read the complete article [pdf 5 mb] - · pdf file3d t-tests were conducted after talairach...

11
www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Research Report Motor imagery-based brain activity parallels that of motor execution: Evidence from magnetic source imaging of cortical oscillations Sarah Kraeutner a,b , Alicia Gionfriddo a,c , Timothy Bardouille c,d , Shaun Boe a,b,c,n a Laboratory for Brain Recovery and Function, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada b Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada c School of Physiotherapy, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada d Biomedical Translational Imaging Centre (BIOTIC), IWK Health Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada article info Article history: Accepted 1 September 2014 Available online 22 September 2014 Keywords: Magnetoencephalography Motor imagery Motor learning Neuroimaging abstract Motor imagery (MI) is a form of practice in which an individual mentally performs a motor task. Previous research suggests that skill acquisition via MI is facilitated by repetitive activation of brain regions in the sensorimotor network similar to that of motor execution, however this evidence is conicting. Further, many studies do not control for overt muscle activity and thus the activation patterns reported for MI may be driven in part by actual movement. The purpose of the current research is to further establish MI as a secondary modality of skill acquisition by providing electrophysiological evidence of an overlap between brain areas recruited for motor execution and imagery. Non-disabled participants (N¼ 18; 24.773.8 years) performed both execution and imagery of a unilateral sequence button-press task. Magnetoencephalography (MEG) was utilized to capture neural activity, while electromyography used to rigorously monitor muscle activity. Event-related syn- chronization/desynchronization (ERS/ERD) analysis was conducted in the beta frequency band (1530 Hz). Whole head dual-state beamformer analysis was applied to MEG data and 3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity as ME, including activation of contralateral primary motor and somatosensory cortices. However, this activation is signicantly less intense during MI (po0.05). As well, activation during ME was more lateralized (i.e., within the contralateral hemisphere). These results conrm that ME and MI have similar spatial activation patterns. Thus, the current research provides direct electrophysiological evi- dence to further establish MI as a secondary form of skill acquisition. & 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.001 0006-8993/& 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. n Correspondence to: School of Physiotherapy, Dalhousie University, Rm 407, 4th Floor Forrest Building, 5869 University Avenue, PO Box 15000, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada, B3H 4R2. Fax: þ1 902 494 1941. E-mail address: [email protected] (S. Boe). brainresearch 1588 (2014)81–91

Upload: doanduong

Post on 16-Feb-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 1

http://dx.doi.org/100006-8993/& 2014 El

nCorrespondencePO Box 15000, Halif

E-mail address: s

Research Report

Motor imagery-based brain activity parallels thatof motor execution: Evidence from magnetic sourceimaging of cortical oscillations

Sarah Kraeutnera,b, Alicia Gionfriddoa,c, Timothy Bardouillec,d,Shaun Boea,b,c,n

aLaboratory for Brain Recovery and Function, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, CanadabDepartment of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, CanadacSchool of Physiotherapy, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, CanadadBiomedical Translational Imaging Centre (BIOTIC), IWK Health Sciences Centre, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:

Accepted 1 September 2014

Motor imagery (MI) is a form of practice in which an individual mentally performs a motor

task. Previous research suggests that skill acquisition via MI is facilitated by repetitive

Available online 22 September 2014

Keywords:

Magnetoencephalography

Motor imagery

Motor learning

Neuroimaging

.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.00sevier B.V. All rights res

to: School of Physiotheraax, Nova Scotia, Canada,[email protected] (S. Boe).

a b s t r a c t

activation of brain regions in the sensorimotor network similar to that of motor execution,

however this evidence is conflicting. Further, many studies do not control for overt muscle

activity and thus the activation patterns reported for MI may be driven in part by actual

movement. The purpose of the current research is to further establish MI as a secondary

modality of skill acquisition by providing electrophysiological evidence of an overlap

between brain areas recruited for motor execution and imagery. Non-disabled participants

(N¼18; 24.773.8 years) performed both execution and imagery of a unilateral sequence

button-press task. Magnetoencephalography (MEG) was utilized to capture neural activity,

while electromyography used to rigorously monitor muscle activity. Event-related syn-

chronization/desynchronization (ERS/ERD) analysis was conducted in the beta frequency

band (15–30 Hz). Whole head dual-state beamformer analysis was applied to MEG data and

3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that

MI has similar patterns of spatial activity as ME, including activation of contralateral

primary motor and somatosensory cortices. However, this activation is significantly less

intense during MI (po0.05). As well, activation during ME was more lateralized (i.e., within

the contralateral hemisphere). These results confirm that ME and MI have similar spatial

activation patterns. Thus, the current research provides direct electrophysiological evi-

dence to further establish MI as a secondary form of skill acquisition.

& 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1erved.

py, Dalhousie University, Rm 407, 4th Floor Forrest Building, 5869 University Avenue,B3H 4R2. Fax: þ1 902 494 1941.

Page 2: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 182

1. Introduction

Motor learning is the process of acquiring or strengthening askill through repetitive practice and the provision of feedback(Newell, 1991). Motor learning occurs via plastic changes inthe brain that are driven by this repetitive practice. Whilephysical practice is recognized as the ideal vehicle to drivebrain plasticity and thus motor learning, other forms ofpractice have been shown to facilitate skill acquisition. Motorimagery (MI) is a form of practice in which an individualmentally rehearses a motor task (Jeannerod, 1995, 2001).Motor imagery can take two forms, including first person orkinaesthetic imagery (i.e., imagining from “behind their owneyes” Munzert and Zentgraf, 2009), or third person visualimagery (i.e., imagining someone else performing the move-ment). Secondary to physical practice, MI has been used as aform of skill acquisition in numerous domains includingsurgery, sport, and music (Wulf et al., 2010; Moran et al.,2012). Motor imagery is suggested to facilitate skill acquisitionin a manner similar to physical practice; that is plastic changesin the brain occur as a result of repetitive mental practice(Grezes and Decety, 2001; Miller et al., 2010; Schuster et al.,2011). The purported similarity in brain activity between MI andphysical practice has led to an increased interest in the use ofMI as an adjunct to physical practice in neurorehabilitation,particularly with regard to the treatment of upper limb impair-ment post-stroke (Sharma et al., 2009). If MI is to be used as anadjunct to physical practice to facilitate skill acquisition asoutlined above, it is critical that similar brain regions areactivated in both mental and physical practice.

