perceptual salience in the test for auditory comprehension

64
University of Montana University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 1983 Perceptual salience in the Test for Auditory Comprehension of Perceptual salience in the Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language Language Debra L. Croghan The University of Montana Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Let us know how access to this document benefits you. Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Croghan, Debra L., "Perceptual salience in the Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language" (1983). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 3195. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/3195 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Upload: others

Post on 05-Jan-2022

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

University of Montana University of Montana

ScholarWorks at University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana

Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School

1983

Perceptual salience in the Test for Auditory Comprehension of Perceptual salience in the Test for Auditory Comprehension of

Language Language

Debra L. Croghan The University of Montana

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd

Let us know how access to this document benefits you.

Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Croghan, Debra L., "Perceptual salience in the Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language" (1983). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 3195. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/3195

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected].

COPYRIGHT ACT OF 1976

THIS IS AN UNPUBLISHED MANUSCRIPT IN WHICH COPYRIGHT SUB­SISTS, ANY FURTHER REPRINTING OF ITS CONTENTS MUST BE APPROVED BY THE AUTHOR,

MANSFIELD LIBRARY UNIVERSITY OF MONTANA DATE :

Perceptual Salience in the Test for Auditory Comprehension

of Language

Debra L. Croghan B.A. University of Montana 1978

Presented in partial fulf i l lment of the requirement for the

degree of

Master of Arts University of Montana

1983

Chairman, Board of Examiners

by

Approved by

Deaff^ Graduate ScTOfff

Date

UMI Number: EP34811

All rights reserved

INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed,

a note will indicate the deletion.

UMI* Dissertation Publishing

UMI EP34811

Published by ProQuest LLC (2012). Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author.

Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against

unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code

ProQuest*

ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway

P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106- 1346

Croghan, Debra L., M.A., December 1983 Communication Sciences & Disorders

Perceptual Salience in the Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language (54)

Director: Barbara Bain, Ph.D.

The purpose of this study was to investigate: 1) whether children responded significantly differently from chance to the three pictures on a given plate of the Test For Auditory Comprehension of Language (TACL); 2) i f the Picture Potency Formula (PPF) predicted the pictures that children "l iked best"; and 3) i f incorrect responses on the TACL correlated to those chosen by children as the ones they "l iked best". Thirty subjects, between the ages of 4-5 years, served as their own control group, each receiving two different administrations of the TACL in a counter balanced order. One administration followed the proce­dures outl ined in the TACL manual; the other administration consisted of the verbal instruction: "Point to the picture you l ike best". The results indicated that the subjects did respond differently to the three pictures. They chose the middle picture the majority of the time. The PPF did not prove to be an accurate predictor of the subjects' responses. The subjects' incorrect responses did not corre­late to the ones chosen by the subjects as the ones they "l iked best" to a significant degree. Neither did the subjects appear to be influenced by the construction of the pictures nor the pictures they "l iked best" when presented with the l inguistic cues. This study generally supported the validity of the TACL.

i i

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The author expresses her thanks and appreciation to her thesis

committee Barbara Bain, Charles Parker and Wesley Shellen for their

patience and enthusiasm. The t ime and efforts of the staff, students

and parents at the Stevensvil le Preschool and Head Start are also

appreciated. Lastly, a sincere thanks to Sue Lantello and Judy Lawson

for their contributions for the reliabil i ty test.

i i i

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Abstract i i

Acknowledgement i i i

List of Tables and Figures vi

Chapter I . Introduction 1

Review of the l i terature 1

Language Assessment and Assessment Procedures 1

Standardized Tests 4

The Influence of Visual Stimuli in Assessment 5

Perceptual Salience in Pictures 8

The Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language: A Test in Measuring Language Comprehension 12

Chapter I I . Method 16

Subjects 16

Materials and Procedures 16

Perceptual Salience Analysis 17

Analysis of the Results of Perceptual Salience 19

Purpose of the Study 19

Chapter I I I. Results 20

Subjects' Picture Preferences Compared to Chance Responses. . . 20

The Revised Picture Potency Formula as a Predictor of the Subjects' Preferences 21

Correctness According to the TACL Compared to the Subjects' Picture Preference 23

iv

Prevalent Patterns in Comparing the Results 29

Chapter IV. Discussion 30

Subjects' Picture Preference Compared to Chance Responses . . . 30

The Revised Picture Potency Formula as a Predictor of the Subjects' Preference 31

Correctness According to the TACL Compared to the Subjects' Picture Preference 33

Prevalent Patterns in Comparing the Results 35

General Conclusions 36

Implications 37

References 39

Appendix A Instructions for Analyzing the Pictures of the TACL . . 42

B Definit ions of Factors for Analyzing TACL Pictures. . . 43

C Revised PPF Analysis of Pictures, Correct Pictures and Subjects' Preferences for TACL Pictures 44

D Descriptive Information about the Subjects 49

v

LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES

Table 1 Results According to the Picture's Position on the Plate. 22

Figure 1 Revised PPF Factors of the Pictures Successfully Predicted by the Revised PPF 24

Figure 2 2X2 Matrix of Correctness According to the TACL Compared to the Subjects' Picture Preference 25

Table 2 Results of Correctness According to the TACL Compared to the Subjects' Picture Preference. . . 27

Figure 3 Prevalent Patterns in the Results 29

vi

CHAPTER I

Introduction

Speech/language pathologists uti l ize a variety of procedures to

assess and remediate cl ients who display disorders in voice, f luency,

articulation and language. As part of the diagnostic battery

speech/language pathologists may uti l ize standardized tests and con­

sequently must determine i f the specific assessment tool measures what

i t purports to measure, that is, i f i t is valid. The focus of this

study is to determine, in part, the validity of a specific standardized

assessment tool currently used by speech/language pathologists to eva­

luate language comprehension in preschool and young school-aged

children.

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

Language Assessment and Assessment Procedures

Although speech/language pathologists assess cl ients suspected of

voice, f luency, articulation or language disorders, the primary empha­

sis of this study pertains to language disorders. Mil ler (1978)

defined a cl ient with a language deviation as "performing differently

within a particular stage of development" or "performing at an earl ier

stage of development" when "compared to test norms or developmental

data" (p. 282). A language deviation may be present in any or al l of

the language parameters of phonology, syntax, semantics or pragmatics

1

2

(Mil ler, 1978; Nation and Aram, 1977; and McConnell, Love and Clark,

1974) in either the comprehension or production processes (Mil ler,

1978).

One of the clinician's f irst tasks is to determine the presence of

a language deviation, usually by conducting a comprehensive language

assessment. Procedures for assessing language include: standardized

tests; nonstandardized tests; developmental scales; and behavioral

observation (Mil ler, 1978). Mil ler (1978) defined a standardized test,

the primary focus of this paper, as one that had been given to large

numbers of children, had demonstrated reliabil i ty, had validity and had

normative data. The assessment procedures were not mutually exclusive.

"The inclusion of a particular procedure in an assessment protocol

depends on many factors, including the developmental level of the

child, type of information sought, availabil i ty of the procedure and

evaluation setting" (Mil ler, 1978, p. 291).

As previously discussed, the language parameters of phonology,

syntax, semantics, and pragmatics involved two processes, production

and comprehension. Comprehension is the major concern for the present

investigation. The assessment of comprehension is less observable than

production and requires different assessment procedures. Mil ler (1978)

referred to comprehension as "an essentially private event" (p. 286),

in that comprehension may have occurred but there may be no observable

behavior present to indicate that i t had taken place. Mil ler (1978)

defined two major cl inical problems in assessing comprehension. The

3

f irst problem is defining an adequate response which indicates an une­

quivocal comprehension of an utterance. Defining an adequate response

was divided into: 1) natural language responses with nonlinguistic con­

text controlled but not eliminated such as compliance to commands and

answering questions; and 2) contrived behavioral responses with

nonlinguistic context eliminated such as pointing to pictures that

represent sentence utterances. The second major problem was in spe­

cifying the nature of the assessment task and the requirements i t

imposed on the child. The three commonly used tasks, picture-pointing,

object-manipulation and best-fit tasks, required the child to deal with

two and three-dimensional space and recognize, reconstruct or make a

judgment about the stimuli.

