pm.pdf

14
Effectiveness of Marketing Cues on Consumer Perceptions of Quality: The Moderating Roles of Brand Reputation and Third-Party Information Billur Akdeniz University of New Hampshire Roger J. Calantone and Clay M. Voorhees Michigan State University ABSTRACT Consumers usually infer unobservable product quality by processing multiple-quality cues in the environment. Prior research considering the simultaneous effects of marketing cues on consumer quality perceptions is sparse. Despite the growing importance of third-party information, research examining its simultaneous effects with marketing cues on consumers’ decision making is especially absent. This research, drawing on cue-diagnosticity, cue-consistency, and negativity bias theories, proposes and tests a conceptual framework to reveal interplays among various marketing- and nonmarketing-controlled product cues. The first study examines how two- and three-way interactions of high-scope (i.e., brand reputation) and low-scope marketing cues (i.e., price and warranty) affect consumer perceptions. The second study examines a set of interaction effects between third-party quality ratings and marketing cues (i.e., price and warranty) on consumers’ perceptions. Overall, the results reveal theoretical and managerial implications for processing multiple-quality cues in consumers’ inference-making behaviors and suggest that consumers generally aggregate perceptions in more complex ways than suggested in the prior literature when making global product quality evaluations. C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. In markets where product quality is not easily ob- servable, consumers generally make their purchase decisions while experiencing feelings of uncertainty (Erdem & Swait, 1998; Jacoby, Olson, & Haddock, 1971). To cope with such uncertainty and make in- ferences about product quality, consumers search for and process available product-related cues, which can be marketing controlled (e.g., price, advertising, brand- ing) or nonmarketing controlled (e.g., third-party in- formation). Existing literature most often presents the isolated effects of a select set of individual marketing cues, such as price (Rao & Monroe, 1989), advertising (Kirmani & Wright, 1989), warranty (Boulding & Kir- mani, 1993), and brand reputation (Baek, Kim, & Yu, 2010; Erdem & Swait, 2004); such cues have all been researched to a great extent. However, these results are often equivocal as most prior research fails to con- sider the simultaneous effects of multiple cues in the environment (Purohit & Srivastava, 2001). In addition to processing multiple marketing- controlled quality cues (e.g., brand, price and war- ranty), consumers seek out insights from nonmarket- ing-controlled cues such as third-party reviews or ratings (Jiang, Jones, & Javie, 2008). Similar to ex- trinsic marketing cues, the primary role of third-party product information is to reduce information asymme- try when the true product quality is not easily observ- able. This added layer of complexity to the evaluation process requires new research to better explain how all of these potentially competing quality cues may inter- act to form consumers’ overall product evaluations. By eschewing the isolated approach, the current re- search examines simultaneous effects of multiple mar- keting cues on consumers’ quality perceptions. Prior research, with few exceptions, has avoided the obser- vation that product cues seldom operate in solo (e.g., Dawar & Parker, 1994; Dodds, Monroe, & Grewal, 1991; Miyazaki, Grewal, & Goodstein, 2005; Richard- son, Dick, & Jain, 1994). However, understanding how arrays of marketing cues affect quality perceptions is of increasing importance as consumers usually process one product cue in relation to another (e.g., price of a product along with brand name). Thus, the overall objective of this study is to develop and test a concep- tual framework to understand how consumers combine and process various product cues to form perceptions of Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 30(1): 76–89 (January 2013) View this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/mar C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. DOI: 10.1002/mar.20590 76

Upload: cindy-rizka-maulina-ii

Post on 24-Dec-2015

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: pm.pdf

Effectiveness of Marketing Cues onConsumer Perceptions of Quality: TheModerating Roles of Brand Reputationand Third-Party InformationBillur AkdenizUniversity of New HampshireRoger J. Calantone and Clay M. VoorheesMichigan State University

ABSTRACT

Consumers usually infer unobservable product quality by processing multiple-quality cues in theenvironment. Prior research considering the simultaneous effects of marketing cues on consumerquality perceptions is sparse. Despite the growing importance of third-party information, researchexamining its simultaneous effects with marketing cues on consumers’ decision making is especiallyabsent. This research, drawing on cue-diagnosticity, cue-consistency, and negativity bias theories,proposes and tests a conceptual framework to reveal interplays among various marketing- andnonmarketing-controlled product cues. The first study examines how two- and three-wayinteractions of high-scope (i.e., brand reputation) and low-scope marketing cues (i.e., price andwarranty) affect consumer perceptions. The second study examines a set of interaction effectsbetween third-party quality ratings and marketing cues (i.e., price and warranty) on consumers’perceptions. Overall, the results reveal theoretical and managerial implications for processingmultiple-quality cues in consumers’ inference-making behaviors and suggest that consumersgenerally aggregate perceptions in more complex ways than suggested in the prior literature whenmaking global product quality evaluations. C! 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

In markets where product quality is not easily ob-servable, consumers generally make their purchasedecisions while experiencing feelings of uncertainty(Erdem & Swait, 1998; Jacoby, Olson, & Haddock,1971). To cope with such uncertainty and make in-ferences about product quality, consumers search forand process available product-related cues, which canbe marketing controlled (e.g., price, advertising, brand-ing) or nonmarketing controlled (e.g., third-party in-formation). Existing literature most often presents theisolated effects of a select set of individual marketingcues, such as price (Rao & Monroe, 1989), advertising(Kirmani & Wright, 1989), warranty (Boulding & Kir-mani, 1993), and brand reputation (Baek, Kim, & Yu,2010; Erdem & Swait, 2004); such cues have all beenresearched to a great extent. However, these resultsare often equivocal as most prior research fails to con-sider the simultaneous effects of multiple cues in theenvironment (Purohit & Srivastava, 2001).

In addition to processing multiple marketing-controlled quality cues (e.g., brand, price and war-ranty), consumers seek out insights from nonmarket-ing-controlled cues such as third-party reviews or

ratings (Jiang, Jones, & Javie, 2008). Similar to ex-trinsic marketing cues, the primary role of third-partyproduct information is to reduce information asymme-try when the true product quality is not easily observ-able. This added layer of complexity to the evaluationprocess requires new research to better explain how allof these potentially competing quality cues may inter-act to form consumers’ overall product evaluations.

By eschewing the isolated approach, the current re-search examines simultaneous effects of multiple mar-keting cues on consumers’ quality perceptions. Priorresearch, with few exceptions, has avoided the obser-vation that product cues seldom operate in solo (e.g.,Dawar & Parker, 1994; Dodds, Monroe, & Grewal,1991; Miyazaki, Grewal, & Goodstein, 2005; Richard-son, Dick, & Jain, 1994). However, understanding howarrays of marketing cues affect quality perceptions isof increasing importance as consumers usually processone product cue in relation to another (e.g., price ofa product along with brand name). Thus, the overallobjective of this study is to develop and test a concep-tual framework to understand how consumers combineand process various product cues to form perceptions of

Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 30(1): 76–89 (January 2013)View this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/mar

C! 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. DOI: 10.1002/mar.20590

76

Page 2: pm.pdf

product quality. To achieve this objective, Study 1 ex-amines how the valence of a high-scope cue (i.e., brandreputation) affects the diagnosticity of low-scope cues(i.e., price and warranty) in consumers’ quality percep-tions; meanwhile, Study 2 examines how third-partyratings and traditional marketing cues (e.g., price andwarranty) might interact to affect consumers’ qualityinferences.

This research makes several contributions to cur-rent understandings of how consumers leverage qual-ity cues to form product opinions. First, drawing oncue-diagnosticity theory, it provides a parsimoniousconceptual framework to examine the simultaneous im-pact of high- and low-scope quality cues. Second, draw-ing on cue-consistency and negativity bias theories ascomplementary driving mechanisms, it analyzes three-way interactions not only among marketing-controlledcues, but also between nonmarketing- and marketing-controlled cues. Specifically, three-way interactionshighlight potential biases in cue-diagnosticity and neg-ativity bias mechanisms if they fail to account for eachother’s implications. Third, this research examinesthird-party ratings as a popular yet under-researchedproduct quality cue and its interplay with marketingcues in affecting consumers’ perceptions. Finally, itpresents empirical evidence for the credibility and di-agnosticity of brand reputation when presented withother marketing cues.

