage on him/her. 3. If somebody is impolite to me, I become impolite in return. 4. I am willing to invest time and effort to reciprocate a harmful action. 5. If someone puts me in a difficult position, I will do the same to him/her.

Page 161: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

148

6. I will be unfair to someone who has been unfair to me.

Section II: Firm’s Relationship with BCNB

Social Relationship Marketing – captured with 3 items adapted from Palmatier, Gopalakrishna

and Houston (2006b) using 5-point Likert scales with the points labeled Never (1), Rarely (2),

Sometimes (3), Often (4) and Always (5).

1. My firm receives special treatment or status from BCNB. 2. BCNB provides meals, entertainment, or gifts to me or my firm. 3. My firm receives special reports and/or information from BCNB.

Structural Relationship Marketing – captured with 4 items adapted from Palmatier,

Gopalakrishna and Houston (2006b) using 5-point Likert scales with the points labeled Never (1),

Rarely (2), Sometimes (3), Often (4) and Always (5).

1. BCNB provides special value-added benefits to my firm. 2. BCNB customizes special programs or services for my firm. 3. BCNB adapts its policies for my firm. 4. BCNB assigns dedicated personnel to my firm.

Financial Relationship Marketing – captured with 3 items adapted from Palmatier,

Gopalakrishna and Houston (2006b) using 5-point Likert scales with the points labeled Never (1),

Rarely (2), Sometimes (3), Often (4) and Always (5).

1. My firm gets free products or services from BCNB. 2. My firm gets special pricing or discounts from BCNB. 3. BCNB provides my firm with special financial benefits and incentives.

Bank Constructive Conflict – captured with 5 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the

midpoint.

1. BCNB employees and associates work with my firm to solve challenges. 2. BCNB employees and associates discuss mutual problems with my firm. 3. BCNB employees and associates suggest solutions to my firm, if there is a conflict. 4. BCNB employees and associates voice concerns to my firm so that we can resolve

disputes.

Page 162: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

149

5. BCNB employees and associates work with my firm to improve the situation, when complications arise.

Dependence

Dependence was conceptualized as a two-dimensional construct comprised of (1) cost

based dependence and (2) benefit based dependence. Cost based dependence is defined as the

need to maintain the relationship because of the unrealized costs that would be incurred if the

relationship ended (Scheer et al. 2009). Benefit based dependence is defined as the need to

maintain the relationship because of the irreplaceable, unique net value that would be forfeited

if the relationship ended. Additionally, a “global” dependence item was asked of participants,

along with a relative dependence item so that these measures could be included as controls in

the analysis. The items for all of these measures are included below.

Benefit Based Dependence – captured with 3 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the

midpoint.

1. If my firm ended its relationship with BCNB, it would be difficult to accomplish our banking needs.

2. My firm receives benefits from doing business with BCNB that couldn’t be fully duplicated with the next best alternative.

3. If my firm ended its relationship with BCNB, the replacement would not be as effective.

Cost Based Dependence – captured with 3 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the

midpoint.

1. My firm would incur many costs to end its business with BCNB and switch to the next best alternative.

2. It would be costly for my firm to search for and locate an alternative to BCNB. 3. It would be costly for my firm to end its business relationship with BCNB.

Global Dependence

Page 163: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

150

Captured with a 7 point semantic differential scale anchored by (1) My firm is not at all dependent on BCNB and (7) My firm is extremely dependent on BCNB.

Relative Dependence

Captured with a 7 point semantic differential scale anchored by (1) BCNB is more dependent on my firm, and (7) My firm is more dependent on BCNB.

Share of Wallet

Consider all of your firm’s financial services that could be provided by BCNB. What share of that total potential business does your firm currently do with BCNB? _________%

Search for Alternatives – captured with 3 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the

midpoint.

1. My firm does business with competitors of BCNB. 2. My firm actively examines potential business opportunities from competitors of

BCNB. 3. My firm continuously searches for alternatives to BCNB.

Section III: Your Personal Relationship with BCNB

Customer Constructive Conflict – captured with 5 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from

1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the

midpoint.

1. I discuss mutual problems with BCNB. 2. If we have a conflict, I suggest solutions. 3. I work with BCNB to solve challenges. 4. I voice my concerns to BCNB so that we can resolve disputes. 5. When complications arise, I work with BCNB to improve the situation.

Commitment – captured with 3 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (Strongly

Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the midpoint.

1. I view my relationship with BCNB as a long-term partnership. 2. I am willing “to go the extra mile” to do business with BCNB.

Page 164: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

151

3. I feel committed to BCNB.

Relationship Orientation – captured with 3 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the

midpoint.

1. A close relationship with BCNB is important to my success. 2. A strong relationship with BCNB enables BCNB to provide higher quality services. 3. In my dealings with BCNB, a close relationship is important to me.

Bank-Owned Loyalty – captured with 4 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (Strongly

Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the midpoint.

1. I say positive things about BCNB to my coworkers because of the overall quality of BCNB’s services.

2. I would be less likely to do business with BCNB if the associates I normally deal with were no longer there.

3. I plan to do business with BCNB in the future no matter who may be assigned to handle my account(s).

4. Even if the associates I normally deal with were no longer at BCNB, I would recommend BCNB to someone seeking financial services.

Trust – captured with 3 items using 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7

(Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree” labeling the midpoint.

1. BCNB gives me a feeling of trust. 2. I think BCNB is trustworthy. 3. I trust BCNB.

Negative Word of Mouth – This measure asked the participant to consider a hypothetical

situation: “Imagine that BCNB made an error with one of your accounts, costing you significant

time and effort to get the error corrected. Following this situation, how likely would you be to

engage in the following actions? The construct was captured with 4 items using 6-point Likert

scales ranging from 1 (Not at all likely) to 7 (Extremely likely).

1. I would suggest to my friends that they avoid doing business with BCNB. 2. I would tell everyone about BCNB’s poor service. 3. I would encourage friends and relatives to consider other banks.

Page 165: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

152

4. I would discuss BCNB’s mistake with others, but I would also mention BCNB’s explanation of how and why it occurred.

Relational Reciprocity – This research proposes that relational reciprocity is a multi-dimensional

construct composed of 5 dimensions: presumption of innocence, comfort with reciprocal debts,

valuation of reciprocal debts, repayment horizon (time) and accounting of reciprocal debts. The

dimensions are conceptualized as reflective multi-item scales. The combination of these 5 first

order constructs results in a formative scale capturing the domain of relational reciprocity. The

whole range of relational reciprocity is anchored at one end by simple reciprocity and at the

other end by complex reciprocity. Larger scores on this multi-dimensional construct would

indicate increased levels of complex reciprocity.

Relational Reciprocity – Presumption of Innocence – captured with 3 items using 7-point Likert

scales ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor

Agree” labeling the midpoint.

1. In my relationship with BCNB and all of its employees and associates, I always seek an explanation before I assign blame.

2. When it appears that problems are caused by BCNB, I seek further explanation from BCNB before reacting.

3. In my dealings with BCNB, I give them a chance to explain situations that adversely affect me.

Relational Reciprocity – Comfort with Reciprocal debts – captured with 3 items using 7-point

Likert scales ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor

Agree” labeling the midpoint.

1. I am comfortable owing BCNB favors. 2. I am willing to recommend BCNB to others because I know BCNB will not let me

down. 3. In my relationship with BCNB and all of its employees and associates I am

comfortable being owed favors.

Page 166: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

153

Relational Reciprocity – Valuation of Reciprocal debts – captured with 2 items using 7-point

Likert scales ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor

Agree” labeling the midpoint.

1. In my relationship with BCNB and all of its employees and associates I know that a friendly gesture is appreciated, and is more important than the effort it takes to make it.

2. It is more important for BCNB and I to be willing to help each other out than to worry about who owes what to whom.

Relational Reciprocity – Repayment Horizon – captured with 2 items using 7-point Likert scales

ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor Agree”

labeling the midpoint.

1. When BCNB owes me a favor, I do not expect immediate payback. 2. I do not pressure BCNB to return favors quickly.

Relational Reciprocity – Accounting of Reciprocal debts – captured with 3 items using 7-point

Likert scales ranging from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) with “Neither Disagree nor

Agree” labeling the midpoint.

1. I keep track of the favors I owe to BCNB. 2. In the relationship with BCNB, I always know who owes what to whom. 3. If I do something that benefits BCNB (for example recommend a friend), I keep track

of the time and effort I spent on that activity. Bank Secondary Data on Business Customers

In addition to the survey items, data were collected from BCNB for each of the

responding business customers. These data are summarized below to illustrate the data

available for possible analysis.