Regardless of the purpose of the assessment or the procedure uti­

l ized, speech/language pathologists must recognize factors which may

influence the results obtained, otherwise interpretation of these

results may be erroneous. Mil ler (1978) provided three sets of

variables that may affect any language assessment results. These were:

1) situational variables, including setting or place of evaluation, the

person doing the evaluation and the time of day; 2) task variables,

such as input mode, response mode, instructions, stimuli, context,

order of presentation and scoring; and 3) child variables, including

memory, attention, motivation, sensory and physiological integrity and

experimental history and state. Speech/language pathologists must be

able to recognize the influence of these factors in order to accurately

4

interpret the assessment results. Task variables are the concern of

this study.

Standardized Tests

Speech/language clinicians use standardized tests in conjunction

with other procedures because the tests are reportedly reliable across

examiners, measure specified aspects of language behavior, are readily

available and frequently are time efficient. At least four main pur­

poses for administering a standardized test have been mentioned in the

l i terature (Salvia and Ysseldyke, 1978 and Lien, 1976). First, tests

are uti l ized to identify students who are sufficiently different from

their age-mates that they require special attention. Second, standar­

dized tests are used to determine the extent of a language handicap and

i f that handicap warrants speech/language intervention. Furthermore,

most state laws have specified the criteria, in terms of test scores,

necessary for placement of a child in a special education program.

Third, tests are often administered to assist in planning a program and

outl ining therapy objectives. Fourth, tests are administered to deter­

mine the child's progress over a period of t ime. Regardless of the

purpose for which a test is used there are certain criteria which a

test should meet. Lien (1976) stated "a good measuring instrument

measures what i t is supposed to measure to a high degree, consistently,

and with a minimum expediture of t ime, energy and money" (p. 79). The

f irst part of the definit ion concerns the test's validity, the second

concerns reliabil i ty and the last part concerns usabil i ty. I f any of

5

these criteria, especially validity and reliabil i ty, are not met for a

specific test, the quality of the test should be examined.

The Influence of Visual Stimuli in Assessment

Many commonly employed standardized tests use pictures as stimuli

to determine a child's comprehension of language. The Peabody Picture

Vocabulary Test - Revised, PPVT-R, (Dunn and Dunn, 1981) and the Test

for Auditory Comprehension of Language, TACL, (Carrow-Woolfolk, 1975)

are two of many examples, but the manuals of these instruments do not

address the nonlinguistic factors that may distract from the l inguistic

components being measured. The use of pictures introduces "task

variables" (Mil ler, 1978, p. 305) into the input and response modes,

which may in turn influence the child's test performance. The child is

to identify l inguistic constructs, input mode, by pointing to visual

representations of these constructs, the response mode. Further, pic­

tures place additional cognitive requirements upon the child. The

child may understand the l inguistic utterance but may not be able to

identify the picture. The child must "deal with two-dimensional space

and recognize the pictures" (Mil ler, 1978 p. 300). Speech/language

pathologists then must determine i f this "contrived behavioral

response" (Mil ler, 1978, p. 299) is an adequate response indicating

the child's "unequivocal comprehension of an utterance" (Mil ler,

1978)(p. 299).

The possibil i ty also exists that visual stimuli may improve a

child's comprehension of the l inguistic stimuli being measured in a

6

standardized language comprehension test. The following studies sup­

port this assumption. Moeser and Olson (1974) and Moeser and Bergman

(1972) examined the influence of object or picture referents on compre­

hension of l inguistic stimuli. Moeser and Olson (1974) conducted a

study that involved thirty children between the ages of 3:6 to 4:6

years of age. The children, divided into one experimental and two

control groups, were presented visual stimuli, blocks, that correlated

to nonsense words from an artif icial miniature language. The visual

stimuli for the experimental group corresponded to semantic rules which

correlated to syntactic rules. There were no correlations betwen

visual stimuli and syntactic rules of the language for either control

groups. Moeser and Olson (1974) found a significant difference between

the experimental and two control groups in the number of correct two-

word utterances produced. They concluded that "nursery school children

can learn verbal relationships more easily i f the verbal rules reflect

the lawful relationship contained in the reference f ield" (p. 211).

Moeser and Bergman (1972) conducted a study that also used a miniature

artif icial language but with teenage students. They found that the

pairing of pictures with sentences greatly facil i tated the acquisit ion

of language syntax. Although these studies indicated that the use of

pictures improved the child's comprehension, the l ikelihood may also

exist that pictures may distract from the l inguistic stimuli being

measured.

7

When given more than one picture to choose from, as found in many

language comprehension tests, the child's attention may be drawn to one

particular picture. Several authors (Muma, 1978; Hutt, Forrest and

Newton, 1976; Wetheric and Davis, 1972; Odum and Guzman, 1972; and

Luria, 1959) have addressed perceptual salience present in visual sti­

muli. Muma (1978) defined "perceptual salience" as "an inordinate

focus on a perceptual domain: color, size, shape, position and

possibly number" (p. 260). He further stated that "a child becomes

preoccupied with one of these domains" (p. 260). Odum and Guzman (1972

stated that "the differential frequency with which particular dimen­

sions serve as the basis for selection may be considered a measure of

their relative salience" (p. 271) and that "the higher the salience

value of a dimension, the higher the probabil ity of i ts being cogniti-

vely evaluated, regardless of i ts appropriateness for problem solution"

(p. 272). Luria (1959) described an example, that applied to children

between 1:0 to 1:6 years of age, where perceptual salience overrode a

verbal request. The child was shown two objects, one object at some

distance form the child and another between the child and the f irst

object. The child was asked to hand the examiner an object, the one

furthest from him. The child looked at the f irst object but grabbed

the second instead and handed i t . Luria (1959) stated that "the direc­

tive function of the word wil l be maintained up to the moment when i t

comes into confl ict with the conditions of the external situation"

(p.342). He further explained that the l inguistic stimuli "loses i ts

8

directive role i f the immediate orientatiorial reaction is evoked by a

more closely located, or brighter, or more interesting object" (p.342).

Although most language comprehension tests, with pictures, do not apply

to children as young as those described by Luria (1959), the effects

of perceptual salience are evident in older children. Odum and Guzman

(1972) found that salience for some perceptual attributes were develop-

mentally related. They conducted a study with 408 children in grades

kindergarten, 1, 2, 3, 4, and 6, to determine the salience of the

attributes: form, color, number and position. The found that kin­

dergarten and f irst grade children chose color and form more frequently

than the other attributes; second graders chose color followed by

number; third graders chose number followed by form and the fourth and

sixth graders chose form and then color. Some visual attributes were

shown to be more perceptually salient than others and in the example of

very young children distracted from the l inguistic stimuli. Perceptual

salience as i t relates to pictures found in a language comprehension

test is the major focus of this study.