At present, it remains unclear whether the neural corre-lates that underlie MI parallel those observed for physicalpractice (Grezes and Decety, 2001; Hétu et al., 2013), which werefer to throughout as motor execution (ME). In non-disabledindividuals, the majority of neuroimaging studies haveassessed task-related activation during MI using functionalmagnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (Grezes and Decety, 2001;Hétu et al., 2013). Studies utilizing fMRI have largely deter-mined that MI engages brain areas that overlap with ME,including the premotor (PMC), cingulate, and parietal cortices(Porro et al., 1996; Hanakawa et al., 2003; De Lange et al. 2008),however, this activation may be influenced by the type(kinaesthetic vs. visual) of MI employed (Guillot et al., 2009;Hétu et al., 2013). The primary motor cortex (referred tothroughout as ‘motor cortex’) has also been proposed to playa critical role in MI, especially when employing kinaestheticMI, though activation of the motor cortex during MI has notbeen consistently reported and thus has yet to be verified(De Lange et al., 2008; Hétu et al., 2013). In fact, a recent meta-analysis by Hétu et al. (2013) examining the brain regions thatunderlie MI concluded that while similar cortical areas wererecruited across the studies, only 22 of 75 studies reportedactivation of the motor cortex. Furthermore, whether MI alsorecruits ventral and dorsal PMC (vPMC and dPMC, respec-tively), which are implicated in the preparation and guidanceof movement (Rizzolatti et al., 1996; Binkofski et al., 2000),is conflicting in the literature (Lotze and Halsband, 2006;Munzert et al., 2009). Thus, due to the potential differencesin areas recruited between ME and MI, MI is suggested to rely

on a more widespread neural network in comparison with ME(Burianová et al., 2013).

Methodological approaches utilized in examining brainactivity during MI may also contribute to variability in theresults observed. In their meta-analysis, Hétu et al. (2013)reported that only two of 75 studies used electromyography(EMG) in addition to visual inspection to control for overtmuscle activity. Thus, the resemblance in activation betweenMI and ME observed in many of these previous studies might bedriven by brain activity associated with actual execution (i.e.,cortical output to lower motor neurons). As such, it remainsunclear whether ‘pure’ MI elicits the same spatial patterns asME. Furthermore, the majority of studies examining brainactivity underlying MI utilize either fMRI or positron emissiontomography (PET) (Hétu et al., 2013), both of which rely onindirect measures of brain activity with low temporal resolution(Sutton et al., 2009; Cumming, 2014). Accordingly, these mea-sures provide rich spatial information, but are limited in theirability to directly measure electrophysiological activity.

Unlike fMRI and PET, electroencephalography (EEG) providesa direct measure of brain activity with high temporal resolution(Niedermeyer, 1996). Analysis of EEG data during a sustainedtask (such as MI) can reveal changes in brain activity byassessing an increase or decrease in the magnitude of ongoingcortical oscillations, known as event-related synchronizationand desynchronization (ERS/ERD) (Pfurtscheller and Lopes daSilva, 1999; Schoffelen and Gross, 2009). For example, EEGstudies have shown that ERD occurs over contralateral sensor-imotor areas in the beta frequency band (15–30 Hz) during MIand motor preparation and execution (Pfurtscheller andNeuper, 1997; Neuper et al., 2006; Formaggio et al., 2010). EEGis limited however in its ability to localize the underlyingsources of beta ERD during MI and ME due mainly to spatialsmearing of the electric potentials (Niedermeyer, 1996).

Similar to EEG, magnetoencephalography (MEG) obtains a

direct measure of brain activity (Baillet et al., 2001). Unlike

EEG however, MEG features considerably better spatial reso-

lution (on the order of millimetres; Baillet et al., 2001) that

provides the ability to more precisely identify source-level

activity in the brain (Baillet et al., 2001; Schoffelen and Gross,

2009; Hari et al., 2010). For instance, Burianová et al. (2013)

sought to identify the neural correlates of actual and ima-

gined finger movements using both MEG and fMRI. In agree-

ment with previous research, there was considerable overlap

between the brain areas activated during MI and ME for both

the MEG and fMRI data. Differences between MI and ME were

noted however, with activity observed in brain areas crucial

for visuospatial processing (e.g. left inferior parietal lobule,

parahippocampus, right superior temporal gyrus and super-

ior frontal gyrus) but not in areas related to somatosensory

coordination during the MI condition. While this work capi-

talized on the ability of MEG to measure brain activity

associated with MI with high temporal and spatial resolution,

it is one of only a few studies to do so (Nakagawa et al., 2011;

Di Rienzo et al., 2014). Moreover, this prior work examined

brain activity associated with a simple motor task that was

limited to flexion and extension movements of the fingers

(Burianová et al., 2013). In light of the proposed use of MI to

facilitate skill acquisition, ideally our understanding of how

Page 3: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 1 83

brain activity underlying MI parallels that of ME would bederived from a more complex, skilled movement.

The current study seeks to extend the literature examin-ing the neural correlates of MI by measuring electrophysio-logical activity at the source level during the performance of askilled motor task, and during imagery of the same task.By monitoring muscle activity throughout, brain activity willbe isolated to MI only. We hypothesize that similar patternsof activity in MI and ME will be observed, including activity incontralateral motor cortex and posterior parietal areas. Estab-lishing that spatial patterns of brain activation during rigor-ously controlled MI overlap with those of ME will providefurther support for its use as an adjunct to physical practicein facilitating skill acquisition.

Fig. 1 – Group averaged source-level ERS/ERD analysis ofmotor execution overlaid on a template brain. Areas ofsignificant activation (po0.05) were determined from 3dt-tests of motor vs. rest blocks and included contralateralM1 and S1.

2. Results

2.1. Behavioural results

A significant difference (po0.05) was observed for the numberof correct sequences between block 1 and block 2 of ME(37.3712.3 and 47.1713.4, respectively).

2.2. EMG results

Two participant's data was excluded from further analysesbecause less than 33% of MI trials were free from muscleactivity. For the remaining 16 participants, a total of 144 trialswere discarded, which corresponded to 30.0% of the totalnumber of trials performed (480). Of note, at least 1 trial perparticipant was excluded from analysis, with an average of4.572.9 trials excluded during each block (Supplementaryinformation Table 1).