Bank CVI – an index used by the bank to indicate some measure of quality of the customer. The

CVI is the sum of the following items for each customer:

1. Primary Checking Relationship 1 point 2. Primary Deposit Relationship and Primary Loan Relationship 1 point

Page 167: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

154

3. If number of primary deposit relationships plus primary loan relationships is: Between 2 and 5 1 point Greater than 5 2 points

4. Market Size – if percentage of primary customers/households reside in subjects zip code is greater or equal to 10% 1 point

5. Profit Percentile (weighted) – if profit percentile is: 40 – 69% 1 point 70 – 89% 2 points 90% plus 3 points

6. If number of years with BCNB is: 1 – 3 years 1 point 3 plus years 2 points Group Profit Contribution – the annual profit contribution for all accounts tied to this account in

dollars.

Group Profit Percentile – an index where the largest “Group Profit Contribution” = 100% and all

others are scaled accordingly.

SIC Code – Standard Industrial Classification Code from the U.S. Securities and Exchange

Commission.

Years with Institution – the # of years the account has been opened.

Max Years – the # of years that the longest account tied to this account has been open.

Profit Contribution – the annual profit contribution for this account.

Profit Percentile – an index where the largest “Profit Percentile” = 100% and all others are

scaled accordingly.

Profit Contribution per year – the Group Profit Contribution divided by the Years with

Institution.

Total Relationships (P) – Primary relationships assigned to the customer where they are

considered to be the primary owner.

Total Relationships (S) – Secondary relationship assigned to the customer where they are not

considered the primary owner.

Page 168: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

155

Services Used – Total service used in conjunction with this account. Services include the

following: CDs, Checking, Commercial Notes, Credit Cards, Credit Lines, Debit Cards, Installment

Loans, Investments, Money Markets, Mortgage Loans, On-line Banking, Retirement Accounts,

Safe Deposit Boxes, and Savings.

Service Used from All Accounts – Total services (same list as above) used from all accounts

linked to this account.

Average Deposit Balance – average monthly balance for all deposit services used by this

account.

Average Balances of the following services: loan balance; credit card; CD; credit line;

commercial note; checking; installment loan; investment balance; mortgage loan; money

market; retirement; and savings.

5.5.3 Sample Selection

The sample included a census of BCNB’s business customers. Rather than worry about

any bias associated with randomly sampling from within their customer database, the entire

database of approximately 4,400 business customers was included in the mailing. In addition to

the survey responses, BCNB provided us with some key performance indicators from their

database for all those customers who responded to the survey. Due to a lack of controls to

specifically address the situation, businesses owning multiple accounts at BCNB received one

survey for each account. While this could be problematic for data analysis, in reality only one

business returned multiple surveys. Because there was only one business that returned multiple

surveys, one of the two was randomly selected for deletion before the data analysis.

Surveys were received from March 3rd through April 14th. Surveys received after April

14th were not included in this analysis. In total 4,400 surveys were sent out to business

customers, 453 of which were returned as undeliverable due to bad addresses, closed accounts

Page 169: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

156

or other reasons. 492 surveys were returned from business owners, of these 28 were blank,

leaving 464 useable surveys (although some have some missing data) for a response rate of

11.76% (12.46% if blank surveys are included).

5.5.4 Measurement Model and Scale Creation

Separate exploratory factor analyses were performed on the antecedent variables, the

mediating variables and the outcome variables. Table #46 below shows the outcomes of the

exploratory factor analysis on the antecedent variables. As can be seen in Table #46, the

relationship orientation (RO1-3) and norms of reciprocity (PR1-6 and NR1-6) all loaded on their a

priori factors. The relationship investment variables (SoRM, StRM, and FRM) all loaded onto one

factor.

Table #46 - Antecedent Variables EFA

Component

1 2 3 4 Eigenvalue 6.097 4.470 2.979 1.638

PRN1 .633 PRN2 .862 PRN3 .791 PRN4 .829 PRN5 .760 PRN6 .609 NRN1 .808 NRN2 .743 NRN3 .847 NRN4 .844 NRN5 .842 NRN6 .719

SoRM1 .712 SoRM2 .640 SoRM3 .692 StRM1 .759 StRM2 .775 StRM3 .720 StRM4 .698 FRM1 .689

Page 170: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

157

FRM2 .763 FRM3 .819 RO1 .826 RO2 .758 RO3 .809

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis. Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.

rotation converged in 5 iterations.

A second exploratory factor analysis was performed with the complex reciprocity

variables – comfort, presumption of innocence, accounting, valuation and repayment horizon.

The results of this EFA are reported in table #47 below.

1 2 3 4

Eigenvalue 2.905 2.482 1.711 1.047

CRC1 .707CRC2 .755CRC3CRPI1 .731CRPI2 .854CRPI3 .828CRA1 .796CRA2 .805CRA3 .691CRV1CRV2CRT1 .870CRT2 .861

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.

Table #47 - EFA for Re la tiona l Rec iproc ity

Component

rotation converged in 7 iterations.

As we can see in Table #47, the factor analysis suggested only 4 (rather than 5) factors. The

valuation items did not seem to load on any of the factors, and the third comfort item (CRC3)

did not load onto any factors as well. Overall this EFA explained approximately 63% of the

variance in the complex reciprocity multidimensional construct.

Page 171: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

158

Finally an EFA was performed on the outcome latent variables – customer constructive

conflict (CC1-CC5), search for alternatives (SFA1-3), bank owned loyalty (BL1-4) and negative

word of mouth (NWOM1-4). The results are reported in table #48.

1 2 3 4

Eigenvalue 5.046 2.767 1.784 1.271

BL1 .603

BL2 -.689

BL3 .791

BL4 .701

NWOM1 .863

NWOM2 .900

NWOM3 .898

NWOM4 .615

SFA1 .682

SFA2 .870

SFA3 .829

CC1 .781

CC2 .712

CC3 .825

CC4 .855

CC5 .843

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.

Ta ble #48 - Outc om e va ria ble EFA Re s ults

Component

rotation converged in 5 iterations.

Based on these EFA results, BL1 was examined and found to be outside of the scope of the bank-

owned loyalty construct. Given the lack of fit into the theoretical construct, and the lack of

loading with the other BL items, BL1 will be dropped from further analysis. It should be noted

that BL2 is reverse coded – which makes sense given the negative loading on the bank-owned

loyalty component. These 4 factors explained approximately 67% of the variance in these items.

A measurement model was analyzed for the 6 antecedent latent constructs including:

positive norm of reciprocity – PR, negative norm of reciprocity – NR, relationship orientation –

RO, structural relationship marketing – StRM, social relationship marketing – SoRM, and

Page 172: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

159

financial relationship marketing – FRM was estimated using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) in

EQS 6.1 for windows. The measurement model was estimated using maximum likelihood

estimation with elliptical reweighted generalized least squares (ERLS) because ERLS estimates

are equivalent to maximum likelihood for normally distributed data, and superior to ML for non-

normal data (Zou and Cavusgil 2002).

The measurement model fit the data very well. All items loaded on the a priori factors,

with positive and significant variances, and factor loadings of at least 0.487. The measurement

model had acceptable fit statistics: χ2 = 581.329 (d.f. = 260), p < .001; Bentler-Bonnett Normed

Fit Index = .931; Bentler-Bonnett Non-Normed Fit Index = .954; CFI = .960; Standardized RMR =

.052; RMSEA = .054.

Examination of the modification indices suggested some changes in items on certain

factors; however further examination of both the items and the suggested factors ruled out the

possibility of moving these items to another factor. Given that the relationship marketing

factors all loaded onto one factor in the EFA results reported above, it seemed valuable to

investigate whether this 6 factor model was more fitting for the data than a 4 factor model with

all the relationship marketing variables loaded onto the same factor (agreeing with the EFA).

This second CFA produced a χ2 = 750.486 (d.f. = 269). Using the χ2 difference test gives us a Δχ2

= 169.157 (d.f. = 9) which is a significant improvement favoring the 6 factor model (p < .0001).

Therefore the 6 factor model will be used.

A second measurement model was estimated for the latent constructs of complex

reciprocity – comfort, presumption of innocence, valuation, accounting and time horizon. While

the EFA suggested dropping the valuation construct, we will begin under the assumption that it

should be included. The first measurement model had marginal fit statistics (χ2 = 170.738 (d.f. =

55), p < .001; Bentler-Bonnett Normed Fit Index = .870; Bentler-Bonnett Non-Normed Fit Index =

Page 173: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

160

.868; CFI = .907; Standardized RMR = .081; RMSEA = .070) and the modification indices

suggested a number of items that might better fit in other factors. The first item that would fit

better in a different factor was CRC2; however after reading the item, it was not conceptually

similar to the suggested factor. Additionally, the standardized factor loading of CRC2 was very

low (.329) and it only explained 10% of the variance in the factor. Given these outcomes, it was

decided to remove CRC2 from further analysis. Note that the final measurement model did

include the valuation construct – even though the EFA suggested otherwise. The new

measurement model exhibited improved fit statistics (χ2 = 118.105 (d.f. = 44), p < .001; Bentler-

Bonnett Normed Fit Index = .901; Bentler-Bonnett Non-Normed Fit Index = .902; CFI = .935;

Standardized RMR = .065; RMSEA = .063). Further, a χ2 difference test gives us a Δ χ2 = 52.633

(d.f. = 11) which is a significant improvement over the measurement model with CRC2 included

(p < .0001).

