might high-ability pupils...

24
No 9 Cornelia Wulff Hamrin Kimmo Sorjonen Chris Magnusson Might high-ability pupils be teachers’ headache: the “justice” in rating?

Upload: others

Post on 02-Feb-2021

17 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • No 9

    Cornelia Wulff Hamrin

    Kimmo Sorjonen

    Chris Magnusson

    Might high-ability pupils be

    teachers’ headache:

    the “justice” in rating?

  • About the authors: Cornelia Wulff Hamrin is a a senior lecturer of psychology at the Department of Occupational Health Sci-

    ence and Psychology, University of Gävle. Kimmo Sorjonen is a lecturer at Karolinska Institutet. Chris Magnusson has a PhD in

    psychology and psychologist.

    This work is published via open access and is licensed with a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International

    (CC BY-NC 4.0) license.

    Research Report No. 9

    urn:nbn:se:hig:diva-33147

    Distribution:

    Gävle University Press

    SE-801 76 Gävle, Sweden

    [email protected]

  • Might high-ability pupils be teachers’

    headaches: the “justice” in rating?

    Cornelia Wulff Hamrin, Kimmo Sorjonen & Chris Magnusson

    Faculty of Health and Occupational Studies Department of Occupational Health Science and Psychology

  • Abstract

    Pupils’ grades in school are affected by several factors, including mental ability and classroom be-

    haviour. The aim of the present study was to explore any age-related differences in classroom be-

    haviour and academic skills with respect to higher or lower mental ability and gender among 6–11-

    year-old pupils (n=194) in a Swedish school setting. Raven’s matrixes were used, and ANOVAs

    were carried out. There were differences in teacher ratings in Swedish and Mathematic, partly ex-

    plained by interaction effects. There were significant differences between the measurement points.

    The high ability pupils´ lower teacher ratings in Mathematics, may depend on underachievement.

    Keywords: Classroom behaviour, mental ability, maturation, academic skill ratings, pupils

  • Sammanfattning

    Elevers skolbetyg beror på flera faktorer, inklusive mental förmåga och klassrumsbeteende. Syftet

    med föreliggande studie var att undersöka åldersskillnader i klassrumsbeteende och skolprestationer,

    hos elever 6-11 år gamla (n=194), med högre respektive lägre begåvning, i en svensk skolkontext.

    Ravens matriser användes och ANOVA analyser genomfördes. Det fanns skillnader i läraraskatt-

    ningar i svenska och matematik, som delvis förklaras av interaktionseffekter. Det var signifikanta

    skillnader mellan mättilfällena. De högre begåvade elevernas lägre omdömmen i matematik, kan

    bero på underprestation.

    Keywords: Klassrumsbeteende, mental förmåga, mognad, skattningar av akademiska färdigheter, elever

  • Acknowledgements

    This study was funded by Majblomman and the Swedish Ministry of Education and Research. The

    authors also wish to thank the pupils and teachers who were involved in the project.

    .

  • Table of Contents

    Abstract 5

    Sammanfattning 6

    Acknowledgements 7

    Introduction 1 Teacher ratings 1 The importance of mental ability for school achievement 2 Classroom behaviour and other behaviour 2 Sex differences 3 Aim 3

    Method 4 Sample 4 Material 4

    Mental Ability 4 Teachers’ ratings 4

    Procedures 4

    Results 5

    Discussion 8 Limitations and conclusions 8

    References 11

  • 1

    Introduction

    Teachers’ ratings of pupils’ skills are affected by a great number of factors, as are pupils’ school

    achievement and classroom behaviour. This study focuses on elementary school girls and boys, and

    their teachers’ rating of their skills and classroom behaviour.

    A pupil’s level of performance depends to a certain degree on their personal attitude towards

    their ability to achieve, whether they feel they can master the situation, and whether they have a

    feeling of self-efficacy. Naturally, other factors such as upbringing and intelligence are also of im-

    portance (Jenner, 2004). However, Grosin (2004) has argued that the key factors for achievement

    are the school climate and factors including parents’ educational and socioeconomic levels, minority

    status, attitudes towards school from the home environment, and the pupil’s pre-knowledge and

    understanding. He concluded that the most important aspects for success in school are the pupil’s

    pre-knowledge and pedagogical and social climate factors in the school, consisting of norms, expec-

    tations, and values that are focused on pupils’ needs (Grosin, 2004).

    With respect to brain development, girls mature earlier than boys, as reflected in their better

    concentration ability and working memory capacity. Cognitively, boys have greater variations in

    mental ability than girls. Boys have shown a slightly higher prevalence for externalizing problems,

    and tend to have lower reading abilities at an early age compared to girls (Ingvar, 2010). At certain

    ages, small sex-related differences in achievement scores for reading (Swedish National Agency for

    Education, 2012b) and mathematics (Swedish National Agency for Education, 2016) have been

    found between girls and boys attending compulsory school in Sweden. School achievement is es-

    sential for pupils’ school satisfaction, and also has consequences for achievement and satisfaction

    later in working life (Wulff, Bergman & Sverke, 2009). However, motor restlessness and conduct

    problems are not appreciated in the classroom, and might contribute to poor achievement. Mental

    ability may have an impact on adjustment as well as achievement.