2.3. MEG results

Group-level beamformer maps of ME vs. rest (Fig. 1) showedbeta ERD within the contralateral hemisphere at the motorcortex, primary somatosensory cortex (S1), inferior parietallobule (IPL), cingulate, and precuneus. Within the ipsilateralhemisphere, beta ERD encompassed the inferior frontal gyrus(IFG), cingulate, and precuneus. Maximum activation, defined asthe peak voxel within a spatially separate cluster, was localizedto four distinct areas (refer to Table 1 for peak maxima). Group-level beamformer maps of MI vs. rest (Fig. 2) showed beta ERDwithin the contralateral hemisphere at themotor cortex, S1, IPL,cingulate, and cuneus (Fig. 2). Within the ipsilateral hemi-sphere, beta ERD encompassed the premotor cortex (PMC;including SMA), IPL, precuneus, cingulate, and cuneus (Fig. 2).Maximum activation was localized to nine distinct areas (referto Table 1 for peak maxima). Beta ERS was also observedbilaterally at the primary visual cortices during MI. Statisticaltesting between blocks 1 and 2 were not significantly differentwithin either the ME or MI condition.

2.4. Motor imagery vs. execution

Statistical testing between conditions identified six regionsthat significantly differed in activation between MI and ME

(Fig. 3). Contralateral motor cortex and S1 showed greater ERDduring ME than during MI, although these regions exhibitedERD during both conditions (Fig. 3). Thus, activity during MEwas stronger in the contralateral hemisphere than during MI.A significant difference in ERD was observed in the contral-ateral precuneus due to the absence of activation during MI.Similarly, a significant difference between conditions wasobserved in bilateral ERD of the cuneus and bilateral ERS ofV1 due to the absence of activation during ME. Of note, nosignificant difference in IPL or premotor activation (i.e., SMAor Brodmann area 6) was observed between conditions. Thepeaks of greatest difference between MI and ME were loca-lized to five distinct areas within the motor cortex, IPL, andV1 (Table 1).

2.5. Time–frequency response analysis

Time–frequency response plots were generated for the motorcortex, S1, IPL, and premotor cortices to investigate thetemporal dynamics of beta ERS/ERD at regions of interest(motor cortex and IPL are shown in Fig. 4). ERD was present incontralateral motor cortex and S1 during both MI and MEfollowing the ‘Go’ cue, however ERD strength peaked laterduring MI while remaining consistently activated during ME.ERD was also observed in ipsilateral PMC and contralateralIPL during both ME and MI, with beta ERD onset occurringlater during MI. In contrast with ME, a distinct beta reboundwas not observed during MI in the above regions. To inves-tigate changes in ERD across the session, time–frequency

Page 4: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

Table 1 – Talairach coordinates of peak maxima for brain regions activated during motor execution (ME) and motor imagery(MI) vs. rest, and for brain regions with significantly different activation in ME vs. MI.

Condition Lateralization Talairach coordinates Brodmann Focus point

x y z

ME Contralateral 4 �40 38 4 Right middle cingulateContralateral 28 �28 46 4/3 Right post-central gyrusContralateral 32 �24 38 4/3 Right post-central gyrusIpsilateral �32 �36 �34 3 Left inferior parietal lobule

MI Contralateral 12 �84 6 17 Right calcarine gyrusContralateral 36 �8 30 3 Right pre-centralContralateral 52 �20 50 1/6 Right post-centralIpsilateral �12 �88 �2 17 Left calcarine gyrusIpsilateral �24 �48 34 47 Left inferior parietal lobuleIpsilateral �24 �40 22 47 Left inferior frontal gyrusIpsilateral �24 �12 22 11 Left inferior frontal gyrusIpsilateral �28 �40 42 2 Left post-centralIpsilateral �36 �32 34 2/3 Left inferior parietal lobule

ME vs. MI Contralateral 32 �28 46 4 Right post-central/pre-centralMidline 4 �88 18 17/18 Superior occipital gyrusIpsilateral �12 �32 54 4 Left paracentral/post-centralIpsilateral �28 �36 34 47 Left inferior parietal lobuleIpsilateral �32 �24 54 4/6 Left pre-central

Fig. 2 – Group averaged source-level ERS/ERD analysis ofmotor imagery overlaid on a template brain. Areas ofsignificant activation (po0.05) were determined from 3dt-tests of imagery vs. rest blocks and included contralateralM1, S1 and IPL, as well as ipsilateral PMC.

Fig. 3 – Group averaged source-level ERS/ERD comparison ofmotor execution and motor imagery overlaid on a templatebrain. Significant differences in activation (po0.05) weredetermined from 3d t-tests of execution vs. imagery blocksand included differences in contralateral M1 and S1.

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 184

response plots were generated for the above noted regions forblock 1 and block 2 of each condition. ERD was observed toincrease from block 1 to block 2 during both ME and MI (motorcortex and PMC are shown in Fig. 5).

2.6. MI trial comparison

Statistical testing was conducted between whole head activa-tion maps generated for ‘contaminated MI’ and ‘pure MI’(Fig. 6). Notable differences in activation were observedbetween the contaminated and pure MI, including strongerERD within contralateral motor cortex, weaker ERD withincontralateral S1 and bilateral cingulate, an absence of activitywithin bilateral cuneus, contralateral IPL, ipsilateral precuneus

Page 5: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

Fig. 4 – Group averaged time–frequency response plots depicting beta band source-level ERS/ERD of motor execution andmotor imagery averaged across all trials in contralateral M1 (A) and IPL (B). ERD is observed in both conditions following the“Go” cue (0s), however, the strength of the ERD is greater during motor execution.

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 1 85

and PMC, and the presence of ERD within the contralateralsuperior parietal lobule.

3. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to establish an overlap inpatterns of brain activity between MI and ME to furthersupport the use of MI as an adjunct to physical practice infacilitating skill acquisition. Direct electrophysiological evi-dence was provided to show that similar brain regions areactive during MI and ME of the same task. The removal of MItrials in which muscle activity was present ensured thiscomparison targets brain activity resulting from MI only.

