issues on language processing

Upload: danielevirgillito

Post on 05-Apr-2018

227 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    1/46

    UNIVERSIT DEGLI STUDI DI CATANIA

    SCUOLA SUPERIORE DI CATANIA

    Daniele Virgillito

    ISSUES ON LANGUAGE PROCESSING:

    FROM THEORY TO BRAIN INVESTIGATIONS

    DIPLOMA DI LICENZA

    Relatore: Chiar.mo Prof. Marco Mazzone

    ANNO ACCADEMICO 2006/2007

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    2/46

    Daniele Virgillito 2

    Contents

    Abstract ......................................................................................................................3

    Sommario ..................................................................................................................4

    Acknowledgments...................................................................................................5

    1. Linguistic theories and findings ...................................................................71.1 The layers of language...............................................................................7

    1.2 Chomskys perspective .............................................................................9

    1.3 Models of language processing..............................................................10

    1.4 Time for meaning.....................................................................................12

    1.5 Looking at the brain.................................................................................16

    2. ERPs: what brainwaves can tell about language......................................18

    2.1 Event-related Potentials ..........................................................................18

    2.2 ERP correlates of semantics and syntax................................................19

    2.3 More than meaning and structure.........................................................22

    2.4 Semantics or syntax?................................................................................27

    2.5 Chomskian account..................................................................................29

    3. Mood and language........................................................................................32

    3.1 Positive mood effects on language ........................................................32

    Conclusions.............................................................................................................42

    Bibliography ...........................................................................................................44

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    3/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 3

    Abstract

    The debate on language processing has seen its centre of gravity

    progressively moving from the investigation about inner architecture

    of language to the empirical observation of how language is

    instantiated in the human brain.

    The present work firstly intends to offer a review of the

    proposed theoretical models defining general dynamics of meaning

    and syntactic processing.It will present the main findings on this topic deriving from

    studies on language carried out through the recording of event-related

    potentials (ERPs), a research methodology based on the statistical

    analysis of data coming from electroencephalogram (EEG) in

    cooccurrence with linguistic stimuli.

    These data seem to globally speak in favour of those

    frameworks that, detaching themselves from the theoretical paradigm

    imposed by Noam Chomsky in the last fifty years, conceive syntactic

    component as interagent with other components.

    It will then show the preliminary results of a study on the effect

    of emotive states on language processing, carried out at ERP Lab of

    NICI (Nijmegen Institute for Cognition and Information, Nijmegen,

    Netherlands) in collaboration with Dorothee Chwilla and Constance

    Vissers.

    N400 ERP component (index of the cognitive effort of

    semantic integration) in correspondance with unexpected linguistic

    stimuli, appears to be mitigated, as predicted on the basis of

    Federmeier et al. (2001), by positive mood.

    This seems to encourage too the idea that language processing is

    closely tied to other components of our cognitive system.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    4/46

    Daniele Virgillito 4

    Sommario

    Il dibattito sullelaborazione del linguaggio ha visto il suo

    baricentro progressivamente spostarsi dallindagine sullarchitettura

    interna dei sistemi linguistici allosservazione empirica di come il

    linguaggio instanziato nel cervello umano.

    Il presente lavoro intende offrire in primo luogo una rassegna

    dei modelli teorici messi in campo per la definizione dei processi di

    elaborazione del significato e della struttura sintattica.Presenter le principali conquiste dei relativi studi sul

    linguaggio realizzati attraverso la rilevazione dei potenziali evento-

    relati (ERPs), una metodologia di ricerca basata sullanalisi statistica

    dei dati provenienti dallelettroencefalogramma (EEG) in cooccorrenza

    con stimoli linguistici.

    Questi dati sembrano favorire complessivamente quei modelli

    che, prendendo le distanze dal modello teorico imposto da NoamChomsky negli ultimi cinquantanni, concepiscono la componente

    sintattica del linguaggio come strettamente interagente con le altre

    componenti.

    Illustrer infine i risultati preliminari di uno studio sulleffetto

    degli stati emotivi sullelaborazione del significato, condotto presso il

    Laboratorio ERP del NICI (Nijmegen Institute for Cognition and

    Information, Nijmegen, Paesi Bassi) in collaborazione con Dorothee

    Chwilla e Constance Vissers.

    La componente ERP N400 (indice dello sforzo cognitivo di

    integrazione semantica) in corrispondenza ad elementi linguistici

    inaspettati, appare essere mitigata, come previsto sulla base di

    Federmeier et al. (2001), da uno stato emotivo positivo.

    Anche questo sembra incoraggiare lidea che lelaborazione del

    linguaggio sia strettamente legata ad altre componenti del nostro

    sistema cognitivo.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    5/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 5

    Acknowledgments

    This work is one of the fruits of a scientific path begun in 2004,

    which led me from Sicily to Bologna to the Netherlands.

    It would not have been possible without the continuous

    guidance and support of its inspirator: Prof. Marco Mazzone.

    I had the privilege of attending his course on Language Theory

    (2005), fortunately still running at Scuola Superiore di Catania, which

    broadened my horizons on the nature of language and cognition

    through enlightening explanations and fruitful discussions on a vast

    range of topics.