Finally, a measurement model was estimated for the outcome latent variables:

Customer constructive conflict (FCC1-5), search for alternatives (SFA1-3), bank owned loyalty

(BL1-4) and negative word of mouth (NWOM1-4). Repeated attempts at fitting a proper model

through dropping items, fixing the variance of particular items and allowing individual items to

correlate proved ineffective. The items measuring the latent constructs of search for

alternatives and bank owned loyalty simply did not fit properly into a CFA. Further examination

of these items suggests that the search for alternative variables actually should be considered a

formative scale measuring that construct. In this context, it is likely that the bank owned loyalty

measure, with its inclusion of references to the associates the customer normally deals with,

may be perceived as inappropriate by respondents. In fact, less than half (46.5%) of the

respondents answered that there was one primary person that they dealt with at the bank. It is

possible that the wording of the question confused the other respondents as they did not have a

Page 174: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

161

familiar contact at the bank. The latent construct of commitment (C1-3) was used as a proxy for

loyalty.

A final measurement model was estimated for the outcome latent variables: customer

constructive conflict, negative word of mouth and commitment. The individual items in this

new measurement model all had positive and significant variances, and standardized factor

loadings of at least .478. The model exhibited acceptable fit statistics (χ2 = 132.879 (d.f. = 51), p

< .001; Bentler-Bonnett Normed Fit Index = .967; Bentler-Bonnett Non-Normed Fit Index = .973;

CFI = .979; Standardized RMR = .039; RMSEA = .060).

As a final step, all latent constructs were entered into one final measurement model.

Not surprisingly, all items had positive and significant variances. Standardized factor loadings

were at least .465. The overall model exhibited acceptable fit statistics (χ2 = 1672.711 (d.f. =

1036), p < .001; Bentler-Bonnett Normed Fit Index = .923; Bentler-Bonnett Non-Normed Fit

Index = .965; CFI = .969; Standardized RMR = .052; RMSEA = .039). This measurement model is

summarized in table #49 below.

Table #50 (below) summarizes the means, standard deviations and correlations

between all variables used in our model. Note that the construct “complex” represents a

formative composition of the 5 complex reciprocity measures. Services used, and Services Used

from all accounts are proxy variables for exchange complexity. Interaction frequency was not

captured in this survey, but will be in the next survey and tested in the model there. Negative

word of mouth is the proxy for “Negative Reciprocity” in the model. And as previously

mentioned, SFA was conceptualized as a formative measure – combining SFA2 and SFA3 to

capture the customer’s level of search for alternatives.

Page 175: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

162

ItemN

orm

of P

osit

ive

Reci

proc

ity

Nor

m o

f Neg

ativ

e Re

cipr

ocit

y

Soci

al R

elat

ions

hip

Mar

keti

ng

Stru

ctur

al

Rela

tion

ship

M

arke

ting

Fina

ncia

l Re

lati

onsh

ip

Mar

keti

ng

Firm

Con

stru

ctiv

e Co

nflic

t

Com

mit

men

t

Rela

tion

ship

O

rien

tati

on

Ong

oing

Rec

ipro

city

- Co

mfo

rt

Ong

oing

Rec

ipro

city

- A

ccou

ntin

g

Ong

oing

Rec

ipro

city

- Pr

esum

ptio

n of

In

noce

nce

Ong

oing

Rec

ipro

city

- V

alua

tion

Ong

oing

Rec

ipro

city

- Ti

me

Neg

ativ

e W

ord

of

Mou

th

PRN1 0.584PRN2 0.854PRN3 0.775PRN4 0.834PRN5 0.695PRN6 0.483NRN1 0.728NRN2 0.635NRN3 0.782NRN4 0.857NRN5 0.854NRN6 0.700SoRM1 0.759SoRM2 0.640SoRM3 0.696StRM1 0.769StRM2 0.793StRM3 0.742StRM4 0.677FRM1 0.714FRM2 0.829FRM3 0.877FCC1 0.712FCC2 0.622FCC3 0.793FCC4 0.853FCC5 0.837

C1 0.765C2 0.864C3 0.812

RO1 0.831RO2 0.694RO3 0.739CRC1 0.769CRC3 0.578CRA1 0.867CRA2 0.675CRA3 0.489CRPI1 0.555CRPI2 0.771CRPI3 0.892CRV1 0.489CRV2 0.620CRT1 0.778CRT2 0.847

NWOM1 0.870NWOM2 0.872NWOM3 0.939NWOM4 0.465

Latent ConstructTable #49 - Overall CFA results for all items in the theoretical model

Page 176: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

12

34

56

78

910

1112

1314

1516

1718

1920

r p N r.3

90p

.000

N44

4r

.399

.745

p.0

00.0

00N

443

457

r.2

82.5

99.6

80p

.000

.000

.000

N43

945

345

2r

.406

.874

.936

.838

p.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

00N

436

451

451

451

r.1

66.0

90.0

74.1

44.1

12p

.000

.056

.115

.002

.018

N44

345

445

244

944

5r

-.068

-.009

.021

-.003

.003

-.152

p.1

59.8

42.6

56.9

52.9

44.0

01N

437

450

447

444

441

448

r.0

55.1

27.2

10.1

35.1

88-.0

53.0

64p

.250

.007

.000

.004

.000

.263

.182

N43

344

544

344

043

644

343

8r

.087

.121

.089

.094

.117

-.118

.038

.361

p.0

70.0

11.0

61.0

48.0

15.0

13.4

29.0

00N

433

445

443

440

436

443

438

450

r.1

77.1

64.1

15.1

01.1

37.0

04-.0

21.1

61.1

36p

.000

.001

.016

.036

.004

.933

.662

.001

.005

N42

943

643

543

242

843

643

042

742

7

μ =

.14.

95

σ =

3.93

μ =

6.11

σ

= 2.

52

μ =

7.95

σ

= 3.

59

μ =

4.95

σ

= 2.

36

μ =

18.9

4

σ =

7.49

(5) R

M

(6) P

R

(7) N

R

(8) S

ervi

ces

Use

d***

*

(9) S

ervi

ces

Use

d fro

m a

ll ac

coun

ts

(10)

sha

re

Tabl

e #5

0 - M

eans

, Sta

ndar

d D

evia

tions

and

Cor

rela

tions

Bet

wee

n S

tudy

Var

iabl

es

(1) R

O

(2) S

oRM

(3) S

tRM

(4) F

RM

μ =

34.9

4

σ =

5.25

μ =

14.3

4

σ =

7.11

μ =

2.22

σ

= 1.

50

μ =

3.14

σ

= 2.

79

μ =

.787

σ

= .2

88

roset
Typewritten Text
163
Page 177: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