The remainder of the article is organized as follows.The next section discusses the hypothesis development,method, and results of Study 1, followed by the hy-potheses, method, and results of Study 2. Finally, thediscussion of the results along with their managerialimplications is presented. Limitations and future re-search avenues conclude the paper.

STUDY 1: EFFECTS OF BRANDREPUTATION, WARRANTY, AND PRICEON PERCEIVED QUALITY

Hypothesis Development

In markets where the true quality of a product is notreadily observable, consumers often rely on productcues to evaluate quality (e.g., Kirmani & Rao, 2000;Rao & Monroe, 1989). During this evaluation process,consumers are usually faced with multiple product cues(e.g., brand name, price, warranty). According to cue-diagnosticity theory, consumers prioritize cues basedon their diagnosticity in differentiating between prod-uct alternatives (Slovic & Lichtenstein, 1971; Skowron-ski & Carlston, 1987). Diagnosticity refers to perceivedreliability of a cue in distinguishing between alterna-tive categorizations of products (e.g., high quality vs.low quality). For instance, when exposed to multiple-quality cues, consumers tend to rank their relative im-portance based on their ability to discern between ahigh-quality and low-quality product. In these situa-

tions, cues deemed to be more diagnostic are rankedhigher and used more often in driving the purchase de-cision than less diagnostic cues (Purohit & Srivastava,2001).

The purpose of Study 1 is to build a conceptualframework and provide empirical evidence for the cue-prioritization process by leveraging the diagnosticityof a product cue as the theoretical mechanism to ex-plain why a specific cue has more impact on qual-ity perceptions than another one. More specifically,this study builds upon previous research that cate-gorizes product cues as high- and low-scope cues ac-cording to their diagnosticity level (Gidron, Koehler,& Tversky, 1993). High-scope cue valence is estab-lished over time. Thus, considerable effort and invest-ment are necessary to change the valence of a high-scope cue whereas low-scope cues are transitory innature and their valence can be changed quickly andinexpensively. Furthermore, consumers perceive high-scope cues to be stable, credible, and diagnostic as theylead to a more accurate categorization of products. Con-versely, consumers perceive low-scope cues as ambigu-ous and less diagnostic as firms can easily use them tosend false signals about a product (Hoch & Deighton,1989).

As a starting point for classifying quality cues ashigh scope versus low scope, prior research in market-ing has provided preliminary evidence for the diagnos-ticity of several cues (e.g., Purohit & Srivastava, 2001).This study focuses on three popular marketing cues—namely, warranty, price, and brand reputation. Eachmarketing cue is defined and evidence is provided tosupport their classification as either a high- or low-scope cue.

A product warranty is a guarantee, provided bythe manufacturer, to fix product-related problems ina given period of time (Srivastava & Mitra, 1998).Warranties signal a manufacturer’s confidence in prod-uct quality and provide consumers with an assuranceof quality to increase their confidence in the prod-uct (Erevelles, 1993; Kirmani & Rao, 2000; Shimp &Bearden, 1982). Similar to warranty, price is qualitycue that is directly controlled by the manufacturer. Al-though both of these cues can provide some diagnosticvalue to consumers when evaluating quality, they can-not be separated from the source from which they aresent. More specifically, because manufacturers havecomplete control over warranty and price of their prod-ucts, both of these cues can be changed quickly and eas-ily. As a result, consumers are well aware that manu-facturers can leverage both price and warranty as falsesignals to mislead consumers. For example, if a low-quality manufacturer offers a high warranty and ab-sorbs higher costs of warranty fulfillment by charginga higher price that is still lower than that of a high-quality manufacturer, warranties will not be very di-agnostic for distinguishing a high-quality from a low-quality product (e.g., Boulding & Kirmani, 1993; Erdem& Swait, 1998). Due to the potential for gamesmanshipon the part of the manufacturer, consumers may deem

EFFECTIVENESS OF MARKETING CUES ON PERCEPTIONS OF QUALITY 77Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 3: pm.pdf

both warranty and price as having low diagnosticity,thereby making them low-scope cues.

Unlike warranty and price, substantial time andinvestment are necessary to build a high-reputationbrand; hence, firms cannot be as opportunistic in ma-nipulating brand reputation as a false signal of productquality. Brand reputation represents the embodimentof the cumulative effect of all past and present market-ing activities (e.g., Wernerfelt, 1988). It is also a productcue that consumers frequently use to cope with uncer-tainty in their decision making (e.g., Baek Kim, & Yu,2010; Dodds, Monroe, & Grewal, 1991; Gammoh, Voss,& Chakraborty, 2006; Washburn, Till, & Priluck, 2004).Thus, the stable and credible nature of brand reputa-tion makes it highly diagnostic and, consequently, ahigh-scope cue. According to the cue-diagnosticity the-ory, brand reputation is likely to play a more dominantrole in consumers’ quality perceptions relative to othercues that are considered to be low scope (e.g., Boulding& Kirmani, 1993).

When assessing the quality of a product, consumersusually do not assess these cues in isolation. Rather,most consumers are simultaneously exposed to multi-ple cues with varying levels of diagnosticity, and theyneed to make sense of these cues to develop qualityperceptions. In these instances, an examination of theinteraction among cues is necessary. Although ampleresearch has investigated the main effects of productcues, limited research has explored the complexity ofthe interplay among those cues and, more specifically,interactions between high- and low-scope cues. For in-stance, Purohit and Srivastava (2001) found that agood (bad) reputation does not only have a direct effecton quality perception, but also has an indirect effectthrough an increase (decrease) in the diagnosticity ofthe low-scope cues. Previous research has also demon-strated that consumers tend to perceive price informa-tion as being more diagnostic in their quality evalua-tions when presented with a consistent brand cue thanwhen the brand cue is absent or inconsistent (Brucks,Zeithaml, & Naylor, 2000). Another study found a sig-nificant interaction between price and brand name suchthat, under a strong brand name, a higher price resultsin a higher perceived quality than does the lower pricecue (Miyazaki, Grewal, & Goodstein, 2005). Similarly,for warranty cue, Srivastava and Mitra (1998) showedthat firm reputation influences the extent to which cus-tomers use warranty cues in inferring product quality.Boulding and Kirmani’s (1993) results support that abetter warranty leads to higher perceptions of qualitywhen the warrantor reputation is high (vs. low).

The current study specifically examines how the va-lence of a high-scope cue (i.e., brand reputation) affectsthe diagnosticity and the usage of low-scope cues toinfer about product quality. Although high-scope cuesare more distinct and less dependent on the existenceand valence of other available cues in the environment,the diagnosticity of low-scope cues depends on the ex-istence and valence of high-scope cues. Prior researchsuggests that a high-scope cue can facilitate or impede

a low-scope cue by changing its diagnosticity (Purohit &Srivastava, 2001). The positive (negative) implicationssuggested by high-scope cues transmit over to the low-scope cues, making them more (less) diagnostic. Forexample, if a firm has high reputation, it is less likelyto send a false signal through low-scope cues, therebymaking low-scope cues more diagnostic. Otherwise, itwill deteriorate its reputation, upset its customers, andincur costs due to the false signal. However, if a firmhas low reputation, it is more likely to send false sig-nals because—unlike a high-reputation firm—it doesnot have much to lose by upsetting consumers with falseinformation. Thus, it is posited that low-scope cues (i.e.,price and warranty) become more (less) diagnostic froma consumer’s point of view when the high-scope cue (i.e.,brand reputation) is positive (negative).

H1: Brand reputation moderates the effect ofwarranty on quality perception such that,under high (vs. low) reputation, the effect ofa stronger (vs. weaker) warranty is higheron perceived quality.

H2: Brand reputation moderates the effect ofprice on quality perception such that, un-der high (vs. low) reputation, the effect of ahigher (vs. lower) price is higher on perceivedquality.