    I hope that the SSC intellectual space devoted to neurosciences

    of language which he created and promoted will be constantly

    growing and attracting more students.

    The experimental part of my thesis comes from five months

    spent in Nijmegen, a Dutch small and ancient town very close to

    Germany. I was a trainee at NICI (Nijmegen Institute for Cognition

    and Information) under the supervision of Dr. Dorothee Chwilla,

    director of the ERP Lab.

    I want to thank her and Dr. Constance Vissers for offering me

    the chance of getting hands-on experience on event-related potentials

    and for their support during the project.

    A dank u wel goes to all the people who helped me with and

    during the experiments: Hubert, Gerardt, Nan, Sybrine, Dan and

    many others; and to all the participants who lent me their heads and

    three hours of their time.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    6/46

    Daniele Virgillito 6

    Id like to thank all my mates at Scuola Superiore, for the

    unforgettable moments spent with some of them, for letting me

    improve my mimicking skills, and for their effort in keeping SSC alive.

    A special thank goes to all the people who suffer(ed) the

    distance from me: it is hard to go far from who you love in order to

    follow your dreams.

    In conclusion, Id like to give my endless and deepest thanks to

    the one who gave me, alone, most of the things I have now, my

    mother. Without her love, sacrifices and encouragement some of thethings I have done would have sounded simply impossible to me.

    Thanks to all the people I love and who believe in me.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    7/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 7

    1. Linguistic theories and findings

    1.1The layers of language

    A large part of theories on language has depicted it as a rigidly

    structured system, made up of different parts, combining but not

    interacting each other. Each level of language structure would then be

    separated from the others and ruled by its own inner principles.

    Even in a simple sentence, like John is an American boy, we could

    distinguish the different plans we are addressing. We can analyze

    sounds, and the way they are structured in abstract entities (called

    phonemes) that allow us to recognize words because of slight

    articulatory differences (boy is not toy); we can observe the

    morphological structure (from the ancient greek morph, shape), i.e. the

    way words are built and modified by adding certain linguistic

    elements (American is an adjective derived from the noun America plus

    a suffix: [[America]N+an]A); it is also possible to look at the way words

    are ordered and structured in a sentence, and how these rules differ or

    converge in world languages (the phrase American boy lets us see how

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    8/46

    Daniele Virgillito 8

    English orders coupled adjectives and nouns, namely in a different

    way than many other languages do).

    In addition to this, we can understand the message the sentence

    intends to convey because single words are either linked to non-

    linguistic referents or help us to understand the existing relations

    between them: this is called the semantic aspect of language.

    As proposed by Evans1, one could imagine this theoretical

    framework like a cake, composed of several different layers, each

    corresponding to a plan of language (phonetic, morphologic, syntactic,

    semantic). As we will explain soon, many linguistic theories insist in

    addressing empirical questions taking just one of the layers into

    account, never looking at a whole slice of the cake. This isautomatically translated in viewing linguistics as a collection of

    separated sub-disciplines.

    For the present purposes, we will attempt to look more in depth

    at the relationship between two of the mentioned linguistic levels,

    namely the syntactic and the semantic one, i.e. between the way words

    are structured and their meaning. As we will see, this has represented

    a huge battleground for modern studies on language.

    1 See Evans et al. (2007).

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    9/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 9

    We will now move to a brief sketch of some of the assumptions

    underlying the approach outlined above.

    1.2 Chomskys perspective

    The separatist perspective is consistent with a particular view

    of mind structure, the one interpreting it as a set of different modules,

    each devoted to a specific cognitive function and dealing with a certain

    type of information (see Fodor 1983).

    Modules would be organized in a hierarchic way, leading from

    the lowest levels to the higher ones, namely from the simplest specific

    cognitive operations to the most complex and general ones. Modelsbased on such view of mental processes have for this reason been

    labeled as bottom up, as opposed to top-down, i.e. based on high-

    level influence over lower stages of elaboration.

    In the realm of linguistics, one theoretical approach has tried to

    offer a totally modular perspective on language, inspired by the work

    of Noam Chomsky and his Generative Grammar paradigm. Chomsky

    (1957) argues that a sentence like Colorless green ideas sleep furiously

    shows the necessary features to be recognized as grammatical by a

    native speaker of English. This happens independently from other

    kinds of knowledge: in this sense it is particularly significant to point

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    10/46

    Daniele Virgillito 10

    out that, according to the chomskian view, the syntactic elaboration of

    the sentence proceeds autonomously from the meaning of the

    individual words. It represents an algorithmic and modular process,

    separated at all from semantics.

    This means that during sentence processing, a syntactic

    structure would be built up first, according to syntactic information

    alone (order of constituents, word inflections), and at a later stage

    semantic and pragmatic information would be activated.

    As we will now quickly see, this neat separation of syntax and

    semantics has been questioned by other models of language

    processing, drawing also on experimental evidence coming from

    psycholinguistic research.

    1.3 Models of language processing

    Different approaches to how language is processed differ in the

    stage at which syntactic structure is built and semantic information is

    activated. The generative model, as outlined in 1.2, can be defined as

    a syntax-first perspective on sentence perception.