Perceptual Salience in Pictures

Standardized tests that uti l ize pictures to measure a child's

comprehension of l inguistic constructs introduce nonlinguistic factors

that may influence the child's response. First, language comprehension

tests, that use pictures, require that children identify l inguistic

constructs, input mode, by pointing to a two-dimensional picture,

response mode. I t is then assumed that the child's responses measure

9

the child's comprehension of the l inguistic constructs. Second, the

results of several studies (Moeser and Olson, 1974 and Moeser and

Bergman, 1972) indicated that visual stimuli, comparable to pictures in

tests, improved comprehension of l inguistic stimuli. Finally, some

visual attributes have been shown to be more perceptually salient and

thus chosen over other attributes. Speech/language pathologists must

be confident that they have measured the child's comprehension of

l inguistic stimuli and not the child's response to nonlinguistic fac­

tors, which may be found in pictures. Most tests of language compre­

hension that uti l ize pictures have not addressed the criteria for

selecting the pictures. I f the criterion has been addressed i t is

vague. Dunn and Dunn (1981), for example, stated that one of the cri­

teria used in preparing the test plates for the PPVT-R was that "each

i l lustration should have approximately the same eye appeal (equal inten­

sity and complexity of detail)" (p. 31). These terms were not defined

nor were data presented which would substantiate equal intensity and

complexity of detail. Perhaps the lack of pictorial criteria was

related to "no normative data on pictorial stimuli for child

populations" (Reese and Lipsitt, 1970, p. 199). There were, however,

numerous studies (Legenza and Knafle, 1978 and 1976; Manzo and Legenza,

1975; Whipple, 1953; Rudisil l , 1952; and Hildreth, 1936) which

addressed pictorial features preferred by children. These features

preferred by children wil l be viewed as they related to perceptual

salient features in pictures used in comprehension tests. Hutt et al.

10

(1975) found that young children, below 7 years of age, attended

visually to the pictures they preferred. Thus, perceptual salience,

the inordinate focus on a perceptual domain, can be related to pre­

ference. I t is assumed that children attend to and talk about what

interests them or their preferences. Nelson (1973) analyzed the f irst

50 words acquired by 18 children between 1 and 2 years of age. Her

results indicated that children were selective in their choice of

words. She stated that the "child labels what interests him in his

perceptual world" (p. 115). Few of the studies on children's picture

preference are recent due to the emphasis on the audio-visual media in

current l i terature. The majority of the studies to be discussed have

been based on analysis of test book pictures. Many of the factors pre­

ferred by children can be applied to pictures used in tests for the

comprehension of language in terms of the attention value of perceptual

salience.

Several authors found color, action and animals in pictures to be

preferred by children of various ages. Hildreth (1936) stated that 138

children between 3 and 6 years of age preferred color pictures over

black and white ones 66.6 percent of the time. Rudisell (1952) found

the 70-80 percent of her population, 27 kindergarten children and from

150 to 200 in each of the elementary grades 1-6, preferred color pho­

tographs over black and white ones. Whipple (1953) stated that "even a

sl ight use of color attracted the student's attention more than black

and white pictures" (p. 266) with 150 fourth graders. Manzo and

11

Legenza (1975) l isted color as a factor for their Picture Potency

Formula, to be discussed below. In addition to color in pictures,

action was another factor preferred by children. Whipple (1953) deter­

mined that pictures that presented storytell ing action were more effec­

tive in stimulating interest than lack of action. Action was also a

factor used in the Picture Potency Formula (Manzo and Legenza, 1975).

Finally, pictures with animals were preferred by children in Hildreth's

study (1936) and animals are part of the Picture Potency Formula's fac­

tor "things with potential for movement" (Manzo and Legenza, 1975, p.

1086). The studies cited examined only a few pictorial factors that

relate to language comprehension tests. The Picture Potency Formula

addressed several additional ones. The Picture Potency Formula, deve­

loped by Manzo and Legenza (1975), used 10 factors that determined the

language stimulation value of pictures. The formula, validated by

Legenza (1977) with 47 kindergarten and f irst graders, categorized pic­

tures into high potency pictures, those that stimulated a large amount

of language, medium potency or low potency pictures (as determined by

10 factors which were tall ied and converted into scaled scores). The

10 factors were: 1) different things, the total number of different

things in the picture were counted; 2) significant things, the chief

f igures around which other things were apparently set; 3) total things,

the total number of al l things were counted except for nondescript

things such as blades of grass; 4) number of different colors; 5)

actions, the number of actions in progress; 6) number of children

12

present; 7) total number of people present, counting children again; 8)

total number of things with potential for movement other than people,

e.g., cars, motion toys, planes, animals, etc. (the scaled score for

animals is twice that of other things in this section); 9) the size of

the plate; and 10) empathy, the compatibil i ty with the interests and

experiences of the children with whom i t was used. Basically, the more

items in a factor, the more different things, more people, etc., the

higher the language stimulation value for the picture. Legenza and

Knafle (1978) further found that three factors, number of children,

number of people, and number of actions, were consistently present in

pictures to which children responded highly. The Picture Potency

Formula provides a means for analyzing pictures in order to determine

i f a specific picture is more or less appealing than other pictures.

Some of the factors wil l be applied to pictures in a language compre­

hension test.

The Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language:

A Test That Uti l izes Pictures In Measuring Language Comprehension

The Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language (TACL), developed

by Elizabeth Carrow-Woolfolk (1975), is a language assessment tool

used by speech/language clinicians. I t assess "oral language compre­

hension without requiring language expression from the child" (Carrow,

1968, p. 103) and i t "allows for easy interpretation of responses by

the examiner" (Davis, 1977) for children 3:0 to 6:11 years of age.

13

Carrow-Woolfolk (1975) described the test as consisting of "101 plates

of l ine drawings". Each plate "contains three black-and-white drawings

(or in the case of colors, colored circles) which represent referential

categories that can be signaled by form classes and function words,

morphological constructions, grammatical categories and syntactic

structure" (Carrow, 1971, p. 300). The plates which are stimuli for

the l inguistic constructs "provide three pictures, one representing the

referent for the l inguistic form being tested and another representing

a contrasting l inguistic form; the third picture is included in order

to decrease the chances of guessing in a two-item choice" (Carrow,

1971, p. 300).

The TACL, because i t uti l ized pictures to measure the child's

comprehension of l inguistic constructs, may be also introducing

nonlinguistic factors, such as perceptually salient features of the

pictures, which may influence the child's responses on the test.

Several authors (Mil len and Prutting, 1979 and Hatten, 1978) have

implied that some pictures, of the TACL, were more perceptually salient

than others which may have resulted in an inaccurate measure of the

child's comprehension of the l inguistic stimuli. This issue, however,

was not tested empirically. Hatten (1978), in his review of the TACL,

stated that "in one item the three picture stimuli included a man, a

man and a woman, and a woman, the verbal cue being 'she'; since the

female in the two-person picture has more striking features and

darkened hair she has, in our experience, most often been pointed to by

14

younger children. In such an instance i t is clear that the child

understand the pronoun 'she' but fails the item for extraneous

reasons" (p. 454). Mil l en and Prutting (1979) compared the TACL, the

Northwestern Syntax Screening Test (receptive) and the Bellugi-K1ima

Comprehension Test for specific grammatical features and found percep­

tual salience to be an influential variable in the children's responses

on the TACL. They stated that the "decoy picture stimuli on the TACL,

designed to measure the pronoun 'he', consisted of pictures of a monkey

r iding a bicycle, a girl riding a bicycle, and a boy riding a bicycle.

All children who responded incorrectly to the stimulus 'he' selected

the picture of the monkey r iding the bicycle" (p. 168). They further

noted that "the picture which correctly represented the stimulus, ' the

man painted the house', consisted of a picture of a house painted

bright blue. Neither of the two decoys contained the color blue. The

blue house, therefore, presented an extremely salient feature that may

have influenced some of the children's selections" (p. 168).

Speech/language pathologists need to be aware of the factors that may

distract a child's attention from the l inguistic stimuli due to the

perceptually salient features of a picture. I f a child's response is

influenced by the perceptually salient factors of a picture the vali­

dity of the test must be questioned, since the test is not measuring

what is purports to measure, i .e. comprehension of l inguistic

15

constructs. Carrow-Woolfolk (1975) provided several validity studies

but did not address pictorial factors. 1

Icarrow-Woolfold stated, in a personal conversation (1982), that the main objective in designing the TACL pictures was to display the l inguistic utterance. She did, however, try to make the pictures "not very different from each other."