3.1. ERS/ERD findings

In agreement with previous studies that employed indirectmeasures of brain activity (i.e. fMRI and PET), similar brainregions were recruited during MI and ME (Grezes and Decety,2001; Hanakawa et al., 2003; Hétu et al., 2013) includingcontralateral motor cortex, S1 and IPL, ipsilateral precuneus,and bilateral cingulate gyri. These regions have been shownto be involved in action planning and preparation, actionguidance, visuospatial processing, and actual execution(Rizzolatti et al., 1996; Rizzolatti et al., 1998; Tunik et al., 2007).

As indicated previously, ERD has been shown to occurin contralateral sensorimotor areas in the beta band(Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva, 1999; Schoffelen andGross, 2009) during both execution and imagination of move-ment. As beta oscillatory activity is inhibitory, ERD in thisfrequency band is generally thought to be representative ofbrain activation (Pfurtscheller and Neuper, 1997; Pfurtscheller

and Lopes da Silva, 1999). Although our findings show thatbrain regions recruited for MI overlap with those recruited forME, there were five notable differences in the functionalmaps between MI and ME. First, ME activation was morelateralized to the contralateral hemisphere. Specifically,while consistent group-level activation of the motor cortexand S1 were found during MI, the ERD signal displayed a lateronset and was smaller during MI in comparison with ME.Taken with our rigorous control for overt muscle activity, thisresult supports motor cortex activation in the absence ofactual movement. Secondly, in agreement with previousresearch (Burianová et al., 2013), the pattern of activationwas more widespread during MI, suggesting a larger networkof brain regions underlies MI performance. In addition to theoverlapping areas recruited in both conditions, ipsilateralPMC, IPL, and cuneus were additionally recruited during MI.The activation of these areas during MI only is sensible as MIrelies on a more widespread network, including heavierrecruitment of ipsilateral areas, and their functions parallelthose required for MI. For instance, these areas are implicatedin motor planning and attention, integration of visuospatialinformation and inhibitory control (Rushworth et al., 2003;Hoshi and Tanji, 2007; Grafton, 2009). Previous research hasshown increased involvement of these ipsilateral areas,specifically for novice imagers (Milton et al., 2007; Changet al., 2011). Lastly, an ERS signal was present only during theMI condition in V1, which has been previously shown tooccur when the eyes are closed (Barry et al., 2007).

It should be noted that over the single session examinedin the current work we observed minimal changes in both thepattern of brain activity and behavioural changes associatedwith skill acquisition. Given the single session of data as wellas the nature of the task, it follows that limited changes

Page 6: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

Fig. 5 – Group averaged time–frequency response plots depicting beta band source-level ERS/ERD during block 1 and block 2 ofmotor execution (ME) and motor imagery (MI) in contralateral M1 (A) and ipsilateral PMC (B). ERD observed following the “Go”cue (0s) is shown to increase from block 1 to block 2, however, the strength of the ERD is greater during ME.

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 186

associated with skill acquisition would have occurred(Lacourse et al., 2005). The overlap between brain regionsrecruited for ME and MI, shown here via a direct electro-physiological measure, suggests that repetitive performanceof this form of MI has the potential to drive plastic changes inthe brain similar to those that result from repetitive physicalpractice (Miller et al., 2010; Schuster et al., 2011). Thus, thecurrent study provides additional support for the use of MI asa secondary modality of skill acquisition. Although activationpatterns observed during MI were significantly less contral-ateral dominant and smaller in magnitude than those of ME,these findings may indicate that training is required to makeMI more effective in facilitating learning. A study by Ono et al.(2013) showed that accuracy and reproducibility of the ERDsignal increased, for the purposes of brain-computer

interfaces, over five sessions of MI training. The ERS/ERDpatterns reported in the present study are likely reflective of anovice level of MI proficiency. Thus, imagination of motortasks in our group may rely more on areas involved invisuospatial processing and response inhibition, as our find-ings show that the ERD signal during MI was more wide-spread and this pattern is reflected in novice vs. expertimagers (Milton et al., 2007; Chang et al., 2011). Secondly, asthe ERD signal was delayed and weaker during MI as com-pared to ME, activation of the motor cortex may become morerobust following training. This claim requires further studyfor confirmation. A study by Boe et al. (2014) demonstratedthat activation in the sensorimotor cortices lateralizes to thehemisphere contralateral to the limb ‘performing’ the taskover the course of three practice sessions. This modulation of

Page 7: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

Fig. 6 – Group averaged source-level ERS/ERD comparison ofwhole head spatial maps generated for ‘contaminated MI’(i.e., MI trials with EMG activity present), and ‘pure MI’(i.e., MI trials with no EMG activity present) overlaid on atemplate brain. Significant differences in activation (po0.05)were determined from 3d t-tests of ‘contaminated MI’ vs.‘pure MI’ trials and included differences in contralateral M1and IPL, and ipsilateral PMC.

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 1 87

brain activity was promoted using source-level neurofeed-back during MI practice. Reviewing the current findings inlight of longitudinal MI training studies, it seems that pat-terns of activation between MI and ME may change as one'sability to perform MI improves. Future work then shouldconsider the longitudinal effect of repeated MI sessions.

3.2. EMG monitoring

An important methodological consideration of the presentstudy was control of overt muscle activity via EMG. In theirscoping review, Hétu et al. (2013) reported that the vastmajority of studies either did not control for muscle activityor only controlled for motor-related activity via visual mon-itoring. As the latter method is sensitive only to large overtmovements, weak isometric contractions and any smallmovements that may occur during the performance of MImay go unnoticed. The result of not monitoring muscleactivity via EMG is that patterns of activity thought to resultfrom MI may be in part driven by actual execution (i.e.,muscle activity). To control for this confound, we rejectedany MI trials during which activity in the muscles relevant tothe task occurred. Indeed, a comparison of spatial mapsgenerated based on contaminated and pure MI resulted indifferent patterns of activation. In contrast with previousstudies, activation during MI in the present study was notfound in the superior parietal lobule, supramarginal gyrus,angular gyrus, fusiform gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, cerebel-lum, or putamen. The absence of activity in these regions inthe present study may be explained due to the more rigorouscontrol for muscle activity and thus, activation found in these

areas in previous studies may have been driven in part byactual muscle activity. As the majority of these areas, namelythe superior parietal lobule, supramarginal gyrus and angulargyrus, are involved in processing environmental cues andexternal spatial orientation (Corbetta et al., 2002; Corbettaand Shulman, 2002; Szczepanski and Kastner, 2009; Doricchiet al., 2010; Taylor and Thut, 2012), the absence of activity inthese areas in the present study may suggest that MI reliesmore heavily on motor planning and proprioception thanprocessing information regarding one's body in space (Rouniset al., 2007). Secondly, the cerebellum and putamen are bothimplicated in movement regulation and learning (Rizzolattiet al., 1996; Heitger et al., 2012; Hardwick et al., 2013). Thus,the lack of activation found in our study within these areasmay again reflect novice MI abilities.