    The so-called constraint-based models (see for example

    MacDonald, Pearlmutter & Seidenberg 1994) propose that meaning is

    partially accessed during syntactic structure building, and can

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    11/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 11

    somehow influence sentence structuring. Some semantic features, like

    animacy, would tend to be active during the processing and so play an

    early and important role in the final outcome of the analysis. In

    addition to this, a series of factors such as expectancy and frequency of

    use of a syntactic structure would influence the recognition of the

    actual configuration of a sentence.

    Trueswell et al. (1994) experimentally shows with an eye-

    tracking study the effect of meaning over syntactic parsing. The test

    reveals longer fixation-durations on disambiguating phrases like by the

    lawyer in The defendant examined by the lawyer (see fig.1): one could

    plausibly imagine, on a semantic basis, that examined is not a past

    participle but the finite verb bound to the subject the defendant.

    Figure 1

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    12/46

    Daniele Virgillito 12

    Bever, Sanz & Townsend (1998) assign a much more important

    role to semantics. According to them, only semantic and pragmatic

    information is used in a first stage to build an initial representation of

    the sentence, that consequently constrains its syntactic analysis. This

    and similar approaches have been labeled as semantics-first and

    their model can be approximately summarized by the phrase

    semantics proposes, syntax disposes. It is natural that these theories

    are mainly based on non-modular accounts of mental processes, since

    they outline a top-down model of language processing.

    Psycholinguistic research has been trying to empirically test the

    approaches sketched above, attempting to question theoretical models

    by experimentally looking at the way human beings process language.This has led to a strong criticism towards the chomskian framework.

    In the next paragraph, we will recall one of the first and best

    known findings against Chomskys claims.

    1.4Time for meaning

    Language works as part of the human cognitive system and it

    can then be subject to objective scientific measurement. Human

    behaviour is linked to inner mental processes and variations in the

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    13/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 13

    latter ones can lead to variation in behaviour. It is generally assumed

    that statistically significant time differences between behavioural

    responses point to differences in the corresponding mental processes.

    One of Chomskys first claims was that sentence complexity

    depended only on the numbers of syntactic transformations that the

    so-called deep structure (specifying the logical role of each element) had

    to undergo in order to get to the final outcome, the superficial structure

    (i.e., the actual sentence including the final phrase order and

    structure). This has been called the Derivational Theory of Complexity

    (henceforth, DTC).

    For instance, the deep structure

    [[John]NP [[read]V [the book]NP ]VP ]S

    can give rise to the following sentences, differing just in the

    transformational rule(s) applied:

    Transformational Rule Sentence

    Active John read the book.

    Passive The book was read by John.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    14/46

    Daniele Virgillito 14

    Negative John didnt read the book.

    Wh-Question What did John read?

    Passive + Negative The book was not read by John.

    Passive + Wh-Question What was read by John?

    One of the first studies addressing the psychological reality of

    the DTC hypothesis was the one carried out by Slobin (1966). His

    participants had to listen to a sentence while looking at a picture,

    depicting objects and human beings interacting in different ways. The

    task assigned was judging as quickly as possible, by pressing a button,

    whether the picture matched with the sentence.

    The linguistic materials were organized according to two

    independent variables, i.e. active/passive and reversible/irreversible,

    mixed in four final conditions.

    The latter variable (reversibility) consisted in the possibility of

    inverting Agent and Theme in the same sentence with a plausible

    result: The sentence The horse was kicked by the cow is reversible,

    whereas The fence was kicked by the cow is not, since the Theme the fence

    is inanimate and cannot replace, due to the verbs restrictions, the

    Agent the cow.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    15/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 15

    Although the data obtained in the active condition was

    compatible with the chomskian approach, the surprising finding was

    that participants mean reaction times (RTs) in verifying passive

    reversible sentences were higher than those shown for passive

    irreversible ones (see fig.2 for an example).

    Figure 2

    This result was interpreted in the following way: since no

    syntactic divergence can be found between these two kinds of

    sentence, being reversibility a purely semantic feature, the difference

    in the RTs represented a reliable index of semantic influence over

    language processing.

    Slobins is one of the first experimental studies addressing the

    congruence of a theoretical linguistic model with the actual way

    language is processed.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    16/46

    Daniele Virgillito 16

    1.5Looking at the brain

    The natural place for studying cognition in vivo is where mental

    processes are born and spread in time: the human brain. Over the last

    years many brain-imaging techniques have been developed in order to

    find neural correlates of cognitive models. They attempt to find

    reliable relationships between certain patterns of brain activation and

    specific supposed cognitive (sets of) operations.

    At the present state of affairs, these techniques mainly differ in

    their capacity and goals: resonance-based methods, such as fMRI,

    manage to obtain a very detailed spatial resolution of the studied

    processes, but no accurate time resolution. That is to say that they

    easily localize brain activity in specific areas, but cannot exactly

    correlate it with external occurring factors since the latter are usually

    presented very fast.

    EEG (electroencephalogram)-based techniques, on the other

    hand, dont succeed in obtaining an accurate space mapping of

    cerebral activation, but find their strength in their extremely accurate

    time resolution. They can then easily correlate external stimuli

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    17/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 17

    presented at a very fast rate with variations in brain activity unfolding

    in a limited time.