CHAPTER I I

METHOD

Subjects

The subjects were 30 preschool children (16 female and 14 male)

between four and f ive years of age (the mean age was four years f ive

months). The subjects were enrolled in either the Stevensvil le

Preschool or Head Start and had age appropriate language and cognitive

development according to their instructors. All subjects had passed a

pure tone screening according to the ANSI, 1969, standards; the

Peek-a-Boo series of the Keystone Vision Screening Test and a speech

and language screening administered by a state l icensed speech/language

pathologist. Hearing, vision and speech/language screenings were

completed within three months of the subjects' participation in the

experiment. All subjects were monolingual, speaking Standard American

English.

Materials and Procedures

The subjects served as their own control group, each receiving two

different administrations of the TACL in a counterbalanced order. One

administration followed the procedures outl ined in the TACL manual

(Carrow-Woolfolk, 1975). The other test administration consisted of

only the verbal instructions: "Point to the picture you l ike best".

Both administrations followed the procedures outl ined in the manual.

16

17

The test manual described the administration as in a "one-to-one

setting" (p.9) with the test booklet placed with the "i l lustrations

facing the child, in a quiet room, free from outside disturbances and

visual distractions and well l ighted" (p. 9) and at "a table or desk

which the examiner and tester can sit facing each other comfortably

with sufficient space to accommodate the test booklet and scoring

sheet" (p. 9). The sequence of the two administrations were alternated

between subjects. The specific pictures chosen by the subjects for

each test format was recorded. The administration of both formats was

given by a state l icensed speech/language pathologist.

Perceptual Salience Analysis

AN analysis of the TACL pictures was conducted by the present

investigator to determine i f one picture in a given plate was percep­

tually more salient when compared to the alternate two pictures. (The

analysis followed the instructions in Appendix A) The pictures were

analyzed according to: 1) numbers and amount of actions (Manzo and

Legenza, 1975; Whipple, 1953; and Hildreth, 1936); 2) number of

children (Manzo and Legenza, 1975); 3) number of people (Manzo and

Legenza, 1975); 4) number of different things (Manzo and Legenza,

1975); 5) number of all things (Manzo and Legenza, 1975); 6) color

(Manzo and Legenza, 1975; Whipple, 1953; Rudisil l , 1952; and Hildreth,

1935); and things with potential for movement, including animals (Manzo

and Legenza, 1975 and Hildreth, 1936). (Specific definit ions of the

factors are in Appendix B.) The factors were not considered in a

18

heirarchial order, though Legenza and Knafle (1978) found action,

number of children and number of people to be consistently in pictures

that had a high potency. The aforementioned factors were labelled as

the "revised PPF" since the factors originated from the Picture Potency

Formula (Manzo and Legenza, 1975). Thus, as Mil len and Prutting (1979)

noted, plate 74 had one picture with more of the factor "color"

whereas, the other two pictures did not. Plate 74 also had more

"different things" and "all things" and therefore was considered more

perceptually salient than the other two (see Appendix C). Another

example is in plate 61, which Hatten (1978) noted as having striking

features, where one picture was determined perceptually more salient

than the alternate two pictures. Picture 2 in this plate contained the

factors "number of people" and "number of al l things" since i t was the

only one of the three that had more than one person present.

A pi lot study was conducted to determine the useabil ity of the PPF

factors for analyzing the TACL pictures. The pilot study indicated

that the factors "color" and "potential for movement" were not explicit

enough for reliable judgements to be made. The scaled scores were not

sufficiently specific and were therefore eliminated. The review defi­

nit ions (SEE APPENDIX B) were then uti l ized in a reliabil i ty test of

the analysis of the TACL pictures. Two examiners agreed on 100 of the

101 plates.

19

Analysis of the Results of Perceptual Salience

The analysis of the 101 TACL plates, uti l izing the revised PPF,

indicated that 59 of the plates had one picture that was determined to

be more perceptually salient than the other two (See Appendix C). The

remaining 42 plates were analyzed as equal in terms of their perceptual

salience since no single picture contained more factors than the other

two. Further analysis of the 59 plates showed that 28 were the target

picture for the l inguistic utterance, according to the TACL. I t is

possible that due to the perceptually salient feature of the target

pictures that the child may chose the correct picture regardless of

comprehension of the l inguistic utterance.

Purpose of the Study

The possibil i ty exists that pictures of the TACL are not equal in

terms of their perceptual salience. Furthermore, the perceptual

salience may influence a child's responses on the test. The purpose of

this study is to investigate whether the TACL measures what i t purports

to measure, that of the child's comprehension of l inguistic utterances.

Chapter I I I

Results

The purpose of this study was to investigate three questions:

1) Do children respond significantly different from chance to the

three pictures on a given plate of the TACL?

2) I f the pictures differ significantly from chance, does the

revised Picture Potency Formula predict the subject's picture

preference?

3) Do the incorrect responses on the TACL correlate to those cho­

sen by the subjects as the ones they "l iked best"?

The results of this study wil l be discussed as they pertain to each of

the research questions. The .05 confidence level wil l be used as the

level of significance throughout al l analyses. An additional section

wil l discuss the interaction of the results.

Subjects' Picture Preferences Compared to Chance Responses

Chi squared (X^) (Siegel, 1956) was used to determine i f the sub­

jects responded significantly different from chance to the three pic­

tures on any given plate of the TACL (Carrow-Woolfolk, 1975). The

results of an item by-item, one-sample X^ (Siegel, 1956) indicated that

73 of the 101 TACL plates received responses that differed signifi­

cantly from chance. X^, as a statistical measure, did not indicate

which of the three pictures was different only that there was a signi­

f icant difference. The responses were then analyzed as to the

20

21

subjects' selection of pictures according to the pictures' position on

the plate (See Table 1). The positions were labelled 1, 2 & 3 going

from left to right on the plate. Position 2 was most frequently

selected for 66 of the 73 plates, position 3 was selected in f ive pla­

tes and f inally position 1 was selected on two plates. (The position

with the most responses was the one that had at least one more response

than the alternate two.) The chance frequency distribution for the

total number of responses, 30 subjects with 101 responses each or 3030

total responses, was 1010 for each picture position. The actual

distribution differed from chance significantly. The rank order,

according to picture position, was f irst position 2 with 1615 respon­

ses, followed by position 3 with 845 and f inally position 1 with 570

total responses. The pictures in position 2 were selected more fre­

quently than the pictures in positions 1 and 3.

The Revised Picture Potency Formula As a Predictor Of The Subjects' Preferences

A revised version of the Picture Potency Formula (PPF) (Manzo &

Legenza, 1975) was applied to the 73 significantly different plates to

determine i f the revised PPF predicted which picture on a plate the

subjects would choose. The revised PPF predicted that the subjects

would choose one picture over the alternate two for 45 of the 73 pla­

tes. Only 15 of the 45 pictures, predicted by the revised PPF,

corresponded to the picture on the plate that the subjects chose more

TABLE 1 RESULTS ACCORDING TO THE PICTURES' POSITION ON THE PLATE

Picture Position on Plate

Position distribution of plates showing

a significant difference

Total number of t imes that the 101

pictures were chosen by the

30 subjects

Number of t imes the PPF predicted the

subjects' responses

1 picture on

the left 2 570 0

2 picture in the middle

66 1615 13

3 picture on the right

5 845 2

73* 3030** 15

* The remaining 28 plates did not show a significant difference at the .05 level.

** Significant at the .001 level

23

often as the one they "l iked best" (See Table 1). Thirteen of the 15

pictures were in position 2, followed by two in position 3 and zero in

position 1.

A further analysis was completed in order to determine i f certain

revised PPF factors were more prevalent, in the 15 pictures, than were

other factors. According to the PPF, the one picture on a plate that

was determined as more l ikely to be chosen than the other two was the

picture that contained more PPF factors. Each of the seven revised PPF

factors was present in at least one of the 15 pictures (See Figure 1).

