psy 369: psycholinguistics

36
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production: Speech Errors

Upload: piper

Post on 25-Jan-2016

25 views

Category:

Documents


5 download

DESCRIPTION

PSY 369: Psycholinguistics. Language Production: Speech Errors. Problems with speech errors. Even very carefully verified corpora of speech errors tend to list the error and then “ the target”. However, there may be several possible targets. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Language Production:Speech Errors

Page 2: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Even very carefully verified corpora of speech errors tend to list the error and then “the target”.

However, there may be several possible targets. Saying there is one definitive target may limit conclusions

about what type of error has actually occurred. Evidence that we are not very good at perceiving

speech errors.

Problems with speech errors

Page 3: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

How well do we perceive speech errors? Ferber (1991)

Method: Transcripts of TV and radio were studied very carefully

to pick out all the speech errors.

Problems with speech errors

Did you hear what he said?!

The tapes were played to subjects whose task was to record all the errors they heard.

The errors spotted by the subjects were compared with those that actually occurred.

Page 4: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

How well do we perceive speech errors? Ferber (1991)

Problems with speech errors

Results: subjects missed 50% of all the errors and of the half they identified

50% were incorrectly recorded (i.e. only 25% of speech errors were correctly recorded).

Conclusion: we are bad at perceiving errors.

Page 5: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Experimental speech errors Can we examine speech errors in under more

controlled conditions? SLIP technique: speech error elicitation technique

Motley and Baars (1976)

Page 6: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Say the words silently as quickly as you canSay them aloud if you hear a tone

Page 7: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

dog bone

Page 8: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

dust ball

Page 9: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

dead bug

Page 10: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

doll bed

Page 11: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

barn door

“darn bore”

Page 12: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

• This technique has been found to elicit 30% of predicted speech errors.

• Lexical Bias effect: error frequency affected by whether the error results in real words or non-words

Experimental speech errors

“wrong loot” FOR “long root”

“rawn loof” FOR “lawn roof “

Some basic findings

More likely

Page 13: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Influence of semantics (Motley, 1980)

Experimental speech errors

Hypothesis: If preceded by phonologically and semantically

biasing material (PS) If preceded by only phonologically biasing material

(P).

Some basic findings

Predicted to be more likely

Page 14: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Influence of semantics (Motley, 1980)

Experimental speech errors

Method: 2 matched lists 20 word pairs as targets for errors

e.g. bad mug mad bug Each preceded by 4 - 7 neutral “filler”

word pairs

Some basic findings

mashed bunsmangy bears

Then 4 interference word pairs 2 phonological PLUS

2 semantic (SP)

angry insect

ornery fly

angled inset

older flu

or semantically neutral controls (P)

bad mug

small catsrainy daysred cars

Page 15: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Results: More errors in the Semantic and Phonological (SP) condition than in the Phonological (P) condition.

Conclusion: Semantic interference may contribute to a distortion of

the sound of a speaker’s intended utterance

Experimental speech errors

Influence of semantics (Motley, 1980)

Some basic findings

Page 16: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Freudian slips The psycholinguistic approach

Assume that “the mechanics of slips can be studied linguistically without reference to their motivation.” (Boomer and Laver, 1968)

Freudian approach Held that speech errors “arise from the concurrent action - or

perhaps rather, the opposing action - of two different intentions”

Intended meaning + disturbing intention speech error

Page 17: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Freudian slips“In the case of female genitals, in spite of many

versuchungen [temptations] - I beg your pardon, versuche [experiments]…”

From a politician “I like Heath. He’s tough - like Hitler - (shocked silence from reporters) - Did I say Hitler? I meant Churchill.”

Are these cases of disturbing intentions or merely cases of lexical substitution (phonologically or semantically related words)?

Page 18: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Freudian slips

Of the 94 errors listed in Psychopathology of Everyday Life 85 were made in normal speech.

Ellis, (1980)

51 (60%) involved lexical substitution in which the substituting word was either similar in phonological form (27) to the intended word or related in meaning (22).

Page 19: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Freudian slips

Of the 94 errors listed in Psychopathology of Everyday Life 85 were made in normal speech.

Ellis, (1980)

Only 10/94 of the errors reported by Freud were spoonerisms, and 4 were from Meringer and Mayer, 1895 (an early, linguistically oriented study).