This research, in addition to investigating these two-way interactions, extends the literature by accountingfor a more complex set of relationships among productcues. Specifically, H1 and H2 contend that the diagnos-ticity of brand reputation is the primary mechanismdriving its interaction with low-scope cues on qualityperceptions. However, when considering the interplaybetween three different product cues with varying va-lences, relationships may be more complex than whatcan simply be explained by cue-diagnosticity theory.Prior research has indicated that tenets from both nega-tivity bias and cue-consistency theory can be adopted ascomplementary mechanisms to cue-diagnosticity the-ory to explain a three-way interaction among price,warranty, and brand reputation.

Cue-consistency theory suggests that multiplesources of information are more useful when they pro-vide corroborating information than when they offerdisparate information (Maheswaran & Chaiken, 1991).As an extension to this theory, negativity bias theorysuggests that in situations where cues offer conflictinginformation, consumers weigh the negative informationmore heavily than the positive information in their in-ference formation because negative cues can be viewedas more salient and useful (e.g., Ahluwalia, 2002; An-derson, 1965; Herr, Kardes, & Kim, 1991). Consistentwith these theoretical frameworks, previous researchhas shown that, when two cues are consistent (i.e., highprice–high warranty), their effect will be stronger than

78 AKDENIZ, CALANTONE, AND VOORHEESPsychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 4: pm.pdf

that of inconsistent cue pairs (i.e., high price–low war-ranty or low price–high warranty); when cues are in-consistent, the more negative cue becomes more salientand dominates consumers’ evaluation (Miyazaki, Gre-wal, & Goodstein, 2005). Thus, it is posited that low-scope cues interact to have a stronger effect on qualityperceptions when these cues are consistent and theyare paired with a positive valence high-scope cue (i.e.,brand reputation). However, in line with negativity biastheory, when low-scope cues provide inconsistent in-formation, the more negative cue dominates consumerperceptions and, as a result, the effect of inconsistentpairs will not be significantly different from the lowprice–low warranty pair.

H3a: Under high brand reputation, the interac-tion effect of price and warranty on qual-ity perception is stronger when they provideconsistent (i.e., high price–strong warranty)versus inconsistent (i.e., high price–weakwarranty or low price–strong warranty) in-formation. However, the interaction effect ofinconsistent pairs will not be significantlydifferent from the interaction of low price–weak warranty pair.

Unique perspectives offered by cue-diagnosticity andnegativity bias theories create the potential for diverg-ing predictions. It is possible for hypotheses based oncue-diagnosticity theory to be biased if they fail to ac-count for the valence of the information provided by thecues; likewise, negativity bias theory can become biasedif it does not explicitly account for the diagnosticity ofthe information provided. For instance, as proposed inH1 and H2, it is expected that a positive, high-scope cuecould offset the effects of negative, low-scope cues. Inthese situations, cue-diagnosticity and negativity biastheories are at odds, and it is likely that effects of eachtheory are contingent on one another. In this vein, itis argued that the negativity bias effect is contingenton the diagnosticity of the cues involved. Thus, in addi-tion to H3b, it is posited that, if a high-scope cue (i.e.,brand reputation) has negative valence, negativity biasdominates consumers’ evaluations and the negativityof the high-scope cue attenuates the interaction effectof low-scope cues, regardless of their valence.

H3b: Under low brand reputation, the interactioneffect of price and warranty on quality per-ception is not significantly different amongconsistent or inconsistent cue pairs (regard-less of their valence).

Method

Design and Participants. A 2 (low/high brand repu-tation) " 2 (weak/strong warranty) " 2 (low/high price)

between-subjects design was used to test the hypothe-ses. The sample consisted of 182 (55% male) undergrad-uates (juniors and seniors in the marketing major) whoparticipated in the experiment for course credit. Partic-ipants were randomly assigned to one of the eight cellswith each cell containing from 21 to 24 participants.

Procedure. In the experimental task, participantswere asked to evaluate and provide their thoughts andfeelings about a hypothetical car brand (brand XYZ).Brand XYZ had specific brand reputation, warranty,and price features with respect to the industry aver-age values explained in a car purchase scenario. Be-fore reading the scenario, participants were asked somequestions in order to understand their self-efficacy inpurchasing a car. In addition, they were asked to writedown the number of cars (including their current car)they have owned or used as their own cars in the past.On a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree),the average self-efficacy of participants was 4.9 and theaverage number of cars owned was 3. Next, the “De-cision Making Scenario for a Car Purchase” was intro-duced. In the scenario, participants were asked to imag-ine that they had just graduated from college, founda well-paying job, and decided to purchase a compactsedan car. In their search process, they came across sev-eral brands of compact sedan cars and included someof them in their consideration set. They were told toassume that brands in their consideration set have al-most identical technical specifications and, hence, theycould not decide on one brand over another based onthese specifications. As they read the scenario and an-swered survey questions, they were told to imagine thatbrand XYZ with specific market-related attributes asdescribed in the scenario was one of the brands in theirconsideration set.

Manipulation Checks. The description of brandXYZ’s market-related attributes included the repu-tation, warranty, and price manipulations. Manipu-lations were presented with respect to the indus-try average values to eliminate the possibility thatparticipants might not be aware of whether manipu-lated values are in the low or high range. To emphasizemanipulations with respect to their industry averagevalues, they were also depicted in a table following thescenario. Brand reputation was manipulated by tellingparticipants how previous models of the same brandwere rated by Consumer Reports. Similar to the pro-cedure used by Boulding and Kirmani (1993), partici-pants in the high (low) brand reputation condition weretold that brand XYZ had been rated above (below) av-erage, with a value of 4.8 (1.5) compared to an industryaverage value of 3.3, by Consumer Reports. They werealso told that brand reputation is measured on a scaleof 1–5, with 1 = low and 5 = high. As in Purohit andSrivastava’s (2001) study, warranty was manipulatedrelative to the average industry coverage in terms ofnumber of years and mileage amount. Participants inthe strong (weak) warranty condition were told that

EFFECTIVENESS OF MARKETING CUES ON PERCEPTIONS OF QUALITY 79Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 5: pm.pdf

brand XYZ offers a warranty of 10 years/100,000 (3years/36,000) miles, which is better (worse) than theindustry average of 6 years/60,000 miles. Finally, pricewas also manipulated relative to the industry aver-age value for compact sedan cars. For the high (low)price condition, participants were told that brand XYZhas a price tag of $30,000 ($9900), which is above(below) the industry average of $20,000. All the ma-nipulation levels were chosen at extreme levels thatstill fell within a feasible range of real industry val-ues (Boulding & Kirmani, 1993). After reading the sce-nario, participants were asked to type their thoughtsand feelings about brand XYZ and complete a ques-tionnaire that collected information on the dependentmeasure, manipulation checks, and standard demo-graphics. The entire task took about 20 minutes tocomplete.

Dependent Measure. Participants’ perception ofproduct quality was measured by five items on a 7-point scale adopted from Purohit and Srivastava (2001)as follows (Cronbach’s ! = 0.97):! Brand XYZ is most likely going to be of high

quality, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7(strongly agree).! Brand XYZ is likely to be reliable, ranging from 1(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).! I would worry about the quality of brand XYZ,ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (stronglyagree).! Compared to the other car brands, the quality ofbrand XYZ is much worse (1) to much better (7).! My overall impression of brand XYZ’s quality isvery bad (1) to very good (7).

A factor analysis confirmed that the five items loadedon a single factor.

Results

Regarding manipulation checks, participants’ percep-tions about brand reputation, warranty, and price ofbrand XYZ were measured in two different ways. First,participants were asked to rate reputation, warranty,and price (i.e., “Please rate the brand reputation, war-ranty, and price of brand XYZ as described in the sce-nario.”) on a 3-point scale (i.e., low, average, or high).Second, participants were asked to write down repu-tation, warranty, and price attributes of brand XYZas they recalled them from the scenario. As manip-ulations were presented with respect to the industryaverage values, participants were also asked to rateand write down the industry average reputation, war-ranty, and price information as they recalled it from thescenario.

A 2 " 2 " 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) showedthat perceptions of brand reputation, warranty, andprice were significantly affected by the manipulations.Participants in the high-reputation condition recalled

Table 1. ANOVA Results—Study 1.