Eleven of the 15 pictures contained a combination of factors (See

Appendix A for factor definit ions). The factors, ranked according to

the number of pictures in which they were present, were f irst "numbers

of all things" found in 11 pictures; then "number of different things"

and "action" in 8 pictures; followed by "number of people" in 5

pictures; "number of children" in 4 pictures; and f inally "color" and

"potential for movement" each was found in one picture.

Correctness According To The TACL Compared To The Subject's Picture Preference

A 2 X 2 matrix (Figure 2) was developed to determine i f the

subjects' responses that were scored by the TACL as incorrect

corresponded to the pictures that the subjects preferred or "l iked

best". The categories for the matrix were:

A. correct response according to the TACL and the picture chosen

as being preferred by the subjects;

24

FIGURE I REVISED PPF FACTORS OF THE PICTURES SUCCESSFULLY PREDICTED BY THE REVISED PPF

PICTURE POSITION WITH THE REVISED PPF FACTORS

PLATE

NUMBER

i

ACTION

2

Chi Id-

r(?n

J

Peooie

4 Nunoer

o

Di f fer-

ent

th inos

3

Number

of a l l

th ings

6

Color

7 Poten-

t io 1

for

Move-

rent

Picturr

wi th

the

TOS t

fc iCtorr PREVALENT FACTORS

t 0 0 0 2 9 n o ? 4.5,7

a .2 2 ? 0 . 0 n ? 1,2,3,4,5

35 1,3 0 0 •> ? n n ? 4,5

43 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 7 5

59 2 2 2 0 ? n 0 ? 1,2.3,5

f i1 2 0 2 p ? n n ? 1.3,4,5

fi? 2 2 o c n 9 0 i ) 9 1,2.3.5

73 2 0 0 0 n n n ? 1

74 0 0 0 3 3 n ? 4.5.6

7R 2 0 0 0 n • n n ? 1

7f i 2.3 0 0 2 0 n 0 ? 4.5

P? 0 3 3 0 n- n n ? 2.3

OR ? 0 0 7 o n n 9 1.4.5

? 0 0 9 1 -> •> n n 9 1

inn 0 0 o n n 4.5

-o indie ites t iat al 3 pi ttures wore >oua 1 "or that "actor and/o * the actor was n )t pro ent

1 pictu ~e on .ho 1 e "t nf l ie p i to

? pictu -e i n ".lie mi 1d I e

1 -p in 'ho r i •jht nf the p ate

25

FIGURE 2

2 x 2 M a t r i x o f C o r r e c t n e s s A c c o r d i n g t o t h e TACL compared to the Subjects' Picture Preference

TACL Score

Correct Incorrect A B

Preferred Correct TACL Incorrect TACL Picture picture and picture and

subjects' subjects' Subjects' preferred preferred Picture picture picture Preference

C D Correct TACL Incorrect TACL

Not the picture and not picture and not Preferred the subjects' the subjects' Picture preferred preferred

picture picture

26

B. incorrect response by the TACL and the picture preferred by

the subjects;

C. correct response according to the TACL but not the preferred

picture by the subjects;

D. incorrect response by the TACL but not the picture preferred

by the subjects.

Three different statistical measures were uti l ized to determine i f

a significant difference existed between the categories. The Fisher

Exact Probabil ity Test (Siegel, 1956) was applied to 75 items. (These

items had an expected frequency of less than 5 for any cell which made

the test for two independent samples (Siegel, 1956) inappropriate.)

The test for two independent samples was used for 13 items.

Finally, the X^ one sample test was preformed on 11 items in which

there were no responses in two of the four categories, resulting in an

analysis of only two categories.

An item-by-item analysis, uti l izing the aforementioned statistical

measures, of the 101 test items indicated that 22 items had a frequency

distribution of responses that were significantly different from chance

(See Table 2). The 22 items were then analyzed as to the category that

contained the most responses. Category C had the most responses for 7

of the 22 items; followed by categories A and B with 2 items each; and

f inally category D with 1 item. X^ for two independent samples was

applied to the cumulative responses in each category for the 101 items.

Thirty subjects gave 101 responses that yielded 3030 total responses,

27

TABLE 2

RESULTS OF CORRECTNESS ACCORDING TO THE TACL COMPARED TO THE

SUBJECTS' PICTURE PREFERENCE

Number of plates* Total Number of with the most Responses by the

responses in that 30 subjects to the Categories category 101 test plates

A Correct TACL picture and subjects' preferred picture

B Incorrect TACL picture and subjects' preferred picture

C Correct TACL picture and not the subjects' preferred picture

D Incorrect TACL picture and not the subjects' preferred picture

TOTAL

2 834

2 391

17 1307

_1 508

22 3030

* The plates with response frequencies that were significantly different from chance.

28

of these category C had 1307 responses, followed by category A with

834, then category D with 508 responses and f inally category B with 391

responses.

Prevalent Patterns In Comparing The Results

Nineteen of the 101 TACL plates were analyzed in two of the

research questions (See Figure 3). The 19 plates had results that were

significantly different from chance with regard to the subjects respon­

ses to the three pictures on a given plate and when comparing the

correctness of the responses, according to the TACL, and the pictures

that the subjects preferred. Four of the 19 plates contained pictures

where the PPF successfully predicted the subjects' choice. The PPF

correctly predicted the subjects' preference as the middle picture on

al l four plates. Three of these plates corresponded to category C and

one to category B; none were in categories A or D. The PPF factors

"number of different things" and "number of al l things" were present in

all four of the pictures, although two pictures, plates 8 and 61, con­

tained additional factors.

29 FIGURE 3 Preva len t Pa t te rns in the Resu l ts

PLATE

*p i c pos i

w i t rr

res::

ture t ion h the OS t

onses

Ca te wit

• mo res

gory** h the St

ponser

•Pict posit

co r rec P r ed i c Revise

ure ion t ly tod by d PPF

Dom i n Rev i se PPF Facto

ant*** j

7 3 c

1 2 c 2 1.2,3 4,5

12 2 c

13 2 c

24 2 A

27 7 c

31 ? c

35 7 c 2 a -

58 9 c

61 7 B 7 1.3.4 5

63 7 c

67 7 D

68 7 B

76 ? C 7 4.5

78 2 r

RD 2 r

OT 2 r

94 2 A

95 2 r

*2 = middli 1 p ic t i j re po • i t i on and 3 = p ic u re o i the i i qh t

r *A = co r re : t TAC! p i c t i re am 1 sub j ' l e ts ' I i re te r i ed p i i : tu re ; B = i ncor i cor re i

' ec t r,

f t TACI \CL p

p i c t c tu re j ro ani

and s no t

ib jec t he su t

, 1 pre i . i ec ts

e r red p re f i

p 1 e tu i r red i

e ; C = i c tu re ;

and D n ic tu

inco k e

- rec t rACL p c tu re and n-) t the sub je i t s ' p i e fe r red

'*1 f.inn: 2=rh i drpn: 3 = npn )1P: 4 diffe •ent tJ i nas : and 5 a l 1 t h i nas .

Chapter IV

Di scussion

The discussion is organized according to the three research

questions. In addition prevalent patterns, general conclusions and

implications of this study are presented.

Subjects' Picture Preference Compared To Change Responses

The subjects responded significantly different from chance to the

three pictures on a given plate of the TACL. Specifically, the sub­

jects chose the picture in the middle of the plate as the one they

"l iked best" over the pictures in positions 1 and 3. Recall that 66 of

the 73 plates, with results that differed significantly from chance,

had the most responses in the middle picture position and that 1615 of

the 3030 total responses were in the middle position.

The age of the subjects, age 4-5 years, and therefore their

cognitive maturity may have influenced the responses to the middle pic­

ture. The children in the present study may not have fully understood

the term "l ike" for the task that determined their picture preferences.