    Teacher ratings

    Teachers judge their pupils’ achievements and skills both consciously and unconsciously (SOU,

    1995; Wagner & Sternberg, 1985) and through stereotyping. According to the Swedish Ministry of

    Education and Research (1994), “The teacher must take all available information into account with

    respect to course requirements and make an overall judgment of the pupils’ skills when determining

    their marks” (p16). Mental ability is also highly correlated with school achievement (Gustafsson &

    Balke, 1993; Johnson, McGue, & Iacono, 2005; Smedler & Törestad, 1996; Wulff, Bergman &

    Sverke, 2009). Several studies have been conducted in this field; some examples are described be-

    low.

    In one study among elementary school students, teachers nominated the students they considered

    gifted, and these students were given special education. Afterwards, the students’ intelligence was

    measured, and ratings of giftedness from parents, teachers, and self-reported data were compared.

    Grades were also taken into account. Teachers’ and parents’ ratings of giftedness were rather accu-

    rate, and correlated. The ratings were related to the students’ grades (Rothenbusch, Voss, Golle &

    Zettler, 2018). In another study, fourth-grade students were assessed with respect to inattentive be-

    haviour, both by their classroom teachers and by tutors. Also, the students were weak in mathemat-

    ics, and might develop learning difficulties in mathematics. It seemed important for these children

    to have tailored instruction in small groups. The tutors’ ratings were more accurate than the class-

    room teachers’ (Malone & Fuchs, 2014).

    There are different elementary school curricula in different countries. Hubert, Guimard, Florin,

    and Tracy (2015) carried out a study among French students in nursery/elementary school. Self-

    regulation was measured with a tool where the child should have a motor response to a verbal com-

    mand. Achievements in mathematics and French (i.e. literacy) were measured, and IQ was assessed

    with Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices. Self-regulation was related to school achievement,

    especially mathematics. The measures were related between two time points. There were no sex

    differences in self-regulation. Furthermore, literacy was important for math achievement. There

    were indirect paths between the measurement points concerning the impact of self-regulation on

    mathematical achievement and the impact of literacy on mathematical achievement (Hubert, et al.,

    2015).

  • 2

    Teachers, students, and peers generally make judgements about students’ IQ and achievements.

    Teachers’ judgements about students’ IQ are relatively accurate. A student’s self-perception and

    self-judgement are based on IQ test results and teachers’ and peers’ judgements. One study found

    that judgments seemed to be based more on verbal ability than mathematical ability (Pretzlik, Ols-

    son, Nabuco, & Cruz, 2003). That study used two subsamples, with different ways of measuring IQ

    in each; in the London subsample, IQ was measured by WISC-R (Wechsler, 1963), and in the Por-

    tuguese subsample, it was measured using Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Pretzlik et al.,

    2003). The focus of the present study is on differences in achievement and classroom behaviour

    between pupils with higher and lower mental ability, with respect to age.

    The importance of mental ability for school achievement

    Marks and mental ability have been found to be positively related (Cattle & Horn, 1966; Gustafsson

    & Balke, 1993; Kulik & Kulik, 1982; Smedler & Törestad, 1996; Thurstone, 1938). Gustafsson and

    Balke (1993) reported that general mental ability was related to both school achievement and the

    general ability to perform well in school. Conversely, an overview article on the differences between

    high-performing pupils and pupils in the mainstream or below reported that mental ability was not

    important for success in school, and that success was more influenced by home environment, moti-

    vation, and teaching methods (Swedish National Agency for Education, 2012a).

    Depending on theoretical framework, mental ability may be viewed as a general factor, a hierar-

    chical construct, or a multifactorial construct. Verbal ability is especially important for school

    achievement (Smedler & Törestad, 1996), and long-term and short-term memory has implications

    for numerical ability. Processing speed seems to be crucial, in that pupils with lower mathematical

    ability have lower processing speed (Bull & Johnstone, 1997). However, often just a general meas-

    ure of ability is used instead of specific abilities, and this approach has been adopted in the present

    study. According to Schmidt and Hunter (2004), a measure of general mental ability is more accurate

    for predicting achievement than specific abilities.

    Johnson, McGue, and Iacono (2005) found that genetic factors had a strong impact, and that these

    factors explained a substantial proportion of behaviour and school achievement. They also reported

    that inattention and disruptive behaviour often run in families. Inattention, disruptive behaviour, and

    learning problems are related to lower IQ (Johnson et al., 2005). In some cases, disruptive behaviour

    has been associated with high IQ and high performance among boys; and similar results have been

    found among girls, albeit those with a lower performance level than the boys (Johnson et al., 2005).