Effects df F Value Partial "2 Power

Reputation (R) 1 714.12### 0.804 1.00Warranty (W) 1 31.27### 0.152 1.00Price (P) 1 15.87### 0.084 0.98R " W 1 9.24### 0.050 0.86R " P 1 3.70## 0.021 0.48W " P 1 4.45## 0.025 0.56R " W " P 1 1.24† 0.007 0.20Overall model 7 112.19### 0.820 1.00Residual 174Number of observation 182Adjusted R2 0.81

###p < 0.01; ##p < 0.05; †n.s.

the brand with higher reputation compared to thosein the low brand reputation condition (M = 2.71 vs.M = 1.27; F(1, 180) = 289.23, p < 0.001). Participants inthe strong warranty condition recalled the brand’s war-ranty as being greater than participants in the weakwarranty condition did (M = 2.79 vs. M = 1.27; F(1,176) = 383.07, p < 0.001). Also, participants in thehigh price condition recalled the brand as having ahigher price than participants in the low price condi-tion did (M = 2.69 vs. M = 1.50; F(1, 176) = 124.98, p <

0.001). Furthermore, it was observed that, in the recallquestions, approximately 85.3% of participants recalledmanipulations correctly. Finally, a 2 " 2 " 2 ANOVAdesign used to check for interactions found that, whenthe dependent variable is the manipulation for repu-tation; the only significant variable was the reputationcue (similar results for warranty and price). No othersignificant main or interaction effects of other extrin-sic cues on reputation were found (similar results forwarranty and price).

The ANOVA results (Table 1) indicated significantmain effects of reputation F(1, 174) = 714.12, p < 0.01),warranty F(1, 174) = 31.27, p < 0.01), and price F(1,174) = 15.87, p < 0.01) as well as interaction effectsbetween reputation and warranty F(1, 174) = 9.24,p < 0.01) and reputation and price F(1, 170) = 3.70,p < 0.05) on perceived quality. From partial eta-squared values ("2), it was observed that, compared toprice (partial "2 = 0.084) and warranty (partial "2 =0.152), reputation explained a greater portion of the to-tal variance (partial "2 = 0.804) in the perceived quality.This result supported the role of brand reputation as ahigher scope cue than that of price and warranty.

Perceived quality means as a function of manipu-lated variables are displayed in Table 2, and the resultsare graphically depicted in Figure 1. To test the hypoth-esized relationships, a series of planned contrasts wereused. Previous literature has discussed that plannedcontrasts are more efficient than F-tests in testing hy-potheses that predict differences in specific cell means(Kirk, 1995; Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1985). In the currentstudy, H1 states that when a brand has a high (vs. low)reputation, an increase in warranty results in higherperceptions of quality among consumers. As planned

80 AKDENIZ, CALANTONE, AND VOORHEESPsychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 6: pm.pdf

Table 2. Mean Perceived Quality Values and Planned Contrasts—Study 1.

High Reputation Low Reputation

Warranty

Strong Weak Strong Weak

Price M SD M SD M SD M SD

High 6.04 0.44 5.46 0.72 2.76 0.59 2.60 0.93[1] [2] [3] [4]

Low 5.75 0.53 4.48 1.08 2.65 0.55 2.27 0.59[5] [6] [7] [8]

Contrasts for testing hypothesesH1: {[1] + [5] > [2] + [6]}### and {[3] + [7] = [4] + [8]}†

H2: {[1] + [2] > [5] + [6]}### and {[3] + [4] = [7] + [8]}†

H3a: {[1] > [2]}### and {[1] > [5]}## and {[2] = [6]###} and {[5] = [6]}###

H3b: {[3] = [4]}† and {[3] = [7]}† and {[7] = [8]}† and {[4] = [8]}†

Note: M = mean; SD = standard deviation.Numbers in brackets denote the treatment cells.###p < 0.01; ##p < 0.05; †n.s.

Figure 1. Plot of planned contrasts—Study 1. (a) H1, (b) H2, (c) H3a, (d) H3b.

contrasts in Table 2 and Figure 1a revealed, partic-ipants in the high brand reputation cells perceivedquality to be significantly higher with stronger war-ranty offers (M = 5.89 vs. M = 4.97; F(1, 174) = 36.53,p < 0.01). However, for low reputation, an increase inwarranty did not imply higher perceptions of brandquality. It was found that participants did not per-ceive quality significantly higher when it was sold viastronger warranty under the low reputation condition(M = 2.70 vs. M = 2.43; F(1, 174) = 0.48, n.s.). There-fore, H1 is supported.

The data also confirmed H2, suggesting that when abrand had high reputation, consumers were more likelyto perceive higher quality with high prices (M = 5.75vs. M = 5.12; F(1, 174) = 17.89, p < 0.01). However,for low reputation brands, perceptions of quality werenot necessarily higher with higher prices (M = 2.68 vs.M = 2.46; F(1, 174) = 2.56, n.s.; see Figure 1b).

H3a and H3b propose a three-way interaction amongbrand reputation, price, and warranty. Planned con-trasts (Table 2) and Figure 1c depict that, when brandreputation was high, high price had a higher effect on

EFFECTIVENESS OF MARKETING CUES ON PERCEPTIONS OF QUALITY 81Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 7: pm.pdf

perceived quality when warranty was stronger (Strong= 6.04, Weak = 5.46; F(1, 174) = 25.58, p < 0.01). Also,under high reputation, strong warranty had a higher ef-fect on perceived quality when price was higher (High =6.04, Low = 5.75; F(1, 174) = 4.04, p < 0.05). Therefore,the results revealed a higher perceived quality withconsistent pair cues. Nevertheless, the study findingssuggested that inconsistent pairs still led to a higherperceived quality than the low price–weak warrantypair under a high reputation. Hence, H3a was par-tially supported. Specifically, under a high reputation,strong warranty–low price had a higher effect on per-ceived quality than weak warranty–low price (Strong= 5.75 Weak = 4.48; F(1, 174) = 10.90, p < 0.01)while high price–weak warranty had a higher effecton perceived quality than the low price–weak warrantypair (High = 5.46, Low = 4.48; F(1, 174) = 13.24, p< 0.01). Overall, under the high brand reputation sit-uation, the findings suggested a significantly higherperceived quality for brands with consistently positivecues (i.e., high price and strong warranty). Yet theyimplied that, when cues were inconsistent, perceivedquality could still be significantly higher than that ofthe low price–weak warranty condition. Finally, for thelow reputation condition, as H3b predicted, no signifi-cant interaction effects (all Fs < 1, n.s.) were found (seeTable 1d).

STUDY 2: EFFECTS OF THIRD-PARTYRATINGS, WARRANTY, AND PRICE ONPERCEIVED QUALITY

Hypothesis Development

When consumers evaluate between multiple productalternatives, the only available cues to infer aboutquality are not marketing-related cues. Increases ininformation-sharing channels along with the rise of In-ternet and social media tools have triggered the emer-gence of intermediate information sources. Different in-fomediaries have made it easier for customers to learnabout product features and quality before making apurchase. In this research, product-related cues notnecessarily controlled by marketing but accessible invarious infomediaries are referred to as third-party in-formation. Third-party information pertains to experi-ences with products or companies and can include firmsacross multiple industries (e.g., Consumer Reports) orcan be industry-specific (e.g., edmunds.com; Benedick-tus, Brady, Darke, & Voorhees, 2010).

The primary role of third-party information is to helpconsumers cope with uncertainty in their purchase de-cisions. Third-party information, including expert rat-ings, opinions, consumer reviews, and certification pro-grams, has become increasingly popular in affectingcustomers’ inferences and decisions. A recent surveyfound that 86% of consumers consult reviews beforemaking a major purchase decision, and 90% of these

consumers trust third-party reviews (Miller, 2008). Tosome extent, previous research has examined the in-fluence of third-party information on product or firmperformance (Duan, Gu, & Whinston, 2005; Eliashberg& Shugan, 1997; Shaffer & Zettelmeyer, 2002) and itsrole on the effectiveness of marketing cues from a man-ufacturer’s point of view (Basuroy, Desai, & Talukdar,2006; Blair & Innis, 1996; Chang & Wildt, 1996; Chen &Xie, 2005; Moon, Bergey, & Iacobucci, 2010). However,from a consumer’s point of view, despite the abundanceand popularity of third-party product information, em-pirical research examining the moderating role of third-party product information on the impact of marketingcues on consumer perceptions remains very limited.