Hutt (1975), in a study that investigated the effects of picture

novelty, found that children under f ive years had diff iculty in

interpreting the term "l ike" when asked to choose their "most l iked"

picture. Furthermore, subjects of this age may not have developed

30

31

definite preferences for pictures. Aitken and Hutt (1974) reported

that children 3-4 years of age expressed picture preferences

unreliabil i ty and idiosyncratically. Further, Hutt et al. (1976)

reported that " i t is only after the age of 5 years that children mani­

fest a consistent and reliable preference for certain attributes of

visual stimuli" (p. 63). Myatt and Carter (1979) found that kindergar­

ten children did not have clearly distinguished picture preferences,

when shown six different picture styles. The subjects in the present

study may have chosen the middle picture due to lack of understanding

of the task and/or their immature abil i ty to determine a specific pre­

ference.

Another possibil i ty for the responses to the middle picture may be

due to the position of the picture on the plate. Wood (1960) stated

that "many persons would tend to use the third position too often for

the answer in f ive-choice items" (p. 58) for multiple choice tests.

The present study indicated that although i t was not a test per se, the

subjects chose the middle position for three-choice items.

The Revised Picture Potency Formula As A Predictor of The Subjects' Preferences

The results indicated that the PPF did not predict the majority of

the subjects' picture choices. The PPF correctly predicted only 15 of

the 73 plates that had response frequencies that were different from

chance. Thirteen of the 15 pictures were in the middle picture posi­

t ion. The previous discussion indicated that the subjects tended to

choose the middle pictures more often than pictures in position 1 and

32

3. Thus, the pictures in the middle position that were predicted by

the PPF had a greater probabil ity of corresponding to the subjects'

choices than pictures in positions 1 or 3. The 13 pictures in the

middle position, predicted by the PPF as the subjects' choice, could

have been chosen in part due to their position on the plate and not due

to the PPF's accuracy as a preference predictor.

The PPF by definit ion predicts that the more complex the picture

the more responsive the children wil l be. The present study did not

support this assumption. An analysis of the 73 plates, which had

responses different from chance, indicated that 41 of the pictures,

that the subjects chose, did not have any PPF factors that made them

more predominant than the other two (See Appendix D). This could be

due to the subjects' preference for the middle picture, 40 of the 41

were in the middle position, or i t could be that the subjects preferred

the simpler picture. French (1953) found that 449 young children,

kindergarten through third grade, preferred simple pictures over more

complex ones. The results of this study indicated that the children

preferred the middle picture regardless of the picture's charac­

teristics.

The results of this study using the PPF did not correspond to the

original PPF f indings. Legenza and Knafle (1978), uti l izing the ori­

ginal PPF, found that the factors "action", "number of children" and

"number of people" to be consistently present in pictures that children

responded to the most. This was not supported in the present study.

33

The pictures predicted by the PPF which corresponded to the subjects'

actual choices contained the factor "number of al l things", followed by

"number of different things" and "actions". This discrepancy could be

in part due to the altering of the PPF for the present study. The ori­

ginal PPF (Manzo & Legenza, 1975) was found to be ambiguous and lacking

in operational definit ions in a pilot study. The original PPF uti l ized

scaled scores, the revised did not. The original "number of al l

things", for example, used a scaled score of 1 for one to three total

things in the picture. The revised PPF gave credit to each thing. The

TACL pictures frequently had only 1-3 items present and i t was thought

that a blanket score of 1 would not adequately differentiate the pic­

tures. "Things with potential for movement" for example was too ambi­

guous in the original definit ions of "things such as cars, motion toys,

animals. . .except people". The revised PPF further excluded plants

and trees. Another possible reason for the discrepancy between the two

studies was in the way the PPF was analyzed. The original study used

the PPF to predict a picture's language stimulation potency. The pre­

sent study uti l ized the PPF to predict the visual preference value of

the pictures. A correlation between the verbalization of children this

age and their preferences for pictures may not exist.

Correctness According To The TACL Compared To The Subjects' Picture Preference

The third research question, "do the responses determined by the

TACL as incorrect correlate to those chosen by the subjects as the ones

34

they "l iked best"? is represented as category B in the 2X2 matrix

(Figure 2). Category B (the incorrect response according to the TACL

but the picture preferred by the subjects) should have received more

total responses than category D ( incorrect responses according to the

TACL and not the picture preferred by the subjects) i f the subjects had

chosen the picture they l iked best when their response was incorrect.

The results indicated that category D received more total responses

than category B, 508 and 391 responses respectively (Table 2). A

review of the data indicated that the subjects did not choose the pic­

ture that they "l iked best" regardless of correctness. The total

number of responses for categories C and D, the categories that repre­

sented the pictures not preferred by the subject, was 1815 responses

compared to 1225 responses for categories A and B, the categories

representing the preferred responses.

Overall the subjects responded with more correct responses,

regardless of their picture preference, than incorrect responses. The

total number of responses for the categories A and C, correct responses

regardless of preference was 2141 out of 3030 possible responses. This

indicates that the subjects chose the correct picture, for both test

administrations, more often than either of the alternative incorrect

pictures. Furthermore, the subjects responded to the l inguistic cues

for the test when i t was administered in a protocol manner. The

subjects' test scores ranged from 53 to 85 correct responses of 101

possible, with a mean of 72.03 (See Appendix D). According to the TACL

35

manual, the means were 69.375 and 69.6471 for the subjects' age groups

4-0 to 4-5 years and 4-6 to 4-11 years respectively. All of the sub­

jects scored within one standard deviation of the mean, with one excep­

t ion. Subject 11 (See Appendix D) received a score of 53 which is

approximately one and one-half standard deviations below the mean for

her age group. These results indicated that the subjects were not

responding to the middle picture when l inguistic cues were provided. A

comparison of the subjects preferred pictures and those scored by the

TACL as correct showed that of the 66 significantly different pictures

in the middle position only 25 were the correct response. I f the sub­

jects were responding to the middle pictures and not the l inguistic

cues they would have gotten only 31 of the 101 test items correct. The

results indicated that the l inguistic cues were more salient than the

visual aspects of the pictures.

Prevalent Patterns In Comparing The Results

The subjects' preference for the middle picture was predominant

when the results of the three research questions were viewed as they

relate to each other. Recall that 19 of the 101 plates had differences

both in terms of the subjects preference compared to chance and for

correctness compared to the subjects preferences (Figure 3). The sub­

jects chose the middle picture for 18 of the 19 plates. Furthermore,

the four pictures that the PPF accurately predicted were in the middle

position.

36

Two of the aforementioned nineteen plates wil l be examined more

carefully. Plate 24, "f ind the middle car", and plate 61, "she", were

significant for both the results of the subjects' preference compared

to chance and for correctness compared to the subjects' preferences.

Plate 24 had the most responses for the middle picture and category A,

the correct response and the one preferred by the subjects. This plate

had the same picture in all three picture positions, but the subjects

chose the middle picture. The middle picture was chosen due to i ts

position and obviously not due to factors found in the picture. The

results for plate 61 should be noted as this was the plate previously

noted by Hatten (1978) that . . i t is clear that the child

understands the pronoun 'she' but fails the item for extraneous reasons

(p. 454)." This plate had the most responses for the middle picture

and category B, incorrect according to the TACL but the picture pre­

ferred by the subjects. (This picture was also accurately predicted by

the PPF.) The results for this plate indicated that Hatten's obser­

vation may have some validity except that overall the subjects chose

the middle picture.

General Conclusions

The results of the present study indicated that the subjects

generally chose the middle picture when given the choice of the three

pictures on the majority of the test plates of the TACL. The choice of

the middle position appeared to be due to the position of the picture

37

on the plate and not due to any specific features of the pictures them­

selves. The revised PPF was not an accurate predictor of the subjects

responses, unless the predictions coincided with the middle picture.