    The researchers proposed that these findings may be due to having too little academic stimulus in

    school.

    A comparative twin and control study conducted in Stockholm, Jerusalem, and an Israeli kibbutz

    measured IQ (Raven’s Progressive Matrices), mathematical achievement, and language achieve-

    ment at two or three measurement points. The purpose was to examine whether the influence of

    genetics was reduced in certain environments. In Jerusalem the boys outperformed the girls, and in

    Stockholm the girls were better than the boys. No significant differences were seen between twins

    and controls (Fischbein, Guttman, & Nathan, 1999).

    In another study, mental ability was related indirectly to school satisfaction and job satisfaction.

    The mediating link was achievement (Wulff, Bergman, & Sverke 2009). Gifted children often end

    up higher in the working life hierarchy (Judge, Higgins, Thoresen & Barrick, 1999), but some pupils

    are underachievers. Heyder, Kessels and Steinmayr (2017) concluded that girls had better grades

    than boys in the subject of language in secondary school. However, boys had higher scores on one

    IQ test; the verbal intelligence test. The reason for boys’ lower grades might be underachievement,

    since verbal ability is also important for other subjects in school. Self-efficacy, text-reduction strat-

    egies, and anxiety predicted underachievement among highly gifted fourth-grade students (primary

    school). Both underachievers and achievers improved their learning outcomes after an intervention

    aimed at improving learning strategies. Underachievers already used learning strategies, but im-

    proved their outcome when they learned how to do this correctly during the intervention (Obergries-

    ser & Stoeger, 2015). One can conclude that mental ability has an impact on success in school, but

    it is not the only potentially influencing factor. In addition, high mental ability generally does not

    explain the gender gap in school achievement.

    Classroom behaviour and other behaviour

    Schools have different guidelines, cultures, and norms with respect to classroom behaviour. A re-

    view article pertaining to classroom behaviour discussed how pupils could learn proper behaviour

    in elementary school, since maladaptive aggressive behaviour in elementary school can persist into

  • 3

    middle school (Greer-Chase, Rhodes, & Kellam, 2002). In general, teachers have expectations of

    their pupils’ behaviour. A study by Lane, Givner, and Pierson (2004) found that self-control (i.e.,

    temper control) and cooperative ability were important behavioural factors for succeeding in school,

    and assertiveness had a minor impact. Looking at classroom behaviour from another perspective,

    Brendgen, Wanner, Vitaro, Bukowski, and Tremblay (2007) concluded that verbal abuse from

    teachers had long-term consequences for the well-being of pupils that extended into young adult-

    hood. Girls were found to be especially vulnerable (Brendgen et al., 2007).

    Henricsson and Rydell (2006) studied social competence and its impact on school achievement

    and behavioural problems during the first six years in school, and found that internalizing and ex-

    ternalizing problems were persistent characteristics through the years. The authors concluded that

    positive peer interaction had some significance for positive development, and that teacher relations

    had less significance (Henricsson & Rydell, 2006). In line with this, Zhang and Sun (2011) reported

    a bidirectional relationship between pupils’ externalization of their problems and the subsequent

    conflicts with their teachers. Internalizing problems were found to be related to subsequent conflicts

    between the preschool child and the teacher. Conduct and classroom behaviour may have an impact

    on pupils’ achievement in the sense that they act as a filter through which much of the learning

    process occurs in the classroom.

    Sex differences

    There is an increasing awareness of sex differences in schools. A review article by Wernersson

    (2010) presented aspects of boys’ shortcomings in predominantly Swedish schools, and concluded

    that on the individual level, boys’ and girls’ differences in achievement may be explained by their

    differing attitudes towards school and schoolwork. Some achievement-related sex differences might

    be an extended consequence of the sex-segregated labour market of today’s society, and boys have

    many potential paths into the labour market. Wernersson (2010) also concluded that boys were

    struggling with their male identity. Schools were seen as a female arena, and in combination with a

    negative work attitude, this did not encourage boys to engage in schoolwork. Kimmel’s (2010) re-

    port on the “boy crisis” in Sweden and in America (i.e. the increasing problems that boys experience

    in different areas including school) presented almost the same results. Kimmel concluded that mas-

    culinity norms and stereotypes needed to change. Patterns of aggression and violence were dis-

    cussed, as it was concluded that if boys have good (and non-violent) role models during their up-

    bringing, they will not come to view aggressive acts as regular boyish behaviour. To conclude, there

    is a gender gap in school achievement, and boys tend to mature later than girls; but a number of

    potential explanations look to other aspects, such as the conflicts that boys experience regarding

    their male identity.