The purpose of Study 2 is to fill this gap in the lit-erature by examining the interactions between third-party information and marketing cues on consumers’perceptions of quality. One stream of research suggeststhat the existence of relevant third-party informationattenuates the effect of marketing cues on consumerquality perception (Albrecht, 1981; Basuroy, Desai, &Talukdar, 2006). Another stream proposes a hedonicassumption suggesting that the correlation betweenmarketing cues and perceived quality increases withthe introduction of third-party information. For exam-ple, results have demonstrated that, in the presenceof positive third-party reviews, the effects of price andadvertising on perceived quality increases (Archibald,Haulman, & Moody, 1983). Despite the mixed results, itis evident that, when third-party product-related infor-mation is made available, consumers are willing to pro-cess it along with other marketing-related attributes ofthe product.

Previous research has indicated that independentthird-party product information from a credible sourceis often perceived as being more credible and less biasedthan marketing cues (Darke, Bohner, Einwiller, Erb,& Hazlewood, 1998). A trustworthy and independentthird-party agent has no reason to give a product a high-quality rating when it is actually of low quality as doingso would hurt the agent’s credibility in consumers’ eyes.In this study, third-party quality ratings from an inde-pendent and highly credible source (i.e., J. D. Powerquality ratings) are used as a proxy to third-party in-formation, functioning as a high-scope cue (Benedick-tus et al., 2010). Furthermore, cue-consistency theorysuggests that third-party information and marketingcues will have a positive effect on quality perceptionswhen they provide corroborating versus disparate infor-mation. Thus, it is posited that consumers perceive ahigher product quality with a better warranty or higherprice when the third-party information about the prod-uct is credible and has a positive valence.

H4: Third-party ratings moderate the effect ofwarranty on quality perception such thatunder high (vs. low) ratings, the effect of astronger (vs. weaker) warranty is higher onperceived quality.

82 AKDENIZ, CALANTONE, AND VOORHEESPsychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 8: pm.pdf

H5: Third-party ratings moderate the effect ofprice on quality perception such that underhigh (vs. low) ratings, the effect of a higher(vs. lower) price is higher on perceived qual-ity.

Drawing from the driving mechanism with cue-consistency and negativity bias in Study 1, a three-wayinteraction among third-party quality ratings, price,and warranty is proposed. It is posited that the interac-tion of low-scope cues (i.e., price and warranty), to theextent that they provide consistent (vs. inconsistent) in-formation, is higher on quality perceptions under highthird-party quality ratings. Moreover, when they pro-vide inconsistent information, as the more negative cuedominates consumer perceptions, the effect of inconsis-tent pairs will not be significantly different from thelow price–low warranty pair.

H6a: Under high third-party ratings, the interac-tion effect of price and warranty on qual-ity perception is stronger when they provideconsistent (i.e., high price–strong warranty)versus inconsistent (i.e., high price–weakwarranty or low price–strong warranty) in-formation.

Similar to Study 1, when third-party ratings havenegative valence, the negativity bias will hold and thenegative impact of third-party ratings will attenuatethe interaction effect of price and warranty.

H6b: Under low third-party ratings, the interac-tion effect of price and warranty on qual-ity perception is not significantly differentamong consistent or inconsistent cue pairs(regardless of their valence).

Method

Design and Participants. A 2 (low/high third-partyrating) " 2 (weak/strong warranty) " 2 (low/high price)between-subjects design was used to test the hypothe-ses. The sample consisted of 178 (61% male) undergrad-uates (juniors and seniors in the marketing major) whoparticipated in the experiment for course credit. Par-ticipants were randomly assigned to one of eight cells,with each cell containing from 20 to 25 participants.

Procedure. In this study, third-party rating, war-ranty, and price of the car brand (brand XYZ) weremanipulated. The experimental task was similar toStudy 1, in which participants were asked to evaluateand provide their thoughts and feelings of a hypothet-ical car brand with specific quality rating, warranty,and price features explained in the car purchase sce-nario. Before reading the scenario, participants were

asked some questions in order to understand their self-efficacy in purchasing a car. They were also asked towrite down the number of cars (including their currentcar) they have owned or used as their own cars in thepast. On a scale of 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (stronglyagree), the average self-efficacy of participants was 4.7and the average number of cars owned was 2.6.

Manipulation Checks. In the car purchase scenario,manipulations were presented with respect to the in-dustry average values to eliminate the possibility thatparticipants might not be aware of whether the manip-ulated values are in the low or high range. Participantsin the high (low) quality rating condition were told thatbrand XYZ had been rated by J. D. Power with a valueof 6 (2), which is higher (lower) than the industry av-erage value of 4. They were also told that quality ismeasured on a 7-point scale (1 = poor, 7 = excellentquality). Warranty and price manipulations were keptthe same. Similar to Study 1, after reading the scenario,participants were asked to type their thoughts and feel-ings about brand XYZ and complete a questionnairethat collected information on the dependent measure,manipulation checks, and standard demographics. Theentire task took about 20 minutes to complete. In orderto ensure the independence of the samples across thetwo studies, students who had already completed Study1 were not allowed to participate in Study 2 and wereprovided with an alternative extra credit study.

Dependent Measure. Similar to Study 1, the depen-dent variable was perceived quality, which was mea-sured by averaging the five items (Cronbach’s ! = 0.97)adopted from Purohit and Srivastava (2001). Again, fac-tor analysis confirmed that the five items loaded on asingle factor.

Results

Regarding manipulation checks, participants’ percep-tions about third-party rating, warranty, and price ofbrand XYZ were measured in two different ways. First,participants were asked to rate third-party rating, war-ranty, and price (i.e., “Please rate the third-party rat-ing, warranty, and price of brand XYZ as described inthe scenario.”) on a 3-point scale (i.e., low, average, orhigh). Second, participants were asked to write downthird-party rating, warranty, and price attributes ofbrand XYZ as they recalled them from the scenario. Asmanipulations were presented with respect to industryaverage values, participants were also asked to rateand write down the industry third-party rating, war-ranty, and price information as they recalled it from thescenario.

A 2 " 2 " 2 ANOVA showed that perceptions of third-party rating, warranty, and price were significantly af-fected by the manipulations. Participants in the highcondition recalled the brand as having a higher third-party rating than in the low condition (M = 2.88 vs. M =

EFFECTIVENESS OF MARKETING CUES ON PERCEPTIONS OF QUALITY 83Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 9: pm.pdf

Table 3. ANOVA Results—Study 2.

Effects df F value Partial "2 Power

Third-party rating (TPR) 1 457.18### 0.729 1.00Warranty (W) 1 2.61† 0.015 0.36Price (P) 1 11.72### 0.064 0.93TPR " W 1 3.79## 0.022 0.49TPR " P 1 9.18### 0.051 0.85W " P 1 2.05† 0.012 0.29TPR " W " P 1 1.14† 0.007 0.18Overall model 7 72.97### 0.750 1.00Residual 170Number of observation 178Adjusted R2 0.74

###p < 0.01; ##p < 0.05; †n.s.

1.04; F(1, 176) = 2087.64, p < 0.001). Participants in thestrong warranty condition recalled the brand as havinga better warranty than in the weak warranty condition(M = 2.93 vs. M = 1.16; F(1, 176) = 1272.48, p < 0.001).Participants in the high condition recalled the brandas having a higher price than in the low condition (M= 2.86 vs. M = 1.22; F(1, 176) = 398.77, p < 0.001).Furthermore, it was observed that more than 91% ofthe participants recalled the manipulations correctly.Finally, a 2 " 2 " 2 ANOVA was used to check for inter-actions; it was found that, when the dependent variablewas the manipulation for third-party rating, the onlysignificant variable was the rating cue (similar resultsfor warranty and price). No other significant main orinteraction effects of other cues on third-party ratingwere found (similar results for warranty and price).