A potential limitation of this study is related to theprovision of neurofeedback to half of the study participants.The data included in this study was generated as part of alarger study designed to assess the effectiveness of neuro-feedback on modulating brain activity over multiple sessionsof MI (Boe et al., 2014). The lack of a between-group difference(i.e., FB vs. control) in both activation pattern and behaviouraloutcome in this prior study provided justification for collap-sing the groups to address the purpose of the present study.Secondly, there is the possibility of an order effect on thebrain activation observed, as the conditions were not coun-terbalanced. The conditions were not counterbalanced inorder to minimize any influence of behavioural changeinduced by physical practice on activity observed during MI,as the objective of the study was to compare brain activityduring MI with that of ME.

4. Conclusions

The process of acquiring or strengthening a skill throughrepetitive practice is driven by underlying changes in thebrain (Newell, 1991; Grezes and Decety, 2001); thus it is criticalthat brain regions activated in mental practice overlap withthose activated in physical practice in order to use MI tofacilitate skill acquisition. The current study provides directelectrophysiological evidence that extends upon previousPET/fMRI findings in support of using MI as an adjunct tophysical practice to facilitate skill acquisition. Source-levelanalysis of MEG data allowed us to capture this directelectrophysiological evidence while preserving spatial resolu-tion in order to isolate specific regions of interest (Bailletet al., 2001; Schoffelen and Gross, 2009). Secondly, by mon-itoring muscle activity, we were able to determine brainregions activated by MI alone. In contrast with previousstudies, this study reports an absence of activity in areasinvolved in spatial cognition that are often observedduring ME.

MI has clear implications for use in conjunction withphysical practice to facilitate skill acquisition in both theclinical setting and for expert learning of fine motor skills(Sanders et al., 2008; Sharma et al., 2009; Keller, 2012).However, the current study suggests that imagination ofmotor tasks may initially rely more on areas involved invisuospatial processing and response inhibition, with less

Page 8: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 188

involvement of areas critical to spatial orientation. Thus, asactivation patterns between MI and ME may change with MItraining (Ono et al., 2013; Boe et al., 2014), a certain level of MIproficiency may be necessary to drive effective skill acquisi-tion. Future work should assess the effectiveness of MI in skillacquisition using a longitudinal design, and further elucidatethe dose of MI training required for effective skill acquisition.

5. Experimental procedures

5.1. Participants

Eighteen right handed participants (8 male, 24.773.8 years)agreed to partake in the study. All participants were free ofneurological disorders and each provided written, informedconsent. Prior to the onset of the study, participants werescreened for compatibility with MEG (e.g., magnetic artefacts)according to institutional procedure. Participants were part ofa larger study examining the effect of neurofeedback on MIperformance (Boe et al., 2014). Thus participants were ran-domly assigned to either a neurofeedback (FB) or controlgroup based on the order of recruitment using a tablegenerated prior to study onset. The study was conductedwith approval from the Research Ethics Board at the IWKHealth Centre.

5.2. Experimental task/paradigm

All participants began the experiment with a familiarizationsession. During this familiarization session, participantswatched a gender-matched video describing the type of MIto be performed (i.e., kinaesthetic) and the task to be per-formed/imagined. Kinaesthetic MI was chosen as this type ofMI is proposed to better facilitate basic motor skill learning inrehabilitation (Mulder, 2007). The task used was a sequentialbutton press paradigm performed with the non-dominant(left) hand. Briefly, a seven-digit sequence (4-2-3-1-3-4-2) wasperformed using a four-key response pad (Photon Control Inc.Burnaby, BC, Canada), with the numbers 1–4 representing theindex, middle, ring and little finger. The script accompanyingthe video emphasized the poly sensory aspects of MI, direct-ing the participants to attend to sensory information relatedto task performance (e.g., the feeling of the fingers moving upand down, and the clicking of buttons as they are pressed,etc.), which has been shown to facilitate MI performance(Braun et al., 2008). Following the video, participants observedthe button sequence, completed the sequence with visualcues, and finally completed the sequence with no visual cuesto establish equivalent task proficiency across participants.

The experimental session included two ten-minute ‘test’blocks and two ten-minute ‘MI’ blocks. Test blocks involvedactual performance of the task, and MI blocks involvedimagined performance of the task via MI. In all blocks,participants switched between rest and task/MI based onauditory cues provided in 10 s intervals. Before the first MIblock, a standardized script was read to the participant toreiterate instructions on how to perform MI. All participantswere instructed to close their eyes during MI, and open theireyes following the rest cue. Additionally, participants in the

FB group received and were instructed to utilize the neuro-feedback to assess their own performance during MI blocks.This neurofeedback was based on activity in bilateral sensor-imotor cortices, as reported previously (Boe et al., 2014).

5.3. Data acquisition

Neuroimaging data was collected using a 306 channel MEGsystem (Elekta Neuromag, FL). Prior to MEG scanning, threeanatomical landmarks (nasion and left/right pre-auricularpoints) and a 150–200 point head shape were digitized. EMGelectrodes were placed over the corresponding flexor andextensor muscles of the digits (anterior and posterior aspectsof the left forearm, respectively) using a bipolar configurationwith a 1 cm inter-electrode distance. The EMG signals wereused to ensure the absence of muscle activity during MI.The electrooculogram was also obtained using four electro-des, with one superior and one inferior to the left eye, andone just lateral to the left and right eye. An electrode attachedat the collarbone served as a ground. Additionally, four headposition indicator (HPI) coils were placed on the participant'shead, two on the forehead and one on each mastoid process.During scanning, HPI coils were activated continuously togenerate alternating magnetic fields at frequencies between293 and 321 Hz. Behavioural (i.e., response pad) data andevent markers indicating the timing of stimuli and responseswere also collected throughout. All data were acquired con-tinuously at a sampling rate of 1500 Hz and a bandwidth of0.1–500 Hz, and recorded to a file for analysis.