    One of the best known EEG-based techniques, namely ERPs

    (event-related potentials), will be the object of the next chapter. As we

    will see, this experimental methodology has proven to be very useful

    to investigate language: it provided evidence for unravelling the

    complexity of linguistic processes and trying to link some of them to

    more general cognitive domains.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    18/46

    Daniele Virgillito 18

    2. ERPs: what brainwaves can tell about language

    2.1 Event-related Potentials

    Event-related Potentials (ERPs) represent one of the most

    widespread experimental techniques in brain imaging studies about

    language.2

    They are based on Electro-Encephalogram (EEG), a recording of

    the spontaneous electrical human brain activity and its variations over

    time. It is generally measured via electrodes attached on the scalp,

    mounted in an elastic cap.

    ERPs are extracted from the raw EEG by time-locking it to a

    particular sensorimotor or cognitive event, so that it is possible to

    examine the brain response to a specific kind of stimulus. To properly

    extract ERPs, it is necessary to average a large number of records time-

    locked to the same stimulus type.

    The outcome of the averaged signal is a series of negative and

    positive voltage peaks, each one having a specific latency and

    2 See Kutas & Van Petten (1988).

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    19/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 19

    distribution across the scalp. These peaks are called ERP components:

    they can be either exogenous or endogenous.

    The first ones are the earliest components (around 100 ms after

    stimulus onset) and their features mirror the physical properties of

    stimuli. Cognitive studies (in particular linguistic ones) only address

    the so-called endogenous components: these derive from higher levels

    of information processing (their latencies are often over 300 ms) and

    are mainly influenced by psychological factors.

    Endogenous ERP components are usually labeled according to

    two parameters: polarity and peak latency. P300, for instance is a

    positive voltage fluctuation peaking around 300 milliseconds after the

    stimulus onset. We will now review two of the best known language-related ERP components.

    2.2 ERP correlates of semantics and syntax

    The "separatist" approach sketched in chapter 1 has influenced

    the early stage of ERP studies on language. Many scholars encouraged

    the view that semantic and syntactic processing proceeded in

    autonomous ways and were mirrored by different and independent

    ERP components too (see fig. 5 for an example in the same sentential

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    20/46

    Daniele Virgillito 20

    context). This assumption was particularly clear at the beginning, but

    changed somehow as new results appeared.

    The N400 component (a negative potential peaking around 400

    ms after word onset) has traditionally been labeled as an index of

    semantic integration. Kutas and Hillyard (1980) discovered that all

    open class words elicited this kind of negativity. In addition to this,

    they found out that words that rendered sentences semantically

    anomalous elicited a more negative component.

    Figure 3

    One of their most famous examples was He spread the warm bread

    with socks: compared to the brainwave elicited by the final word butter

    in the same sentence, a clearly larger negativity was observed. For this

    reason, N400 has been linked to the ease with which a word is

    semantically integrated in a sentential context (see figure 3).

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    21/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 21

    The brain response to syntactic processing was quite different. A

    later positivity (peaking at about 600 ms after word onset) was

    observed in relation to verb agreement anomalies, or wrong

    constituent order, e.g. The spoilt child throw the toys on the floor, or The

    expensive very tulip... (Hagoort et al. 1993). A late positivity was also

    elicited by garden-path sentences, i.e. initially syntactically ambiguous

    sentences in which the first (more plausible) structural analysis turns

    out to be the wrong one, like in The woman convinced her children are

    noisy (see fig. 4 for an example), where the parser plausibly supposes

    at the beginning that her is a possessive adjective while it is a object

    pronoun, as clear from the overall structure of the sentence.

    Figure 4

    The so called P600 was then defined as the brain reaction to

    grammaticality anomalies, and was thought to mirror a process of

    syntactic reanalysis and reprocessing.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    22/46

    Daniele Virgillito 22

    Figure 5

    As we will now see, new experimental results coming from

    more recent studies suggested partially different and broader

    perspectives on the roles played by N400 and P600 components.

    2.3More than meaning and structure

    As outlined above, the N400 was thought to represent a

    semantic processing component, modulated by meaning relations

    existing in the mental lexicon. As shown by many studies addressing

    the function of N400 in non-anomalous sentences, an N400 effect was

    clearly associated with a lower cloze probability. That is to say that

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    23/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 23

    this negativity was not only depending on semantic factors (although

    some results could speak in favour of this conclusion: see fig. 6).

    Figure 6

    An N400 was clearly elicited by non-anomalous but unexpected

    words, as the final word in He mailed the letter without a thought,

    compared to He mailed the letter without a stamp (Kutas & Hillyard

    1984). It was also observed in world knowledge violations, such as in

    Dutch trains are white: Dutch speakers know, in fact, that they are

    yellow (Hagoort et al. 2004). As a matter of fact, N400 can be defined

    as a general index of word integration, sensitive to semantic,

    pragmatic and discourse factors.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    24/46

    Daniele Virgillito 24

    Several ERP studies showed a clear P600 effect as a response to

    non syntactic anomalies. Vissers, Chwilla, & Kolk (2006) investigated

    the brain response to misspellings at the word level (see fig.7)

    Figure 7

    Their paradigm involved the comparison between a high-cloze

    condition (the critical, misspelled pseudohomophone word was the

    most expected one) and a low-cloze one (the critical, misspelled

    pseudohomophone word was not expected at all), e.g. In the library the

    pupils borrow books/boeks vs. In the library the pupils borrow chairs/chayrs.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    25/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 25

    A clear late positivity (P600) was found in high-cloze sentences

    containing misspellings. This encouraged the perspective according to

    which P600 would reflect a more general process of monitoring in

    language perception, aiming at the detection of a possible processing

    error.