The ANOVA results (Table 3) indicated significantmain effects of third-party rating (F(1, 170) = 457.18,p < 0.01) and price (F(1, 170) = 11.72, p < 0.01) aswell as interaction effects between third-party ratingand warranty (F(1, 170) = 3.79, p < 0.05) and third-party rating and price (F(1, 170) = 9.18, p < 0.01).From partial "2 values, it was observed that, comparedto price (partial "2 = 0.064) and warranty (partial "2 =0.015), third-party rating explains a greater portion ofthe total variance (partial "2 = 0.729) in the perceivedquality. This result supported the role of third-partyrating as a more credible cue than that of price andwarranty.

Perceived quality means as a function of manipu-lated variables are displayed in Table 4; the resultsare graphically depicted in Figure 2. To test the hy-pothesized relationships, similar to Study 1, a series ofplanned contrasts were used. H4 states that, when abrand has high (vs. low) third-party quality rating, anincrease in warranty results in higher perceptions ofquality. As planned contrasts in Table 4 and Figure 2arevealed, participants in high third-party rating cellsperceived brand quality to be significantly higher withstronger warranty offers (M = 5.51 vs. M = 5.03; F(1,170) = 5.95, p < 0.05). However, with low third-partyratings, an increase in warranty did not necessarilylead to a higher perceived quality (M = 2.30 vs. M =2.34; F(1, 170) = 0.06, n.s.). Therefore, H4 is supported.

The data also confirmed H5, indicating that if thebrand has high (vs. low) third-party quality rating,consumers are more likely to have higher perceptionsof quality with above industry average prices. Withhigh third-party rating, consumers’ perception of brandquality was higher with high prices (M = 5.72 vs. M =4.82; F(1, 170) = 19.5, p < 0.01); however, with lowthird-party rating, high prices did not lead to higherperceptions of quality (M = 2.35 vs. M = 2.29; F(1, 170)= 0.08, n.s.; see Figure 2b).

H6a and H6b propose a three-way interaction amongthird-party rating, warranty, and price. Planned con-trasts (Table 4) and Figure 2c revealed that, under highthird-party rating, high price had a higher effect on per-ceived quality when the warranty was stronger (Strong= 5.79, Weak = 5.65; F(1, 170) = 4.84, p < 0.05). Inaddition, a strong warranty had a higher effect on per-ceived quality when price was higher (High = 5.79, Low= 5.24; F(1, 170) = 5.21, p < 0.05). Therefore, with high-quality ratings, the data revealed a higher perceivedquality with consistent pair cues compared to inconsis-tent pairs. However, for the comparison of the inconsis-tent pairs with the low price–weak warranty conditionunder high third-party ratings, the data showed thathigh price–weak warranty had a higher effect on per-ceived quality than the low price–weak warranty pair(High = 5.65, Low = 4.41; F(1, 170) = 14.82, p < 0.01),although the effect of low price–strong warranty on per-ceived quality was not significantly different from thatof weak warranty–low price (Strong = 5.24, Weak =4.41; F < 1, n.s.). Therefore, the findings partially sup-ported H6a. Finally, under low third-party ratings, asH6b predicts, no significant interaction effects (all Fs <

1, n.s.) were found (see Table 2d).

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION

In markets where product quality is not easily observ-able, consumers rely on available product cues to in-fer product quality and cope with uncertainty in theirdecision making. Consumers are generally exposed tomultiple-quality cues, which seldom operate in isola-tion. Despite this, prior research examining how con-sumers process and integrate various types of cues intheir decision making has been scarce, producing equiv-ocal results. Therefore, the purpose of this research wasto build a conceptual framework to examine how mul-tiple cues interact with each other in consumers’ qual-ity perceptions. In order to examine rather complex in-teraction relationships, cues were first categorized asmarketing controlled (e.g., price, warranty, and brandreputation) and nonmarketing controlled (e.g., third-party information). Second, based on prior research,cues were classified as high- or low-scope cues based onthe diagnosticity of the cue.

Two experimental studies were used to examinetwo- and three-way interactions among various typesof cues. Study 1 analyzed the interaction effects of mul-tiple marketing-controlled cues (i.e., brand reputation,

84 AKDENIZ, CALANTONE, AND VOORHEESPsychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 10: pm.pdf

Table 4. Mean Perceived Quality Values and Planned Contrasts— Study 2.

High Third-Party Rating Low Third-Party Rating

Warranty

Strong Weak Strong Weak

Price M SD M SD M SD M SD

High 5.79 0.73 5.65 0.45 2.31 0.82 2.40 1.07[1] [2] [3] [4]

Low 5.24 0.94 4.41 1.33 2.30 0.85 2.29 0.95[5] [6] [7] [8]

Contrasts for testing hypothesesH4: {[1] + [5] > [2] + [6]}## and {[3] + [7] = [4] + [8]}†

H5: {[1] + [2] > [5] + [6]}### and {[3] + [4] = [7] + [8]}†

H6a: {[1] > [2]}## and {[1] > [5]}## and {[2] = [6]###} and {[5] = [6]}†

H6b: {[3] = [4]}† and {[3] = [7]}† and {[7] = [8]}† and {[4] = [8]}†

Note: M = mean; SD = standard deviation.Numbers in brackets denote the treatment cells.###p < 0.01; ##p < 0.05; †n.s.

Figure 2. Plot of planned contrasts—Study 2. (a) H4, (b) H5, (c) H6a, (d) H6b.

price, and warranty) on consumers’ quality perceptions.Regarding the two-way interactions, the results pro-vide strong support for cue-diagnosticity theory suchthat a high-scope cue such as brand reputation posi-tively moderates the effects of lower scope cues suchas price and warranty. Specifically, the data suggestedthat consumers will perceive stronger warranty (orhigher price) claims as being more credible when sentby a manufacturer with a high brand reputation. Fur-thermore, the data indicated that consumers did notperceive a higher product quality with a stronger war-

ranty (or higher price) when brand reputation was low.These findings also provide additional support for priorresearch on the interaction of high- and low-scope cues(e.g., Purohit & Srivastava, 2001) such that positive(negative) implications suggested by high-scope cuesare expected to transmit over to the low-scope cues,making them more (less) diagnostic. In other words,consumers perceive that, although a manufacturer witha high reputation is less likely to make false claims viamarketing cues as this will weaken its credibility; amanufacturer with a low reputation is more likely to

EFFECTIVENESS OF MARKETING CUES ON PERCEPTIONS OF QUALITY 85Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 11: pm.pdf

send false promises as it does not have much to lose.Actually, in most cases involving a manufacturer withlow reputation, consumers tend to infer higher pricesor better warranty cues as an offsetting mechanism forlow reputation.

In addition to investigating these two-way interac-tions, the current research extended the literature byaccounting for a possibly more complex set of relation-ships among product cues. Regarding three-way inter-actions among brand reputation, warranty, and price,the study findings provided interesting implications forcue-diagnosticity and cue-consistency theories as wellas negativity bias. First, under a high brand repu-tation, consumers perceive a higher level of productquality with the interaction of consistent and a high-valence pair of low-scope cues (i.e., high price–strongwarranty) than with the interaction of inconsistent paircues (i.e., high price–weak warranty or low price–strongwarranty). In line with H1 and H2, this specific resultof three-way interaction supports cue-diagnosticity aswell as cue-consistency theory, which asserts that mul-tiple sources of information are more useful when theyprovide corroborating information than when they of-fer disparate information. Second, the research focusedon perceived quality differences between inconsistentpairs and the consistent but low-valence pair of low-scope cues. Negativity bias predicts that, when cuesprovide inconsistent information, the more negative cuedominates consumer perceptions; hence, the effect ofinconsistent pairs is not be significantly different fromlow price–weak warranty pair. However, the resultssuggested that, when cues are inconsistent, consumers’perceptions of quality can still be significantly higherthan those in the low price–weak warranty condition.