5.4. Data analysis

5.4.1. Behavioural analysisThe number of correct sequences was determined for eachindividual and then averaged across the group for block 1 andblock 2 of ME. A paired t-test was used in order to assesschanges in behaviour that occurred throughout the session.An alpha value of po0.05 denoted significance.

5.4.2. EMG analysisAnalysis of the EMG data obtained during MI was performedto identify and reject data segments that contained muscleactivity. The absence of activity in the left flexor and extensormuscles of the digits during MI for each trial was determinedby a comparison between the EMG envelope signal ampli-tudes in the ten seconds following the “Go” cue with the fourseconds prior to the cue (rest). A Fourier transform deter-mined the amplitude spectra of the EMG envelope for bothintervals. Across all participants, the strongest amplitudes inthe EMG envelope amplitude spectrum were observedbetween 25 and 100 Hz. A paired t-test was used to determineif there was a statistically significant difference in theamplitude spectra between MI and baseline in this frequencyrange in each trial (po0.05). Only participants that showed nosignificant increase in muscle activity from rest to MI in atleast 33% of trials were included in further analyses. For theremaining participants, trials for which one or both EMGchannels showed an increase in muscle activity from rest toMI intervals were excluded from further analysis. Theremaining data was then carried forward to the MEG analysis.

Page 9: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 1 89

5.4.3. MEG data analysisDual-state beamforming (Vrba et al., 2010) was applied towhole head MEG data to determine source level activationduring ME and MI (Diwakar et al., 2011). Head positionthroughout the scan was then calculated and checked foreach participant to ensure that displacement did not exceed5 mm or 31. Individual participant's functional MEG data wasthen co-registered to a template brain (TT_avg152T1) basedon the digitized anatomical landmarks and head shape, usingsoftware supplied by the MEG vendor (MRIlab). Prior to thebeamformer analysis, data were low-pass filtered and down-sampled (at 70 and 250 Hz, respectively). Following this, a 15–30 Hz band-pass filter was applied to the MEG data. For thepurposes of this analysis, ME/MI and rest intervals weredefined to be seven seconds of data: ME and MI beginningthree seconds following the cue and rest ending one secondbefore the cue. These intervals were chosen to avoid tran-sient brain responses at MI onset, and anticipatory responsesprior to the cue. Whole head dual-state beamformer (Vrbaet al., 2010) was then applied, with the resultant source-levelmaps representing the change in magnitude of the betarhythm (i.e., ERS/ERD) during MI or ME as compared to therespective rest interval for each participant. Source levelmaps for each participant were then transformed into theTalairach-Tournoux coordinate space using Analysis of Func-tional Neuroimaging (AFNI; Cox, 1996; Cox and Hyde, 1997)software, for the purposes of group analysis.

5.4.4. Group analysisGroup-level testing for statistically significant differencesbetween conditions was conducted in AFNI using an alphavalue of po0.05. 3D paired t-tests were performed to inves-tigate group-level patterns of activity during ME and MI.Specifically, the source level maps of beta ERS/ERD duringME for all subjects were tested for a significant differencefrom zero for each block as well as a combination across bothblocks. The same test was also completed for the source levelmaps of beta ERS/ERD during MI. Source level maps of betaERS/ERD during ME for all subjects were tested for a signifi-cant difference from source level maps of beta ERS/ERDduring MI for the combined blocks. Finally, to investigatechanges in brain activation across the session, source levelmaps of beta ERS/ERD generated for block 1 were tested forsignificant difference from source level maps of beta ERS/ERDfor block 2 for both ME and MI.

5.4.5. Time–frequency analysisTime–frequency response (TFR) plots were generated duringboth MI and ME in the beta band for regions of interest (ROIs)to show the time course of activity averaged across bothblocks, as well as for each separate block to investigatechanges of ERS/ERD throughout the session. ROIs wereselected based on previous literature and results from thegenerated source-level maps (Burianová et al., 2013; Hétuet al., 2013). The locations of the ROI were determined in eachparticipant's head coordinate frame using the inverse of theTalariach-Tournoux spatial transform determined by AFNI.Estimation of the time course of activity at these locationswas achieved using the so-called virtual electrode beamfor-mer (Vrba et al., 2010) construct applied to the beta band

filtered MEG data. Morlet wavelet analysis was applied usinga wavelet size of 128 samples and 32 frequency bins to thevirtual electrode data in the beta band after parsing each trial,with a time interval of 5 s prior to and 13 s after the onset cue.The first and last second of data were cropped to remove edgeeffects due to the wavelet transformation. Time–frequencyresponses were averaged across trials and then normalized tothe baseline interval (four seconds prior to the ‘Go’ cue).Finally, TFRs were averaged at a group level for each condi-tion to allow for a comparison of beta rhythm ERS/ERDbetween ME and MI.

5.4.6. MI trial comparisonTo investigate the impact of muscle activity on spatialpatterns of activation, MI trials excluded due to the presenceof EMG activity was subjected to the data analysis outlinedabove. 3D paired t-tests were then performed to investigatedifferences in group-level patterns of activity for whole headmaps of ‘contaminated MI’ (i.e., trials with EMG activity) and‘pure MI’ (i.e., trials without EMG activity).

Acknowledgments

SB acknowledges salary support from the Heart and StrokeFoundation of Canada in the form of an Early Career ResearchAward. AG and SK are supported by NSERC training awards.This work was funded by grants awarded to SB and TB fromthe Nova Scotia Health Research Foundation (MED-DI 1551)and Faculty of Health Professions, Dalhousie University.

Appendix A. Supporting information

Supplementary data associated with this article can be foundin the online version at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.001.

r e f e r e n c e s

Baillet, S., Mosher, J.C., Leahy, R.M., 2001. Electromagnetic brainmapping, Signal Processing Magazine. IEEE 18 (6), 14–30, http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/79.962275.