The PPF factors that were most consistent in predicting the subjects'

preferences ("number of different things" and "number of all things")

appeared to be contrary to previous evidence that young children prefer

simple pictures over complex ones. The results of this study further

indicated that the subjects were not influenced by the construction of

the TACL pictures nor the pictures they "l iked best" when presented

with the l inguistic cues. One possible exception was noted in the

l i terature and confirmed by the present study where the subjects may

have responded to the picture and not the l inguistic cue. In general

this study supports the validity of the TACL. The TACL measures what

i t purports to measure, that of children's understanding of l inguistic

concepts.

Imp!ications

The results of this study indicated that children with normal

language development, between the age 4-5 years, attended to the

l inguistic cues of the TACL and were not significantly influenced by

the perceptually salient features of the pictures. This does not

necessarily apply to children suspected of a language deviation, for

whom the TACL is used to identify, nor younger or older children.

Children who exhibit language deviations and younger children may be

more susceptible to visual distractions. Speech/language pathologists

38

need to be aware of the potentially misleading influence of the visual

stimuli and indeed only further research would empirically answer this

question.

The results further indicated that the revised PPF was not an

accurate indicator of picture preference for the subjects of this

study. This is not to say that i t may not be a valuable tool in deter­

mining picture preferences of older children who have developed defi­

nite preferences. Once again, further research may indicate whether

older or younger children respond in a similar manner.

Even though the present study supported the validity of the TACL,

i t does not contend that the TACL should be uti l ized as the sole

measure of a child's understanding of l inguistic concepts. As with any

diagnostic tool i t should only be used as a part of a comprehensive

diagnostic battery.

39

REFERENCES

Carrow, E. "The Development of Auditory Comprehension of Language Structure in Children". Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 33, 1968, 99-111.

Carrow, E. "Comprehension of English and Spanish by Preschool Mexican-American Children". Modern Language Journal, LV, 1971, 299-306.

Carrow-Woolfolk, E. Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language, Austin, Texas: Learning Concepts, 1975.

Davis, J. "Reliabil i ty of Hearing-Impaired Children's Responses to Oral and Total Presentations of the Test of Auditory Comprehension of Language". Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 11, 1977, 520-527.

Dunn, L. and Dunn, L. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised, Circle Pines, Minn.: American Guidance Service, 1981.

French, J. "Children's Preferences for Pictures of Varied Complexity of Pictorial Pattern." Elementary School Journal, 53, Sept. 1952-May 1953, 90-95.

Hatten, J. "Tests and Reviews: Miscellaneous-Listening" (Test for Auditory Comprehension of Language), Buros, 0. (Ed.), The Eighth Mental Measurements Yearbook, Highland Park, N.J.: The Gryphon Press, 1978, 611-614.

Hildreth, G. "Color and Picture Choices of Young Children", Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1936, 427-435.

Hutt, C. "Degrees of Novelty and Their Effects on Children's Attention and Preference". Brit ish Journal of Psychology, 66, 1975, 487-492.

Hutt, C., Forrest, B., Newton, J. "The Visual Preferences of Children" Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17, 1976, 63-68.

Legenza, A. and Knafle, J. "The Effective Components of Children's Pictures", Paper presented at the 28th Annual Meeting of the National Reading Conference, St. Petersburg Beach, Florida, Nov. 30-Dec. 2, 1878.

Legenza, A. and Knafle, J. "Assessing Pictures through Children's Responses", Paper presented at the 21st Annual Meeting of the College Reading Association, Cincinnati, Ohio, Oct. 26-29, 1977.

40

Lein, A. Measurement and Evaluation of Learning, Dubuque, Iowa: Wm. C. Brown Company Publisher, 1976.

Luria, A. "The Directive Function of Speech in Development and Dissolution", Word, 15, 1959, 341-352.

Manzo, A. and Legenza, A. "A Method for Assessing Language Stimulation Value of Pictures", Language Arts, 52, 1975, 1085-1089.

McConnell, F., Love, R., Clark, B. "Language Remediation in Children", Dickson, S. (Ed.) Communication Disorders Remedial Principles and Practices, Glenview, I l l inois: Scott, Foresman and Company, 1974.

Mil ler, J. "Assessing Children's Language Behavior", Schiefelbusch, R. (Ed.) Bases Of Language Intervention, Baltimore: University Park Press, 1978, 269-318.

Mil len, C. and Prutting, C. "Inconsistencies Across Three Language Comprehension Tests for Specific Grammatical Features", Language, Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools, 10, 1979, 162-170.

Moeser, S. and Bergman, A. "The Role of Reference in the Acquisit ion of a Miniature Artif ical Language", Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 1972, 759-769.

Moeser, S. and Olson, A. "The Role of Reference in Children's Acquisit ion of a Miniature Artif icial Language", Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 17, 1974, 204-218.

Muma, J. Language Handbook: Concepts, Assessment and Intervention, Eng1ewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall, 1978.

Myatt, B. and Carter, J. "Picture Preferences of Children and young Adults", Education Communication and Technology Journal, 27(1), 1979, 45-3T:

Nation, J. and Aram, D. Diagnosis of Speech and Language Disorders, Saint Louis: The C. V. Mosby Company, 1977.

Nelson, K. "Structure and Strategy in Learning to Talk", Monographs of Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (1-2), 1973, 1-137.

Odum, R. and Guzman, R. "Development of Hieracrchies of Dimensional Salience", Developmental Psychology, 6 (2), 1972, 271-287.

Reese, H. and Lipsitt, L. Experimental Child Psychology, New York: Academic Press, 1970.

41

Rudisil l , M. "Children's Preferences for Color Versus Other Qualit ies in Il lustrations", Elementary School Journal, 52, Sept. 1951-May 1952, 444-451.

Salvia, J. and Ysseldyke, J. Assessment in Special and Remedial Education, Boston: Houghton Miff l in Co., 1978.

Wetheric, N. and Davis, P. "Inductive Thinking in Children of Primary School Age: The 'Door and Key' Test", Brit ish Journal of Psychology, 63 (4), 1972, 555-559.

Whipple, G. "Appraisal of the Interest Appeal of I l lustrations" Elementary School Journal, 53, Sept. 1952-May 1953, 262-269.

Wood, D. "Test Construction", Charles E. Merri l l Books, Inc., Columbus, Ohio, 1960.

APPENDIXES

APPENDIX A

42

Appendix A

Instruction for Analyzing the Pictures of the TACL

Analyze the three pictures on each plate of the TACL according to

the seven revised PPF factors. Definit ions of the seven factors are

found in Appendix B. Record the pictures according to their positions

on the plate. Position 1 is the picture on the left, position 2 is in

the middle and position 3 is on the right. Record the number of the

picture on the row for the corresponding plate number according to the

factors present in each picture (See Example). Some pictures may

contain more than one factor or no factors and some factors may be pre­

sent in more than one picture or not in any. The picture within a

given plate with more of any factor (more people, more different

things, etc.) are to be recorded and those with less of the factor not

recorded for that specific factor. I f the three pictures contain equal

numbers of the factor mark "0" in that column. The column "picture

with the most factors" is determined by adding the number on the plate

and determining the single picture with the most factors present. I f

two or three pictures have equal number's of factors mark "0" in the

column for the "picture with the most factors present".