    In the study cited above, this component appeared only in the

    high-cloze pseudohomophone condition, namely the one in which it is

    plausible to suppose that a strong conflict occurred between two

    elements: the phonological representation of the word (perfectly

    congruent with the sentential constraints, and highly expected) and its

    orthographic realization, which triggered a monitoring response.

    As summarized by Vissers et al. (2006),

    The ERP data confirmed the present prediction in that only

    pseudohomophones embedded in a high-cloze context gave rise to a

    P600 effect. Because the words from which the pseudohomophones

    were derived were highly expected, initially the pseudohomophones

    were easily integrated into the higher order meaning representation

    of the context. After all, the phonological representation of the

    pseudohomophone is congruent with the sentential constraints. But

    when the subject detected the misspelling, which signals a possible

    processing error, a monitoring response was triggered.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    26/46

    Daniele Virgillito 26

    Such perspective on P600s functional role has been confirmed

    by further studies addressing a different kind of mismatch at the

    sentence level. Vissers, Chwilla, Van de Meerendonk & Kolk (2008),

    for instance, presented participants several pictures of spatial arrays

    followed by a sentence giving a correct or incorrect description of the

    picture.

    For example:

    Picture Sentence

    The triangle stands above the square.

    The triangle stands below the circle.

    As predicted, the mismatches created a conflict between the

    conceptual representation on the basis of the picture and the actual

    sentence, and therefore led to a P600 effect.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    27/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 27

    2.4Semantics or syntax?

    According to the experimental results illustrated above, no clear

    definition of the functional significance of language-related

    components can be established. Until a few years ago, however, N400

    and P600 were thought to belong to separated, albeit broad, areas of

    sentence processing.

    Further language ERP studies have shown quite controversial

    late positivities in unusual situations (see Kolk & Chwilla 2007). The

    most surprising finding was what we could define a semantic P600.

    As outlined in 2.2, late positivities traditionally occurred in

    sentences showing syntactic anomalies or ambiguities. In particular,

    many experiments addressed a morphosyntactic P600, namely the one

    elicited by anomalies in subject-verb agreement.

    The unexpected result came from semantically anomalous

    sentences like The cat that from the mice fledsing (literal translation

    from Dutch: De kat die voor de muizen vluchtte, see Kolk, Chwilla, Van

    Herten & Oor 2003). Meaning anomalies were supposed to elicit an

    N400 effect and no positivity, but the opposite happened (see fig. 8).

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    28/46

    Daniele Virgillito 28

    Figure 8

    It was however possible to reconcile this surprising finding with

    a syntactic view of P600. The sentence type reported above did not

    involve any kind of syntactic anomaly or ambiguity. Although it is a

    highly implausible sentence (mice are supposed to flee from cats), its

    structure doesnt violate any grammatical rule.

    As a matter of fact, readers processing language dont always

    apply a rigid parse ruled by grammar. The processing of sentences is

    often guided by heuristics-based principles. Heuristic is represented in

    this case by a set of strategies based on plausibility, intended to

    process sentence meanings by overcoming a rule-based analysis of

    sentence structure. In everyday language it is in fact often possible to

    derive a correct structural representation of a sentence simply drawing

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    29/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 29

    on the semantic features of the lexical items, and combining them in a

    plausible way.

    Processing the sentence The cat that from the mice fledsing. could

    have led to assume that the plausible subject would be the mice, and

    then trigger a P600 due to the morphosyntactic anomaly: no agreement

    between the mice (plural) and the verbal form (singular). No

    correspondence between the predicted and the observed inflection.

    This interpretation was questioned by Van Herten, Kolk &

    Chwilla (2005). The same kind of sentences was presented to

    participants, with one difference: the two noun phrases never differed

    in grammatical number, avoiding then agreement errors between the

    plausible subject and its related verb form.Notwithstanding this last modification, a late positivity occurred

    at the verb level. This challenged the mentioned syntactic account of

    P600 and can thus be explained only in terms of monitoring during

    language perception (see 2.3).

    2.5 Chomskian account

    Electrophysiology of language has proven to show a very

    heterogeneous panorama of brain responses to linguistic processes,

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    30/46

    Daniele Virgillito 30

    leading to difficulties in deciphering the functional origins and range

    limits of each ERP component.

    In spite of the limits shown by current accounts of language-

    related brain activation patterns, a remainder of the chomskian

    approach is still present in the debate.