This specific result constitutes a significant contribu-tion to previous research and cue-diagnosticity and neg-ativity bias theories. Miyazaki Grewal, and Goodstein(2005) found that, in a two-way interaction frameworkwhen cues were inconsistent, quality evaluations werenot significantly different from the condition whereboth cues were of low valence. In the current research,the two-way interaction was extended into a three-wayinteraction by examining it under high (vs. low) brandreputation. Second, negativity bias may become biasedif it does not explicitly account for the diagnosticity ofthe information provided (i.e., high- vs. low-scope cue).In this case, the preliminary evidence from a three-wayinteraction showed that a high-scope cue could offsetthe possible negativity bias caused by a negative low-scope cue in an inconsistent pair. It is possible thatthe diagnosticity of a high-scope cue is so powerful thata single negative low-scope cue cannot limit the im-pact of a high-scope cue on quality perceptions in theconsumer’s eyes. Therefore, under a high brand repu-tation, a strong warranty can still lead to higher per-ceived quality with low prices or a high price can stilllead to higher perceived quality with weaker warrantythan the low price–weak warranty pair. However, inthe presence of two negative low-scope cues, the effect

of a positive high-scope cue is reduced to the extent thatoverall evaluations of perceived quality erode.

Third, this research examined the perceived qualitydifferences among various cue pairs under low brandreputation. This time, the negativity bias emergingfrom the high-scope cue dominated consumers’ eval-uations and led to an attenuation of perceived qualityoverall. More specifically, consumers did not perceiveproduct quality differently between consistent or in-consistent price and warranty pairs (regardless of theirvalence) when these cues were sent by a manufacturerwith a low reputation.

Study 2 analyzed interaction effects betweennonmarketing-controlled (i.e., third-party information)and marketing-controlled product cues (i.e., price andwarranty) on consumer quality perception. In terms ofthe two-way interactions, the results confirmed thatthird-party ratings from an independent and crediblesource positively moderated the effects of lower scopecues as price and warranty. Specifically, consumers per-ceived stronger warranty (higher price) claims leadingto higher perceived product quality when supported byhigh third-party quality ratings by J. D. Power. Fur-thermore, consumers did not perceive a higher productquality with a stronger warranty (or higher price) offerwhen the product did not have high third-party ratings.Drawing on Benedicktus et al. (2010), independent andcredible third-party information was expected to oper-ate as another high-scope cue in this research context.Although not necessarily similar to brand reputationcue, it has similar properties to high-scope cues. Forinstance, a trustworthy third-party agent has no rea-son to give a product a high-quality rating when it isactually of low quality as, if anything, doing so willhurt the agent’s credibility in consumers’ eyes. Fur-thermore, prior research shows that independent third-party product information from a credible source is of-ten perceived as being more credible and less biasedthan marketing cues (Darke et al., 1998). Results fortwo-way interactions suggested preliminary evidencefor the high-scope role of third-party information. Fur-thermore, they confirmed cue-consistency in the sensethat third-party information and marketing cues posi-tively affected quality perceptions when they providedsupporting versus contradictory information.

As in Study 1, three-way interactions among third-party ratings, price, and warranty were examined inStudy 2; the results showed that that, under high third-party ratings, consumers perceived a higher perceivedproduct quality with the interaction of consistent andhigh-valence pair of low-scope cues (i.e., high price–strong warranty) than with the interaction of incon-sistent pair cues (i.e., high price–weak warranty or lowprice–strong warranty). Similar to H1 and H2, this spe-cific finding lent support to both cue-diagnosticity andcue-consistency theories. In addition, perceived qualitydifferences between inconsistent pairs and the consis-tent but low-valence pair of low-scope cues were ex-amined. Consumers still perceived a higher perceived

86 AKDENIZ, CALANTONE, AND VOORHEESPsychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 12: pm.pdf

quality with high price–weak warranty than the lowprice–weak warranty condition. Although negativitybias predicted no significant difference between incon-sistent pairs and the consistent low-valence pair, whenhigh-scope features of third-party ratings were takeninto account, they made up for the potential nega-tivity bias created by a weaker warranty in the highprice–weak warranty pair. An interesting finding wasthat the effect of low price–strong warranty on per-ceived quality was not significantly different from thatof weak warranty–low price. In this case, no offsettingmechanism of third-party ratings on the negative va-lence of price was found. This finding potentially intro-duces another contingency condition for the operationof negativity bias. Negativity bias has to account fornot only the difference between high-scope and low-scope cues, but also the difference between how con-sumers perceive certain low-scope cues. Finally, sim-ilar relationships under low third-party ratings wereexamined. Consumers did not perceive product qualitydifferently among consistent or inconsistent price andwarranty and price pairs (regardless of their valence)when the product itself received low-quality ratings bya reputable third party.

Managerial Implications

The findings from this research offer both theoreti-cal and managerial implications. Study 1 and Study2 demonstrated that brand reputation and quality rat-ings of a brand had a greater impact on consumer qual-ity perceptions than low-scope marketing cues. In theAmerican auto industry, the importance of brand namecredibility has been a concern, as evident in one of Gen-eral Motor’s studies of the Saturn brand. In BusinessWeek’s February 26, 2009 issue, the damage to Saturn’sbrand image was explained via an experiment that thecompany conducted. When the automaker showed buy-ers the Aura family sedan model of Saturn with thenameplate removed, the car got a score of 3.4 of 4.0; withthe Saturn badge on the hood, the same car scored only2.0 (Welch, 2009). Consumers’ confidence in brands andthe quality of cars has been a great issue with mostautomakers.

Another interesting implication is that perceptionsof quality were not affected by high warranty offeringswhen the reputation or the quality rating of the manu-facturer was low. Therefore, without investing in brandreputation or actual quality of a product, provision of anabove industry average warranty strategy can backfirebecause extended warranty strategy claims may resultin consumers to perceiving the brand to be advertisingits quality and distinguishing itself from other brandsbased on warranty instead of emphasizing the real in-surance role of warranty against failure (e.g., Cooper& Ross, 1985). The same is true for the pricing strat-egy. Increasing price tags for new models replacing oldones (e.g., Buick Enclave vs. Buick Rendezvous) or in-creasing prices to compete in the luxury segment (e.g.,

Hyundai Genesis) should also be supported by the over-all reputation of the brand name or the quality imageof the manufacturer.

Considering the main effects of price and warrantyon quality perceptions, this research demonstrated thatprice had a more consistent signaling role than war-ranty in both studies. In addition, in three-way inter-actions, it was found that high-scope features of third-party ratings could not offset the negativity bias cre-ated by the low price–strong warranty condition onperceived quality. However, the credibility of warrantyvaried from context to context. In an environment withcredible third-party product information, the individ-ual significance of warranty seemed to be undermined.However, the warranty cue is perceived as a diagnosticwhen it interacts with the high third-party quality rat-ings. Overall, this result by itself emphasizes the inter-play between marketing-controlled and nonmarketing-controlled cues and suggests that consumers may ag-gregate perceptions across multiple existing cues whenmaking global quality evaluations.

Limitations and Future Research

As with any study, this research has some limitations,which provide avenues for future research. First, thisresearch used simple stimuli and an artificial setting.To serve the objectives with clarity, manipulations werekept at a simple level compared to an actual car pur-chase. Second, the experiments were conducted withstudent participants. Third, the product category ofcars was chosen as quality is an indispensable at-tribute of cars, yet it is not always easy to observe be-fore making a purchase. However, for college studentparticipants, car buying might not be the most rele-vant context. In future research, more relevant partici-pants with higher self-efficiency in buying cars might beselected.

The results provided strong support for existing the-oretical conjectures. They also provided preliminaryevidence of the contingency of negativity bias effectswhen cue-diagnosticity was taken into account. In fu-ture studies, the dynamics between cue-diagnosticity,cue-consistency, and negativity effects might be testedwith more complex stimuli and other product cate-gories. Furthermore, in addition to perceived quality,other components of consumers’ decision-making pro-cess, such as perceived risk, anticipated satisfaction,and purchase intentions are worth examining as depen-dent measures in the proposed theoretical frameworkproposed in this research.

REFERENCES

Ahluwalia, R. (2002). How prevalent is the negativity effect inconsumer environments? Journal of Consumer Research,29, 270–279.

Albrecht, J. W. (1981). A procedure for testing the signalinghypothesis. Journal of Public Economics, 15, 123–132.