E.g. Eiwess-scheibchen (“small slices of egg white”) Eischeissweibchen (lit. “egg-shit-female”)

Alabasterbüchse (“alabaster box”) Alabüsterbachse (büste = breast)

Page 20: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Freudian slips

Hence, it appears that “Freud’s theory can be translated into the language of modern psycholinguistic production models without excessive difficulty.”

Ellis, (1980)

Page 21: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Experimental Freudian slips? Hypothesis: Spoonerisms more likely when

the resulting content is congruous with the situational context.

Method: 90 males, same procedure previously used by Motley, 1980 (SLIP). 3 Conditions: “Electricity”, “Sex”, and Neutral.

Page 22: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics
Page 23: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

car tires

Page 24: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

cat toys

Page 25: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

can tops

Page 26: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

cup trays

Page 27: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

tool kits

“cool tits”

Page 28: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Same word pairs in all conditions, spoonerism targets were non-words (e.g. goxi furl foxy girl), targets preceded by 3 phonologically biasing word pairs not semantically related to target words.

Experimental Freudian slips?

Page 29: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Results: Electricity set: 69 E, 31 S Sex set: 36 E, 76 S Neutral set: 44 E, 41 S

Hence errors were in the expected direction. Conclusion: subjects’ speech encoding systems are

sensitive to semantic influences from their situational cognitive set.

Experimental Freudian slips?

Page 30: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Hypothesis: subjects with high levels of sex anxiety will make more “sex” spoonerisms than those with low sex anxiety.

Method: 36 males selected on the basis of high, medium, & low sex

anxiety (Mosher Sex-Guilt Inventory). SLIP task same as previous experiment but with 2 additional

Sex targets and 9 Neutral targets.

Experimental Freudian slips?

Page 31: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Results: looked at difference scores (Sex - Neutral) High sex anxiety > medium > low. Overall: Sex spoonerisms > Neutral spoonerisms.

Conclusion: appears to support Freud’s view of sexual anxiety being revealed in Slips of the Tongue

BUT: the experimenters (Baars and Motley) went on to show that any type of anxiety, not just sexual produced similar results.

SO: anxiety was at play but it was more general, so the priming

was more global.

Experimental Freudian slips?

Page 32: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Conclusions Speech errors have provided data about the

units of speech production.

Phonology - consonants, vowels, and consonant clusters (/fl/) can be disordered as units. Also, phonetic features.

Syllables which have morphemic status can be involved in errors. Separation of stem morphemes from affixes (inflectional and derivational).

Stress? Stress errors could be examples of blends..

Page 33: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Conclusions

Syntax -grammatical rules may be applied to the wrong unit, but produce the correct pronunciation (e.g. plural takes the correct form /s/, /z/, or /iz/.

Indicates that these parts of words are marked as grammatical morphemes.

Phrases (e.g. NP) and clauses can be exchanged or reversed.

Words - can exchange, move, or be mis-selected.

Speech errors have provided data about the units of speech production.

Page 34: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

From thought to speech How does a mental concept get turned into a spoken utterance? Levelt, 1989, 4 stages of production:

1 Conceptualising: we conceptualise what we wish to communicate (“mentalese”).

2 Formulating: we formulate what we want to say into a linguistic plan.– Lexicalisation

– Lemma Selection– Lexeme (or Phonological Form) Selection

– Syntactic Planning3 Articulating: we execute the plan through muscles in the vocal tract.4 Self-monitoring: we monitor our speech to assess whether it is what we

intended to say, and how we intended to say it.

Page 35: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

Models of production As in comprehension, there are serial

(modular) and interactive models Serial models - Garrett, Levelt et al. Interactive models - Stemberger, Dell

Levelt’s monitoring stage (originally proposed by Baars) can explain much of the data that is said to favour interaction between earlier levels

Page 36: PSY 369: Psycholinguistics

An model of sentence production Three broad stages:

Conceptualisation deciding on the message (= meaning to

express)

Formulation turning the message into linguistic

representations Grammatical encoding (finding words and

putting them together) Phonological encoding (finding sounds and

putting them together)

Articulation speaking (or writing or signing)

Message

Lexicon

Grammatical

Form

Articulation

FunctionalProcessing

PositionalProcessing