    In particular, language-specificity of ERP components, in spite

    of the counterevidence presented above, has been defended by

    Friederici and other scholars trying to outline a brain activation model

    consistent with the separatist thesis, i.e. the one supporting the

    autonomy of syntactic processes. We will now move to the analysis of

    part of the chomskian approach to neurolinguistic findings (see

    Friederici et al. 2006).The so-called ELAN (early left anterior negativity) was the

    brainwave found in correlation with phrase structure violations. Due

    to its very short latency (between 100 and 250 ms from the stimulus

    onset) and its independence from attentional factors, ELAN was

    defined as an index of early syntactic processing, involving the

    computation of local phrase structures according to word category

    information. The definition of a low-level phrase would be influenced

    by word-level features. A P600 would then be correlated with a final

    integration process leading to the structuring of the overall sentence.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    31/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 31

    Although these results point to a syntactic-only brain mapping

    of linguistic processes, they seem as pointed out by Mazzone (in press)

    somehow to contradict the theoretical assumptions of the chomskian

    Minimalist Program (Chomsky 1995). According to the latter, the same

    operation of Merge (the making up of a new syntactic unit from two

    syntactic objects) should be working both at the local phrase level (e.g.

    the phrase The old man) and at the highest phrase level, i.e. the sentence

    (e.g. The old man bought a small cake).

    The model outlined above seems to correlate Merge at different

    levels with qualitative differences in the cerebral activation timecourse

    and mapping. fMRI studies, in fact, showed the predominance of two

    distinct brain areas: the frontal operculum and Brocas area, and ERPrecording highlighted two different components too.

    These and many other findings discourage the view that

    linguistic processes proceeds through fixed serial stages and are

    centered on syntax. On the one hand, sub-components of language, i.e.

    the layers (see 3.3) in which language is normally dissected, seem

    to continuously interact during related cognitive dynamics; on the

    other hand, it emerges that non-linguistic factors can affect linguistic

    processes, for example their timecourse, and the effort required.

    It is the case of the role of emotive states in semantic processing:

    we will talk about this in the following chapter.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    32/46

    Daniele Virgillito 32

    3. Mood and language

    3.1 Positive mood effects on language

    Event-related potentials have proven to represent a very

    profitable experimental paradigm for assessing not only the basic

    nature of cognitive processing, but also how external psychological

    factors can globally affect the dynamic of specific processes.

    One of the factors involved in these assessments has been

    emotive state. As clearly stated in Federmeier et al. (2001),

    Empirical research concurs with the everyday intuition that

    our moods influence our thinking, judgment, and perceptions.

    Transient, mild positive mood has been shown to increase

    integrative decision-making and subjective risk assessment and

    to facilitate flexibility of thinking and problem solving.

    We will now review two of the most acknowledged findings on

    the effect of positive mood over the language domain.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    33/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 33

    Kirson (1990) used the sentence verification task (asking subjects

    to state if a sentence is true or false). The experiment provided

    participants with sentences describing category exemplar relationships

    (e.g. A robin/coin is a bird). True sentences contained both exemplars

    that were a near associate of the category word and those with a more

    distant association (e.g. A robin/parrot is a bird).

    After inducing either a neutral or a positive mood by showing

    subjects respectively either a boring or an amusing (as rated by a

    group of students) videotape, reaction times were recorded during

    sentence verification. As expected, sentences depicting a close

    relationship were verified in less time than those describing a distant

    relationship. This difference was smaller, however, with positivemood induction, as it specifically shortened participants time to verify

    distant sentences (see fig. 9).

    These findings, replicated in a lexical decision task, supported

    the notion that the semantic distance between distant exemplars and

    their categories was functionally reduced due to the positive mood

    condition.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    34/46

    Daniele Virgillito 34

    Figure 9

    A second study carried out by Federmeier, Kirson, Moreno &

    Kutas (2001) investigated positive mood effect on language processing

    through ERP recording.

    Participants were presented with sentence pair contexts (e.g.

    They wanted to make the hotel look more like a tropical resort. So, along the

    driveway they planted rows of) ended with (1) the most expected

    ending, as determined by cloze probability ratings (expected

    exemplars, e.g. palms), (2) an unexpected item from the expected

    semantic category (within category violations, e.g. pines), or (3) an

    equally unexpected item from a different, though related, semantic

    category (between category violations, e.g. tulips).

    Mood was manipulated exposing participants to different kind

    of photos, intended to engender particular emotive responses. The

    effectiveness of the induction was positively tested after the

    experiment through a questionnaire.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    35/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 35

    As summarized by Federmeier et al. (2001),

    Under neutral mood, N400 amplitudes were smallest for

    expected items and smaller for unexpected items when these

    came from the expected category. In contrast, under positive

    mood, N400 amplitudes to the two types of unexpected items

    did not differ (see fig.10). Positive mood seemed to specifically

    facilitate the processing of distantly-related, unexpected items.

    Figure 10

    In both studies, positive mood contributes to the process of

    semantic retrieval and integration. It facilitates in both cases the access

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    36/46

    Daniele Virgillito 36

    to distant concepts and, according to the N400 effect found, makes

    their processing easier.

    We will now present the preliminary results of our study on the

    effect of emotive state on language processing.

    Our study replicated Vissers, Chwilla, & Kolk (2006)

    investigating brain response to misspellings at the word level, with the

    addition of a mood manipulation.

    We first constructed 127 simple declarative sentence fragments

    and used these in a cloze test with 25 subjects to obtain highly

    expected (high-cloze) critical words. Of these 127 sentences, 116

    sentences were completed with the same word by 91% of the

    participants. These were used as the high-cloze context sentencefragments in this study.

    We then created 116 low-cloze context sentences by exchanging

    the critical word from a high-cloze context fragment with the critical

    word from another high-cloze context fragment. For example, we

    exchanged the critical word from In that library the pupils borrow books

    to take home with the critical word from The pillows are stuffed with

    feathers which makes them feel soft resulting in the following low-cloze

    fragment The pillows are stuffed with books which makes them feel hard. The

    critical word was always in mid-sentence position.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    37/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 37

    A further experimental manipulation was Lexicality (correct

    word vs. pseudohomophone derived from the correct word).

    Every critical word occurred in a correct version and a

    pseudohomophonic version. The pseudohomophone was created by

    changing the vowel of the second syllable, keeping phonology the

    same.

    The two experimental manipulations Context and Lexicality

    were crossed. As a result, there were four conditions and thus, four

    experimental sentence types: high-cloze correct word sentences, high-

    cloze pseudohomophone sentences, low-cloze correct word sentences,

    and low-cloze pseudohomophone sentences; yielding a total set of 464

    sentences. The four versions of each sentence were counterbalancedacross lists. Each list contained each sentence context (in a high-cloze

    or a low-cloze version) and each critical word (in a correct word or a

    pseudohomophone version) only once.

    So, each list contained 29 high-cloze correct word sentences, 29

    high-cloze pseudohomophone sentences, 29 low-cloze correct word

    sentences, and 29 low-cloze pseudohomophone sentences. To each list,

    60 filler sentences were added: 30 correct sentences, 10 sentences with

    a pseudohomophone at the beginning of the sentence, 10 sentences

    with a pseudohomophone in the middle of the sentence, and 10

    sentences with a pseudohomophone at the end of the sentence.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    38/46

    Daniele Virgillito 38

    With the purpose of comparing the monitoring effect supposed

    to be elicited by high-cloze pseudohomophones with the classical

    syntactic P600, we added to the material a set of sentences

    containing morphosyntactic violations. The related ERP results will

    not be included in the present work.

    For the EEG study, participants were seated in an experimental

    room. Sentences were presented in serial visual presentation mode at

    the center of a PC monitor. Word duration was 345 ms and the

    stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) was 645 ms. Sentence final words

    were followed by a full stop. The intertrial interval was 2 s.

    Words were presented in black capitals on a white background

    in a 9 cm by 2 cm window at a viewing distance of approximately 1m. Each sentence was preceded by a fixation cross (duration 510 ms)

    followed by a 500 ms blank screen. The experimental list was split up

    into six blocks; there was a brief pause between blocks and each block

    was preceded by two filler items.

    The mood manipulation was carried out by means of two sets of

    video fragments, each shown before and between each experimental

    block to two groups of participants. They were intended to engender

    positive and negative mood conditions (henceforth, PMC and NMC).

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    39/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 39

    The clips were taken from Happy Feet (G. Miller, Warner

    Bros. 2006) and Sophies Choice (A.J. Pakula, Universal Pictures

    1982) for the positive and negative manipulation respectively.

    The effectiveness of the mood manipulation was assessed

    through an online self-rating of participants emotive state.

    Participants were instructed to attentively read the sentences.

    Because eye movements distort the EEG recording, participants were

    trained to make eye movements, e.g., blinks, only in the period

    between the end of the last sentence and the beginning of the next one.

    The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded with 27 tin

    electrodes mounted in an elastic electrode cap (Electrocap

    International; see fig.11 for the montage).

    Figure 11

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    40/46

    Daniele Virgillito 40

    The electrode positions included standard International 1020

    system locations over the left and right hemispheres at the frontal (F3,

    F4, F7, and F8), midline (Fz, Cz, Pz, and Oz), parietal (P3 and P4), and

    temporal (T5 and T6) sites. Eight extra electrodes were placed at the

    frontal (F3A, F4A, F7A, and F8A), midline (Fza and Oz), and parietal

    (P3P and P4P) sites. In addition, eight electrodes were placed at

    nonstandard electrode positions previously found to be sensitive to

    language manipulations (e.g., Holcomb & Neville, 1990): left and right

    anteriortemporal sites (LAT and RAT: 50% of the distance between

    T3/4 and F7/8), left and right temporal sites (LT and RT: 33% of the

    interaural distance lateral to Cz), left and right temporoparietal (LTP

    and RTP: Wernicke's area and its right hemisphere homologue: 30% ofthe interaural distance lateral to a point 13% of the nasioninion

    distance posterior to Cz), and left and right occipital sites (OL and OR:

    50% of the distance between T5/6 and O1/2). The left mastoid served

    as reference. Electrode impedance was less than 3 k. The electro-

    oculogram (EOG) was recorded bipolarly; vertical EOG was recorded

    by placing an electrode above and below the right eye and the

    horizontal EOG was recorded via a right to left canthal montage. The

    signals were amplified (time constant = 8 s, bandpass = 0.0230 Hz),

    and digitized online at 200 Hz.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    41/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 41

    Presentation of stimuli and recording of performance data were

    accomplished by a Macintosh computer.