    Aim

    The aim of this study was to explore sex and skill differences with respect to age/maturation and

    ability in pupils attending the lower classes in a Swedish compulsory school. This is important since

    teachers’ ratings have an impact on pupils’ success in school, and levels of maturation differ between

    same-age pupils at the age of starting school. High ability pupils were compared to lower ability

    pupils, and gender differences explored. The research questions were:

    1. Are there age differences among pupils in terms of classroom behaviour?

    2. Are there age differences among pupils in terms of teachers’ ratings regarding mathematics,

    taken classrooms behaviour at the first measurement point into account?

    3. Are there age differences among pupils in terms of teachers’ ratings regarding Swedish, taken

    classrooms behaviour at the first measurement point into account?

  • 4

    Method

    Sample

    This study was part of a longitudinal study, approved by the ethics committee in Stockholm, in

    which data were gathered at three time points (2009 and Subsequent years). The present set of par-

    ticipants were drawn from a sample of 224 elementary school pupils. Three children were in special

    education courses for intellectually disabled children, and were not included in the study. Parents of

    20 (9.2%) of the children did not want their children to participate in the study. Pupils from three

    classes of each of grades 1, 2, and 3 participated at the first data collection point; three classes from

    each of grades 2, 3, and 4 participated at the second data collection point; and three classes from

    each of grades 3, 4, and 5 participated at the third data collection point. In total, 94.4% of the pupils

    were followed from the first to the third data collection point. At the second data collection point,

    eleven children had moved, and a total of 198 children participated in the data collection. On this

    second occasion, 194 children were screened using four different tests to measure any difficulties

    with learning to read, write, or count, and Raven’s matrices were included.

    There were some sets of incomplete data. In most cases, data existed for 197 children, but the

    Raven’s matrices measurements only included data from 194 children. Drop outs were those for

    whom no data existed regarding the Raven’s matrices, and comprised four children who had moved

    to other schools.

    Material

    Follow-up data As noted above, the study had three measurement points in three consecutive years. Mental ability

    was tested with Raven’s matrices, and teachers’ ratings were collected with an inventory. The

    measures are described below.

    Mental Ability

    Mental ability was measured by Raven’s matrices, a non-culturally-biased intelligence test battery

    in which education and background are of minor importance (Raven, 2000). The teachers were not

    informed of the test results. The response scale was divided into five levels. The mean was 3.16. In

    our analyses the variable was reversed so that high values represented high mental ability. Mental

    ability was dichotomized into those at or below the 75th percentile (low, n=176) and those at or

    above the 90th percentile (n=18). The percentile 10–75 was coded 0 and the percentile 90 and above

    as 1.

    Teachers’ ratings

    Classroom behaviour

    Teachers’ ratings of classroom behaviour were measured on Likert scales with 1 indicating good

    behaviour/lack of difficulties and 5 indicating bad behaviour/existing difficulties (some items were

    reversed). Items from Teachers questionnaire were used. The questionnaire was constructed for this

    study. Classroom behaviour (as is an established concept), consisted of five items (the ability to sit

    still, the ability to concentrate, aggressive behaviour, ability to comprehend instructions, and the

    ability to control behaviour and emotions). Classroom behaviour had a Cronbach’s alpha of .83 for

    the first and second data collection points and .81 at the third data collection point.

    Skills

    The teachers rated the pupils’ skills on a five-point Likert scale. The items were reversed, 5 is indi-

    cating high skills. Teachers’ ratings of mathematics and Swedish (a mean of the reading and the

    writing item). Teachers’ ratings were measured at all time points (The Teacher questionnaire was

    used).

    Procedures

    Descriptive data analyses were carried out in order to calculate means. The means are presented in

    Figures 1–3. A time × mental ability × sex analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed with

    teacher ratings in mathematics and Swedish as dependent variables, respectively. The impact of

    classroom behaviour at the first data collection was also examined as an interaction term.

  • 5

    Results

    The teachers’ ratings of the pupils’ classroom behaviour are shown in Figure 1. There seemed to be

    a peak at the second measurement point for both girls and boys. Classroom behaviour had improved

    at the last measurement point for all groups. However, there were minor differences in means be-

    tween girls and boys. Single means, not repeated ANOVA.

    Figure 1. Teacher ratings of classroom behavior at the measurement points. Note: Girls and boys with low and high mental ability respectively. Sd time 1: girls lo= 0.82, girls hi=1.06; boys lo=1.02, boys hi=1.11. Sd time 2: girls lo=0.86, girls hi=1.28; boys lo=1.07, boys hi=1.07. Sd time 3: girls lo=0.89, girls hi=1,17; boys lo=0.96, boys hi=1.09

    Table 1 presents the results for rated skills in mathematics. There was no significant main effect of

    time. However there were significat two-way intercations (time*classroom behaviour, time*gen-

    der), and a tendency of a interaction time*ability. There was also a significant three-way interaction.

    In Figure 2 means are presented and it is shown that high ability pupils have lower skills in ma-

    temathic than lower ability pupils according to teatcher ratings. However the teacher rated skills in

    mathematic were increasing with time above all for high ability pupils.

    Table 1. Teacher ratings in Mathematics

    Wilks’ Lambda F df sig eta

    time .994 .407 2, 145 .666 .006

    time*classroom behaviour .909 7.244 2, 145 .001 .091

    time*ability .962 2.874 2, 145 .060 .038

    time*gender .954 3.531 2, 145 .032 .046

    time*ability*gender .931 5.365 2, 145 .006 .069

    0

    0.5

    1

    1.5

    2

    2.5

    3

    time 1 time 2 time 3

    classroom behaviour

    girls lo boys lo girls hi boys hi

  • 6

    Figure 2 Teacher ratings of Mathematics at the measurement points.

    Note: Girls and boys with low and high mental ability respectively. Sd time 1: girls lo=1.05 , girls hi=1.41; boys lo=1.01, boys hi=0.98. Sd time 2: girls lo=1.04, girls hi=1.04; boys lo=0.86, boys hi=1.03. Sd time 3: girls lo=1.01, girls hi=0.92; boys lo=0.95, boys hi=1.51

    Table 2 presents the results for rated skills in Swedish. There was no significant main effect of

    time. However, there were significant two-ways interactioneffects of time*classroom behaviour

    and time * gender. There is a tendency for a three-way interaction time*ability*gender. In Figure 3

    means are shown and high ability pupils had lower teacher rated skills in Swedish than low ability

    pupils at measurement point (time) two and three. For high ability girls, teacher rated skills in

    Swedish were decreasing with time.

    Table 2 Teacher ratings in Swedish

    Wilks’ Lambda F df sig eta

    time .996 .323 2, 145 .725 .004

    time*classroom behaviour .940 4.648 2, 145 .011 .060

    time*ability .997 .183 2, 145 .833 .003

    time*gender .943 4.371 2, 145 .014 .057

    time*ability*gender .962 2.880 2, 145 .059 .038

    0

    0.5

    1

    1.5

    2

    2.5

    3

    3.5

    4

    4.5

    5

    time 1 time 2 time 3

    Mathematic

    girls lo boys lo girls hi boys hi

  • 7

    Figure 3 Teacher ratings of Swedish at the measurement points.

    Note: Girls and boys with low and high mental ability respectively. Sd time 1: girls lo=0.87 , girls hi=1.09; boys lo=1.05, boys hi=0.80. Sd time 2: girls lo=0.95, girls hi=1.28; boys lo=1.01, boys hi=1.17. Sd time 3: girls lo=0.89, girls hi=1.43; boys lo=0.97, boys hi=1.69

    Table 3 presents the results for rated skills in mathematics, separated by gender. There were main

    effects of time, however more a tendency among the girls. There were two-way interaction effects

    time*classroom behaviour, however more a tendency among the boys. There was a significant two-

    way interaction among the boys time*abiity.

    Table 3 Teacher ratings in Mathematics girls and boys

    Wilks’ Lambda F df sig eta

    girls

    time .924 3.035 2, 74 .054 .076

    time *classroom behaviour .825 7.833 2, 74 .001 .175

    time*ability .984 .604 2, 74 .549 .016

    boys

    time .911 3.361 2, 69 .040 .089

    time *classroom behaviour .918 3.064 2, 69 .053 .082

    time*ability .806 8.290 2, 69 .001 .194

    Table 4 presents the results for rated skills in Swedish, separated by gender. There were no signifi-

    cant main effect of time, and no significant interactions among the girls. Among boys there is a

    significat two-ways intercationeffects time*classroom behaviours.

    Table 4 Teacher ratings in Swedish girls and boys

    Wilks’ Lambda F df sig eta

    girls

    time .969 1.182 2, 74 .313 .031

    time *classroom behaviour .965 1.328 2, 74 .271 .035

    time*ability .964 1.382 2, 74 .257 .036

    boys

    time .994 .206 2, 69 .814 .006

    time *classroom behaviour .907 3.526 2, 69 .035 .093

    time*ability .960 1.427 2, 69 .247 .040

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    time 1 time 2 time 3

    Swedish

    girls lo boys lo girls hi boys hi

  • 8

    Discussion

    The main purpose was to examine sex and skill differences with respect to age/maturation and ability

    in pupils attending the lower classes in a Swedish compulsory school. Overall, the high-ability stu-

    dents had lower rated skills than pupils with lower ability according to mean differences. There were

    significant two-way interactions effects time*classroom behaviour, time*gender in both Mathemat-

    ics and Swedish, and a three-way interaction (time*ability*gender) in Mathematics. For girls and

    boys respectively, there was a main effect of time in Mathematics, and a two-way interaction effect

    time*classroom behaviour in Mathematic. Just for boys there was a two-way interaction effect

    time*classroom behaviour in Swedish. Further, there was a two-way interaction among the boys

    time*ability in Mathematic. Classroom behaviour seems to improve by time with respect to means.

    Time seems to have an impact on the pupils´ teacher rated skills, as well as their classroom be-

    haviour at the first measurement point. However, there were no main effect of classroom behaviour,

    just for time. There were minor differences with respect to gender as were seen in the time*gender

    interactions and when examine girls and boys respectively. There were minor differences with re-

    spect to mental ability. According to multiple regression analyses, mental ability and classroom be-

    haviour at the first measurement point and classroom behaviour at the second point explained teach-

    ers’ ratings of the pupils’ skills in both Swedish and mathematics. However, if the pupils thought it

    was fun with those subjects was not a significant explanatory factor (Wulff Hamrin & Magnusson,

    in manuscript).

    We have no legitimate reason to include behaviour or maturation in the judgments of pupils’

    skills in Sweden. According to Greer-Chase, Rhodes, and Kellam (2002), maladaptive behaviour

    from an early age will persist throughout higher grades in school. Well-behaved students might be

    expected to also perform well, but this is not always true; Johnson, McGue, and Iacono (2005) found

    the opposite in some cases, especially among boys. Moreover, Sternberg (1997) made a distinction

    between intelligence and intelligent behaviour. External behaviour is not always significant for in-

    ternal talents. Grosin (2004) highlighted the influence of school climate on pupils’ achievement

    possibilities. Improving the school climate might also have a positive impact on conduct, as it may

    provide an environment in which it is easier for teachers to handle maladaptive behaviour among

    the pupils.

    Another finding in the present study was that the high-ability pupils had lower rated skills than

    the pupils with lower ability. This is a contradiction to some previous research (see e.g. Rothenbusch

    et al., 2018). Worth to notice is that the teachers were unaware of the pupils’ scores on the test of

    mental ability.The present results suggest that a low-rated skill might stem from underachievement,

    especially among boys, depending on psychological factors; this is in line with previous work (Hey-

    der, Kessels & Steinmayr, 2017; Obergriesser & Stoeger, 2015). Previous research has found boys

    to have poorer achievement and marks than girls in school (Ingvar, 2010; Kimmel, 2010; Werners-

    son, 2010). One explanation might be that boys mature later, and another is that this has to do with

    expectations concerning boys’ behaviour. In this respect, it might be regarded as a gender role con-

    flict; boys are expected not to do their schoolwork (Ingvar, 2010; Kimmel, 2010; Wernersson, 2010).

    A common view is that ability depends partly on genetic factors and partly on environmental

    factors. However, according to previous research, mental ability and marks are positively related

    (Bull & Johnstone, 1997; Fischbein et al., 1999; Gustafsson & Balke, 1993; Kulik & Kulik, 1982;

    Smedler & Törestad, 1996; Sternberg, 1997; Sternberg & Jarvin, 2003). Moreover, a lack of inat-

    tention or emotional disturbance will increase achievements (Henricsson & Rydell, 2006). Having

    a supportive school context also increases performance (i.e. Grosin, 2004). Motivation, teachers,

    learning strategies, and the home environment have all been found to be important for success in

    school (Swedish National Agency for Education, 2012a).

    Limitations and conclusions

    A strength of this study is that it is based on longitudinal data, which made it possible to take into

    account aspects such as pupils’ maturation and its consequences. However, the study could only

    report on possible relations, and is limited with respect to causation. Moreover, the study examined

    only a selection of possible variables; there are a number of other factors that might be of importance,

    but these were beyond the present scope.

    These results raise some questions for future research, for example regarding the role of pupils’

    maturity and the importance of teachers’ expectations and misconceptions with respect to behaviour.

    In Sweden there are nine years of compulsory school (from about age 6 to 15). Education at the

  • 9

    upper secondary and tertiary levels is voluntary, but parents cannot refuse to let their children attend

    compulsory school. It is possible that maturation should be taken into account in deciding when a

    child should start school.

  • 11

    References

    Brendgen, M., Wanner, B., Vitaro, F., Bukowski, W. M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Verbal abuse by the

    teacher during childhood and academic, behavioral, and emotional adjustment in young adulthood.

    Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(1), 26–38. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.26

    Bull, R., & Johnstone, R. S. (1997). Children’s arithmetical difficulties: contributions from processing

    speed, item identification, and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65(1),

    1–24. doi:10.1006/jecp.1996.2358

    Fischbein, S., Guttman, R., & Nathan, M. (1999). Genetic and environmental influences on pupil perfor-

    mances. Twin Research, 2, 183–195. doi:10.1375/twin.2.3.183

    Greer-Chase, M., Rhodes, W. A., & Kellam, S. G. (2002). Why the prevention of aggressive disruptive

    behaviors in middle school must begin in elementary school. The Clearing House, 75(5), 242–245.

    doi:10.1080/00098650209603948

    Grosin, L. (2004). Skolklimat, prestation och anpassning i 21 mellan- och 20 högstadieskolor. For-

    skningsrapport 71, Pedagogiska institutionen [School climate, achievement and adjustment in 21

    lower and upper secondary schools. Department of Education Research Report 71]. Stockholm:

    Stockholm University.

    Gustafsson, J.-E., & Balke, G. (1993). General and specific abilities as predictors of school achievement.

    Multivariate Behavioural Research, 28(4), 407–434. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2804_2

    Henricsson, L., & Rydell, A.-M. (2006). Children with behaviour problems: the influence of social com-

    petence and social relations on problem stability, school achievement and peer acceptance across the

    first six years of school. Infant and Child Development, 15(4), 347–366. doi:10.1002/icd.448

    Heyder, A., Kessels, U., & Steinmayr, R. (2017). Explaining academic-track boys’ underachievement in

    language grades: not a lack of aptitude but students’ motivational beliefs and parents’ perceptions?

    British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(2), 205–223. doi:10.1111/bjep.12145

    Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1966). Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general

    intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57(5), 253–270. doi:10.1037/h0023816

    Hubert, B., Guimard, P., Florin, A., & Tracy, A. (2015). Indirect and direct relationships between self-

    regulation and academic achievement during the nursery/elementary school transition of French stu-

    dents. Early Education and Development, 26(5–6), 685–707. doi:10.1080/10409289.2015.1037624

    Ingvar, M. (2010). Biologiska faktorer och könsskillnader i skolresultat. Ett diskussionsunderlag för de-

    legationen för jämställdhet i skolans arbete för analys av bakgrunden till pojkars sämre skolprestat-

    ioner jämfört med flickors [Biological factors and sex differences in school achievement: a discuss-

    ion paper for the Delegation for Gender Equality in School with respect to boys’ poor school achie-

    vements compared to girls]. Stockholm: Swedish Ministry of Education and Research.

    Jenner, H. (2004). Motivation och motivationsarbete i skola och behandling. Myndigheten för skolut-

    veckling. Forskning fokus nr 19 [Motivation and motivation work in school and treatment. Swedish

    National Agency for School Improvement research focus no. 19].

    Johnson, W., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Disruptive behavior and school grades: genetic and

    environmental relations in 11-year-olds. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(3), 391–405.

    doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.391

    Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999). The Big Five personality traits, general

    mental ability, and career success across the life span. Personnel Psychology, 52(3), 621–652.

    doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1999.tb00174.x

    Kimmel, M. (2010). Boys and school: a background paper on the “boy crisis”. Swedish government of-

    ficial report 2010:53. Stockholm: Swedish Ministry of Education and Research.

    Kulik, C.-L. C., & Kulik, J. A. (1982). Effects of ability grouping on secondary school students: a meta-

    analysis of evaluation findings. American Educational Research Journal, 19(3), 415–428.

    doi:10.3102/00028312019003415

    Lane, K. L., Givner, C. C., & Pierson, M. R. (2004). Teacher expectations of student behavior: social

    skills necessary for success in elementary school classrooms. The Journal of Special Education,

    38(2), 104–110. doi:10.1177/00224669040380020401

    Malone, A. S., & Fuchs, L. S. (2014). Comparing the contribution of teacher versus tutor ratings of inat-

    tentive behavior in predicting mathematics achievement. Remedial and Special Education, 35(6),

    378–386. doi:10.1177/0741932514539855

  • 12

    Obergriesser, S., & Stoeger. H. (2015). The role of emotions, motivation, and learning behavior in under-

    achievement and results of an intervention. High Ability Studies, 26(1), 167–190.

    doi:10.1080/13598139.2015.1043003

    Pretzlik, U., Olsson, J., Nabuco, M. E., & Cruz, I. (2003). Teachers’ implicit views of intelligence predict

    pupils’ self-perceptions as learners. Cognitive Development 18(4), 579–600. doi:10.1016/j.cog-

    dev.2003.09.008

    Raven, J. C. (2000). Standard progressive matrices (including the parallel and plus versions). Oxford:

    Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Rothenbusch, S., Voss, T., Golle, J., & Zettler, I. (2018). Linking teacher and parent ratings of teacher-

    nominated gifted elementary school students to each other and to school grades. Gifted Children

    Quarterly, 62(2), 230–250. doi:10.1177/0016986217752100

    Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. (2004). General mental ability in the world of work: occupational attainment

    and job performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1), 162–173.

    doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.162

    Smedler, A.-C., & Törestad, B. (1996). Verbal intelligence: a key to basic skills. Educational Studies,

    22(3), 343–356. doi:10.1080/0305569960220304

    SOU (1995). Bildning och kunskap. Särtryck ur skola för utbildning [Education and knowledge. A special

    publication from the School for Education]. SOU 1992:94. Stockholm: Tryckeri Balder AB.

    Sternberg, R. J. (1997). The concept of intelligence and its role in lifelong learning and success. American

    Psychologist, 52(10), 1030–1037. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.10.1030

    Sternberg, R. J., & Jarvin, L. (2003). Alfred Binet’s contributions as a paradigm for impact in psychology.

    In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The Anatomy of Impact: What Makes the Great Works of Psychology Great

    (pp. 89–107). Washington, DC, USA: American Psychological Association.

    Swedish Ministry of Education and Research (1994). Läroplaner för det obligatoriska skolväsendet och

    de frivilliga skolformerna. Lpo 94 / Lpf 94. [Curriculum for compulsory and non-compulsory

    schools].

    Swedish National Agency for Education (2012a). Högpresterande elever, höga prestationer och under-

    visningen. Rapport nr 379 [High-achieving pupils, high achievements, and education. Report no.

    379]. Available from http://www.skolverket.se/publikationer (accessed 12-12-10). Swedish National Agency for Education (2012b). PIRLS 2011. Läsförmågan hos svenska elever i årskurs

    4 i ett internationellt perspektiv. Rapport nr 381 [PIRLS 2011. Reading ability among 4th-grade

    Swedish pupils in an international perspective. Report no. 381]. Available from

    http://www.skolverket.se/publikationer (accessed 2012-12-11). Swedish National Agency for Education (2016). TIMSS 2015. Svenska grundskoleelevers kunskaper i

    matematik och naturvetenskap i ett internationellt perspektiv. Rapport nr 448 [TIMSS. Knowledge

    in mathematics and science among Swedish pupils in compulsory school in an international perspec-

    tive. Report no. 448]. Available from http://www.skolverket.se/publikationer (accessed 2020-05-11).

    Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary Mental Abilities. Psychometric monographs No 1. Chicago, IL, USA:

    University of Chicago Press.

    Wagner, R. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Practical intelligence in real-world pursuits: the role of tacit

    knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(2),

    436–458. doi:10.1037/0022-

    3514.49.2.436

    Wernersson, I. (2010). Könsskillnader I skolprestationer – idéer om orsaker. Rapport V från delegation

    för jämställdhet i skolan [Sex differences in school achievement – ideas about causes. Report V from

    the Delegation for Gender Equality in School]. SOU 2010:51. Stockholm: Swedish Ministry of Ed-

    ucation and Research.

    Wulff, C., Bergman, L. R., & Sverke, M. (2009). General mental ability and satisfaction with school and

    work: a longitudinal study from ages 13 to 48. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(4),

    398–408. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.015

    Zhang, X., & Sun, J. (2011). The reciprocal relations between teachers’ perceptions of children’s behavior

    problems and teacher–child relationships in the first preschool year. The Journal of Genetic Psychol-

    ogy, 172(2), 176–198. doi:10.1080/00221325.2010.528077.

    http://www.skolverket.se/publikationerhttp://www.skolverket.se/publikationerhttp://www.skolverket.se/publikationerhttp://search.proquest.com.ezp.sub.su.se/indexingvolumeissuelinkhandler/60992/Journal+of+Personality+and+Social+Psychology/01985Y08Y01$23Aug+1985$3b++Vol.+49+$282$29/49/2?accountid=38978

  • Research Report Series

    1. Work Values among Swedish Male and Female Students. Katarina Wijk. Department of Occupa-

    tional and Public Health Sciences 2011.

    2. The Doll, the Globe and the Boomerang - Chemical Risks in the Future, Introduced by a Chinese

    Doll Coming to Sweden. Ernst Hollander. Department of Business and Economic Studies 2011.

    3. Divergences in descriptions of the internal work environment management, between employees

    and the management, a case study. Katarina Wijk and Per Lindberg. Department of Occupational

    and Public Health Sciences 2013.

    4. Energy and nutrients from horse manure - Life-cycle data inventory of horse manure management

    systems in Gävleborg, Sweden. Jay Hennessy and Ola Eriksson. Department of Engineering and

    Sustainable Development 2015.

    5. Prospects for biodiesel in Gävleborg County: Feedstock and production. Pontus Norell.

    Department of Building Engineering, Energy Systems and Sustainability Science 2019.

    6. Biogas value chain in Gävleborg: Feedstock, production and use. Muhammad Arfan.

    Department of Building Engineering, Energy Systems and Sustainability Science 2019.

    7. Social inclusion through segregation? Liya Kalinnikova Magnusson and Jeremias Rosenqvist.

    Department of Educational Sciences 2020.

    8. Exploring the Viability of Electric Vehicles: Sustainable Transportation in Gävleborg. Peder

    Kjellén and Katja Tasala Gradin. Department of Building Engineering, Energy Systems and Sus-

    tainability Science 2020.

  • Published by:

    Gävle University Press

    Postal address: SE-801 76 Gävle, Sweden

    Visiting address: Kungsbäcksvägen 47

    Telephone: +46 26 64 85 00

    www.hig.se