EFFECTIVENESS OF MARKETING CUES ON PERCEPTIONS OF QUALITY 87Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 13: pm.pdf

Anderson, N. H. (1965). Averaging vs. adding as a stimulus-combination rule in impression formation. Journal of Ex-perimental Psychology, 70, 394–400.

Archibald, R. B., Haulman, C. A., & Moody, C. E., Jr. (1983).Quality, price, advertising, and published quality ratings.Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 347–356.

Baek, T. H., Kim, J., & Yu, J. H. (2010). The differential rolesof brand credibility and brand prestige in consumer brandchoice. Psychology & Marketing, 27, 662–678.

Basuroy, S., Desai, K. K., & Talukdar, D. (2006). An empiricalinvestigation of signaling in the motion picture industry.Journal of Marketing Research, 43, 287–295.

Benedicktus, R. L., Brady, M. K., Darke P. R., & Voorhees, C.M. (2010). Conveying trustworthiness to online consumers:Reactions to consensus, physical store presence, brand fa-miliarity, and generalized suspicion. Journal of Retailing,86, 322–335.

Blair, M. E., & Innis, D. E. (1996). The effects of product knowl-edge on the evaluation of warranted brands. Psychology &Marketing, 13, 445–456.

Boulding, W., & Kirmani, A. (1993). A consumer-side experi-mental examination of signaling theory: Do consumers per-ceive warranties as signals of quality? Journal of ConsumerResearch, 20, 111–123.

Brucks, M., Zeithaml, V. A., & Naylor, G. (2000). Price andbrand name as indicators of quality dimensions for con-sumer durables. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sci-ence, 28, 359–374.

Chang, T., & Wildt, A. R. (1996). Impact of product informationon the use of price as a quality cue. Psychology & Market-ing, 13, 55–75.

Chen, Y., & Xie, J. (2005). Third-party product review and firmmarketing strategy. Marketing Science, 24, 218–240.

Cooper, R. and Ross, T. W. (1985). Product warranties anddouble moral hazard. The Rand Journal of Economics, 16,103–113.

Darke, S. C., Bohner, G., Einwiller, S., Erb, H. P., & Hazle-wood, J. D. (1998). Accuracy motivation, consensus infor-mation, and the law of large numbers: Effects on attitudejudgment in the absence of argumentation. Personality andSocial Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1205–1215.

Dawar, N., & Parker, P. (1994). Marketing universals: Con-sumers’ use of brand name, price, physical appearance, andretailer reputation as signals of product quality. Journal ofMarketing, 58, 81–95.

Dodds, W. B., Monroe, K. B., & Grewal, D. (1991). Effectsof price, brand, and store information on buyers’ prod-uct evaluations. Journal of Marketing Research, 28, 307–319.

Duan, W., Gu, B., & Whinston, A. B. (2005). Do on-line reviews matter? An empirical investigation ofpanel data. Retrieved August 4, 2010 as SSRN fromhttp://ssrn.com/abstract=616262.

Eliashberg, J., & Shugan, S. M. (1997). Film critics: In-fluencers or predictors? Journal of Marketing, 61, 68–78.

Erdem, T., & Swait, J. (1998). Brand equity as a signalingphenomenon. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 7, 131–157.

Erdem, T., & Swait, J. (2004). Brand credibility, brand con-sideration, and choice. Journal of Consumer Research. 31,191–198.

Erevelles, S. (1993). The price-warranty contract and prod-uct attitudes. Journal of Business Research, 27, 171–181.

Gammoh, B. S., Voss, K. E., & Chakraborty, G. (2006). Con-sumer evaluation of brand alliance signals. Psychology &Marketing, 23, 465–486.

Gidron, D., Koehler, D. J., & Tversky, A. (1993). Implicit quan-tification of personality traits. Personality and Social Psy-chology Bulletin, 19, 594–604.

Herr, P. M., Kardes, F. R., & Kim, J. (1991). Effects of word-of-mouth and product-attribute information on persuasion:An accessibility-diagnosticity perspective. Journal of Con-sumer Research, 17, 454–462.

Hoch, Stephen J., & Deighton, J. (1989). Managing what con-sumers learn from experience. Journal of Marketing, 53,1–20.

Jacoby, J., Olson, J., & Haddock, R. A. (1971). Price, brandname, and product composition characteristics as determi-nants of perceived quality. Journal of Applied Psychology,55, 570–579.

Jiang, P., Jones, D. B., & Javie, S. (2008). How third-partycertification programs relate to consumer trust in onlinetransactions: An exploratory study. Psychology & Market-ing, 25, 839–858.

Kirk, R. E. (1995). Experimental design: Procedures for thebehavioral sciences. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Kirmani, A., & Rao, A. R. (2000). No pain, no gain: A criticalreview of the literature on signaling unobservable productquality. Journal of Marketing, 64, 66–79.

Kirmani, A., & Wright, P. (1989). Money talks: Perceived ad-vertising expense and expected product quality. Journal ofConsumer Research, 16, 344–353.

Maheswaran, D., & Chaiken, S. (1991). Promoting systematicprocessing in low-motivation settings: Effect of incongru-ent information on processing and judgment. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 61, 13–25.

Miller, S. (2008). Kudzu.com survey reveals online re-views significantly impact consumer spending. (accessedon Nov 10, 2011) [available at http://www.reuters.com/article/idUS159105+09-Dec-2008+PRN20081209].

Miyazaki, A. D., Grewal, D., & Goodstein, R. C. (2005). Theeffect of multiple extrinsic cues on quality perceptions: Amatter of consistency. Journal of Consumer Research, 32,146–153.

Moon, S., Bergey, P. K., & Iacobucci, D. (2010). Dynamic effectsamong movie ratings, movie revenues, and viewer satisfac-tion. Journal of Marketing, 74, 108–121.

Purohit, D., & Srivastava, J. (2001). Effect of manufacturerreputation, retailer reputation, and product warranty onconsumer judgments of product quality: A cue diagnosticityframework. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 10, 123–134.

Rao, A. R., & Monroe, K. B. (1989). The effect of price, brandname, and store name on buyers’ perceptions of productquality: An integrative review. Journal of Marketing Re-search, 26, 351–357.

Richardson, P. S., Dick, A. S., & Jain, A. K. (1994). Extrin-sic and intrinsic cue effects on perceptions of store brandquality. Journal of Marketing, 58, 28–36.

Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1985). Contrast analysis: Fo-cused comparisons in the analysis of variance. New York:Cambridge University Press.

Shaffer, G., & Zettelmeyer, F. (2002). When good news aboutyour rival is good for you: The effect of third-party informa-tion on the division of channel profits. Marketing Science,21, 273–293.

Shimp, T. A., & Bearden, W. O. (1982). Warranty and other ex-trinsic cue effects on consumers’ risk perceptions. Journalof Consumer Research, 9, 38–46.

88 AKDENIZ, CALANTONE, AND VOORHEESPsychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

Page 14: pm.pdf

Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1987). Social judgmentand social memory: The role of cue diagnosticity in negativ-ity, positivity, and extremity biases. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 52, 689–699.

Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1971). Comparison of Bayesianand regression approaches to the study of information pro-cessing in judgment. Organizational Behavior and HumanPerformance. 6, 649–774.

Srivastava, J., & Mitra, A. (1998). Warranty as a signal ofquality: The moderating effect of consumer knowledge onquality evaluations. Marketing Letters, 9, 327–336.

Washburn, J. H., Till, B. D., & Priluck, R. (2004). Brand al-

liance and customer-based brand-equity effects. Psychology& Marketing, 21, 487–508.

Welch, D. (2009, February 26). Lessons from Saturn’s fall.Business Week.

Wernerfelt, B. (1988). Umbrella branding as a signal of newproduct quality: An example of signaling by posting a bond.Rand Journal of Economics, 19, 458–466.

Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to: BillurAkdeniz, Assistant Professor, Department of Marketing, Whit-temore School of Business and Economics, University of NewHampshire, Durham, NH 03824 ([email protected]).

EFFECTIVENESS OF MARKETING CUES ON PERCEPTIONS OF QUALITY 89Psychology and Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar