kigo4789 - aare · australian association for research in education- aare international education...

48
1 KIGO4789 THE TEACHING OF NARRATIVES AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28 TH Nov-2 ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good: Positioning Education Research MUTUOTA KIGOTHO MACQUARIE UNIVERSITY SYDNEY NSW 2109 AUSTRALIA Email: [email protected]

Upload: others

Post on 23-Jul-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

1

KIGO4789

THE TEACHING OF NARRATIVES

AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE

INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE

28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004

Melbourne Australia

Doing the Public Good: Positioning Education Research

MUTUOTA KIGOTHO

MACQUARIE UNIVERSITY

SYDNEY NSW 2109

AUSTRALIA

Email: [email protected]

Page 2: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

2

Abstract

This paper presents a case for the use of narrative as an instructional tool. Available

literature on the narrative and reviewed within the last thirty years seems to suggest

that narrative might be a significant tool of instruction. Given the variety of uses that

researchers in narrative have explored so far, the paper suggests that the narrative has

the potential to influence the school system and that the public might be better served

by enhancing and encouraging students and teachers to continue the practice of using

narrative in teaching and learning especially in the lower secondary school.

Introduction

Knowledge of narrative structure assists students to improve their narrative writing

skills and studies have shown that teaching narrative structure to students improves

their narrative writing skills. This has been shown in instances where English

narratives have been used to assist students whose mother tongue is English to create

and write compositions. The current study extends the parameters of previous studies

in using a similar methodology to assist Kenyan students to improve their writing of

narratives in English. Based on accepted findings that the structure of Kenyan

narratives is similar to that of other narratives it was believed that teaching the

structure of the Kenyan narrative to students, their writing of narratives in English

would improve considerably.

Several teaching methods have been used to teach structure in narratives. These

methods fit within the two broad frameworks of modelling and the story map.

Straight-forward modelling (e.g. Sipe; 1993; Cairney, 1990) has been used

extensively. The term 'modelling' has been defined as the process of offering behavior

for imitation through showing or telling (Hung, 2002; Gallimore and Tharp, 1990;

Tharp, 1993). Sipe (1993) uses modelling as a narrative teaching strategy by assisting

students to imitate aspects of the plot to create narratives 'deliberately modelled on

older ones' (Sipe, 1993 p. 18). Sipe (1993) prefers to use the term 'transformations'

since the students are allowed to adjust the plot giving it a twist in whatever direction

they wished. Sipe's version of modelling is exemplified below.

Page 3: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

3

In addition to Sipe's modelling the various uses of the story map as a narrative-

teaching strategy will also be exemplified. The story map is a graphic representation

of the structure of the story. There is a significant amount of literature related to the

story map available and thus this paper discusses the less available strategy referred to

as modelling (Sipe 1993) as well as the use of intertextual links as a form of

modelling (Cairney, 1990).

The narrative teaching strategies are illustrated in figure I below.

Modelling Modelling (Sipe, 1993; Cairney, 1990)

The story map Story maps (Beck and McKeown, 1981; Davis and McPherson, 1989)

Story schema/flow charts) Mandler and Johnson, 1977; Gordon and Braun,1982, 1983; Kroll and Anson, 1984; Geva, 1983; Ollson, 1989)

Story Maps with Character Perspectives- SMCP (Emery and Mihalevich,1992; Emery, 1996)

Character Perspective Charts -CPC (Shanahan and Shanahan, 1997)

Figure 1 Narrative teaching strategies

Modelling

The basic form of modelling is used as follows: First the teacher identifies what is to

be modelled. An example of this could be the writing process model suggested by

Hayes and Flower (1980a). This model views the composing process as comprised of

three phases namely, planning, translating and reviewing. As in the discussion of this

model above, these phases have sub-processes for instance planning has organizing

and goal-setting as sub-processes while reviewing has reading and editing. Secondly,

the teacher applies the model in teaching the composing process by offering

instruction based on the three named aspects while monitoring student progress. By

imitating a model as explained by the teacher students learn to create texts such as a

written narrative.

As a narrative teaching strategy, modelling includes the use of transformations (Sipe,

1993), and establishing intertextual links (Cairney, 1990).

Page 4: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

4

The use of transformations as a form of modelling

According to Sipe (1993) transforming a story involves retelling it. However

transformations are distinguishable from retellings in that a retelling attempts to be

faithful to the original story while a transformation can range farther afield. Using the

terms adapted by Sipe (1993) transformations manifest themselves in four forms

namely; parallelism, deconstructions, extended versions of the original tale, and the

use of illustrations (Sipe, 1993 p.18).

The first form of transformation is the parallel method. As the name suggests, the

parallel model denotes the creation of a new story written as a parallel to an old tale.

For example: Calmenson's (1989) The Principal's New Clothes has been written as a

parallel of The Emperor's New Clothes by Hans Christian Andersen. Sipe (1993)

indicates that the new story's humour depends in part on their knowledge of the old

story (p. 19). The students imitate the plot but change the names of characters and

adjust the action of the story.

The second form of transformation is deconstruction. The term 'deconstruction' as

used by Sipe (1993) refers to the creation of new stories loosely based on traditional

stories. For example, Sleeping Ugly by Jane Yolen (1981) is a deconstruction of

Sleeping Beauty. 'Only the title, a magic spell, a long sleep and a kiss remind us or the

original story' (Sipe, 1993 p. 19). In this example of imitation, 'Beauty' -a noun- is

substituted by 'Ugly'- an adjective turned into a noun.

The third form of transformation refers to the 'conscious manipulations of a traditional

story, a form of extended language play (Martinez and Nash, 1992, cited in Sipe, 1993

p.19). The "Chicken Little" story is one example of a traditional story that has been

transformed by various writers. William Stobbs (1968) has the protagonist Henny-

Penny picking up corn in the courtyard when she is hit by something from the sky.

She runs scared and sets out to tell the King that "the sky's a-going to fall". Along the

way, she comes across a variety of the bird flock such as the cock, duck, the goose

and the turkey and all join in the trip to see the King. Then they come across the

cunning fox. Intending to kill and eat them, the fox tricks them into entering his cave.

One by one, the fox cuts their heads off dumping them in a waiting van as he waits for

his next victim. But the hen is lucky since in an attempt to snap her head, the fox

Page 5: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

5

injured her but not fatally. She runs away and forgoes her mission to inform the King

about the "falling sky". Stephen Kellog's Chicken Little (1988) has been cited in Sipe

(1993 p. 19) as an example of a conscious manipulation of this traditional story.

While remaining faithful to the original story Kellog gives his story a new twist when

he radically alters the ending so that in the new story the fox is arrested and put in jail.

The fourth form of transformation involves the story being transformed only by the

use of new illustrations. Sipe (1993 p. 19) cites the example of Hansel and Gretel by

Anthony Browne (1990) where the words of Brothers Grimm story are retained. The

illustrations in the new story set the story in modern-day England giving an entirely

different feeling to the old tale (Sipe, 1993 p. 19).

Other modelling techniques used by Sipe (1993) included addressing literary issues of

style, theme, characterization and topic in text. By the use of modern colloquial

language students composed an imitation of the story of Goldilocks. By retaining

details and the organization of the original story, the students gave the story a modern

version by turning it into a written version of rap.

Additional questions of literary style were dealt with by comparing and contrasting

pairs of stories- the old story and the new story. Students were provided with large

pieces of paper on which they drew charts where they could 'compare and contrast the

written versions of each pairs of stories visually' (Sipe, 1993 p. 20).

The researcher then got the students to draw a chart that summarized the kinds of

transformations that in their view were possible with stories. The students arrived at

nine modes of transformations. These are given as-

• the use of language to alter the style from an old tale to a modern one

• change or add the details in the plot

• change a few of the main events in the plot

• keep a few of the main events but change most of the plot

• change the setting

• characters and detail

Page 6: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

6

• alter occupations of characters, their gender, reversal of roles, write a sequel to the

original story, and finally, keep the words of the original story but change the

illustrations (Sipe, 1993 p. 22).

Sipe (1993) offers a summary of their finding stating that-

"We were impressed by the quality of the writing that resulted and by the degree to

which students' stories forged intertextual links (Cairney, 1990) with what they had

read". Following De Beaugrande (1980) and Kristeva (1980), Cairney (1990 p. 480)

explains 'intertextuality' as the process of interpreting one text by means of a

previously composed text.

Intertextual links as a form of modelling

According to Cairney (1990 p. 478) "the germs of writing can often be traced to

previous literary experiences with texts written, read or heard". Cairney seems to

suggest that what children read or hear influences what they write about. In an attempt

to establish the existence or otherwise of intertexual links, Cairney asked eighty grade

six students from the Riverina region of New South Wales to report on whether they

ever thought of stories they have read when writing a story. If students gave a positive

response, the researcher asked to be given an example. The student was further asked

at what point they recalled the earlier story, and they were also expected to provide

the name of the earlier story they were referring to. Ninety per cent of the students

indicated that they were aware of intertextual links, while ten per cent could not recall

any specific instances.

Cairney (1990) found that there were those students that had attempted to copy or

reproduce a genre they had read. However, other students came up with an original

plot. Some students also indicated that they had based their writing around a central

character borrowed from a story they had read. Some students used specific ideas

without copying the plot of a story. An idea or event from a story triggered the writer's

memory of another text. Some students copied the plot and ideas, essentially copying

the story. Students attempted to rewrite the story they had read. There were other

students that attempted to transfer content from expository to narrative texts. Such

students 'attempted to reduce the complexity of the writing process by concentrating

Page 7: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

7

upon a topic for which they saw themselves as expert' (Cairney, 1990 p. 482). There

were other students that created a narrative from several other narratives. Such

students considered by the researcher as being of high ability (they constituted the top

six per cent) evidenced an awareness of writing a story based upon a number of other

stories (Cairney, 1990 p. 482).

The study by Cairney (1990) suggests that what children hear, for instance, oral

narratives might influence what they write. Orr (1986) holds a similar view in stating

that

No literary text is written in a vacuum. Besides the general culture surrounding thetext, and the author's own horizon, (i.e. his experiences, prejudices use of languagesystem worldview and so on), there are perhaps more importantly, other textsespecially literary texts (p. 814), (cited in Cairney, 1990 p. 480)

Similar sentiments are found in the work of Kristeva (1980) who is cited in Cairney

(1990, p. 480) as seeing intertextuality as a form of dialogue with the total texts of

reader's experiences. The work of Sipe (1993) and Cairney (1990) demonstrates

modelling as a useful narrative teaching strategy where teachers encourage students to

use imitation to create stories. This strategy has its limitations, some of which include

the high dependence on the availability of published stories for imitation.

The story map

The story map (Beck and McKeown, 1981; Davis and McPherson, 1989) has been

extensively used to teach narrative writing. While retaining a basic typology, a

variety of terms are used to describe the story map. Such terms include the story

schema, (Gordon and Braun, 1982), and flow-chart (Geva, 1983; Ollman, 1989), the

Story Map with Character Perspectives- SMCP (Emery and Mihalevich, 1992; Emery,

1996), and Character Perspective Charts- CPC (Shanahan and Shanahan, 1997). This

information is represented in Figure I above. The story map is a widely recognized

area of narrative research and a significant number of researchers agree on the vital

part the story map has played in this area of study. That literature is widely available

and it will not be discussed in much detail in the current study. The story map can be

summed up in a brief mention of ‘the story schema’ below.

Page 8: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

8

The story schema

A ‘story schema’ has been defined as ‘an individual’s mental representation of story

components and the way these components fit together’ (Whaley, 1981). Story

schema has also been defined as ‘an idealized internal representation of the parts of a

typical story and the relationships among those parts’ (Mandler and Johnson, (1977).

To obtain a visual representation of the schema, the details of a story are reorganized

in such a manner as to form a tree diagram as shown in figure 2 below.

STORY

setting event structure

episode

beginning development ending

Figure 2 The story structure (Source: Kroll and Anson, 1984 p. 156)

Kroll and Anson (1984) have relied on Mandler and Johnson (1977) story grammar

model to draw this representation of structure. In this model the teacher begins by

explaining the story elements to the students. Such elements include ‘title’, ‘setting’,

‘theme’, ‘plot’, ‘characters’, ‘resolution’ and ‘episode’. The pupils are shown a

schematic representation of the story in the form of a tree diagram.

Figure 3 shows the network of story categories and the main relationships –then,

cause or and, connect episodes. Terminology according to the authors was also

adapted for classroom use so that story parts and relationships could be discussed in

terms familiar to the students.

Page 9: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

9

Figure 3 Story Title

Setting Theme Plot Resolution

Goal of main character, or

Intended message of author.

Major

Setting

Minor setting A Episode A

1.Location 1. Starter event (Initiating

event)

2.Time 2. Inner response (internal response)

3.Characters 3. Action (Attempt)

4. Outgoing state 4. What happens ? (Outcome )

Cause or then or and

Minor Setting B Episode B

1.

2

3.

4.

Minor setting C Episode C

1.

2.

3.

4.

Figure 3 Schematic representation of the story structure (Source: The Reading Teacher Nov. 1983

pp.117

When using story schema as a model for teaching narratives, several roles have been

suggested for the teacher, (Gordon and Braun, 1983). Since the analysis is complex

there is need to select well-formed narratives as they have clear structure. There is

also need to rewrite stories so that they are made more suitable for story grammar

instruction since as Reedy and Riley (2000 p. 66) explain, there is a danger of children

being left to infer that telling a story chronologically is the same thing as writing a

Page 10: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

10

'narrative'. The two researchers state that children write 'narrative' as opposed to

'recounts' when the resulting stories bear evidence of the crucial move to "describing a

dramatic disruption of the ongoing events which leads to a final resolution" (p. 66).

The visual representation in the form of a diagram is also important. Psychologists are

generally agreed that children tend to recall what they see more than they recall what

they hear. This seems to stress the power of vision. Another task would be related to

the fact that the teacher needs to formulate questions that would be asked to assist

students develop expectations about contents of the story. The teachers need to

distinguish narratives that have plots as opposed to personal experience narratives

(Reedy and Riley, 2000 p.66). In narratives for instance, characters are depicted as

fitting within an event structure or plot. It is for the teacher to point out this

relationship, and not leave it to the pupil to infer. At this point it is crucial that

students grasp knowledge of the structure of the story. Knowledge of the structure and

its visual representation will assist in recall (Mandler and Johnson, 1977). This will

have a positive effect on the pupils since the instruction about narrative structure was

intended to activate the pupils’ passive knowledge of structure and then get the pupils

to practice the use of such knowledge of narrative structure in composing their own

narratives. This will assist in making the students implicit knowledge of structure

explicit.

The step-by-step method of teaching narrative writing suggested by Gordon and

Braun (1983) presents a practical approach that teachers could use to teach writing.

Given below is a summary of the use of the schema as a narrative teaching strategy.

• The students are exposed to a story to be used for instruction. The teacher can read

such a story in class or post it on the wall for the students to read at their own free

time. Students receive instruction on the structure of a familiar narrative.

• Expose the students to a simplified global organization structure of a narrative. A

simple tree diagram showing story, setting, an event structure and episodes with a

beginning, a development and an ending. This provides the basic structure to be

used as a model for instruction.

• The teacher explicitly relates story content to the story structure pointing out to the

student elements of story structure targeted for instruction.

Page 11: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

11

• Student practices writing story parts such as orientation, the main event and a

closure the application of such story elements in the narratives they write. Each

part is practiced well enough before moving to the next part.

Owing to the clarity of the steps used the current study adopted this method of

instruction and used it along with transposition. The third form of transposition, one

that involves the 'conscious manipulation of a traditional story, a form of extended

language play' has been selected as the most appropriate additional model. This model

gives the student a free hand to create a narrative bearing a twist of ones choice.

Factors that affect quality in narrative are given as characterization- the creation of a

central protagonist with individual motives, traits and dispositions (Kroll and Anson,

1984 p. 179). Good characterization also meant giving the central character a motive

for his or her action. "Several children gave special attention to creating a rich and

interesting central character" (p. 179). Some children - a relatively small number -

used dialogue to create the central character, most often a comic characterization (p.

180). Another factor affecting quality include- starting a story with dialogue. Kroll

and Anson give an example of a writer that appears impressive by opening a story

with the stream of action, moves back to provide background, and then commence the

main events of the narrative. There were other writers that employed short sentences

quite deliberately to achieve the effect of suspense. Still other writers tried

experimenting with words for instance, one writer states "On a Sunday when all the

animals would be hanging around 'boringly'". Then there were writers that used a

device common in animal tales: use of alliterative names for the animals (for example

'Larry the lion', 'Eric the elephant'). Kroll and Anson (1984) have listed the elements

listed above as factors other than structure that serve to determine quality in writing.

A possible limitation of story schema is that events in the story are likely to focus on

what happens to the main character in the story. This might lead to the other

characters in the story being ignored (Shanahan and Shanahan, 1997). Such a

criticism might not be valid since it has been shown that by the use of embedding, it is

possible to chart out events in a story to include more than one character.

Page 12: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

12

Arising from the analyses of narratives written by children (Kroll and Anson, 1984), it

can be concluded that the story schema is a suitable model for teaching narrative

writing among children with a view to enhancing structure in stories. The current

study extends Gordon and Braun (1982, 1983) and aims at raising schema awareness

among Kenyan secondary school pupils.

The story map particularly the story schema has been selected as the principal model

to be used in explicating narrative structure. This model was reinforced by the use of

scaffolding as suggested by Vygotsky's (1978) social cultural theory known as the

Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD). Vygotsky has defined the ZPD as- 'the

distance between actual developmental level as determined by independent problem

solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving

under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers' (Vygotsky, 1978

p.86). Vygotsky's theory would be useful in assisting students use local narratives as a

model for 'awakening' or make active their passive knowledge of narrative structure.

The ZPD represents a variety of internal developmental processes that are able to

operate only when the child is interacting with people in his environment and in

cooperation with his peers (Vygotsky, 1978 p. 90). The term 'scaffold' has been used

to represent a process wherein an adult provides support to a child learning to master a

problem. In the case of this intervention, the researcher provided the scaffolding. In

the current study the intervention was aimed at activating students’ knowledge of

structure so that they might use that knowledge to create narratives that were elaborate

and more coherent. This is made credible since as Vygotsky (1978) argues 'what a

child can do with assistance today she will be able to do by herself tomorrow' (p. 87).

Method

The subjects

The subjects were 177 Form one students from Kenya from two public rural girls'

schools aged between 15 and 18. School A had 92 students while school B had 85.

This age group was selected because the children are in their first year of secondary

school, and at this age, oral narratives still generate significant attention among

students. The children selected for this study had eight years of exposure to English

language in the primary school. In Kenya children undergo four years of secondary

school and four years of university learning. In the secondary school, the students are

Page 13: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

13

taught narrative writing and then tested in the national examinations. Forms one and

two constitute the lower secondary and would be the Australian equivalents of Years

Nine and Ten. In developing countries, children begin school at various ages.

The research design

The aim of the current study was to determine whether the implementation of a

teaching strategy designed to activate students' implicit knowledge of the structure of

the traditional Kenyan oral narratives, could enable Kenyan students to write better

quality narratives. At the beginning of the program, the students were required to

write three narratives as part of a pretest in order to establish their narrative writing

competence in English. As part of the intervention, the researcher spent six weeks

teaching one class of students in each school. These two classes comprised the

experimental group. At the end of the instruction period, the students were required to

write another three narratives known as the posttest. This was done to verify whether

the intervention had improved the quality of the narratives the students wrote. Quality

ratings were obtained by having raters, blind to the conditions and nature of

instruction, provide ratings for the stories. Measures used to determine the writing

quality of the narratives included structure, coherence and cohesion. These measures

are discussed below. To ensure that any improvement in quality could be attributed to

the intervention, a second class of pupils from each school was tested at the same time

as the experimental groups using similar instruments and in similar conditions. These

two classes comprised the control group, and during the intervention period they

received only their regular classroom program. The quality of narratives written by

the control group was not expected to improve significantly between the two times

they were tested. Campbell and Stanley, (1966) have called a scheme such as the one

described here as an intervention study comprising a pretest posttest control group

design.

Procedure

The first school was visited in the second half of term two, while the second school

was visited in the first half of term three. At the first meeting, the researcher stated the

purpose of the research. This was given as: to investigate the relationship between

students' understanding of oral narrative structure and their ability to compose written

Page 14: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

14

narratives. Students would choose whether they wished to participate in the study or

not. Students that accepted to take part in the study would be required to write six

narratives as part of the study. Three narratives would be written before the teaching

took place, and three at the end. It was also made clear that those who chose to

participate were free to withdraw at any point in the study if they so wished. Consent

was sought from those pupils aged over sixteen, while parents of younger students

gave consent on behalf of their children. Within the intervention period, including

testing, each of the experimental groups received thirty-six forty-minute lessons

within a period of six weeks. The writing tasks were comprised of three story types.

Story type one required that students write a narrative with a traditional setting. In

story type two students were required to develop a story from a given prompt. Story

type three was free writing where students were given topic headings and asked to

write on a theme of their choice.

Instruction and the lesson plan

The researcher selected seven local narratives known to the students, typed them and

then posted them at the back of the class. The students were encouraged to read the

narratives for familiarity of plot, characterization, content, form, narrative structure,

morals and themes.

Given below is a summary of the lesson plan-

The teacher introduces the lesson by referring to the copy at the back of the

classroom. The teacher then guides the students through the story pointing out to them

the various story elements as they appear in the text allowing for some discussion in

class. The teacher poses questions as the story is covered in class.

The key instructional areas focus on the orienting background information that aims

at providing detailed information regarding foreshadowing, and then characterization

in the narrative. The issue of the complicating action and the conflict resolution is also

highlighted. In narratives that have more than one episode, the teacher identifies the

episodes guiding the students through how the main character deals with all issues

raised in the story. The students are guided in relating the story structure with the

composing process.

It was intended that as a result of this strategy, the following would result. The

students would be able to predict a story content prior to or during the instructional

Page 15: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

15

period. Further, the strategy would guide students’ comprehension by serving as a

framework for organising what is learned, as well as framing oral and written story

recall. The strategy would also aid in planning a story before, as well as during

writing. The strategy would assist in revising a story at the local level (one aspect of

the structure), or at the global level (the whole organisational framework). Finally,

the students would use questions based on text structure to monitor meaning during

writing.

Measures

In this section, structure, coherence and cohesion as measures that were used to judge

the quality of the scripts written are examined. In the current study, the primary

consideration was to investigate the impact of an intervention based upon instruction

that involved making explicit students' implicit knowledge of narrative structure, the

current study investigated what effect such an intervention would have upon students'

writing competence.

Story structure

Story structure was measured using a slightly modified scale for scoring the inclusion

and quality of the parts of a story (Graham and Harris, 1996). The scale was intended

to measure the schematic structure of written stories and it has eight story parts and a

possible total score of nineteen points. The story parts include: introduction of the

main character; description of the locale or place where the narrative is set,

information about the time of the story, an initiating event (or starter event), the goal

formulated by the main character in reaction to the initiating event, a planned action in

an effort to achieve a goal, the ending result, and the final reaction of the main

character to the outcome. Each story element receives a numerical score for its

inclusion and quality of development. In the modified form of this scale, three

elements of expression including vocabulary, grammar and voice, as well as

multiplicity of episodes and chaining have been included. Character has also been

expanded to cover the protagonist and the antagonist. The total possible score has

been revised and brought to thirty-eight. The scale is given as appendix 1.

Page 16: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

16

Major Setting - The orienting background information gives a setting that provides

elaborate details of the time and place of a narrative. The major setting provides

general background information about when and where the narrative happened. For

instance a narrative could begin thus-

Example 1 an elaborate setting

1) In Kikuyuland long before our parents were born,

2) there lived a famous hunter known as Mwangi.

3) He was a six-foot, well-built and strong man respected among the Kikuyu.

4) He came from the Anjiku clan.

5) He loved his family and neighbours.

6) One day, Mwangi woke up feeling excited and in need of an adventure.

Minor setting- Line 6 in example one above constitutes the minor setting. This

provides more specific information about a particular day or event. It is usually

marked by the phrase "One day…" For instance- "One day Mwangi woke up feeling

excited and in need of an adventure".

Line 1-5 in example one above represents an elaborate setting. The major setting is

covered by the first five sentences. A writer that gives such an elaborate major setting

is awarded two points. A writer that includes a minor setting in the story gets two

marks. An elaborate setting earns a total of four marks two marks for the major setting

and two marks for the minor setting.

The abstract- this part of the orientation indicates to the reader what will happen or

what might happen in the narrative without giving the narrative away. There is usually

a mention of something that is significant in the narrative. Berman and Slobin, (1994)

use the term foreshadowing. Though not always present in a narrative, researchers

such as Berman and Slobin have suggested that when they appear, story items that

foreshadow what will follow in the story- (abstracts) - make narratives clearer-

Every narrative is about something. The abstract, if there is one, tells us what a

narrative is about. It is usually about the maximally reportable event, though the

relocation of the orientation section can alter this perception on the part of the

listeners.

Page 17: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

17

Usually the abstract comes quite early in the story, though it could also be slipped in

at a latter point in a narrative. The abstract summarises the point in advance or states a

general proposition that the narrative will exemplify (Cortazzi & Jin, 2000, p. 104).

These researchers have suggested the example given below-

Something terrible happened to me… at the weekend. I locked my baby in the car.

Another illustration is derived from the study reported by Labov (1982)-

- My brother put a knife in my head. Like kids, you get into a fight, and I twisted his

arm from behind him (Labov, 1982, p. 226).

Writers that include an abstract in their narratives are awarded two marks. If a

narrative has no abstract, then a mark of zero is scored.

Characters

When a character is introduced in a narrative, it would be expected that the writer use

linguistic features that reveal traits that make the character stand out. One way of

doing this is by providing an adjective (or adjectives) describing the character(s). To

illustrate this point several examples of adjectives are given below.

-Hawk’s patience was unequalled.

- Hen was careless.

- The King of El Molo was vindictive, rash and impatient.

-The foolish, greedy and gluttonous hyena got on her feet.

-The witty and cunning hare refused to give up. His resilience was admirable.

- Mweru, the tortoise was a cool and calculating animal.

Another way by which characters manifest themselves is by writers using

comparisons that can either be similes or metaphors. Some examples to illustrate this

point are suggested below

Similes Metaphors

Mwai is as cunning as a fox. The fox smiled (used in reference to Mwai)

Joe is as treacherous as Judas was. Judas was leaning on the wall (referring to Joe)

Jo was as cute as Cleopatra was. Cleopatra leaped to her feet (referring to Jo)

Page 18: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

18

Kamau was as stupid as a goat. The goat blurted out (referring to Kamau).

Examples

Mwangi was a famous hunter in Kambaland.

He was well known for his hunting skills and gigantic body.

Mwangi was also a generous and helpful person.

In the current study, a total of nineteen elements were scored. Three of these elements

were judged as either being present or absent. These included the abstract, multiplicity

at the point of the main event, and multiplicity at closure. A narrative will either have

an abstract or have none at all. Similarly, a narrative will either be a single episode

narrative or have multiple episodes. Such elements were judged either as zero when

they were absent, or judged as two when they were present. For all other story

elements, a score of zero was assigned if the element was absent, a score of one if the

element was included and not elaborated, and a score of two if the element was

mentioned and elaborated. By totalling the scores for the individual elements, a total

score was calculated at 38 possible points.

In this study, it was intended that students would use the activated implicit knowledge

of the structure of traditional narratives to assist them create new narratives based on

such knowledge. The objective remained to determine how students applied such

knowledge of structure in creating new narratives. Once the narratives were written,

they had to be assessed for writing quality. Coherence and cohesion were used to

determine the quality of the written narratives.

Coherence

Coherence refers to the overall discourse level property of unity or how well a text

held together. Coherence depends on how ideas are linked so that they are easily

understood (compare Bamberg, 1984; Van Dijk, 1977; Brostoff, 1981). Coherence is

defined as the logical transition, or flow, of ideas or events throughout a story.

Coherence in text was measured by a scale designed by Bamberg (1984). With slight

modifications, the scale has been followed by other researchers and found to be

effective (Fitzgerald & Spiegel, 1986; Fitzgerald & Teasley, 1986). The scale to

measure coherence is given as Appendix 2. The current study adopts seven

dimensions of the text (e.g. Fitzgerald and Spiegel (1986, p. 268) and has used them

Page 19: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

19

in making scoring decisions on a six-point scale. The seven dimensions were the

degree to which: (a) the writer identified the theme or topic; (b) the writer stayed on

the topic or theme and did not digress; did not shift topics or digress; (c) the writer

oriented the reader by creating a context or situation; (d) the writer organized details

according to a discernible plan and sustained it throughout the composition; (e) the

writer used cohesive ties to link sentences and/or paragraphs together; (f) the writer

concluded with a statement that created a sense of closure; and (g) the discourse

flowed smoothly as indicated by the lack of grammatical and/or mechanical errors.

Cohesion

In the current study, cohesion refers to the visible linguistic features of a text that

enable a reader to refer back to an item mentioned earlier so that the reader sees the

text as hanging together (Halliday & Hasan, 1976, pp.5-10). Cohesion analysis

suggests that what makes a text hang together are visible features of language. In

contrast, coherence analysis posits that what makes a text hang together is the

connectedness of ideas in a text. Ideas are not visible features of text, but the writer

presents cues that the reader shares with the writer.

Halliday and Hasan (1976) refer to the linguistic features of cohesion as 'cohesive

ties'. The ties that have been pointed out by Halliday and Hasan (1976), as most

significant ties include pronominals (e.g. he, she they, us, me), demonstratives (e.g.

these those), the definite article - the-, additives (e.g. and, also), adversatives (e.g. but,

however, yet), causals (e.g. because, if), temporals (e.g. when, sometimes, finally,

then), similar lexical items (the hen, the hen, the hen), collocations (e.g. the boy, the

lad, the young man). All these cohesive ties were counted and examined against the

total number of words in a story. Following Cooper, (1983) all the variables were

controlled for length of protocol by calculating them per 100 words (see also

Fitzgerald and Spiegel, 1986). Isaacson, (1988) has used a count of words as a means

of determining fluency of text.

Page 20: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

20

Results and discussion

Structure

The data below demonstrates the general patterns of the results of an analysis of

structure from the stories written in both schools. In this section, the question of

whether the intervention improved the structure of children's stories will be

considered. This question will be examined in three ways. Firstly, some scripts that

children produced were judged not to be stories (i.e. non-story scripts). Therefore, it is

important to consider the question of whether the intervention decreased the incidence

of non-story scripts. A second way to address the question is to determine whether the

intervention influenced structure in a global sense. Finally, it is important to ascertain

which specific aspects of structure the intervention influenced.

The effect of the intervention on non-story production

Shown below are three tests Pearson's Chi square, Cochran's Q and McNemar's test

carried out to analyse the data to determine: whether any differences were observable

at pretest, and to determine if any differences in performance existed between the

experimental and control groups.

Pearson Chi square was used at pretest to determine if any differences existed

between experimental and control groups on each story type. The Chi square was

further used at posttest to determine whether there were any differences between

experimental and control groups on each story type.

Cochran's Q was used at pretest to determine whether any differences existed between

the story types for experimental and control groups separately. The test was further

used at posttest to determine whether differences existed between story types for

experimental and control groups examined separately.

McNemar's test was conducted to determine whether the stories produced any

differences between pretest and posttest, and if so, determine the nature of change if

any on each story type for experimental and control group separately. Results from

these three tests are shown below. The figure 4 below shows percentage of students

producing non-stories. It is suggested that Figure 4 is viewed alongside Table 1

below.

Page 21: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

21

Figure 4 Differences between experimental and control group on each story type atboth pretest and posttest

At pre-test the two groups did not differ significantly on any of the three story types.

However, there is a trend for the control group to produce fewer non-stories than the

experimental group on story type 1.

Table 1 Results of Chi-square test (X2 and p values shown) comparing the experimental and controlgroups on each story type at pretest

Story type 1 Story type 2 Story type 3

X2 2.810 0.080 0.074

P 0.094 0.476 0.516

This trend is important because it is responsible for the fact that the effect of story

type on non-story production was different for the two groups (See figure 1 above).

For the control group the most difficult task (i.e. more non-stories) was story type 2,

the easiest task (i.e. fewer non-stories) was story type 1, and the difference between

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

18

exppre cntlpre exppost cntlpost

% s

tude

nts

prod

ucin

g no

n-st

orie

s

story type 1story type 2story type 3

Page 22: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

22

them was statistically significant. In terms of difficulty, story type 3 was in between

these two extremes and was not significantly different from either. Table 2 below

shows Cochran's Q test

Table 2 Results of Cochran's Q test (Q and p values shown) comparing differences between the threestory types at pretest for experimental and control groups separately.

Experimental

Control

Q P Q P*Between 3story types

1.909 0.491 8.778 0.011

**Between1&2

8.333 0.006

**Between1&3

1.286 0.453

**Between2&3

2.882 0.143

*Df=2 Degrees of freedom**Df=1There were differences between the three story types for the control group (Story type

1 > story type 3>story type 2). However, pairwise multiple comparisons revealed that

only the extreme difference (i.e. between 1 and 2) was significant.

There were no differences between the three story types for the experimental group.

However, the fact that the experimental group tended to produce more non-stories in

story type 1 tended to close the gap between the two extremes, thereby eliminating the

effect of story type on non-story production seen in the control group.

The only significant change over time was that the experimental group produced

fewer non-stories for story type 1. (See Table 3 below)

Table 3: Results of McNemar's tests (p values shown) comparing pretests and posttest scores on eachstory type for experimental and control groups separately.

Experimental Control

Story 1 0.016 0.500

Story 2 0.629 1.000

Story 3 0.435 0.754

Page 23: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

23

The experimental group produced significantly fewer non-stories at post-test than they

did at pre-test. The trend towards a difference between the two groups observed at

pre-test was eliminated, with neither group producing any non-stories on story type 1

at post-test.

At post-test the two groups did not differ significantly on any of the three story types,

and there was an impact of story-type non-story production for the experimental

group as well as for the control group.

Table 4 Results of Chi-square test (X2 and p values shown) comparing the experimental and controlgroups on each story type at post test

Story type 1* Story type 2 Story type 3

X2 - 0.021 1.797

P - 0.526 0.155

* No student produced a non-story for story type 1

No significant differences between experimental and control groups on story types 2

and 3.

For both the experimental and the control groups (NB like for the control group at

pre-test) the most difficult task (i.e. more non-stories) was story type 2, the easiest

task (i.e. fewer non-stories) was story type 1, and the difference between them was

statistically significant. In terms of difficulty, Story type 3 was in between these two

extremes for both groups however, because of slight differences in relative

performance of the two groups, the pattern of significance was different. For the

experimental group there were fewer non-stories with story type 3 than with story

type 2, there being no difference between story type 1 and story type 3. However, the

converse was true for the control group, there were fewer non-stories with story type 1

than with story type 3, there being no difference between story type 2 and story type

Page 24: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

24

3. Cochran's Q shown in Table 5 below demonstrates the effect of the intervention on

what students wrote.

Table 5 Results of Cochran's Q test (Q and p values shown) comparing differences between the threestory types at posttest for experimental and control groups separately.

Experimental Control

Q P Q P*Between 3 storytypes

21.733 0.000 14.111 0.000

**Between 1&2 14.000 0.000 13.000 0.000**Between 1&3 3.000 0.250 7.000 0.016**Between 2&3 9.308 0.003 2.250 0.210

*Df=2 Degrees of freedom**Df=1

There are significant differences between story types for both experimental and

control. Pairwise multiple comparisons revealed that, for both the experimental and

control groups, significantly more non-stories were produced on story type 1 than for

story type 2 (i.e. the extremes). Differences for story type 3 were also significant, but

the pattern was different for the two groups. For the experimental group there were

fewer non-stories with story type 3 than with story type 2, there being no difference

between story type 1 and story type 3. However, the converse was true for the control

group, there were fewer non-stories with story type 3 than with story type 1, there

being no difference between story type 2 and story type 3.

In terms of non-stories, the intervention improved the performance of children in the

experimental group on a story that was administered at pre-test, taught explicitly in

the intervention, and tested again at post-test, but not on the other two story types.

The effect of the intervention on the global structure of stories

Only on those scripts that were judged to be stories were ratings made on the 19

characteristics of structure. For the purpose of this second analysis, each characteristic

was coded as being either present or absent. For each story script, these ratings were

summed to produce a global structure score (range 1-19). An analysis of variance was

then conducted to explore the difference in these global structure scores between pre-

test and post-test (time), between the experimental and control groups (group),

Page 25: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

25

between the three story types (story type). Only those children who never produced a

non-story were included in this analysis (nexperimental=57, ncontrol=71).

There was a significant effect for time (F1,116=23.95,p=0.000) however, the

significant time by group interaction (F1,116=23.022,p=0.000) indicated that the

change over time was different for the two groups (figure 1: NB there were no other

significant effects).

15.4

15.6

15.8

16

16.2

16.4

16.6

16.8

17

17.2

pre post

experimentalcontrol

Figure 5 Mean global structure for experimental and control groups at pretest and posttest

Figure 5 shows mean structure scores, averaged across story type, for the

experimental and control groups at pre-test and post-test

Pairwise multiple comparisons (i.e. with bonferroni adjustment and overall

alpha=0.05) revealed that there was no significant difference between the groups at

pre-test (F1,116=.006,p=.938). However, the fact that the experimental group

improved significantly over time (F1,116=45.428,p=0.000) whereas the control group

did not (F1,116=.005,p=.945), resulted in a significant difference between the groups

at post-test (F1,116=14.705,p=.000).

In summary, the intervention served to improve the structure of children's stories in a

global sense. Unlike the non-story analysis, there was no significant effect of story-

type. This means that for some children (i.e. most likely the weaker students) story

type influences the likelihood producing a story, but for the better students (i.e. those

Page 26: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

26

who don't produce non-stories) story type does not affect the structure of the stories

they produce.

The effect of the intervention to specific aspects of the structure of stories

Given that the previous analysis indicated that there was a global effect of the

intervention on the structure of stories children produce, it remains to determine just

which aspects of structure are affected.

For both groups on all story types, all indicators other than abstract in orientation,

multiplicity and chaining in both the main event and at conflict resolution, and

perhaps character orientation and action of the primary character at conflict

resolution, are present in the vast majority of student stories at pre-test. Therefore it is

only on these indicators that there is room for improvement over time.

Pre-test post-test comparisons (i.e. McNemar's tests) were conducted separately for

each group on each story type separately, on each of the 19 binary structure variables

(NB because of the large number of tests a conservative error rate of alpha=0.01 is

adopted). The sample size varies across group and story type because of a difference

in the numbers of non-stories (table 6).Table 6: Probability values (p) associated with McNemar's test of pre-test post-test differences for thetwo groups and the three story types separately

Story1 Story2 Story3

Experimental

n=81

Control

n=84

Experimental

N=67

Control

n=66

Experimental

N=80

Control

n=75

q1 - 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.375 1.000

q2 - 0.250 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.625

q3 0.000 0.541 0.625 1.000 0.349 0.571

q4 1.000 1.000 - 1.000 0.500 1.000

q5 1.000 - 1.000 1.000 - 0.500

q6 1.000 0.500 0.125 0.688 1.000 0.625

q7 0.453 0.453 0.003 0.453 0.508 0.727

q8 1.000 0.016 0.008 0.453 1.000 0.727

q9 1.000 - 0.125 1.000 0.375 1.000

q10 1.000 0.500 0.500 0.357 1.000 1.000

q11 1.000 - 0.219 0.424 1.000 0.180

q12 0.500 1.000 1.000 1.000 - 1.000

q13 0.000 1.000 0.000 0.850 0.030 0.584

q14 0.027 1.000 0.002 0.855 0.216 0.458

q15 0.039 0.238 0.143 0.815 0.035 0.248

q16 1.000 0.791 1.000 0.227 0.031 1.000

q17 1.000 1.000 0.500 0.727 1.000 1.000

q18 1.000 1.000 0.500 0.289 1.000 0.688

q19 0.063 0.031 0.250 0.453 0.549 0.118

Page 27: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

27

- indicates instances where the measure was present in all children (i.e. in 100% of

cases).

The only significant effects (i.e. where p<0.01) are for the experimental group on

story types 1 and 2. However, trends (i.e. where 0.01<p<0.05) are apparent for the

experimental group on story type 3 and for the control group on story type 1.

On story type 1, the experimental group exhibited significant improvement on the

abstract in the orientation, and on multiplicity during conflict resolution. There was

also a trend to improvement during conflict resolution for both chaining and primary

character orientation.

On story type 2, the experimental group improved on both multiplicity and chaining

during both the main event and the conflict resolution.

On story type 3, there was also a trend to improvement for children in the

experimental group for multiplicity, and orientation and action of primary character at

conflict resolution.

In contrast with the results for the experimental group, the only features worth noting

for the control group were the trend toward improvement for chaining during the main

event, and for voice in expression on story type 1.

The children in the experimental group were better able to reproduce the hen and

hawk story that they were trained on. They also improved on story type 2. The

strongest effects of the intervention are on multiplicity and chaining.

Page 28: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

28

Coherence

Dimensions of measuring coherence

The purpose of this section is to examine the question of whether the intervention

improved the coherence of the scripts students produced. In chapter three, coherence

was shown to be one of the main criteria for judging quality of narratives. In the

literature reviewed, it was suggested that coherence research (Bamberg, 1984;

Fitzgerald & Spiegel, 1986; Fitzgerald & Teasley, 1986) has identified seven

dimensions that could be used to judge coherence in a text. The elements include:

topic (identification of topic or theme), digression (how well a writer stayed on topic

or theme), orientation (provided a context or setting), organisation (story details were

planned and well organised), cohesive ties (the sentences were linked to paragraphs),

conclusion (the story was brought to a closure) and grammar (the quality of grammar

used in the narrative was accurate). Scripts produced in response to story types 2 and

3 were analysed according to these seven dimensions of coherence.

Two independent trained raters, one being the experimenter, assessed all scripts on

each of the coherence dimensions (see appendix 2). The level of agreement between

these two raters (i.e the interrater reliability) was assessed using intraclass correlation

coefficients as shown in table 8. The average intraclass correlation coefficient was

0.516 (range 0.03662-0.6121) which indicates a moderate level of agreement between

the two raters.

Separate analyses of variance were conducted on the seven coherence variables (topic,

digression, orientation, organisation, cohesive ties, conclusion and grammar)

exploring the effects of group, story type and time, and their interactions.

Coherence measures across the story types

The main effect for story type was significant for cohesive ties, conclusion and

grammar (Table 9). For both cohesive ties and grammar better results were found with

story type 2, (yet this was the one with many non-stories) whereas for conclusion

better results were found with story type 3 (Table 10).

Page 29: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

29

Table 7.: Average rating (sd in brackets) for 7 dimensions of coherence

Story type 2 story type 3

Topic 3.753 3.784

(0.074) (0.067)

Digression 3.68 3.784

(0.075) (0.069)

Orientation 3.729 3.819

(0.08) (0.074

Organisation 3.372 3.43

(0.077 (0.072)

Cohesive ties 3.384 3.606

(0.068) (0.069

Conclusion 3.972 3.682

(0.085) (0.076)

Grammar 3.539 3.844

(0.081) (0.068)

The main effects for time and group were significant for all coherence variables,

however so was the time by group interactions (table 7 above). This latter finding

indicated that the effect of time was different for the two groups.

Pair-wise multiple comparisons, exploring the significant time by group interactions,

revealed a similar pattern for topic, digression, orientation, organisation and

conclusion (see figure 7). On each of these measures (table 8), there was no

significant difference between the two groups at pre-test; the experimental group

showed significant improvement over time whereas the control group did not; and the

experimental group significantly outperformed the control group at post-test.

A slightly different pattern was observed for cohesive ties and grammar (see figure 6).

On both of these measures (table 9), the experimental group significantly

outperformed the control group at pre-test; the experimental group showed significant

improvement over time whereas the control group did not; and the experimental group

significantly outperformed the control group at post-test.

Page 30: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

30

DISCUSSION

Structure:

A significant finding is that the intervention appeared to assist the students in writing

structurally better narratives: that is they wrote stories that had a conflict, and the

conflict was resolved and hence there was a reduced incidence of non-stories and

recounts. These findings add to related research that has indicated that when students

are offered instruction about the way narratives are structured and as they are guided

through the composing process, they are able to write in 'the true narrative form'

(Riley & Reedy, 2000). This research therefore builds on other significant and related

research that has stressed the central role played by instruction focusing upon the

structure of narratives. Instruction in narrative structure has been found to assist both

in organising and connecting those ideas to one another (Gordon & Braun, 1982;

Kroll & Anson, 1984; Fitzgerald & Spiegel, 1986, Fitzgerald & Teasley, 1986;

Yoshinaga-Itano & Downey, 1992; O'Brien, 1996).

A significant number of students in the experimental group were able to write

narratives with abstracts. Where the idea of the abstract was provided, as was the case

with story type two, the students completed the given prompt and developed the story

coherently after instruction about narrative structure (Labov, 1972, 1982; Kernan,

1977; Wu, 1995; Cortazzi & Jin, 2000).

A significant number of narratives written after the intervention had more developed

characters. Character traits identified in the orientation were clearly elaborated upon

within the main event and in the conflict resolution. In a significant number of

narratives, the characters retained their stated character traits, while in instances where

the narrative called for a character change, the writers stated this explicitly.

Students wrote narratives that had multiple events and episodes both at the main event

and at the conflict resolution. There was increased evidence of chaining of events. The

students related the orientation to the main event and to the conflict resolution. This

contributed to narrative coherence that was an interesting development since it had

not been seen adequately in the pretest narratives or in the control group at posttest.

Page 31: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

31

This is in accord with related findings available in the research and explained in the

literature review (Bamberg, 1997; Yang, 1989; Witte & Faigley, 1981; Neuner, 1987;

O'Brien, 1992; Kroll & Anson, 1984; Allen et al., 1994).

Narratives written by the experimental group after the intervention were judged to be

better in quality and in structure. The narratives were judged to be more elaborate

particularly at the level of orientation.

There were story elements that the students were able to produce at pretest, and so

even after the intervention, such elements could only be marginally improved or there

could be no change at all. These elements include: action, antagonist, protagonist,

along with aspects of expression including vocabulary, grammar and voice. There was

no room for improvement.

There were some instances of modelling where the students used ideas drawn from

the traditional narratives, gave them new names, new settings and then modified the

tales to create their own narratives. Students used ideas from a traditional story and

gave the story a new twist. In doing so, they responded to the requirements of the

writing task. Such a strategy demonstrated that students have shifted from knowledge

telling to knowledge transforming.

A significant number of traditional narratives begin with the stock phrase "Once upon

a time" Such a stock phrase appeared to be problematic when the students were

provided with prompts in story type two. As the prompts stated "Write a story

beginning with…": some students were unable to develop such prompts probably

because of over-reliance on the traditional way of telling a narrative. This could be

explained as a problem of transferring from the oral to the written interface.

Coherence:

The students in the experimental group outperformed those in the control group in the

coherence variables of topic identification, staying on topic without digressing,

providing orienting background information, organisation of content, using cohesive

ties to link sentences to paragraphs, providing a conclusion, and using correct

Page 32: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

32

grammar. Further, in terms of fluency, students in the experimental group wrote

narratives that were longer demonstrating confidence and maturity in their writing.

Results from this data suggest that the intervention assisted the students in various

ways. Students wrote more coherently and hence better quality stories in English.

Their stories were clear and logical and had elaborate orienting background

information. They wrote without digressing and stayed on topic.The data also

suggests that the students developed writing skills that enabled them to write stories

that had logical conclusions.

The intervention appears to have led to improvement in the organization of their

written narratives as the intervention equipped the students with the cognitive skills of

planning. The intervention also seems to have led to the development of logical

thinking, clarity of ideas and application of the knowledge of the world. The

implication would appear to be that if one can plan a narrative, then one has the

potential to plan other types of written text including expository, explanatory,

description and argumentation.

The intervention appeared to assist students create characters that resolved problems.

By extension, the students became better at problem solving. With consistent training,

the method of sensitising students about narrative structure seems to have the

potential to turn novice writers into expert writers (Scardamalia and Bereiter (1987).

Cohesion:

Narrative literature seems to emphasise the place of causal and temporal ties in the

creation of better narratives (Stein & Trabasso, 1984; Ozyurek & Trabasso, 1997).

Data from this study does not show this. The cohesion analysis shows a drop in the

use of pronouns while increasing the use of the definite article "the". No other

cohesive ties appear significant. Such findings are in line with what other studies have

found (for instance Neuner, 1987; Tierney & Mosenthal, 1983).

Witte and Faigley (1981) indicate that texts that have been rated highly are denser in

the cohesive ties of synonymy, hyponymy and collocation. This study does not show

Page 33: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

33

this. It is possible that the number of essays analysed might have been a factor since

Witte and Faigley analysed only ten essays while Tierney and Mosenthal (1983)

analysed 24 essays. A total of 177 essays were analysed for cohesion in the current

study.

The point made through cohesion data analysed is that cohesive ties are significant

when they are used to add semantic and syntactic quality to a story, but seen in

isolation, they might not add to the quality of a narrative. The researcher would not

recommend counting of ties as a way of determining quality in a text since the notion

would appear wasteful. Counting of items in a text is one way of assessing writing,

but as Isaacson, (1988) has observed "the procedure is time consuming, sometimes

requiring several passes through the composition".

It can be argued that activating students' implicit knowledge of narrative structure

making it explicit may assist them to generate, plan and organise the content of the

narratives they write in English. To achieve this, it is suggested that the teachers of

English use local materials to develop programs that allow students to re-examine the

structure of oral narratives and then offer instructions about the composing process

alongside instructions about narrative structure. Children appear to love storytelling

since it is a familiar cultural activity. They can identify with storytelling. It is

suggested in this study that when children know that what they are writing is correct,

they develop self-confidence and such an action enhances the children's self esteem.

This study has the potential to make these children improve their narrative writing

skills and in doing so develop a passion for writing.

This study stresses the central role of structure in assisting students write better

narratives. It also demonstrates that a good story relies on structure, causal relations,

temporal relations, clarity of goals of the main character as well as a world-knowledge

of plausible events. Quality in a written text relies on coherence brought about by

logic, clarity and cohesion. This study agrees with Riley and Reedy (2000) when they

suggest that as a starting point "children must explicitly be taught the features of plot

distinctive to narrative" (p. 68). Only then will they be able to write in the true

narrative form.

Page 34: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

34

The subjects in this study were required to generate content, organise and plan such

content. The traditional narratives were not analysed for cohesion and coherence. The

current research attempted to overcome such criticism by introducing another story

type requiring a writer to produce dynamic narratives by developing a given prompt

along suggested lines. It was expected that learning the structure of the traditional

narratives would generalize to the constructive ones. The data analysis seems to

justify such expectations.

Vygotsky’s Zone of Proximal Development (1986) suggests that students learn faster

when the teacher assists them in the identification of what they already know, before

they are assisted in the creation of new texts. In this study, the researcher used

traditional narratives to activate students’ knowledge of structure in narratives.

Vygotsky refers to this type of learning as “assisted learning”. This study used

narratives as a source of content (words), agreeing with Vygotskian psychology that

posits that: “words fulfil a crucial role in thought and problem-solving activity”

(Vygotsky, 1986, p. 13). This study suggests that by activating students’ passive

knowledge of narrative structure, the teacher assists students to enhance their creative

faculties and hence write coherent narratives that have a clear structure. The teacher

awakens a variety of internal developmental processes that are able to operate only

when the child is interacting with people in his or her environment while examining

traditional narratives. Vygotsky’s (1978) argument is that “once such processes are

internalised they become part of the child’s independent developmental achievement”

(p. 90). In this study, students created narratives that were independently judged to be

of quality that was better than the narratives they wrote prior to the intervention.

This study might have produced in-depth results if the researcher had more time and

resources. In a follow-up study, it would be of interest to see whether the results

would be different were the researcher to train serving high school teachers to do the

intervention and then train another set of teachers to do the rating. Such a method

might eliminate the fact that the researcher was the person carrying out the

intervention. The fact that he was an outsider may have had an impact on the students'

Page 35: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

35

Reference List

Abrahamson, C. E. (1998). Storytelling as a pedagogical tool in highereducation. Education, 118(3), 440-453.

Alexander, P.A., Schallert, D.L. & Hare, U.C (1991). Coming to terms: Howresearchers in learning and literacy talk about knowledge. Review ofEducational Research , 61, 315-343.

Allen, M.S, Haden, Kertoy, M.K. Sherblom J.C. & Pettit, J.M. (1994) Children'snarrative productions: A comparison of personal event and fictionalstories. Applied Psycholinguistics, (15), 149-176.

Applebee, A. N. (1978). The Child's Concept of Story. Chicago and London:University of Chicago Press.

Bacia, J. (1994). Creating Popular Fiction: How to Write Novels that Sell.Sydney: Allen and Unwin.

Bain, A. (1867) English composition and rhetoric: a manual. New York: DAppleton and company.

Bamberg, B. (1983). "What Makes a Text Coherent?" College Compositionand Communication, 34(4), 417-429.

Bamberg, B. (1984). Assessing Coherence: A Reanalysis of Essays Writtenfor the National Assessment of Educational Progress, 1969-1979.Research in the Teaching of English, 18(3), 305-319.

Bamberg, M. G. W. (1987). The Acquisition of Narratives: Learning to useLanguage. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Bamberg, M. (Ed.). (1997). Narrative Development: Six Approaches. Mahwah,and London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in Experimental and SocialPsychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bauman, R. (1986). Story, Performance and Event: Contextual studies of oralnarrative. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Beaugrande, R. & Dressler, W. (1981) Introduction to text linguistics. London:New York. Longman.

Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (1981). Developing Questions That PromoteComprehension: The Story Map. Language Arts, 58(8), 913-918.

Ben-Amos, D. (1973). Besprechungen. Journal of American Folklore, 86, 232-234.

Page 36: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

36

Benson, M. S. (1996). Structure, Conflict, and Psychological Causation in theFictional Narratives of 4- and 5-Year-Olds. Merrill-Parmer Quarterly,42(2), 228-247.

Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1982). From Conversation to Composition:The Role of Instruction in a Developmental Process. In R. Glaser (Ed.),Advances in Instructional Psychology. Hillsdale: Lawrence ErlbaumPublishers. Vol. 2 pp. 1-64.

Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: Acrosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumAssociates.

Biakolo, E. (1999). On the Theoretical Foundations of Orality and Literacy.Research in African Literatures, 30(2).

Birney, R. C., Burdick, H., & Teevan, R. C. (1969). Fear of Failure. New York:Reinhold Company.

Bishop, W. (1992). Working words: The process of Creative Writing.California: Mayfield Pub. Co.

Brewer, W. F. (1985). The Story Schema: Universal and Culture SpecificPurposes. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy,Language and Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Britton, B. K., & Pelligrini, A. D. (Eds.). (1990). Narrative Thought andNarrative Language. Hillsdale, N.J: Earlbaum.

Brostoff, A. (1981). Coherence: "Next to" Is Not "Connected to". CollegeComposition and Communication, 32, 278-294.

Brown, G., & Yule, G. (1983). Discourse analysis. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press.

Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J (2002) Making Stories. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

Cairney, T. (1990). Intertextuality: Infectious echoes from the past. TheReading Teacher, 43, 478-484.

Cairney, T.H. (1995) Pathways to literacy. London: Casell.

Calkins, L. M. (1986). The Art of Teaching Writing. Portsmouth: Heinemann.

Campbell, D.T. & Stanley, J.C. (1966) Experimental and Quasi-experimentaldesigns for research. Chicago: Rand Nally

Capps, L. & Ochs, E. (1995) Out of Place: Narrative Insights Into AgoraphobiaDiscourse Processes, 19, 407-439.

Page 37: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

37

Carter, K. (1993). The place of Story in the Study of Teaching and TeacherEducation. Educational Researcher, 22(1), 5-12.

Cassady, M. (1994). The Art of Storytelling: Creative ideas for preparation andperformance. Colorado Springs: Meriwether.

Chafe, W. L. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definitness, subjects, topics,and point of view. In C. N. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic New York:Academic Press. (pp. 25-55).

Champion, T. B. (1998). "Tell Me Somethin' Good": A Description of NarrativeStructures among African American Children. Linguistics andEducation, 9(3), 251-286.

Charolles, M. (1983). Coherence as a principle in the interpretation ofdiscourse. Text, 3(1), 71-97.

Cooper, C. R., & Odell, L. (Eds.). (1977). Evaluating writing: Describing,measuring, judging. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers ofEnglish.

Cortazzi, M., & Jin, L. (2000). Evaluating evaluation in narrative. In S. Hunston& G. Thompson (Eds.), Evaluation in Text Oxford: Oxford UniversityPress. (pp. 102-120).

Crowhurst, M. (1987). Cohesion in Argument and Narration at Three GradeLevels. Research in the Teaching of English, 21(2), 185-201.

Crowley, S. (1977). Components of the composing process. CollegeComposition and Communication, 28(2), 166-169.

Davis, Z. T., & McPherson, M. D. (1989). Story Map instruction: A road mapfor reading comprehension. The Reading Teacher, 232-240.

DeBeaugrande, R. (1984). Forward to the Basics: Getting Down to Grammar.College Composition and Communication, XXXV(3), 358- 367.

DeBeaugrande, R. (1984). Text production: Toward a science of composition.Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

DeBeaugrande, R. (1984). Writer, Reader, Critic: Comparing Critical Theoriesas Discourse. College English, 46(6), 533-559.

Doyle, A. E. (1982). The limitations of Cohesion. Research in the Teaching ofEnglish, 16, 390-393.

Emery, D. W., & Milhalevich, C. (1992). Directed Discussion of CharacterPerspectives. Reading Research and Instruction, 31(4), 51-59.

Emery, D. W. (1996). Helping readers comprehend stories from thecharacters' perspectives. The Reading Teacher, 49(7), 543-541

Page 38: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

38

Enkvist. (1990). Coherence in writing. In U. Connor & A. M. Johns (Eds.),Coherence in Writing: Teachers of English to speakers of otherlanguages.

Ervin-Tripp, S.M. and Kuntay, A (1997) Occasioning and Structure ofConversational Stories. In T. Givon (Ed.) Conversation: CognitiveCommunication and Social Perspectives. Armsterdam. J. BejaminsPublishing Company

Fairbanks, C. M. (1996). Telling Stories: Reading and Writing ResearchNarratives. Journal of Curriculum Supervision, 2(4), 320-340.

Fang, Z. (1998). A Study of Changes and Development in Children's WrittenDiscourse Potential. Linguistics and Education, 9(4), 341-367.

Feez, S. & Joyce, H. (1998) Writing skills: Narrative and Non-Fiction TextTypes Phoenix Education Pty. Ltd.

Finnegan, R. Oral literature and writing in the South Pacific. Pacific QuarterlyMoana, 7(2), 22-35.

Finnegan, R. (1967). Limba Stories and Storytelling. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Finnegan, R. (1970). Oral Literature in Africa. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Finnegan, R. (1974). How oral is oral literature? Bulletin of the School ofOriental and African Studies, 35, 52-64.

Finnegan, R. (1977). Oral Poetry: Its Nature, Significance and Social Context.New York and London: Cambridge University Press.

Finnegan, R. (1988). Literacy and Orality. Oxford: Blackwell.

Finnegan, R. (1990). What is Oral Literature Anyway? Comments in the Lightof Some African and Other Comparative Material. In J. M. Foley (Ed.),Oral Formulaic Theory: A Folklore Casebook. New York: GarlandPublishing Inc.

Finnegan, R. (1991). Oral traditions and the verbal arts: a guide to researchpractices. London: Routledge.

Finnegan, R. (1998). Tales of the city: a study of narrative and urban life.Cambridge (England): Cambridge University Press.

Fitzgerald, J., & Spiegel, D. L. (1986). Textual Cohesion and Coherence inChildren's Writing. Research in the Teaching of English, 20(3), 263-280.

Fitzgerald, J., & Teasley, A. B. (1986). Effects of Instruction in NarrativeStructure on Children's Writing. Journal of Educational Psychology,78(6), 424-432.

Page 39: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

39

Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Adam, S. (1995). Structure and coherence ofpreschooler's personal narratives over time: Implications for childhoodamnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 32-56.

Fleckenstein, K. S. (1992). An Appetite for Coherence: Arousing and FulfillingDesires. College Composition and Communication, 43, 81-87.

Fletcher, R. (1993). What a Writer Needs. Portsmouth: Heinemann.

Flower, L. S., & Hayes, J. R. (1980a). The cognition of discovery: Defining arhetorical problem. College Composition and Communication,3 (2), 21-32.

Flower, L. S., & Hayes, J. R. (1980b). The dynamics of composing: Makingplans and juggling constraints. In L. E. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.),Cognitive processes in writing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Flower, L. S., & Hayes, J. R. (1981). The pregnant pause: An Inquiry into thenature of planning. Research in the Teaching of English, 15, 229-244.

Foley, J. M. (1995). The Singer of Tales in Performance. Bloomington: IndianaUniversity Press.

Fowler, R. (1971). The Languages of Literature: Some linguistic contributionsto criticisms/with an essay written in collaboration with Peter Mercerand two essays by F.W. Bateson. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Freckenstein, K. S. (1992). An Appetite for Coherence: Arousing and FulfillingDesires. College Composition and Communication, 43(1), 81-87.

Gallimore, & Tharp. (1990). A Theory of Literacy. In L. C. Moll (Ed.), Vygotskyand Education: instructional implications and applications ofsocialhistorical psychology (pp. 192-). Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press.

Gardill, C. M., & Jitendra, A. K. (1999). Advanced Story Map Instruction:Effects on the Reading Comprehension of Students with LearningDisabilities. Journal of Special Education, 33(i1), 21.

Gee, J. P. G. (1989). Two styles of narrative construction and their Linguisticand Educational Implications. Journal of Education, 171(1), 97-115.

Gee, J. P. G. (1989). The narrativization of experience in the oral style.Journal of Education, 171(1), 75-96.

Gernsbacher, M. A. (Ed.). (1994). Handbook of Psycholinguistics. San Diego:Academic Press.

Gernsbacher, M. A., & Givon, T. (Eds.). (1995). Coherence in SpontaneousText Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

Geva, E. (1983). Facilitating reading comprehension through flowcharting.

Page 40: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

40

Reading Research Quarterly, XVIII(4), 384-405.

Goldenhersh, B. (1994). Story ladders: A Reading Writing Connection. OhioReading Teacher, XXIX(2), 10-13.

Gordon, C. J., & Braun, C. (1982). Story Schemata: Metatextual Aid toReading and Writing. Paper presented at the New Inquiries in Reading:Research and Instruction., Rochester, New York.

Gordon , C.J. & Braun, C. (1983) Using story schema as an aid to reading andwriting. The Reading Teacher pp. 116-121.

Gordon, C. J. (1989). Teaching Narrative Text Structure: A Process Approachto Reading and Writing. In D. K. Muth (Ed.), Children's Comprehensionof Text: Research into Practice (pp. 79-102). Newark: InternationalReading Association- IRA.

Grabe, W., & Kaplan, R. B. (1996). Theory and Practice of Writing: An AppliedLinguistic Perspective. London: Longman.

Graham, S., & Harris, J. (1989). Improving learning disabled student's skills atcomposing essays: self-instructional strategy training. ExceptionalChildren, 56(3).

Graves, D. (1986). Writing: Teachers and Children at Work. Portsmouth, NH:Heinemann.

Gumperz, J. J., Kaltman, H., & O'Connor, M. C. (1984). Cohesion in Spokenand Written Discourse: Ethnic Style and the Transition to Literacy. In D.Tannen (Ed.), Coherence in Spoken and Written Discourse (Vol. XII,pp. 3-31). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.

Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing NarrativeStructure in Parent-Child Reminiscing Across the Preschool Years.Developmental Psychology, 33(2), 295-307.

Halliday, M.A.K., (1994) An Introduction to Functional Grammar London. NewYork. Sydney. Auckland: Arnold.

Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. Singapore:Longman Singapore Publishers Ltd.

Hardy, B. (1968) "Towards a Poetics of fiction: An Approach throughnarrative" Novel, 2, 5-14.

Hardy, B. (1977) Towards a poetics of fiction: An approach through narrative.In M. Meek, A. Warlow, G. Barton, (eds) The Cool Web. The Pattern ofchildren's Reading. London: Bodley Head.

Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (1996). Making the Writing Process WorK:Strategies for composition and self-regulation. Cambridge, MA:Brookline Books.

Page 41: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

41

Harris, R. (2000) Rethinking writing, The Athlone Press: London.

Harris, R. (1986) The Origin of Writing, Duckworth: London.

Harris, J., & Reitchl, K. (Eds.). (1997). Prosimetrum: Cross-culturalPerspectives on narrative in prose and verse. Suffolk: Brewer.

Haswell, R. H. (1989). Textual Research and Coherence: Findings, Intuition,Application. College English, 51(3), 305-319.

Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. S. (1980). Identifying the Organization of WritingProcesses. In L. E. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), CognitiveProcesses in Writing (pp. 3-30). New Jersey: Lawrence ErlbaumAssociates, Publishers.

Hillocks, G. (1986). Research on Written Composition. Urbana, IL: NationalCouncil of Research in English.

Hillocks, G. (1987) "Synthesis of Research on Teaching Writing". InEducational leadership vol. 44 No. 8 pp 71-82.

Hoggan, K. C., & Strong, C. J. (1994). The Magic of "Once Upon a Time":Narrative Teaching Strategies. Language, Speech, and HearingServices in Schools, 25, 76-89.

Hoover, M. (1997). Effects of Textual and Cohesive Structure on DiscourseProcessing. Discourse Processes, 23, 193-220.

Houston, G., Goolrick, F., & Tate, R. (1991). Storytelling as a Stage inProcess Writing: A Whole Language Model. Teaching ExceptionalChildren, 40-43.

Hudson, J. A., Fivush, R., & Kuebli, J. (1992). Scripts and Episodes: theDevelopment of Event Memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6, 483-505.

Hudson, J. A., & Shapiro, L. R. (1991). Effects of task and topic on children'snarratives. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), New Directions indeveloping narrative structure (pp. 89-136). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.

Hung, D. W. L. (2002). Learning through Video-Based Narratives within theCultural Zone of Proximal Development. International Journal ofInstructional Media, 29(1), 125-140.

Hunt, K. W. (1977). Early Blooming and Late Blooming Syntactic Structures.In C. R. Cooper & L. Odell (Eds.), Evaluating Writing (pp. 91-104).Urbana: National Council of Teachers of English.

Hunt, K. W. (1983). Sentence combining and the teaching of writing. In M.Martlew (Ed.), The psychology of written language: A developmentalapproach (pp. 99-125). New York: J. Wiley.

Page 42: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

42

Hymes, D. (1998). When is oral narrative poetry? Generative form and itspragmatic conditions. Pragmatics: International PragmaticsAssociation, 8(4), 475-500.

Idol, L. (1987). Group Story-Mapping: A Comprehension Strategy for BothSkilled and Unskilled Readers. Journal of Reading Disabilities, 20(4),196-205.

Isaacson, S. (1988). Assessing the writing product: qualitative and quantativemeasures. Exceptional Children, 54(6), 528 (527).

Johnson, N. S., & Mandler, J. M. (1980). A Tale of Two Structures: Underlyingand Surface forms in Stories. Poetics, 9, 51-86.

Kabira, W. M. (1983). The Oral Artist. Nairobi: East African EducationalPublishers.

Kabira, W. M., & Adagala, K. (Eds.). (1985). Kenyan Oral Narratives. Nairobi:East African Educational Publishers.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (Ed.). (1980). Some fundamental aspects of languagedevelopment after age 5*. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (Ed.). (1981). The grammatical marking of thematicstructure in the development of language production. New York:Academic Press.

Kellogg, R. (1973). Oral literature, New Literary History (pp. 55-66).Charlottesville Virginia: University of Virginia.

Kellogg, R. T. (1994). The Psychology of Writing. New York: Oxford UniversityPress.

Kernan, A. B. (1973). The idea of Literature, New Literary History.

Kernan, K. T. (1977). Semantic and expressive elaboration in children'snarratives. In S. Ervin-Tripp & C. Mitchell (Eds.), Child discourse (pp.91-102). New York: Academic Press.

Kinginger, C. (2002). Defining the Zone of Proximal Development in USForeign Language Education. Applied Linguistics, 23(2), 240-261.

Kintch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a Model of Text Comprehensionand Production. Psychological Review, 85(4), 363-394.

Knappert, J. (1984). Reflections on the study of oral literature. PacificQuarterly Moana, 8(4), 16-20.

Krause, K.L.D (1997) Planning, Writing apprehension and essay writingcompetence. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Macquarie University,Sydney

Page 43: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

43

Kroll, B. M., & Anson, C. M. (1984). Analysing Structure in Children's FictionalNarratives. In H. Cowie (Ed.), Development of Children's ImaginativeWriting (pp. 153-182). London and Canberra: Coom Helm.

Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral Versions ofPersonal Experience. Seattle,: University of Washington Press.

Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city: Studies in the Black Englishvernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

Labov, U. (1982). Speech actions and reactions in personal narrative. In D.Tannen (Ed.), Analyzing discourse: Text and talk (pp. 219-247).Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

Larson, J. (1971). Toward a linear rhetoric of essay. College Composition andCommunication, 22, 140-145.

Larson, J., & Maier, M. (2000). Co-Authoring Classroom Texts: ShiftingParticipant Roles in Writing Activity. Research in the Teaching ofEnglish, 34, 469-497.

Le Guin, U. K. (1981). It Was a Dark and Stormy Night; or, Why Are WeHuddling about the Campfire? In W. J. T. Mitchell (Ed.), On Narrative(pp. 187-195). Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.

Lord, A. B. (1960). The Singer of Tales. New York: Atheneun.

Macdonald, M. R. (Ed.). (1999). Traditional storytelling today: an internationalsourcebook. London: Fitzroy Dearborn.

Maimon, E. (1979). Talking to strangers. College Composition andCommunication, 30, 364-369.

Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things passed:Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 111-151.

Mandler, J. M. (1978). A Code in the Node: The Use of a Story Schema inRetrieval. Discourse Processes, 1, 14-35.

Markels, R. B. (1983). Cohesion Paradigms in Paragraphs. College English,45(5), 450-464.

Matsuhashi, A. (1982). Explorations in the real-time production of writtendiscourse. In M. Nystrand (Ed.), What writers know: The language,process and structure of written discourse (pp. 269-290). New York:Academic Press.

McCabe, A. (1992). All Kinds of Good Stories. Paper presented at theSpeeches/ conference papers-ERIC Document ED355474, Annualmeeting of the National Reading Conference (42nd, San Antonio, TXDecember 2-5, 1992.

Page 44: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

44

McCrimmon, J. M. (1963). Writing with a purpose. Boston: Houghton MifflinCompany.

McCulley, G. A. (1985). Writing Quality, Coherence and Cohesion. Researchin the Teaching of English, 19(3), 269-282.

McKamey, E. S. (1991). Storytelling for Children with Learning Disabilities: AFirst-hand Account. Teaching Exceptional Children, 46-48.

Miles, J. F. (Ed.). (1981). Oral traditional literature: A Festschrift for AlbertBates Lord. Columbus, Ohio: Slavica Publishers.

Ministry of Education English Syllabus - Kenya (1984) Jomo KenyattaFoundation. Nairobi.

Murray, D. M. (1982). Learning by teaching: Selected Articles on Writing andTeaching. Montclair, NJ: Boynton/Cook Publishers Inc,.

Muth, K. D. (Ed.). (1989). Children's comprehension of Text: Research intoPractice. Newark, Delaware: International Reading Association Inc.

Mwangi, R. (1982). Kikuyu Folktales. Nairobi: Kenya Literature Bureau.

Nandwa, J., & Bukenya, A. (1983). African Oral Literature for Schools.Nairobi: Longman.

Nelson, K., & Grundel, J. (1986). Children's scripts. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Eventknowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 231-247).Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Neuner, J. L. (1987). Cohesive Ties and Chains in Good and Poor FreshmanEssays. Research in the Teaching of English, 21(1), 92-105.

Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood CliffsNJ: Prentice Hall.

Njururi, N. (1966) Agikuyu folk tales London : Oxford University Press.

Nipold, M. A. (1988). Later Language Development; Ages Nine throughNineteen. Boston, MA: College Hill Press.

Nystrand, M., Gamoran, A., Kachur, R., & Prendergrast, C. (1997). Openingdialogue: Understanding the dynamics of language and learning in theEnglish classroom. New York: Teachers College Press.

O'Brien, D. (1992). Writing in the Primary School. Melbourne: LongmanCheshire Pty Ltd.

O'Brien, D. (1996). Writing for Zambian Schools. Lusaka: Zambia EducationalPublishing House.

Obiechina, E. (1975). Culture, Tradition, and Society in the West African

Page 45: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

45

Novel. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Okpewho, I. (1992). African Oral Literature: Backgrounds, Character, andContinuity. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press.

Ollman, H. E. (1989). Cause and effect in the real world. Journal of Reading,33, 224-225.

Olson, D.R. (1996) Language and literacy: What writing does to language andmind. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 16, 3-13.

Olson, D. R., & Torrance, N. (Eds.). (1991). Literacy and Orality. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.

Ong, W. (1982). Orality and Literacy: Technologizing of the Word. London:Methuen.

Ong, W. J. (1995). Orality and Literacy. London: Routledge.

Perl, S. (1979). The composing process of unskilled college writers. Researchin the Teaching of English, 13, 317-336.

Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Threeways of looking at at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum Press.

Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1992). Parental styles of narrative elicitation:Effect on children's narrative structure and content. First Language, 12,299-321.

Propp, V. (1968). The Morphology of the Folktale(Original published in 1928 inRussian) (L. Scott, Trans. 2nd ed.). Austin: University of Texas Press.

Purves, A. C. (1984). In search of an Internationally- Valid Scheme for ScoringCompositions. College Composition and Communication, XXXV(4),426-438.

Richmond, J. (1990). What Do We Mean by Knowledge About Language? InR. Carter (Ed.), Knowledge About Language and the Curriculum LINCReader. London: Hodder and Stroughton.

Riley, J., & Reedy, D. (2000). Developing Writing for Different Purposes:Teaching about Genre in the Early Years. London: Paul ChapmanPublishers.

Rimmon-Kenan, S. (1983). Narrative fiction: Contemporary Poetics London,New York. Methuen.

Rosenberg, S. (1987) (Ed.) Advances in applied psycholinguistics: Reading,writing and language learning Vol. 2 Cambridge: New York. CambridgeUniversity Press.

Page 46: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

46

Rubright, L. (1996) Beyond the beanstalk: Interdisciplinary learning throughstorytelling. Portsmouth: NH.Heinemann.

Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Notes on the schema for stories: In D. Bobrow & A.Collins (Eds.), Representation and understanding: Studies in cognitivescience. New York: Academic Press.

Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Understanding and Summarizing Brief Stories. In D.Laberge & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Basic Processes in Reading:Perception and comprehension (pp. 265-303). Hillsdale, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Scardamalia, M., Bereiter, C., & Goelman, H. (1982). The role of productionfactors in writing ability. In M. Nystrand (Ed.), What writers know: Thelanguage, process and structure or written discourse (pp. 173-210).New York: Academic Press.

Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1986). 'Research on Written Composition'. InM. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Teaching (pp. 778-803). New York: Longman.

Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1987). Knowledge telling and knowledgetransforming in written composition. In S. Rosenberg (Ed.), Advancesin Applied Psycholinguistics (Vol. 2, pp. 142-175). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.

Scheub, H. (1975). The Xhosa Ntsomi. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Selzer, J. (1984). Exploring Options in Composing. College Composition andCommunication, 35(3), 276-284.

Shanahan, T., & Shanahan, S. (1997). Character Perspective Charting:Helping children to develop a more complete conception of story. TheReading Teacher, 50(8), 668-677.

Shapiro, L. R., & Hudson, J. A. (1991). Tell me a make-believe story:coherence and cohesion in young children's picture-elicited narratives.Developmental Psychology, 27(6), 960-974.

Shapiro, L. R., & Hudson, J. A. (1997). Coherence and Cohesion in Children'sstories. In J. Costermans & M. Fayol (Eds.), Processing InterclausalRelationships: Studies in the production and comprehension of text (pp.23-48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Shrubshall, P. (1997). Narrative Argument and Literacy: A Comparative Studyof the Narrative Discourse Development of Monolingual and Bilingual5-10-Year-Old Learners. Journal of Multilingual and MulticulturalDevelopment, 18(5), 402-421.

Shuman, A. (1986). Storytelling Rights. Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.

Page 47: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

47

Silva, T. (1992). L1 vs L2 writing: ESL graduate students' perceptions. TESLCanada Journal/Revue TESL du Canada, 10(1), 27-45.

Sipe, R. S. (1993). Using transformations of traditional stories: Making thereading-writing connection. The Reading Teacher, 47(1), 18-26.

Stein, N. L. (Ed.). (1979). How Children Understand Stories: A DevelopmentalAnalysis (Vol. 2). Norwood: Abex.

Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An Analysis of Story Comprehension inElementary School Children. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), New Directions inDiscourse Processing (pp. 53-120). Norwood, New Jersey: Ablex.

Stein, N. L., & Policastro, M. (1984). The concept of a story: A comparisonbetween children's and teacher's viewpoints. In H. Mandl & N. L. Stein& T. Trabasso (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text (pp. 113-155). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Stein, N. L., & Albro, E. R. (Eds.). (1997). Building Complexity andCoherence: Children's Use of Goal-Structured Knowledge in TellingStories. In Michael Bamberg (Ed.) Narrative Development: SixApproaches (pp. 5-44) Hillsdale, NJ: London: Lawrence ErlbaumAssociates, Publishers.

Stromqvist, S., & Day, D. (1993). On the development of narrative structure inchild L1 and adult L2 acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14(2), 135-158.

Tannen, D. (Ed.). (1982). Analyzing discourse: Text and Talk. Washington,DC: Georgetown University Press.

Thiong'o, N. (1972) Homecoming: essays on African and Caribbean Literatureculture and politics. London: Heinemann.

Thiong'o, N. (1971) A Grain of Wheat. London: Heinemann.

Tierney, R. J., & Mosenthal, J. H. (1983). Cohesion and Textual Coherence.Research in the Teaching of English, 17(3), 215-229.

Todorov, T. (1973) The notion of literature. In R. Cohen New LiteraryHistory,1, pp. 5-15,

Toolan, M.J. (1988) Narrative: A critical linguistic introduction. London; NewYork. Routledge.

Trabasso, T. Secco, T. & van den Broek, (1984) Causal cohesion and storycoherence. In H. Mandl, N.L Stein & Trabasso, T. (Eds.) Learning andcomprehension of text (pp. 83-111). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence EarlbaumAssociates.

Vallecorsa, A. L., Ledford, R. R., & Parnell, G. G. (1991). Strategies forTeaching Composition Skills. Teaching Exceptional Children, 52-55.

Page 48: KIGO4789 - AARE · AUSTRALIAN ASSOCIATION FOR RESEARCH IN EDUCATION- AARE INTERNATIONAL EDUCATION RESEARCH CONFERENCE 28TH Nov-2ND Dec 2004 Melbourne Australia Doing the Public Good:

48

van Dijk, T. A. (1977). Text and Context: Explorations in the Semantics andPragmatics of Discourse. London: Longman.

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in Society (M. Cole & S. John-Steiner & S. Scribner& E. Souberman, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Vygotsky, L. (1983). The Prehistory of Written Language. In M. Martlew (Ed.),The psychology of written composition (pp. 279-291). New York:J.Wiley.

Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and language (E. Haufmann & G. Vadar,Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Wellek, R. & Warren, A. (1962) Theory of literature. Harmondworth,Middlesex: Penguin Books.

Witte, S. P., & Faigley, L. (1981). "Coherence, Cohesion and Writing Quality".College Composition and Communication, 32, 189-204.

Witte, S. P., & Cherry, R. D. (1986). Writing Processes and Written Productsin Composition Research. In C. R. Cooper & S. Greenbaum (Eds.),Studying Writing: Linguistic Approaches (Vol. 1, pp. 112-153). BeverlyHills: Sage Publications.

Wu, S. (1995). Evaluating Narrative Essays: A Discourse AnalysisPerspective. Regional English Language Centre - RELC Journal, 26(1),1-26.

Yang, A. W. (1989). Cohesive chains and writing quality. Word, 40(1-2), 235-247.

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., & Downey, D. M. (1992). When a Story Is Not a Story: AProcess Analysis of the Written Language of Hearing-ImpairedChildren. The Volta Review, 95, 131-158.