Barry, R.J., Clarke, A.R., Johnstone, S.J., Magee, C.A., Rushby, J.A.,2007. EEG differences between eyes-closed and eyes-openresting conditions. Clin. Neurophysiol.: Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin.Neurophysiol. 118 (12), 2765–2773, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2007.07.028.

Binkofski, F., Amunts, K., Stephan, K.M., Posse, S., Schormann, T.,Freund, H.J., Zilles, K., Seitz, R.J., 2000. Broca’s region subservesimagery of motion: a combine cytoarchitectonic and fMRIstudy. Hum. Brain Mapp. 11 (4), 273–285.

Boe, S., Gionfriddo, A., Kraeutner, S., Tremblay, A., Little, G,Bardouille, T., 2014. Laterality of brain activity during motorimagery is modulated by the provision of real-timeneurofeedback. NeuroImage 101, 159–167, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.066.

Braun, S., Kleynen, M., Schols, J., Schack, T., Beurskens, A., Wade,D., 2008. Using mental practice in stroke rehabilitation: aframework. Clin. rehabilitation 22 (7), 579–591.

Page 10: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 190

Burianova, H., Marstaller, L., Sowman, P., Tesan, G., Rich, A.N.,Williams, M., Johnson, B.W., 2013. Multimodal functionalimaging of motor imagery using a novel paradigm.NeuroImage 71, 50–58, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.001.

Chang, Y., Lee, J.-J., Seo, J.-H., Song, H.-J., Kim, Y.-T., Lee, H.J.,Kim, J.G., 2011. Neural correlates of motor imagery for elitearchers. NMR Biomed. 24 (4), 366–372, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/nbm.1600.

Corbetta, M., Kincade, J.M., Shulman, G.L., 2002. Neuralsystems for visual orienting and their relationships tospatial working memory 14, 508–523J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14,508–523.

Corbetta, M., Shulman, G.L., 2002. Control of goal-directed andstimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3,201–215.

Cox, R.W., 1996. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization offunctional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput.Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173.

Cox, R.W., Hyde, J.S., 1997. Software tools for analysisand visualization of fMRI data. NMR Biomed. 10, 171–178.

Cumming, P, 2014. PET neuroimaging: the white elephant packshis trunk?. NeuroImage 84, 1094–1100, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.020.

De Lange, F.P., Roelofs, K., Toni, I., 2008. Motor imagery: a windowinto the mechanisms and alterations of the motor system.Cortex: J. Devoted Study Nerv. Syst. Behav. 44 (5), 494–506,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.09.002.

Di Rienzo, F., Guillot, A., Daligault, S., Delpuech, C., Rode, G.,Collet, C., 2014. Motor inhibition during motor imagery: a MEGstudy with a quadriplegic patient. Neurocase 20 (5), 524–539.

Diwakar, M., Huang, M.-X., Srinivasan, R., Harrington, D.L.,Robb, A., Angeles, A., Lee, R.R., 2011. Dual-core beamformerfor obtaining highly correlated neuronal networks in MEG.NeuroImage 54 (1), 253–263, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.023.

Doricchi, F., Macci, E., Silvetti, M., Macaluso, E., 2010. Neuralcorrelates of the spatial and expectancy components ofendogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in thePosner task. 20. Cereb.Cortex1574–1585, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp215 (New York, N.Y. : 1991).

Formaggio, E., Storti, S.F., Cerini, R., Fiaschi, A., Manganotti, P.,2010. Brain oscillatory activity during motor imagery in EEG-fMRI coregistration. Magnetic Resonance Imaging 28 (10),1403–1412, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mri.2010.06.030.

Grafton, S.T., 2009. Embodied cognition and the simulation ofaction to understand others. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 97–117,http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04425.x.

Grezes, J., Decety, J., 2001. Functional anatomy of execution,mental simulation,observation, and verb generation ofactions: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 12, 1–19.

Guillot, A., Collet, C., Nguyen, V.A., Malouin, F., Richards, C.,Doyon, J., 2009. Brain activity during visual versus kinestheticimagery: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30 (7), 2157–2172,http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20658.

Hanakawa, T., Immisch, I., Toma, K., Dimyan, M.A., Van Gelderen,P., Hallett, M., 2003. Functional properties of brain areasassociated with motor execution and imagery. J. Neurophysiol.89 (2), 989–1002, http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00132.2002.

Hardwick, R.M., Rottschy, C., Miall, R.C., Eickhoff, S.B., 2013.A quantitative meta-analysis and review of motor learning inthe human brain. NeuroImage 67, 283–297, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.020.

Hari, R., Parkkonen, L., Nangini, C., 2010. The brain in time: insightsfrom neuromagnetic recordings. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1191,89–109, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05438.x.

Heitger, M.H., Ronsse, R., Dhollander, T., Dupont, P.,Caeyenberghs, K., Swinnen, S.P., 2012. Motor learning-induced

changes in functional brain connectivity as revealed by means

of graph-theoretical network analysis. NeuroImage 61 (3),

633–650, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.067.Hetu, S., Gregoire, M., Saimpont, A., Coll, M.-P., Eugene, F.,

Michon, P.-E., Jackson, P.L., 2013. The neural network of motor

imagery: an ALE meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37 (5),

930–949, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.017.Hoshi, E., Tanji, J., 2007. Distinctions between dorsal and ventral

premotor areas: anatomical connectivity and functional

properties. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 234–242.Jeannerod, M., 1995. Mental imagery in the motor contex.

Neuropsychologia 33 (11), 1419–1432.Jeannerod, M., 2001. Neural simulation of action: a unifying

mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage 14 (1 Pt 2),

S103–S109, http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0832.Keller, P.E., 2012. Mental imagery in music performance:

underlying mechanisms and potential benefits. Ann. N. Y.

Acad. Sci. 1252, 206–213, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-

6632.2011.06439.x.Lacourse, M.G., Orr, E.L.R., Cramer, S.C., Cohen, M.J., 2005. Brain

activation during execution and motor imagery of novel and

skilled sequential hand movements. NeuroImage 27 (3),

505–519, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.025.Lotze, M., Halsband, U., 2006. Motor imagery. J. Physiol. 99,

386–395.Miller, K.J., Schalk, G., Fetz, E.E., den Nijs, M., Ojemann, J.G.,

Rao, R.P.N., 2010. Cortical activity during motor execution,

motor imagery, and imagery-based online feedback. Proc.