Example: I 2 3 4 5 6 7 Picture With Number of Number Potential The Most

Plater Number of Number of Different of All for Factors

Number Action Children People Things Things Color Movement Present

X 1,2 1,2 0 0 10 0 1

Z 1 0 0 0 2 3 0 0

APPENDIX B

43

Appendix B: Definit ions of Factors for Analyzing TACL Pictures

1. Action - number and/or amount of action, count any action in progress and record the picture with the most action(s) present (e.g., walking is more than sitt ing)

2. Children - count the number of children and record the picture with the most children present

3. People - count the number of people, counting children again, and record the picture with the most people present

4. Different things - count the total number of different things and record the pictures with the most number of different things present (e.g., 5 people = 1 thing; 2 hi l ls = 1 thing)

5. All things - count the total number of al l things except for non­descript things such as blades of grass and record the picture with the most number of al l things present (e.g., 2 trees = 2 things; water = 1; sky = 1)

6. Color - number and amount of color is counted, record pictures with more color or more colors other than black and white

7. Things with potential for movement - count things such as cars, motion toys, trucks, animals, etc., but exclude people, record pictures with more things with potential for movement (plants and trees are not counted)

APPENDIX C

p L

F.

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

i l

1 2

il

14

15.

16

17

i l

19

20

21

22

23

24

4 4

A p p e n d i x C - R e v i s e d P P F A n a l y s i s o f P i c t u r e s , C o r r e c t P i c t u r e s

and Sub jec ts ' P re fe rences fo r TACL P ic tu res

P i c t u r c p o s i t i o n w i t h t h e m o s t o f t h e f a c t o «

1 2 3 4 5 o 7

a m o u n t o f a c t i o n

1,3

N u m b e r o f c h i i d -

1 , 2

N u m b e r o f p o o p i e

1 , 2

N u m b e r of d i f f ­e r e n t t h i n g s

1 , 2

N u m b e r of al l t h i n g s

P o t « ? i -t i o t t o r m o v e -nent

1,3

P L c t u r e p o s i t ­i o n w i t h th«5 n o s t f a c t o r s

C o r r e c

T A C L p : c

S u b j e c t p r o f e r -e n c e i n p l c r . e s with,.} " > i a n ; f -; c o n t d i f f e r e n c e

P r e f e r e n c e t h a t h a s n o d o m i n a n t f a c t o r s

5 E

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

42

43

44

45

46

47

48

R e v i s e d P P F A n a l y s i s o f P i c t u r e s , C o r r e c t P i c t u r e s

4 5

and Sub jec ts ' P re fe rences fo r TACL P ic tu res

P i c r u r o p o s i t i o n w i t h t h e m o s t o f t h

}

2.3

1.3

N u m b e f

d i f f ­e r e n t t h m g s

1,3

Numbcr o r . 3 1 i ; h i n q s

1,3

f a c t o r

6

P n t c n -' j I w i : n tno r n c s t f a c t o r s

Corrpc T A C L

J U ! I I ' ? C T p r i ' f • ; r -o n c o i n : > I t n s " 11 n j j i a n i r -iCi ' in t d; f f o r o n c e

P r o f e r o n c c t h a t h a s n o d o m i n a n t f a c t o r s

1 , 2

1 , 2 1 , 2 1,3

2,3

1,3 1,3

1 .2

1 , 2

E

49

50

51

Ji

_53_

54

_55

_56

57

ii

ii

60

i l

ii

ii

ii

ii

ii

il

ii

ii

Ii

2!_

72

4 5

R e v i s e d P P F A n a l y s i s o f P i c t u r e s , C o r r e c t P i c t u r e s

and Sub jec ts ' P re fe rences fo r TACL P ic tu res

p o s i t

2

ch iId-r e n

i , o n w i t h

3

i J u m b e r o t p e o p l e

2,3

2,3

1 , 2

2,3

2,3

1 , 2

2,3

t h e m o s t o f t h i

4 5

N u m b e r o f d i f f ­e r e n t t h i n g s

2,3

2,3

1,3

1,2

Numbe r or al l t h i n g s

f a c t o r

6 7

PQten-t ! 3 i f o r m o v o -m e n t

P i c t u r e p o s i i o n w i t h t h e moi t

C o r r e c

T A C L pic^i.ur1

p o s i t -f a c t o r s } i o n

S u b i t ? c t s p r e f e r ­e n c e i n p l j t e s w i t h a nrJ-d i f f -e r e n c e

P r e f e r e n c e t h a t h a s n o d o m i n a n t f a c t o r s

E

73

74

75

76

77

78

_ZI 80

81

82

83

84

85

86

87

88

89

90

91

!L

93

94

95

96

4 7

R e v i s e d P P F A n a l y s i s o f P i c t u r e s , C o r r e c t P i c t u r e s

and Sub jec ts ' P re fe rences fo r TACL P ic tu res

P i c t u r e p o s i t i o n w i t h

a m o u n t o f . a c t i o n

2,3

2,3

2,3

1,3

1 , 2

c h i l d -r e n

N u m b e r o f p e o p l e

1 . 2

t h e m o s t o f t h e f a c t o r

N u m b e r of d i f f ­e r e n t t h i n g s

2,3

N u m b e r o r a l l t h i n g s

2,3

C o l o r

P < ? t e n -t l a l f o r m o v e ­m e n t

P i c t u r e p o s i t i g n w i t h the most f a c t o r s

C o r r e c t

T A C L p iciur1

p o s i t ­i o n

S u b i e c t : p r e f e r ­e n c e ? . n p l a t e s with ,3

f issr-d i f r -e r e n c e

P r e f e r e n c e t h a t h a s n o d o m i n a n t f a c t o r s

4 8

Revised PPF Analysis of Pictures, Correct Pictures

and Subjects ' Preferences for TACL Pictures

97

98

99

100

101

P i c t u r e p o s i t i o n w i t h t h e m o s t o f t h >

1 2 3 4 5

a m o u n t o f a c t i o n

2,3

c h i l d ­r e n

N u m o e r o f d i f f -e r e n t t h i n g s

1,3

N u m b e r o t a i t c h i n q s

» f a c t o r

6

2,3

P p t e n -t l a i

Picture QOZ i r ion with

r o s t f a c t o r s

C o r r o c T A C L pic vurr ros i -i o n

S u b i e c t p r e f e r ­e n c e i n r j l o t e s w i ! : h t

i o n l t -i C . ) n t d i f f ­e r e n c e

P r e f e r e n c e t h a t ^ a s n o d o m i n a n t f a c t o r s

0 indicates that a l l 3 p ictures were equal for that factor and/or the factor was not present.

1 picture posi t ion on the lef t of the plate

2 p icture posi t ion in the middle of the plate

3 p icture posi t ion on the r ight of the plate

APPENDIX D

4 9 Appendix D - Descr ipt ive Informat ion About The Subjects

Su

bje

ct

G) 01 < T

AC

L

Ra

w

Sc

ore

01 0 O .2 t— ^ •4- ^ 0 c , s e o

Cj

E Ll. o' •c

Su

b je

c.

t

CT> < T

AC

L

Ra

w

Sc

ore

Ord

er

of

Te

s

Pre

se

nta

tio

r

0)

5 S

U. D"

i 4-7 85 1-2 F 21 4-8 80 1-2 F

2 4-1 75 2-1 F 22 4-3 77 2-1 M

3 4-4 76 1-2 F 23 4-6 69 1-2 M

4 4-3 76 2-1 F 24 4-5 73 2-1 M

5 4-4 71 1-2 F 25 4-4 70 1-2 M

6 4-0 71 2-1 F 26 4-8 65 2-1 F

7 4-1 70 1-2 F 27 5-0 65 1-2 M

8 4-5 83 2-1 F 28 4-1 67 2-1 M

9 4-7 69 1-2 F 29 4-2 68 1-2 M

10 4-4 58 2-1 F 30 4-6 78 2-1 M

11 4-4 53 1-2 F

12 4-11 80 2-1

13 4-51 81 1-2 F

14 4-11 72 2-1 F

15 4-3 62 1-2 M

16 4-10 80 2-1 M

17 4-7 71 1-2 M

18 4-9 76 2-1 M

19 4-4 73 1-2 F

20 4-8 69 2-1 M

* 1 = TACL Test Administrat ion

2 = Test for Picture Preference