    For N400 (300 to 500 ms epoch), an interaction of cloze

    probability and mood was found for the midline and the lateral sites.

    For the midline sites an N400 was present for the positive mood

    condition but absent for the sad condition. For the lateral sites the

    interaction indicated that the N400 in the PMC was more broadly

    distributed across the scalp than in the NMC.

    Further analyses are still under way on the pseudohomophone

    and syntactic condition.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    42/46

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    43/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 43

    governed by a set of segregated modules, independent from other

    cognitive factors and must be separately studied in all its different

    compartments.

    This paradigm could be extended to other kinds of cognitive

    integration tasks, such as the visual one.

    Moreover, future ERP works could also address different facets

    of semantic integration (e.g. at the verb level), or be integrated with

    computational tools in order to compare and refine ontologies with

    live data on how the brain seems to deal with language.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    44/46

    Daniele Virgillito 44

    Bibliography

    Chomsky, N., 1957. Syntactic Structures. Mouton & Co., The Hague/Paris.

    Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

    Chwilla, D. J., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (1995). The N400 as a function of

    the level of processing. Psychophysiology, 32(3), 274285.

    Bever, T.G., Sanz, M., Townsend, D.J., 1998. The emperors psycholinguistics.

    Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 27 (2), 261284.

    Evans, V., Benjamin Bergen & Joerg Zinken, 2007. The Cognitive Linguistics

    Reader. London: Equinox.

    Federmeier, K. D., Kirson, D. A., Moreno, E. M., and Kutas, M. (2001). Effects

    of transient, mild mood states on semantic memory organization and use: An

    event-related potential investigation in humans. Neuroscience Letters,

    305, 149-152.

    Fodor, J., 2003. The modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology,

    Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

    Friederici, A., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R.I., Anwander, A., 2006. The

    brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: Functional

    localization and structural connectivity, in PNAS, 103 (7), 2458-2463.

    Hagoort, P., Brown, C., Groothusen, J., 1993. The syntactic positive shift (SPS)

    as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. In: Garnsey, S.M. (Ed.),

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    45/46

    Issues on language processing: from theory to brain investigations 45

    Language and Cognitive Processes. Special Issue: Event-Related Brain

    Potentials in the Study of Language, vol. 8(4). Lawrence Erlbaum

    Associates, Hove, pp. 439483Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M., Petersson, K.M., 2004. Integration of

    word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science

    304, 438441.

    Kirson, D.A., 1990. The Effect of Mood on Access to Remote Category Associates ,

    Department of Psychology, University of Denver.Kolk, H.H., Chwilla, D.J., van Herten, M., Oor, P.J., 2003. Structure and limited

    capacity in verbal working memory: a study with event-related potentials .

    Brain Lang. 85, 136.

    Kolk, H.H., Chwilla, D.J., 2007. Late Positivities in unusual situations.

    Brain Lang. 100, 257262.

    Kutas, M., Hillyard, S.A., 1980. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials

    reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207, 203205.

    Kutas, M., Hillyard, S.A., 1984. Brain potentials during reading reflect word

    expectancy and semantic association. Nature 307, 161163.

    Kutas, M., Van Petten, C.K.F., 1994. Psycholinguistics electrified. Event-related

    brain potential investigations. In: Gernsbacher, M.A. (Ed.), Handbook of

    Psycholinguistics. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 83143.

    MacDonald, Maryellen C., Pearlmutter, Neal, and Seidenberg, Mark S., 1994.

    Lexical Nature of Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution. Psychological Review

    101:676-703.

  • 7/31/2019 Issues on Language Processing

    46/46

    Daniele Virgillito 46

    Mazzone, M., in press. Autonomia del linguistico? Alcune tendenze delle

    neuroscienze attuali, in Atti del II Convegno CODISCO

    (Coordinamento Dottorati Italiani Scienze Cognitive).

    Osterhout, L., Holcomb, P.J., 1992. Event-related potentials elicited by syntactic

    anomaly. J. Mem. Lang. 31, 785806.

    Slobin, D. I., 1966. Grammatical transformations and sentence comprehension in

    childhood and adulthood. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal

    Behavior, 5, 219-227.

    Trueswell, J. C. & Tanenhaus, M. K., 1994. Toward a lexicalist framework for

    constraint-based syntactic ambiguity resolution. In Clifton, C., Frazier, L.

    and Rayner, K. (Eds.) Perspectives in Sentence Processing. Lawrence

    Erlbaum Assoc.: Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 155-179.

    Van Herten, M., Kolk, H.H.J., Chwilla, D.J., 2005. An ERP study of P600 effects

    elicited by semantic anomalies. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 241255.

    Van Herten, M., Kolk, H. H. J., & Chwilla, D. (2005). An ERP study of

    P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22,

    241255.

    Vissers, C.T., Chwilla, D. J., & Kolk, H. H. J., 2006. Monitoring in Language

    Perception: the effect of misspellings of words in highly constrained

    sentences. Brain Research.

    Vissers C.T., Kolk H.H., van de Meerendonk N., Chwilla D.J., 2008.

    Monitoring in language perception: evidence from ERPs in a picture-