Ta

ble

#50

- Con

tinu

ed

12

34

56

78

910

1112

1314

1516

1718

1920

r-.1

13-.0

40-.0

03.0

09-.0

08.0

18.1

62-.0

15-.0

55-.2

79p

.018

.396

.945

.856

.862

.707

.001

.750

.250

.000

N44

244

944

844

544

144

944

343

843

843

7r

.628

.351

.368

.247

.374

.192

-.092

.021

.026

.162

-.024

p.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

53.6

59.5

92.0

01.6

17N

442

446

445

442

438

446

440

436

436

432

445

r.0

63-.0

27-.0

18-.0

08-.0

29.1

24.0

72-.0

29-.0

09.0

23.1

15.0

28p

.204

.576

.717

.868

.564

.011

.142

.556

.859

.639

.019

.563

N41

341

741

641

340

941

941

240

740

740

641

541

6r

.759

.419

.438

.290

.439

.166

-.093

.059

.046

.290

-.288

.642

.067

p.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

51.2

16.3

39.0

00.0

00.0

00.1

74N

443

448

449

444

442

448

442

438

438

435

448

446

416

r-.3

13-.1

94-.1

55-.0

95-.1

67-.0

51.1

13-.1

17-.0

26-.1

99.2

28-.2

38-.0

07-.3

82p

.000

.000

.001

.045

.000

.283

.017

.014

.580

.000

.000

.000

.890

.000

N44

245

145

044

744

344

944

544

144

143

444

644

441

644

6r

.213

.286

.315

.311

.346

.113

.160

.051

.057

.108

.001

.137

.064

.260

-.033

p.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

00.0

17.0

01.2

88.2

36.0

25.9

86.0

04.1

93.0

00.4

93N

437

441

440

437

433

441

435

431

431

427

440

440

412

440

443

r.1

26.0

79.1

13.0

99.1

06.0

25.2

26-.0

50.0

16.0

82.1

22.0

76.0

67.1

01.0

63.3

96p

.009

.099

.018

.040

.027

.605

.000

.306

.742

.092

.011

.111

.173

.035

.189

.000

N43

543

743

643

443

043

743

142

742

742

443

643

741

143

743

643

7r

.271

.077

.110

.019

.081

.205

-.140

-.037

-.019

.135

-.035

.458

.018

.254

-.179

-.057

-.071

p.0

00.1

03.0

20.6

85.0

89.0

00.0

03.4

42.6

90.0

05.4

62.0

00.7

22.0

00.0

00.2

31.1

37N

442

446

445

443

439

446

441

436

436

433

446

445

415

446

447

442

436

r.2

65.1

96.1

83.1

15.1

89.2

72-.1

72-.0

64-.0

49.0

18-.0

94.2

85.0

26.2

55-.2

19.0

29-.1

56.3

87p

.000

.000

.000

.016

.000

.000

.000

.185

.307

.705

.048

.000

.601

.000

.000

.542

.001

.000

N43

844

144

043

843

444

143

743

243

242

844

144

041

244

244

443

843

244

5r

.177

.131

.113

.152

.149

.117

-.050

.022

.017

.097

.012

.130

.003

.104

-.099

.225

-.057

.114

.291

p.0

00.0

06.0

18.0

01.0

02.0

14.2

95.6

49.7

22.0

44.8

07.0

06.9

46.0

29.0

38.0

00.2

35.0

17.0

00N

437

441

440

437

433

439

435

431

431

426

439

438

411

439

442

438

432

442

440

r.3

74.2

82.3

22.2

63.3

29.2

45.0

32-.0

37.0

21.1

76.0

17.4

01.0

75.3

55-.1

49.6

25.5

41.4

86.4

57.5

20p

.000

.000

.000

.000

.000

.000

.507

.453

.668

.000

.723

.000

.137

.000

.002

.000

.000

.000

.000

.000

N42

242

442

342

241

842

341

941

341

341

142

342

339

942

442

642

742

742

742

742

7

μ =

53.8

8

σ =

7.51

μ =

25.0

8

σ =

5.85

μ =

3.05

σ

= 1.

39

μ =

15.1

7

σ =

4.08

μ =

13.0

0

σ =

5.66

μ =

7.22

σ

= 2.

83

μ =

8.13

σ

= 3.

72

μ =

5.40

σ

= 3.

12

(17)

CR

A

(18)

CR

PI

(19)

CR

V

(20)

CR

T

μ =

16.9

9

σ =

3.01

μ =

11.5

2

σ =

2.07

μ =

9.97

σ

= 2.

54

(21)

ong

oing

(11)

SFA

(12)

FC

C

(13)

Gui

lt

(14)

C

omm

itmen

t

(15)

NW

OM

(16)

CR

C

roset
Typewritten Text
164
Page 178: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

165

5.5.5 Hypothesis Testing

The following hypotheses were tested using Structural Equation Modeling.

H1: Relationships characterized by greater complex reciprocity will exhibit increased share of wallet over time.

H2: Relationships characterized by greater complex reciprocity will exhibit decreased search for alternatives.

H3: Relationships characterized by less complex reciprocity will exhibit a higher level of negative reciprocal responses to detrimental actions.

H4: Relationships characterized by greater complex reciprocity will exhibit higher levels of constructive conflict in response to detrimental acts.

H5: Relationships characterized by greater complex reciprocity will exhibit a higher degree of guilt in the face of anticipated unpaid reciprocal debts.

H6: Customer relationship orientation will lead to greater complex reciprocity.

H7: The customer’s perception of selling firm relational investments will lead to greater complex reciprocity.

H8: Customers scoring higher on the positive norm of reciprocity scale are more likely to be involved in relational reciprocity.

H9: Customers scoring higher on the negative norm of reciprocity scale are more likely to be involved in discrete reciprocity relationships.

H10: As the content of exchange increases in complexity, relational reciprocity will become more likely.

H11: As the interaction frequency increases, relational reciprocity will become more likely. – not tested in this research due to lack of information on interaction frequency.

The model depicted in Figure #29 was tested with the structural equation modeling

methodology in EQS 6.1 for windows. Items for each latent construct in the overall

measurement model (table 53) were used to estimate the structural model. As with the

measurement models, the structural path model was estimated with the procedure of

maximum likelihood (ML) followed by reweighted generalized least squares (ERLS). The original

path model demonstrated acceptable fit (χ2 = 960.385 (d.f. = 474), p < .001; Bentler-Bonnett

Page 179: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

166

Normed Fit Index = .910; Bentler-Bonnett Non-Normed Fit Index = .946; CFI = .952; Standardized

RMR = .063; RMSEA = .055). Table #51 summarizes the structural model results.

Table #51 Structural Model Results (N = 345)

Effects Hypothesized Path Coef1 t-Value Conclusion H6 Relationship Orientation → Comp. rec. 1.687 6.075 Supported H7 Relationship Marketing → Comp. rec. -0.004 -0.034 Not supported H8 Pos. Norm of Reciprocity → Comp. rec. 0.251 1.280 Not supported H9 Neg. Norm of Reciprocity → Comp. rec. -0.089 -0.543 Not supported

H10 Exchange Complexity → Comp. rec. .314 1.188 Not supported H11 Interaction Frequency → Comp. rec. Na Na Not tested H1 Complex reciprocity → Share of Wallet 0.024 3.692 Supported H2 Complex reciprocity → Search For Alt. -0.254 -3.649 Supported H3 Complex reciprocity → Neg. Rec. -0.238 -5.691 Supported H4 Complex reciprocity → Firm Const. Conf. 0.308 6.272 Supported H5 Complex reciprocity → Guilt 0.032 1.134 Not supported

Complex reciprocity → Commitment 0.367 6.803 Post-Hoc test

1Standardized path coefficient

Because different types of relationship marketing have been shown to have unique

impacts on relationship outcomes (Palmatier et al. 2006b), an alternative model was compared

that separated social, structural and financial relationship marketing efforts to look for any

unique impacts on the complex reciprocity construct. This second structural model also

provided acceptable fit statistics, but there were no differences in significant paths from the first

model tested. Therefore, results of this alternative model are not discussed further.

Of the antecedents investigated, relationship orientation proved to have a major impact

on the process of reciprocation. In fact this was the only antecedent that had a significant

impact (β = 1.687) on the complex reciprocity construct. This was also the largest standardized

coefficient in the entire path model (see figure #30 below).

Page 180: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

167

Of the outcomes, 5 of the 6 hypothesized relationships were supported. Complex reciprocity

has a positive impact on share of wallet (β = 0.024), customer constructive conflict (β = 0.308)

and commitment (β = 0.367). Complex reciprocity also reduced the customer’s search for

alternatives (β = -0.254), and likelihood of negative reciprocity (β = -0.238) given a detrimental

situation.

In addition to the structural path model reported in Figure #30 and Table #51, a

mediation analysis was conducted following Baron and Kenny (1986). Of the 6 hypothesized

antecedents, only one – relationship orientation (RO) was found to be significant. The mediator

proposed was complex reciprocity, which was found to be a significant mediator in the

structural model. Four of the five hypothesized outcome variables were significant, and

additionally a post-hoc test found that commitment was a significant outcome variable as well.

Page 181: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

168

For each of these 5 outcome variables the same mediation test was performed with ordinary

least squares regression. The results are summarized in table #52.

In the first step (Path 1), the mediator (complex reciprocity) was regressed on the independent

variable (RO) – this step verifies that the independent variable significantly predicts the

mediator (standardized β = 0.374, p < .001). The second path that was tested was to regress

each of the outcome variables on the independent variable (RO). In each case the coefficient

here was significant and in the predicted direction (see table #52). This step verifies that the

independent variable significantly predicts the outcome variable when the mediator is not

controlled for. The final step (Path 3) is to regress the dependent variables on both the

independent (RO) and the mediator (complex reciprocity) variables. In order to conclude that

the path is mediated the coefficients in step 1 and 2 need to be significant and in the

hypothesized directions – in every case this criteria was met. Additionally, a fully mediated path

is signified by the independent variable becoming insignificant when controlling for the

mediator (step 3) – none of the paths tested supported a fully mediated path.

Path 1 Path 2

RO → Ongoing Reciprocity

RO → Dependent Variable

Dependent Variable

βA RO βA RO βA RO βA Ongoing Reciprocity Conclusion

Share of Wallet

0.177 (ρ < .001)

0.140 (ρ = .008)

0.126 (ρ < .017)

Partial Mediation

Search for Alternatives

- 0.113 (ρ = .018)

- 0.163 (ρ = .002)

0.084 (ρ = .109)

Direct Effects

Negative Reciprocity

- 0.313 (ρ < .001)

-0.308 (ρ < .001)

- 0.034 (ρ < .494)

Direct Effects

Constructive Conflict

0.628 (ρ < .001)

0.560 (ρ < .001)

0.192 (ρ < .001)

Partial Mediation

Commitment0.759

(ρ < .001)0.731

(ρ < .001)0.085

(ρ = .012)Partial

MediationA - all coefficients reported are standardized coefficients

Table #52 - Mediation Analyses for the Structural Path ModelPath 1 and 2 combined

RO + Ongoing Recip → Dependent Variable

0.374 (ρ < .001)

Page 182: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

169

Partial mediation is shown by a significant path from the mediator and independent

variable to the outcome variable in step 3. As shown in table #52, 3 of the 5 paths suggested

partial mediation. The outcome variables Search for Alternatives and Negative Reciprocity

suggested a direct effects only path from the independent to the dependent variables, and

therefore did not support the hypothesized mediating role of complex reciprocity. The paths

from RO to (1) Share of Wallet, (2) Constructive Conflict, and (3) Commitment all supported a

partially mediated path.

5.5.7 Discussion

5.5.7.1 Episodic Reciprocity

So what has been learned about reciprocity? Prior to this research, there existed a large

quantity of research examining factors which impacted individuals’ reciprocal responses. All of

this research had been conducted in a laboratory environment, and the measures of

reciprocation were the objective outcomes of various tasks or economic games. Although

theory suggests an important psychological component of reciprocity – reciprocal debt – none

of the extant research had attempted to capture this perceptual aspect of reciprocation. In

order to better understand the process of reciprocation, it is imperative that this debt be

measured as it will help to better explain why people respond differently to the same situation.

What factors specific to the exchange, and which factors specific to the exchange parties will

impact the magnitude of this perceived reciprocal debt? Why do people differ in their

perception of the debt, and further why might they differ in actions when they attempt to

resolve the debt they perceive? These questions cannot be answered without first measuring

the reciprocal debt created by actions within a relationship.

Page 183: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

170

This research developed a scale which was able to capture the reciprocal debt created

by impactful actions between exchange partners. Through both a lab experiment, and through

surveys administered to exchange partners in long term business relationships, this measure of

reciprocal debt performed well and was both reliably predicted by a partner’s action, and it also

was an important and significant predictor of relational outcomes. Evidence supported the role

of reciprocal debt as a mediator which helps determine the response an individual will have to a

given action, thus increasing our understanding of the exchange process.

While many of the proposed moderating relationships were not supported, there were

numerous factors which had an impact on the reciprocal debt created. The recipient’s

perceptions of the relative outcomes of the action appeared to moderate the impact of an

action on the debt created, and also had a strong direct effect. When good things happen, it is

important that the beneficial impact on the selling firm is not the focus of any communications.

The more a firm benefits, relative to their partner, the greater the reciprocal debt created which

should lead to improved outcomes over time. On the other hand, when a firm must make a

decision that has some negative impact on itself and its exchange partners, it is imperative that

all those affected understand that the focal firm is suffering as well. As the perceived disparity

between the negative impact on a partner firm and that on the focal firm decreases, exchange

partners feel less of a need for retribution or reparation. As an example if a selling firm must

increase prices to their customers because of increased costs or profit losses, it would be critical

that the customers understand the negative financial situation for the selling firm to minimize

any customer churn created by this detrimental situation.

This research was also the first to investigate the locus of negative reciprocal debts in

both ongoing relationships, and in a laboratory environment. While not hypothesized, it was

Page 184: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

171

shown that most people expect that the responsible party should make up for a detrimental

situation. This finding is probably not that surprising in and of itself; however an important

implication for the lab-based reciprocity research, which often only allows participants the

opportunity to resolve these detrimental situations through punishment, is that future

laboratory designs should allow for the option/possibility of the wrong-doer making up for the

situation. It appears that most people would prefer to not have to punish their partner. It is

possible that punishment and retribution are thought of as last resorts only to be used if the

responsible party fails to reconcile the situation. It would be interesting to investigate what

factors might sway people’s preference for retribution over reparation in these situations. Are

there certain characteristics of the relationship or parties that impact this preference? Is the

decision made only on the basis of the offending action? Are some things too detrimental to

allow a partner a second chance? Relationship marketers would benefit from a better

understanding of when their partners’ preference will change with respect to punishment and

reparation. Could it be there is some time component that affects this choice?

Also important in the formation of reciprocal debt is the customer’s attribution of

control of the action. While the proposed moderating impact of the attribution of control was

not found, the direct effect of this important perception provides important implications for

relationship marketers. It is important that beneficial situations are presented as if they were

controlled by the partner organization, otherwise the feeling of a need to repay the partner will

be decreased. Similarly, if something bad happens that is not the focal firm’s fault, it is

important that they communicate that other organizations or forces were responsible for the

detrimental impact on their partner. If people realize that something outside the control of

their partner caused the negative impact, they will be more forgiving in their desire for

Page 185: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

172

retribution or reparations from their partner. This finding helps to illustrate the importance of

quality communication in the face of challenging situations.

Similarly, the customer’s perception of the justifiability of a detrimental action can

reduce the desire to punish the responsible firm. If firms can present these strategic decisions

and the reasoning behind them, the negative impact on their existing relationships should be

reduced. This suggests that transparency and honesty are important in marketing relationships,

especially when potentially harmful decisions must be made. If the selling firm must make a

change in policy that adversely affects its customers, it is imperative that the customers

understand why that decision had to be made.

Finally the customer’s adherence to the positive norm of reciprocity also impacted the

creation of reciprocal debt. Those who scored higher on the positive norm of reciprocity tended

to reward their partners better given a beneficial situation. On the other hand, the negative

norm of reciprocity did not have any direct or interactive effect on the creation of reciprocal

debt. Is it possible that an overall business norm exists which defines the expected response

given a detrimental situation? Or could these non-findings be more illustrative of indecision

about the allocation of the locus of a reciprocal debt? Is it possible that those higher in the

negative norm of reciprocity are more likely to try to punish their partner? Future research

should work towards a better explanation of this process and how the locus and the negative

norm of reciprocity might interact to impact the perceived reciprocal debt.

The results of this research clearly suggest that reciprocal debt needs to be considered

in the process of exchange. Failure to do so leaves out an important mediating mechanism that

helps to shape the outcomes of any exchange. While support was not found for many of the

proposed moderators, most did directly impact the creation of reciprocal debt. Further,

Page 186: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

173

reciprocal debt was shown to reliably predict the exchange outcomes. While theory had long

established the existence of this relational mechanism, this is the first research that has

conceptualized and measured this perceived debt. Future research should further investigate

factors which impact the magnitude of debt created, the perceived locus of that debt, and what

factors might impact the resolution of that debt within the relationship.

5.5.7.2 Complex reciprocity

The second research project investigated a multi-dimensional measure of complex

reciprocity, hypothesized to range from simple reciprocity in arms length transactions to more

complex reciprocity in closer, relational exchange. This project viewed reciprocity as a relational

enabling process which helps partners develop deeper, more enmeshed relationships consistent

with the social exchange literature. This research developed a multi-dimensional scale to

capture this complex reciprocity and positioned that complex reciprocity as a relational

mediator which encourages beneficial relationship outcomes in exchange relationships. The

new scale captured a range of characteristics that would describe reciprocity as partners

transition to relational exchange. Overall, five dimensions were captured that comprise

complex reciprocity: comfort with reciprocal debts; presumption of innocence; repayment

duration; accounting of reciprocal debts; and valuation of reciprocal debts.

Of the proposed antecedents to complex reciprocity, this analysis found that only the

customer’s relationship orientation directly impacted the level of complex reciprocity. As

participants viewed relational exchange as more beneficial for them, they tended to engage in

more complex complex reciprocity. Those who did not view relational exchange as beneficial

engaged in much simpler complex reciprocity (scored lower). It was surprising to see that this

was the only significant predictor of complex reciprocity in this analysis. Future research will

Page 187: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

174

benefit by examining other possible antecedents, and possibly by modeling a different path to

complex reciprocity through these proposed antecedents. Importantly, this research found

evidence that complex reciprocity is an important relational mediator within ongoing business

to business relationships.

The outcomes of complex reciprocity include increased share of wallet, constructive

conflict and commitment, and decreased search for alternatives and negative reciprocity. This

suggests that as relationships can develop into more complex reciprocity, firms will benefit by

greater positive relational outcomes, and decreased punishment in the face of adversity and a

reduced chance that their customers will exit and find an alternative. Clearly in this context, it is

important for businesses to work towards developing more complex reciprocity with their

customers.

This brings up an important question for future research: Is complex reciprocity always

preferable? Are there conditions that make simple reciprocity more appealing and profitable?

Future research should investigate this, as clearly business relationships do endure for long

periods of time as “arms-length” transactions. Within the context of this research, it appears

that complex reciprocity is preferable, but are there other industries or contexts where a more

simple reciprocity is the preferred exchange process?

5.5.7.3 Summary

In total, this research clearly identified and measured the psychological component of

reciprocity – reciprocal debt. Reciprocal debt was shown to be an important mediating

mechanism in the process of episodic exchange, advancing both base theories of reciprocity and

the marketing literature which often mentions, but rarely measures, reciprocity in a relational

context. Important insights were gleaned about relational factors which impact the creation of

Page 188: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

175

reciprocal debt, and other questions remain to be answered. Secondly this research examined

the characteristics of reciprocal exchange in ongoing business relationships. Proposed

reciprocity dimensions were shown to be important predictors of established relational

outcomes. Prior theory and empirical evidence has suggested that reciprocation plays an

important role as a mediating mechanism in social exchange, this research was the first to

conceptualize and measure how reciprocity differs based on McNeil’s Social Exchange Theory.

While little was clarified about the role to achieving complex reciprocity, it was clear that the

development of complex reciprocity has important and impactful implications for marketing

relationships. Overall this research has (1) advanced the base reciprocity theory; (2) improved

the understanding of reciprocity in marketing exchange relationships; and (3) provided

important managerial implications for the development of more successful marketing

relationships. Additionally, many avenues for future research are provided and these authors

hope that other researchers will continue to broaden our understanding of the reciprocation

process.

Page 189: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

176

CHAPTER 6 CONCLUSIONS

This dissertation approached the process of reciprocation from two distinct

perspectives. The first half addressed the episodic reciprocity that occurs during the act of any

given interaction between two exchange participants. In that way it can be applied across a

broad range of contexts. The second approach looked at the role of relational reciprocity over

the course of an exchange relationship. This portion of the research measured the perceptions

and actions of real business partners to investigate how reciprocity may change as a relationship

broadens or develops over time. This chapter will summarize the contributions this dissertation

makes to both reciprocity theory, and to marketing practitioners.

6.1 Contributions to Theory

While numerous researchers, both in and outside of marketing, have discussed the

importance of reciprocity for business relationships, this is the first research to conceptualize

and measure a reciprocal debt as an important mediating mechanism connecting one party’s

action with their partner’s response. Prior marketing research had narrowly focused on finding

correlated strategies between channel partners as evidence of reciprocal behavior. This view is

overly narrow as it doesn’t allow for repaying reciprocal debts through alternative means.

While early reciprocity theory talked much about the debt that is created when someone does

something for us, the extant research has failed to measure that debt. This research developed

a scale and empirically validated the impact of creating reciprocal debt in an exchange partner.

In the investigation of relational reciprocity, this research broadened the view of the

role of reciprocity to position it as a mediating mechanism in the process of exchange over time.

Reciprocity theory clearly implicates a delay in the process of reciprocation, and actually

suggests that this delay helps to build stronger relationships, however extant marketing

Page 190: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

177

literature fails to account for the process of reciprocation over time. While the majority of

existing reciprocity theory investigates the immediate response by one party to the actions of

another, this research conceptualized and measured relational reciprocity – a partner’s

perceptions and attitudes about the process of relational exchange – and found that it helps

explain many beneficial outcomes for exchange partners. The more complex is their relational

exchange, the more successful is the relationship as judged by numerous popular relationship

performance measures.

Extant reciprocity research has been primarily conducted in sterile laboratory settings.

Exchange partners typically do not know each other, nor do they have any expectation of a

continued relationship over time. When studying an exchange process like reciprocation – that

takes time to consummate – it would be more appropriate to study the process in real

relationships. This research extends the investigation of reciprocal behavior into real world

business relationships. By surveying customers of a real bank, this research was able to test

theoretical propositions in “the real world”, offering validity to the findings and the importance

of reciprocity for ongoing relationships.

Finally, this research clearly shows support for the role of reciprocity as a relational

mediator. Through three different studies, 2 different conceptualizations of reciprocity

(reciprocal debt and relational reciprocity) both were found to mediate the exchange process in

various types of settings and exchange contexts. Early research on relationship marketing

(Morgan and Hunt 1994), and a recent meta-analysis (Palmatier et al. 2006a) both find evidence

of a missing mediator. Reciprocity has been suggested as that missing mediator by a number of

marketing researchers. This is the first research to conceptualize and measure the role of

reciprocity as a relational mediator.

Page 191: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

178

6.2 Contributions to Practitioners

A clearer understanding of the role of reciprocity in marketing relationships will allow

managers to more effectively invest in relationship marketing programs. Establishing programs

that develop the greatest amount of positive reciprocal debt in your customers will prove to be

the best investments in relationship marketing. Researchers have continued to question the

effectiveness of relationship marketing investments, is it possible that companies are using

programs that reduce the chance to build a debt in their customers? What boundary conditions

will allow customers to repay that debt and benefit the company? Is there some business norm

that exists that will result in a mirrored response strategy? Clearly there are many questions left

unanswered, but this research is a first effort at establishing the importance of reciprocal debt in

marketing exchange. Throughout the research one consistent finding was that building a

greater feeling of debt in your exchange partner resulted in greater outcomes for you.

This research also informs a firm’s communication strategy. When a firm must make

some change in their policies that adversely affects their customers, this research suggests that

they can minimize the negative impact on themselves by explaining the reasoning behind the

harmful changes. Harmed parties reacted less harshly to actions they considered justified by

their exchange partners. If a selling firm must increase prices to remain profitable, transparency

with their customers may result in a reduction in lost sales due to the price increase. Also it is

important that the firm gets credit for strategies that help their customers. Be sure to take

credit for actions that will benefit your exchange partners and they will repay you in the future.

Obviously this may backfire if it appears to be taken advantage of, but being honest when your

firm is able to offer some benefit will result in improved relational outcomes.

Page 192: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

179

Finally, the work on relational reciprocity suggests one route to building a stronger,

more embedded customer base. While there are numerous dimensions of a successful

relationship, it is clear that the 5 dimensions of relational reciprocity can signal to your partner

that you are investing in building a long lasting, committed relationship. If they are willing to

reciprocate those actions it will result in improved outcomes for the firm. While there are many

strategies to building better customer relationships, doing favors and trusting your partner to

return them is one way to signal your commitment to them and to the future of the

relationship.

6.3 Future Research Directions

Of course this research raises some questions for the future of reciprocity theory. As

with all research, it will be important to test these findings in a number of different contexts.

Importantly, are there types of industries that lend themselves more to reciprocal relationships,

or do these findings hold in a wide variety of industries and business relationships? It seems

that the ideal context with which to impact a relationship through reciprocity is one where the

buyer and seller work together in a collaborative or highly interactive role such that there were

multiple opportunities to reciprocate your partner’s actions.

Many of the proposed moderators did not seem to affect the reciprocal response in this

research. Does that suggest that there is a standard reciprocal response, or does it suggest that

there may be other important moderators that impact the degree of reciprocation given a

particular action? On the flip side, what boundary conditions would decrease the chance of

reciprocation? Also, this research did not investigate what might make an exchange partner

prefer retaliation instead of compensation after they’ve been slighted. Understanding that

would be valuable in many business relationships.

Page 193: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

180

With respect to the relational reciprocity findings, it was interesting that relationship

orientation was the only significant antecedent to relational reciprocity. It is difficult to believe

that there aren’t any other antecedents to relational reciprocity. What other variables might

make a party more likely to engage in more complex reciprocity? On the other hand, what

conditions might make complex reciprocity less desirable? Many business relationships exist at

a more arms-length level, the naïve interpretation of our findings is that all relationships should

be pushed towards complex reciprocity, but is that necessarily so?

This research was the first to take a very close look at a very important relational

process – that of reciprocation. Marketing researchers have long cited reciprocity as an

important process, but have failed to include in their conceptual models and their empirical

studies. While it is possible that more questions were raised by this research than were

answered, it is the hope of these researchers that future relationship marketing researchers will

begin to include this important variable in their research. Reciprocity obviously has an

important position in marketing relationships.

Page 194: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

181

APPENDIX

Exhibit #1 – University of Missouri Email Cover letter

Page 195: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

182

Exhibit #2 UAB Email Cover Letter

Page 196: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

183

Exhibit #3 – Customer Survey

Page 197: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

184

Page 198: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

185

Page 199: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

186

Page 200: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

187

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Anderson, Erin and Barton Weitz (1989), "Determinants of Continuity in Conventional Industrial Channel Dyads," Marketing Science, 8 (4), 310. ---- (1992), "The Use of Pledges to build and Sustain Commitment in Distribution Channels," Journal of Marketing Research, 29 (1), 18-34. Anderson, James C. and James A. Narus (1990), "A Model of Distributor Firm and Manufacturer Firm Working Partnerships," Journal of Marketing, 54 (1), 42-58. Bagozzi, Richard P. (1975), "Marketing as Exchange," Journal of Marketing, 39 (4), 32-39. ---- (1995), "Reflections on Relationship Marketing in Consumer Markets," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 23 (4), 272-77. ---- (1992), "The Self-Regulation of Attitudes, Intentions, and Behavior," Social Psychology Quarterly, 55 (2), 178-204. Baron, R. M. and D. A. Kenny (1986), "The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations," Journal of personality and social psychology, 51 (6), 1173-82. Becker, Howard (1956), Man in Reciprocity: Introductory Lectures on Culture, Society and Personality. New York: Praeger Publishers, Inc. Becker, Lawrence C. (1986), Reciprocity. New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Bendapudi, Neeli and Robert P. Leone (2003), "Psychological Implications of Customer Participation in Co-Production," Journal of Marketing, 67 (1), 14-28. Berkowitz, Leonard (1973), "Reactance and the unwillingness to help others," Psychological Bulletin, 79 (5), 310-17. Berry, Leonard L. (1995), "Relationship Marketing of Services--Growing Interest, Emerging Perspectives," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 23 (4), 236-45. Bolton, Gary E., Jordi Brandts, and Axel Ockenfels (1998), "Measuring Motivations for the Reciprocal Responses Observed in a Simple Dilemma Game," Experimental Economics, 1 (3), 207-19. Bolton, Gary E. and Axel Ockenfels (2000), "ERC: A theory of equity, reciprocity, and competition," The American Economic Review, 90 (1), 166.

Page 201: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

188

Bowman, Douglas and Das Narayandas (2004), "Linking Customer Management Effort to Customer Profitability in Business Markets," Journal of Marketing Research 41 (4), 433-47. Brehm, Jack W. and Ann H. Cole (1966), "Effect of a favor which reduces freedom," Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 3 (4), 420-26. Bucklin, Louis P. (1973), "A Theory of Channel Control," Journal of Marketing, 37 (1), 39-47. Caceres, Ruben Chumpitaz and Nicholas G. Paparoidamis (2007), "Service quality, relationship satisfaction, trust, commitment and business-to-business loyalty," European Journal of Marketing, 41 (7/8), 836-67. Cannon, Joseph P. and William D. Perreault Jr (1999), "Buyer-Seller Relationships in Business Markets," Journal of Marketing Research, 36 (4), 439-60. Carlsmith, Kevin M., John M. Darley, and Paul H. Robinson (2002), "Why Do We Punish? Deterrence and Just Deserts as Motives for Punishment," Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 83 (2), 284-99. Chen, Ya-Ru, Xiao-Ping Chen, and Rebecca Portnoy (2009), "To whom do positive norm and negative norm of reciprocity apply? Effects of inequitable offer, relationship, and relational-self orientation," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45 (1), 24-34. Cialdini, Robert B. (2009), Influence: Science and Practice (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Eduction, Inc. Cialdini, Robert B. and Kelton V. L. Rhoads (2001), "Human Behavior and the Marketplace.," Marketing Research, 13 (3), 8-13. Clayson, Dennis E. (2004), "A Test of the Reciprocity Effect in the Student Evaluation of Instructors in Marketing Classes," Marketing Education Review, 14 (2), 11-21. Crosby, Lawrence A., Kenneth A. Evans, and Deborah Cowles (1990), "Relationship quality in services selling: An interpersonal influence perspective," Journal of Marketing, 54 (3), 68. Dahl, Darren W., Heather Honea, and Rajesh V. Manchanda (2005), "Three Rs of Interpersonal Consumer Guilt: Relationship, Reciprocity, Reparation," Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15 (4), 307-15. Dawson, Scott (1988), "Four Motivations for Charitable Giving: Implications for Marketing Strategy to Attract Monetary Donations for Medical Research," Journal of Health Care Marketing, 8 (2), 31-37. De Wulf, Kristof, Gaby Odekerken-Schroder, and Dawn Iacobucci (2001), "Investments in consumer relationships: A cross-country and cross-industry exploration," Journal of Marketing, 65 (4), 33-51.

Page 202: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

189

Doney, Patricia M. and Joseph P. Cannon (1997), "An examination of the nature of trust in buyer-seller relationships," Journal of Marketing, 61 (2), 35. Dwyer, F. Robert, Paul H. Schurr, and Sejo Oh (1987), "Developing Buyer-Seller Relationships," Journal of Marketing, 51 (2). Eder, Paul, Karl Aquino, Carl Turner, and Americus Reed (2006), "Punishing Those Responsible for the Prison Abuses at Abu Ghraib: The Influence of the Negative Reciprocity Norm (NRN)," Political Psychology, 27 (6), 807-21. Eisenberger, Robert, Norman Cotterell, and Joan Marvel (1987), "Reciprocation Ideology," Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 53 (4), 743-50. Eisenberger, Robert, Patrick Lynch, Justin Aselage, and Stephanie Rohdieck (2004), Who Takes the Most Revenge? Individual Differences in Negative Reciprocity Norm Endorsement: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Vol 30(6) Jun 2004, 789-799. El-Ansary, A. and L. W. Stern (1972), "Power and measurement in the distribution channel," Journal of Marketing Research, 9, 47-52. Emerson, Richard M. (1962), "Power-Dependence Relations," American Sociological Review, 27 (1), 31-41. Falk, Armin, Simon Gächter, and Judit Kovács (1999), "Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic incentives in a repeated game with incomplete contracts," Journal of Economic Psychology, 20 (3), 251-84. Fang, Eric, Robert W. Palmatier, Lisa K. Scheer, and Ning Li (2008), "Trust at Different Organizational Levels," Journal of Marketing, 72 (2), 80-98. Fehr, Ernst and Simon Gachter (2000), "Fairness and retaliation: The economics of reciprocity," The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 14 (3), 159. ---- (1998), "Reciprocity and economics: The economic implications of Homo Reciprocans," European Economic Review, 42 (3-5), 845. Finney, F. Robert (1978), "Reciprocity: Gone But Not Forgotten," Journal of Marketing, 42 (1), 54-59. Frazier, Gary L. (1983), "Interorganizational Exchange Behavior in Marketing Channels: A Broadened Perspective," Journal of Marketing, 47 (4), 68-78. Frazier, Gary L., James D. Gill, and Sudhir H. Kale (1989), "Dealer Dependence Levels and Reciprocal Actions in a Channel of Distribution in a Developing Country," Journal of Marketing, 53 (1), 50-69.

Page 203: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

190

Frazier, Gary L. and Raymond C. Rody (1991), "The use of influence strategies in interfirm relationships in industrial product channels," Journal of Marketing, 55 (1), 52. Frazier, Gary L. and John O. Summers (1984), "Interfirm Influence Strategies and Their Applications within Distribution Channels," Journal of Marketing, 48 (3), 43-55. ---- (1986), "Perceptions of Interfirm Power and Its Use Within a Franchise Channel of Distribution," Journal of Marketing Research, 23 (2), 169-76. Frenzen, Jonathan K. and Harry L. Davis (1990), "Purchasing Behavior in Embedded Markets," Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (1), 1-12. Gaski, John F. (1984), "The Theory of Power and Conflict in Channels of Distribution," Journal of Marketing, 48 (3), 9-29. Gouldner, Alvin W. (1960), "The Norm of Reciprocity: A Preliminary Statement," American Sociological Review, 25 (2), 161-78. Greenglass, Esther R. (1969), "Effects of prior help and hindrance on willingness to help another: Reciprocity or social responsibility," Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 11 (3), 224-31. Gronroos, Christian (1999), "Relationship Marketing: Challenges for the Organization," Journal of Business Research, 46 (3), 327-35. Gundlach, Gregory T. and Ernest R. Cadotte (1994), "Exchange Interdependence and Interfirm Interaction: Research in a Simulated Channel Setting," Journal of Marketing Research, 31 (4), 516-32. Heide, Jan B. and George John (1992), "Do norms matter in marketing relationships?," Journal of Marketing, 56 (2), 32. Hibbard, Jonathan D., Nirmalya Kumar, and Louis W. Stern (2001), "Examining the Impact of Destructive Acts in Marketing Channel Relationships," Journal of Marketing Research, 38 (1), 45-61. Houston, Franklin S. and Jule B. Gassenheimer (1987), "Marketing and Exchange," Journal of Marketing, 51 (4), 3-18. Jap, Sandy D. (1999), "Pie-expansion efforts: Collaboration processes in buyer-supplier relationships," Journal of Marketing Research, 36 (4), 461-75. Jap, Sandy D. and Shankar Ganesan (2000), "Control Mechanisms and the Relationship Life Cycle: Implications for Safeguarding Specific Investments and Developing Commitment," Journal of Marketing Research 37 (2), 227-45.

Page 204: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

191

Jap, Sandy D., Chris Manolis, and Barton A. Weitz (1999), "Relationship Quality and Buyer-Seller interactions in Channels of Distribution," Journal of Business Research, 46 (3), 303-13. Johnson, Jean L. and Ravipreet S. Sohi (2001), "The influence of firm predispositions on interfirm relationship formation in business markets," International Journal of Research in Marketing, 18 (4), 299-318. Keysar, Boaz, Benjamin A. Converse, Wang Jiunwen, and Nicholas Epley (2008), "Reciprocity Is Not Give and Take: Asymmetric Reciprocity to Positive and Negative Acts," Psychological Science, 19 (12), 1280-86. Kumar, Nirmalya, Lisa K. Scheer, and Jan-Benedict E. M. Steenkamp (1995a), "The Effects of Perceived Interdependence on Dealer Attitudes," Journal of Marketing Research 32 (3), 348-56. ---- (1995b), "The effects of supplier fairness on vulnerable resellers," Journal of Marketing Research, 32 (1), 54-65. ---- (1998), "Interdependence, Punitive Capability, and the Reciprocation of Punitive Actions in Channel Relationships," Journal of Marketing Research 35 (2), 225-35. Lawler, Edward J. and Samuel B. Bacharach (1987), "Comparison of Dependence and Punitive Forms of Power," Social Forces, 66 (2), 446. Lee, Dong-Jin, Insik Jeong, Hyoung Tark Lee, and Hye Jin Sung (2008), "Developing a model of reciprocity in the importer–exporter relationship: The relative efficacy of economic versus social factors," Industrial Marketing Management, 37 (1), 9-22. Lerner, Melvin J. and Rosemary R. Lichtman (1968), "Effects of Perceived Norms on Attitudes and Altruistic Behavior Toward a Dependent Other," Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 9 (3), 226-32. Leventhal, Gerald S., Thomas Weiss, and Gary Long (1969), "Equity, reciprocity, and reallocating rewards in the dyad," Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 13 (4), 300-05. Li, Zhan G. and Rajiv P. Dant (1997), "An Exploratory Study of Exclusive Dealing in Channel Relationships," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 25 (3), 201. Loch, Christoph H. and Yaozhong Wu (2008), "Social Preferences and Supply Chain Performance: An Experimental Study," Management Science, 54 (11), 1835-49. Logan, Charles H. and Gerald G. Gaes (1993), "Meta-Analysis and the Rehabilitation of Punishment," Justice Quarterly, 10 (2), 245-64. Macneil, Ian R. (1986), "Exchange Revisited: Individual Utility and Social Solidarity," Ethics, 96 (3), 567-93.

Page 205: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

192

---- (1980), The New Social Contract. Binghampton, N.Y.: Yale University Press. Marsh, Lawrence C. and David R. Cormier (2002), Spline Regression Models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Michaels, James W. (1983), "Equity and Reciprocity Motivations as a Function of the Intentionality of Underpayment," Journal of Psychology, 114 (2), 179. Miller, Nancy J. and Rita C. Kean (1997), "Reciprocal Exchange in Rural Communities: Consumers' Inducements to Inshop," Psychology & Marketing, 14 (7), 637-61. Morgan, Robert M. and Shelby D. Hunt (1994), "The Commitment-Trust Theory of Relationship Marketing," Journal of Marketing, 58 (3), 20. Moyer, Reed (1970), "Reciprocity: Retrospect and Prospect," Journal of Marketing, 34 (4), 47-54. Palmatier, Robert W. (2008), Relationship Marketing. Cambridge, Mass.: Marketing Science Institute. Palmatier, Robert W., Rajiv P. Dant, Dhruv Grewal, and Kenneth R. Evans (2006a), "Factors Influencing the Effectiveness of Relationship Marketing: A Meta-Analysis," Journal of Marketing, 70 (4), 136-53. Palmatier, Robert W., Srinath Gopalakrishna, and Mark B. Houston (2006b), "Returns on Business-to-Business Relationship Marketing Investments: Strategies for Leveraging Profits," Marketing Science, 25 (5), 477-93. Palmatier, Robert W., Cheryl Burke Jarvis, Jennifer R. Beckhkoff, and Frank R. Kardes (2009), "The Role of Customer Gratitude in Relationship Marketing," Journal of Marketing, Forthcoming. Palmatier, Robert W., Lisa K. Scheer, Kenneth R. Evans, and Todd J. Arnold (2008), "Achieving relationship marketing effectiveness in business-to-business exchanges," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 36 (2), 174-90. Palmatier, Robert W., Lisa K. Scheer, Mark B. Houston, Kenneth R. Evans, and Srinath Gopalakrishna (2007a), "Use of relationship marketing programs in building customer-salesperson and customer-firm relationships: Differential influences on financial outcomes," International Journal of Research in Marketing, 24 (3), 210-23. Palmatier, Robert W., Lisa K. Scheer, and Jan-Benedict E. M. Steenkamp (2007b), "Customer Loyalty to Whom? Managing the Benefits and Risks of Salesperson-Owned Loyalty," Journal of Marketing Research, 44 (2), 185-99. Palmer, Adrian (2002), "Cooperative marketing associations: an investigation into the causes of effectiveness," Journal of Strategic Marketing, 10 (2), 135-56.

Page 206: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

193

Perugini, Marco , Marcello Gallucci, Fabio Presaghi, and Anna Paola Ercolani (2003), "The personal norm of reciprocity," European Journal of Personality, 17 (4), 251-83. Pervan, Simon J., Liliana L. Bove, and Lester W. Johnson (2009), "Reciprocity as a key stabilizing norm of interpersonal marketing relationships: Scale development and validation," Industrial Marketing Management, 38 (1), 60-70. Pervan, Simon J. and Lester W. Johnson (2003), "Understanding Process in Relationship Marketing: A Focus on Reciprocity," Journal of Customer Behaviour, 2, 85-109. Ping, Robert A. Jr. (2003), "Antecedents of satisfaction in a marketing channel," Journal of Retailing, 79 (4), 237. Rabin, Matthew (1993), "Incorporating fairness into game theory and economics," The American Economic Review, 83 (5), 1281. Reynolds, Kristy E. and Sharon E. Beatty (1999), "Customer benefits and company consequences of customer-salesperson relationships in retailing," Journal of Retailing, 75 (1), 11. Rynning, M. (1989), "Reciprocity in a Gift-Giving Situation," Journal of Social Psychology, 129 (6), 769. Scheer, Lisa K., C. Fred Miao, and Jason Garrett (2008a), "The Effects of Supplier Capabilities on Industrial Customers' Loyalty: The Role of Dependence," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Forthcoming. Scheer, Lisa K., Robert W. Palmatier, and C. Fred Miao (2008b), "Rethinking the Role and Nature of Dependence in Marketing Relationships - A Meta Analysis." ---- (2009), "Rethinking the Role and Nature of Dependence in Marketing Relationships - A Meta Analysis." Schopler, John and Vaida D. Thompson (1968), "Role of attribution processes in mediating amount of reciprocity for a favor," Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 10 (3), 243-50. Sethi, Rajiv and E. Somanathan (2003), "Understanding reciprocity," Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 50 (1), 1. Siguaw, Judy A., Penny M. Simpson, and Thomas L. Baker (1998), "Effects of Supplier Market Orientation on Distributor Market Orientation and the Channel Relationship: The Distributor Perspective," The Journal of Marketing, 62 (3), 99-111. Stern, Louis W. (1967), "The Concept of Channel Control," Journal of Retailing, 43 (2), 14. Stern, Louis W. and Torger Reve (1980), "Distribution Channels as Political Economies: A Framework for Comparative Analsysis," Journal of Marketing, 44 (3), 52-64.

Page 207: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

194

Thibaut, John W. and Harold H. Kelley (1961), The Social Psychology of Groups. New York: John Wiley & Sons, INC. Weitz, Barton A. and Sandy D. Jap (1995), "Relationship Marketing and Distribution Channels," Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 23 (4), 305-20. Wikipedia (2009), "Ethic of Reciprocity," Vol. 2009: Wikipedia. Williamson, Oliver E. (1996), The Mechanisms of Governance. New York: Oxford University Press. ---- (2007), "Transaction Cost Economics: The Process of Theory Development," in Great Minds in Management, Ken G Smith and Michael A. Hitt, Eds. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Zou, Shaoming and S. Tamer Cavusgil (2002), "The GMS: A Broad Conceptualization of Global Marketing Strategy and Its Effect on Firm Performance," Journal of Marketing, 66 (4), 40-56.

Page 208: RECIPROCITY IN MARKETING RELATIONSHIPS A Dissertation

195

VITA

Donald J. Lund was born on July 19, 1973 in Chicago, Illinois. He received both his

Bachelor’s of Science in Psychology, and his MBA with a focus in finance, from Missouri State

University. Prior to earning his Ph.D. degree from the University of Missouri in Business

Administration with an emphasis on marketing, he had 10 years of experience working in

numerous capacities for Domino’s Pizza. After managing multiple Domino’s stores in Southwest

Missouri, he built 3 Domino’s stores in Madison, Wisconsin. He has already joined the

University of Alabama – Birmingham as an assistant professor of Marketing.