Natl. Acad. Sci. 107 (9), 4430–4435, http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/

pnas.0913697107.Milton, J., Solodkin, A., Hlustık, P., Small, S.L., 2007. The mind of

expert motor performance is cool and focused. NeuroImage 35

(2), 804–813, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.

neuroimage.2007.01.003.Moran, A., Guillot, A., Macintyre, T., Collet, C., 2012. Re-imagining

motor imagery: building bridges between cognitive

neuroscience and sport psychology. Br. J. Psychol. 103 (2),

224–247, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02068.x

(London, England : 1953).Mulder, T., 2007. Motor imagery and action observation: cognitive

tools for rehabilitation. J. Neural Transm. 114 (10), 1265–1278,

http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00702-007-0763-z (Vienna, Austria :

1996).Munzert, J., Lorey, B., Zentgraf, K., 2009. Cognitive motor

processes: the role of motor imagery in the study of motor

representations. Brain Res. Rev. 60, 306–326.Munzert, J., Zentgraf, K., 2009. Motor imagery and its implications

for understanding the motor system. Prog. Brain Res. 174,

219–229.Nakagawa, K., Aokage, Y., Fukuri, T., Kawahara, Y., Hashizume,

A., Kurisu, K., Yuge, L., 2011. Neuromagnetic beta oscillation

changes during motor imagery and motor execution of skilled

movements. Neuroreport 22 (5), 217–222, http://dx.doi.org/

10.1097/WNR.0b013e328344b480.Newell, K., 1991. Motor skill acquisition. Annu. Rev. Pychol. 42,

213–237.Niedermeyer, E., 1996. Dipole theory and electroencephalography.

Clin. Electroencephalogr. 27 (3), 121–131.Neuper, C., Wortz, M., Pfurtscheller, G., 2006. ERD/ERS patterns

reflecting sensorimotor activation and deactivation. Prog.

Brain Res. 159, 211–222, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123

(06)59014-4.Ono, T., Kimura, A., Ushiba, J., 2013. Daily training with realistic

visual feedback improves reproducibility of event-related

desynchronisation following hand motor imagery. Clin.

Neurophysiol.: Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124 (9),

1779–1786, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.03.006.

Page 11: Read the complete article [PDF 5 MB] - · PDF file3D t-tests were conducted after Talairach normalization. Source-level analysis showed that MI has similar patterns of spatial activity

b r a i n r e s e a r c h 1 5 8 8 ( 2 0 1 4 ) 8 1 – 9 1 91

Pfurtscheller, G., Lopes da Silva, F.H., 1999. Event-related EEG/MEG

synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles.

Clin. Neurophysiol.: Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110

(11), 1842–1857.Pfurtscheller, G., Neuper, C., 1997. Motor imagery activates primary

sensorimotor area in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 239 (2–3), 65–68.Porro, C.A., Francescato, M.P., Cettolo, V., Diamond, M.E., Baraldi,

P., Zuiani, C., di Prampero, P.E., 1996. Primary motor and

sensory cortex activation during motor performance and

motor imagery: a functional magnetic resonance imaging

study. J. Neurosci.: Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 16 (23), 7688–7698.Rizzolatti, G., Luppino, G., Matelli, M., 1998. The organization of

the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalogr.

Clin. Neurophysiol. 106 (4), 283–296.Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Matelli, M., Bettinardi, V., Paulesu, E.,

Perani, D., Fazio, F., 1996. Localization of grasp representations

in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution. Exp. Brain

Res. 111, 246–252.Rounis, E., Yarrow, K., Rothwell, J.C., 2007. Effects of rTMS

conditioning over the fronto-parietal network on motor

versus visual attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19 (3), 513–524, http:

//dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.513.Rushworth, M.F., Johansen-Berg, H., Gobel, S., Devlin, J., 2003. The

left parietal and premotor cortices: motor attention and

selection. NeuroImage 20, S89–S100, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/

j.neuroimage.2003.09.011.Sanders, C.W., Sadoski, M., van Walsum, K., Bramson, R., Wiprud,

R., Fossum, T.W., 2008. Learning basic surgical skills with

mental imagery: using the simulation centre in the mind.

Med. Edu. 42 (6), 607–612, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-

2923.2007.02964.x.

Schoffelen, J.M., Gross, J., 2009. Source connectivity analysis withMEG and EEG. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30 (6), 1857–1865, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20745.

Schuster, C., Hilfiker, R., Amft, O., Scheidhauer, A., Andrews, B.,Butler, J., Ettlin, T., 2011. Best practice for motor imagery: asystematic literature review on motor imagery trainingelements in five different disciplines. BMC Med. 9, 75http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-9-75.

Sharma, N., Baron, J.-C., Rowe, J.B., 2009. Motor imagery afterstroke: relating outcome to motor network connectivity. Ann.Neurol. 66 (5), 604–616, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ana.21810.

Sutton, B.P., Ouyang, C., Karampinos, D.C., Miller, G.A., 2009.Current trends and challenges in MRI acquisitions toinvestigate brain function. Int. J. Psychophysiol. : Off. J. Int.Organ. Psychophysiol. 73 (1), 33–42, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.020.

Szczepanski, S.M., Kastner, S., 2009. Transcranial magneticstimulation studies of visuospatial attentional control. F1000Biol. Rep. 1, 81, http://dx.doi.org/10.3410/B1-81.

Taylor, P.C.J., Thut, G., 2012. Brain activity underlying visualperception and attention as inferred from TMS-EEG: a review.Brain Stimul. 5 (2), 124–129, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.003.

Tunik, E., Rice, N.J., Hamilton, a., Grafton, S.T., 2007. Beyondgrasping: representation of action in human anteriorintraparietal sulcus. NeuroImage 36 (Suppl. 2), ST77–ST86,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.026.

Vrba, J., Taulu, S., Nenonen, J., Ahonen, A., 2010. Signalspace separation beamformer. Brain Topogr. 23 (2), 128–133,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10548-009-0120-7.

Wulf, G., Shea, C., Lewthwaite, R., 2010. Motor skill learning andperformance: a review of influential factors. Med. Edu. 44 (1),75–84, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03421.x.